Abstract
Previous research has demonstrated that stress modulates the competitive interaction between the hippocampus and striatum, two structures known to be critically involved in motor sequence learning. These earlier investigations, however, have largely focused on blood oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) responses. No study to date has examined the link between stress, motor learning and levels of striatal and hippocampal gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA). This knowledge gap is surprising given the known role of GABA in neuroplasticity subserving learning and memory. The current study thus examined: a) the effects of motor learning and stress on striatal and hippocampal GABA levels; and b) how learning- and stress-induced changes in GABA relate to the neural correlates of learning. To do so, fifty-three healthy young adults were exposed to a stressful or non-stressful control intervention before motor sequence learning. Striatal and hippocampal GABA levels were assessed at baseline and post-intervention/learning using magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Regression analyses indicated that stress modulated the link between striatal GABA levels and functional plasticity in both the hippocampus and striatum during learning as measured with fMRI. This study provides evidence for a role of GABA in the stress-induced modulation of striatal and hippocampal systems.
Subject terms: Learning and memory, Motor control
Using fMRI and MRS measurements from healthy, young adults, Dolfen et al examined the effects of motor learning and stress on striatal and hippocampal GABA levels, as well as effects on neural correlates of learning. They demonstrated that stress modulated the link between striatal GABA levels and functional plasticity in the hippocampus and striatum, which reveals a role for GABA in the stress-induced modulation of these systems.
Introduction
There is a plethora of evidence that both the hippocampus (HC) and striatum (STR) are involved in motor sequence learning (MSL)1–10. During the acquisition of new movement sequences, the pattern of activation in these regions is antagonistic, such that activation increases in the STR and decreases in the HC as a function of learning (e.g., see refs. 7,9–11). Importantly, these particular dynamics during encoding have been linked to successful motor learning and subsequent motor memory retention1,9,10,12. As these neural signatures are critical for the learning and memory process, recent research has started to examine whether these brain responses can be altered through experimental interventions (e.g., see refs. 13–16).
An intervention that has shown promise to modify the relative engagement of hippocampal and striatal systems during learning is acute stress. Specifically, previous studies indicate that stress boosts striatal activation at the expense of hippocampal functioning during spatial navigation and probabilistic classification learning17–21. This shift increases striatal-dependent habitual control of learning (thought to be adaptive to deal with acute stress) but comes at the cost of flexibility of learning, supported by the HC. Recent research in our group extended these findings with evidence that stress prior to MSL favours the recruitment of motor cortical regions, known to be highly connected to the STR22, and results in a stronger disengagement of the hippocampal system during learning13. Moreover, our findings suggested that inter-subject variability in the brain responses to stress in these regions determines the impact of stress on motor learning and memory retention.
These prior studies provided critical insights into how learning and stress alter blood oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) responses in cortico-hippocampal and cortico-striatal networks. However, the exact neurochemical substrates supporting these processes are unknown. This knowledge gap is surprising given the critical role of neurometabolites in learning and memory processes. In the study of neuroplasticity associated with motor learning, the main inhibitory neurotransmitter γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) has received considerable attention. Animal models examining the role of GABA in learning-related motor cortical plasticity indicate that long-term potentiation-like synaptic changes are associated with decreases in GABA and a release of inhibition23,24. In line with this, sensorimotor GABA levels in humans, measured non-invasively with magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS), prior to motor learning and responsiveness of motor cortical GABA to non-invasive brain stimulation have been found to be predictive of subsequent motor behaviour25,26. Furthermore, previous studies in humans have demonstrated decreases in sensorimotor GABA as a result of learning27–29. These data collectively demonstrate the importance of GABA physiology in the context of motor learning. Surprisingly, although the role of the STR and the HC in motor learning is well documented, the physiology of striatal and hippocampal GABA has never been studied in this context.
Investigations into how stress influences GABA in learning- and memory-relevant structures, including the STR and HC, are relatively scarce. A hallmark of the physical stress response is the release of corticosteroids (cortisol in humans) whose actions are influenced by mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors in the brain30. These receptors are expressed in all brain areas but with an increased density in the excitatory non-GABAergic principal cells of the HC. Previous work has therefore primarily focused on stress-induced modulation of non-GABAergic mechanisms in the HC (refs. 18,19,31,32; for reviews, see refs. 20,33). Accordingly, the effect of stress on functioning of GABAergic hippocampal interneurons is less understood. Nevertheless, in line with evidence of deleterious effects of stress on hippocampal functioning (see refs. 34,35 for reviews), previous work in rodents reported increased GABA levels in the HC after stress exposure36 (see ref. 37 for a potential mechanism of this change). Similar to the HC, the effect of stress on striatal GABA physiology remains scarcely described. The limited work in animals documented both stress-induced decreases38–40 as well as increases in striatal GABA41. In the human brain, however, studies examining stress-induced changes in GABA levels are limited to the prefrontal cortex42,43. Hence, reports on hippocampal and striatal regions are currently lacking.
Given that GABA plays a key role in motor learning25,27,29 and stress alters learning-related responses in the STR and the HC17–21, we investigated how stress and motor learning alter GABA levels in the HC and the STR. Moreover, we examined the relationship between GABA and BOLD signals across the STR and HC. Given that previous research assessing activity and connectivity from BOLD images has demonstrated that a competitive interaction between these two regions is crucial for motor memory processes44, investigations into how GABA in one region influences BOLD in the other will foster a greater understanding of the nature of this interplay during learning. In this study, participants were exposed to a stressful (Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test, SECPT45,46) or non-stressful control intervention prior to performing a MSL task while functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data were acquired. GABA levels were assessed using MRS in the HC and STR before the intervention, as well as following MSL. As acute stress has been shown to reduce hippocampal activation13,17,47,48, stress is hypothesized to increase GABAergic inhibition in the HC and to result in higher hippocampal GABA levels at the end of training in the stress as compared to the control group. Based on the diverse population of GABAergic neurons in the STR, striatal GABA measures presumably reflect activity of GABAergic interneurons and, when activated, e.g., during motor learning, activity of GABAergic (principal) projection neurons49,50. Given that stress boosts activation in motor cortical regions during MSL13 and these regions have massive glutamatergic projections to striatal GABAergic projection neurons51–53, we hypothesized stress to potentiate activity of these GABAergic neurons during learning. Therefore, we expected stress to result in increased GABA release in the STR and hence higher GABA levels post learning in the stress as compared to the control group. Last, we expected learning and stress-related changes in GABA levels to be related to task-related BOLD signals in the STR and HC (e.g., see ref. 54). In particular, based on the known interplay between striatal and hippocampal regions during MSL (e.g., see ref. 1), we predicted that GABA levels in one memory systems would relate to BOLD signal in the other memory system.
Results
In the current study, participants were trained on a MSL task during fMRI (Fig. 1). Prior to MSL, subjects were exposed to either a stress (SECPT) or control intervention. MRS data were acquired at baseline (i.e., pre-intervention) and post intervention/learning. In line with our previous work13,55 and given the critical role of glucocorticoids in the impact of stress on learning and memory20,56, the primary group comparison presented in the main text focused on controls and stressed participants with an increase in cortisol (i.e., stress cortisol responders, SCRs) (see “Methods”).
Effectiveness of stress induction
To measure the effectiveness of the stress induction by the SECPT, subjective and physiological responses were repeatedly measured during the experiment (Fig. 1). Subjective and autonomic responses to the intervention are summarized in Table 1. With respect to the subjective response to stress, the SECPT was rated as significantly more stressful, unpleasant and painful as compared to the control manipulation (unpaired two-sample t-tests, Control vs. SCR, all ps < 0.001). Both blood pressure and heart rate significantly increased in response to the SECPT but not in response to the control intervention [3 (Time: pre vs. during vs. post) × 2 (Groups) repeated-measures (RM) analyses of variance (ANOVAs); Time × Group interaction: all Fs ≥ 8.573, ηp2 ≥ 0.144, all ps < 0.001; see Table 1 for between-group comparisons for each time point). Finally, a 6 (Time) × 2 (Groups) RM ANOVA on cortisol concentration (nmol/l) revealed a significant Time × Group interaction [F(2.303, 112.865) = 12.239, ηp2 = 0.153, p < 0.001]. As shown in Fig. 2a, the SECPT triggered an endocrine response, which resulted in significantly elevated cortisol concentrations in the SCR as compared to the control group at T25’ (p < 0.001) and T60’ (p < 0.001). As the cortisol concentration in controls was higher at the start of the baseline MRS measurements (B1) as compared to MRS post intervention/learning (T60) (p = 0.009) (Fig. 2a), we performed correlational analyses to investigate the link between cortisol concentrations at the start of each MRS time point and corresponding GABA+ measures. These follow-up analyses revealed no significant correlations (all psuncorr ≥ 0.20).
Table 1.
