Abstract
Purpose:
This study examined whether national trends in unstructured in-person socializing with peers (i.e., socializing without goals or supervision) among adolescents could help explain recent declines in adolescent risk behaviors (e.g., substance use, fighting, theft).
Methods:
The sample contained n=44,842 U.S. 12th-grade students (ages 17–18) from the Monitoring the Future survey (years 1999–2017). Analyses examined (1) prevalence trends, (2) latent factor structure of risk behaviors and unstructured in-person socializing, (3) whether trends in the unstructured in-person socializing factor accounted for the relationship between time (i.e., survey year) and the risk behavior factor.
Results:
Adolescent risk behaviors and unstructured in-person socializing declined by approximately 30% in the U.S., and both formed coherent latent factors. After adjusting for sociodemographics, declines in unstructured in-person socializing accounted for approximately 86% of declines in risk behaviors.
Conclusions:
The prevalence of risk behaviors and unstructured in-person socializing behaviors declined among U.S. 12th graders from 1999 to 2017. It is unknown whether such effects are directly causal and/or influenced by unmeasured variables. However, the results provide evidence that national declines in unstructured in-person socializing are a likely component of the explanation for national declines in adolescent risk behaviors.
Keywords: Adolescent, Risk Behavior, Substance Abuse, Delinquency, Unstructured Socializing, Epidemiology, Multivariate Modeling
Introduction
Adolescent behavior in the U.S. and other western countries has changed substantially over the past two decades. Multiple epidemiological studies demonstrate that youth are now significantly less likely to engage in risk behaviors (RB) such as substance use, conduct problems (e.g., fighting and stealing), and early sexual activity. For example, several alcohol use behaviors (e.g., any past-month drinking and binge drinking, past-month frequency of drinking) declined among adolescents by approximately 5–20% during the first decade of the 2000s across much of North America, Europe, and Australasia [1–4]. Similar declines have been observed for tobacco, cannabis, and illicit drugs [5–7] as well as sexual activity [8] and conduct problems [5, 9].
In the developmental psychology literature, Problem Behavior Theory (PBT)[10] demonstrates that the co-occurrence of substance use, conduct problems, and early sexual activity can be modeled as a single latent variable that can predict patterns of multiple forms of behavior among youth [10, 11]. In other words, it may be more useful to examine the covariance among these behaviors as a single underlying trait rather than studying each behavior in isolation. Using Problem Behavior Theory as a theoretical foundation, recent analyses demonstrate that risk behaviors cluster within youth in a similar fashion across multiple countries [12, 13]. Moreover, population-level declines in risk behaviors might be better described as a decrease in the underlying propensity toward all of these behaviors, rather than as contemporaneous but independent declines in individual behaviors [14–16]. This suggests that explanations for declines in risk behaviors should be able to account for trends in multiple behaviors rather than only invoking behavior-specific explanations (e.g., youth tobacco policies).
Problem Behavior Theory also provides a psychosocial framework for understanding the impact of various person- and environmental-level variables on risk behavior susceptibility [10, 11]. For example, poverty, school quality, and parental oversight all contribute to the balance of factors that influence adolescent risk behaviors. One critical variable that contributes to this balance is social context [17]. Specifically, Problem Behavior Theory suggests that in order to understand the occurrence and maintenance of adolescent risk behaviors, one must understand the “… social ecology of adolescent life,” which “… provides socially organized opportunities to learn risk behaviors together and normative expectations that they be performed together.” [17].
However, social context is a difficult concept to operationalize. Youths’ “social ecology” is comprised of multiple unique social situations that change from hour-to-hour and day-to-day. Thus, determining if and how risk behaviors are related to social context requires that we identify specific types of social contexts. To address this, we turn to the concept of unstructured in-person socializing (UIS) [18]. This framework posits that risk behaviors can emerge when youth are placed in situations with three characteristics: (1) presence of peers (2) lack of authority figure(s), and (3) no set agenda, goal, or activity [18]. There is significant evidence that youth who are consistently exposed to situations with these characteristics are more likely to engage in risk behaviors [19, 20].
If UIS is a strong predictor of risk behaviors, then it is prudent to examine whether national trends in UIS are associated with recent national trends in risk behaviors [21, 22]. Results from a recent study point toward this possibility. From 2002 to 2010, youth from multiple countries became significantly less likely to report socializing in person with friends every night of the week, and countries that underwent the largest decline in substance use also underwent the largest decline in face-to-face contact with peers [16]. The aim of the present study was to expand on this work by using nationally (U.S.) representative survey data on youth behavior to examine two questions: (1) Has the prevalence of UIS declined among U.S. adolescents? (2) Do trends in UIS statistically explain trends in adolescent risk behaviors? Although such an analysis cannot prove a direct causal relationship between UIS and risk behaviors, a strong association between the trends in these behaviors would indicate overlapping causal explanations.
