Abstract
Objectives: Many studies have indicated a close relationship between ADHD and mood symptoms in university students. In the present study, we explore the role of daily functional impairments and executive functioning in the ADHD–mood relationship. Method: A total of 343 adults (126 males) filled out (a) the Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scale, (b) the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale, (c) the Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale, and (d) the Executive Function Index Scale. Results: The correlation between mood symptoms and ADHD was .48 (moderate correlation) and dropped to .15 (weak correlation) when controlling for functional problems and executive functioning. Hierarchical regression analyses showed that both functional impairments and executive functioning significantly explained 42% to 53% of the variance of mood symptoms. The addition of ADHD symptoms to the model slightly increased the explained mood variance by only 1%. Conclusion: These findings underline the role of experienced difficulties in triggering mood symptoms in ADHD symptomatology.
Keywords: ADHD, daily life functional problems, executive functions, mood symptoms, DASS, CAARS
Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is traditionally defined as a neurodevelopmental disorder, characterized by inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. The disorder is well recognized as a lifelong condition for about two thirds of the diagnosed individuals (Alexander & Harrison, 2013; Buitelaar et al., 2011; Gray et al., 2014). Based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) ADHD diagnostic criteria, some studies have reported an ADHD prevalence rate in adults to fall between the range of 2% and 8% (Alexander & Harrison, 2013; for review, see Green & Rabiner, 2012). Several studies showed that ADHD symptoms are relatively context-dependent, and that environmental influences contribute, to some extent, to the variations in the level of ADHD symptoms (for meta-analysis, see Nikolas & Burt, 2010; for review, see Purper-Ouakil et al., 2004). With this in mind, the present study focuses on adult university students with symptoms of ADHD (i.e., college environment). Living in such an environment might be stressful and may contribute to the severity of mood and ADHD symptoms. Norwalk et al. (2009) have reported that compared with high schools, colleges have less structured academic environments, and as a result provide more distractions than what students experienced in high schools. These distractions, in turn, may lead to an increased level of inattentive symptoms. This may explain why there is an increasing number of college students who report ADHD symptoms (see Weyandt et al., 2013, 2017; Wolf et al., 2009). It is important also to note that college students with ADHD exhibit more symptoms of comorbid mood disorders and elevated levels of psychological distress compared with those without ADHD (see Prevatt et al., 2015; Weyandt et al., 2013). Indeed, Alexander and Harrison (2013) showed that ADHD symptoms were strongly associated with depression, anxiety, and stress in university students.
Tuckman (2007) has proposed that the mood symptoms in ADHD might arise due to certain characteristics of ADHD. For instance, the weak ability to meet certain deadlines or to complete tasks may cause an anxious/negative mood response toward such shortcomings (Alexander & Harrison, 2013; Tuckman, 2007). In this vein, it could be assumed that students with high levels of ADHD report mood symptoms, which are not manifestations of mood disorders per se, but rather caused by the ADHD and related functional limitations (Hamed et al., 2015) and most especially when those students are not diagnosed (Williamson & Johnston, 2015). In this regard it has been found that “poor academic achievement owing to ADHD may lead to anxiety” (“Assessing Adults With ADHD and Comorbidities,” 2009).
Executive functions may also represent a crucial factor in the ADHD–Mood relationship, as it can predict functional impairments in university students with ADHD (Wood et al., 2017). For example, Dvorsky and Langberg (2014) showed that executive functions, tapping motivation and organizational skills, mediate the association between ADHD symptoms and the overall daily functioning, as well as academic achievement (measured by grade point average). These authors have argued that university students are usually expected to independently manage several activities that require organization skills and being engaged in goal-directed activities, such as preparing different types of assignments, adhering to a course schedule, planning ahead for exams, and time management. In support of this perspective, Dorr and Armstrong (2018) have shown that ADHD symptoms and self-reported executive functioning explain functional impairments in university students in the United States.
In sum, it is suggested that ADHD symptomatology, including executive function deficits and daily life impairments, may cause an elevated level of mood problems in university students. However, it is still unknown to what extent executive functioning and daily life impairments contribute to the ADHD–mood symptoms relationship. This is the focus of the present study. Specifically, we test whether the association between the severity of ADHD symptoms and mood symptoms is influenced by both functional impairments and poor executive functioning in daily life. Based on the presented literature, we expect that high levels of ADHD symptoms are related to high levels of negative mood and that this association is reduced when controlling for functional impairment and executive dysfunction. We also explore which specific daily functional impairments and executive functions can predict mood symptoms best.
Method
Participants
Three hundred forty-three undergraduate students (126 males and 217 females) were recruited from the University of Groningen to participate in the present study via an advertisement posted on a university platform for research participation (i.e., SONA). All students gave informed consent before their participation, and they all received study credits for their participation. The Ethics Committee Psychology of the University of Groningen approved the study. The mean age of the study sample was 20.52 years (SD = 2.24), ranging from 18 to 31 years. A number of participants reported to have a diagnosis with ADHD and/or mood disorders. No systematic diagnostic assessment was performed to confirm the reported diagnosis. Table 1 presents information about the reported disorders and percentage of students who reported each disorder.
Table 1.
Reported disorder | Number of participants (%) | Males (%) |
---|---|---|
ADHD | 57 (16.6) | 25 (7.3) |
Anxiety | 14 (4.1) | 5 (1.5) |
Depression | 14 (4.1) | 2 (0.6) |
Anxiety and depression | 14 (4.1) | 3 (0.9) |
ADHD with anxiety and/or depression | 7 (2) | 3 (0.9) |
Note. No systematic diagnostic assessment was performed to confirm the reported diagnosis.
