Abstract
Introduction
Rates of light smoking have increased in recent years and are associated with adverse health outcomes. Reducing light smoking is a challenge because it is unclear why some but not others, progress to heavier smoking. Nicotine has profound effects on brain reward systems and individual differences in nicotine’s reward-enhancing effects may drive variability in smoking trajectories. Therefore, we examined whether a genetic risk factor and personality traits known to moderate reward processing, also moderate the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine.
Methods
Light smokers (n = 116) performed a Probabilistic Reward Task to assess reward responsiveness after receiving nicotine or placebo (order counterbalanced). Individuals were classified as nicotine dependence ‘risk’ allele carriers (rs16969968 A-allele carriers) or non-carriers (non-A-allele carriers), and self-reported negative affective traits were also measured.
Results
Across the sample, reward responsiveness was greater following nicotine compared to placebo (p = 0.045). For Caucasian A-allele carriers but not non-A-allele carriers, nicotine enhanced reward responsiveness compared to placebo for those who received placebo first (p = 0.010). Furthermore, for A-allele carriers but not non-A-allele carriers who received nicotine first, the enhanced reward responsiveness in the nicotine condition carried over to the placebo condition (p < 0.001). Depressive traits also moderated the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine (p = 0.010) and were associated with blunted reward responsiveness following placebo but enhanced reward responsiveness following nicotine.
Conclusion
These findings suggest that individual differences in a genetic risk factor and depressive traits alter nicotine’s effect on reward responsiveness in light smokers and may be important factors underpinning variability in smoking trajectories in this growing population.
Implications
Individuals carrying genetic risk factors associated with nicotine dependence(rs16969968 A-allele carriers) and those with higher levels of depressive personality traits, showmore pronounced increases in reward learning following acute nicotine exposure. These findingssuggest that genetic and personality factors may drive individual differences in smoking trajectoriesin young light smokers by altering the degree to which nicotine enhances reward processing.
Clinical trial registration
NCT02129387 (pre-registered hypothesis: www.clinicaltrials.gov)
Introduction
Although rates of heavy smoking across the general population have declined, the proportion of young light smokers has increased in recent years.1 Approximately 25% of smokers in the U.S. report smoking fewer than 5 cigarettes/day, and these individuals tend to be under 30 years of age.2 Young light smokers display highly varied smoking trajectories; over two years, 7–32% will quit, 21–35% will increase their use, and the rest will maintain their baseline smoking frequency.3 Relative to non-smoking, light smoking confers a 3- to 5-fold increased risk of lung cancer and a 3-fold increased risk of death from cardiovascular disease.4 Accordingly, understanding the factors that influence smoking trajectories in this population is critical for reducing smoking-related morbidity and mortality.
Questionnaire and interview-based studies on smoking motivations indicate that light smokers smoke to reduce negative affect and to improve concentration (for a review, see5). However, self-report measures of smoking motivation have several well-documented limitations, including a lack of sensitivity to the immediate cognitive/affective and physiological effects of acute nicotine.6,7 Given these limitations, the National Cancer Institute published a monograph concluding that behavioral and physiological endophenotypes, including those quantified using laboratory-based nicotine challenge studies, are needed to better characterize individual differences in nicotine’s effects.8
Effects of Nicotine on Reward Processing
Mounting evidence indicates that acute nicotine enhances reward processing. Studies using the Probabilistic Reward Task (PRT)9 – a well-validated behavioral measure of reward learning – have shown that in rodents with chronic nicotine exposure and humans with nicotine dependence, nicotine enhances the ability to learn from prior rewards.10–12 Reward learning is reliant on phasic striatal dopamine signaling,13,14 and increases in reward learning following acute nicotine exposure are consistent with nicotine’s ability to amplify reward-related striatal dopamine activity.15
Opposite findings have been observed during nicotine withdrawal. Nicotine-dependent individuals who have undergone a period of abstinence show reduced reward learning on the PRT10 that correlates with craving intensity.16 In rats with chronic nicotine exposure, the deficit in reward learning is reversed following nicotine re-exposure.10 This suggests that in the context of nicotine dependence, a decrease in reward learning during withdrawal and remediation of this decrease by nicotine re-exposure is likely a key factor that maintains smoking. However, less is known about the role of reward processing in light smokers. Many light smokers easily abstain from smoking beyond the initial period of nicotine withdrawal, so smoking to alleviate withdrawal symptoms cannot fully explain continued smoking behavior in this population. An alternate possibility is that pre-existing factors that moderate the effects of nicotine on reward processing may determine individual differences in smoking trajectories among light smokers.