Methods
Survey
Monitoring the Future (MTF) is a nationally representative annual cross-sectional survey of U.S. 12th, 10th, and 8th-grade students (ages 18–17, 16–15, and 13–14, respectively). The present study used only the 12th-grade sample because assessments of relevant behaviors were less extensive in 8th- and 10th-grade samples. MTF samples from approximately 130 schools using multistage random sampling. Survey administration follows a structured protocol, and approximately 75–80% of questionnaires are completed. MTF distributes different questionnaires (i.e., “Forms”) containing universal questions and questions unique to specific forms [23]. In this study, only “Form Two” data were analyzed because this form contained the necessary items. Our University Institutional Review Board determined that this research was exempt from review because the data are de-identified and publicly accessible.
Risk behavior (RB) factor indicator variables
In order to calculate prevalence estimates and annual percent change, we used dichotomized (yes or no) versions of nine risk behaviors: Past 30-day use of (1) alcohol (2) cigarettes (3) cannabis (4) cocaine; and past-year (5) fight at school (6) hurt someone on purpose (7) threaten someone with a weapon, (8) steal something worth <$50, and (9) steal something worth >$50. These variables were selected based on Problem Behavior Theory [11]. We conducted sensitivity analyses using the original, multi-category response option versions of these variables (Appendix A).
Unstructured in-person socializing (UIS) factor indicator variables
UIS was assessed with the four items originally used to develop the concept of unstructured socializing [18]. These items assessed past-year frequency of: (1) Going out for fun in the evening (during a typical week), (2) Going to parties, (3) Riding around in a car for fun, (4) Getting together with friends. Response options for item 1 were: “Less than one,” “one,” “two,” “three,” “four or five,” “six or seven.” Response options for items 2–4 were: “Never,” “A few times a year,” “Once or twice a month,” “At least once a week,” “Almost every day.” To remain consistent with the recoding of risk behaviors, we dichotomized UIS variables at the approximate 50th percentile of the full sample distribution (see Table 1). As with the risk behavior construct, we conducted sensitivity analyses using the original, multi-category response option versions of these variables (Appendix A).
Table 1.
Prevalence: 1999 vs. 2017 |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
APCa | (95% CI) | Year: 1999 | (95% CI) | Year: 2017 | (95% CI) | |
Risk Behaviors | ||||||
Used alcohol (past 30 days) | −2.2% | (−2.4%, −2.0%) | 51.0% | (48.5%, 53.4%) | 34.4% | (32.1%, 36.9%) |
Used cigarette (past 30 days) | −5.7% | (−6.1%, −5.3%) | 35.3% | (33.0%, 37.6%) | 9.7% | (8.3%, 11.3%) |
Used cannabis (past 30 days) | 0.3% | (−0.1%, 0.7%) | 24.4% | (22.4%, 26.6%) | 23.6% | (21.6%, 25.7%) |
Used cocaine (past 30 days) | −6.0% | (−7.4%, −4.5%) | 2.3% | (1.7%, 3.2%) | 0.9% | (0.6%, 1.4%) |
In physical fight (past year) | −2.1% | (−2.6%, −1.5%) | 14.7% | (13.1%, 16.5%) | 10.5% | (9.0%, 12.1%) |
Hurt someone on purpose (past year) | −1.6% | (−2.1%, −1.0%) | 13.3% | (11.7%, 15.0%) | 8.6% | (7.3%, 10.1%) |
Threaten someone with weapon (past year) | −1.8% | (−2.9%, −0.7%) | 3.8% | (3.0%, 4.9%) | 2.6% | (2.0%, 3.6%) |
Steal something worth <$50 (past year) | −2.3% | (−2.6%, −1.9%) | 31.0% | (28.8%, 33.3%) | 22.6% | (20.6%, 24.8%) |
Steal something worth ≥$50 (past year) | −2.5% | (−3.2%, −1.9%) | 10.6% | (9.2%, 12.2%) | 7.6% | (6.4%, 9.0%) |
Unstructured In-Person Socializing b | ||||||
Go out during the evenings for fun at least 3 times per week (vs. twice per week or less) | −1.8% | (−2.0%, −1.6%) | 51.3% | (48.8%, 53.8%) | 34.8% | (32.4%, 37.3%) |
Go to parties at least once per month (vs. a few times per year or less) | −1.7% | (−1.8%, −1.5%) | 73.6% | (71.5%, 75.7%) | 52.2% | (49.7%, 54.7%) |
Ride around in a car just for fun at least once per week (vs. twice per month or less) | −1.3% | (−1.4%, −1.1%) | 64.3% | (62%, 66.6%) | 55.0% | (52.6%, 57.4%) |
Get together with friends informally “almost every day” (vs. at least once per week or less) | −2.3% | (−2.5%, −2.1%) | 49.5% | (47.1%, 51.9%) | 29.9% | (27.7%, 32.2%) |
APC = “Annual Percent Change” (exponentiated coefficient of log-binomial model − 1)*100. APC represents the relative change in the prevalence of a behavior each year (−5% indicates a 5% decline in the prevalence per year, i.e., Relative Risk of 0.95)
Dichotomized at approximate 50th percentile of the distribution of the original ordinal variable
Note: All trends statistically significant p<0.05 except for cannabis.
