Abstract
Autism arises in high and low-risk families. De novo mutation contributes to autism incidence in low-risk families as there is a higher incidence in the affected of the simplex families than in their unaffected siblings. But the extent of contribution in low-risk families cannot be determined solely from simplex families as they are a mixture of low and high-risk. The rate of de novo mutation in nearly pure populations of high-risk families, the multiplex families, has not previously been rigorously determined. Moreover, rates of de novo mutation have been underestimated from studies based on low resolution microarrays and whole exome sequencing. Here we report on findings from whole genome sequence (WGS) of both simplex families from the Simons Simplex Collection (SSC) and multiplex families from the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange (AGRE). After removing the multiplex samples with excessive cell-line genetic drift, we find that the contribution of de novo mutation in multiplex is significantly smaller than the contribution in simplex. We use WGS to provide high resolution CNV profiles and to analyze more than coding regions, and revise upward the rate in simplex autism due to an excess of de novo events targeting introns. Based on this study, we now estimate that de novo events contribute to 52–67% of cases of autism arising from low risk families, and 30–39% of cases of all autism.
Subject terms: Next-generation sequencing, Autism spectrum disorders, Autism spectrum disorders
Yoon, Munoz, et al. investigate the rate of de novo coding and non-coding variants in families with high- and low-risk for autism using whole-genome sequence data from collections of families with autism. They demonstrate that de novo intronic variants increase the risk of autism, that the contribution of de novo variants is significantly larger in low-risk families, and that de novo variants contribute to 30-39% of cases of all autism.
Introduction
Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a strong genetic component. This was first inferred by the greater concordance of monozygotic twins than dizygotic twins and an increase in risk to the siblings of an affected child. The importance of genetics was later directly demonstrated by finding a higher incidence of de novo gene disruption in affected than unaffected siblings within families with only a single affected offspring (simplex families)1–12. This observation, plus the higher incidence of autism in males than females (3:1), the observed rate of ~25% concordance for boy siblings, and the nearly 50% incidence of autism in the third born male child of multiplex families13 led to what we call the “unified genetic theory for sporadic and inherited autism” in 2007. In this theory we conjectured two risk classes of families. We proposed that highly penetrant de novo mutations contribute to most low-risk families and that transmission from a carrier parent contributed to autism in most high-risk families. We used data on sibling risk and brood size to estimate that roughly half of autism arises from low-risk families and half from high-risk families14,15.
We still lack a clear demonstration that low and high-risk families are truly distinct in their genetic causation. The role of de novo mutations has been firmly demonstrated in collections of simplex families that would be expected to contain mainly low-risk families. On the other hand, the incidence of de novo mutation in multiplex families, which would be expected to be almost exclusively high-risk, has been equivocal5,7, and it is known that common variation plays a substantial role in autism heritability16,17. Unlike the simplex collections, largely drawn from blood, many of the multiplex collections are drawn from EBV-immortalized lymphoid cells maintained in culture. These, due to oligo-clonality and/or cell-line genetic drift, have an excess of somatic artifacts, rendering measurement of de novo rates problematic. We have sought to compare the rates in simplex and multiplex collections by controlling for drift in the latter. Our results of low rates in multiplex confirm a recent study of the same population18.
Basing the contribution of de novo variants in simplex collections solely on large copy number events (CNV) and the likely-gene damaging (LGD) coding mutations will lead to underestimates. A great deal of uncertainty still exists for the role of missense mutations, the full contribution from CNVs and other structural rearrangements, and only preliminary attempts have been made in the non-coding space19–25. Thus, quantifying the role of de novo mutation in simplex families is still an open problem.
In this paper we used the newly generated whole genome sequencing (WGS) data from large multiplex (AGRE) and large simplex (SSC) family collections to compare the contributions of de novo variants in low and high-risk families and to evaluate the role of de novo intronic variants in SSC. We consider large scale copy number events, small indels, and substitutions. First, however, we identify the AGRE multiplex samples with extensive somatic genomic drift by the presence of two hallmarks: (1) excess single nucleotide variations throughout the entire genome; and (2) a preponderance of variants with unexpected allele ratios. Removing these samples, we then measure rates of de novo events in the remainder. From this new data, we make better estimates of contribution of de novo mutation in collections of simplex and multiplex families. Our analysis points to a substantial contribution of de novo variants in introns. From our results we update estimates of the contribution of de novo events in high and low-risk families and in autism overall.
Results
Comparing de novo rates in multiplex and simplex families
Filtering multiplex samples with excessive cell-line genetic drift
Some of the AGRE DNAs used for WGS were extracted from cultured lymphoblastoid cell lines (LCL), and some from whole blood (WB). In contrast all of the SSC DNA was extracted from whole blood (WB). Samples from LCL pose a risk: without care, the large number of events acquired during cell culture or immortalization will be mistaken for germline mutation. These artifacts are made evident in the AGRE by examining the frequency and allele ratios of the substitutions observed in the children but not in either parent (Fig. 1). For each child-sample we display in a scatter plot its number of “acquired” substitutions adjusted for coverage (X-axis) and the mean ratio of heterozygosity for those substitutions (Y-axis). The distributions of these two properties are shown in the margins for the LCL from AGRE (blue), the WB from AGRE (green), and the WB from the SSC (yellow). Clearly the WB data from AGRE and SSC show similar distributions. On the other hand, the LCL data is multiphasic, with one phase in agreement with WB.
To identify the usable samples, we modeled the WB data as a two-dimensional Gaussian distribution on the variables of allele count and ratio (see Fig. 1 and Supplementary Data 1). We then established an ellipse that would exclude 0.1% of the Gaussian distribution. This ellipse empirically excludes about 0.3% (13 out of 3756) of the WB samples. We consider the samples in the ellipse to be free of cell-line genetic drift. We then apply the same filter to the LCL of the AGRE, and thereby exclude an excess of 50% of those (1062 out of 1929). We note that our method of filtering agrees very well with the machine learning method of Ruzzo, et al.18, even though the latter is conditioned on the variant and not the sample. Consistent with our analysis based on substitutions, the same samples with detected cell-line genetic drift have an excess of acquired CNVs (Supplementary Fig. 1 top two panels), and many of these CNVs do not have integer copy number (Supplementary Fig. 1 bottom two panels).