Measures
Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scale (CAARS)
The CAARS consists of 66 items, which are assessed on a 4-point scale (scored from 0 = not at all/never to 3 = very much/very frequently). The behavioral ADHD symptoms are subdivided into the following four subscales: (a) inattention/memory problems, (b) hyperactivity/restlessness, (c) impulsivity/emotional liability, and (d) problems with self-concept. In addition, the CAARS includes three subscales measuring the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) ADHD symptoms: inattentive, hyperactive-impulsive symptoms, and ADHD symptoms total. The scale includes specific items, which are able to identify individuals who are at risk of having ADHD diagnosis, which together make up the ADHD Index subscale (Conners et al., 1999). Raw scores on the CAARS subscales were converted into T scores. According to the manual, T scores above 65 represent clinically significant symptoms in those attending mental health clinic and a T score of 70 represents clinical symptoms in adults without identified problems (Conners et al., 1999). Generally, higher scores indicate more ADHD problems. Only data from the ADHD Symptoms Total subscale of the CAARS are considered for data analysis in the present study as it reflects the official symptoms reported at the DSM-IV and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
The scale is a valid and reliable measure of adult ADHD symptoms (Erhardt et al., 1999): test–retest reliability ranges between .85 and .92, sensitivity and specificity are high for all four subscales. The CAARS also represents a cross-culturally valid measure of current ADHD symptoms in adults (Christiansen et al., 2012).
Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS)
The DASS assesses negative moods of depression, anxiety, and stress using three subscales: the depression, anxiety, and stress, each consisting of 14 items. Responses were given on a 4-point scale (scored from 0 = did not apply to me at all to 3 = applied to me very much). Examples of items are “I felt sad and depressed” and “I found it difficult to relax.” Based on the DASS norms, participants can be classified into five distinct categories: normal, mild, moderate, severe, and extremely severe, reflecting the severity level of mood symptoms relative to the population (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). The validity and reliability of the DASS have been considered strong. The internal consistency is high for each subscale (Cronbach’s alphas are .94, .88, and .93 for depression, anxiety, and stress, respectively; see Nieuwenhuijsen et al., 2003; Parkitny & McAuley, 2010). Crawford and Henry (2003) tested the convergent and discriminate validity of the DASS by correlating it with measures of depression and anxiety (The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale [HADS] and the personal disturbance scale [sAD], and positive and negative affectivity (The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule [PANAS]). The authors suggested excellent reliability of the DASS with adequate convergent and discriminant validity.
Executive Function Index Scale (EFI)
The EFI was used to measure executive functions in daily life contexts. The EFI has been developed in a normal population, so it can be used for clinical and nonclinical purposes. The scale consisted of 27 items covering five factors, namely motivational drive, organization, impulse control, empathy, and strategic planning. The motivational drive subscale addresses behavioral drive, activity level, and interest in novelty. Items of the organization subscale address abilities like multitasking, sequencing, and keeping things in mind, which are necessary for organized goal-directed behavior. The impulse control subscale measures self-inhibition ability and the tendency toward risk-taking behavior and social conduct. The empathy subscale reflects an individual’s concerns for others’ well-being, the tendency to behave in a prosocial way, and the level of a cooperative attitude. Finally, the strategic planning subscale assesses disposition to plan and thinking ahead, as well as the tendency to use strategies (Spinella, 2005). Participants rate themselves on a 5-point Likert-type scale (scored from 1 = not at all to 5 = very much).
A higher total score of the EFI indicates better executive functioning (Spinella, 2005). The EFI was developed in a nonclinical population. The EFI shows strong correlations with other self-rating executive functioning instruments, which were validated in clinical and neuroimaging studies such as the Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (Spinella, 2005). The EFI demonstrates good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α ranged from .69 to .82). The scale was originally developed from a college student community population to measure the level of executive functions skills instead of classifying individuals as having normal or deficient executive functioning. However, several studies indicated that the EFI is suitable for healthy populations showing enough variance (Kruger, 2011; Weatherly & Ferraro, 2011). Scores on the EFI have been found to predict scores on other scales measuring everyday behaviors such as the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ) that reflect the use of cognitive and metacognitive strategies (e.g., self-monitoring and planning), and academic effort regulation in college students (Garner, 2009).
Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale (WFIRS)
The WFIRS consisted of 70 items measuring adult’s impairment in seven major life domains/subscales: family, work, college, life skills, self-concept, social functioning, and risk taking. Items represent impairments in a number of everyday situations not overlapping directly with ADHD symptoms. Each item is measured on a 4-point scale (scored from 0 = never or not at all to 3 = very often or very much). In addition, participants had an option to response “not applicable” for items which were not applied for them; for instance, “road rage” for adults who do not drive. Items with “not applicable” response were not counted for the overall score of the domain that they belong to (WFIRS; Canadian ADHD Resource Alliance, 2019; Weiss, 2010).