Subjective ratingsa | |||
Pain | Stress | Unpleasantness | |
Control | 1.39 ± 4.79 | 1.26 ± 4.34 | 1.1 ± 4.33 |
SCR | 68.15 ± 21.07 | 55.47 ± 24.9 | 88.87 ± 12.13 |
Control vs. SCR | p < 0.001 | p < 0.001 | p < .001 |
Autonomic responsesb | |||
Pre | During | Post | |
SBP (mm Hg) | |||
Control | 122.19 ± 10.45 | 120.85 ± 9.50 | 120.37 ± 10.05 |
SCR | 130.00 ± 13.33 | 144.54 ± 20.38 | 133.19 ± 13.63 |
Control vs. SCR | p = 0.021 | p < 0.001 | p < 0.001 |
DBP (mm Hg) | |||
Control | 74.67 ± 6.65 | 73.96 ± 5.81 | 74.07 ± 5.59 |
SCR | 75.08 ± 9.16 | 90.92 ± 17.65 | 83.54 ± 14.76 |
Control vs. SCR | p = 0.852 | p < 0.001 | p = 0.003 |
HR (bpm) | |||
Control | 64.48 ± 8.79 | 66.44 ± 9.05 | 68.19 ± 9.23 |
SCR | 63.42 ± 8.87 | 87.65 ± 17.10 | 71.27 ± 11.82 |
Control vs. SCR | p = 0.664 | p < 0.001 | p = 0.294 |
Values are means ± SDs. N control group = 27; N SCR group = 26. Subjective ratings were given on a 100 mm visual analogue scale.
Bpm beats per minute, DBP diastolic blood pressure, HR heart rate, SBP systolic blood pressure, SCR stress cortisol responders.
ap-Values based on unpaired two-sample t-tests.
bp-Values based on pairwise comparisons following RM ANOVAs with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. It is noteworthy that during the SECPT, the measurement pre-feet submersion is taken within the stressful context (including video monitoring), which likely contributed to the group difference in SBP observed at baseline.
Performance on the MSL task
MSL consisted of 20 blocks of practice followed by an immediate post test (after a 2 min break) of 4 practice blocks, in order to minimize the confounding effect of fatigue on end-training performance57. Motor performance was measured in terms of speed (mean inter-response interval between two consecutive correct key presses in s) and accuracy (% of correct transitions). It is noteworthy that as performance accuracy remained stable with low error rates (see Supplementary Results section 2.5), the analyses presented in the main text focused on performance speed. A 20 (Blocks of practice) × 2 (Groups. SCR vs. Control) RM ANOVA on performance speed during training revealed that participants became faster with practice [Block: F(4.269, 217.735) = 61.117, ηp2 = 0.545, p < 0.001]. Performance improvement was not statistically different between groups [Group: F(1,51) = 1.399, ηp2 = 0.027, p = 0.242; Block × Group: F(4.269, 217.735) = 0.566, ηp2 = 0.011, p = 0.699] (Fig. 2b). Similar results were obtained for the immediate post test [4 × 2 RM ANOVA; Block: F(3, 153) = 2.944, ηp2 = 0.055, p = 0.035; Group: F(1,51) = 0.817, ηp2 = 0.016, p = 0.370; Block × Group: F(3,153) = 1.699, ηp2 = 0.032, p = 0.170]. In summary, and consistent with our previous work13,55, stress did not modulate performance during MSL.
MRS of GABA
GABA-edited MRS of the left STR and the left HC was performed before the intervention (baseline) and immediately after MSL (post intervention/learning) (Fig. 1). Data quality metrics and MRS voxel tissue fractions for each region of interest and corresponding analyses are detailed in the Supplementary Results (Section 2.6 and Supplementary Table 3). The consistency of voxel placement was high for both regions, as shown by the heatmaps in Fig. 3a that depict spatial overlap of the MRS voxels. STR and HC MRS spectra are depicted in Fig. 3b, c.
To investigate the effect of stress/learning on GABA+ levels, for each region, a 2 (Time: baseline vs. post) × 2 (Groups: SCR vs. Control) RM ANOVA was conducted. There was no significant effect of group or time on STR GABA+ levels, neither was there a time × group interaction (Fig. 4a, left panel) [Time: F(1,50) = 0.088, ηp2 < 0.001, p = 0.768; Group: F(1,50) = 0.144, ηp2 < 0.001, p = 0.563; Time × Group: F(1,50) = 0.144, ηp2 < 0.001, p = 0.706]. Similarly, the 2 × 2 RM ANOVA performed on HC GABA+ yielded no significant effects [Time: F(1,44) = 0.089, ηp2 = 0.002, p = 0.766; Group: F(1,44) = 0.524, ηp2 = 0.012, p = 0.473; Time × Group: F(1,44) = 0.974, ηp2 = 0.034, p = 0.329] (see Fig. 4a, right panel). Altogether, these results indicate that, at the group level, neither stress (prior to MSL) nor MSL influenced GABA+ levels in the STR and the HC.
BOLD responses during the MSL task
A primary objective of the current research was to relate the STR/HC GABA+ measures to BOLD responses during MSL. Before presenting these data, we report here patterns of task-related brain activation in our regions of interest (ROI) during MSL, independent of the relationship to GABA+. ROIs included bilateral hippocampi and bilateral STR (caudate nuclei and putamen). Corresponding results from the full sample and performed on the whole brain are described in Dolfen et al.13. Contrasts of interest were the main effect of practice and its linear modulation by performance speed during MSL practice. Modulation contrasts identify regions where the magnitude of the BOLD responses increased (or decreased) as speed of performance increases over the course of practice. Results did not show any significant differences in brain activation, in the ROIs between SCR and controls for the main effect of task practice. However, stress altered the pattern of dynamical activation in hippocampal regions during practice (i.e., modulation of brain activation by performance). Specifically, in stressed participants, the right hippocampal activation decreased significantly more in proportion to performance speed as compared to controls ([SCR-Control], (30 -36 -2 mm), T = 3.05, psvc = 0.036) (Fig. 5a). Stress did not modulate striatal dynamical patterns: activation significantly increased as a function of practice in both groups (Fig. 5b and see Supplementary Table 5).
Link between GABA+ and BOLD responses during MSL
We investigated the link between STR/HC GABA+ measures and BOLD responses in our ROIs and, in particular, whether this link was modulated by stress. To do so, we conducted fMRI regression analyses using either baseline GABA+ or ΔGABA+ (i.e., ΔGABA+; computed as raw change in GABA+ from baseline to post intervention/learning; see Fig. 4b) as covariates. For each contrast of interest, we describe any group differences in the relationship between brain activation and STR/HC GABA+ measures. Results of these between-group comparisons are summarized in Table 2 and follow-up within-group regression results are presented in Supplementary Table 6.
Table 2.
Region | X mm | Y mm | Z mm | # Voxels | T | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Regression with STR baseline GABA+ | ||||||
Main effect of practice | ||||||
[Control-SCR] | ||||||
No suprathreshold clusters | ||||||
[SCR-Control] | ||||||
Hippocampus | −18 | −24 | −10 | 31 | 3.13 | 0.031 |
Modulation by speed of performance | ||||||
No suprathreshold clusters | ||||||
Regression with HC baseline GABA+ | ||||||
Main effect of practice | ||||||
[Control-SCR] | ||||||
No suprathreshold clusters | ||||||
[SCR-Control] | ||||||
Hippocampus | 28 | −36 | 0 | 30 | 2.98 | 0.031 |
Modulation by speed of performance | ||||||
No suprathreshold clusters | ||||||
Regression with ΔGABA+ STR | ||||||
Main effect of practice | ||||||
[Control-SCR] | ||||||
Hippocampus | −20 | −14 | −20 | 79 | 3.42 | 0.015 |
[SCR-Control] | ||||||
No suprathreshold clusters | ||||||
Modulation by speed of performance | ||||||
[Control-SCR] | ||||||
No suprathreshold clusters | ||||||
[SCR-Control] | ||||||
Putamen | 20 | 14 | 6 | 218 | 3.39 | 0.016 |
(extending to caudate) | ||||||
Regression with ΔGABA+ HC | ||||||
No suprathreshold clusters |
Significance level set at pcorr < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons (FWE) over small volumes. All results survived Holm–Bonferroni correction for multiple testing. Due to missing fMRI data, one control participant was not included in the regressions with HC GABA+: N control = 23, N SCR = 22. Regressions with STR GABA+: N control = 26, N SCR = 26.
ΔGABA+ post intervention/learning minus baseline, HC hippocampus, STR striatum, SCR stress cortisol responders.
Baseline GABA+ and BOLD responses
We first correlated baseline HC/STR GABA+ levels to the brain activation maps of the main effect of practice. The regression analysis with HC baseline GABA+ showed that stress modulated the link between HC baseline GABA+ levels and right hippocampal BOLD activation during task practice (Fig. 6a). Within-group, follow-up analyses revealed that HC baseline GABA+ was negatively related to hippocampal activation in the control group, i.e., lower HC GABA+ levels were associated with overall higher hippocampal activation, but not in the stress group. A group difference was also found in the link between STR baseline GABA+ and left hippocampal activation (Fig. 6b). Specifically, in the SCR but not in the control group, there was a significant positive relationship between STR baseline GABA+ and left hippocampal activation, such that lower STR baseline GABA+ was associated with overall less hippocampal activation. Next, we correlated baseline GABA+ measures with the individual brain activation maps of modulation by performance. No group differences in the link between HC/STR baseline GABA+ and BOLD modulation were observed in our ROIs.