Comparing AGRE and SSC for small scale de novo mutations in genes
For the rest of the analysis, we selected three groups from the trios passing the sample cell-line genetic drift filters: 1,874 unaffected children from SSC; 1,869 affected children from SSC; and 1,107 affected children from AGRE. There were not enough of the unaffected siblings from AGRE for meaningful analysis. In selected children, we identified small scale de novo events, both substitutions and small indels, using previously published methods2,11 (see Materials and Methods, Supplementary Data 2 and Supplementary Data 3 for the list of all small scale de novo variants identified). These small events were then divided into six likely functional classes and counted: LGD (likely gene disrupting, consisting of nonsense, frameshift, or splice site mutations); synonymous (SYN); intronic substitutions (ISB) and small indels (IID); and intergenic substitutions (IGSB) and indels (IGID). We consider the SYN, IGSB, and IGID as free of any bias due to affected status because they will be largely without functional consequence. We do not consider in this paper missense substitutions, because their functional significance is too difficult to assess. Moreover, we exclude from consideration events on the X-chromosome.
We are interested in determining the extent to which LGD incidence is increased due to the affected status of the child within its family type. These must be adjusted for coverage, parental age, and possible cell-line genetic drift. The three groups of children were not identical in (1) depth of WGS coverage and (2) distribution of the ages of their parents (see Supplementary Fig. 2). Moreover, there is the possibility that (3) we have not completely filtered all cell line drift. These three factors clearly influence the rate of the observed de novo variants. To control for these variable rate factors, we used SYN mutations. SYN and LGDs are both located in protein coding regions and are likely to be affected similarly by parental ages, sequence coverage, and drift. We thus use de novo SYN incidence as a normalization factor for the comparison of LGD incidence in different populations, as we and others have done in the past26.
We outline here the method we use to compare the incidence of the subject class of LGD de novo variants in a group of SSC unaffected and a group of either AGRE or SSC affected using a normalization by of de novo SYN variants. We assume that the subject class (LGD) may be biased based of the affected status while the normalization class (SYN) is not, but that both classes are equally influenced by the covariates parental ages, sequence coverage and cell-line genetic drift. We are interested in two measures quantifying the role of the subject class in the affected group, the ascertainment differential (AD) and percent contributory (PC). The AD measures the excess of events above the estimate of expected number of observations of that subject class given the null hypothesis that these mutations had not contributed to the diagnosis. We compute the expected number of observations in the affected by this equation:
1 |
where ES.a is the expected number of observations of the subject class in the affected group, S.u is the actual observed number of that class for the unaffected group, N.a is the observed number of the normalization class in the affected, and N.u is the observed number of that class in unaffected. The ascertainment differential (AD) for the affected group is defined as the difference between the number of observed events and the expected number, divided by the number of children in the group, and expressed as a percentage of children:
2 |
where the S.a is the observed number of the subject class in the affected group and C.a is the number of affected children in that group. We interpret AD as the percent of affected children that were diagnosed with autism due to a contribution from the given subject class, given the assumption that the expected number of contributory events per child is smaller than 1. We define the second measure, the percent contributory (PC), with this formula:
3 |
We interpret PC as the percent of the subject class events in the affected group that have contributed to the disorder.
We compute a p-value of the AD under the null hypothesis: the subject class does not contribute to the disorder. This p-value is determined empirically as the likelihood of the observed AD arising from the data when we permute the labels “affected” and “unaffected”. Specifically, we measure the probability to achieve AD larger or equal to the observed AD under normal distribution fitted to ADs derived from 1,000 trials permuting the affected and unaffected, keeping the numbers of each group fixed. We also compute confidence intervals (CI) for the AD and the PC using 1,000 bootstrap iterations, sampling with repetition the same number of children from the set of the affected and unaffected.
With these methods we examined de novo LGD, a class of variant that is known to contribute to autism in simplex families. While most of the previous studies of ascertainment bias in this class was based on whole exome sequencing of the SSC, in this instance we have WGS from both the SSC and AGRE.
The AD and PC for the de novo LGDs in affected children from SSC are 6.48% (CI: 3.85 to 8.80) and 42.8% (CI: 27.8 to 54.3) respectively with p-value 8 × 10−7 (Table 1). These results are consistent with the well-established causal role of de novo LGDs in simplex autism2,8,10,12,27, and are comparable to previous results from WES. The AD and PC for the affected children for the multiplex families in AGRE are 1.42% (CI: −1.25 to 4.15) and 13.6% (CI: −13.4 to 35.1) with p-value of 0.14.
Table 1.
SSC unaffected | Affected | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group | Synonymous number | LGD number | Synonymous number | LGD number | Expected LGD number | Delta | p-value | AD | PC |
SSC affected | 484 | 157 | 499 | 283 | 161.9 | 121.1 | 8 × 10−07 | 6.48% (3.85–8.80) | 42.8% (27.8–54.3) |
AGRE affected | 309 | 116 | 100.2 | 15.8 | 0.14 | 1.42% (−1.25–4.15) | 13.6% (−13.4–35.1) |
The table shows the numbers of de novo LGDs (“LGD number” columns) identified in 1,874 unaffected children from SSC, 1,869 affected children from SSC and 1,107 affected children the AGRE collection together with the number of de novo synonymous mutations (“synonymous number” columns), used for normalization, identified in the same children. The table also shows the expected number of de novo LGDs (“expected LGD number” column) in the two affected groups under the null model that the incidence of the such variants in the affected children is the same as the incidence in the unaffected children. The rest of the columns quantify the causal role of the de novo LGDs in the affected children from the simplex SSC and the multiplex AGRE collections. The “delta” column shows the excess of the de novo LGDs in the two affected groups. The “p-value” is the probability of achieving a delta larger or equal to the observed one under the null model, computed through permutation of the affect status. The “AD” column shows the ascertainment differential or our estimate of the percent of the affected children that have autism in part due to a de novo LGD; the “PC” column shows the percent contributory statistic that is the percent of the de novo LGDs identified in the affected children that contributed to the disorder. Finally, the table shows the 95% confidence interval for the AD and PC statistics computed by bootstrap. See the Results section for detailed description of the methods used.
These results suggest that de novo LGDs contribute to autism in the high-risk multiplex families, but the contribution is most likely less than a quarter of the contribution of such variants in simplex families. The AD measures in the two affected groups are statistically different, with p-value of 0.001 computed by directly comparing the two groups of affected.
