Abstract
General anesthesia should induce unconsciousness and provide amnesia. Amnesia refers to the absence of explicit and implicit memories. Unlike explicit memory, implicit memory is not consciously recalled, and it can affect behavior/performance at a later time. The impact of general anesthesia in preventing implicit memory formation is not well-established. We performed a systematic review with meta-analysis of studies reporting implicit memory occurrence in adult patients after deep sedation (Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation of 0–1 with spontaneous breathing) or general anesthesia. We also evaluated the impact of different anesthetic/analgesic regimens and the time point of auditory task delivery on implicit memory formation. The meta-analysis included the estimation of odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). We included a total of 61 studies with 3906 patients and 119 different cohorts. For 43 cohorts (36.1%), implicit memory events were reported. The American Society of Anesthesiologists (ASA) physical status III–IV was associated with a higher likelihood of implicit memory formation (OR:3.48; 95%CI:1.18–10.25, p < 0.05) than ASA physical status I–II. Further, there was a lower likelihood of implicit memory formation for deep sedation cases, compared to general anesthesia (OR:0.10; 95%CI:0.01–0.76, p < 0.05) and for patients receiving premedication with benzodiazepines compared to not premedicated patients before general anesthesia (OR:0.35; 95%CI:0.13–0.93, p = 0.05).
Keywords: implicit memory, general anesthesia, awareness, anesthesia brain monitor, benzodiazepines
1. Introduction
One of the most important goals of general anesthesia is to ensure the patient’s unconsciousness and unresponsiveness during induction and maintenance [1] as well as to achieve post-operative amnesia, usually detected by the absence of an explicit recall [2].
A popular assessment tool to evaluate responsiveness during anesthesia is the Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation Scale (OAA/S) [3]. The OAA/S score, which evaluates the patient’s behavioral response, speech, facial expressions, and ocular activity, ranges from a score of 5 (awake state and responsiveness) to 0 (unconscious state and unresponsiveness even to noxious stimuli). The use of neuromuscular blocking agents (NMBAs), decreasing muscle tone and preventing sudden muscle movements in response to a noxious stimulus, allows surgery under light general anesthesia, which can increase the risk of awareness and connected consciousness [1], even when an anesthesia brain monitor (ABM) is used [4]. While anesthesia awareness has an incidence of only 0.1–0.2% [1], connected consciousness, as detected by the isolated forearm technique, has a higher incidence, up to 34.8% [4]. The experience of such conscious episodes can lead to an increased risk of post-traumatic stress disorders [5,6].
The explanation for the low incidence of surgical recall may be due to the amnestic properties of the benzodiazepines and other hypnotics administered during general anesthesia [7]. Therefore, the gap between the incidence of anesthesia awareness and explicit recall, and the incidence of connected consciousness can be explained by the fact that patients may be aware of surgical events at the time they occur but may be unable to remember them later [8].
While explicit memory is recalled spontaneously, or may be provoked by postoperative events or questioning, implicit memory is not consciously recalled, and may affect behavior or performance at a later time. Implicit memory refers to any change in experience, thought, or action that is attributable to a past event [8] and can be detected with psychological tests, such as free association test, category member generation [9], word stem completion test [10], or the process dissociation procedure [11].
Several variables during anesthesia seem to interfere with implicit memory formation. They can be related to (i) anesthesia (like the type and dosage of hypnotic/analgesic drugs delivered for induction and/or maintenance, the usage of NMBA, anesthesia duration, or the use of ABM), (ii) the timing of the auditory task adopted for implicit memory formation (before skin incision or during surgical stimulation, repeated presentation of the words during the whole anesthesia period or otherwise), and (iii) the time point of implicit memory testing after the return of consciousness. However, the literature contains contrasting results regarding what has a significant impact on implicit memory formation during surgery and anesthesia.
Therefore, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis to evaluate the influence of these variables on implicit memory formation.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Search Strategy
We performed a systematic review and meta-analysis of previously published studies that investigated the occurrence of implicit memory after deep sedation or general anesthesia. Here, deep sedation refers to a spontaneously breathing patient with an OAA/S score of 0 or 1 (no response to mild prodding or shaking and no response to a noxious stimulus).
For the study design and the presentation of this report, we followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA, www.prisma-statement.org) (accessed on 16 August 2021) and we registered this meta-analysis in the PROSPERO international prospective register of systematic reviews (at the University of York, York, UK. Registration number: CRD42020170668).
We conducted a comprehensive English-language literature search of Medline, EMBASE and Google Scholar databases and used the following Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) terms: implicit memory, implicit recall, awareness, learning, free association, free recall, category member generation, word stem completion, and process dissociation procedure. These MeSH terms were also combined with the following additional terms using the ‘AND’ function: general anesthesia, anesthesia, sedation, OAA/S, intravenous, inhalational, minimum alveolar concentration (MAC). The search period included articles published between 1980 and March 2021. The date of the last search was 31 March 2021.
Two authors (FL, DO) independently identified potentially eligible articles by assessing titles and abstracts and reviewed full-text versions of these articles to select the studies to be included in this systematic review. In case of any disagreements regarding inclusion at either the title or abstract screening or the full-text review stage, a third author (MC) was consulted to resolve the issue.
2.2. Eligibility and Inclusion
We included controlled or observational trials if they involved adult patients (≥18 years) and listed implicit memory testing as a study endpoint.
A neuropsychologist (EM) critically evaluated the intraoperative methods, the post-operative psychological test batteries and the adopted criteria for the definition of implicit memory. Her evaluation was based on the analysis of the auditory task to be remembered that was presented to the patient during anesthesia, as well as on the psychological tests applied for the detection of implicit memory formation. Only if the authors performed the following standardized psychological tests for implicit memory detecting, the study was included: category generation task (CGT), sentence generation task (SGT), free association test (FAT), word stem completion test (WSC), word recognition test (WRT), general knowledge (GK), forced-choice recognition (FCR), preference, familiarity fame judgements (FFJ), process dissociation procedure (PDP) were considered [12,13,14].
We excluded studies that involved pediatric patients, brain and head-neck surgery, that did not clearly specify the used anesthetic regimen and the method for implicit memory testing (both, during intraoperative memory strategy (i.e., acquisition of words/sentences) and/or during the post-operative interview testing), or had no clearly defined criteria for implicit memory incidence. If studies evaluated implicit memory using (i) post-operative hypnosis, (ii) intra-operative behavioral or therapeutic suggestions, (iii) sounds (i.e., listening to a bird singing), or (iv) had not a control group, we excluded them. We also did not consider review articles and case reports.