Sociodemographics
Sociodemographic covariates included: (1) lives with mother and/or father; (2) parents have a college education or higher; (3) mother employment status (father employment status was not available); (4) number of siblings (5) race (6) urbanicity of living environment. Age and gender were not considered because the MTF sampling design ensures that the distribution of these variables remains the same each year.
Sample
Data from 12th-grade students for years 1999–2017 of MTF were combined into a dataset containing a total of n=44,842 observations. Year-specific sample sizes ranged from n=2,096 to n=2,579. Approximately 44% were under age 18, 49% were male, and 58% were white (non-weighted).
Missing data
Among all respondents, 1.5% did not answer any risk behavior questions, 0.24% did not answer any UIS questions, and 0.22% did not answer any risk behavior or UIS questions. Missing data for individual questions ranged from 2.1–6.3% for risk behaviors and 0.6–6.9% for UIS behaviors. Respondents missing on either all risk behavior questions or all UIS questions tended to also be missing on demographic and sociodemographic variables (e.g., age, parent education). Among respondents who provided demographic data, males and “other” race were more likely to be missing on all risk behaviors. The complete case method was used to estimate prevalence trends, and full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation was used in the structural equation modeling (SEM). Robust standard errors were used in both. Sensitivity test results are provided in the supplemental material (Appendix A).
Analyses
The four analysis steps are described in detail below. Analyses were conducted with Stata 14 and MPlus version 8 [24, 25].
Step 1: Prevalence trends
We estimated the survey-weighted prevalence of each binary risk behavior and UIS behavior for each year (1999–2017). We then employed log-binomial regression to determine the slope of change in the prevalence of each behavior during the years under study. Log-binomial regression is a generalized linear model that is well-suited for the current study aims because it yields risk ratio (RR) estimates (as opposed to odds ratio estimates produced by logistic regression) that directly correspond to the proportional change in prevalence for a given change in the independent variable. This allowed us to calculate the annual percent change (APC) in the prevalence of a behavior by including survey year (continuous variable) as a model predictor. Specifically, we calculated APC as (RR-1) x 100. For example, an RR of 0.95 indicates a 5% decline (i.e., APC of −5%) in the prevalence per year. Supplemental figures are provided (Appendix B).
Step 2: Factor analysis
We conducted two exploratory factor analyses using tetrachoric correlation and principal components extraction to determine whether (1) the nine dichotomous risk behavior indicator variables (e.g., past 30-day cannabis use, past-year theft) formed a coherent latent RB factor; (2) the four dichotomous unstructured in-person socializing indicator variables (e.g., spending time with friends) formed a coherent latent UIS factor. We considered a first unrotated principal component that explains ≥40% of total variability in the indicator variables as evidence of factor cohesiveness [26]. Lower values of R2 (explained variance) would have suggested that a single latent factor would be insufficient to summarize between-person variation in the indicator variables. Supplemental analyses are provided (Appendices A and C).
Step 3: Factor alignment
Declines in the prevalence of individual risk behaviors (i.e., declines in “indicator” variables of a latent factor) stem from two sources: (1) declines in the mean of the latent factor and (2) measurement noninvariance of the factor indicator parameters, (i.e., the way in which an indicator variable measures the latent factor is not stable over time). The first potential source – declines in the mean of the latent factor – is assumed to reflect a decline in a general underlying tendency to engage in any type of risk behavior.
Alternatively, the second potential source – measurement noninvariance – might reflect instances in which the prevalence of a specific behavior declines because of a specific environmental change that only affects that particular behavior. For example, tobacco policies have lowered the prevalence of youth cigarette use but presumably had little if any effect on the prevalence of youth cocaine use or youth fighting in school. In this case, the indicator variable for cigarette smoking would likely exhibit measurement noninvariance: declines in cigarette smoking might be observed even in the absence of overall declines in the postulated risk behavior factor[15].