Comparing AGRE and SSC for large scale de novo mutations
We developed a pipeline for detection of de novo CNV events which was based on two different methods and a set of stringent filters to avoid false-positive calls (e.g. cryptic transmission). The pipeline is described in the Materials and Methods and Supplementary Note 1 and all detected events are listed in Supplementary Data 4. As we did for the small-scale events, we compared the affected children in SSC and AGRE to the unaffected in SSC and restricted the analysis only to the children that passed the sample cell-line genetic drift filters. As discussed above the three groups of children differ in the ages of their parents and in the sequence coverage. We ignored the difference of parental ages because there is no well-documented relationship between parental ages and rates of de novo CNV. Moreover, we see no evidence for such relationship in our data. We examined the effects of coverage in the three groups on the power of our pipeline to detect de novo CNVs by simulating artificial CNVs of various sizes (see legends and Materials and Methods). The results (Supplementary Fig. 4) show that the power is the same for the affected and unaffected children from SSC across all CNV sizes (from 1 kb to 100 kb). But the power to detect small (<4 kb) CNVs in the data from the AGRE affected is diminished. In comparing the SSC to AGRE families, therefore, we ignored the CNVs smaller than 4 kb. We simplified the definitions of AD and PC to avoid the normalization for coverage and parental ages by using the number of children instead of the number of de novo variants of a normalization class.
Table 2 summarizes the relevant incidence data for de novo CNV from the three groups of children separately. We further partition CNVs into: ‘intergenic’, meaning not overlapping the transcript of any gene listed in RefGene; ‘coding’, meaning any event overlapping the coding regions of a RefGene; and ‘genic noncoding’, meaning any event that overlaps a transcript of a RefGene but without overlapping coding regions (Fig. 2).
Table 2.
SSC unaffected | affected | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effect | Group | CNV number | CNV rate | CNV number | CNV rate | Expected CNVs number | Delta | p-value | AD | PC |
all | SSC affected | 86 | 0.046 | 157 | 0.084 | 85.8 | 71.2 | 2 × 10−06 | 3.81% (2.16–5.47) | 45.4% (29.2–57.9) |
AGRE affected | 56 | 0.051 | 50.8 | 5.2 | 0.29 | 0.47% (−1.15–2.13) | 9.3% (−29.3–34.4) | |||
coding | SSC affected | 44 | 0.023 | 106 | 0.057 | 43.9 | 62.1 | 4 × 10−07 | 3.32% (2.04–4.63) | 58.6% (40.9–71.0) |
AGRE affected | 34 | 0.031 | 26.0 | 8.0 | 0.12 | 0.72% (−0.52–2.06) | 23.6% (−21.3–52.1) | |||
intergenic | SSC affected | 26 | 0.014 | 34 | 0.018 | 25.9 | 8.1 | 0.15 | 0.43% (−0.34–1.23) | 23.7% (−24.9–55.6) |
AGRE affected | 15 | 0.014 | 15.4 | −0.4 | 0.52 | −0.03% (−0.93–0.83) | −2.4% (−122.3–43.7) | |||
genic noncoding | SSC affected | 16 | 0.009 | 17 | 0.009 | 16.0 | 1.0 | 0.41 | 0.06% (−0.56–0.70) | 6.1% (−92.9–57.3) |
AGRE affected | 7 | 0.006 | 9.5 | −2.5 | 0.74 | −0.22% (−0.80–0.43) | −35.0% (−316.3–43.3) |
The table presents our results for the contribution to autism of de novo CNVs as a whole (rows labeled ‘all’ in the “effect” column) and separately for the subsets of CNVs labeled as coding (CNVs that overlap with a coding exon), intergenic (CNVs that do not affect any genes) and genic noncoding (CNVs that affect genes but not coding regions). We analyzed only de novo CNVs of at least 4KB that we could detect with identical power in SSC and AGRE (see Supplementary Fig. 4). We used the number of children in a group as normalization factor to compute the “expected CNVs number” under the null model and we show the rate of the de novo CNVs in three groups in the “CNV rate” columns. Otherwise, we used the same three groups of children and the same statistics (delta, p-value, AD, PC) to quantify the role of a class of de novo mutation as described in Table 1’s legend.
In the SSC there is strong statistical evidence for an excess for events that disrupt coding regions (p-value = 4 × 10−7, AD = 3.32% (CI: 2.04 to 4.63), PC = 58.6% (40.9 to 71.0)). PC increases as copy number events hit more genes (see Supplementary Fig. 5), rising from 40% for events hitting one or more genes to 85% for events hitting five genes of more. The increasing impact of multigenic events has been previously noted3. There is no signal in the ‘intergenic’ or the ‘genic noncoding’ class (p-values = 0.15 and 0.41 respectively) for CNVs with size of at least 4 kb. In AGRE, there is no evidence for all CNV events (p-value: 0.29). There was a marginally significant excess of coding de novo CNVs (p-value: 0.12, AD = 0.72%, PC = 23.6%). This AD for coding CNVs in AGRE is about one quarter of the AD for SSC, with the difference being highly significant (p-value: 0.0007) using permutation testing.
Aggregated signals from de novo CNVs and LGDs
We analyzed the aggregated burden of de novo LGD and de novo coding CNVs in the affected of the SSC and AGRE by a permutation method (Table 3). The AD for the aggregated events is 9.80% (CI: 6.91 to 12.46, p-value = 5 × 10−11) for the SSC affected, and 2.15% (CI: −0.64 to 5.03, p-value = 0.062) for the AGRE affected. The aggregated ADs are roughly equal to the sum of the ADs for the individual event types. The aggregated AD for the affected children in the multiplex AGRE families is only marginally significant and its magnitude is a quarter of the AD for the simplex families in SSC. The direct comparison of the rates of the aggregated events shows that the two groups of affected children are significantly different (p-value: 10−5).
Table 3.
Group | Events number | Expected events number | delta | p-value | AD |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
SSC affected | 389 | 205.7 | 183.3 | 5 × 10−11 | 9.80% (6.91–12.46) |
AGRE affected | 150 | 126.2 | 23.8 | 0.062 | 2.15% (−0.64–5.03) |
The table shows our joined analysis of the de novo LGDs and the coding de novo CNVs that allowed us to avoid the assumptions of independence between the two classes. The “events number” column shows to total number of de novo LGDs and de novo coding CNVs larger than 4KB in the two groups of affected children. The “expected events number” was computed by using the appropriate normalization factor for the two classes: number of de novo synonymous variants for de novo LGDs and the number of children for the coding de novo CNVs. Table 1’s legend describes the “delta”, “p-value”, and “AD” columns.
