Abstract
Reading-related responses in the lateral ventral temporal cortex (VTC) show a consistent spatial layout across individuals, which is puzzling, since reading skills are acquired during childhood. Here, we tested the hypothesis that white matter fascicles and gray matter microstructure predict the location of reading-related responses in lateral VTC. We obtained functional (fMRI), diffusion (dMRI), and quantitative (qMRI) magnetic resonance imaging data in 30 adults. fMRI was used to map reading-related responses by contrasting responses in a reading task with those in adding and color tasks; dMRI was used to identify the brain’s fascicles and to map their endpoint densities in lateral VTC; qMRI was used to measure proton relaxation time (T1), which depends on cortical tissue microstructure. We fit linear models that predict reading-related responses in lateral VTC from endpoint density and T1 and used leave-one-subject-out cross-validation to assess prediction accuracy. Using a subset of our participants (N=10, feature selection set), we find that i) endpoint densities of the arcuate fasciculus (AF), inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), and vertical occipital fasciculus (VOF) are significant predictors of reading-related responses, and ii) cortical T1 of lateral VTC further improves the predictions of the fascicle model. In the remaining participants (N=20, validation set), we show that a linear model that includes T1, AF, ILF and VOF significantly predicts i) the map of reading-related responses across lateral VTC and ii) the location of the visual word form area, a region critical for reading. Overall, our data-driven approach reveals that the AF, ILF, VOF and cortical microstructure have a consistent spatial relationship with an individual’s reading-related responses in lateral VTC.
Keywords: Visual word form area, White matter, Gray matter, Reading, Vision
1. Introduction
The interplay between brain structure and function in concerting human cognition has spiked considerable interest in recent years. One of the most fascinating observations in this context is the consistent spatial arrangement of category-selective regions in ventral temporal cortex (VTC) across individuals (Glezer and Riesenhuber, 2013; Grill-Spector et al., 2017; Grill-Spector and Weiner, 2014; Kanwisher, 2010; Weiner et al., 2018; Weiner and Grill-Spector, 2010). VTC contains several visual category-selective regions that respond more strongly to their preferred category than others, including regions that are selective to faces (Kanwisher et al., 1997), scenes (Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998), bodies (Peelen and Downing, 2005), words (Cohen et al., 2000) and possibly numbers (Grotheer et al., 2016b, 2016a; Shum et al., 2013, but see Grotheer et al., 2018). These category-selective regions play a critical role in visual perception; for instance, a lesion of a category-selective region produces a specific inability to recognize items of the category that region is selective to (Gaillard et al., 2006; Konen et al., 2011; Rossion et al., 2003; Schiltz et al., 2006); similarly, electrical stimulation of a category-selective region specifically disrupts the perception of the respective category (Jonas et al., 2012; Julian et al., 2016; Parvizi et al., 2012; Rangarajan et al., 2014). Strikingly, recent research showed that anatomical landmarks can predict the location of these category-selective regions in VTC. For example, the mid-fusiform sulcus predicts the location of face-selective regions in the fusiform gyrus (Weiner et al., 2014) and the intersection between the anterior lingual sulcus and the collateral sulcus predicts the location of the place-selective region (Weiner et al., 2018). However, presently, there is an active debate regarding what factors drive this strikingly consistent spatial organization of VTC.
Several hypotheses have been put forward to explain the consistent localization of category-selective regions across individuals, including: i) genetics, which may innately determine the location of these regions (Abbasi et al., 2020; McKone et al., 2012; Polk et al., 2007), ii) foveal and peripheral eccentricity biases coupled with consistent viewing demands of certain stimuli (e.g., foveation on words during reading) (Behrmann and Plaut, 2015; Hasson et al., 2002; Malach et al., 2002), (iii) the underlying cortical microarchitecture, which is supported by the observation that different category-selective regions are located within different cytoarchitectonic regions of VTC (Gomez et al., 2017; Weiner et al., 2017) and iv) white matter connections, as category-selective regions are part of more extended brain networks and hence need to communicate with other regions across the brain (Haxby et al., 2000; Osher et al., 2016; Papagno et al., 2011a; Saygin et al., 2016, 2012). Note that these hypotheses are not mutually exclusive as all of these factors together may constrain the location of functional regions (e.g., see Behrmann and Plaut, 2015 for a unifying framework).
Here, we primarily focus on the last hypothesis and evaluate if and how the location of major white matter fascicles relates to functional responses in VTC during a reading task. We focus on the white matter connections of reading for two main reasons: First, given that reading is a learned skill, it is particularly puzzling, that there is a consistent spatial layout of reading-related responses across individuals. Indeed, recent evidence shows that learning to read changes functional responses in the lateral portion of VTC (lateral VTC) (Ben-Shachar et al., 2011; Cantlon et al., 2011), leading not only to changes in distributed responses (Nordt et al., 2019), but also to the emergence of a region selective for words (Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2018; Dehaene et al., 2010; Nordt et al., 2020). This region, which is often referred to as the visual word form area (VWFA, Cohen et al., 2000; Dehaene and Cohen, 2011), is located in the occipitotemporal sulcus (OTS), and is critically involved in reading (Gaillard et al., 2006; Hirshorn et al., 2016). The VWFA is more commonly found in the left hemisphere than the right hemisphere, which aligns with an overall left-hemispheric lateralization for reading and language more broadly (Behrmann and Plaut, 2015; Dehaene et al., 2010; Powell et al., 2006). Second, recent research has provided evidence for the idea that white matter connections predict the location of category-selective regions involved in processing faces, words, places, and tools (Bi et al., 2015; Osher et al., 2016; Saygin et al., 2016, 2012), and, in particular, play a crucial role in constraining the spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC (Saygin et al., 2016). Specifically, Saygin and colleagues reported that pairwise white matter connections between cortical regions, the so called “white matter fingerprint”, at age 5 predict functional responses in VTC to words at age 8. However, an open question is which white matter fascicles constitute this “white matter fingerprint” and predict the spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC.
To address this gap in knowledge, we examined which white matter fascicles of the human brain predict the map of reading-related responses across lateral VTC as well as the location of the VWFA. White matter fascicles are the large-scale bundles that connect distant cortical regions. Studies have identified the fascicles that connect to the VWFA (Bouhali et al., 2014; Grotheer et al., 2019; Lerma-Usabiaga et al., 2018; Yeatman et al., 2013) and those that interconnect function regions related to reading across the brain (Grotheer et al., 2019). These fascicles include i) the arcuate fasciculus (AF), which connects the temporal and the frontal lobes (Catani et al., 2002), ii) the posterior arcuate fascicle (pAF), which connects the temporal and the parietal lobes (Weiner et al., 2016), iii) the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), which connects the occipital lobe with the anterior tip of the temporal lobe (Catani et al., 2002), iv) the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), which connects the occipital and the frontal lobes (Catani et al., 2002) and v) the VOF, which connects the occipital and the parietal lobes (Takemura et al., 2016; Weiner et al., 2016; Yeatman et al., 2014b). In addition, lesion studies (Papagno et al., 2011b) as well as studies correlating behavioral measures of reading performance with diffusion measures (Cummine et al., 2013; Yablonski et al., 2019), suggest that the uncinate fasciculus (Catani et al., 2002) also plays a critical role in reading, particularly in word retrieval and morphological processing. However, no previous studies tested the hypothesis that these fascicles covary with the spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC. In other words, it is unknown if the endpoints of these fascicles in lateral VTC co-localize with the spatial map of reading-related response as well as the location of the VWFA. Critically, recent methodological innovations in white matter tractography, including constrained spherical deconvolution (CSD, Tournier et al., 2019, 2012) and anatomically-constrained tractography (ACT, Smith et al., 2012), now enable researchers to resolve white matter tracts close to the white matter / gray matter boundary, thereby allowing us to directly investigate if and how fascicles contribute to the consistent spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC.