2.3. Data Extraction
The following information was extracted from the articles and categorized: patient age (≤50 years or >50 years), American Society of Anesthesiologist physical status classification (only ASA I-II or also ASA III–IV), type of anesthetic approach (general anesthesia or deep sedation -OAA/S 0-1 with spontaneous breathing), duration of anesthetic procedure (≤60 min or >60 min), premedication with benzodiazepines (yes or no), anesthesia induction with benzodiazepines (yes or no), anesthesia maintenance (inhalational or intravenous), drugs (nitrous oxide use, benzodiazepines, opioids, NMBAs) used during anesthesia maintenance (yes or no), ABM use (yes or no), ABM-guided anesthesia (yes or no), surgical stimulation during the auditory task (yes or no), time point of auditory task delivery (throughout the entire anesthesia period or only for a time interval) (yes or no), time point of postoperative memory testing (≤24 h or >24 h).
We also estimated the anesthetic and analgesic level for each study. Therefore, two authors (FL and DO), independently categorized the anesthesia and analgesia regimen of each study (based on anesthetic and analgesic drugs and dosage applied during surgery) as ‘light’, ‘adequate’, or ‘deep’. Any disagreements were resolved by consensus with input from a third author (MC). Everyone was blinded to the outcome for this estimation. Therefore, another author (EM) prepared separate files not containing implicit memory rates, with a randomized sequence of anesthetic/analgesic regimens to be analyzed.
Every study considered for the meta-analysis was categorized according to these variables, and in case of different cohorts analyzed in the same trial, they were considered separately.
2.4. End-Points
The primary end-point was the incidence of implicit memory after general anesthesia or deep sedation. This incidence rate is defined as the rate between cohorts with patients developing an implicit memory to the total number of cohorts included in our analysis.
Secondary endpoints were the association between the considered variables and the implicit memory rates after general anesthesia and/or deep sedation or following only inhalational or only intravenous anesthesia maintenance regimens.
2.5. Assessment of Risk of Bias
The risk of bias of the included studies was assessed with the Cochrane risk of bias tool [15].
2.6. Statistical Analysis
For statistical analysis, we created a database for each endpoint (implicit memory after (i) general anesthesia and deep sedation, (ii) only general anesthesia, (iii) only inhalational maintenance anesthesia, or (iv) only intravenous maintenance anesthesia). The rates of implicit memory for these four endpoints were also evaluated according to anesthesia duration, benzodiazepine use (at premedication, at induction or during maintenance of anesthesia), the type of anesthetic regimen during maintenance of anesthesia (light or deep, with or without nitrous oxide), the type of analgesic regimen during maintenance of anesthesia (light or deep, with or without opioids), NMBA use, surgical stimulation during auditory task, the time point(s) of auditory task (during all the anesthetic period or only for a time interval), the usage of the ABM and ABM-guided anesthesia, and the timing of memory testing after surgery.
We estimated Odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) by normal approximation and we performed the meta-analyses of single proportions within a frequentist framework, using both random- and fixed-effect models. For continuity correction, we added 0.5 to the frequencies of every study, and we used a logit transformation to calculate the overall proportions. Confidence intervals for the individual studies were computed with the Clopper–Pearson method. The random-effect model was computed with inverse-variance weighting using the Der Simonian–Laird method to account for heterogeneity. We tested the heterogeneity across studies with Cochran’s Q statistic and the I2 statistic. A value of p < 0.1 indicated heterogeneity and we defined I2 > 50 to be substantial. All p-values were 2-tailed, with statistical significance set at <0.05. Funnel plots helped to visually assess potential publication bias, and formal linear regression tests of the funnel plot asymmetry were performed. We used R (version 3.3.1 for Windows) and the meta package for the analyses [16].
3. Results
From the 1167 potentially relevant studies that we initially identified in the literature, we excluded 1106 studies because they did not meet the inclusion criteria, were duplicates, or were incomplete in their methods or in their outcome data. Therefore, 61 studies, [9,10,11,12,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73] involving a total of 3906 patients and 119 different cohorts, formed the basis for meta-analysis. The mean (±standard deviation) age and weight of the included patients was 45 (±10.49) years and 74.4 (±6.56) kg, respectively. Supplementary Table S1 presents the general patient characteristics and the applied cohorts.
In general, the patients were scheduled mainly for general, gynecological, orthopedic, maxilla-facial, ophthalmic, plastic, vertebral, cardio-vascular, and urological surgery. A total of seven trials enrolled patients undergoing deep sedation without surgical stimulation.
Figure 1 presents the PRISMA flow diagram of the study selection process and Figure 2 shows the risk of BIAS summary of included studies. Among trials, we found a low risk of BIAS and a medium heterogeneity.
3.1. Implicit Memory Rates after General Anesthesia and OAA/S 1 Sedation
A total of 61 studies [9,10,11,12,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73] (44 of them were randomized controlled trials (RCTs)) with a total of 3906 patients grouped into 119 cohorts evaluated implicit memory after deep sedation or general anesthesia. For 43 cohorts (36.13%), implicit memory formation was reported at postoperative evaluation.
We found a statistically significant difference in implicit memory formation for ASA physical status classification.
ASA III–IV patients had a significantly higher risk of implicit memory formation (OR [95%CI]: 3.48 [1.18–10.25], p < 0.05) compared to ASA I–II patients. Further, patients with deep sedation (OAA/S 0–1 and spontaneous breathing), compared to patients under general anesthesia, had a significantly lower risk of implicit memory formation (OR [95%CI]: 0.10 [0.01–0.76]) (Table 1).
Table 1.