ΔGABA+ and BOLD responses
Similarly, individual brain activation maps of the main effect of practice were correlated to STR/HC ΔGABA+. The regression with STR ΔGABA+ showed that stress modulated the link between STR ΔGABA+ and left hippocampal activation during MSL (Fig. 7a). Specifically, a larger decrease in STR GABA+ over training in stressed participants was associated with overall higher activation in the left HC. To ensure that our results were specific to GABA+ changes, a partial correlation was computed in the SCR group between STR ΔGABA+ and HC BOLD, while controlling for STR baseline GABA+. Importantly, the relationship in the SCR group remained significant after controlling for STR baseline GABA+ (p = 0.042).
With respect to the link between magnitude of BOLD modulation by performance and ΔGABA+, regression analyses with STR ΔGABA+ yielded group differences in a right striatal cluster including the putamen and caudate (Fig. 7b). In line with our hypotheses, the findings indicated that practice-related increases in STR BOLD during MSL are linked to increases in STR GABA+ from baseline to post in stressed participants. It is noteworthy that the link between STR ΔGABA+ and STR BOLD modulation in the SCR group remained significant when controlling for STR baseline GABA+ (p = 0.025). No group differences in the link between HC ΔGABA+ and brain responses in our ROIs were observed.
Discussion
In the current study, we investigated the effects of stress and motor learning on the levels of striatal and hippocampal GABA, and how they relate to BOLD responses in these regions during MSL. Our results indicate that neither stress nor subsequent learning influenced GABA+ at the group level. Importantly, however, stress altered the link between STR GABA+ levels and task-related activation in the HC. These results provide evidence for a link between GABA and BOLD signals across the HC and STR, and ultimately support the view that GABA plays a role in the modulation of striatal and hippocampal systems under stress.
Stress modulates the link between baseline inhibitory tone and BOLD responses
Our data indicated that the two groups differed with respect to the relationship between baseline hippocampal GABA levels and task-related functional activation in the HC. The role of the HC in MSL is well documented. In line with evidence of hippocampal involvement in spatial information processing (e.g., see refs. 58,59) and associating sequential events (e.g., see ref. 60), hippocampal activation during MSL is described to support the sequential and abstract nature of the to-be-learned material. Furthermore, the HC is crucial for the development of an abstract allocentric representation of the movement sequence and is thought to trigger sleep-dependent memory consolidation processes1. Although previous studies underscore the importance of hippocampal recruitment during motor memory encoding1,9,10,12, the neurochemical mechanisms supporting this remain to be explored in humans. Our findings suggest that low baseline inhibitory tone in the HC, presumably reflecting decreased GABAergic interneuron activity61, was predictive of larger task-related hippocampal activation in the control group. The direction of the relationship is in accordance with a recent study showing a similar inverse link between hippocampal GABA and hippocampal BOLD magnitude during thought suppression62. Importantly, the current study showed that stress disrupted this link, suggesting that inter-subject variability in hippocampal baseline inhibitory tone does not relate to hippocampal functional (BOLD) plasticity under stress.
Interestingly, inter-subject variability in striatal baseline inhibitory tone accounted for differences in the magnitude of hippocampal BOLD responses in the stress but not the control group. Analogous to the HC, baseline striatal GABA levels in this study might reflect striatal GABAergic interneuron activity and, therefore, higher striatal GABA levels can be interpreted as greater GABAergic inhibition on striatal principal neurons63. Importantly, the observation that higher baseline striatal inhibitory tone is related to more hippocampal BOLD activation in stressed participants is in line with the known competitive interplay between striatal and hippocampal systems64. When considering previous evidence of stress-induced decreases in hippocampal functional activation in the same time window17,18,32,65, it’s tempting to speculate that a high baseline striatal inhibitory tone might protect the HC from negative effects of stress. Alternatively, it might suggest that the high striatal inhibitory tone prevents the known stress-induced shift from hippocampal-dependent towards striatal-dependent control over learning17,18 and results in the maintenance of hippocampal activation. It is noteworthy that such interpretations are certainly speculative and we cannot differentiate between these possibilities. Importantly, our results not only show that stress promoted the link between striatal GABA tone and hippocampal responses but also that stress disrupted the link between hippocampal inhibitory tone and hippocampal functional activity. Altogether, these findings suggest a potential role for striatal GABA in hippocampal functional plasticity under stress.
Stress modulates the link between GABA changes and BOLD responses
Under stress, as compared to control conditions, greater learning-related increases in striatal activation were associated with a net increase in striatal GABA levels. Although inferences about neural mechanisms are relatively limited based on MRS, this pattern of results potentially suggests that our post GABA measurement in the STR might be driven by changes in GABA release from projection neurons. This possibility is indeed highly likely when considering the local neuronal population in the STR that encompasses both GABAergic inhibitory interneurons and GABA releasing principal neurons. The small group of GABAergic interneurons exert control over the principal GABAergic projection neurons, which account for 74% of the striatal neuronal population66. Importantly, previous work in rodents has shown that these striatal GABAergic projection neurons are, in contrast to the striatal inhibitory interneurons, largely inactive at rest and need glutamatergic cortical inputs to discharge52,53. These observations confirm that the baseline striatal GABA levels discussed above might indeed mainly reflect interneuron activity. They also suggest that experimental interventions such as learning/stress might activate the large population of striatal GABAergic projection neurons and therefore induce GABA release. When considering the local neuronal population in the STR, the possibility seems indeed likely that increased striatal activation is paralleled by increased activation of GABAergic projection neurons and thus increases in striatal GABA. Although we did not observe an intervention-related increase in striatal GABA at the group level, the significant positive relationship between net GABA changes and striatal activation found in our stress group is in line with this interpretation. Moreover, stress also modulated the link between changes in striatal GABA levels and hippocampal BOLD responses during task practice. Our results then collectively revealed that, an intervention-related increase in striatal GABA in the stress group is correlated with larger learning-related increases in striatal activation and overall lower hippocampal activation. These results further highlight a stress-induced competition between striatal and hippocampal systems across GABA and BOLD signals. In particular, they are in line with previous literature on the effect of stress on striatal-hippocampal interactions (for a review, see ref. 34).
Stress and the interplay between hippocampal and striatal systems
Accumulating evidence suggests that stress promotes striatal-related behaviour (also called ‘rigid’) at the expense of more ‘flexible’ behaviour supported by the HC (for a review, see ref. 67). This shift has extensively been studied within the context of spatial navigation and procedural classification learning. In both types of learning, the task can be solved by using flexible spatial or allocentric strategies (based on multiple cues/implicit cue patterns) and rigid stimulus-response (S-R) or egocentric learning strategies (based on explicit single cues)67. At the neural level, learning in these tasks has consistently been shown to rely on multiple memory systems. Specifically, the use of rigid S-R strategies depends on the striatal-based procedural memory system68, whereas more spatial or allocentric strategies require the hippocampal-based declarative memory system58,69. fMRI studies show that the stress-induced shift in strategy on the behavioural level is paralleled by a shift at the neural level. Specifically, stress results in decreased hippocampal activation17,18,32,65 and increased striatal activation70. In the current study, we extend these previous findings and showed that GABA levels in the STR are linked to both hippocampal and striatal functional activation under stress. Moreover, in line with evidence of inhibitory connections between these areas, whereby inactivation of the HC enhanced STR-dependent learning71,72 and disruption of the STR facilitates HC-dependent learning73, we observed a similar competitive interaction pattern across BOLD and GABA signals under stress. Evidence that stress affects the relative involvement of striatal and hippocampal networks in learning and favours striatal-dependent processes at the expense of hippocampal-dependent processes (see ref. 34 for a review) further supports such an interaction in the stress rather than in the control group.
Stress, GABA and motor learning
Contrary to our expectations, stress and learning did not induce changes in striatal or hippocampal GABA at the group level. It is worth noting that the lack of a significant effect is in contrast to what has previously been observed in the primary motor cortex (M1). Indeed, MRS studies have provided evidence of disinhibition in sensorimotor regions (i.e., decreases in sensorimotor GABA levels) over the course of motor learning, and MSL in particular27,28. The decrease in inhibition has been described to facilitate learning-related plasticity25. Given the direct glutamatergic projections from M1 to the STR22, one could also have expected to observe learning-related changes in GABA downstream in striatal regions. Although BOLD responses increased in cortico-striatal networks as a function of the robust behavioural improvements, we found no evidence that the learning episode modulated striatal GABA at the group level. Similarly, although our previous work showed that stress induced a boost in activation in motor cortical areas13 and striatal-dependent processes are typically facilitated by stress (e.g., see ref. 70), the administration of stress prior to learning in this study also unexpectedly did not influence striatal GABA levels.