The WFIRS shows moderate convergent validity with other measures of functioning such as the Columbia Impairment Scale and the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale (Takeda et al., 2017), and strong convergent validity with functional impairments self-reported scales in student population such as the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory and the Current Symptom Scale (Hadianfard et al., 2017). Previous psychometric studies have also shown that the WFIRS has good psychometric characteristics in a normal population (see Weiss et al., 2018). In this regard, the study of Canu et al. (2016) is of interest because it showed that the WFIRS provides enough variance in university students. In sum, the WFIRS shows adequate convergent, concurrent, and discriminate validity as well as good internal consistency. For family, work, college, life skills, self-concept, social functioning, and risk taking, α were .86, .91, .90, .89, .94, .88, and .88, respectively (see Canu et al., 2016; Gajria et al., 2015).
Data Analysis
Means and standard deviations of scores on all questionnaires were calculated. Spearman correlations were calculated to test the correlations between scores on the DASS subscales (tapping depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms), the DSM-IV ADHD Total Symptoms of the CAARS (ADHD symptoms), the EFI (executive functions), and the WFIRS (daily functional impairments) scales.
Whether the relationship between mood and ADHD symptoms is influenced by factors of daily functional impairments and executive functioning is tested using nonparametric partial correlations. Here, the association between scores on the DASS subscales and scores on the DSM-IV ADHD Total Symptoms of the CAARS is tested after controlling for total scores on the EFI and the WFIRS.
A regression analysis was performed to test the contribution of ADHD symptoms to the severity of mood symptoms. The independent variable was scores on the CAARS DSM-IV Total Symptoms subscale and the dependent variable was the total score on the DASS. Following on from this, a subsequent hierarchical regression analysis was performed to investigate the contribution of ADHD symptoms to mood symptoms, after controlling for functional impairments and executive functioning. The total scores on the EFI and the total scores on the WFIRS were entered in Step 1, and scores on the CAARS DSM-IV Total Symptoms subscale were entered in Step 2. The dependent variable was the total score on the DASS. Because the data violated the normality assumption of linear regression analysis, we decided to report bootstrap confidence intervals for all regression coefficients. Bootstrapping was executed using a bias-corrected approach with 1,000 sample replicates. Values of p from bootstrapping will be reported.
Previous studies have revealed inconsistent outcomes regarding gender differences in ADHD and mood symptom representations. For example, although males have more ADHD symptoms compared with females (Gershon, 2002), females demonstrate increased levels of depression, anxiety, and stress compared with males (Gudjonsson et al., 2009; Panevska et al., 2015). A more recent review by Williamson and Johnston (2015) suggested gender differences in ADHD prevalence, comorbidities, and functional impairments in the adult population. Biederman et al. (2004) indicated that despite of the previously reported evidence for gender differences in ADHD and mood symptoms, gender did not moderate the association between ADHD and other psychiatric disorders such as major depression and anxiety. In addition, the number of females is higher than (almost twice as) the number of males in the present study sample. Thus, gender differences are treated as a confounding factor. Consequently, we checked whether the outcomes are influenced by the gender. To do so, hierarchical regression analysis was performed. In Step 1, the gender was entered as a dummy variable. In Step 2, the total scores on the EFI and the total scores on the WFIRS were entered. In Step 3, scores on the DSM Total Symptoms subscale of the CAARS were added.
To explore which specific daily functional impairments and executive dysfunctions can predict mood symptoms, regression analyses were performed. Scores on the EFI and WFIRS subscales were implemented as independent variables. The dependent variable in both analyses was the total score on the DASS.
Results
Mean and standard deviation of scores on the CAARS, WFIRS, EFI, and DASS are presented in Table 2. Tables 3 and 4 show the prevalence of severity of mood and ADHD symptoms according to both the DASS and CAARS cut-offs scores.
Table 2.
Gender | CAARS |
DASS |
EFI |
WFIRS |
|||||||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Inattention/memory | Hyperactivity/restlessness | Impulsivity/emotional lability | Problems with self-concept | DSM-IV inattentive Symptoms | DSM-IV hyperactive symptoms | DSM-IV ADHD symptoms total | ADHD Index | Depression | Anxiety | Stress | MD | ORG | IC | EM | SP | Total score | Family | Work | School | Self-concept | Life skills | Social functioning | Risk taking | Total score | |
Female | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
M | 52.99 | 48.65 | 50.65 | 49.24 | 53.08 | 47.67 | 50.86 | 50.49 | 7.82 | 8.39 | 12.78 | 14.55 | 16.62 | 17.43 | 25.62 | 23.19 | 97.54 | 0.53 | 0.30 | 0.54 | 0.70 | 1.00 | 0.41 | 0.42 | 3.89 |
SD | 11.06 | 8.86 | 10.25 | 10.65 | 11.72 | 11.02 | 11.71 | 10.03 | 8.99 | 8.24 | 8.92 | 2.69 | 3.86 | 3.69 | 2.93 | 4.24 | 10.75 | 0.42 | 0.82 | 0.47 | 0.54 | 0.80 | 0.42 | 0.36 | 2.75 |
Males | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
M | 53.10 | 47.87 | 48.02 | 48.02 | 61.85 | 53.01 | 59.52 | 50.13 | 7.86 | 7.30 | 11.48 | 14.57 | 16.10 | 16.24 | 23.66 | 21.32 | 91.78 | 0.66 | 0.49 | 0.80 | 0.86 | 0.73 | 0.57 | 0.67 | 4.81 |
SD | 10.90 | 10.14 | 9.71 | 9.08 | 13.81 | 12.57 | 14.24 | 9.63 | 7.32 | 6.21 | 8.06 | 2.91 | 3.43 | 3.41 | 3.46 | 5.07 | 9.68 | 0.49 | 0.57 | 0.57 | 0.55 | 0.71 | 0.47 | 0.43 | 2.86 |
Note. CAARS = Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scale; DASS = Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale; EFI = Executive Function Index Scale; WFIRS = Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale; DSM-IV = The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed.; MD = motivational drive subscale; ORG = organization subscale; IC = impulse control subscale; EM = empathy subscale; SP = strategic planning subscale; total score = sum score of all subscales.