While we will primarily focus on the role of white matter fascicles in driving the spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC, we also examined an additional hypothesis that has not been tested previously. That is, we asked whether differences in the gray matter microstructure across the cortical surface may also contribute to the consistent localization of category-selective regions in VTC. Recently developed quantitative MRI (qMRI) methods (Edwards et al., 2018; Lutti et al., 2014; Mezer et al., 2013) now enable assessing characteristics of the tissue microstructure of the gray matter in vivo (Gomez et al., 2017; Lutti et al., 2014; Natu et al., 2019; Weiskopf et al., 2013). QMRI measures proton relaxation time (T1), which differs between category-selective regions selective to faces and places (Gomez et al., 2017), changes during development (Gomez et al., 2017; Natu et al., 2019), and correlates with people’s performance in certain tasks, such as face processing (Gomez et al., 2017). As T1 depends on the local cortical microstructure (such as myelination, cell density, cell distribution, and iron concentration), which also covaries with the location of functional regions (Weiner et al., 2017), it is an open question whether T1 may be an additional factor predicting the spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC.
To address the hypothesis that white matter fascicles and gray matter microstructure contribute to the consistent functional organization of VTC, we used a mutimodal approach in which we obtained functional (fMRI), diffusion (dMRI) and quantitative MRI (qMRI) data in 30 adult participants. Using fMRI, we identified reading-related responses in lateral VTC by contrasting activations during a reading task with those elicited by adding and color tasks. These three tasks were performed on identical visual stimuli (letter/number morphs) as in prior studies (Fig. 1a, Grotheer et al., 2019, 2018), allowing us to disentangle the impact of the performed task from that of the visual stimulus the task is being performed on. Using this contrast, we (1) mapped activations during reading across lateral VTC, and (2) identified a region in the mid occipito-temporal sulcus (mOTS) that shows a significant preference for the reading task over the other tasks (T≥3, voxel level) (Fig. 1b). This functional region of interest (fROI) likely corresponds to the VWFA-2 (Grotheer et al., 2018; Lerma-Usabiaga et al., 2018; White et al., 2019). Using dMRI data, we generated a white matter connectome in each participant, automatically identified the white matter fascicles that connect to lateral VTC (Fig. 1c), and mapped the endpoint density of these fascicles to the cortical surface. Using qMRI, we meassured proton relaxation time (T1) in each particpant and mapped T1 across the cortical surface (Fig. 1d). To determine which are the informative features of the white matter anatomy that predict the spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC, we used data from 10 randomly selected participants (the feature selection set) and derived linear models relating fascicle endpoint densities to reading-related reponses in lateral VTC (Fig. 1e). Next, to test if gray matter properties improve the prediction of reading-related responses in lateral VTC compared to using only fascicle endpoints, we tested if adding the T1 value at each vertex improves the model. Finally, using independent data from 20 new subjects (the validation set), we tested how well a model that combines the most informative features of the white and gray matter anatomy predicts the map of reading-related responses across lateral VTC as well the location of the VWFA-2.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
30 typical adult participants (15 female, mean age ±SE: 27±1 years, 1 left-handed) were recruited from Stanford University and surrounding areas and participated in two experimental sessions. Subjects gave their informed written consent and the Stanford Internal Review Board on Human Subjects Research approved all procedures.
2.2. Stimuli and design
In the fMRI experiment, we presented well-controlled character-like stimuli, which could be used for a reading task, a math task, and a color memory task (Fig. 1a). These stimuli, which were morphs of numbers and letters, allowed us to map reading-related responses while keeping the visual input constant (Grotheer et al., 2019, 2018). At the beginning of each trial, subjects were presented with a cue (Add, Read, or Color), indicating which task they should perform. In the reading task, subjects were instructed to read the word in their head, and to indicate which word had been presented. In the adding task, participants were asked to sum the values of the stimuli and to indicate the correct sum. Finally, in the color task, participants were asked to memorize the color of the stimuli and to indicate which color was shown during the trial. After the cue, 4 images were shown sequentially, followed by an answer screen. All images in a trial were either number morphs (>80% number + <20% letter) or letter morphs (>80% letter + <20% number), i.e. stimuli that mostly contained information from one category, but held just enough evidence from the other category to be recognizable as both letters and numbers. The same stimuli appeared in all tasks. The answer screen was presented for 2 seconds and showed the correct answer as well as one incorrect answer at counterbalanced locations left and right of fixation. Participants performed 6 runs, each lasting six minutes, and the task order was randomized across runs and participants. Prior to the experiment, subjects were given training to ensure that they could perform the task with at least 80% accuracy.
2.3. Functional MRI data acquisition and preprocessing
fMRI data was collected at the Center for Cognitive and Neurobiological Imaging at Stanford University, using a GE 3 tesla Signa Scanner with a 32-channel head coil. We acquired 48 slices covering the entire cortex using a T2∗-sensitive gradient echo sequence (resolution: 2.4 mm x 2.4 mm x 2.4 mm, TR: 1000 ms, TE: 30 ms, FoV: 192 mm, flip angle: 62°, multiplexing factor of 3). A subset (N=20) of the fMRI data were used for previous studies (Grotheer et al., 2019, 2018).
A whole-brain, anatomical volume was also acquired, once for each participant, using a T1-weighted BRAVO pulse sequence (resolution: 1mm x 1 mm x 1 mm, TI=450 ms, flip angle: 12°, 1 NEX, FoV: 240 mm). The anatomical volume was segmented into gray and white matter using FreeSurfer (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/), with manual corrections using ITKGray (http://web.stanford.edu/group/vista/cgi-bin/wiki/index.php/ItkGray). From this segmentation, each participant’s cortical surface was reconstructed. Each participant’s anatomical brain volume was used as the common reference space for all analyses, which were always performed in individual native space. In order to be able to derive linear models specific to lateral VTC, we first drew an anatomical boundary of lateral VTC in the FreeSurfer average brain.
This anatomical boundary stretched from the mid-fusiform sulcus (medial boundary) to the inferior temporal gyrus (lateral boundary) and from the anterior end of the occipito-temporal sulcus (OTS, anterior boundary) to the posterior transverse collateral sulcus (posterior boundary). This boundary was chosen to match previous work (Nordt et al., 2019) except for the anterior boundary, which we shifted more anteriorly to ensure that the entire OTS is included. This anatomically defined lateral VTC boundary, which is made available in GitHub (https://github.com/VPNL/predictFuncFromStructCode), was transformed from the FreeSurfer average brain to each individual’s native space (black outline in Fig. 1b,c) using cortex-based alignment. All further analyses were restricted to this anatomical expanse.