Contribution of de novo intron mutations to children from simplex families
De novo CNVs
The ~30X WGS data enables us to observe smaller de novo CNVs than was possible with microarrays or WES. By simulation we show that, for the SSC, we have power of nearly 90% to detect events of size 1 kb from the WGS data, with equal power for both affected and unaffected children (Supplementary Fig. 4). In Table 4 we tabulate all single coding gene events further partitioned by class: coding and genic noncoding. The latter is divided into ‘intercoding intronic’, meaning exclusively between coding exons; and ‘peripheral’ meaning genic events overlapping UTRs or the introns splitting UTRs. There is clear signal from single gene events (p-value = 0.027), a finding that is predicted, but was not clear from our previous studies based on microarrays due to insufficient resolution. The AD 1.18% divides between coding (p-value = 0.12) and intercoding intronic (p-value = 0.0068). Although not statistically different, the PC for the intercoding intronic class (57.8%) is higher than for the coding class (24.6%). The peripheral regions show no signal, but the counts are low (5 in affected and 4 in unaffected). Other researchers report de novo targets in ‘regulatory’ domains in the periphery of genes21, and we discuss this later.
Table 4.
SSC unaffected | SSC affected | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
effect | CNV number | CNV rate | CNV number | CNV rate | expected CNVs number | delta | p-value | AD | PC |
all | 53 | 0.028 | 75 | 0.040 | 52.9 | 22.1 | 0.027 | 1.18% (−0.03–2.34) | 29.5% (−0.7–50.9) |
coding | 31 | 0.017 | 41 | 0.022 | 30.9 | 10.1 | 0.12 | 0.54% (−0.28–1.42) | 24.6% (−16.4–53.7) |
intercoding intronic | 11 | 0.006 | 26 | 0.014 | 11.0 | 15.0 | 0.0068 | 0.80% (0.22–1.47) | 57.8% (20.7–82.3) |
peripheral | 4 | 0.002 | 5 | 0.003 | 4.0 | 1.0 | 0.34 | 0.05% (−0.27–0.37) | 20.2% (−304.9–100.0) |
The increased power for CNV detection through whole-genome sequencing allowed us to examine the role small CNVs that affect a single gene. We analyzed all de novo CNVs including the ones smaller than 4KB and focused on the 1,874 unaffected and the 1,869 affected children from SSC. We quantified the role of all single-gene de novo CNVs (labeled as ‘all’) in the effect columns and separately for the non-overlapping subsets labeled as ‘coding’, ‘intercoding intronic’ and ‘peripheral’ (see Fig. 2 for definition of these terms). The format of the table and the analysis was identical to what is described in the Table 2’s legend.
Small de novo events
We identified extra support for the role of intercoding intronic regions by examining the de novo insertion-deletion events (indels) in intercoding intronic regions (IID) in SSC. These events were detected by the same sequence analysis pipeline we used to identify de novo LGD variants. The rate of de novo indels is influenced by age of parents (Supplementary Figure 6) and coverage, so we used de novo intergenic indels (IGID) as the normalization class for the comparison of incidences of de novo IIDs in the affected and unaffected children in SSC. We excluded the variants that directly affected the canonical splice-site regions. Our expectation of overall statistical signal was low, due to larger background and lower likelihood of impact for small events. Indeed, we see large numbers of intronic de novo indels in the SSC, with no signal of an ascertainment bias overall (Table 5, rows 1) in affected versus their unaffected siblings (p-values of 0.25).
Table 5.
SSC unaffected | SSC affected | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
gene | event | normalization | functional | normalization | functional | expected functional | |||||
gene set | number | type | number | number | number | number | number | delta | p-value | AD | PC |
all genes | 19,512 | IID | 5,859 | 3,916 | 5,768 | 3,932 | 3,855.2 | 76.8 | 0.25 | 4.11% (−7.52–15.85) | 2.0% (−3.7–7.4) |
autism LGD targets | 748 | IID | 5,859 | 297 | 5,768 | 345 | 292.4 | 52.6 | 0.026 | 2.82% (−0.16–5.55) | 15.3% (−1.0–28.0) |
all NDD LGD targets | 1,521 | IID | 5,859 | 560 | 5,768 | 645 | 551.3 | 93.7 | 0.008 | 5.01% (1.17–8.69) | 14.5% (3.6–23.8) |
autism missense targets | 3,560 | IID | 5,859 | 1,153 | 5,768 | 1,171 | 1,135.1 | 35.9 | 0.25 | 1.92% (−3.38–7.33) | 3.1% (−5.6–11.3) |
autism synonymous targets | 1,570 | IID | 5,859 | 561 | 5,768 | 569 | 552.3 | 16.7 | 0.30 | 0.89% (−2.89–4.90) | 2.9% (−10.0–15.3) |
all genes | 19,512 | ISB | 58,274 | 37,814 | 59,340 | 38,057 | 38,505.7 | −448.7 | 0.90 | −24.01% (−59.04–15.00) | −1.2% (−2.9–0.7) |
autism LGD targets | 748 | ISB | 58,274 | 3,023 | 59,340 | 2,965 | 3,078.3 | −113.3 | 0.92 | −6.06% (−14.37–2.88) | −3.8% (−9.3–1.8) |
We tabulated the numbers of de novo IID and ISB identified in the 1,874 unaffected and the 1,869 affected from the Simons Simplex Collection (the same groups as used in Table 4), separately for affected and unaffected children, and for five subsets of genes for the IID and two sets of genes for the ISB. We quantified the contributions to autism of each of the seven variants subsets. We used de novo intergenic indels (IGID) and de novo intergenic substitutions for normalization for the IID and ISB, respectively. The “functional number” columns show the number of IID or ISB and the “normalization number” columns show the number of IGID or IGSB. The methods and statistics presented (i.e. delta, p-value, AD, PC) are described in Table 1’s legend and in the Results section. Supplementary Data 5 shows the genes in each of the five sets. The first one is labeled “all genes” and represents all coding RefSeq genes. The other four are defined by the genes targeted by de novo mutations in children affected with one of these neuro developmental disorders (NDD): autism, schizophrenia, intellectual disability, developmental delay, or epilepsy. We used Denovo-db36 as a repository of the published de novo variants in these disorders. The “all autism LGD targets” refers to the genes targeted by de novo LGD mutation in a child with autism. The “all NDD LGD targets” are the genes with de novo LGDs in children with any of the five analyzed NDDs. The “autism missense targets” and “autism synonymous targets” are defined as genes with de novo missense and synonymous mutations respectively in children with autism.