Variable | Implicit Memory | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Yes | No | OR | L95%CI | U95%CI | p-Value | |
Age | ||||||
≤50 years | 31 | 58 | ||||
>50 years | 11 | 10 | 2.06 | 0.79 | 5.38 | 0.22 |
ASA | ||||||
I–II | 23 | 56 | ||||
III–IV | 10 | 7 | 3.48 | 1.18 | 10.25 | 0.04 |
Type of anesthesia | ||||||
General anesthesia | 42 | 61 | ||||
Deep sedation | 1 | 15 | 0.10 | 0.01 | 0.76 | 0.017 |
Duration | ||||||
≤60 min | 4 | 16 | ||||
>60 min | 25 | 39 | 2.56 | 0.77 | 8.56 | 0.20 |
Premedication | ||||||
No benzodiazepines | 34 | 52 | ||||
Benzodiazepines | 7 | 23 | 0.47 | 0.18 | 1.20 | 0.16 |
Induction | ||||||
No benzodiazepines | 40 | 70 | ||||
Benzodizepines | 3 | 6 | 0.88 | 0.14 | 4.37 | 1.0 |
Maintenance | ||||||
Intravenous | 29 | 42 | ||||
Inhalational | 14 | 34 | 1.68 | 0.77 | 3.67 | 0.26 |
No N2O during maintenance | 21 | 46 | ||||
N2O during maintenance | 22 | 30 | 1.61 | 0.76 | 3.42 | 0.30 |
No benzodiazepines | 42 | 74 | ||||
Benzodiazepines | 1 | 2 | 0.88 | 0.02 | 17.42 | 1.0 |
No opioids | 6 | 23 | ||||
Opioids | 37 | 53 | 2.68 | 0.99 | 7.21 | 0.08 |
No NMBA | 4 | 18 | ||||
NMBA | 39 | 58 | 3.02 | 0.95 | 9.62 | 0.09 |
No light anesthetic regimen | 29 | 52 | ||||
Light anesthetic regimen | 14 | 24 | 1.05 | 0.47 | 2.33 | 1.0 |
No deep anesthetic regimen | 14 | 25 | ||||
Deep anesthetic regimen | 29 | 51 | 1.02 | 0.46 | 2.25 | 1.0 |
No light analgesic regimen | 25 | 41 | ||||
Light analgesic regimen | 18 | 35 | 0.84 | 0.40 | 1.80 | 0.80 |
No deep analgesic regimen | 18 | 25 | ||||
Deep analgesic regimen | 25 | 51 | 1.02 | 0.46 | 2.25 | 1.0 |
Monitoring | ||||||
No ABM monitoring | 24 | 48 | ||||
ABM monitoring | 15 | 28 | 1.07 | 0.48 | 2.38 | 1.0 |
No AMB-guided anesthesia | 36 | 57 | ||||
ABM-guided anesthesia | 7 | 18 | 0.62 | 0.23 | 1.62 | 0.45 |
Listening to the auditory task | ||||||
No during surgical stimulation | 5 | 19 | ||||
During surgical stimulation | 38 | 57 | 2.53 | 0.87 | 7.37 | 0.13 |
No during all the maintenance period | 31 | 52 | ||||
During all the maintenance period | 7 | 16 | 0.73 | 0.27 | 1.98 | 0.71 |
Timing of memory testing | ||||||
≤24 h | 31 | 54 | ||||
>24 h | 12 | 20 | 1.05 | 0.45 | 2.42 | 1.0 |
ASA: American Society of Anesthesiologist physical status classification; deep sedation: Observer Assessment of Alertness/Sedation score 0–1; NMBA: neuromuscular blocking agent; ABM: anesthesia brain monitor; OR: odds ratio; L95%CI and U95%CI: lower limit and upper limit of the 95% confidence interval (CI).
3.2. Implicit Memory Rates after General Anesthesia
A total of 50 studies [9,10,11,12,17,18,19,20,22,23,24,25,26,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,45,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,58,60,61,62,63,65,68,69,70,71,73] (37 of them were RCTs) with a total of 3645 patients grouped into 103 cohorts evaluated implicit memory formation after general anesthesia. For 42 cohorts (40.78%) implicit memory formation at postoperative evaluation was reported.
Based on the variables analyzed, we did not find a statistically significant difference in implicit memory rates (Table S2). Benzodiazepines premedication seems to have a protective role in preventing implicit memory formation, p = 0.05 (OR [95%CI]: 0.35 [0.13–0.92])
3.3. Implicit Memory Rates after General Anesthesia with Inhalational Maintenance
A total of 34 studies [9,10,11,12,17,18,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,28,29,30,31,33,35,38,41,42,43,45,49,52,58,60,63,65,69,70,71,73] (27 of them were RCTs) with a total of 2402 patients grouped into 67 cohorts evaluated implicit memory occurrence after general anesthesia with inhalational maintenance. Implicit memory formation at post-operative evaluation was found for 28 cohorts (41.79%).
There was no significant difference in the implicit memory rates for the analyzed variables (Table S3). Benzodiazepines premedication seems to have a protective role in preventing implicit memory formation, p = 0.09 (OR [95%CI]: 0.26 [0.07–1.03])
3.4. Implicit Memory Rates after General Anesthesia with Intravenous Maintenance
A total of 22 studies [11,19,29,32,34,35,37,39,40,41,48,50,51,52,53,55,56,61,62,68,70,71] (15 of them were RCTs) with a total of 1243 patients grouped into 36 cohorts evaluated implicit memory occurrence after general anesthesia with intravenous anesthesia maintenance. Implicit memory formation at post-operative evaluation was described for 14 cohorts (38.89%).
There was no significant difference in the implicit memory rates for the analyzed variables detection (Table S4).
4. Discussion
This systematic review and meta-analysis suggests that while the patients’ comorbidity may increase the likelihood of implicit memory formation, general anesthesia with premedication may decrease the likelihood of implicit memory formation. Further, deep sedation (Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation of 0–1 with spontaneous breathing) seems more protective in implicit memory formation than general anesthesia regimens.
4.1. Patient Characteristic Considerations
According to this meta-analysis, an ASA physical status III–IV, compared to ASA physical status I–II, significantly increased the risk of implicit memory formation (OR 3.48). For several reasons, this may mainly be attributed to the tendency to use light anesthesia in these patients. The patients’ comorbidity (e.g., presence of limited cardiac reserve) and other factors (e.g., reduced cardiac output, hypovolemia, hemodynamic instability) may influence anesthetic delivery and predispose the patient to a period of light anesthesia [74]. Light anesthesia, the increased anesthetic requirement of some patients (e.g., younger age, tobacco smoking, long-term use of certain drugs (alcohol, opiates, or amphetamines), and machine malfunction or misuse, resulting in an inadequate delivery of the anesthetic, are usually associated with the risk of awareness as well as explicit and implicit memory formation [75].