The functional data was analyzed using the mrVista toolbox (http://github.com/vistalab) for Matlab, as in previous work (Grotheer et al., 2019, 2018). The data was motion-corrected within and between scans and then manually aligned to the anatomical volume. The manual alignment was optimized using rigid-body robust multiresolution alignment (Nestares and Heeger, 2000). No smoothing was applied. The time course of each voxel was high-pass filtered with a 1/20 Hz cutoff and converted to percentage signal change. A design matrix of the experimental conditions was created and convolved with the hemodynamic response function (HRF) implemented in SPM (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) to generate predictors for each experimental condition. Response coefficients (betas) were estimated for each voxel and each predictor using a general linear model (GLM).
2.4. Functional regions of interest and functional activation maps
We evaluated reading-related responses by contrasting the reading task with the adding and the color task in the experiment. The resulting T-maps were used to identify a reading-related functional region of interest (fROI) in the left mid occipito-temporal sulcus (mOTS) (green outline in Fig. 1b). This region likely corresponds to the anterior segment of the visual word form area (Cohen et al., 2000; Dehaene and Cohen, 2011) or VWFA-2 (Grotheer et al., 2018; Lerma-Usabiaga et al., 2018; Stigliani et al., 2015; Weiner et al., 2017). It was defined in each participant’s cortical surface using both functional and anatomical criteria. Specifically, we took only those vertices that (i) showed a preference for the reading task over the other tasks beyond the threshold of T≥3 and (ii) fell within the mOTS, which is easily recognizable on the cortical surface, as the next more lateral sulcus relative to the mid-fusiform sulcus. The resulting VWFA-2 fROI could be identified in 28/30 participants (fROI size±SE: 301±52 mm3).
Following this, each subject’s reading-related T-map was z-scored to control for inter-subject variability in overall response magnitude and mapped onto that individual’s inflated cortical surface using FreeSurfer (Fig. 1b). Responses at each vertex within the anatomical boundary of lateral VTC were used to derive linear models to predict reading-related responses based on the brain’s white matter fascicles and gray matter tissue microstructure. In these models, data from individual participants were concatenated vertically. As activations during reading were lower and less frequent in the right than the left hemisphere, which is consistent with the current literature (Behrmann and Plaut, 2015; Grotheer et al., 2019), here we focus on the left hemisphere only.
2.5. Diffusion MRI data acquisition and processing
Diffusion-weighted MRI (dMRI) data was collected from the same participants during a different day than the fMRI data, at the same facility and with the same 32-channel head-coil. DMRI was acquired using a dual-spin echo sequence in 96 different directions, 8 non-diffusion-weighted (b=0) images were collected, 60 slices provided full head coverage (resolution: 2 mm × 2 mm × 2 mm, TR: 8000 ms, TE: 93.6 ms, FoV: 220 mm, flip angle: 90°, b: 2000 s mm−2).
DMRI data was preprocessed using a combination of tools from mrTrix3 (github.com/MRtrix3/mrtrix3) and mrDiffusion toolbox (http://github.com/vistalab/vistasoft) for Matlab. First, we denoised the data using i) a principal component analysis, ii) Rician based denoising, and iii) Gibbs ringing corrections (Kellner et al., 2016; Veraart et al., 2016b, 2016a). Second, we corrected for eddy currents and motion using FSL (Smith et al., 2004) (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/), and we performed bias correction using ANTs (Tustison et al., 2010). Third, dMRI data was registered to the average of the non-diffusion weighted images and aligned to the corresponding high-resolution anatomical brain volume using rigid body transformation. Fourth, voxel-wise fiber orientation distributions (FODs) were calculated using constrained spherical deconvolution (CSD) (Tournier et al., 2007). For this, we used the Dhollander algorithm to estimate the three-tissue response function (Dhollander et al., 2016) with automated selection of the number of spherical harmonics. The FODs were then used for tractography.
We used MRtrix3 (Tournier et al., 2019) (RC3, http://www.mrtrix.org/) to generate a white matter connectome for each subject. For each connectome, we used probabilistic fiber tracking with the following parameters: algorithm: IFOD1, step size: 0.2 mm, minimum length: 4 mm, maximum length: 200 mm, FOD amplitude stopping criterion: 0.1, maximum angle: 13.5°. We used anatomically constrained tractography (ACT) (Smith et al., 2012), which utilizes information of different tissue types from the FreeSurfer (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) segmentation of each participant’s high-resolution anatomical scan to optimize tractography. ACT also allowed us to identify the gray-white matter interface (GWMI). Seeds for tractography were randomly placed at this interface within the anatomical boundary of lateral VTC. This enabled us to base our predictions on tracts that reach the gray matter. Each connectome consisted of 3 million streamlines.
Finally, we used Automated Fiber Quantification (AFQ, Yeatman et al., 2012b, https://github.com/yeatmanlab/AFQ) to automatically segment the connectome of each participant into well-established fascicles. The resulting classified connectome was optimized by removing tracts that were located more than 4 standard deviations away from the mean of their respective fascicle (similar to Yeatman et al., 2014, 2012). We conducted all subsequent analyses on these classified white matter tracts, as we were interested in identifying which fascicles predict the spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC. We focused on those 6 fascicles that connect the temporal lobe with other parts of the brain and therefore may originate or end in lateral VTC (Fig. 1c). These tracts are: i) the uncinate fasciculus (UCIF), ii) the inferior frontal occipital fasciculus (IFOF), iii) the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), iv) the arcuate fasciculus (AF), v) the posterior arcuate fasciculus (pAF) and vi) the vertical occipital fasciculus (VOF). In order to determine which of these fascicles are predictive of reading-related responses, we first mapped the endpoint density of each fascicle onto the cortical surface. The resulting endpoint density maps indicate the amount of streamlines that terminate at the gray-white matter interface adjacent to each cortical vertex (for details see: Calamante et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2012). We hypothesized, that reading-related responses in lateral VTC may co-localize with those fascicles that play a role in connecting this part of the reading network to the rest of the brain, but not other fascicles. As such, we used the endpoint densities of each fascicle at each vertex in lateral VTC as predictors in linear models of reading-related responses.
2.6. Quantitative MRI data acquisition and preprocessing
Quantitative MRI (qMRI, Mezer et al., 2013) data was collected within the same session and with the same head coil as the dMRI data. T1 relaxation times were measured from four spoiled gradient echo images with flip angles of 4°, 10°, 20° and 30° (TR: 14 ms, TE: 2.4 ms). The resolution of these images was later resampled from 0.8 × 0.8 × 1.0 mm3 to 1mm isotropic voxels, and qMRI data was aligned with the high-resolution anatomical scan using rigid body transformation. We also collected four additional spin echo inversion recovery (SEIR) scans with an echo planar imaging read-out, a slab inversion pulse and spectral spatial fat suppression (TR: 3 s, resolution: 2 mm × 2 mm × 4 mm, 4 echo time set to minimum full, 2x acceleration, inversion times: 50, 400, 1200, and 2400 ms). The purpose of these SEIRs was to remove field inhomo-geneities.