To enhance our ability to detect signal, we therefore focused on the introns of genes that we expected to have a more favorable signal to noise ratio, namely those genes already identified as probable targets by prior studies. From Denovo-db28, we obtained two lists of genes targeted by de novo LGDs, either in children with autism (748 genes) or more generally in multiple neuro developmental disorders (1,521 genes) (see Table 5 and Supplementary Data 5). In both of these sets of genes, we see statistical signal for IID, with AD of 2.82% (CI: from −0.16 to −5.55) and p-value of 0.026 for the autism targets and AD of 5.01% (CI: from 1.17–8.69) and p-value of 0.008 for all neurodevelopmental disorder targets. In contrast, the control sets of genes show no significant difference of IID rates between affected and unaffected siblings. As with de novo CNVs, we see no signal from indels in the peripheral regions (See Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table 1).
We similarly examined the role of de novo intercoding intronic substitutions (ISB) using IGSB for normalization. We saw no signal for de novo ISB in either the overall intronic space or the introns of target genes. Given the enormous background of neutral substitutions, loss of signal from functional mutation is not surprising. We quantified our expectations by power calculations. The likelihood of observing statistically significant signal (p-value < = 0.05) from intronic substitutions in the intronic space is shown in Supplementary Figure 7, for all gene targets and for all candidate target genes. The power to detect a 5% contribution in a collection the size of the SSC is nearly 0 overall and around 0.2 for the autism target genes. Yet, it is unlikely that de novo intronic small indels contribute to the disorder while de novo intronic substitutions do not: the former cause greater damage per event, but the latter are about ten times more common.
We searched for mechanism of action by exploring the properties of the de novo INDs and ISBs that could reasonably be hypothesized to be contributory. These properties were: lengths of the indel; lengths of the affected introns; the proximity of the indel site to consensus splice sites; the degree of conservation at the mutated site; and the likelihood of creation of a new splice site. We also searched by the length of the largest open reading frame at the indel site, which might indicate the mutation affected an unannotated exon. We associated all de novo intronic events (both indels and substitutions) with each of the above properties, and then asked if the distributions of these properties differed significantly among subsets of the de novo events. These subsets were determined by mutation type (indel or substitution), the affected status of the child, and the target gene class (e.g., ‘all genes’ and ‘affected LGD targets’). None of our efforts produced a statistically significant signal. However, some of our observations were in a positive direction. Our entire analysis is reported in the Supplementary Note 2, Supplementary Table 2, and Supplementary Figure 8–34.
Estimating the burden of de novo events to autism
Based on the results we have presented, we make a fresh estimate of the contribution of de novo events to autism incidence. We start with simplex families (Table 6). In this paper we estimate de novo CNVs contribute in ~4% of families. This is slightly lower than previous estimates, but our methods are very conservative. The estimates from LGDs (9%) and from missense (12%) derive from previous studies. These sum to 25%. In the present work we estimate the contribution from de novo CNVs and de novo small indels in the intronic space to be ~1% and ~5% respectively. Based on the study by An, et al.21 we further estimate contributory de novo promoter events at about 7%. The projected contributions from these two noncoding sources sum to 13%, about half the projected 25% contribution from coding events. Together they total 38%. We have not tallied certain de novo variant types, such as de novo intronic substitutions and structural variants at highly repetitive regions of the genome. Moreover, parental29,30 and somatic31–33 mosaicisms are often excluded in searches for de novo events. Thus, we may have underestimated contribution from de novo events. Given all the uncertainty, we estimate that de novo events of all types contribute to 35–45% of autism from simplex families.
Table 6.
Type | Contribution |
---|---|
•Coding | 25% |
∘CNV | 4% |
∘LGD2 | 9% |
∘missense2 | 12% |
•Non-coding | 13% |
∘Intronic | 6% |
·CNV | 1% |
small indels | 5% |
∘promoter21 | 7% |
Total | 38% |
The contributions to autism from simplex families of the various types of de novo coding and non-coding events are estimated from the literature or from this work. Contributions from de novo CNVs derive from this work. The contribution from small indels within introns is based on this work. The other contributions are estimates from the literature cited.
Next, we estimate the contribution of de novo variants separately for the high-risk and low-risk families. We can approximate the de novo contribution in the high-risk families, H, as equal to our estimate of the contribution in multiplex families. Based on our findings that the contribution in multiplex families is about a quarter of the contribution in simplex families (Table 3), and given our estimate of the contribution in simplex families, S = 35–45%, we estimate that H = 9–11%. The contribution to low-risk families, L, can then be computed using the estimates for the proportion of simplex families of low (pLs = 0.6) and high (pHs = 0.4) risk we made in Ronemus, et al.15. From the following equation:
4 |
we arrive at the estimate of the contribution to the low-risk families L = 52–67%.
Finally, using the de novo contributions in low (52–67%) and high-risk (9–11%) families and the estimate that half of autism cases originate from low-risk families and half from high-risk families14,15, we compute that the de novo contribution to overall autism is 0.5*(52–67%) + 0.5*(9–11%), or 30–39%. We arrive at a similar estimate using a second, more direct way for calculating the de novo contribution to autism overall that depends on the empirical measure of the proportion of autism cases in simplex and multiplex families. The growing SPARK collection provides such an estimate. In release 4 of the registered families there are 13,775 affected children from 6,505 multiplex families and 65,184 children from simplex families, giving us proportions of 0.17 (13,775 / (13,775 + 65,184)) cases in multiplex families and 0.83 cases in simplex cases. Combining these proportions with estimated de novo contributions in simplex and multiplex cases we arrive at 0.83 * 35–45% + 0.17 * 9–11% = 30.5–39.1%.
Discussion
In our prior work14,15 we presented a simple model (the unified hypothesis) connecting de novo mutation and transmission in which we postulated high and low-risk autism families. The model explained the observation we made from analysis of multiplex families that the chance for autism is nearly 50% in a male born into a family with two or more prior children with autism14. This latter observation was confirmed by others using independent collections13, and is now confirmed in the latest release of the SPARK collection comprising more than 70,000 families with autism (work in progress). The model also explained our observation, based on a small study, that causal de novo mutation was relatively absent in multiplex families1. Unlike our observation of sibling risk in multiplex families, this result was contradicted in some other studies5,7. We felt these contradictory studies were flawed, having failed to take into account genetic drift of cells in culture. We resolve this question in the current study.
Unlike the samples of the SSC, in which DNA derives directly from blood, the DNA of many autism sample collections, including much of the AGRE, derive from cultured EBV infected lymphocytes. We demonstrated here that DNAs from some of the cultured cells of AGRE, though not all, display far greater cell-line genetic drift than seen in blood derived DNAs (see Fig. 1). Our results confirm the recent parallel study by Ruzzo et al.18 on the same data set. They too seek to remove artifacts of drift, although we use different methods. They filtered individual events, using machine learning techniques, while we filtered samples. The drift in samples creates the appearance of large numbers of de novo events in those samples, both substitutions and copy number, and we eliminated all events in such samples. When such samples are removed, the remaining samples from AGRE match the de novo variant characteristics for frequency and allele bias that we see in the SCC (Fig. 1).