Our goal was to estimate declines in latent factor means (source one) while adjusting for measurement noninvariance (source two). To accomplish this, we used the factor alignment method [25, 27] to identify noninvariant indicators and incorporate the results into the structural equation models (described below). Factor alignment identifies measurement noninvariance by comparing factor indicator loading and threshold parameters within each group (e.g., each survey year) to those from a reference group (year 1999). Additional details about factor alignment are available [27, 28]. Supplemental results. are available (Appendix D).
Step 4: Structural equation models (SEM)
Below, we describe the three parts (A, B, C) of the SEM analysis. All analyses that examined the effect of survey year also included paths from year-specific dummy variables to specific measurement model indicator variables to correct for the measurement noninvariance identified in factor alignment results from step 3 (described above). For each of the three parts of the analyses below, we conducted sensitivity tests (1) using the complete case method (rather than FIML) for missing data, and (2) using a graded-response model with the original, multi-category versions (rather than binary versions) of risk behaviors and unstructured in-person socializing behavior variables (Appendix A).
Part A (Figure 1.1): To examine the relationship between risk behaviors (RB) and unstructured in-person socializing (UIS), we created an unadjusted model with a path (i.e., regression) from the latent UIS factor (independent variable) to the latent RB factor (outcome variable). The adjusted model included sociodemographsic covariates.
Part B (Figure 1.2; Table 2 Models 1 and 2): We examined latent factor trends by creating an unadjusted model with a path from survey year (independent variable) to the RB factor (outcome variable) and another path from survey year to the UIS factor. The adjusted model included sociodemographic covariates. In both the unadjusted and adjusted models, we fixed the covariance between the RB and UIS factors to zero to estimate independent factor trends.
Table 2.
Dependent Variable: Continuous Latent Risk Behavior Variable (lower values indicate less risk behavior) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 (Corresponds to Figure 1.2) | Model 3 (Corresponds to Figure 1.3) | ||||
Independent Variables | Beta | 95% CI | Beta | 95% CI | Beta | 95% CI |
| ||||||
Survey Year |
−0.026
|
(−0.029, −0.023)c |
−0.027
|
(−0.030, −0.024)c |
−0.004
|
(−0.007, −0.001)a |
Lives with Father | −0.162 | (−0.195, −0.129)c | −0.134 | (−0.166, −0.102)c | ||
Lives with Mother | −0.354 | (−0.400, −0.308)c | −0.281 | (−0.326, −0.235)c | ||
Father College Educated | −0.112 | (−0.144, −0.08)c | −0.072 | (−0.103, −0.042)c | ||
Mother College Educated | −0.010 | (−0.042, 0.022) | −0.010 | (−0.040, 0.020) | ||
Mother Employed | 0.056 | (0.027, 0.085)c | −0.034 | (−0.062, −0.007)a | ||
Number of Siblings | 0.032 | (0.016, 0.047)c | 0.031 | (0.016, 0.046)c | ||
Race (Other vs. White) | −0.142 | (−0.176, −0.108)c | −0.030 | (−0.062, 0.003) | ||
Rurality vs. Urbanicity | 0.021 | (0.015, 0.028)c | 0.009 |
(0.003, 0.015) b | ||
Unstructured Socializing | 0.657 | (0.640, 0.673)c |
Notes: (1) Models are linear regression; (2) Coefficients interpreted in terms of standard deviations of risk behavior (i.e., “the risk behavior factor decreased by 0.026 standard deviations per year”); (3) Changes in the design of the MTF survey limit the ability to examine minority populations; (4) Father employment status was not available; (5) Robust standard errors and Full Information Maximum Likelihood applied; (6)
p<.05
p<.01
p<0.001
Part C (Figure 1.3; Table 2 Model 3): Finally, we combined models from Parts A and B and estimated direct and indirect (i.e., via UIS) effects of survey year on the RB factor, using the UIS factor as a mediating variable.
Results
Prevalence trends of individual behaviors (Table 1)
The prevalence of most individual risk behaviors and UIS behaviors declined by approximately 2% per year (i.e., a relative risk of approximately 0.98)(Table 1) – corresponding to an approximately 30% decline from 1999–2017. Notably, the prevalence of past 30-day cannabis use remained stable at approximately 24%. Figures of log-linear trends are provided in supplemental material.