With respect to the HC, fMRI investigations have shown that the HC is primarily active early during MSL and its activation gradually decreases thereafter7,9–11. Moreover, our previous work indicated that stress resulted in a larger practice-related decrease of hippocampal BOLD responses during learning13. These changes in functional activation were not paralleled by changes in hippocampal GABA levels. Although previous literature examining the interaction among GABA, stress and learning in the motor domain is limited, insights can be gleaned from the declarative memory domain. Specifically, the relationship between hippocampal GABA levels and learning processes has recently been studied in associative learning74. In line with our study, the authors did not observe a change in hippocampal GABA over learning (note that in contrast to our study, MRS was assessed after multiple training sessions over a period of weeks); however, baseline GABA was tightly linked to memory performance.
Our findings raise the possibility that functional plasticity (induced by the stress intervention and/or motor learning) is not paralleled by GABAergic modulation in the STR and the HC. This explanation is certainly possible, yet there are also other factors that might contribute to the null effects at the group level reported here. First, due to higher field inhomogeneity (caused for example by the neighbouring ventricles), signal-to-noise ratio in subcortical regions such as the STR and the HC is lower than in the cortex75. Hence, changes in GABA are overall harder to detect and might be smaller than in motor cortical regions. Second, to obtain sufficient signal-to-noise ratio, the MRS voxel is rather large and, as a result, includes not solely the specific ROI. The consequences of this are twofold: (1) inter-subject variability in the size of different structures might contribute to variability in the anatomical composition of the voxel and (2) differences in physiological properties of the included structures (e.g., relative density of glutamatergic vs. GABAergic receptors) will influence estimates of GABA levels averaged across the entire voxel. Nonetheless, our regression results provide a link between inter-subject variability in GABA measures and functional plasticity during MSL.
Limitations and future directions
MRS measures of GABA combine a mixture of GABA pools (pre-synaptic, synaptic and extracellular) from all types of GABAergic neurons in the MRS voxel. Accordingly, MRS does not allow us to draw conclusions on the exact underlying molecular mechanisms. In addition to the methodological considerations, this work provided no evidence of a link between GABA measures and behaviour. Therefore, this work is inconclusive on the behavioural relevance of GABA levels. Furthermore, to conclude whether striatal and hippocampal activation under stress truly depends on GABAergic inhibition in the STR, experimental manipulations of GABA are required rather than the individual-differences regression approach used here. It is also worth noting that BOLD data were acquired during MSL, whereas GABA was assessed pre-stress and post learning. Accordingly, our post-learning MRS time point was not acquired during the peak stress response (see Fig. 2a). It is unclear whether the relationships between GABA and BOLD signals reported in the current manuscript would remain if data were acquired simultaneously. Follow-up studies should consider using online functional MRS (e.g., see refs. 27,76) rather than a single post-learning measurement as used here, allowing a more temporally, fine-grained assessment.
Given our research aims, we used an MRS sequence optimized for the detection of GABA (i.e., Mescher–Garwood point-resolved spectroscopy (MEGA-PRESS)), which comes at the cost of the accurate measurement of other metabolites77. Interestingly, when considering modulation of other neurochemicals during learning, a recent study provided the first evidence of selective glutamate modulation in the HC during associative learning78. In the study of Stanley et al.78, the modulation of glutamate was limited to the first epochs of memory encoding. These effects are not only consistent with evidence from lesion and molecular studies for the relevance of the early hippocampal involvement in memory formation79,80, but also with studies in MSL, suggesting that early hippocampal activation is critical to tag the memory trace for sleep-dependent memory consolidation1. It is noteworthy that the MEGA-PRESS sequence used in the current study allows to quantify Glx (a combined measure of glutamate and glutamine) but these measures are thought to present low sensitivity to glutamate changes (due to high glutamine levels in the voxel)81. Therefore, future studies using glutamate-specific acquisition sequences should investigate MSL-related modulation of this particular metabolite of interest.
Conclusion
This is the first study, to our knowledge, which examined the effects of stress and learning on the levels of striatal and hippocampal GABA, two structures known to be critically involved in MSL1,4. Our results demonstrate that neither stress nor subsequent learning had an effect on GABA at the group level. However, stress modulated the relationship between striatal GABA and hippocampal activation during motor sequence memory acquisition, providing evidence for a GABA-related link between striatal and hippocampal memory systems under stress.
Material and methods
The research presented in this study is part of a larger experimental protocol completed by the same cohort of participants (see Supplementary Fig. 1 for the full design). A subset of corresponding results has been published in Dolfen et al.13.
Participants
Eighty young (mean age: 22.2, range: 18–31, 48 females), right-handed (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory82), healthy individuals participated in this research. Participants had no history of neurological or psychiatric diseases and were free of medications. Participants presented no signs of chronic pain (Pain Catastrophizing Scale83), extreme stress (Perceived Stress Scale84), excessive daytime sleepiness (Epworth Sleepiness Scale85), anxiety (Beck Anxiety Inventory86) or depression (Beck Depression Inventory87). All participants reported normal sleep quality and quantity during the month and the night prior to the study, as evaluated with the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index88 and the St Mary’s Hospital questionnaire89, respectively. We did not include extreme morning or evening chronotypes (Circadian Rhythm questionnaire90), or shift workers. All participants provided written informed consent before the start of the study, in accordance with the local ethics committee approval (Medical Ethics Committee University Hospital Leuven, Belgium; B322201525025).
As glucocorticoids play a critical role in the impact of stress on learning and memory20,56, and earlier studies have shown that not all individuals show a cortisol response to the SECPT intervention (i.e., SCRs)46, individual cortisol data were analysed during collection. SCRs are defined as participants with a stress-induced increase larger than 15.5% or 1.5 nmol/l91 (classification used in refs. 13,55). Data acquisition continued until the number of SCRs (and control participants) reached the estimated sample size. Accordingly, 34 (21 females; 62%) and 46 (27 females; 59%) participants were subjected to the control and stress intervention, respectively. In the stress group, 17 were classified as stress cortisol non-responders (SCNR) and 29 participants as SCR. Four participants in the control group were excluded, because they were classified (using the criterion mentioned above) as cortisol responders. Two participants (one control and one SCR) were discarded, because they were statistical outliers (average ± 3 SDs) in performance speed and accuracy at the immediate post-training test. One control participant was excluded due to excessive motion during the fMRI training session (>2 voxels) and one participant (SCR group) was excluded because of a deviation from the experimental protocol. Three additional participants (one in each group) were excluded due to missing MRS data at one of the time points. Accordingly, a total of 69 participants were included in the behavioural and stress physiology analyses [Control group (N = 27, 16 females); Stress group (N = 42)]. In line with our previous work, the primary group comparison presented in the main text focused on the controls (N = 27, 16 females) and cortisol responders in the stress group (N = 26, 13 females). For completeness, all results from the relatively small set of stress cortisol non-responders (N = 16, 13 females) are detailed in the Supplementary Results. Participant characteristics for each of the three groups can be found in Supplementary Table 1. As the missing MRS data resulted in a slightly different sub-sample of participants as compared to Dolfen et al.13, results from the behavioural and stress physiology analyses from the current sample are presented in the results section but are fully in line with those presented in Dolfen et al.13.
Additional participants were excluded from region-specific MRS analyses and corresponding regression analyses with behaviour and brain activation. With respect to the STR, one participant (Control group) with extreme baseline GABA+ values (average + 3 SDs) was excluded. In the end, a total of 52 striatal voxels were included for each time point (corresponding to 26 control (15 females) and 26 SCR (13 females)). With respect to MRS data quality in the HC, five participants were excluded due to spectral artefacts at one of the time points resulting in no discernible GABA peak (three in the Control group, two in the SCR group). One outlier in GABA+ levels (average + 3 SDs) was identified at baseline and post intervention (both SCR group). Thus, a total of 46 hippocampal voxels were included for each time point (24 control (13 females) and 22 SCR (12 females)).
Experimental procedure
The experimental procedure is depicted in Fig. 1. Participants followed a regular sleep/wake schedule (according to their own schedule ±1 h) starting 3 days before the experimental session. Sleep diaries and wrist actigraphy (ActiGraph wGT3X-BT, Pensacola, FL) were used to assess compliance to this schedule. On the day of the experimental session, participants were instructed to wake up at the latest 1 h before the start of the experimental session to account for the cortisol awakening response92. They were also not allowed to brush their teeth, eat and drink (apart from water) for 1 h before the start of the experiment, to guarantee adequate saliva sampling for cortisol assessment (see below).