With noisy samples removed, we observe, as did Ruzzo, et al.18, that the incidence of de novo LGDs in autism from multiplex families is significantly lower than the incidence in autism from simplex families (p-value: 0.001). In addition, we similarly observe a significant lower incidence for de novo CNVs that target genes (p-value: 0.0007). By combining these mutational types into one class, we then observed less contribution from de novo events in AGRE than in SSC affecteds, with a p-value of 10−5. These results therefore confirm our initial observations, based on the AGRE collection when it was still new, that causal de novo events are more prevalent in the simplex than in multiplex autism.
Yet we do see a higher incidence of de novo LGD and CNVs targeting genes in multiplex affected compared to the simplex unaffected. Neither alone has statistical difference. However, if we aggregate de novo CNVs and LGDs we find an elevated incidence with a p-value of 0.062. Here we differ from Ruzzo, et al.18 who examined only de novo LGDs in the same population and found no difference in incidence. Using the simplex unaffected as baseline, the ascertainment differential (AD) of de novo LGDs and CNVs targeting genes in AGRE are each about one quarter the AD for simplex affected.
WGS data enables us to directly examine the role of de novo variants in the parts of the genome that are non-coding. Although others have seen evidence of target in the promotor regions of genes using specialized search tools, in the absence of such tools we see no signal in the intergenic regions. However, our analysis does find evidence of a role for de novo CNVs in introns (p-value 0.0068, Table 4) and de novo small indels in introns of known neuro-developmental risk genes or the smaller subset of autism candidate genes (p-values 0.008, and 0.026, respectively, Table 5). We see no evidence for a role for de novo substitutions in this area, but this is expected from power considerations: there are vastly more substitutions than small indels or CNVs, and each of the substitutions would be of less impact than indels or CNVs (Supplementary Figure 7).
The ascertainment differential from CNVs is 0.80% and from small indels is 5.01%, yielding a combined AD of about 6% in simplex autism. Although, due to a limited power, we cannot observe a contribution from nucleotide substitutions in introns in the SSC population, we speculate that the contribution from substitutions within introns is of similar magnitude to that of the intronic small indels, or about 5%. Larger datasets would be needed to provide an empirically derived estimates from this source.
The extent to which introns were targets for disrupting de novo mutation, which we estimated at one half of the rate of disruptions in coding regions, surprised us. Given the complexities of splicing and gene regulation it is perhaps not a surprise that we saw no evidence for a predominant molecular mechanism. To understand these mutations better will require a case by case study of the RNA expression in the candidate cells, a study which is possible because almost the entire SSC cohort is also represented as immortalized cells.
We have made preliminary estimates of the contribution of de novo events to children with autism from low-risk (52–67%) and high-risk (9–11%) families, and to autism overall (30–39%). It is important to emphasize “preliminary” because, with the possible exception of de novo LGDs and CNVs in simplex collections, all the components used to make these estimates are not robust. Improved estimates would require many more families, more WGS on quads, and improved demographic data. However, 30–39% is possibly an underestimate, as it does not fully take into account mutations in highly repetitive regions, many other kinds of structural rearrangements, somatic mutations and parental mosaicism. Indeed, consideration of parental age and autism incidence would be more consistent with a substantially higher number (Taylor, et al.34 and studies in progress). Our estimates do not conflict with estimates of the proportion of families in which genetic transmission contributes. These latter estimates range variously from 40 to 90%16,35–37. As multiple mechanisms (de novo, transmission, epigenetic, and environmental) will contribute in many families, the sum of the proportionate contributions from each source over the entire population will exceed 100%.
Materials and methods
Whole genome datasets
The whole genome sequencing data for both the Simons Simplex Collection (SSC) and the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange (AGRE) were generated at the New York Genome Center. One lane of 150 paired-end reads on the Illumina 10X instrument was utilized per genome, achieving a depth of coverage of ~30X. We selected both affected and unaffected children from 1,878 of the SSC quad families (comprised of mother, father, an affected and an unaffected child) and 2,208 children (2,131 affected and 77 unaffected) from 859 nuclear families from AGRE for which we were able to identify both de novo small-scale and CNV variants. 279 of the AGRE children had DNAs extracted from whole-blood and the remainder had DNA extracted from LCLs. 1,869 affected and 1,874 unaffected children from the SSC and 1,107 affected and 36 unaffected children from AGRE passed the cell-line genetic drift filters. Supplementary Data 1 lists all children assessed in the manuscript. Since the study was limited to previously existing coded data, the CSHL IRB designated this study as “Not human subjects research” and therefore exempt from review.
De novo substitutions and indels methods
We identified de novo substitutions and de novo short indels using the multinomial genotyper we have described previously11 and used extensively in our analysis of whole exome sequencing data from the Simons Simplex Collection2,15,26. To accommodate the lower coverage in the whole genome sequencing data, we set the filters for acceptable de novo candidate to denovoScr ≥70 and chi2Score > 0.001. The identified de novo variants in the cell-line genetic drift-free trios are listed in Supplementary Data 2 and Supplementary Data 3 for the SSC and AGRE, respectively.
De novo CNV pipeline
We assembled a pipeline for the identifying de novo CNVs based on two different methods, “EWT de novo CNV finder” and “HMM de novo CNV finder,” both developed in house. Both methods analyze whole genome sequence data from a population of individuals jointly to deal with sample and regional biases, but are otherwise conceptually different. We combine the candidate de novo variants from the two methods and apply further stringent filters to guard against cryptic transmission. Detailed description of the de novo CNV pipeline components is presented in Supplementary Note 1, Supplementary Fig. 35, and Supplementary Data 6. We applied de novo CNV pipeline separately over the SSC and AGRE data and the final list of the de novo CNVs in the cell-line genetic drift-free children is presented Supplementary Data 4.