Exploratory factor analysis
Consistent with prior studies [14, 15], the RB factor explained 51.1% of the total variance in individual behaviors. RB factor loadings ranged from 0.67–0.79 (Appendix C). The UIS factor explained 56.7% of the variance in individual behaviors. UIS factor loadings ranged from 0.68–0.80 (Appendix C).
Factor alignment
Factor alignment analyses revealed a high degree of invariance (i.e., stability) among indicators. The observed noninvariance was concentrated among indicator thresholds for cannabis and cigarettes (consistent with prior results[15]). Approximately 5% and 1% of the parameters were noninvariant for the RB and UIS factor indicators, respectively (Appendix D). Both results fall below the 25% noninvariance guideline[27], which justifies factor trend analyses.
SEM Part A: Association between risk behavior (RB) and unstructured in-person socializing (UIS) factors (Figure 1.1)
A model containing only the RB factor (outcome variable) regressed on the UIS factor (independent variable) yielded a standardized (i.e., range from −1 to 1) coefficient for UIS of 0.655 (95% CI: 0.639, 0.671) – indicating that a one standard deviation increase in UIS corresponded to a 0.655 standard deviation increase in RB, thus accounting for approximately 43% of the total variance in RB. After adjusting for sociodemographic variables, the UIS coefficient changed minimally (β=0.657, 95% CI: 0.641, 0.673) (Figure 1.1), and the model accounted for 45% of the variance in RB. When we sub-grouped the data by each year, unadjusted coefficients for UIS ranged from 0.559– 0.695 (median: 0.638).
SEM Part B: Trends in risk behavior (RB) and unstructured in-person socializing (UIS) factors (Figure 1.2; Table 2 Models 1 and 2)
In an unadjusted model, the RB factor (outcome variable) mean declined by 0.026 (95% CI: −0.029, −0.023) standard deviations per year (Table 2 Model 1) – corresponding to a decline of approximately 0.47 standard deviations from 1999 to 2017. The UIS factor (outcome variable) declined by 0.036 (95% CI: −0.039, −0.033) standard deviations per year – a decline of approximately 0.65 standard deviations from 1999 to 2017. The sociodemographic-adjusted model (Figure 1.2) yielded essentially the same results as the unadjusted model (Survey year β= −0.027 and −0.036 for RB and UIS, respectively). Importantly, almost all sociodemographic variables were statistically significant predictors of the RB factor. However, adjusting for sociodemographics had minimal impact on the coefficient for the survey year variable (compare survey year coefficient in Model 1 vs. Model 2 in Table 2) – suggesting that these sociodemographics did not affect the relationship between survey year and risk behaviors.
SEM Part C: Mediation model (Figure 1.3; Table 2 Model 3)
In the adjusted model, including the UIS factor as a mediator between survey year (independent variable) and the RB factor (outcome variable) reduced the magnitude of the direct effect for survey year from −0.027 standard deviations per year to −0.004 standard deviations per year (95% CI: −0.007, −0.001) and yielded an indirect effect of −0.024 (95% CI: −0.026, −0.022) (Figure 1.3; Table 2 Model 3). Thus, UIS trends accounted for 85.7% of the covariation between survey year and the RB factor (i.e., 85.7% of the decline in RB from 1999–2017). Additionally, the adjusted direct effect of UIS (independent variable) on RB (outcome variable) remained the same (β=0.657, 95% CI: 0.640, 0.673), indicating that UIS continued to explain approximately 43% of the overall variability in RB. Complete case and graded response model sensitivity analyses yielded results essentially identical to the primary results (Appendix A).
Discussion
This study examined the relationship between national declines in risk behaviors (RB) and national declines in unstructured in-person peer socializing (UIS) among U.S. 12th graders from 1999 to 2017. We found a strong and consistent association between these two types of behavior. Moreover, we found a strong association between national declines in unstructured in-person socializing and national declines in risk behaviors. Although our analyses cannot establish a direct causal relationship between unstructured in-person socializing and risk behavior, we believe our results clarify important components of a larger network of relations that involve these constructs by highlighting that explanations for risk behavior trends need to be able to account for unstructured in-person socializing trends.
Below we present a conceptual framework (Figure 2) to examine the role of unmeasured, causally antecedent variables (i.e., “upstream” causes) and the relative merits of different explanations for our results. For example, one potential explanatory pathway is Time → Changes in antecedent variables that only affect UIS → Declines in UIS → Declines in RB. However, another equally plausibly pathway is Time → Changes in antecedent variables that affect both UIS and RB → simultaneous declines in UIS and RB. Our results provide evidence that certain sociodemographic trends are less likely to be part of the list of possible antecedent variables, but additional work is still needed. For example, trends in antecedent variables such as parenting behaviors or participation in organized activities [29, 30] may be driving declines in UIS and RB.