Both the spoiled gradient echo and the SEIR scans were processed using the mrQ software package (https://github.com/mezera/mrQ) for Matlab to estimate the proton relaxation time (T1) in each voxel, as in previous studies (Gomez et al., 2017; Grotheer et al., 2019; Mezer et al., 2013; Yeatman et al., 2014a). The mrQ analysis pipeline corrects for RF coil bias using the SEIRs scans, which produces accurate T1 fits across the brain. The T1 map of each subject was co-registered to the same anatomical whole-brain volume as the dMRI and fMRI data and mapped to the inflated cortical surface of each participant (Fig. 1d). We used the T1 maps to evaluate if gray matter tissue microstructure contributes to the prediction of reading-related responses. For this, T1 at each vertex within the anatomical boundary of lateral VTC was used as an additional predictor in linear models of reading-related responses.
2.7. Linear model: feature selection
In order to determine if white and gray matter anatomy predict the spatial layout of reading-related responses, we derived linear models that relate the endpoint density of fascicles as well as gray matter T1 to reading-related responses. In our models, we z-scored both the reading-related responses as well as the anatomical predictors in order to control for inter-subject variability. First, using a randomly selected subset of 10 out of 30 participants (the feature selection set), we evaluated which white and gray matter features best predict reading-related responses (i.e. the difference in responses elicited by the reading task and the adding and color tasks) in lateral VTC. During this feature selection, we evaluated which white matter fascicles are predictive of the map of reading-related responses by deriving separate linear models relating the reading-related response map to each fascicle’s endpoints in lateral VTC. We tested how well the endpoint density of each fascicle at each vertex within the anatomically defined lateral VTC predicts reading-related responses using leave-one-subject-out cross-validation. That is, we derived the model using data of all but one subject and then predicted responses in the left-out subject, iterating across all participants. In order to assess the performance of the model, we then measured the correlation between the predicted to the measured reading-related response map in the left-out subject. We used Bonferroni corrected t-tests to evaluate if the correlation between the predicted and measured reading-related responses is significantly greater than 0. Finally, those fascicles that lead to significant predictions were combined into a new linear model that we will refer to as the combined fascicle model.
We tested if different fascicles capture independent variance in the reading-related responses by evaluating if the combined fascicle model (a weighted sum of the endpoints of the AF, ILF and VOF) outperforms the best individual fascicle model. For this, first, we fit a new linear model that uses the endpoints of the AF, ILF and VOF as predictors. Next, we compared predicted and measured reading-related responses in leave-one-subject-out cross-validations, as described above. We used Bonferroni corrected t-tests to evaluate if the correlation between the predicted and measured reading-related responses is significantly greater than 0 in each model. Next, we derived both models using the data of all 10 participants and formally tested if the combined fascicle model outperforms the best individual fascicle model using a simulated likelihood ratio test with 1000 simulations. Given that this analysis indicated the combined fascicle model to outperform the individual fascicle models, we used the combined fascicle model in further steps.
After determining which fascicles are predictive of reading-related responses, in the last step of the feature selection, we assessed whether information about tissue properties in the gray matter, i.e., T1 at each vertex, improves the prediction of reading-related responses relative to the combined fascicles model. First, we tested if T1 is an informative predictor of reading-related responses on its own, by training a new linear model with just this one predictor and then correlating the predicted and the measured reading-related responses in leave-one-subject-out cross-validations, as above. Next, we added T1 as an additional predictor to the combined fascicle model and compared predicted and measured reading-related responses in leave-one-subject-out cross-validations for the combined fascicle model and the combined fascicle+T1 model. We used Bonferroni corrected t-tests to evaluate if the correlation between the measured reading-related responses and those predicted by each model is significantly greater than 0. We tested if the model that includes T1 outperforms the combined fascicle model using a simulated likelihood ratio test with 1000 simulations. Given that these analyses indicated T1 at each vertex to be an informative predictor, we added this predictor to our final model, which we refer to as fascicles+T1.
2.8. Linear model: model validation
To ensure independence of feature selection and model testing, we tested the predictiveness of the fascicles+T1 model in the remaining 20 participants (validation set). For this, at each vertex, we compared the measured reading-related responses with the responses predicted by the fascicles+T1 model using leave-one-subject-out cross-validation in the independent subjects. We used a t-test to evaluate if the correlation between the predicted and measured reading-related responses is significantly greater than 0.
We also assessed whether the predictiveness of our model depends on the selectivity of each vertex. Thus, in each participant’s lateral VTC we identified the 10, 20 and 30% vertices with the strongest preference for reading, those with the strongest preference against reading and those with no strong preference (based on absolute z-score). These sets of vertices where identified in both the measured and the predicted reading-related response maps. Then, we assessed the spatial overlap between measured and predicted vertices using the dice coefficient DC (Dice, 1945): , where |A| is the measured vertices, |B| is the predicted vertices and |A∩B| is the intersection between these two sets of vertices. Additionally, we calculated what is the chance level DC by randomly selecting 10, 20 and 30% of lateral VTC vertices in the predicted map and calculating the overlap between these randomly chosen vertices and those vertices that show the strongest preference for reading in the measured reading-related responses. Finally, we tested the significance of the prediction by comparing the measured DCs to the chance level DC for the 20% sets of vertices, using Bonferroni corrected t-tests. We only performed the t-tests on the 20% sets of vertices and not the 10% and 30% sets, as these three sets are not independent (10% is a subset of 20, which is a subset of 30%).
2.9. Linear discrimination analysis
We also tested how well the anatomical features chosen above (i.e., the endpoint density of the ILF, AF, VOF in combination with cortical T1), predict the location of the VWFA-2 in the validation set. For this, we used a linear discrimination analysis (LDA) and derived a linear classifier that uses fascicle endpoints and T1 to determine which vertices in lateral VTC are within the VWFA-2 and which are outside this fROI. We used a leave-one-subject-out cross-validation approach iterating across those 20 subjects not used during feature selection. To determine the accuracy of this classifier, first, we visually compared the predicted and the measured VWFA-2 fROIs on the cortical surface of each held-out subject. Next, we assessed the prediction accuracy by quantifying the spatial overlap between the predicted and the measured VWFA-2 using the DC. We also performed a similar analysis using a 7mm disk ROI (radius was chosen to match the average radius of the fROIs) placed in the center of the VWFA-2 to test whether our model predicts only the location of the VWFA-2 or also its shape. We reasoned that, if white and gray matter anatomy also predict the shape of the VWFA-2, the prediction of the disk ROI should be significantly worse than the prediction of the VWFA-2 fROI. As a control, we generated a chance level DC by randomizing the vertices that are within and outside of the predicted VWFA-2 and quantifying the spatial overlap between these randomly chosen vertices and the measured VWFA-2.
Finally, to assess if the predicted VWFA-2 responds more strongly to the reading task than the adding and color tasks, i.e. shows the desired preference for reading, we extracted the responses of the predicted VWFA-2 during the fMRI experiment in each held-out subject. We used repeated measures ANOVA’s with task and stimulus as factors to evaluate the effect of the performed task and the stimulus on neural responses within the predicted VWFA-2 fROI.