Potential Moderating Influence of Genetic Factors
Nicotine dependence is highly heritable, for a review, see,17 suggesting that genetic factors may contribute to variability in smoking trajectories. In support of this, Genome-Wide Association Studies have identified a consistent link between the single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) rs16969968 of the nicotine acetylcholine receptor (nAChR) alpha-5 subunit gene (CHRNA5) or its proxy (rs1051730) and increased smoking frequency.18–20 The rs16969968 SNP encodes an Asp398Asn polymorphism resulting in an aspartic acid (G-allele) change to asparagine (A-allele).21,22 Expression of the A-allele reduces nAChR function.21 Reduced nAChR function is proposed to facilitate the development of nicotine dependence via impacting dopamine-mediated reward signaling21 (although a separate theory posits that A-allele carriers smoke to remediate cognitive deficits23). Indeed, prior work has shown that the A-allele was associated with a pleasurable “buzz” when recalling reactions to smoking one’s first cigarette,24 suggesting that this genetic marker may moderate nicotine’s reward-enhancing effects. Variation in nAChR function has also been suggested to alter nicotine intake by disrupting the inhibitory signaling in the habenula, which is responsible for the aversive effects of high doses of nicotine.25
Potential Moderating Influence of Negative Affective Traits
Smoking plays an important role in regulating negative affect26 and individual differences in negative affective traits may moderate the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine in light smokers. Depression and anxiety are highly comorbid with smoking and have been linked to multiple stages of the smoking trajectory, including smoking initiation,27 progression to regular smoking,28 development of nicotine dependence29 and risk for smoking cessation failure.30 These same traits have been associated with impaired reward processing. For example, individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD; particularly those with high levels of anhedonia),9,31 remitted MDD,32 as well as individuals exposed to acute stress,33 show disrupted reward learning on the PRT. However, this deficit is normalized in remitted34 and acutely depressed35 individuals who smoke. These findings suggest that negative affective traits may promote greater smoking because nicotine temporarily remediates pre-existing reward processing deficits. However, studies testing this hypothesis in light smokers are lacking.
The Present Study
Individual differences in genetic risk and negative affective traits may provide a key source of variability in smoking trajectories. The aim of this study was to provide the first empirical test of whether rs16969968 allelic variation and negative affective traits moderate changes in reward learning following acute nicotine in young light smokers. We hypothesized rs16969968 risk allele carriers, as well as individuals with greater negative affective traits, would show greater increases in reward learning following nicotine relative to placebo. Consistent with prior links between rs16969968 allelic variation, negative affective traits, and disrupted striatal dopamine function,21 we predicted that nicotine would have a normalizing effect on reward learning in these individuals.
Method
Participants
Young light smokers (n = 123) aged 18–24 were recruited as part of a broader study (see Supplement). Participants smoked 5–35 cigarettes/week for the past 3 months with no previous smoking level exceeding 35 cigarettes/week. History of smoking and drug use was evaluated using modified sections from the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA)36,37 and the Timeline Follow-back for tobacco and other nicotine use.38 Participants were excluded if they used psychoactive drugs, including smokeless tobacco (e.g., vaping), on a weekly basis [exceptions to this were alcohol (<30 drinks per week), marijuana (<3 uses per week), and caffeine (any amount)], if they met criteria for a mood, anxiety or psychotic disorder on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (SCID-Research Version),36 if they reported a history of head injury with loss of consciousness for greater than 10 minutes, current significant medical or neurological illness, were pregnant, breastfeeding, had motor impairment preventing task completion or cognitive or intellectual impairment. All participants were asked to refrain from smoking for at least 12 hours before each testing session. Procedures were approved by the Southern Illinois University Human Subjects Committee and all participants gave written informed consent before participating.
Quantified Smoke Delivery System (QSDS)
Author DG developed the QSDS which produces reliable standard doses of smoke-delivered nicotine with a low variation of plasma nicotine concentration.39 This system delivers smoke into the participant’s mouth by means of a motorized syringe. Relative to placebo (ultra-low nicotine but normal “tar”), QSDS-delivered nicotine produces the same electroencephalographic, hormonal,40,41 mood,42 and cognitive performance enhancements40 as ad-lib smoking, yet with lower variability in blood nicotine concentration. This allows for improved characterization of individual differences in nicotine-related effects.
Cigarettes
Cigarettes were Camel Lights™ with an FTC-procedure estimated machine-delivered 0.8mg nicotine and Quest3™ (0.05mg nicotine [ultra-low nicotine placebo]).
Measures of Nicotine Dependence and Negative Affective Traits
Nicotine dependence was assessed using the Hooked on Nicotine Checklist (HONC),43 which assesses diminished autonomy over smoking and is considered especially well-suited for use in light smokers.44 Anhedonia was measured using the Fawcett-Clark Pleasure Capacity Scale,45 which is a 36-item self-report measure of capacity for pleasure (higher scores indicate greater pleasure/less anhedonia). Depressive and anxious personality traits were measured using the Depressive and Anxious facet scales of the NEO Personality Inventory-Neuroticism subscale,46 and participant’s tendency to smoke in response to negative emotions was measured using the Smoking Motivation Questionnaire’s Negative Affect Reduction subscale.47
Probabilistic Reward Task (PRT)
Reward learning was assessed using the Probabilistic Reward Task (PRT).9 On each trial of this computerized task, participants are presented with schematic faces with two eyes and a nose. Next, a horizontal line mouth is presented quickly (100ms) and participants indicate whether the mouth was long (11mm) or short (10mm). The task consists of three blocks of 100 trials, and on 40% of correct trials, participants receive a monetary reward (“Correct! You won 20 cents”). Long and short mouths are presented at equal frequency, however, unbeknownst to participants, correct identification of one mouth (“rich stimulus”) is rewarded three times more frequently than the other (“lean stimulus”). Among healthy controls, this asymmetrical reinforcement ratio induces a behavioral response bias toward the rich stimulus,9 reflective of an individual’s sensitivity to reward. Prior studies show that nicotine significantly enhances response bias.11 To avoid practice effects, two task versions were administered: one where the mouth length varied and another where the nose length varied (order counterbalanced). The PRT was administered approximately 5 minutes after nicotine (or placebo) administration, and nicotine and placebo sessions occurred at least 24 hours apart.