Table 3.
DASS subscales | Normal |
Mild |
Moderate |
Severe and extremely severe |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Males | Females | Males | Females | Males | Females | Males | Females | |
Depression | 83 (65.9%) | 161 (74.2%) | 14 (11.1%) | 15 (6.9%) | 20 (15.9%) | 18 (8.3%) | 9 (7.1%) | 23 (10.6%) |
Anxiety | 73 (57.9%) | 133 (61.3%) | 15 (11.9%) | 13 (6.0%) | 22 (17.5%) | 35 (16.1) | 16 (12.7%) | 36 (16.6%) |
Stress | 84 (66.7%) | 136 (62.7%) | 16 (12.7%) | 29 (13.4%) | 20 (15.9%) | 29 (13.4%) | 6 (4.8%) | 23 (10.6%) |
Note. DASS = Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale.
Table 4.
T scores in clinical range | Inattention/memory problems | Hyperactivity/restlessness | Impulsivity/emotional lability | Problems with self-concept | DSM-IV inattentive symptoms | DSM-IV hyperactive symptoms | DSM-IV ADHD symptoms total | ADHD Index |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
70 > T score > 65 | 11 | 9 | 10 | 16 | 23 | 13 | 14 | 17 |
T score ≥ 70 | 41 | 11 | 19 | 12 | 56 | 23 | 49 | 15 |
Note. CAARS = Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scale; DSM-IV = The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed.
The correlations, as presented, in Table 5 show that higher total scores on the DASS were strongly associated with higher total scores on the WFIRS (rs = .66, p =.000) and moderately associated with lower total scores on the EFI (rs = −.37, p =.000). The total scores on the DASS were also correlated with scores on all subscales of the WFIRS and EFI, except for the scores on the empathy subscale of the EFI. As can be seen from Table 5, correlations between the DASS subscales and scores on the CAARS DSM-IV ADHD Total Symptoms subscale were moderate (rs = .43–.48): Higher scores on the CAARS are associated with higher scores on the DASS. However, after controlling for the total scores on the EFI and the total scores on the WFIRS, these correlations with scores on the CAARS DSM-IV ADHD Total Symptoms subscale turned out to be low and less significant (rs = .02, p = .70 for the Depression scale, rs = .17, p = .001 for the Anxiety scale, rs = .22, p = .000 for the Stress scale, and rs = .15, p = .004 for the total scores on the DASS).
Table 5.
DASS | DSM-ADHD Total Symptoms subscale of the CAARS | EFI |
WFIRS |
||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MD | ORG | IC | EM | SP | Total score | Family | Work | College | Life Skills | Self-concept | Social functioning | Risk taking | Total score | ||
Depression | .44** | −.29** | −.42** | −.22** | −.01 | −.26** | −.42** | .39** | .24** | .53** | .54** | .67** | .52** | .35** | .68** |
Anxiety | .43** | −.10 | −.35** | −.24** | .02 | −.13* | −.29** | .43** | .23** | .38** | .45** | .49** | .38** | .33** | .55** |
Stress | .46** | −.05 | −.38** | −.29** | .04 | −.15** | −.30** | .39** | .18** | .38** | .42** | .50** | .45** | .33** | .55** |
Total score DASS | .48** | −.16** | −.41** | −.27** | .02 | −.19** | −.37** | .45** | .23** | .47** | .51** | .62** | .50** | .37** | .66** |
Note. Total score DASS = sum score of scores on the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale; CAARS = Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scale; EFI = Executive Function Index Scale; WFIRS = Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale; DSM = The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; MD = motivational drive subscale; ORG = organization subscale; IC = impulse control subscale; EM = empathy subscale; SP = strategic planning subscale; total score = sum score of all subscales.
p < .05. **p < .005.
A simple regression analysis predicting the severity of mood symptoms from ADHD symptoms indicated that the level of ADHD symptoms measured by the CAARS DSM-IV Total Symptoms scale explained 21% of the variance of mood symptoms measured by the total score on the DASS (R = .462, R2 = .214, B = .765, p = .000).
Subsequent hierarchical regression analysis showed that the total scores on both the WFIRS and EFI significantly accounted for about 41% of the variance of the total scores on the DASS (R2 = .406, see the outcomes of Step 1 in Table 6). Here, only the total scores on the WFIRS represented a significant predictor. When adding scores on the CAARS DSM-IV ADHD Total Symptoms scale to the model, the explained mood variance slightly increased by only 1% (R2 change = .010; see the outcome of Step 2 in Table 6).
Table 6.
Steps | Predictors | Coefficients | Model | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β (B) | Bias (BCa) | SE | Bootstrap 95% confidence interval |
R | R 2 | R2 change | Adjusted R2 | F | |||
Lower | Upper | ||||||||||
Step 1 | EFI | .061 (0.127) | .002 | 0.118 | −0.105 | 0.355 | .637** | .406** | .406** | .403** | 115.919** |
WFIRS | .675 (5.294)** | .033 | 0.568** | 4.276 | 6.483 | ||||||
Step 2 | EFI | .116 (0.239)* | .000 | 0.118* | −0.006 | 0.461 | .645** | .416** | .010** | .411** | 80.243** |
WFIRS | .622 (4.876)** | .044 | 0.604** | 3.841 | 6.251 | ||||||
ADHD symptoms | .139 (0.231)* | −.004 | 0.097* | 0.041 | 0.407 |
Note. DASS = Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale; BCa = bias-corrected and accelerated; EFI = Executive Function Index Scale; WFIRS = Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale; DSM = The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.
p < .05. **p < .005.