We compared our list of detected de novo CNVs to published de novo CNVs in the children we examined from SSC (Sanders, et al.6) and AGRE (Leppa, et al.7). We detected all 17 of the de novo CNVs reported in Leppa et al. and these are included in our analysis. We also detected all but 6 (likely due to differences in the sample labels) of the 204 de novo CNVs reported in Sanders et al. We also excluded 75 additional of the Sanders et al. de novo CNVs due to mosaicism detected in the parents, to polymorphisms in the parental population at levels above what our stringed filters allowed, or to children that did not meet our cell-line genetic drift filters. Supplementary Data 7 lists the Sanders et al. events not included in the current analysis and the reasons they were excluded. Supplementary Data 4, that lists the de novo CNV included in our analysis, indicates which of the de novo CNVs have been previously reported and clearly demonstrates that the WGS data enabled us to identify substantial number of new de novo CNVs smaller than resolution of the previous methods.
Power to detect de novo CNVs
In our analysis, we compared rates for de novo CNVs across different groups children from the two collections, AGRE and SSC. Although the sequence data from the two collections had similar quality and coverage, it was essential to know if the small differences that existed affected the power to detect de novo CNVs. We used a simulation-based test for measuring the power to detect de novo CNVs from whole-genome data for a collection of trios.
The procedure used the bin values (the csb values described in Supplementary Note 1) generated from the “EWT de novo CNV finder” method and specifically tested the power to detect de novo deletions in male children using the EWT de novo CNV finder. We applied the test for deletions of 10 different sizes from 1 kb to 10 kb in three groups of children: the affected and unaffected males from SSC, and the affected males from AGRE. In each case, we simulated 10,000 autosomal deletions and checked how many of the simulated deletions could be detected by the EWT de novo CNV finder. Each simulated de novo deletion was generated using the following steps. First, we chose a random male trio; a random region of the given size on the X-chromosome that did not overlap with the pseudoautosomal regions, telomeric or centromeric regions; and a random region of the same size on one of the autosomes that did not overlap with telomeric and centromeric regions. We then took the real bin value matrix for the selected chromosome for all trios for the test groups of children (male and female), and replaced the bin values for the male child of the selected trio with the bin values from the selected X-chromosome region for the same child. The updated matrix was fed to the EWT de novo CNV finder. Finally, for each simulated deletion we checked if the selected autosomal region overlapped with any of the candidate de novo deletions for the selected child generated by the finder. See Supplementary Figure 4 for results.
Power to detect de novo substitutions given different coverages
We measured the power to detect de novo substitutions separately for each trio from the SSC and AGRE collection (Supplementary Fig. 2 and Supplementary Data 1). For every trio we selected 10,000 random genomic positions and obtained the number of reads covering each of these positions in the mother, father, and the child (coverage). We then simulated a count data for a de novo mutation. Parents had counts for the reference allele equal to the observed coverage and 0 reads supporting the alternative allele. We used a random value from a binomial distribution Binomial (p = 0.47, N = coverage for the child) to obtain a count for the alternative allele for the child. The count for the reference allele was that set to the difference of the child’s coverage and the count for the alternative allele. The resulting 2 (reference and alternative allele) by 3 (mother, father, child) count matrix was fed to the multinomial genotyper. The proportion of the 10,000 positions successfully classified as a de novo variant by the multinomial genotyper was used as an estimate of the power to detect SNV in for the trio.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We are grateful for the useful discussions with Robert B Darnell, Kuan-Ting Lin, Adrian R. Krainer, Tuuli Lappalainen and Mike Zody. We thank all the families at the participating SSC sites, as well as the principal investigators (A. L. Beaudet, R. Bernier, J. Constantino, E. H. Cook, Jr., E. Fombonne, D. Geschwind, D. E. Grice, A. Klin, D. H. Ledbetter, C. Lord, C. L. Martin, D. M. Martin, R. Maxim, J. Miles, O. Ousley, B. Peterson, J. Piggot, C. Saulnier, M. W. State, W. Stone, J. S. Sutcliffe, C. A. Walsh, and E. Wijsman) and the coordinators and staff at the SSC sites for the recruitment and comprehensive assessment of simplex families, and the Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative (SFARI) staff for facilitating access to the SSC. This work was supported by the Simons Center for Quantitative Biology at Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, SFARI Grants SF497800 (to M.W. and I.I.), SF529232, SF677963, SF666590 (to I.I.), the Centers for Common Disease Genomics grant (UM1 HG008901). The Centers for Common Disease Genomics are funded by the National Human Genome Research Institute and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute.
Author contributions
S.Y., A.M., M.W. and I.I. conceived the study; S.Y., A.M., A.M.K., A.B., M.W. and I.I. analyzed the data. S.Y., A.M., K.K.B., M.W. and I.I. interpreted the results. A.M.K., A.B., K.P., D.L. and I.I. developed the computational methods. B.Y., Y.L., P.A., S.M., C.R., L.W. performed computational work and data management. Z.W. performed experimental work. S.Y., A.M., M.R., K.K.B., M.W. and I.I. wrote the manuscript.
Data availability
We obtained existing whole-genome sequence datasets for the Simons Simplex Collection (SSC) and the AGRE collections that have already been used in published manuscripts18,21,25. The SSC whole-genome sequence data can be obtained from Simons Foundation Research Initiative (SFARI; https://www.sfari.org). The AGRE whole-genome sequence data can be obtained from the Hartwell Foundation’s Autism Research and Technology Initiative (iHART; http://www.ihart.org). Access to these resources is subject to approval by the respective institutions. We used these whole-genome sequencing data to identify de novo substitutions, de novo small indels, and de novo CNV using the methods described above and in Supplementary Note 1. We provide the complete lists of children and identified de novo variants together with all attributes used in our analysis in Supplementary Data 1–5.
Code availability
The source code we used in our analysis is included in the public GitHub repository https://github.com/iossifovlab/denovoInHighAndLowRiskPaper. In addition, we provide the input data for these scripts together with the results of executing the scripts over the provided input. The input is a copy of the Supplementary Data associated with this manuscript, and we include it in the source code repository for convenience. Thus, interested readers can: (1) study the code and the complete set of results that we discuss in the manuscript, (2) re-run the scripts on the provided input to reproduce precisely our analysis, and (3) use the scripts over input related to their work to perform similar analyses. The official release (v1.0) of the repository can be accessed through 10.5281/zenodo.5137986.
Competing interests
The authors declare the following competing interests: Kristin K. Baldwin is a co-founder of Certego Therapeutics. The remaining authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Peer review informationCommunications Biology thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editors: Jung-Eun Lee and George Inglis.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Seungtai Yoon, Adriana Munoz.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s42003-021-02533-z.