2.10. Data and code availability
The fMRI and qMRI data were analyzed using the open source mrVista software (available in GitHub: http://github.com/vistalab/vistasoft) and mrQ software (available in GitHub: https://github.com/mezera/mrQ) packages, respectively. The dMRI data were analyzed using open source software, including MRtrix3 (Tournier et al., 2012) (http://www.mrtrix.org/) and AFQ (Yeatman et al., 2012b) (https://github.com/yeatmanlab/AFQ). We make the entire pipeline freely available; custom code for dMRI preprocessing, tractography and further analyses are available in GitHub (https://github.com/VPNL/fat). Code for reproducing all figures and statistics are made available in GitHub as well (https://github.com/VPNL/predictFuncFromStructCode). The data generated in this study will be made available by the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
3. Results
3.1. Endpoints of the ILF, AF, and VOF predict reading-related responses
In the current study we tested the hypothesis that white matter fascicles and cortical microstructure (T1) predict where reading-related responses fall in lateral ventral temporal cortex (lateral VTC) in a given adult individual. First, during feature selection, we evaluated which white matter fascicles predict reading-related responses in lateral VTC. Six fascicles connect to the temporal lobe: i) the uncinate fasciculus (UCIF), ii) the inferior frontal occipital fasciculus (IFOF), iii) the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), iv) the arcuate fasciculus (AF), v) the posterior arcuate fasciculus (pAF), and vi) the vertical occipital fasciculus (VOF). We automatically identified these fascicles in each individual and mapped their endpoint density to the cortical surface within an anatomical boundary of lateral VTC. We then visually inspected the resulting endpoint density maps (Fig. 2a shows each of these maps in a representative subject). Across subjects, we found that i) the endpoints of the ILF, AF, and pAF are distributed across most of lateral VTC, ii) the VOF and IFOF have endpoints only in the posterior end of lateral VTC, and iii) the UCIF rarely has any endpoints in lateral VTC.
To determine which of these fascicles are predictive of reading-related responses, we next derived linear models that relate reading-related responses in lateral VTC to endpoint density separately for each of these fascicles. We trained one model for each fascicle and evaluated the performance of the model using leave-one-subject-out cross-validation. Each model’s performance was evaluated by correlating the predicted responses with the measured reading-related responses in each held-out participant. Results of this analysis show that the endpoint density of the ILF, AF, and VOF predict reading-related responses in lateral VTC. That is, linear models relating the endpoints of each of these fascicles to reading-related responses show a significant cross-validated correlation between the predicted and measured responses (t-tests against 0 with a Bonferroni adjusted threshold of p≤0.008; ILF: p=0.007, AF: p=0.007, VOF: p=0.003; Fig. 2b; mean R±SE: ILF: 0.19±0.05, AF: 0.18±0.05, VOF: 0.16±0.04).
To better understand the relationship between each significant fascicle’s endpoint density and reading-related responses, next, we compared the endpoint density maps described above (Fig. 2a shows these maps in a representative subject) with the meassured reading-related responses (Fig. 2c-meassured shows a representative subject; see Supplementary Fig. 1a for correlations between these maps). We find that regions in lateral VTC that have a high endpoint density for the AF and ILF (red in Fig. 2a) coincide with regions that show a preference for reading (red in Fig. 2c-measured). In contrast, regions that show high endpoint density for the VOF (red in Fig. 2a), which predominantly showed endpoints in the posterior end of lateral VTC, showed a negative preference for reading (blue in Fig. 2c-measured).
Our data suggests that the endpoints of the ILF, AF, and VOF can predict reading-related responses in lateral VTC. However, an open question is whether these fascicles provide overlapping or complimentary predictions of reading-related responses. The latter seems plausible as the endpoint densities of these three fascicles have different distributions across lateral VTC (Fig. 2a) and generated different predictions of reading-related responses (Fig. 2c). To test these possibilities, we generated a new linear model that relates responses during reading to a weighted combination of ILF, AF, and VOF endpoints in lateral VTC. Then, we compared the performance of this combined model and the best individual fascicle model using leave-one-subject-out cross-validation in the 10 subjects used for feature selection. We find that a linear model with three fascicles improved prediction of reading-related responses. That is, there is a higher correlation between the predicted and measured reading-related responses compared to a linear model based on a single fascicle (mean R±SE: best individual fascicle (ILF): 0.19 ± 0.05, combined fascicle model: 0.27 ± 0.03; Fig. 3a; example prediction in a representative individual in Fig. 3b). This improvement is statistically significant (p=0.001, simulated likelihood ratio test comparing models derived from the data of all 10 participants).
Overall, this analysis indicates a consistent spatial relationship between the endpoint densities of the AF, ILF, and VOF and reading-related responses in lateral VTC.
3.2. Cortical tissue microstructure improves prediction of reading-related responses
Next, as the last step of feature selection, we tested whether cortical tissue microstructure in lateral VTC, assessed by T1 relaxation time, further improves the prediction of reading-related responses compared to the combined fascicle model. We reasoned that gray matter microstructure may covary with reading-related responses and that regions with high reading-related responses may show a different microstructure than other parts of lateral VTC.
First, using qMRI, we estimated T1 at each vertex in lateral VTC and then evaluated if T1 by itself is an informative predictor of reading-related responses. To this end, we derived a linear model with just this one predictor and compared predicted and measured reading-related responses in leave-one-subject-out cross-validations in the same 10 subjects as in the prior analyses. We find that the predicted and measured reading-related responses correlate significantly (t-tests against 0, p=0.01, mean R±SE: 0.14 ± 0.04), suggesting that cortical T1 on its own is a significant predictor of reading-related responses (see Supplementary Fig. 1b for correlations between reading-related responses and cortical T1). Next, we added the T1 value at each vertex as an additional predictor to the combined fascicle model, to assess if cortical T1 can improve the prediction of reading-related responses compared to a model based on fascicles alone. We found that this model’s prediction of reading-related responses was significantly correlated with the measured responses in the left out subjects (Fig. 3a, t-tests against 0 with a Bonferroni adjusted threshold of p≤0.025, p<0.0001), which is also evident when visually comparing the spatial layout of the measured reading-related responses with the predicted responses (Fig. 3b shows a representative subject). It should be noted though, that the fascicles that are included in the fascicle+T1 model were chosen based on the same 10 subjects that the model was then tested on; we will further investigate the fascicle+T1 model’s performance on independent data (the validation set) in the next sections (see Supplementary Figure 2 for a comparison of model coefficients in individuals of the feature selection and validation sets).
Crucially, we found that adding gray matter T1 to the combined fascicle model improved the prediction of reading-related responses in lateral VTC (mean R±SE: combined fascicle model: 0.27±0.03, combined fascicle + T1 model: 0.29±0.04). This improvement was statistically significant (p=0.001, simulated likelihood ratio test comparing models derived from the data of all 10 participants). Results of this analysis suggests that cortical T1 explains independent variance in the spatial layout of reading-related responses, which is not captured by the white matter fascicles.
Together, these analyses suggest that the endpoint densities of the AF, ILF and VOF as well as cortical microstructure assessed with T1 show a consistent spatial relationship with reading-related responses in lateral VTC across individuals.