PRT Data Reduction
Aligning with prior studies that have used the PRT,9 a quality control assessment was first carried out. Specifically, trials where the reaction time (RT) was <150ms or >2500ms were excluded, along with trials in which the RT fell ±3SD from the mean. Subjects were excluded from analyses if more than 10% of trials were reaction time outliers. Subjects were also required to perform above chance accuracy (≥55%) to ensure that they were exposed to the intended asymmetrical (3 : 1) reinforcement schedule.
Next, signal detection analysis48 was used to calculate response bias and discriminability (the ability to distinguish between the mouth sizes) for each block of the task using the formulae:
To compute values for cases that had a zero in the formula, 0.5 was added to every cell in the matrix. Although discriminability is commonly the key outcome of interest in signal detection tasks, response bias was the key outcome of interest in this study because it provides a measure of the degree to which the participant implicitly learns to alter their behavior as a function of the asymmetrical reinforcement schedule. Hence, response bias provides a behavioral readout of an individual’s responsivity to reward.
Genetic Information
DNA extraction and genotyping were completed by the Hope Center DNA/RNA Purification Core at Washington University School of Medicine. DNA was extracted from saliva samples collected using Oragene DNA Self-Collection Kits (DNA Genotek, Ottawa, Canada). Single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) genotyping assays for rs16969968 were run in duplicate using the KBiosciences Competitive Allele-Specific PCR SNP genotyping system (KASPar). Alleles (A = minor, G = major) were coded to test a dominant genetic model, i.e., comparing carriers with at least one copy of the risk allele (A/A or A/G) to those without (G/G).
Statistical Analyses
The effects of nicotine on response bias and discriminability were analyzed with a 2 (Drug: placebo, nicotine) × 3 (Block: 1, 2, 3) × 2 (Order: placebo first, nicotine first) analysis of variance (ANOVA). Drug and Block were within-subjects factors and Order was a between-subjects factor. Order was included given prior evidence showing that order of nicotine administration may moderate nicotine’s effects on reward learning.11
Genetic effects were examined using a 2 (SNP: A-allele carrier, non-A-allele carrier) × 2 (Drug) × 2 (Order) × 3 (Block) ANOVA. SNP was a between-subjects factor. Given that allele frequency is known to differ in Caucasian individuals relative to individuals of other races,24 we ran this analysis both in the whole sample and in Caucasians alone. Finally, the moderating effects of negative affective traits were examined with regression models for repeated-measures data using the xtreg procedure in STATA 13.0 that included effects of Drug, Order, and Block, along with Trait and Drug × Trait terms. Tests for moderating effects of Sex were also examined and are reported in the Supplement.
Results
Sample Characteristics
In total, 116 participants completed the PRT, and 106 had valid data for both the placebo and the nicotine conditions (all participants retained in the analysis had a minimum of 80% valid trials; Mean ± SD = 294 ± 10). Of the 10 participants who were excluded, 4 were A-allele carriers and 6 were non-A-allele carriers. n = 115 participants provided a saliva sample for genotyping, therefore one individual was not included in the genetic subgroup analysis. Sample characteristics are shown in Table 1.
Table 1.
Sample characteristics
Whole sample (n= 106) | A-allele carriers (n = 52) | Non-A-allele carriers (n = 53) | Test | df | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age, M (SD) | 20.32 (1.93) | 20.29 (1.94) | 20.28 (1.88) | t = 0.02 | 103 | 0.99 |
Male, N (%) | 68 (64.2) | 33 (63.5) | 35 (66.0) | χ 2 = 0.12 | 1 | 0.73 |
Education, M (SD) | 3.13 (0.90) | 3.08 (0.93) | 3.13 (0.79) | t = 0.33 | 103 | 0.74 |
Caucasian, N (%) | 79 (74.5) | 44 (84.6) | 34 (64.2) | χ 2 = 5.50 | 1 | 0.02 |
Hispanic, N (%) | 13 (12.3) | 8 (15.4) | 5 (9.4) | χ 2 = 0.93 | 1 | 0.34 |
Age 1st smoke, M (SD) | 16.22 (2.14) | 16.48 (1.72) | 15.92 (2.47) | t = 1.34 | 103 | 0.19 |
Years smoked, M (SD) | 3.35 (2.33) | 3.23 (1.97) | 3.42 (2.66) | t = 0.40 | 103 | 0.69 |
HONC, M (SD) | 3.59 (2.86) | 3.58 (2.96) | 3.58 (2.81) | t = 0.01 | 103 | 0.99 |
Smoking Pros, M (SD) | 22.55 (6.67) | 23.96 (6.91) | 21.26 (6.23) | t = 2.10 | 103 | 0.04 |
Smoking Cons, M (SD) | 25.54 (7.91) | 24.88 (8.49) | 26.15 (7.40) | t = 0.82 | 103 | 0.42 |
Fawcett, M (SD) | 141.60 (18.04) | 141.79 (19.41) | 141.13 (16.82) | t = 0.19 | 103 | 0.85 |
NEO-Dep, M (SD) | 14.45 (5.77) | 14.44 (6.10) | 14.45 (5.47) | t = 0.01 | 103 | 0.99 |
NEO-Anx, M (SD) | 16.03 (4.65) | 15.96 (4.78) | 16.09 (4.57) | t = 0.15 | 103 | 0.89 |
SMOQ-Neg, M (SD) | 68.62 (22.97) | 68.60 (21.60) | 68.64 (24.46) | t = 0.01 | 103 | 0.99 |
Note. M = Mean; SD = Standard deviation; HONC = Hooked on Nicotine Checklist; Smoking Pros = measure assessing positive attitudes to smoking; Smoking Cons = Measure assessing negative attitudes to smoking; Fawcett = Fawcett-Clarke Pleasure Scale; NEO-Dep = Depression facet of the NEO Personality Inventory Neuroticism Scale; NEO-Anx = Anxious facet of the NEO Personality Inventory Neuroticism Scale; SMOQ-Neg = Smoking Motivations Questionnaire-Avoid negative emotions subscale. One individual did not provide a salvia sample for genotyping and was therefore not included in the genetic subgroup analysis.