At ~8.30 a.m., participants were positioned in the MR scanner for the first MRI session. During this session, baseline (i.e., pre-intervention/learning) MRS data were acquired (MRS base, Fig. 1). After this baseline session, participants were randomly assigned to one of two groups according to whether they were exposed to a control or a stress intervention (i.e., SECPT). The intervention was administered (at ~10 a.m.) in a testing room in the vicinity of the MRI scanner on average 30 min (range: 29–34) before the training on the MSL task (a self-initiated bimanual finger-tapping task, see next section), which took place in the MRI scanner. This timing was chosen, because SECPT-induced secretion of cortisol is known to reach peak levels after 25 min45,46. The MSL session was immediately followed by post-intervention/learning MRS (MRS post, Fig. 1). The post-intervention/learning MRS sequence thus started ~60 min after the start of the stress intervention.
To measure the time course of cortisol concentration, salivary samples were collected throughout the study (see Fig. 1 and below for details). At arrival (before baseline MRS measurements) and immediately before the control/stress intervention, vigilance was measured subjectively using the Stanford Sleepiness questionnaire and objectively using a Psychomotor Vigilance Task. A random serial reaction time task was administered before MSL (15 min after the intervention), in order to assess the effect of the intervention on general motor execution. Results with respect to the assessment of vigilance, general motor execution and sleep prior to the experimental session are reported in the Supplementary Results. The groups did not differ with respect to sleep characteristics, general motor performance and vigilance.
Assessment of general motor execution
A random serial reaction time task93, implemented in Matlab Psychophysics Toolbox version 3, was used to assess general motor execution after the control/stress intervention but prior to MSL (Fig. 1). It is noteworthy that this task was performed while participants lied supine in the scanner but were not scanned. During this task, eight squares were presented on the screen, each corresponding to one of the eight keys on the specialized keyboard and to one of the eight fingers (no thumbs). The colour of the outline of the squares alternated between red and green, indicating rest and practice blocks, respectively. During the practice blocks, participants had to press as quickly as possible the key corresponding to the location of a green filled square that appeared on the screen. After a response, the next square changed to green (response-stimulus interval = 0 ms) following a random order. After 48 presses, the practice block automatically turned into a rest block and the outline of the squares changed from green to red. The task included four practice blocks, separated by 15 s rest intervals. Performance was measured in terms of speed (response time in ms) and accuracy (number of correct key presses).
MSL task and behavioural measures
Participants were scanned while they were trained on a bimanual finger-tapping task implemented in Matlab Psychophysics Toolbox version 3. The task required participants to tap an eight-element finger sequence (eight fingers, no thumbs) on a specialized keyboard as rapidly and accurately as possible. The sequence to perform (4-7-3-8-6-2-5-1, where 1 and 8 correspond to the little fingers of the left and right hands, respectively) was presented on the screen during task practice. At the start of the training, a brief pre-training phase was included during which participants performed the sequence repeatedly and slowly until three consecutive correct sequences were completed. MSL consisted of 20 practice blocks and was followed by an immediate post test (after a 2 min break) of four practice blocks, in order to minimize the confounding effect of fatigue on end-training performance57. Each practice block was indicated with a green cross displayed in the middle of the screen with the sequence of numbers shown slightly above. Each practice block included 48 key presses (ideally corresponding to 6 correct sequences) after which the cross automatically turned red, indicating a rest block (duration 15 s). During the practice blocks, participants were instructed to continuously tap the sequence in a self-initiated manner until a stop signal (red cross) was given. During rest blocks, a sequence of eight asterisks (*-*-*-*-*-*-*-*) replaced the sequence of numbers and participants were instructed to keep their fingers still and look at the red fixation cross. Motor performance was measured in terms of speed (mean inter-response interval between two consecutive correct key presses in s) and accuracy (% of correct transitions). In addition, in order to assess the relationship between motor learning and measures of GABA (see MRS methods below), an online learning measure was computed as the percentage change in performance from blocks 1–2 to 19–20 of MSL. For both the training and the immediate post test, performance speed was analysed using a Block by Group RM ANOVA. In case of violation of the sphericity assumption, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied. Online gains in performance speed were compared using an unpaired two-sample t-test (two-sided). Results related to the analysis of online gains are reported in the Supplementary Results (Section 2.4 and Fig. 3b).
Stress induction method
In the stress condition, participants were exposed to a modified version of the SECPT45,46,94. The procedure is described in detail in Dolfen et al.13,55. The task required participants to immerse their feet in ice water (0–2 °C) as long as possible, while being videotaped for pretended analyses of facial expression and monitored by an unsociable and non-reinforcing experimenter. The duration of the cold-water stimulation was not provided to the participants; however, participants were instructed to withdraw their feet after 3 min. In contrast to the stress condition, participants in the control condition submerged their feet for 3 min in warm water (35–37 °C). They were neither monitored by an unsociable experimenter nor being videotaped.
With respect to subjective measures, participants were asked to rate their subjective feeling of stress, pain and unpleasantness on a visual analogue scale from 0 (‘Not at all’) to 100 (‘Very much’) immediately following the control/stress manipulation. Heart rate and blood pressure (systolic and diastolic) were assessed using an automatic upper arm blood pressure monitor (BP6000, Braun, Germany) before (pre), during and immediately following (post) feet immersion. Finally, for each participant, salivary cortisol samples were collected between the start of the baseline MRI session (~8.30 a.m.) and end of the post-intervention MRI session (~11.30 a.m.) using Salivette collection devices (Sarstedt Salivette) (see Fig. 1 for detailed timeline of cortisol measurements). After collection, the samples were stored at −20 °C until analysed, using immunoassay (analyses performed by Dresden Labservice GmbH, Germany). Subjective ratings were compared between groups using unpaired two-sample t-tests (two-sided). Physiological measures were analysed using Time by Group RM ANOVAs. In case of violation of the sphericity assumption, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied. Results of planned pairwise comparisons were corrected using Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.
Magnetic resonance imaging
Data were acquired with a Phillips Achieva 3T MRI system using a 32-channel head coil.
Magnetic resonance spectroscopy
MRS methods and quality measures are reported according to standards as outlined in the MRS‐Q guidelines95.
MRS data acquisition: For each MRS time point (baseline and post), MRS data were acquired from two separate voxels positioned over the left STR and left HC. Prior to each MRS time point, a (low-resolution) three-dimensionl (3D) T1-weighted structural image was acquired with a magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient echo (MP-RAGE) sequence (repetition time (TR)/echo time (TE) = 9.6/4.6 ms; voxel size = 1.2 × 1.2 × 2.0 mm3; field of view (FoV) = 250 × 250 × 222 mm3; 111 coronal slices). These lower-resolution anatomical scans were acquired in ~2 min and were used solely for the positioning of the MRS voxels. A single high-resolution T1-weighted 3D MP-RAGE sequence (TR = 9.5 ms, TE = 4.6 ms, inversion time = 858.1 ms, FA = 9°, 160 slices, FoV = 250 × 250 mm2, matrix size = 256 × 256 × 160, voxel size = 0.98 × 0.98 × 1.20 mm3) was also obtained for each participant in a different experimental session (see Supplementary Fig. 1 for the full design). Using SPM12 (Welcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK), the high-resolution T1-weighted image was independently co-registered to the two low-resolution images acquired in the baseline and post MRS time points, creating time point-specific high-resolution images used during further MRS data processing (see below).
MRS data were acquired from 40 × 25 × 25 and 30 × 30 × 30 mm3 voxels positioned over the HC and the STR, respectively, using the MEGA-PRESS sequence96 (14 ms sinc-Gaussian editing pulses applied at a frequency offset of 1.9 p.p.m. in the edit-ON experiment and 7.46 p.p.m. in the edit-OFF experiment, TR = 2 s, TE = 68 ms, 2 kHz spectral width, excitation water suppression). Three hundred and twenty and 224 averages were acquired for the HC and the STR, respectively, corresponding to total scan durations of 11 and 8 min. A higher number of averages were acquired for the HC voxel, to ensure sufficient signal-to-noise ratio97. An additional 16 water-unsuppressed averages were acquired from the same voxel and interleaved to allow for real-time frequency correction98, which is especially important after fMRI scanning99. With the exception of number of averages, and thus the total scan duration, these parameters were identical for the two MRS voxels. The HC voxel was centred on the left HC in the coronal view and positioned parallel to the long (antero-posterior) axis of the hippocampal body in the sagittal view. The STR voxel was centred over the left putamen. In the coronal and axial views, we checked that the voxel did not overlap with the ventricle and, as a consequence, only part of the caudate nucleus was covered. The order in which HC and STR voxels were acquired was counterbalanced across participants.
MRS data analyses: Data were analysed using the Gannet software 3.0 toolkit100. Individual spectra were frequency- and phase-corrected using spectral registration101. The GABA+ signal from the difference spectrum was modelled at 3.0 p.p.m. with a five-parameter Gaussian model, whereas the creatine and choline signals from the non-edited spectrum were fitted with a two-Lorentzian model. For each time point, GABA+ was measured as a ratio to total creatine within each voxel. It is noteworthy that the estimated GABA levels correspond to GABA+ macromolecules102,103 and are hence referred to as GABA+ levels in this manuscript. A GABA+ change measure was computed for use in correlational analyses with behaviour and functional activation by subtracting baseline GABA+ levels from post-intervention/learning levels (i.e., ΔGABA+, with a positive value indicating an increase in GABA+ levels).