3.3. Model based on fascicle endpoints and cortical T1 predicts both the peaks and the troughs of reading-related responses
To further assess the robustness of the fascicles+T1 model, during model validation, we tested how well this model predicts reading-related responses in 20 new participants. First, we tested how well the combined model – the linear model that uses AF, ILF, VOF and cortical T1 as predictors – captures the map of reading-related responses in this independent data, again using leave-one-subject-out cross-validation. Results reveal that, on average, the predicted and the measured reading-related responses in each held-out subject correlate significantly above chance (Fig. 4 shows measured and predicted maps side-by-side in 3 example subjects; mean across all 20 participants R±SE: 0.28±0.03; mean R was significantly greater than 0, t-test, p<0.0001). This analysis confirms the predictive nature of these combined features of the white and gray matter anatomy in 20 new participants.
Next, in addition to our evaluation of the entire VTC above, we also assessed whether the fascicles+T1 model best predicts the vertices with the strongest reading-related responses (referred to as vertices with a preference for reading, red in Fig. 5), or whether it predicts vertices that have a preference for the other tasks (preference against reading, blue in Fig. 5), or those with no strong preference (green in Fig. 5) equally well. Thus, in each participant’s lateral VTC, we identified the top 10%, 20% and 30% vertices that show the strongest preference for reading, both for the measured reading-related responses and for the reading-related responses predicted by the fascicles+T1 model. Then, we quantified their overlap using the dice coefficient (DC, Dice, 1945). We found substantial spatial overlap between the measured and the predicted 10, 20, and 30% vertices with the strongest preference for reading (Fig. 5-red, DC±SE: 10%: 0.20±0.02, 20%: 0.34±0.02, 30%: 0.44±0.01; t-tests performed on 20% vertices are significantly above chance with a Bonferroni adjusted threshold of p≤0.01), suggesting that this model accurately captures those vertices that show a strong preference for reading, or in other words, the peaks of reading-related responses.
Next, we evaluated if the significant overlap between measured and predicted responses is specific to those vertices with the highest reading-related responses, or if the fascicles+T1 model also predicts those vertices with the lowest reading-related responses, i.e. vertices that show higher responses during the adding and color tasks than during the reading task. We followed the procedure as described above and found high spatial overlap between the measured and the predicted 10%, 20% and 30% vertices with a preference against reading (Fig. 5-blue, DC±SE: 10%: 0.23±0.03, 20%: 0.31±0.02, 30%: 0.41±0.02; t-tests performed on 20% vertices are significantly above chance with a Bonferroni adjusted threshold of p≤0.01), suggesting that the fascicles+T1 model also accurately predicts the troughs of reading-related responses.
Finally, we also tested how well the model predicts vertices that have neither a strong preference for reading or a strong preference against reading. In contrast to vertices with strong positive or negative preferences, the prediction of 10, 20, 30% vertices with weak preferences did not differ from chance (Fig. 5-green, DC±SE: 10%: 0.11±0.005, 20%: 0.21±0.007, 30%: 0.32 ± 0.008; t-tests performed on 20% not significantly different from chance). Together, these data show that our model accurately captures both the peaks and the troughs of reading-related responses in lateral VTC.
3.4. Fascicle endpoints and cortical T1 predict the location of the visual word form area
The visual word form area (VWFA, Cohen et al., 2000; Dehaene and Cohen, 2011), a functional region of interest (fROI) in the occipito-temporal sulcus (OTS), plays a critical role in reading (Gaillard et al., 2006). The VWFA has two clusters in the left hemisphere: one on the posterior OTS (also referred to as pOTS-words/VWFA1) and one on the mid OTS (also referred to as mOTS-words/VWFA-2) (Lerma-Usabiaga et al., 2018). Since our reading-related fROI coincides with VWFA-2 (Grotheer et al., 2018), we tested if our fascicles+T1 model can also accurately predict the location and boundaries of this region in individual subjects.
To this end, first, we identified the VWFA-2 by selecting those voxels in the mid OTS (mOTS) that show significantly higher responses in the reading task than the adding and the color tasks (threshold: T≥3, voxel-level) in each of the 20 participants used for model testing. We then performed linear discrimination analyses (LDAs) with a combined fascicles+T1 classifier to distinguish vertices that are part of the VWFA-2 from those that are not part of the VWFA-2. As before, we used 20-fold leave-one-subject-out cross-validation. Within each participant, we first plotted the measured and the predicted VWFA-2 side-by-side and visually inspected their spatial correspondence. We find that there is varying overlap between the predicted and the measured VWFA-2 across subjects, with some participants showing almost perfect overlap and some subjects showing more misclassifications (Fig. 6a visualizes this range by showing the participants with the most overlap, the least overlap and average overlap). Interestingly, even in participants with average or no overlap between measured and predicted fROI, the predicted fROI still generally falls within the anatomical boundaries of the mOTS and hence in a biologically plausible location.
Next, we used the DC to quantify the spatial overlap between the predicted and the measured VWFA-2 in each participant and compared the measured DC to the chance level DC. Results show that the fascicles+T1 model predicts the location of the VWFA-2 significantly above chance (t-test, p=0.01, significantly above Bonferroni corrected threshold of p<0.03, Fig. 6b). In order to evaluate if the combined model is informative only about the location or also about the shape of the fROI, we also performed the same analysis with a 7mm disk ROI placed in the center of each participant’s VWFA-2 fROI. We found that the overlap between the predicted and the measured disk ROI is significantly above chance (t-test, p=0.01, significantly below Bonferroni corrected threshold of p<0.03, Fig. 6b) and that there is no difference in prediction accuracy between the VWFA-2 and the disk fROI (t-test, p=0.61). This suggests that the combined model accurately predicts the location, but not the shape of the reading-related fROI in mOTS.
Finally, we tested if the predicted VWFA-2 shows the expected preference for the reading task over the adding and color tasks. To this end, we extracted the responses for each of the tasks from the predicted fROI in each participant. Results show that responses in the predicted VWFA-2 were higher during the reading than the color and the adding tasks (Fig 6c). This difference was statistically significant (main effect of task, repeated measures ANOVA with task and stimulus as factors: F(2,38)=8.24, p=0.001). Moreover, similar to VWFA-2 in our previous work using the same functional experiment (Grotheer et al., 2018), the predicted VWFA-2 showed a task by stimulus interaction (repeated measures ANOVA: F(2,38)=5.93, p=0.006). The response characteristics of the predicted VWFA-2 fROI hence closely match those observed for measured VWFA-2 fROIs.
Overall, our data show that the endpoints of the ILF, AF, and VOF in combination with cortical T1 predict reading-related responses across lateral VTC as well as the location of the VWFA-2.
4. Discussion
In the current study, we used a multimodal approach to test the hypothesis that white matter fascicles and gray matter microstructure covary with reading-related responses in the lateral ventral temporal cortex (lateral VTC). We find that i) the endpoint densities of the arcuate fasciculus (AF), inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), and vertical occipital fasciculus (VOF) predict reading-related responses, ii) gray matter microstructure, as assessed by proton relaxation time (T1) further improves the prediction of reading-related-responses, compared to using only information from endpoints of white matter fascicles, and iii) a model that combines the endpoints of the AF, ILF, and VOF with cortical T1 accurately predicts the map of reading-related responses in lateral VTC as well as the location of the visual word form area, a category-selective region that plays a critical role in reading.