Effects of Nicotine on Response Bias
A main effect of Block emerged from the ANOVA, F(2,208) = 36.12, p < 0.001, ƞ p2 = 0.26, where averaged across drug conditions, response bias in blocks 2 and 3 was significantly higher than in block 1 (both ps < 0.001), indicating that the task was effective at inducing a response bias toward the more frequently rewarded stimulus. Furthermore, a main effect of Drug emerged, F(1,104) = 4.11, p = 0.045, ƞ p2 = 0.04, where averaged across blocks, response bias was significantly higher in the nicotine relative to the placebo condition (Fig. 1A). Finer-grained analyses showed that this was due to nicotine increasing correct identification of the rich stimulus on trials following non-rewarded trials (see Supplement and Fig. S1).
Figure 1.
Results showed a main effect of Drug (A), where averaged across blocks and order, response bias was significantly higher in the nicotine relative to the placebo condition. There was also a trend-level Drug x Order interaction (B), which was driven by higher response bias in the nicotine relative to the placebo condition in those who received the placebo first, but not in those who received nicotine first. Furthermore, response bias in the placebo condition was higher for those who received nicotine first relative to those who received the placebo first. *p < 0.05.
There was also a trend-level Drug × Order interaction, F(1,104) = 3.52, p = 0.06, ƞ p2 = 0.03, which was further explored in light of prior findings.11 Bonferroni-corrected post hoc tests showed that this interaction was driven by higher response bias (averaged across blocks) in the nicotine compared to the placebo condition, but only for those who received the placebo condition first (placebo first: p = 0.01; nicotine first: p = 0.91; Fig. 1B). Furthermore, response bias in the placebo condition was higher for those who received nicotine first compared to those who received placebo first (p = 0.003). We confirmed that the effects were specific to reward processing, as the key findings remained significant when controlling for the effects of nicotine on discriminability (a measure of overall task performance; see Supplement).
Moderating Effects of rs16969968
Although A-allele carriers (n = 52) did not differ from non-A-allele carriers (n = 53) on nicotine dependence severity on the HONC, t(103) = 0.01, p = 0.99, or the age at which they first smoked, t(103) = 1.34, p = 0.19, A-allele carriers scored higher than non-A-allele carriers on a measure evaluating positive attitudes towards smoking, t(103) = 2.10, p = 0.04, consistent with this risk allele’s link with increased risk for nicotine dependence. The groups showed similar levels of negative affective traits (all ps > 0.80).
Whole Sample
When considering the entire sample, neither the main effect of SNP (p = 0.73) nor the SNP × Drug interaction (p = 0.97) was significant.
Caucasians Only
There were 78 Caucasian individuals, of whom 44 were A-allele carriers and 34 were non-A-allele carriers. Although the main effect of SNP (p = 0.93) and the SNP × Drug interaction (p = 0.69) were not significant, a significant SNP × Drug × Order interaction emerged, F(1,74) = 5.40, p = 0.02, ƞ p2 = 0.07 (Fig. 2). This 3-way interaction was unpacked by examining all possible 2-way interactions. First, analyses showed that the SNP × Drug interaction differed at levels of Order, with the interaction being significant for those who received nicotine first, F(1,43) = 4.30, p = 0.045, ƞ p2 = 0.09, but not for those who received placebo first, F(1,31) = 1.67, p = 0.21, ƞ p2 = 0.05. However, none of the post hoc tests of simple effects that comprised this interaction survived Bonferroni correction (all ps > 0.05). Second, analyses showed that the Drug × Order interaction also differed at levels of SNP, with the interaction being significant for the A-allele carriers, F(1,42) = 10.76, p = 0.002, ƞ p2 = 0.20, but not the non-A-allele carriers, F(1,32) = 0.10, p = 0.76, ƞ p2 = 0.003. In this case, several post hoc tests of simple effects were significant after Bonferroni correction. Specifically, for A-allele carriers, response bias (averaged across blocks) was significantly higher in the nicotine relative to the placebo condition for those who received the placebo first (p = 0.01), whereas response bias was trend-level lower in the nicotine relative to the placebo condition for those who received nicotine first (p = 0.06). Similarly, for A-allele carriers, response bias was higher in the placebo condition for those who received nicotine first relative to those who received placebo first (p < 0.001), whereas there was a trend for response bias to be lower in the nicotine condition for those who received nicotine first relative to those who received placebo first (p = 0.08). These findings suggest that in Caucasian individuals carrying a genetic polymorphism linked to increased risk for nicotine dependence, nicotine produced a higher response bias relative to placebo, and the ability for prior nicotine exposure to influence reward processing in the placebo condition was stronger (see Tables S1 and S2 for ANOVA tables).