Lipid contamination and water suppression were visually checked for each MRS time point. Quality of the MRS data was quantitatively assessed using GABA+ fit error, GABA+ signal-to-noise ratio and frequency drift (reflected by the SD of the frequency offset). The MRS voxels co-registered to the time point-specific, high-resolution anatomical images were segmented, to determine the different tissue fractions (grey matter, white matter (WM) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF)) within each voxel and time point (see Supplementary Table 3). GABA+ values and quality metrics were analysed using Time (baseline vs. post) by Group RM ANOVAs.
To investigate the link between GABA+ and task performance, Spearman’s correlations were computed between GABA+ measures and online gain in performance speed during MSL. GABA+ measures of interest were baseline GABA+ and ΔGABA+. It is worth noting that as the GABA+ post measure was highly correlated to ΔGABA+ (r = 0.8), we opted to not include the GABA+ post variable in the correlation analyses, in order to limit the number of statistical tests. Correlations were computed within each group and next compared between groups using Fisher’s test. The Holm–Bonferroni procedure was used to correct for multiple correlations within each region of interest (i.e., four correlations/ROI). Results of these correlational analyses are reported in Supplementary Results Section 2.8. No links between behaviour and GABA measures were found.
Functional MRI data
fMRI data acquisition: BOLD signal during task practice was acquired with a T2* gradient echo-planar sequence using axial slice orientation that covers the whole brain (TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FA = 90°, 54 transverse slices, 2.5 mm slice thickness, 0.2 mm interslice gap, FoV = 210 × 210 mm2, matrix size = 84 × 82 × 54 slices, voxel size = 2.5 × 2.56 × 2.5 mm3).
fMRI preprocessing and data analyses: Functional images were preprocessed and analysed using SPM12 implemented in Matlab (2020b). Preprocessing involved realignment to correct for motion, coregistration of functional and anatomical images, segmentation and spatial normalization to an average subject‐based template created using DARTEL in SPM12 (registered to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space). In order to form WM and CSF ROIs, the tissue maps (resulting from the segmentation of the anatomical image) were thresholded at a partial volume fraction of 0.99. WM masks were then eroded by two voxels in each direction. As CSF regions are typically small compared to WM regions, no erosion was applied104. The eroded WM and CSF masks were then used as noise ROIs. Finally, spatial smoothing was applied to the functional images (Gaussian kernel, 8 mm full-width at half-maximum [FWHM]).
The analysis of fMRI data was conducted in two serial steps accounting for fixed and random effects, respectively. Changes in brain responses were estimated using a general linear model including the responses to motor sequence practice and their linear modulation by performance speed (mean inter-response interval between correct consecutive key presses by block) during MSL practice. Performance speed, rather than accuracy, was chosen as a parametric modulator, because accuracy was not modulated by task practice (see Supplementary Results Section 2.5). The 15 s rest blocks occurring between each block of motor practice served as the baseline condition modelled implicitly in the block design. The regressors consisted of box cars convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function. Movement errors (i.e., incorrect key presses) and key presses during rest were modelled as events of no interest. Movement parameters (derived from realignment of the functional volumes) and the average time series extracted from the noise ROIs were entered as regressors of no interest. High-pass filtering with a cut-off period of 128 s served to remove low-frequency drifts from the time series, and an autoregressive (order 1) plus white noise model and a restricted maximum likelihood algorithm was used to estimate serial correlations in fMRI signal. Subsequently, linear contrasts assessing the main effect of practice and its linear modulation by performance speed during MSL practice were generated. Modulation by speed contrasts identified regions where the magnitude of the responses increased (or decreased) as the interval between consecutive correct key presses decreased across practice. Similar to previous research (e.g., see refs. 10,105–108), the contrast images [SPM(CON)] were further spatially smoothed (Gaussian kernel 6 mm FWHM) and entered in second-level analysis accounting for inter-subject variance. It is noteworthy that the statistical model outlined above also included imaging data from the MSL retest session (see Supplementary Fig. S1). However, as retest data were outside the scope of our research questions, as no MRS data were acquired at retest, fMRI contrasts from the retest session were not considered in the second-level analyses described below.
In the second level, analyses were performed using full factorial ANOVAs. Results related to these analyses are reported in a previous paper13. To investigate the relationship between BOLD responses during MSL and GABA+ measures, we conducted ROI-based voxel-wise regression analyses. Specifically, we regressed the individual brain activation maps of (1) the main effect of practice and (2) the main effect of practice modulated by performance during training with the GABA+ measures of interest (baseline GABA+ and ΔGABA+). A final ANOVA was used to compare these relationships between groups. Statistical inferences were performed on a priori defined ROIs including bilateral hippocampi and bilateral STR (caudate nuclei and putamen) (as defined anatomically according to the AAL brain atlas)109. Analyses were performed using family-wise error correction for multiple comparisons over small volumes with a threshold of p < 0.05 (SVC110,111), followed by Holm–Bonferroni correction for multiple small volumes within each regression analysis (p < 0.05)112. For SVC, spheres (10 mm radius) were centred on coordinates from MSL literature in our ROIS [Hippocampal locations: (−18 −14 −28), (32 −36 −4)45 and (−22 −32 −12)5. Striatal locations: (20 12 2)113].
Statistics and reproducibility
Statistical parametric mapping (SPM12; Welcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK) was used for statistical analyses of BOLD data. All other data were analysed with SPSS Statistics 24 (IBM). Statistical tests in SPSS were performed by conducting Time by Group RM ANOVAs or unpaired two-sample t-tests. For RM ANOVAs, in case of violation of the sphericity assumption, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied. Results of planned pairwise comparisons were corrected using Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Sample size estimation was based on our previous work showing a significant correlation between MSL performance and cortisol response to stress55.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Belgian Research Foundation Flanders (FWO; G099516N, 1524218N) and internal funds from KU Leuven. G.A. also received support from FWO (G0D7918N and G0B1419N) and Excellence of Science (EOS, 30446199, MEMODYN with S.S.). N.D., M.A.G. and M.P.V. received salary support from these grants. M.A.G. is funded by a predoctoral fellowship from F.W.O. (1141320N). Financial support for author B.R.K. was provided by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement (number 703490) and a postdoctoral fellowship from FWO (132635). A.v.L. was supported by FWO (GOA4516N, GOA3718N and I011320N), the Research Fund KU Leuven (AKUL/19/06) and the ‘Asthenes’ long-term structural funding Methusalem grant (METH/15/011) by the Flemish Government. This study applied tools developed from National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants R01 EB016089, R01 EB023963 and P41 EB015909; R.A.E.E. also receives salary support from these grants. M.M. receives salary support from NIH grant K99 EB028828. We thank Mathijs Franssen, Jana Vandecandelaere, Biene Ooms, Gaelle Dumont and Simon Titone for assistance with data collection. We also want to thank Celine Maes for her help with the revision of the manuscript.
Author contributions
G.A., B.R.K., L.S. and N.D. designed the experiments. N.D. conducted the experiments in collaboration with M.V., M.A.G., B.R.K. and G.A. G.A., B.R.K., N.D., M.M., N.A.J.P., R.A.E.E. and A.v.L. contributed to acquisition and analytic tools. N.D., G.A. and B.R.K. analysed the data. N.D., M.P.V., M.A.G., B.R.K., G.A., M.M., N.A.J.P., R.A.E.E., A.v.L., S.S. and L.S. contributed to the manuscript.
Data availability
All source data underlying graphs in the main figures are available as Supplementary Data 1. The approval granted by the local ethics committee does not permit the sharing of individual data. Group-level data supporting our findings that are not available in the manuscript (including supplementary materials) are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Code availability
The analyses in the manuscript are not based on custom algorithms or software. Scripts used for analysis in Matlab (version 2020b) and SPSS Statistics 24 (IBM) are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. Similarly, the scripts used for data collection (Matlab Psychophysics Toolbox version 3) are available upon request from the corresponding author.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Peer review informationCommunications Biology thanks Manfred Klobl, Robert Ross and the other, anonymous, reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary handling editors: Enzo Tagliazucchi and Karli Montague-Cardoso.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors jointly supervised this work: Geneviève Albouy, Bradley R. King.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s42003-021-02535-x.