Importantly, there is also a path in which antecedent variables contribute to national declines in RB but are unrelated to national declines in UIS (Figure 2, bottom path). Our results demonstrate that national declines in UIS account for over 80% of the declines in RB. This suggests that UIS and RB trends have overlapping antecedent causes, such as national changes in parental monitoring behaviors. Stated differently, our finding that UIS trends account for the majority of RB trends suggests that the “upstream” variables responsible for the declines in RB also played a large role in causing declines in UIS and vice-versa. Thus, any “upstream” causes that only affected RB without affecting UIS (e.g., tobacco control policies) may not be a significant contributor to population declines in RB. However, it is important to note that at the individual level, RB and UIS behaviors could affect each other directly via feedback effects (Figure 2, dashed arrows). For example, engaging in UIS may lead an adolescent to an enjoyable RB experience (e.g., alcohol use with peers) during one weekend, and that positive RB experience may increase the probability of engaging in UIS and RB the next weekend. The cumulative impact of these feedback effects is represented by the antecedent variable of the middle path in Figure 2. Stated differently, an adolescent’s prior history of engaging in UIS and RB will impact that adolescent’s survey responses at a particular point in time for both the UIS and RB survey questions.
We emphasize that this study examined cross-sectional, population-level shifts in behaviors, and thus we cannot address the individual-level, within-subject developmental course of these behaviors. That is, we cannot establish whether engaging in behavior X at a time T affects the probability that the same individual will engage in behavior Y at time T+1. Although unstructured in-person peer socializing is believed to contribute to the emergence of risk behaviors longitudinally [19, 20, 31], it is likely situated within a larger etiological network of individual-level characteristics (e.g., genetics) and environmental variables (e.g., socioeconomic status) that contribute to the development of risk behavior for any particular adolescent.
As discussed earlier, unmeasured, causally antecedent variables that may be driving both national declines in unstructured in-person socializing and risk behaviors and will need to be evaluated in future studies. Here we offer several potential candidates. First, parenting behaviors may be part of the explanation [32]. From 2002 to 2014, U.S. parents became more likely to check on their child’s homework, provide positive affirmations, and strongly disapprove of substance use [29]. Parenting behaviors (e.g., oversight and communication) are known predictors of youth socializing and substance use behaviors [5, 21, 29]. Another phenomenon to consider is a rise in perfectionism. Youth seem to perceive greater societal demands and expectations of them and that meeting those expectations is a prerequisite for social approval [33]. These changes may have affected patterns of unstructured in-person socializing and risk behaviors. Finally, specific types of technologies may have a role. An analysis of 7,757 Swedish adolescents suggests that greater time allocated to highly immersive, competitive, and socially-rewarding online games corresponds to less time spent socializing in-person with friends [34]. At present, approximately 80% of American adolescents have computer or video games at home, and close to a quarter play on a daily or near-daily basis for hours at a time [35].
Relatedly, there is speculation that new communication technologies (e.g., smartphones) have generated the observed declines by replacing in-person socializing [21, 36]. However, youth who engage in the most electronic media communication (EMC) seem to be the most likely to socialize in-person and use substances [16, 37–39]. This pattern is consistent with studies from earlier eras. For example, when landline telephones were the dominant communication technology, youth who used substances also talked to friends on landline telephones more frequently and had more frequent face-to-face contact with those friends [40]. Given the similar behavioral dynamics across technologies, perhaps EMC facilitates, rather than replaces, in-person socializing among some youth.
Additionally, the prevalence of several behaviors postulated to mark adolescent maturation declined over the same period. Specifically, U.S. youth are not engaging in traditional “adult behaviors,” such as having part-time employment, going on dates, and obtaining a driver’s license [36]. A leading interpretation is that today’s youth are taking a slower path to adulthood [36]. However, additional research is needed to clarify whether these trends are related to one another empirically – not theoretically – via examination of trends in individual-level behavioral covariation.
Additional limitations warrant comment. Although we examined a small number of sociodemographics, our analysis left many important variables unexamined (e.g., trauma, poverty, cultural differences, perceptions of risk and availability of substances, peer substance use and deviance) [20, 41, 42]. Additionally, the highest risk youth have high truancy rates, and thus our analyses of this school-based survey likely exclude this group.