Figure 2.
Figure shows the significant SNP × Drug × Order interaction in the Caucasian subset of the sample. The Drug x Order interaction was significant in the A-allele carriers (left) but not in the non-A-allele carriers (right). Within the A-allele carriers, there was a significant increase in response bias in the nicotine relative to the placebo condition, but only for individuals who received the nicotine condition first. Furthermore, response bias in the placebo condition was higher for those who received nicotine first compared to those who received the placebo first. These findings suggest Caucasian young light smokers at increased genetic risk for nicotine dependence show greater increases in response bias following nicotine relative to a placebo and also show a greater propensity for prior nicotine exposure to influence reward processing following a placebo. *p < 0.05. SNP, single-nucleotide polymorphism.
Competing theories posit that rather than influencing reward function specifically, nicotine may enhance cognitive function in A-allele carriers.23 To test this, we conducted the same analysis using the PRT discriminability scores. Discriminability measures more general task performance that is unrelated to the asymmetrical reinforcement schedule and therefore provides a proxy measure of reward-independent cognitive functioning. Results showed no main effects or interactions involving SNP for discriminability either in the whole sample or when examining Caucasian participants separately (all ps>0.10). This suggests that our effects were likely specific to reward processing and not cognitive functioning.
Moderating Effects of Mood and Negative Affect Traits
We fitted regression models to examine whether two moderators related to mood (anhedonia and the tendency to smoke in response to negative emotion) and two moderators related to negative affective traits (depressive and anxious traits) moderated the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine. A separate regression model was run for each moderator. To control for multiple testing, we used a Bonferroni-corrected alpha level of 0.05 × 4 moderators/tests = 0.0125. Of the four moderators examined, only the Drug × Depressive traits interaction was significant [B = 0.006, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 0.001–0.011, p = 0.010]. As shown in Fig. 3A, the effects of nicotine on reward learning were stronger for individuals higher on depressive traits. These individuals showed a more blunted response bias following placebo, but potentiated response bias following nicotine.
Figure 3.
Figure shows the moderating effect of depressive traits (A) and nicotine dependence severity (B) on changes in response bias following nicotine relative to a placebo. For visualization purposes, predicted response bias for individuals with depressive trait scores and nicotine dependence scores at the mean (“Mean”), 1 SD above the mean (“High”) and 1 SD below the mean (“Low”) are plotted. The reward-enhancing effects of nicotine were stronger in young light smokers scoring higher on depressive personality traits and also in those reporting a greater sense of being “hooked” on nicotine.
Relationship to Nicotine Dependence
We fitted a final regression model to determine whether the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine were moderated by nicotine dependence on the HONC. The Drug × HONC interaction was significant [B = 0.01, CI = 0.001–0.020, p = 0.03]. As shown in Fig. 3B, a very similar pattern emerged for individuals scoring high on nicotine dependence as for individuals scoring high on depressive traits. Specifically, as dependence increased, the effects of nicotine on response bias became stronger.
Discussion
This study examined moderators of the reward-enhancing effects of acute nicotine in young light smokers. As predicted, relative to a placebo, exposure to acute nicotine enhanced reward responsiveness, evidenced by increased response bias on the PRT. This is consistent with prior studies showing that exposure to nicotine potentiates reward sensitivity,10–12 likely via enhancing phasic striatal dopamine signaling.15 Furthermore, we observed order effects reported in prior studies,11 wherein nicotine enhanced reward responsiveness even in the placebo condition for individuals who received the nicotine condition first, potentially via its effects on motivational salience. This also aligns with findings showing that in rodents, self-administration of nicotine results in excitation of brain reward systems lasting up to 36 days after removal of nicotine availability49 – a finding interpreted as evidence that nicotine may alter or “reset” the sensitivity of reward systems to a new, increased level.
The reward-enhancing effects of nicotine were also moderated by rs16969968 allelic variation. Specifically, in Caucasian participants carrying the A-allele, which is associated with increased risk for nicotine dependence,50 nicotine produced a higher response bias relative to placebo, and this was not observed in non-A-allele carriers. Furthermore, the ability for prior nicotine exposure to influence response bias in the placebo condition was stronger in A-allele carriers than in non-A-allele carriers. These findings support prior work suggesting that the reductions in nAChR function evident in A-allele carriers may alter the effects of nicotine on striatal dopamine signaling.21 Although there are several purported mechanisms by which this allelic variation may impact striatal dopamine, compelling evidence suggests that it may reduce the negative effects of nicotine,51 thereby increasing nicotine’s pleasurable sensations. Competing theories suggest that A-allele carriers may smoke more to remediate cognitive deficits,23 however our effects were specific to response bias and not discriminability (a measure of more general cognitive functioning).