References
- 1.Albouy G, King BR, Maquet P, Doyon J. Hippocampus and striatum: dynamics and interaction during acquisition and sleep-related motor sequence memory consolidation. Hippocampus. 2013;23:985–1004. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Doyon J, et al. Contributions of the basal ganglia and functionally related brain structures to motor learning. Behav. Brain Res. 2009;199:61–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Penhune V, Steele CJ. Parallel contributions of cerebellar, striatal and M1 mechanisms to motor sequence learning. Behav. Brain Res. 2012;226:579–591. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.09.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Albouy G, et al. Maintaining vs. enhancing motor sequence memories: respective roles of striatal and hippocampal systems. Neuroimage. 2015;108:423–434. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.King BR, et al. Cerebral activation during initial motor learning forecasts subsequent sleep-facilitated memory consolidation in older adults. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27:1588–1601. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Schendan H, Searl M, Melrose R. An FMRI study of the role of the medial temporal lobe in implicit and explicit sequence learning. Neuron. 2003;37:1013–1025. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00123-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fletcher PC, et al. On the benefits of not trying: brain activity and connectivity reflecting the interactions of explicit and implicit sequence learning. Cereb. Cortex. 2005;15:1002–1015. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Penhune V, Doyon J. Dynamic cortical and subcortical networks in learning and delayed recall of timed motor sequences. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:1397–1406. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-04-01397.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rieckmann A, Fischer H, Bäckman L. Activation in striatum and medial temporal lobe during sequence learning in younger and older adults: relations to performance. Neuroimage. 2010;50:1303–1312. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Albouy G, et al. Both the hippocampus and striatum are involved in consolidation of motor sequence memory. Neuron. 2008;58:261–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Grafton ST, Hazeltine E, Ivry R. Functional mapping of sequence learning in normal humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1995;7:497–510. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.4.497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Albouy, G. et al. Daytime sleep enhances consolidation of the spatial but not motoric representation of motor sequence memory. PLoS ONE8, e52805 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 13.Dolfen N, et al. Stress modulates the balance between hippocampal and motor networks during motor memory processing. Cereb. Cortex. 2021;31:1365–1382. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hermiller MS, Chen YF, Parrish TB, Voss JL. Evidence for immediate enhancement of hippocampal memory encoding by network-targeted theta-burst stimulation during concurrent fMRI. J. Neurosci. 2020;40:7155–7168. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0486-20.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Thakral PP, Madore KP, Kalinowski SE, Schacter DL. Modulation of hippocampal brain networks produces changes in episodic simulation and divergent thinking. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2020;117:12729–12740. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2003535117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gann MA, et al. Hippocampal and striatal responses during motor learning are modulated by prefrontal cortex stimulation. Neuroimage. 2021;237:118158. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Schwabe L, Wolf OT. Stress modulates the engagement of multiple memory systems in classification learning. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:11042–11049. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1484-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Schwabe L, Tegenthoff M, Höffken O, Wolf OT. Mineralocorticoid receptor blockade prevents stress-induced modulation of multiple memory systems in the human brain. Biol. Psychiatry. 2013;74:801–808. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Vogel S, et al. Stress induces a shift towards striatum-dependent stimulus-response learning via the mineralocorticoid receptor. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:1262–1271. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Vogel S, Fernández G, Joëls M, Schwabe L. Cognitive Adaptation under Stress: A Case for the Mineralocorticoid Receptor. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:192–203. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Schwabe L, et al. Stress modulates the use of spatial versus stimulus-response learning strategies in humans. Learn Mem. 2007;14:109–116. doi: 10.1101/lm.435807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lehéricy S, et al. Diffusion tensor fiber tracking shows distinct corticostriatal circuits in humans. Ann. Neurol. 2004;55:522–529. doi: 10.1002/ana.20030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Trepel C, Racine RJ. GABAergic modulation of neocortical long-term potentiation in the freely moving rat. Synapse. 2000;35:120–128. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(200002)35:2<120::AID-SYN4>3.0.CO;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hess G, Aizenman CD, Donoghue JP. Conditions for the induction of long-term potentiation in layer II/III horizontal connections of the rat motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1996;75:1765–1778. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.5.1765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Stagg CJ, Bachtiar V, Johansen-Berg H. The role of GABA in human motor learning. Curr. Biol. 2011;21:480–484. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kim S, Stephenson MC, Morris PG, Jackson SR. TDCS-induced alterations in GABA concentration within primary motor cortex predict motor learning and motor memory: a 7T magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Neuroimage. 2014;99:237–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kolasinski J, et al. The dynamics of cortical GABA in human motor learning. J. Physiol. 2018;0:1–12. doi: 10.1113/JP276626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Floyer-Lea A, Wylezinska M, Kincses T, Matthews PM. Rapid modulation of GABA concentration in human sensorimotor cortex during motor learning. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;95:1639–1644. doi: 10.1152/jn.00346.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sampaio-Baptista C, et al. Changes in functional connectivity and GABA levels with long-term motor learning. Neuroimage. 2015;106:15–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Joëls M, Baram TZ. The neuro-symphony of stress. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:459–466. doi: 10.1038/nrn2632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Vogel, S. et al. Blocking the mineralocorticoid receptor in humans prevents the stress-induced enhancement of centromedial amygdala connectivity with the dorsal striatum. Neuropsychopharmacology40, 947–956 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 32.Vogel S, et al. A stress-induced shift from trace to delay conditioning depends on the mineralocorticoid receptor. Biol. Psychiatry. 2015;78:830–839. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Joëls M, Angela Sarabdjitsingh R, Karst H. Unraveling the time domains of corticosteroid hormone influences on brain activity: rapid, slow, and chronic modes. Pharm. Rev. 2012;64:901–938. doi: 10.1124/pr.112.005892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Schwabe, L. Memory under stress: from single systems to network changes. Eur. J. Neurosci. 45, 478–489 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 35.Kim JJ, Diamond DM. The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity and lost memories. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:453–462. doi: 10.1038/nrn849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.de Groote L, Linthorst ACE. Exposure to novelty and forced swimming evoke stressor-dependent changes in extracellular GABA in the rat hippocampus. Neuroscience. 2007;148:794–805. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hu W, Zhang M, Czéh B, Flügge G, Zhang W. Stress impairs GABAergic network function in the hippocampus by activating nongenomic glucocorticoid receptors and affecting the integrity of the parvalbumin-expressing neuronal network. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1693–1707. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Acosta GB, Losada MEO, Rubio MC. Area-dependent changes in GABAergic function after acute and chronic cold stress. Neurosci. Lett. 1993;154:175–178. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(93)90200-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Losada MEO. Acute stress and GABAergic function in the rat brain. Br. J. Pharmacol. 1989;96:507–512. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1989.tb11846.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Losada MEO. Changes in central GABAergic function following acute and repeated stress. Br. J. Pharmacol. 1988;93:483–490. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1988.tb10302.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Yoneda Y, Kanmori K, Ida S, Kuriyama K. Stress-induced alterations in metabolism of gamma-aminobutyric acid in rat brain. J. Neurochem. 1983;40:350–356. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1983.tb11289.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hasler, G., van der Veen, J. W., Grillon, C., Drevets, W. C. & Shen, J. Effect of acute psychological stress on prefrontal GABA concentration determined by proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Am. J. Psychiatry167, 1226–1231 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 43.Houtepen LC, et al. NeuroImage: clinical acute stress effects on GABA and glutamate levels in the prefrontal cortex: a 7T 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. NeuroImage Clin. 2017;14:195–200. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Albouy G, et al. Interaction between hippocampal and striatal systems predicts subsequent consolidation of motor sequence memory. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e59490. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Schwabe L, Haddad L, Schachinger H. HPA axis activation by a socially evaluated cold-pressor test. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33:890–895. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Schwabe L, Schachinger H. Ten years of research with the Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test: data from the past and guidelines for the future. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2018;92:155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Pruessner JC, et al. Deactivation of the limbic system during acute psychosocial stress: evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biol. Psychiatry. 2008;63:234–240. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Henckens MJ, Hermans EJ, Pu Z, Joëls M, Fernández G. Stressed memories: how acute stress affects memory formation in humans. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:10111–10119. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1184-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Graveland GA, Williams RS, Difiglia M. A Golgi study of the human neostriatum: neurons and afferent fibers. J. Comp. Neurol. 1985;234:317–333. doi: 10.1002/cne.902340304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Roberts RC, Gaither LA, Peretti FJ, Lapidus B, Chute DJ. Synaptic organization of the human striatum: a postmortem ultrastructural study. J. Comp. Neurol. 1996;374:523–534. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19961028)374:4<523::AID-CNE4>3.0.CO;2-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Lehéricy S. 3-D diffusion tensor axonal tracking shows distinct SMA and pre-SMA projections to the human striatum. Cereb. Cortex. 2004;14:1302–1309. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Nisenbaum ES, Wilson CJ. Potassium currents responsible for inward and outward rectification in rat neostriatal spiny projection-neurons. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:4449–4463. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-06-04449.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Calabresi P, Misgeld U, Dodt HU. Intrinsic membrane properties of neostriatal neurons can account for their low level of spontaneous activity. Neuroscience. 1987;20:293–303. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(87)90021-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Duncan NW, Wiebking C, Northoff G. Associations of regional GABA and glutamate with intrinsic and extrinsic neural activity in humans-A review of multimodal imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014;47:36–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Dolfen N, King BR, Schwabe L, Swinnen S, Albouy G. Glucocorticoid response to stress induction prior to learning is negatively related to subsequent motor memory consolidation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2019;158:32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2019.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.De Quervain DJF, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL. Stress and glucocorticoids impair retrieval of long-term spatial memory. Nature. 1998;394:787–790. doi: 10.1038/29542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Pan SC, Rickard TC. Sleep and motor learning: is there room for consolidation? Psychol. Bull. 2015;141:812–834. doi: 10.1037/bul0000009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Burgess N. Spatial cognition and the brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008;1124:77–97. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Doeller CF, King JA, Burgess N. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:5915–5920. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801489105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Fortin NJ, Agster KL, Eichenbaum HB. Critical role of the hippocampus in memory for sequences of events. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5:458–462. doi: 10.1038/nn834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Klausberger T, Somogyi P. Neuronal diversity and temporal dynamics: the unity of hippocampal circuit operations. Science. 2008;321:53–57. doi: 10.1126/science.1149381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Schmitz TW, Correia MM, Ferreira CS, Prescot AP, Anderson MC. Hippocampal GABA enables inhibitory control over unwanted thoughts. Nat. Commun. 2017;8:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-00956-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Koós, T. & Tepper, J. Inhibitory control of neostriatal projection neurons by GABAergic interneurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 467–472 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 64.Poldrack RA, et al. Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature. 2001;414:546–550. doi: 10.1038/35107080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Wirz L, Reuter M, Wacker J, Felten A, Schwabe L. A haplotype associated with enhanced mineralocorticoid receptor expression facilitates the stress-induced shift from “cognitive” to “habit” learning. eNeuro. 2017;4:1–16. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0359-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Bernácer, J., Prensa, L. & Giménez-Amaya, J. M. Distribution of GABAergic interneurons and dopaminergic cells in the functional territories of the human striatum. PLoS ONE7, e30504 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 67.Wirz L, Bogdanov M, Schwabe L. Habits under stress: mechanistic insights across different types of learning. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2018;20:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.08.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Packard MG, Knowlton BJ. Learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;25:563–593. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Shohamy D, Turk-Browne NB. Mechanisms for widespread hippocampal involvement in cognition. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2013;142:1159–1170. doi: 10.1037/a0034461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Wirz L, Wacker J, Felten A, Reuter M, Schwabe L. A deletion variant of the α2b-adrenoceptor modulates the stress-induced shift from “cognitive” to “habit” memory. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:2149–2160. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3507-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Packard MG, Hirsh R, White NM. Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems. J. Neurosci. 1989;9:1465–1472. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-05-01465.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Schroeder JP, Wingard JC, Packard MG. Post-training reversible inactivation of hippocampus reveals interference between memory systems. Hippocampus. 2002;12:280–284. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Mitchell JA, Hall G. Learning in rats with caudate-putamen lesions: unimpaired classical conditioning and beneficial effects of redundant stimulus cues on instrumental and spatial learning deficits. Behav. Neurosci. 1988;102:504–514. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.102.4.504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Spurny, B. et al. Hippocampal GABA levels correlate with retrieval performance in an associative learning paradigm. Neuroimage204, 116244 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 75.Puts NA, et al. Reduced striatal GABA in unmedicated children with ADHD at 7T. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging. 2020;301:111082. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2020.111082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Chen C, et al. Activation induced changes in GABA: functional MRS at 7 T with MEGA-sLASER. Neuroimage. 2017;156:207–213. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Bell, T. et al. In vivo Glx and Glu measurements from GABA‐edited MRS at 3 T. NMR Biomed. 34, e4245 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 78.Stanley JA, et al. Functional dynamics of hippocampal glutamate during associative learning assessed with in vivo 1H functional magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Neuroimage. 2017;153:189–197. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Wirth S, et al. Single neurons in the monkey hippocampus and learning of new associations. Science. 2003;300:1578–1581. doi: 10.1126/science.1084324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Fyhn M, Molden S, Hollup S, Moser MB, Moser EI. Hippocampal neurons responding to first-time dislocation of a target object. Neuron. 2002;35:555–566. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00784-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Jang DP, et al. Interindividual reproducibility of glutamate quantification using 1.5-T proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Magn. Reson. Med. 2005;53:708–712. doi: 10.1002/mrm.20387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol. Assess. 1995;7:524–532. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.4.524. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress author. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983;24:385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep. 1991;14:540–545. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Beck A, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer R. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1988;56:893–897. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Buysse DJ, et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Ellis BW, et al. The St. Mary’ s Hospital Sleep Questionnaire: a study of reliability. Sleep. 1981;4:93–97. doi: 10.1093/sleep/4.1.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Horne, J. A. & Ostberg, O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int. J. Chronobiol. 4, 97–110 (1976). [PubMed]
- 91.Miller R, Plessow F, Kirschbaum C, Stalder T. Classification criteria for distinguishing cortisol responders from nonresponders to psychosocial stress: evaluation of salivary cortisol pulse detection in panel designs. Psychosom. Med. 2013;840:832–840. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Fries E, Dettenborn L, Kirschbaum C. The cortisol awakening response (CAR): facts and future directions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2009;72:67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Nissen MJ, Bullemer P. Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cogn. Psychol. 1987;19:1–32. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Larra MF, Schilling TM, Röhrig P, Schachinger H. Enhanced stress response by a bilateral feet compared to a unilateral hand Cold Pressor Test. Stress. 2015;18:589–596. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2015.1053452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Peek AL, et al. Brain GABA and glutamate levels across pain conditions: a systematic literature review and meta-analysis of 1H-MRS studies using the MRS-Q quality assessment tool. Neuroimage. 2020;210:116532. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Mescher M, Merkle H, Kirsch J, Garwood M, Gruetter R. Simultaneousin vivo spectral editing and water suppression. NMR Biomed. 1998;11:266–272. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1492(199810)11:6<266::AID-NBM530>3.0.CO;2-J. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Mikkelsen M, Loo RS, Puts NAJ, Edden RAE, Harris AD. Designing GABA-edited magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies: considerations of scan duration, signal-to-noise ratio and sample size. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2018;303:86–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.02.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Edden RAE, et al. Prospective frequency correction for macromolecule-suppressed GABA editing at 3T. J. Magn. Reson Imaging. 2016;44:1474–1482. doi: 10.1002/jmri.25304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Harris AD, et al. Impact of frequency drift on gamma-aminobutyric acid-edited MR spectroscopy. Magn. Reson. Med. 2014;72:941–948. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Edden RAE, Puts NAJ, Harris AD, Barker PB, Evans CJ. Gannet: a batch-processing tool for the quantitative analysis of gamma-aminobutyric acid-edited MR spectroscopy spectra. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging. 2014;40:1445–1452. doi: 10.1002/jmri.24478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Near J, et al. Frequency and phase drift correction of magnetic resonance spectroscopy data by spectral registration in the time domain. Magn. Reson. Med. 2015;73:44–50. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Edden RAE, Puts NAJ, Barker PB. Macromolecule-suppressed GABA-edited magnetic resonance spectroscopy at 3T. Magn. Reson. Med. 2012;68:657–661. doi: 10.1002/mrm.24391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Rothman DL, Petroff OA, Behar KL, Mattson RH. Localized 1H NMR measurements of gamma-aminobutyric acid in human brain in vivo. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1993;90:5662–5666. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.12.5662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage. 2007;37:90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Maquet P, Schwartz S, Passingham R, Frith C. Sleep-related consolidation of a visuomotor skill: brain mechanisms as assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:1432–1440. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-04-01432.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Orban P, et al. Sleep after spatial learning promotes covert reorganization of brain activity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:7124–7129. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0510198103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Dang-Vu TT, et al. Spontaneous neural activity during human slow wave sleep. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:15160–15165. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801819105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Fogel, S. et al. Reactivation or transformation? Motor memory consolidation associated with cerebral activation time-locked to sleep spindles. PLoS ONE12, e0174755 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 109.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15:273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Poldrack RA. Region of interest analysis for fMRI. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 2007;2:67–70. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Poldrack RA, et al. Guidelines for reporting an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2008;40:409–414. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand. J. Stat. 1979;6:65–70. [Google Scholar]
- 113.Penhune V, Doyon J. Cerebellum and M1 interaction during early learning of timed motor sequences. Neuroimage. 2005;26:801–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All source data underlying graphs in the main figures are available as Supplementary Data 1. The approval granted by the local ethics committee does not permit the sharing of individual data. Group-level data supporting our findings that are not available in the manuscript (including supplementary materials) are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
The analyses in the manuscript are not based on custom algorithms or software. Scripts used for analysis in Matlab (version 2020b) and SPSS Statistics 24 (IBM) are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. Similarly, the scripts used for data collection (Matlab Psychophysics Toolbox version 3) are available upon request from the corresponding author.