Previous research has provided compelling evidence for the role of white matter connections in driving the consistent organization of functional regions in VTC (Bi et al., 2015; Osher et al., 2016; Saygin et al., 2016, 2012), including the development of reading-related responses during childhood (Saygin et al., 2016). Most previous studies relied on estimating the “white matter fingerprint”, an approach that utilizes brain parcellations (e.g., anatomical parcels from FreeSurfer) and measures the amount of pairwise white matter tracts between each functional voxel in one parcel to all other cortical parcels. A weighted sum of these pairwise connections is used as a predictor for that voxel’s functional responses (Osher et al., 2016; Saygin et al., 2016, 2012). While the prior approach has been important for showing that there are significant relationships between white matter tracts and functional responses, it did not reveal whether there is a correspondence between specific, known white matter fascicles and the spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC. By developing a new approach that uses well-established components of the human brain’s white matter anatomy, specifically, the major white matter fascicles of the brain, as predictors of functional responses, the current study elucidates the systematic coupling between functional activations and white matter fascicles for the first time.
We found that three fascicles of the brain, the AF, ILF, and VOF, predict the spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC, suggesting that these fascicles play an important role in the visual components of the reading network. Diffusion properties of these fascicles have previously been correlated with people’s performance in reading tasks, supporting the notion that they play a critical role in connecting different parts of the reading network. First, atypical development of FA in the ILF is associated with poor reading proficiency (Su et al., 2018; Yeatman et al., 2012a). Moreover, lesions to the ILF have been associated with pure alexia (Epelbaum et al., 2008), thereby highlighting this fascicle as essential for reading. Further, fractional anisotropy (FA) in the left AF is related to reading ability during childhood development (Wang et al., 2017) and correlates with phonological awareness in both typical (Yeatman et al., 2011) and impaired (Su et al., 2018; Vandermosten et al., 2012) readers. Finally, the VOF, which has been re-discovered recently (Takemura et al., 2016; Weiner et al., 2016; Yeatman et al., 2014b), is proposed to carry top-down feedback signals during reading (Kay and Yeatman, 2017), and diffusion measures in this tract have also been linked to early literacy skills in children (Broce et al., 2019). It is important to note that, in the current study, we did not assess the reading performance of our participants. Future studies could test whether linear models based on brain structure can also predict reading performance. Further, here we only focus on the structural connection of the lateral VTC, which harbors one part of a larger network involved in reading (Ben-Shachar et al., 2007; Grotheer et al., 2019; Wandell and Le, 2017). Future studies could further explore if the same or different fascicles are predictive of reading-related responses in other parts of the network.
Interestingly, our data shows that even though the AF, ILF, and VOF are all predictive of the spatial layout of reading-related responses, high endpoint densities of the AF and ILF predicts vertices showing high responses during reading, whereas high endpoint density of the VOF instead predicts low responses during reading. While previous studies agree that the AF and ILF connect to the VWFA (Bouhali et al., 2014; Grotheer et al., 2019; Yeatman et al., 2013), a region in lateral VTC that shows a preference for reading, there have been conflicting results on whether or not the VOF connects to this region (Bouhali et al., 2014; Grotheer et al., 2019; Yeatman et al., 2013). These conflicting results can likely be explained by the fact that the VWFA is divided into two distinct regions, VWFA-1 and VWFA-2, also referred to as pOTS-words and mOTS-words, respectively, which show different connectivity to the VOF (Lerma-Usabiaga et al., 2018). Here, we focused only on the VWFA-2, as VWFA-1 could not be identified consistently when comparing responses in a reading task with responses during other tasks performed on identical visual stimuli. This matches findings that the VWFA-1 is a more “perceptual” visually-driven region, whereas the VWFA-2 is a higher-level region involved in lexical / semantic processing of words (Lerma-Usabiaga et al., 2018). This, in turn, is in line with the proposal of a posterior-to-anterior gradient in processing level related to reading along the lateral VTC (Taylor et al., 2019; Vinckier et al., 2007). While the VWFA-1 is thought to be connected to the VOF, the more anterior VWFA-2, which was investigated in the current study, is not thought to be connected to the VOF (Lerma-Usabiaga et al., 2018). These prior findings together with our present findings of low reading-related responses in vertices that have a high endpoint density of the VOF suggest a differential pattern of white matter connections across VWFA-1 and VWFA-2. This hypothesis can be further evaluated in future research.
The current work has implications for our understanding of the neural underpinnings of reading. Given that the ILF, AF, and VOF are predictive of the spatial layout of reading-related responses, and white matter structure constraints reading-related responses during development (Saygin et al., 2016), these fascicles likely play a causal role in scaffolding the neural architecture required for reading. Future studies in atypical populations, for instance those evaluating children with dyslexia (e.g. Kraft et al., 2016; Niogi and McCandliss, 2006; Vanderauwera et al., 2017; Yeatman et al., 2012a; Zhao et al., 2016), could test if structural abnormalities in these fascicles precede and predict difficulties in reading acquisition and whether these abnormalities may result in subtle differences in the spatial layout of reading-related responses in lateral VTC (e.g. Kubota et al., 2019). Another interesting direction for future research would be to probe if short-range white matter connections (e.g. Gomez et al., 2015), which are not considered here, also contribute to the spatial layout of reading-related responses in VTC. Rather than evaluating the entire white matter connectome, as in the fingerprinting approach, we would suggest a more targeted approach that evaluates the contribution of pairwise connections between functional regions of interest (Grotheer et al., 2019), as such data would be more easily interpretable.
In the current study, in addition to investigating the role of the white matter, we also began to explore the additional hypothesis that the local gray matter tissue properties contribute to the consistent spatial layout of reading-related response in lateral VTC. The notion that differences in microstructure across cortical regions may co-localize with different functional regions dates back to the very birth of neuroscience (Brodmann, 1909). Recent methodological improvements, particularly the development of quantitative MRI (Lutti et al., 2014; Mezer et al., 2013), now enable us to test this prediction in vivo, for the first time.
We found that proton relaxation time (T1) at each vertex, measured with qMRI, improves the prediction of reading-related responses compared to a model that relies only on white matter fascicles. This aligns well with previous work showing that i) VTC contains several distinct cytoarchitectonic regions (Caspers et al., 2013; Lorenz et al., 2017), ii) functional regions in VTC co-localize with cytoarchitectonic regions (Weiner et al., 2017), and iii) T1 varies between different functional regions in VTC (Gomez et al., 2017; Natu et al., 2019). However, it has also been shown that T1 changes during development and correlates with task performance (Gomez et al., 2017). Thus, an open question is whether T1 development precedes reading acquisition and the emergence of reading-related functional activations, as is the case for white matter connectivity (Saygin et al., 2016), or whether reading acquisition instead changes the local tissue microstructure, for instance due to tissue proliferation. In the latter case, the predictiveness of T1 would be a consequence rather than a cause of the consistent organization of lateral VTC. The current work in adults should be considered a proof-of-concept that gray matter tissue structure can predict functional responses in VTC and future developmental work can investigate if gray matter structure constrains the organization of VTC during development. Such studies could go beyond T1 measurements and probe the predictive value of various different aspects of the gray matter structure that can be assessed in vivo, including both gray matter microstructure, such as myelination (Lutti et al., 2014; Natu et al., 2019; for review see Edwards et al., 2018; Weiskopf et al., 2015), as well as, macrostructure, such as cortical folding (Weiner et al., 2018, 2014) and cortical thickness (Natu et al., 2019; Sowell et al., 2004, 2003).