Even if the impact of physiological changes on the pharmacokinetics/pharmacodynamics of anesthetic drugs as well as on ABM observed with aging [76,77,78] was postulated to influence explicit and implicit memory formation [79], in our study, aging seems not to be a risk factor for implicit memory occurrence. The age-induced decline of implicit memory (e.g., priming) is less than the decline of explicit memory (e.g., recognition) [79]. Explicit memory and implicit memory involve different parts of the brain: while the hippocampus is necessary for explicit memory, the amygdala, a small structure located near the hippocampus, is essential for implicit memory formation [80]. A surgery-induced stress response causing elevated norepinephrine levels and amygdala activity (in addition to other regions) would facilitate the implicit learning of emotionally negative information presented during anesthesia [49]. Although the amygdala remains structurally preserved during normal aging, functional magnetic resonance imaging studies revealed an age-related decrease in its activating response to negative stimuli [81]. Hence, this dichotomous age-induced influence on the amygdala could justify the different literature findings on the impact of aging on implicit memory after anesthesia.
4.2. Anesthesiologic Considerations
Implicit memory formation was higher among the general anesthesia regimens (40.78%) than among the deep sedation regimens (6.25%) evaluated. A high percentage of general anesthesia cohorts used NMBAs (94.1%) and opioids (87.3%), whereas the deep sedation cohorts were conducted without NMBAs (100%) and, with exception of two studies [46,57], without opioids (87.5%). These findings support the literature and reinforce the potential role of general anesthesia in implicit memory occurrence [68]. Sedation is a continuum that proceeds from minimal to deep levels in a dose-response manner, which may ensure the immobility of the patient following repeated or painful stimulation. Instead, general anesthesia is distinct in which unconsciousness, analgesia, and immobilization are provided by different drugs [82]. Both strategies may, then, ensure immobility during the procedure, but with different levels of suppression of consciousness and memory formation [83]. Administering NMBAs for the immobility of the patient could favor light general anesthesia, which, in turn, increases the incidence of explicit recall and awareness [75,84,85]. In our meta-analysis, the use of NMBAs was shown to have the potential of increasing the likelihood of implicit memory (OR 3.02).
In our study opioids were not significantly related to implicit memory formation, supporting literature findings that suggest that implicit memory formation should be considered despite the use of opioids [68,85,86]. Recent evidence shows that hippocampal µ-opioid receptors on GABAergic neurons mediate the stress-induced impairment of memory retrieval [87]. However, opioids, which reduce the amount of anesthetic drugs necessary for the loss of consciousness [88], may also increase the risk of light anesthesia [1], Furthermore, an anesthetic regimen with a low opioid dose, perhaps with less potency such as fentanyl or sufentanil compared with remifentanil, would not prevent endogenous catecholamine release in response to surgical stimulation, which would, in turn, enhance implicit memorization [68].
On the other hand, most of the deep sedation regimens were based on propofol or midazolam, two γ-aminobutyric acid type A receptor agonists with an amnestic effect [80], which were administered in the absence of opioids and NMBAs, at a dosage to prevent a reaction following repeated or painful stimulation.
Even if a difference in implicit memory rates between light and deep general anesthesia regimens was not found, it may depend on the heterogeneity of the studies and the method to establish a ‘light’ or ‘deep’ anesthetic regimen. A ‘subjective’ evaluation of an anesthetic plan, based on the type and the dosage of drugs used during the surgery, was adopted by three authors. An anesthetic regimen judged ‘adequate, not light’, might be sufficient to prevent conceptual priming but insufficient to prevent perceptual priming, which represents the basis of implicit memory generation, during surgery [89]. High noxious stimulation, such as surgical stimulation, may cause hypnotic state fluctuations associated with very short periods of awareness and the reinforcement of memory formation caused by amygdala stimulation via endogenous stress hormone release, which finally leads to implicit memory formation [49,68]. This may also explain why ABM (and ABM-guided anesthesia) has no effect in preventing implicit memory formation. The close proximity of the sensor to the rostral structures of the brain allows detection of EEG signals correlated with the neural functions of the cerebral cortex, which relate mainly to wakefulness and awareness [90]. However, some brain areas (e.g., cerebellum, striatum, hippocampus, amygdala, substantia nigra, ventral tegmental area of the midbrain), that are essential for the formation, re-organization, consolidation, and storage of memory and involved in implicit memory formation, are excluded by brain monitoring [91,92].
Premedication with benzodiazepines, well known for their amnestic properties and their effect on memory formation by impairing the ability to acquire new information [93], can help to avoid awareness and it is particularly suggested when light anesthesia is anticipated [90], Our meta-analysis showed that premedication with benzodiazepines may also avoid implicit memory formation during general anesthesia (OR = 0.35). The potential benefit seems greater during the maintenance of general anesthesia with inhalational agents (OR = 0.26) rather than with an intravenous agent (OR = 0.48). Even if we did not find statistically differences in implicit memory formation comparing inhalational versus intravenous general anesthesia alone, this may result from the different impacts of inhalational and intravenous drugs on memory and a synergism between benzodiazepines and inhalational anesthetic drugs in preventing implicit memory [93,94], It has been shown that sub-hypnotic doses of propofol block hippocampal but not amygdala response to emotionally arousing memory tasks [95], Instead, inhalational anesthetic agents, such as halothane, isoflurane, sevoflurane, and desflurane may impair learning and memory at sub-hypnotic concentrations [94] by slowing the hippocampal θ-rhythm [96] that is implicated in memory processing through activation of plasticity and interregional signal integration, which impairs hippocampus-dependent implicit memory and learning formation [97], This may explain why, in our meta-analysis, premedication with benzodiazepines resulted in greater benefits in inhalational anesthesia than in intravenous anesthesia.