Taken together, the outcomes of these regression analyses suggest that the explained mood variance by ADHD symptoms drops (from 21% to 1%) after controlling for daily functional impairments and executive functioning.
To test whether gender confounded the outcomes, gender was entered as a dummy independent variable in Step 1 in the just mentioned regression analysis. Results revealed that gender did not explain any of the mood variance (R2 = .003, p = .319).
A regression analysis, wherein all the EFI and the WFIRS subscales scores were entered, showed that the subscales accounted for 53% of the variance of the total DASS scores. The significant predictors were scores on the organization and the strategic planning subscales of the EFI and scores on the self-concept, and risk-taking subscales of the WFIRS (see Table 7).
Table 7.
Predictors | Coefficients |
Model |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β (B) | Bias (BCa) | SE | Bootstrap 95% confidence interval |
R | R 2 | Adjusted R2 | F | ||
Lower | Upper | ||||||||
MD | −.053 (−0.424) | .041 | 0.371 | −1.183 | 0.427 | .732** | .535** | .518** | 31.502** |
ORG | −.119 (−0.708)* | .011 | 0.318* | −1.291 | −0.023 | ||||
IC | −.082 (−0.498) | .024 | 0.317 | −1.177 | 0.220 | ||||
EM | .028 (0.191) | −.004 | 0.335 | −0.483 | 0.840 | ||||
SP | .132 (0.625)** | −.009 | 0.204** | 0.263 | 1.013 | ||||
Family | .081 (4.011) | −.081 | 2.612 | −1.287 | 9.101 | ||||
Work | −.035 (−1.038) | −.468 | 1.860 | −5.629 | .897 | ||||
School | .068 (2.902) | .310 | 3.178 | −3.995 | 10.195 | ||||
Life skills | .010 (0.386) | .249 | 2.704 | −4.630 | 6.338 | ||||
Self-concept | .456 (12.948)** | .042 | 1.640** | 9.827 | 16.285 | ||||
Social functioning | .096 (4.729) | −.029 | 3.255 | −2.376 | 11.283 | ||||
Risk taking | .143 (7.827)* | −.135 | 3.484* | .989 | 14.089 |
Note. DASS = Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale; BCa = bias-corrected and accelerated; MD = motivational drive subscale of the EFI; ORG = organization subscale of the EFI; IC = impulse control subscale of the EFI; EM = empathy subscale of the EFI; SP = strategic planning subscale of the EFI; EFI = Executive Function Index Scale; WFIRS = Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale.
p < .05. **p < .005.
Discussion
The present study examined the relationship between ADHD symptomatology and mood problems from the perspective of daily life impairments and executive functioning in university students. In this study, the ADHD and mood symptoms were moderately associated, but after controlling for executive functioning and daily life functional impairments, the association was significantly reduced. This indicates that the elevated level of mood symptoms in ADHD are influenced mainly by daily functional impairments and difficulties in executive functioning. These factors alone predicted a considerable proportion (41%–53%) of the variance of the mood symptoms. The importance of the present study is that mood symptoms can be seen as a result of coping with the negative outcomes individuals with ADHD experience in daily life, and that ADHD symptoms such as inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity do not play a role in the ADHD–mood relationship. This suggestion is compatible with, so far, untested theoretical discussions, especially about undiagnosed university students, who may suffer from ADHD and potentially are not receiving an appropriate treatment (Combs et al., 2015; Fier & Brzezinski, 2010; Panevska et al., 2015).
The association of mood symptoms with ADHD and related problems has a complex nature. Although the present study shows that mood symptoms like depression and stress can be seen as result of coping with increased severity of ADHD symptoms, especially in nonclinical sample adults, the study does not rule out the possibility that ADHD symptoms may rise from mood symptoms (Nankoo et al., 2018) and stressors arising from the university environment (Alexander & Harrison, 2013). Said differently, after enrolling at a university, a new phase of life begins with changes in lifestyle, financial responsibilities, and the rise of academic worries and a preoccupation with postgraduation life (Ibrahim et al., 2013). These worries and stressors may increase anxiety, depression, and stress levels leading to high prevalence rate of mood symptoms (Daddona, 2011; Fier & Brzezinski, 2010; Ibrahim et al., 2013), which in turn may lead to problems with concentration and impulsivity, and behaviors that resemble ADHD symptoms (Alexander & Harrison, 2013).