References
- 1.Sebat J, et al. Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Science. 2007;316:445–449. doi: 10.1126/science.1138659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Iossifov I, et al. The contribution of de novo coding mutations to autism spectrum disorder. Nature. 2014;515:216–221. doi: 10.1038/nature13908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Levy D, et al. Rare de novo and transmitted copy-number variation in autistic spectrum disorders. Neuron. 2011;70:886–897. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sanders SJ, et al. Multiple recurrent de novo CNVs, including duplications of the 7q11.23 Williams syndrome region, are strongly associated with autism. Neuron. 2011;70:863–885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pinto D, et al. Convergence of genes and cellular pathways dysregulated in autism spectrum disorders. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2014;94:677–694. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.03.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sanders SJ, et al. Insights into autism spectrum disorder genomic architecture and biology from 71 risk loci. Neuron. 2015;87:1215–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Leppa VM, et al. Rare inherited and de novo CNVs reveal complex contributions to ASD risk in multiplex families. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2016;99:540–554. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2016.06.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Neale BM, et al. Patterns and rates of exonic de novo mutations in autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2012;485:242–245. doi: 10.1038/nature11011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.O’Roak BJ, et al. Sporadic autism exomes reveal a highly interconnected protein network of de novo mutations. Nature. 2012;485:246–250. doi: 10.1038/nature10989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Sanders SJ, et al. De novo mutations revealed by whole-exome sequencing are strongly associated with autism. Nature. 2012;485:237–241. doi: 10.1038/nature10945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Iossifov I, et al. De novo gene disruptions in children on the autistic spectrum. Neuron. 2012;74:285–299. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.De Rubeis S, et al. Synaptic, transcriptional and chromatin genes disrupted in autism. Nature. 2014;515:209–215. doi: 10.1038/nature13772. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ozonoff S, et al. Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics. 2011;128:e488–e495. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-2825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Zhao X, et al. A unified genetic theory for sporadic and inherited autism. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:12831–12836. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705803104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ronemus M, Iossifov I, Levy D, Wigler M. The role of de novo mutations in the genetics of autism spectrum disorders. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2014;15:133–141. doi: 10.1038/nrg3585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gaugler T, et al. Most genetic risk for autism resides with common variation. Nat. Genet. 2014;46:881–885. doi: 10.1038/ng.3039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Grove J, et al. Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Genet. 2019;51:431–444. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ruzzo EK, et al. Inherited and de novo genetic risk for autism impacts shared networks. Cell. 2019;178:850–866 e826. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.07.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Turner TN, et al. Genome sequencing of autism-affected families reveals disruption of putative noncoding regulatory DNA. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2016;98:58–74. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2015.11.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Turner TN, et al. Genomic patterns of de novo mutation in simplex autism. Cell. 2017;171:710–722 e712. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.08.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.An, J. Y. et al. Genome-wide de novo risk score implicates promoter variation in autism spectrum disorder. Science362, 10.1126/science.aat6576 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 22.Werling DM, et al. An analytical framework for whole-genome sequence association studies and its implications for autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Genet. 2018;50:727–736. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0107-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Munoz, A. et al. De novo indels within introns contribute to ASD incidence. bioRxiv, 10.1101/137471 (2017).
- 24.Yuen RK, et al. Genome-wide characteristics of de novo mutations in autism. NPJ Genom. Med. 2016;1:160271–1602710. doi: 10.1038/npjgenmed.2016.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Zhou J, et al. Whole-genome deep-learning analysis identifies contribution of noncoding mutations to autism risk. Nat. Genet. 2019;51:973–980. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0420-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Iossifov I, et al. Low load for disruptive mutations in autism genes and their biased transmission. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:E5600–E5607. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1516376112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.O’Roak BJ, et al. Multiplex targeted sequencing identifies recurrently mutated genes in autism spectrum disorders. Science. 2012;338:1619–1622. doi: 10.1126/science.1227764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Turner TN, et al. denovo-db: a compendium of human de novo variants. Nucleic Acids Res. 2017;45:D804–D811. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Myers CT, et al. Parental mosaicism in “De Novo” epileptic encephalopathies. N. Engl. J. Med. 2018;378:1646–1648. doi: 10.1056/NEJMc1714579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Breuss, M. W. et al. Quantification of autism recurrence risk by direct assessment of paternal sperm mosaicism. bioRxiv (2017).
- 31.D’Gama AM, et al. Targeted DNA sequencing from autism spectrum disorder brains implicates multiple genetic mechanisms. Neuron. 2015;88:910–917. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Freed D, Pevsner J. The contribution of mosaic variants to autism spectrum disorder. PLoS Genet. 2016;12:e1006245. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Krupp DR, et al. Exonic mosaic mutations contribute risk for autism spectrum disorder. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2017;101:369–390. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2017.07.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Taylor JL, et al. Paternal-age-related de novo mutations and risk for five disorders. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:3043. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11039-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Klei L, et al. Common genetic variants, acting additively, are a major source of risk for autism. Mol. Autism. 2012;3:9. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-3-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, C. et al. Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nat. Genet. 2013;45:984–994. doi: 10.1038/ng.2711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sandin S, et al. The heritability of autism spectrum disorder. Jama. 2017;318:1182–1184. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.12141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.O’Leary NA, et al. Reference sequence (RefSeq) database at NCBI: current status, taxonomic expansion, and functional annotation. Nucleic Acids Res. 2016;44:D733–D745. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkv1189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
We obtained existing whole-genome sequence datasets for the Simons Simplex Collection (SSC) and the AGRE collections that have already been used in published manuscripts18,21,25. The SSC whole-genome sequence data can be obtained from Simons Foundation Research Initiative (SFARI; https://www.sfari.org). The AGRE whole-genome sequence data can be obtained from the Hartwell Foundation’s Autism Research and Technology Initiative (iHART; http://www.ihart.org). Access to these resources is subject to approval by the respective institutions. We used these whole-genome sequencing data to identify de novo substitutions, de novo small indels, and de novo CNV using the methods described above and in Supplementary Note 1. We provide the complete lists of children and identified de novo variants together with all attributes used in our analysis in Supplementary Data 1–5.
The source code we used in our analysis is included in the public GitHub repository https://github.com/iossifovlab/denovoInHighAndLowRiskPaper. In addition, we provide the input data for these scripts together with the results of executing the scripts over the provided input. The input is a copy of the Supplementary Data associated with this manuscript, and we include it in the source code repository for convenience. Thus, interested readers can: (1) study the code and the complete set of results that we discuss in the manuscript, (2) re-run the scripts on the provided input to reproduce precisely our analysis, and (3) use the scripts over input related to their work to perform similar analyses. The official release (v1.0) of the repository can be accessed through 10.5281/zenodo.5137986.