In sum, this study provides evidence that unstructured in-person socializing behaviors (e.g., spending unsupervised time with friends) declined among U.S. 12th graders from 1999–2017, and that this decline is strongly associated with declines in risk behaviors (e.g., substance use). It is unknown whether such effects are directly causal and/or influenced by unmeasured variables. However, the results provide evidence that national declines in unstructured in-person socializing are a likely component of the explanation for national declines in adolescent risk behaviors. To develop a complete understanding of changes in young people’s behavior and well-being in the early 21st century, we encourage future research to examine the meaning of, and reasons behind the declines in unstructured in-person socializing and risk behaviors among adolescents.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements:
We gratefully acknowledge the University of Michigan for making data publicly available. Parts of the study analyses were submitted for presentation as a poster at the 43rd Annual Research Society on Alcoholism Scientific Meeting, June 2020, New Orleans, LA.
Funding: This work was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism R21AA025689, F32AA027941; the National Institute on Drug Abuse R21DA044744, R01 DA042195, and K02DA032573; the National Institute on Aging R01AG053217 and U19AG051426; and the Templeton Foundation. The funding agencies and data providers had no role in the design, conduct, collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of data, or manuscript preparation, review, approval, or the decision to submit for publication. Author JTB wrote the first draft of the manuscript and no honorarium, grant, or other form of payment was given to anyone to produce the manuscript.
Abbreviations:
- (MTF)
Monitoring the Future
- (UIS)
Unstructured in-person socializing
- (RB)
Risk Behaviors
- (PBT)
Problem Behavior Theory
Footnotes
Disclosure of potential conflicts: JTB is a member of the board of directors and treasurer of MySafeRx Inc., a non-profit scientific research organization. He receives no financial compensation from this organization. RFK, AA, BE, MED, and RAG, have no relevant conflicts to declare.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- [1].Clark Goings T, Salas-Wright CP, Belgrave FZ, et al. Trends in binge drinking and alcohol abstention among adolescents in the US, 2002–2016. Drug and alcohol dependence 2019;200:115–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Jackson N, Denny S, Sheridan J, et al. Uneven reductions in high school students’ alcohol use from 2007 to 2012 by age, sex, and socioeconomic strata. Subst Abus 2017;38:69–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Looze M, Raaijmakers Q, Bogt TT, et al. Decreases in adolescent weekly alcohol use in Europe and North America: evidence from 28 countries from 2002 to 2010. Eur J Public Health 2015;25Suppl 2:69–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Patrick ME, Schulenberg JE. Prevalence and predictors of adolescent alcohol use and binge drinking in the United States. Alcohol Res 2013;35:193–200. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Vaughn MG, Nelson EJ, Oh S, et al. Abstention from Drug Use and Delinquency Increasing among Youth in the United States, 2002–2014. Subst Use Misuse 2018;53:1468–1481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Hublet A, Bendtsen P, de Looze ME, et al. Trends in the co-occurrence of tobacco and cannabis use in 15-year-olds from 2002 to 2010 in 28 countries of Europe and North America. Eur J Public Health 2015;25Suppl 2:73–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Ball J, Sim D, Edwards R, et al. Declining adolescent cannabis use occurred across all demographic groups and was accompanied by declining use of other psychoactive drugs, New Zealand, 2001–2012. NZ Med J 2019;132:12–24. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Ethier KA, Kann L, McManus T. Sexual Intercourse Among High School Students - 29 States and United States Overall, 2005–2015. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 2018;66:1393–1397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Pickett W, Molcho M, Elgar FJ, et al. Trends and socioeconomic correlates of adolescent physical fighting in 30 countries. Pediatrics 2013;131:e18–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Jessor R Problem-behavior theory, psychosocial development, and adolescent problem drinking. Br J Addict 1987;82:331–342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Donovan JE, Jessor R. Structure of problem behavior in adolescence and young adulthood. J Consult Clin Psychol 1985;53:890–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].de Looze M, Ter Bogt TF, Raaijmakers QA, et al. Cross-national evidence for the clustering and psychosocial correlates of adolescent risk behaviours in 27 countries. Eur J Public Health 2015;25:50–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Vazsonyi AT, Chen P, Jenkins DD, et al. Jessor’s problem behavior theory: Cross-national evidence from Hungary, the Netherlands, Slovenia, Spain, Switzerland, Taiwan, Turkey, and the United States. Developmental psychology 2010;46:1779–1791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Grucza RA, Krueger RF, Agrawal A, et al. Declines in prevalence of adolescent substance use disorders and delinquent behaviors in the USA: a unitary trend? Psychol Med 2018;48:1494–1503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Borodovsky JT, Krueger RF, Agrawal A, et al. A Decline in Propensity Toward Risk Behaviors Among U.S. Adolescents. J Adolesc Health 2019;65:745–751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].De Looze M, van Dorsselaer S, Stevens G, et al. The decline in adolescent substance use across Europe and North America in the early twenty-first century: A result of the digital revolution? International journal of public health 2019;64:229–240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Jessor R Risk behavior in adolescence: A psychosocial framework for understanding and action*1. Developmental Review 1992;12:374–390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Osgood DW, Wilson JK, O’Malley PM, et al. Routine Activities and Individual Deviant Behavior. American Sociological Review 1996;61:635. [Google Scholar]