Examining the relative contribution of each of the specific daily functional impairments and executive functions revealed that out of the studied functions only poor organization and planning skills, as well as problems with self-concept and risk taking were significant predictors of mood symptoms. This is consistent with those few studies testing the association of planning and organization skills with mood symptoms in university students (Abdallah & Gabr, 2014; Ajilchi & Nejati, 2017; Simmons et al., 2018). For example, Abdallah and Gabr (2014) showed that weak organization skills (i.e., organizing lectures and timetable) are associated with anxiety and stress measured by the DASS. The same holds for the association of depression with risk taking (Bannink et al., 2015; Pailing & Reniers, 2018) and problems with self-esteem (see a meta-analysis by Aboalshamat et al., 2017; Nankoo et al., 2018; Sowislo & Orth, 2013). On the one hand, students may take risky actions (e.g., smoking Cannabis), as a distracting mean, to decrease anxious, depressive, and/or stressful feelings (Arbel et al., 2018; Michael & Ben-Zur, 2007). On the other hand, being engaged continuously in risky behaviors may increase levels of anxiety concerning future career. Regarding the self-esteem, Orth et al. (2008) explained possible ways in which low self-esteem can lead to mood symptoms such as depressive symptoms. Here, students with high levels of low self-esteem may find themselves not fitting with their peers in a challenging university setting. As a result, they may avoid social interactions or persistently seek for extensive positive support from their social ties to increase their self-confidence. This, in turn, increases the chance of being socially rejected and being depressed/anxious/stressed.
Remarkably, and in contrary to previous studies’ suggestions, empathy did not explain mood symptoms. Previous studies indicated that experiencing others’ negative feelings/pain may lead to high levels of psychological distress and negative moods (Schreiter et al., 2013). It may worth to note that empathy was probably not a useful executive function construct to predict mood symptoms, as it may only have a slight or no direct impact on daily functioning.
However, this particular research area (i.e., testing which of the specific executive functions and daily problems may lead to increased mood symptoms) is insufficiently addressed in the literature. As consequence, we call for future studies to replicate the present outcomes in different samples of university students.
The study showed that outcomes are not confounded by gender differences. Indeed, gender did not explain the variance in mood symptoms. Although this is consistent with studies showing no evidence for moderation effects of gender on the association between ADHD and mood symptoms (Biederman et al., 2004), other previous studies indicated that females are more vulnerable to develop mood disorders than males (Gudjonsson et al., 2009; Williamson & Johnston, 2015). However, these studies focused on patients with clinical ADHD and mood disorders. Nonclinical university students represent different samples because they could manage to reach university level and showed academic success. It could be speculated, therefore, that male and female university students use the same strategies (e.g., by sharing these experiences with their peers or seeking for efficient support in academia) to compensate for the elevated level of distress and negative moods. However, future research is required to investigate this speculation. It is important to note that the absence of gender effects could be due to the fact that our sample included more females than males, and thus the present findings could be considered to reflect more the association between ADHD and mood symptoms in females. Clearly, future studies are required to elaborate on the role of gender in the association between ADHD and mood symptoms in nonclinical university students, using equal numbers of males and females.
Conclusion and Relevance
The present study suggests that a considerable proportion of the severity of mood symptoms can be predicted from daily functional problems and difficulties in executive functioning in participants with varying degrees of ADHD symptoms. When controlling for these predictors, the key behavioral symptoms of ADHD (namely, inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity) on their own showed a very minor contribution to mood symptoms.
The study has considerable clinical relevance to those who are working with distressed students in the university setting. The focus of clinicians should be shifted toward looking at how the patients’ distress is inflicted by functional impairments and executive dysfunctions more than symptom severity per se. During the diagnostic assessment, clinicians should be more cautious when giving a diagnosis of mood disorders in students with ADHD. Clinicians may also monitor whether there is an enhancement of mood symptoms in those who show reduced risky activities and less problems with self-concept, organization, and planning in ADHD. Furthermore, repeating the assessments of mood disorders and daily functioning in patients with ADHD over different periods of times is recommended to investigate whether the mood symptoms disappear overtime to focus treatment on ADHD symptoms only. Although the treatment of mood symptoms could wait until treatment that addresses functional impairments is in place, doing so may run a risk of having delayed treatment for individuals who have indeed “genuine” mood disorders and ADHD.
Limitations
In general, the contemporary literature cautions against the singular use of self-report checklists to assess ADHD symptoms in adults. Even adults with ADHD may overestimate or underestimate their ADHD characteristics (McCann & Roy-Byrne, 2004). However, it is still problematic to find other ways (than self-report scales) to assess ADHD in adults. Clinicians mainly use responses to self-reported scales and subjective observations to decide about a clinical ADHD diagnosis in adults.
Another limitation could be the sampling method; the study used a convenient sample from only one university. Thus, the findings cannot be generalized on the whole university student population. The study does not count for cultural differences and equal representations of demographic variables such as race and social-economic level.
In addition, selection procedure of participants (using a posted advertisement) may lead to a bias toward higher participation of student interested in ADHD, including those with ADHD symptoms or a diagnosis of other disorders. As such, the study sample may not be representative. How strong this bias is may be estimated from reference data on prevalence of ADHD among University students. Table 1 shows that about 16% of the sample reported a diagnosis with ADHD. However, having a sample, which may be enriched with a higher proportion of ADHD symptomatology, may be advantageous for statistical power of the study analysis.
Finally, by using only the EFI, the present study did not extensively cover all detailed aspects of executive functioning. Indeed, the EFI was used more as a fast screening tool to estimate the overall level of executive functioning in a large student population. To get more insights into various executive functions, future studies could use the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult version (BRIEF-A), which is the most common measure of executive functions, consisting of 75 items covering several executive functions, namely inhibition, self-monitoring, planning, working memory, shifting, initiation, task monitoring, emotional control, and organization (Roth et al., 2005).
Author Biographies
Saleh M. H. Mohamed, PhD, is a researcher at the university of Groningen. His research involve adult ADHD symptoms and related problems (e.g. deficits in brain laterality, state regulation, error monitoring and executive functioning).