The effects of acute nicotine on reward processing were also moderated by depressive traits. As predicted, individuals higher in depressive traits showed a more blunted response bias in the placebo condition, but an enhanced response bias following nicotine. This is consistent with prior studies showing blunted response bias in non-smokers with current and remitted MDD, but normative or increased response bias in smokers with these conditions.10,34 These findings indicate that young light smokers high on depressive traits may smoke to remediate deficits in reward processing. We observed highly similar effects when examining the moderating effects of nicotine dependence severity, indicating that depressive traits and nicotine dependence may moderate nicotine’s effects on the reward system in a similar manner.
These findings have several important implications for our understanding of how to reduce smoking behavior in young light smokers. First, it is clear that research focusing on the role of CHRNA5 genetic variation on nicotine’s reward-enhancing effects may highlight avenues for novel treatment options in individuals with rs16969968 risk alleles. Furthermore, these findings emphasize the value of further research that examines the utility of pharmacogenetic optimization of smoking cessation interventions. Second, given that high levels of depressive traits may promote maintenance of smoking behavior because nicotine normalizes depression-related impairments in reward responsiveness, interventions aimed at enhancing reward deficits in these individuals may be beneficial in reducing the risk for ongoing smoking. These therapies may also be critical during smoking cessation attempts to minimize further reductions in reward responsiveness.
The order effects observed in the current study also warrant mention, as they further highlight the uniqueness of nicotine’s effects on the reward system relative to other drugs of abuse. To our knowledge, this study is now the third to observe a persistent amplification of reward sensitivity following acute nicotine exposure, which, unlike other reward-enhancing substances such as cocaine, extends beyond the early abstinence period. This effect has previously been observed in rodents, where the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine self-administration (measured via intracranial self-stimulation thresholds) persisted for 36 days after nicotine intake had ceased.49 The authors suggested that this may explain some of the unique properties of nicotine relative to other substances of abuse, such as why extended access to nicotine does not result in the same escalation of drug intake over time, as does extended access to cocaine.52 Similar order effects have also been observed in non-smoking, psychiatrically healthy humans using the same PRT used in the current study.11 Aligning with our results, the authors found that exposure to acute nicotine via a transdermal nicotine patch resulted in increased reward responsiveness relative to placebo. However, reward responsiveness was also elevated following a placebo for those who received nicotine first, even though it had been at least 1 week since they had been exposed to the nicotine condition. Despite the consistency of these order effects across these three different studies, the interpretation of these effects is challenging and the possibility that they are partially driven by practice effects or Type I error cannot be ruled out. Further research is needed to test the robustness of these findings.
Some limitations of this study should be noted. This study was cross-sectional and longitudinal studies are needed to determine whether variability in the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine predicts individual differences in smoking trajectories long-term. Specifically, longitudinal studies could identify whether genetic and trait factors predispose certain individuals to initiate smoking, or whether they make smoking cessation more difficult. This is critical for evaluating whether efforts focused on reducing early experimentation or improving smoking cessation will be most effective at reducing the prevalence of light smoking. Finally, it is important to note that our genetic findings were in Caucasian individuals only, and we lacked the sample size to evaluate our effects in different race subgroups. Future studies should systematically evaluate these effects in individuals from different racial backgrounds.
In sum, our findings indicate that individual differences in the smoking trajectories of young light smokers may be associated with a genetic risk factor and personality traits that moderate nicotine’s effects on reward processing. Accordingly, reducing rates of smoking in young light smokers may be improved by mitigating these effects using therapies that target the reward system, or by using these pre-existing factors to personalize treatment.
Supplementary Material
A Contributorship Form detailing each author’s specific involvement with this content, as well as any supplementary data, are available online at https://academic.oup.com/ntr.
Funding
Research reported in this publication was supported by R01 DA036032 from the National Institute of Drug Abuse (to DGG). DAP was partially supported by R01MH101521 and R37 MH068376 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. AEW received support from the National Health and Medical Research Council, Grant No. GNT1110773.