4.3. Auditory Task Characteristic Considerations
The considerations made so far help form an understanding of the potential role of the time point the auditory task was delivered (continuously or not continuously during anesthesia, during or not during surgical stimulation) for implicit memory formation. While the auditory task was presented during surgical stimulation in only 25% of the deep sedation regimens, it was presented in 88.35% of the total general anesthesia regimens. In addition, no tasks were executed continuously during deep sedation regimens, whereas 23.71% of those during general anesthesia were. Based on our data, implicit memory is not induced by repetitive auditory stimulation during surgery and requires top-down processing, which is suppressed by the respective anesthetic plan [98]. However, while the timing of the presentation of the auditory task is not significantly related to implicit memory formation during general anesthesia, its role cannot be completely overlooked [46,48]. Inadequate analgesia regimens, particularly during surgical stimulation, can lead to fluctuations of consciousness that, while not sufficient for explicit memory formation, can be responsible for implicit memory [52], which is enhanced by the release of endogenous catecholamine and the subsequent simulation of the amygdala [56]. In such situations, auditory stimulation (e.g., listening to auditory tasks) may predispose a patient to implicit memory formation even while unconscious [5,82], Even if not statistically different, listening to the auditory task during surgical stimulation has an OR of 2.53 in implicit memory formation, and this, along with the different surgery stimulus allowed in general anesthesia in respect to deep sedation (with a lower endogenous catecholamine release during deep sedation surgery reasonably possible), might have played a role in our results reflecting higher cases of implicit memory during general anesthesia than deep sedation.
4.4. Limitations
This study has some limitations. First, there were not enough deep sedation regimens to perform a subgroup analysis such as for only general anesthesia, so we were unable to analyze each variable compared to the implicit memory rate for only deep sedation regimens. Second, although a neuropsychologist reduced heterogeneity by excluding some implicit memory detection techniques (e.g., hypnosis, behavioral and therapeutic suggestions, listening to nature sounds as hearing stimuli), not all of the studies considered adopted the same standardized tests at the same time intervals (however, this variable was considered and found not to be significant). Additionally, a high degree of heterogeneity among different forms of conduction of general anesthesia was found, especially with respect to the types and doses of drugs used, although this diversity could reflect daily anesthetic practices.
5. Conclusions
In our meta-analysis, patients with a higher ASA physical status (III–IV) were at a significantly higher likelihood of implicit memory formation. Deep sedation was associated with a significantly lower incidence of implicit memory formation. Meanwhile, the type of anesthetic/analgesic regimen for maintenance does not significantly impact on implicit memory formation. Premedication with benzodiazepines before inhalational general anesthesia seems to be more protective than intravenous one.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Anna Paola Mazzarolo, PsyD, and Paolo Zanatta, of the Department of Anaesthesia of Azienda Ospedaliera Universitaria Integrata Verona, Italy, for their contribution to reviewing the manuscript.
Supplementary Materials
The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/life11080850/s1, Table S1. Caracteristic of considered trials. Table S2. Distribution of cohorts among the considered variables during only general anaesthesia regimens. Table S3. Distribution of cohorts among the considered variables during only inhalational general anaesthesia regimens. Table S4. Distribu-tion of cohorts among the considered variables during only intravenous general anaesthesia regimens.
Author Contributions
F.L., D.P.O. and E.M. conceived of the study, acquired, collected, and analyzed data, drafted and revised the final manuscript. P.T. collected data, performed the statistical analysis, analyzed data, and revised the final manuscript. M.K., R.D.S. and M.C. participated in the conceiving of the study, analyzed data, participated in the discussion of the results, and revised the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research received no external funding.
Institutional Review Board Statement
Not applicable.
Informed Consent Statement
Not applicable.
Data Availability Statement
The data presented in this study are available in Supplementary Tables S1–S4.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Sanders R.D., Tononi G., Laureys S., Sleigh J.W. Unresponsiveness ≠ unconsciousness. Anesthesiology. 2012;116:946–959. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318249d0a7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Eger E.I., Sonner J.M. Anaesthesia defined (gentlemen, this is no humbug) Best Pract. Res. Clin. Anaesthesiol. 2006;20:23–29. doi: 10.1016/j.bpa.2005.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chernik A.D., Gillings D., Laine H., Hendler J., Silver J.M., Davidson A.B., Schwam E.M., Siegel J.L. Validity and reliability of the Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation Scale: Study with intravenous midazolam. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 1990;10:244–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Linassi F., Zanatta P., Tellaroli P., Ori C., Carron M. Isolated forearm technique: A meta-analysis of connected consciousness during different general anaesthesia regimens. Br. J. Anaesth. 2018;121:198–209. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2018.02.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Osterman J.E., Hopper J., Heran W.J., Keane T.M., van der Kolk B.A. Awareness under anesthesia and the development of post-traumatic stress disorder. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry. 2001;23:198–204. doi: 10.1016/S0163-8343(01)00142-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Avidan M.S., Mashour G.A. Prevention of intraoperative awareness with explicit recall: Making sense of the evidence. Anesthesiology. 2013;118:449–456. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e31827ddd2c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Polster M.R., Gray P.A., O’Sullivan G., McCarthy R.A., Park G.R. Comparison of the sedative and amnesic effects of midazolam and propofol. Br. J. Anaesth. 1993;70:612–616. doi: 10.1093/bja/70.6.612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kihlstrom J.F., Dorfman J., Park L. Conscious and Unconscious Memory. Blackwell Companion Conscious. 2017:562–575. doi: 10.1002/9781119132363.ch40. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Westmoreland C.L., Sebel P.S., Winograd E., Goldman W.P. Indirect Memory during Anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 1993;78:237–241. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199302000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Block R., Ghoneim M.M.M., Ping M.S.T.S., Ali M.M.A. Human Learning during General Anaesthesia and Surgery. Br. J. Anaesth. 1991;66:170–178. doi: 10.1093/bja/66.2.170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Flouda L., Pandazi A., Papageorgiou C., Perrea D., Krepi E., Kostopanagiotou G. Comparative effects of sevoflurane and propofol based general anaesthesia for elective surgery on memory. Arch. Med. Sci. 2013;1:105–111. doi: 10.5114/aoms.2013.33351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Millar K., Watkinson N. Recognition of words presented during general anaesthesia. Ergonomics. 1983;26:585–594. doi: 10.1080/00140138308963377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Buchner A., Erdfelder E., Vaterrodt-Plunnecke B. Toward unbiased measurement of conscious and unconscious memory pro-cesses within the process dissociation framework. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1995;124:137–160. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Andrade J. Learning during anaesthesia: A review. Br. J. Psychol. 1995;86:479–506. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02566.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Higgins J.P.T., Altman D.G., Gøtzsche P.C., Jüni P., Moher D., Oxman A.D., Savović J., Schulz K.F., Weeks L., Sterne J.A.C., et al. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2011;343:d5928. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Schwarzer G., Carpenter J., Rücker G. Meta-Analysis with R. Springer; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Jelicic M., Bonke B., Appelboom D. Indirect memory for words presented during anaesthesia. Lancet. 1990;336:249. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(90)91776-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kihlstrom J.F., Schacter D.L., Cork R.C., Hurt C.A., Behr S.E. Implicit and Explicit Memory following Surgical Anesthesia. Psychol. Sci. 1990;1:303–306. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00222.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Bethune D.W., Ghosh S., Gray B., Kerr L., Walker I.A., Doolan L.A., Harwood R.J., Sharples L.D. Learning during General Anaesthesia: Implicit Recall after Methohexitone or Propofol Infusion. Br. J. Anaesth. 1992;69:197–199. doi: 10.1093/bja/69.2.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Brown A.S., Best M.R., Mitchell D.B., Haggard L.C. Memory under anesthesia: Evidence for response suppression. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 1992;30:244–246. doi: 10.3758/BF03330456. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Dwyer R., Bennett H.L., Eger E.I., Peterson N. Isoflurane anesthesia prevents unconscious learning. Anesth. Analg. 1992;75:107–112. doi: 10.1213/00000539-199207000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Jelicic M., Bonke B., Wolters G., Phaf R.H. Implicit memory for words presented during anaesthesia. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 1992;4:71–80. doi: 10.1080/09541449208406243. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Jelicic M., De Roode A., Bovill J.G., Bonke B. Unconscious learning during anaesthesia. Anaesthesia. 1992;47:835–837. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.1992.tb03140.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cork Randall C., Kihistrom John F., Schacter Daniel L. Absence of Explicit or Implicit Memory in Patients Anesthetized with Sufentanil/Nitrous Oxide. Anesthesiology. 1992;76:892–898. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199206000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bonebakker A.E., Bonke B., Klein J., Wolters G., Hop W.C.J. Implicit memory during balanced anaesthesia. Anaesthesia. 1993;48:657–660. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.1993.tb07172.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Jelicic M., Asbury A.J., Millar K., Bonke B. Implicit learning during enflurane anaesthesia in spontaneously breathing patients? Anaesthesia. 1993;48:766–768. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.1993.tb07586.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Munglani R., Andrade J., Sapsford D.J., Baddeley A., Jones J.G. A measure of consciousness and memory during isoflurane ad-ministration: The coherent frequency. Br. J. Anaesth. 1993;71:633–641. doi: 10.1093/bja/71.5.633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Parker C.J.R., Oates J.D.L., Boyd A.H., Thomas J.D. Memory for auditory material presented during anaesthesia. Br. J. Anaesth. 1994;73:123–124. doi: 10.1093/bja/73.1.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Schwender D., Kaiser A., Klasing S., Peter K., Pöppel E. Midlatency Auditory Evoked Potentials and Explicit and Implicit Memory in Patients Undergoing Cardiac Surgery. Anesthesiology. 1994;80:493–501. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199403000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.De Roode A., Jelicic M., Bonke B., Bovill J.G. The effect of midazolam premedication on implicit memory activation during alfentanil-nitrous oxide anaesthesia. Anaesthesia. 1995;50:191–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.1995.tb04553.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gonsowski C.T., Chortkoff B.S., Eger E.I., Bennett H.L., Weiskopf R.B. Subanesthetic concentrations of desflurane and isoflu-rane suppress explicit and implicit learning. Anesth. Analg. 1995;80:568–572. doi: 10.1097/00000539-199503000-00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Van Hooff J.C., De Beer N.A., Brunia C.H., Cluitmans P.J., Korsten H.H., Tavilla G., Grouls R. Information processing during cardiac surgery: An event related potential study. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1995;96:433–452. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(95)00079-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bonebakker A., Bonke B., Klein J., Wolters G., Stijnen T., Passchier J., Merikle P.M. Information processing during general anesthesia: Evidence for unconscious memory. Mem. Cogn. 1996;24:766–776. doi: 10.3758/BF03201101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Donker A.G., Phaf R.H., Porcelijn T., Bonke B. Processing familiar and unfamiliar auditory stimuli during general anesthesia. Anesth. Analg. 1996;82:452–455. doi: 10.1097/00000539-199603000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Russell I.F., Wang M. Absence of memory for intraoperative information during surgery under adequate general anaesthesia. Br. J. Anaesth. 1997;78:3–9. doi: 10.1093/bja/78.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Struys M., Versichelen L., Byttebier G., Mortier E., Moerman A., Rolly G. Clinical usefulness of the bispectral index for titrating propofol target effect-site concentration. Anaesthesia. 1998;53:4–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.1998.00279.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Munte S., Kobbe I., Demertzis A., Lullwitz E., Munte T.F., Piepenbrock S., Leuwer M. Increased Reading Speed for Stories Presented during General Anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 1999;90:662–669. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199903000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Münte S., Lüllwitz E., Leuwer M., Mitzlaff B., Münte T.F., Hussein S., Piepenbroc S.A. No implicit memory for stories played during isoflurane/alfentanil/nitrous oxide anes-thesia: A reading speed measurement. Anesth. Analg. 2000;90:733–738. doi: 10.1097/00000539-200003000-00041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Münte S., Münte T.F., Grotkamp J., Haeseler G., Raymondos K., Piepenbrock S., Kraus G. Implicit memory varies as a function of hypnotic electroencephalogram stage in surgi-cal patients. Anesth. Analg. 2003;97:132–138. doi: 10.1213/01.ANE.0000068828.76045.C3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Münte S., Münte T.F., Mitzlaff B., Walz R., Leuwer M., Piepenbrock S. Postoperative reading speed does not indicate implicit memory in elderly cardiac patients after propofol and remifentanyl anaesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 2001;45:750–755. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-6576.2001.045006750.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Münte S., Schmidt M., Meyer M., Nager W., Lüllwitz E., Münte T.F., Piepenbrock S. Implicit memory for words played during isoflurane-or propofol-based anesthesia: The lexical decision task. Anesthesiology. 2002;96:588–594. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200203000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lubke G.H., Kerssens C., Gershon R.Y., Sebel P.S. Memory Formation during General Anesthesia for Emergency Cesarean Sections. Anesthesiology. 2000;92:1029–1034. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200004000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lubke G.H., Kerssens C., Phaf H., Sebel P.S. Dependence of Explicit and Implicit Memory on Hypnotic State in Trauma Patients. Anesthesiology. 1999;90:670–680. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199903000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.De Roode A., van Gerven J.M., Schoemaker R.C., Engbers F.H., Olieman W., Kroon J.R., Cohen A.F., Bovill J.G. A comparison of the effects of propofol and midazolam on memory dur-ing two levels of sedation by using target-controlled infusion. Anesth. Analg. 2000;91:1056–1061. doi: 10.1097/00000539-200011000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ghoneim M.M., Block R., Dhanaraj V.J., Todd M.M., Choi W.W., Brown C.K. Auditory evoked responses and learning and awareness during general anesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 2000;44:133–143. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-6576.2000.440202.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Stapleton C.L., Andrade J. An Investigation of Learning during Propofol Sedation and Anesthesia Using the Process Dissociation Procedure. Anesthesiology. 2000;93:1418–1425. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200012000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Renna M., Lang E.M., Lockwood G.G. The effect of sevoflurane on implicit memory: A double-blind, randomised study. Anaesthesia. 2000;55:634–640. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2044.2000.01326.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Andrade J., Englert L., Harper C., Edwards N. Comparing the effects of stimulation and propofol infusion rate on implicit and explicit memory formation. Br. J. Anaesth. 2001;86:189–195. doi: 10.1093/bja/86.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gidron Y., Barak T., Henik A., Gurman G., Stiener O. Implicit learning of emotional information under anesthesia. NeuroReport. 2002;13:139–142. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200201210-00032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kerssens C., Klein J., van der Woerd A., Bonke B. Auditory information processing during adequate propofol anesthesia moni-tored by electroencephalogram bispectral index. Anesth. Analg. 2001;92:1210–1214. doi: 10.1097/00000539-200105000-00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kerssens C., Lubke G.H., Klein J., van der Woerd A., Bonke B. Memory function during propofol and alfentanil anesthesia: Pre-dictive value of individual differences. Anesthesiology. 2002;97:382–389. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200208000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kerssens C., Ouchi T., Sebel P.S. No evidence of memory function during anesthesia with propofol or isoflurane with close con-trol of hypnotic state. Anesthesiology. 2005;102:57–62. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200501000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Loveman E., Van Hooff J., Smith D. The auditory evoked response as an awareness monitor during anaesthesia. Br. J. Anaesth. 2001;86:513–518. doi: 10.1093/bja/86.4.513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Russell I.F., Wang M. Absence of memory for intra-operative information during surgery with total intravenous anaesthesia. Br. J. Anaesth. 2001;86:196–202. doi: 10.1093/bja/86.2.196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lequeux P.-Y., Cantraine F., Levarlet M., Barvais L. Absence of explicit and implicit memory in unconscious patients using a TCI of propofol. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 2003;47:833–837. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-6576.2003.00159.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Deeprose C., Andrade J., Harrison D., Edwards N. Unconscious auditory priming during surgery with propofol and nitrous oxide anaesthesia: A replication. Br. J. Anaesth. 2005;94:57–62. doi: 10.1093/bja/aeh289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Lequeux P.Y., Velghe-Lenelle C.E., Cantraine F., Sosnowski M., Barvais L. Absence of implicit and explicit memory during propofol/remifentanil anaesthesia. Eur. J. Anaesthesiol. 2005;22:333–336. doi: 10.1017/S0265021505000566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Stonell C., Leslie K., He C., Lee L. No Sex Differences in Memory Formation during General Anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 2006;105:920–926. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200611000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Wang Y., Yue Y., Sun Y.-H., Wu A.-S. Can bispectral index or auditory evoked potential index predict implicit memory during propofol-induced sedation? Chin. Med. J. 2006;119:894–898. doi: 10.1097/00029330-200606010-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Dobrunz U.E.G., Jaeger K., Vetter G. Memory priming during light anaesthesia with desflurane and remifentanil anaesthesia. Br. J. Anaesth. 2007;98:491–496. doi: 10.1093/bja/aem008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bejjani G., Lequeux P.-Y., Schmartz D., Engelman E., Barvais L. No Evidence of Memory Processing During Propofol-Remifentanil Target-Controlled Infusion Anesthesia With Bispectral Index Monitoring in Cardiac Surgery. J. Cardiothorac. Vasc. Anesth. 2009;23:175–181. doi: 10.1053/j.jvca.2008.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Hadzidiakos D.A., Horn N., Degener R., Buchner A., Rehberg B. Analysis of Memory Formation during General Anesthesia (Propofol/Remifentanil) for Elective Surgery Using the Process-dissociation Procedure. Anesthesiology. 2009;111:293–301. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181ac4a4b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kerssens C., Gaither J.R., Sebel P.S. Preserved memory function during bispectral index-guided anesthesia with sevoflurane for major orthopedic surgery. Anesthesiology. 2009;111:518–524. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181b05f0b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Tian S.Y., Zou L., Quan X., Zhang Y., Xue F.S., Ye T.H. Effect of midazolam on memory: A study of process dissociation procedure and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Anaesthesia. 2010;65:586–594. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.2010.06343.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Ozcan M., Gronlund S., Trojan R., Khan Q., Cure J., Wong C. Does a BIS-Guided Maintenance of Anesthetic Depth Prevent Implicit Memory? Psychology. 2011;2:143. doi: 10.4236/psych.2011.23023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Liu X., Lauer K.K., Ward B.D., Rao S., Li S.-J., Hudetz A.G. Propofol disrupts functional interactions between sensory and high-order processing of auditory verbal memory. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012;33:2487–2498. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Quan X., Yi J., Ye T.H., Tian S.Y., Zou L., Yu X.R., Huang Y.G. Propofol and memory: A study using a process dissociation procedure and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Anaesthesia. 2013;68:391–399. doi: 10.1111/anae.12147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Lequeux P.-Y., Hecquet F., Bredas P. Does Anesthetic Regimen Influence Implicit Memory During General Anesthesia? Anesth. Analg. 2014;119:1174–1179. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000000162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Levantesi L., Oggiano M., Sicuranza R., Canistro G., Sessa F., de Waure C., Congedo E., De Cosmo G. Relationships between Bispectral Index, Implicit Memory, Dream Recall and Minimum Alveolar Concentration in Blended Anaesthesia. J. Anesth. Crit. Care Open Access. 2016;5:00194. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Chen X.D., Xie W., Zhou Q.H. Effect of propofol and sevoflurane on cognitive function among elderly patients undergoing elec-tive surgery under anesthesia. Pak. J. Pharm. Sci. 2018;31:2909–2913. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Elbadawy A.M., Khidr A.M., Saleh A.A. Comparative study of implicit memory during bispectral index guided total intrave-nous anesthesia versus sevoflurane inhalation anesthesia. Egypt. J. Anaesth. 2015;31:9–13. doi: 10.1016/j.egja.2014.09.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kallionpaa R.E., Scheinin A., Kallionpaa R.A., Sandman N., Kallioinen M., Laitio R., Laitio T., Kaskinoro K., Kuusela T., Revonsuo A. Spoken words are processed during dexmedetomidine-induced unresponsiveness. Br. J. Anaesth. 2018;121:270–280. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2018.04.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Aceto P., Valente A., Gorgoglione M., Adducci E., De Cosmo G. Relationship between awareness and middle latency auditory evoked responses during surgical anaesthesia. Br. J. Anaesth. 2003;90:630–635. doi: 10.1093/bja/aeg113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Mitchell D.B., Brown A.S., Murphy D.R. Dissociations between procedural and episodic memory: Effects of time and aging. Psychol. Aging. 1990;5:264–276. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.5.2.264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Ghoneim M.M., Weiskopf R.B. Awareness during Anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 2000;92:597. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200002000-00043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Schnider T.W., Minto C.F., Gambus P.L., Andresen C., Goodale D.B., Shafer S.L., Youngs E.J. The influence of method of administration and covariates on the pharmacokinet-ics of Propofol in adult volunteers. Anesthesiology. 1998;88:1170–1182. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199805000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Carron M., Bertoncello F., Ieppariello G. Profile of sugammadex for reversal of neuromuscular blockade in the elderly: Current perspectives. Clin. Interv. Aging. 2017;13:13–24. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S134108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Linassi F., Kreuzer M., Maran E., Farnia A., Zanatta P., Navalesi P., Carron M. Age infuences on Propofol estimated brain con-centration and entropy during maintenance and at return of consciousenss during total intravenous anesthesia with target-controlled infusion in unparalyzed patients: An observational prospective trial. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0244145. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Ward E.V., Berry C.J., Shanks D.R. Age effects on explicit and implicit memory. Front. Psychol. 2013;4:639. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Samuel N., Taub A., Paz R., Raz A. Implicit aversive memory under anaesthesia in animal models: A narrative review. Br. J. Anaesth. 2018;121:219–232. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2018.05.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Nashiro K., Sakaki M., Mather M. Age differences in brain activity during emotion processing: Reflections of age-related de-cline or increased emotion regulation? Gerontology. 2012;58:156–163. doi: 10.1159/000328465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Becker D.E. Pharmacodynamic Considerations for Moderate and Deep Sedation. Anesth. Prog. 2012;59:28–42. doi: 10.2344/0003-3006-59.1.28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Antognini J.F., Schwartz K. Exaggerated Anesthetic Requirements in the Preferentially Anesthetized Brain. Anesthesiology. 1993;79:1244–1249. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199312000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Ghoneim M.M., Block R.I., Haffarnan M., Mathews M.J. Awareness during anesthesia: Risk factors, causes and sequelae: A re-view of reported cases in the literature. Anesth. Analg. 2009;108:527. doi: 10.1213/ane.0b013e318193c634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Cook T.M., Andrade J., Bogod D.G., Hitchman J.M., Jonker W.R., Lucas N., Mackay J.H., Nimmo A.F., O’Connor K., O’Sullivan E.P., et al. The 5th National Audit Project (NAP5) on accidental awareness during general an-aesthesia: Patient experiences, human factors, sedation, consent and medicolegal issues. Anaesthesia. 2014;69:1102–1116. doi: 10.1111/anae.12827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Veselis R.A. Memory: A guide for anaesthetists. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Anaesthesiol. 2007;21:297–312. doi: 10.1016/j.bpa.2007.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Shi M.M., Fan K.M., Qiao Y.N., Xu J.H., Qiu L.J., Li X., Liu Y., Qian Z.Q., Wei C.L., Han J., et al. Hippocampal µ-opioid receptors on GABAergic neurons mediate stress-induced impairment of memory retrieval. Mol. Psychiatry. 2020;25:977–992. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0435-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Egan T.D. Are opioids indispensable for general anaesthesia? Br. J. Anaesth. 2019;122:e127–e135. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2019.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Squire L.R. Memory and Brain Systems: 1969–2009. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:12711–12716. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3575-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Fahy B.G., Chau D.F. The Technology of Processed Electroencephalogram Monitoring Devices for Assessment of Depth of An-esthesia. Anesth. Analg. 2018;126:111–117. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000002331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Squire L.R., DeDe A.J. Conscious and Unconscious Memory Systems. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 2015;7:a021667. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Alkire M.T., Gruver R., Miller J., McReynolds J.R., Hahn E.L., Cahill L. Neuroimaging analysis of an anesthetic gas that blocks human emotional memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:1722–1727. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711651105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Veselis R.A., Reinsel R., Feshchenko V.A., Wronski M. The Comparative Amnestic Effects of Midazolam, Propofol, Thiopental, and Fentanyl at Equisedative Concentrations. Anesthesiology. 1997;87:749–764. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199710000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Chortkoff B.S., Bennett H.L., Eger E.I. Subanesthetic concentrations of isoflurane suppress learning as defined by the cate-gory-example task. Anesthesiology. 1993;79:16–22. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199307000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Pryor K., Root J., Mehta M., Stern E., Pan H., Veselis R., Silbersweig D. Effect of propofol on the medial temporal lobe emotional memory system: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in human subjects. Br. J. Anaesth. 2015;115:i104–i113. doi: 10.1093/bja/aev038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Perouansky M., Rau V., Ford T., Oh S.I., Perkins M., Eger E.I. Slowing of the hippocampal θ-rhythm correlates with anesthetic-induced amnesia. Anesthesiology. 2010;113:1299. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181f90ccc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Nishikawa K., MacIver M.B. Agent-selective effects of volatile anesthetics on GABAA receptor-mediated synaptic inhibition in hippocampal interneurons. Anesthesiology. 2001;94:340–347. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200102000-00025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Aberg K.C., Albrecht E., Tartaglia E.M., Farron A., Soom P., Herzog M.H. Anesthesia prevents auditory perceptual learning. Anesthesiology. 2009;111:1010–1015. doi: 10.1097/01.anes.0000363310.19041.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data presented in this study are available in Supplementary Tables S1–S4.