Norbert A. Börger, PhD, is a researcher in the field of bio and neuopsychology of developmental disorders. His research focus is on ADHD symptoms and related symptoms in children and adults and related disorders.
Jaap J. van der Meere, PhD, is an emeritus professor in the bio and neuopsychology of developmental disorders at the university of Groningen, Guest professor at the Jyvaskula University Finland, Chair of the Dutch Scientific Journal of Autism: Theory and Practice.
Footnotes
Authors’ Note: Saleh M. H. Mohamed is also affiliated with Beni-Suef University, Egypt.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding: The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
ORCID iD: Saleh M. H. Mohamed https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1637-8281
References
- Abdallah A. R., Gabr H. M. (2014). Depression, anxiety and stress among first year medical students in an Egyptian public university. International Research Journal of Medicine and Medical Sciences, 2(1), 11–19. [Google Scholar]
- Aboalshamat K., Jawhari A., Alotibi S., Alzahrani K., Al-Mohimeed H., Alzahrani M., Rashedi H. (2017). Relationship of self-esteem with depression, anxiety, and stress among dental and medical students in Jeddah, Saudi Arabia. Journal of International Medicine and Dentistry, 4(2), 61–68. [Google Scholar]
- Ajilchi B., Nejati V. (2017). Executive functions in students with depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, 8(3), 223–232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander S. J., Harrison A. G. (2013). Cognitive responses to stress, depression, and anxiety and their relationship to ADHD symptoms in first year psychology students. Journal of Attention Disorders, 17(1), 29–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Arbel R., Perrone L., Margolin G. (2018). Adolescents’ daily worries and risky behaviors: The buffering role of support seeking. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 47(6), 900–911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Assessing adults with ADHD and comorbidities. (2009). Primary Care Companion to the Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 11(1), 25–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bannink R., Broeren S., Heydelberg J., van’t Klooster E., Raat H. (2015). Depressive symptoms and clustering of risk behaviours among adolescents and young adults attending vocational education: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health, 15(1), Article 396. https://bmcpublichealth.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12889-015-1692-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biederman J., Faraone S. V., Monuteaux M. C., Bober M., Cadogen E. (2004). Gender effects on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults, revisited. Biological Psychiatry, 55(7), 692–700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buitelaar J. K., Kan C. C., Asherson P. (Eds.). (2011). ADHD in adults: Characterization, diagnosis, and treatment. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Canadian ADHD Resource Alliance (2019). Retrieved from https://www.caddra.ca/wp-content/uploads/WFIRS-S.pdf
- Canu W. H., Hartung C. M., Stevens A. E., Lefler E. K. (2016). Psychometric properties of the Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale: Evidence for utility in research, assessment, and treatment of ADHD in emerging adults. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1-13. doi: 10.1177/1087054716661421 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christiansen H., Kis B., Hirsch O., Matthies S., Hebebrand J., Uekermann J., . . .Colla M. (2012). German validation of the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS) II: Reliability, validity, diagnostic sensitivity and specificity. European Psychiatry, 27(5), 321–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Combs M. A., Canu W. H., Broman-Fulks J. J., Rocheleau C. A., Nieman D. C. (2015). Perceived stress and ADHD symptoms in adults. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19(5), 425–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conners C. K., Erhardt D., Sparrow E. (1999). Conner’s Adult ADHD Rating Scales: Technical manual. Multi-Health Systems Incorporated. [Google Scholar]
- Crawford J. R., Henry J. D. (2003). The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS): Normative data and latent structure in a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 111–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daddona M. F. (2011). Peer educators responding to students with mental health issues. New Directions for Student Services, 2011(133), 29–39. [Google Scholar]
- Dorr M. M., Armstrong K. J. (2018). Executive functioning and impairment in emerging adult college students with ADHD symptoms. Journal of Attention Disorders, 23(14), 1759–1765. 10.1177/1087054718787883 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dvorsky M. R., Langberg J. M. (2014). Predicting impairment in college students with ADHD: The role of executive functions. Journal of Attention Disorders, 23(13), 1624–1636. 10.1177/1087054714548037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erhardt D., Epstein J. N., Conners C. K., Parker J. D. A., Sitarenios G. (1999). Self-ratings of ADHD symptomas in auts II: Reliability, validity, and diagnostic sensitivity. Journal of Attention Disorders, 3(3), 153–158. [Google Scholar]
- Fier S. M., Brzezinski L. G. (2010). Facilitating the high school-to-college transition for students with psychiatric disabilities: Information and strategies for school counselors. Journal of School Counseling, 8(10), 1–40. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/6220/cf60f99179ad4c242a86732b05100a0daaeb.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Gajria K., Kosinski M., Sikirica V., Huss M., Livote E., Reilly K., . . . Erder M. H. (2015). Psychometric validation of the Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale-parent report form in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 13(1), Article 184. 10.1186/s12955-015-0379-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garner J. K. (2009). Conceptualizing the relations between executive functions and self-regulated learning. The Journal of Psychology, 143(4), 405–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gershon J. (2002). Gender differences in ADHD. The ADHD Report, 10(4), 8–16. [Google Scholar]