Declaration of Interests
For the past 30 years, Gilbert was funded by the National Institute of Drug Abuse. During the past ten years he has received consultation fees from the Duke Center for Smoking Cessation Research as a consultant and advisory board member, and approximately 15 years ago received a consulting fee from Pfizer Pharmaceuticals. Over the past three years, Pizzagalli has received funding from NIMH, Brain, and Behavior Research Foundation, the Dana Foundation, and Millennium Pharmaceuticals; consulting fees from BlackThorn Therapeutics, Boehringer Ingelheim, Compass Pathway, Concert Pharmaceuticals, Engrail Therapeutics, Otsuka Pharmaceuticals, and Takeda Pharmaceuticals; one honorarium from Alkermes; stock options from BlackThorn Therapeutics. Pizzagalli has a financial interest in BlackThorn Therapeutics, which has licensed the copyright to the Probabilistic Reward Task through Harvard University. Pizzagalli’s interests were reviewed and are managed by McLean Hospital and Partners HealthCare in accordance with their conflict-of-interest policies. All other authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
References
- 1.Azagba S, Manzione L, Shan L, King J. Trends in smoking behaviors among US adolescent cigarette smokers. Pediatrics. 2020;145(3):e20193047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Reyes-Guzman CM, Pfeiffer RM, Lubin J, et al. Determinants of Light and Intermittent Smoking in the United States: Results from Three Pooled National Health Surveys. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2017;26(2):228–239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Levy DE, Biener L, Rigotti NA. The natural history of light smokers: A population-based cohort study. Nicotine Tob Res. 2009;11(2):156–163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schane RE, Ling PM, Glantz SA. Health effects of light and intermittent smoking: A review. Circulation. 2010;121(13):1518–1522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lydon DM, Wilson SJ, Child A, Geier CF. Adolescent brain maturation and smoking: what we know and where we’re headed. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;45:323–342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gilbert DG, McClernon FJ, Rabinovich NE, Plath LC, Jensen RA, Meliska CJ. Effects of smoking abstinence on mood and craving in men: Influences of negative-affect-related personality traits, habitual nicotine intake and repeated measurements. Pers Individ Differ. 1998;25(3):399–423. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gilbert DG, McClernon FJ, Rabinovich NE, et al. Mood disturbance fails to resolve across 31 days of cigarette abstinence in women. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2002;70(1):142–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Chassin L, Curran PJ, Presson CC, Sherman SJ, Wirth R.. Developmental trajectories of cigarette smoking from adolescence to adulthood. Phenotypes and endophenotypes: Foundations for genetic studies of nicotine use and dependence (Tobacco Control Monograph No 20) US Department of Health and Human Services, NIH, National Cancer Institute. Bethesda, MD: NIH Publication. 2009(09-6366):189–244. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Pizzagalli DA, Jahn AL, O’Shea JP. Toward an objective characterization of an anhedonic phenotype: A signal-detection approach. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(4):319–327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Pergadia ML, Der-Avakian A, D’Souza MS, et al. Association between nicotine withdrawal and reward responsiveness in humans and rats. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(11):1238–1245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Barr RS, Pizzagalli DA, Culhane MA, Goff DC, Evins AE. A single dose of nicotine enhances reward responsiveness in nonsmokers: Implications for development of dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(11):1061–1065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Whitton AE, Green AI, Pizzagalli DA, Roth RM, Williams JM, Brunette MF. Potent dopamine D2 antagonists block the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine in smokers with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2019;45(6):1300–1308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pizzagalli DA, Evins AE, Schetter EC, et al. Single dose of a dopamine agonist impairs reinforcement learning in humans: Behavioral evidence from a laboratory-based measure of reward responsiveness. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2008;196(2):221–232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Santesso DL, Evins AE, Frank MJ, Schetter EC, Bogdan R, Pizzagalli DA. Single dose of a dopamine agonist impairs reinforcement learning in humans: Evidence from event-related potentials and computational modeling of striatal-cortical function. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(7):1963–1976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rice ME, Cragg SJ. Nicotine amplifies reward-related dopamine signals in striatum. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(6):583–584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Peechatka AL, Whitton AE, Farmer SL, Pizzagalli DA, Janes AC. Cigarette craving is associated with blunted reward processing in nicotine-dependent smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;155:202–207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Agrawal A, Lynskey MT. Are there genetic influences on addiction: Evidence from family, adoption and twin studies. Addiction. 2008;103(7):1069–1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Thorgeirsson TE, Gudbjartsson DF, Surakka I, et al. ; ENGAGE Consortium . Sequence variants at CHRNB3-CHRNA6 and CYP2A6 affect smoking behavior. Nat Genet. 2010;42(5):448–453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Furberg H, Kim Y, Dackor J, et al. Genome-wide meta-analyses identify multiple loci associated with smoking behavior. Nat Genet. 2010;42(5):441–447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Liu JZ, Tozzi F, Waterworth DM, et al. ; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium . Meta-analysis and imputation refines the association of 15q25 with smoking quantity. Nat Genet. 2010;42(5):436–440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bierut LJ, Stitzel JA, Wang JC, et al. Variants in nicotinic receptors and risk for nicotine dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(9):1163–1171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Saccone SF, Hinrichs AL, Saccone NL, et al. Cholinergic nicotinic receptor genes implicated in a nicotine dependence association study targeting 348 candidate genes with 3713 SNPs. Hum Mol Genet. 2007;16(1):36–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Winterer G, Mittelstrass K, Giegling I, et al. Risk gene variants for nicotine dependence in the CHRNA5–CHRNA3–CHRNB4 cluster are associated with cognitive performance. Am J Med Genet B. 2010;153(8):1448–1458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Sherva R, Wilhelmsen K, Pomerleau CS, et al. Association of a single nucleotide polymorphism in neuronal acetylcholine receptor subunit alpha 5 (CHRNA5) with smoking status and with ‘pleasurable buzz’ during early experimentation with smoking. Addiction. 