- [19].Hoeben EM, Meldrum RC, Walker DA, et al. The role of peer delinquency and unstructured socializing in explaining delinquency and substance use: A state-of-the-art review. Journal of Criminal Justice 2016;47:108–122. [Google Scholar]
- [20].Hoeben EM, Weerman FM. Why Is Involvement in Unstructured Socializing Related to Adolescent Delinquency?*. Criminology 2016;54:242–281. [Google Scholar]
- [21].Arnett JJ. Getting better all the time: Trends in risk behavior among American adolescents since 1990. Archives of Scientific Psychology 2018;6:87–95. [Google Scholar]
- [22].Baumer E, Cundiff K. The contemporary transformation of American youth: An analysis of changes in the prevalence of criminal activity, 1991–2015. . Manuscript under review 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Miech RA, Johnston LD, Bachman JG, et al. Monitoring the Future: A Continuing Study of American Youth (12th-Grade Survey). University of Michigan Institute for Social Research Survey Research Center 2017. [Google Scholar]
- [24].StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- [25].Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus: Statistical analysis with latent variables: User’s guide. 8 ‘edition’. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén Los Angeles, 1998–2018. [Google Scholar]
- [26].Carmines EG, Zeller RA. Reliability and validity assessment: Sage publications, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- [27].Muthen B, Asparouhov T. IRT studies of many groups: the alignment method. Front Psychol 2014;5:978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Multiple-Group Factor Analysis Alignment. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 2014;21:495–508. [Google Scholar]
- [29].Han B, Compton WM, Blanco C, et al. National Trends in Substance Use and Use Disorders Among Youth. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2017;56:747–754 e743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Grucza RA, Agrawal A, Krauss MJ, et al. Declining Prevalence of Marijuana Use Disorders Among Adolescents in the United States, 2002 to 2013. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2016;55:487–494 e486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Spillane NS, Schick MR, Kirk-Provencher KT, et al. Structured and Unstructured Activities and Alcohol and Marijuana Use in Middle School: The Role of Availability and Engagement. Subst Use Misuse 2020;55:1765–1773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Pape H, Rossow I, Brunborg GS. Adolescents drink less: How, who and why? A review of the recent research literature. Drug Alcohol Rev 2018;37Suppl 1:S98–S114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Curran T, Hill AP. Perfectionism is increasing over time: A meta-analysis of birth cohort differences from 1989 to 2016. Psychol Bull 2019;145:410–429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Hellström C, Nilsson KW, Leppert J, et al. Influences of motives to play and time spent gaming on the negative consequences of adolescent online computer gaming. Computers in Human Behavior 2012;28:1379–1387. [Google Scholar]
- [35].Common Sense Media Inc. The Common Sense Census: Media Use By Tweens And Teens; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- [36].Twenge JM, Park H. The Decline in Adult Activities Among U.S. Adolescents, 1976–2016. Child Dev 2019;90:638–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Meldrum RC, Clark J. Adolescent Virtual Time Spent Socializing With Peers, Substance Use, and Delinquency. Crime & Delinquency 2013;61:1104–1126. [Google Scholar]
- [38].Gommans R, Stevens GW, Finne E, et al. Frequent electronic media communication with friends is associated with higher adolescent substance use. International journal of public health 2015;60:167–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].Brunborg GS, Andreas JB, Kvaavik E. Social Media Use and Episodic Heavy Drinking Among Adolescents. Psychol Rep 2017;120:475–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Kandel D, Davies M. Friendship Networks, Intimacy, and Illicit Drug Use in Young Adulthood: A Comparison of Two Competing Theories*. Criminology 1991;29:441–469. [Google Scholar]
- [41].Burdzovic Andreas J, Bretteville-Jensen AL. Ready, willing, and able: the role of cannabis use opportunities in understanding adolescent cannabis use. Addiction 2017;112:1973–1982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Burdzovic Andreas J Perceived harmfulness of various alcohol- and cannabis use modes: Secular trends, differences, and associations with actual substance use behaviors among Norwegian adolescents, 2007–2015. Drug and alcohol dependence 2019;197:280–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.