- Gray S., Woltering S., Mawjee K., Tannock R. (2014). The Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): Utility in college students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. PeerJ, 2, Article e2324. 10.7717/peerj.324 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Green A. L., Rabiner D. L. (2012). What do we really know about ADHD in college students? Neurotherapeutics, 9(3), 559–568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gudjonsson G. H., Sigurdsson J. F., Smari J., Young S. (2009). The relationship between satisfaction with life, ADHD symptoms, and associated problems among university students. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12(6), 507–515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadianfard H., Kiani B., Weiss M. D. (2017). Psychometric properties of the Persian version of the Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale–self-report form in Iranian adolescents. Journal of Attention Disorders, 23(13), 1600–1609. 10.1177/1087054717738084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamed A. M., Kauer A. J., Stevens H. E. (2015). Why the diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder matters. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, Article 168. 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ibrahim A. K., Kelly S. J., Adams C. E., Glazebrook C. (2013). A systematic review of studies of depression prevalence in university students. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(3), 391–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruger G. H. (2011). Executive functioning and positive psychological characteristics: A replication and extension. Psychological Reports, 108(2), 477–486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lovibond P. F., Lovibond S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCann B. S., Roy-Byrne P. (2004). Screening and diagnostic utility of self-report Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder scales in adults. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 45(3), 175–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michael K., Ben-Zur H. (2007). Risk-taking among adolescents: Associations with social and affective factors. Journal of Adolescence, 30(1), 17–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nankoo M. M., Palermo R., Bell J. A., Pestell C. M. (2018). Examining the rate of self-reported ADHD-related traits and endorsement of depression, anxiety, stress, and autistic-like traits in Australian university students. Journal of Attention Disorders, 23(8), 869–886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nieuwenhuijsen K., De Boer A. G. E. M., Verbeek J. H. A. M., Blonk R. W. B., Van Dijk F. J. H. (2003). The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS): Detecting anxiety disorder and depression in employees absent from work because of mental health problems. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60(Suppl. 1), i77–i82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikolas M. A., Burt S. A. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on ADHD symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(1), 1–17. 10.1037/a0018010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norwalk K., Norvilitis J. M., MacLean M. G. (2009). ADHD symptomatology and its relationship to factors associated with college adjustment. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13(3), 251–258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orth U., Robins R. W., Roberts B. W. (2008). Low self-esteem prospectively predicts depression in adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(3), 695–708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pailing A. N., Reniers R. L. (2018). Depressive and socially anxious symptoms, psychosocial maturity, and risk perception: Associations with risk-taking behaviour. PLOS ONE, 13(8), Article e0202423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panevska L. S., Zafirova-Ivanovska B., Vasilevska K., Isjanovska R., Kadri H. (2015). Relationship between ADHD and depression among university students in Macedonia. Materia Sociomedica, 27(1), 18–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parkitny L., McAuley J. (2010). The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS). Journal of Physiotherapy, 56(3), 204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prevatt F., Dehili V., Taylor N., Marshall D. (2015). Anxiety in college students with ADHD: Relationship to cognitive functioning. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19(3), 222–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purper-Ouakil D., Wohl M., Michel G., Mouren M. C., Gorwood P. (2004). Symptom variations in ADHD: Importance of context, development and comorbidity. L’Encéphale, 30(6), 533–539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth R. M., Isquith P. K., Gioia G. A. (2005). BRIEF-A: Behavior rating inventory of executive function—Adult version: Professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
- Schreiter S., Pijnenborg G. H. M., Aan Het Rot M. (2013). Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(1), 1–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmons L., Crook A., Cannonier C., Simmons C. (2018). There’s an app for that: The impact of reminder apps on student learning and anxiety. Journal of Education for Business, 93(5), 185–195. [Google Scholar]
- Sowislo J. F., Orth U. (2013). Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 213–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spinella M. (2005). Self-rated executive function: Development of the executive function index. International Journal of Neuroscience, 115(5), 649–667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeda T., Tsuji Y., Kanazawa J., Sakai T., Weiss M. D. (2017). Psychometric properties of the Japanese version of the Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale: Self-report. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 9(3), 169–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tuckman A. (2007). Integrative treatment for adult ADHD: A practical, easy-to-use guide for clinicians. New Harbinger. [Google Scholar]
- Weatherly J. N., Ferraro F. R. (2011). Executive functioning and delay discounting of four different outcomes in university students. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(2), 183–187. [Google Scholar]
- Weiss M. D. (2010). The unique aspects of assessment of ADHD. Primary Psychiatry, 17(5), 21–25. [Google Scholar]
- Weiss M. D., McBride N. M., Craig S., Jensen P. (2018). Conceptual review of measuring functional impairment: Findings from the Weiss functional impairment rating scale. Evidence-Based Mental Health, 21(4), 155–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weyandt L., DuPaul G. J., Verdi G., Rossi J. S., Swentosky A. J., Vilardo B. S., . . . Carson K. S. (2013). The performance of college students with and without ADHD: Neuropsychological, academic, and psychosocial functioning. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 35(4), 421–435. [Google Scholar]
- Weyandt L., Oster D. R., Gudmundsdottir B. G., DuPaul G. J., Anastopoulos A. D. (2017). Neuropsychological functioning in college students with and without ADHD. Neuropsychology, 31(2), 160–172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williamson D., Johnston C. (2015). Gender differences in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A narrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 15–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf L. E., Simkowitz P., Carlson H. (2009). College students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 11(5), 415–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood W. L., Lewandowski L. J., Lovett B. J., Antshel K. M. (2017). Executive dysfunction and functional impairment associated with sluggish cognitive tempo in emerging adulthood. Journal of Attention Disorders, 21(8), 691–700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]