2008;103(9):1544–1552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Fowler CD, Lu Q, Johnson PM, Marks MJ, Kenny PJ. Habenular α5 nicotinic receptor subunit signalling controls nicotine intake. Nature. 2011;471(7340):597–601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gilbert DG.Smoking: Individual Differences, Psychopathology, and Emotion Taylor & Francis: Washington, DC. 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Leventhal AM, Ray LA, Rhee SH, Unger JB. Genetic and environmental influences on the association between depressive symptom dimensions and smoking initiation among Chinese adolescent twins. Nicotine Tob Res. 2012;14(5):559–568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Audrain-McGovern J, Rodriguez D, Rodgers K, Cuevas J. Declining alternative reinforcers link depression to young adult smoking. Addiction. 2011;106(1):178–187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.McKenzie M, Olsson CA, Jorm AF, Romaniuk H, Patton GC. Association of adolescent symptoms of depression and anxiety with daily smoking and nicotine dependence in young adulthood: Findings from a 10-year longitudinal study. Addiction. 2010;105(9):1652–1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hitsman B, Papandonatos GD, McChargue DE, et al. Past major depression and smoking cessation outcome: A systematic review and meta-analysis update. Addiction. 2013;108(2):294–306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Pizzagalli DA, Iosifescu D, Hallett LA, Ratner KG, Fava M. Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: Evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J Psychiatr Res. 2008;43(1):76–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Whitton AE, Kakani P, Foti D, et al. Blunted neural responses to reward in remitted major depression: A high-density event-related potential study. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2016;1(1):87–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bogdan R, Santesso DL, Fagerness J, Perlis RH, Pizzagalli DA. Corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor type 1 (CRHR1) genetic variation and stress interact to influence reward learning. J Neurosci. 2011;31(37):13246–13254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Janes AC, Pedrelli P, Whitton AE, et al. Reward responsiveness varies by smoking status in women with a history of major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(8):1940–1946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Liverant GI, Sloan DM, Pizzagalli DA, et al. Associations among smoking, anhedonia, and reward learning in depression. Behav Ther. 2014;45(5):651–663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bucholz KK, Cadoret R, Cloninger CR, et al. A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: A report on the reliability of the SSAGA. J Stud Alcohol. 1994;55(2):149–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Cottler LB, Robins LN, Grant BF, et al. The CIDI-core substance abuse and dependence questions: Cross-cultural and nosological issues. The WHO/ADAMHA field trial. Br J Psychiatry. 1991;159:653–658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Robinson SM, Sobell LC, Sobell MB, Leo GI. Reliability of the timeline followback for cocaine, cannabis, and cigarette use. Psychol Addict Behav. 2014;28(1):154–162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gilbert DG, Jensen RA, Meliska CJ. A system for administering quantified doses of tobacco smoke to human subjects: plasma nicotine and filter pad validation. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1988;31(4):905–908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gilbert DG, Dibb WD, Plath LC, Hiyane SG. Effects of nicotine and caffeine, separately and in combination, on EEG topography, mood, heart rate, cortisol, and vigilance. Psychophysiology. 2000;37(5):583–595. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Masson CL, Gilbert DG. Cardiovascular and mood responses to quantified doses of cigarette smoke in oral contraceptive users and nonusers. J Behav Med. 1999;22(6):589–604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Gilbert DG, Estes SL, Welser R. Does noise stress modulate effects of smoking/nicotine? Mood, vigilance, and EEG responses. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1997;129(4):382–389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.DiFranza JR, Savageau JA, Fletcher K, et al. Measuring the loss of autonomy over nicotine use in adolescents: the DANDY (Development and Assessment of Nicotine Dependence in Youths) study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;156(4):397–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wellman RJ, Savageau JA, Godiwala S, et al. A comparison of the hooked on nicotine checklist and the Fagerström test for nicotine dependence in adult smokers. Nicotine Tob Res. 2006;8(4):575–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Fawcett J, Clark DC, Scheftner WA, Gibbons RD. Assessing anhedonia in psychiatric patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983;40(1):79–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Costa PT, McCrae RR. Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO personality inventory. Psychol Assess. 1992;4(1):5–13. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Gilbert DG, Sharpe JP, Ramanaiah NV, Detwiler FR, Anderson AE. Development of a Situation× Trait Adaptive Response (STAR) model-based smoking motivation questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences. 2000;29(1):65–84. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Macmillan N, Creelman C.. Detection Theory: A User’s Guide. UK: Camrbidge University Press, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kenny PJ, Markou A. Nicotine self-administration acutely activates brain reward systems and induces a long-lasting increase in reward sensitivity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(6):1203–1211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Stevens VL, Bierut LJ, Talbot JT, et al. Nicotinic receptor gene variants influence susceptibility to heavy smoking. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2008;17(12):3517–3525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Fowler CD, Tuesta L, Kenny PJ. Role of α5* nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the effects of acute and chronic nicotine treatment on brain reward function in mice. Psychopharmacology. 2013;229(3):503–513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Ahmed SH, Kenny PJ, Koob GF, Markou A. Neurobiological evidence for hedonic allostasis associated with escalating cocaine use. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5(7):625–626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.