In conclusion, the current study showed that cortical terminations of three key fascicles, the AF, ILF, and VOF, in combination with gray matter microstructure predicts the map of reading-related responses in lateral VTC as well as the location of the visual word form area. The current study deepens our understanding of the neural substrates of reading and opens a new avenue of research that carefully assesses the role of various features of the gray and white matter anatomy in driving the organization of the VTC and the brain.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the National Institute of Health (NIH; grant 1R01EY023915 awarded to KGS as well as grants RF1MH121868 and R01HD09586101 awarded to JDY), by the Deutsche Forschungsge-meinschaft (DFG; grant GR 4850/1–1 awarded to MG) and by an Innovation Grant from the Stanford Center for Cognitive and Neurobiological Imaging (CNI; awarded to MG).
Footnotes
Declaration of Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Supplementary materials
Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117669.
References
- Abbasi N, Duncan J, Rajimehr R, 2020. Genetic influence is linked to cortical morphology in category-selective areas of visual cortex. Nat. Commun 11, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-14610-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrmann M, Plaut DC, 2015. A vision of graded hemispheric specialization. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci 1359, 30–46. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Shachar M, Dougherty RF, Deutsch GK, Wandell BA, 2011. The development of cortical sensitivity to visual word forms. J. Cogn. Neurosci 23, 2387–2399. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2011.21615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Shachar M, Dougherty RF, Wandell BA, 2007. White matter pathways in reading. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 17, 258–270. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bi Y, Han Z, Zhong S, Ma Y, Gong G, Huang R, Song L, Fang Y, He Y, Caramazza A, 2015. The white matter structural network underlying human tool use and tool understanding. J. Neurosci 35, 6822–6835. doi: 10.1523/JNEU-ROSCI.3709-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouhali F, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Pinel P, Poupon C, Mangin J-F, Dehaene S, Cohen L, 2014. Anatomical connections of the visual word form area. J. Neurosci 34, 15402–15414. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4918-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broce IJ, Bernal B, Altman N, Bradley C, Baez N, Cabrera L, Hernandez G, De Feria A, Dick AS, 2019. Fiber pathways supporting early literacy development in 5–8-year-old children. Brain Cogn. 134, 80–89. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodmann K, 1909. The principles of comparative localisation in the cerebral.. IN: Cortex based on cytoarchitectonics. Springer, Lausanne, Switzerland. [Google Scholar]
- Calamante F, Tournier JD, Jackson GD, Connelly A, 2010. Track-density imaging (TDI): Super-resolution white matter imaging using whole-brain track-density mapping. Neuroimage 53, 1233–1243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cantlon JF, Pinel P, Dehaene S, Pelphrey KA, 2011. Cortical representations of symbols, objects, and faces are pruned back during early childhood. Cereb. Cortex 21, 191–199. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspers J, Zilles K, Eickhoff SB, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Amunts K, 2013. Cytoarchitectonical analysis and probabilistic mapping of two extrastriate areas of the human posterior fusiform gyrus. Brain Struct. Funct 218, 511–526. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0411-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catani M, Howard RJ, Pajevic S, Jones DK, 2002. Virtual in Vivo interactive dissection of white matter fasciculi in the human brain. Neuroimage 17, 77–94. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen L, Dehaene S, Naccache L, Lehéricy S, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Hénaff M-A, Michel F, 2000. The visual word form area. Brain 123, 291–307. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.2.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cummine J, Dai W, Borowsky R, Gould L, Rollans C, Boliek C, 2013. Investigating the ventral-lexical, dorsal-sublexical model of basic reading processes using diffusion tensor imaging. Brain Struct. Funct 220, 445–455. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0666-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene-Lambertz G, Monzalvo K, Dehaene S, 2018. The emergence of the visual word form: Longitudinal evolution of category-specific ventral visual areas during reading acquisition. PLoS Biol 16, e2004103. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2004103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Cohen L, 2011. The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends Cogn. Sci doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Pegado F, Braga LW, Ventura P, Nunes Filho G, Jobert A, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Kolinsky R, Morais J, Cohen L, 2010. How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 330 (80), 1359–1364. doi: 10.1126/science.1194140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dhollander T, Raffelt D, Connelly A, 2016. Unsupervised 3-tissue response function estimation from single-shell or multi-shell diffusion MR data without a co-registered T1 image. In: ISMRM Workshop on Breaking the Barriers of Diffusion MRI, p. 5. [Google Scholar]
- Dice LR, 1945. Measures of the amount of ecologic association between species. Ecology 26, 297–302. doi: 10.2307/1932409. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards LJ, Kirilina E, Mohammadi S, Weiskopf N, 2018. Microstructural imaging of human neocortex in vivo. Neuroimage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epelbaum S, Pinel P, Gaillard R, Delmaire C, Perrin M, Dupont S, Dehaene S, Cohen L, 2008. Pure alexia as a disconnection syndrome: New diffusion imaging evidence for an old concept. Cortex 44, 962974. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein R, Kanwisher N, 1998. A cortical representation the local visual environment. Nature 392, 598–601. doi: 10.1038/33402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaillard R, Naccache L, Pinel P, Clémenceau S, Volle E, Hasboun D, Dupont S, Baulac M, Dehaene S, Adam C, Cohen L, 2006. Direct intracranial, fMRI, and lesion evidence for the causal role of left inferotemporal cortex in reading. Neuron 50, 191–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glezer LS, Riesenhuber M, 2013. Individual variability in location impacts orthographic selectivity in the “visual word form area”. J. Neurosci 33, 11221–11226. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5002-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomez J, Barnett MA, Natu V, Mezer A, Palomero-Gallagher N, Weiner KS, Amunts K, Zilles K, Grill-Spector K, Pascalis Olivier, de, V., 2017. Microstructural proliferation in human cortex is coupled with the development of face processing. Science 355 (80), 68–71. doi: 10.1126/science.aag0311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomez J, Pestilli F, Witthoft N, Golarai G, Liberman A, Poltoratski S, Yoon J, Grill-Spector K, 2015. Functionally defined white matter reveals segregated pathways in human ventral temporal cortex associated with category-specific processing. Neuron 85, 216–228. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grill-Spector K, Weiner KS, 2014. The functional architecture of the ventral temporal cortex and its role in categorization. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 15, 536–548. doi: 10.1038/nrn3747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grill-Spector K, Weiner KS, Kay K, Gomez J, 2017. The functional neuroanatomy of human face perception. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci 3, 167–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grotheer M, Ambrus GG, Kovács G, 2016a. Causal evidence of the involvement of the number form area in the visual detection of numbers and letters. Neuroimage 132, 314–319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grotheer M, Herrmann K-H, Kovács G, 2016b. Neuroimaging evidence of a bi-lateral representation for visually presented numbers. J. Neurosci 36, 88–97. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2129-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grotheer M, Jeska B, Grill-Spector K, 2018. A preference for mathematical processing outweighs the selectivity for Arabic numbers in the inferior temporal gyrus. Neuroimage 175, 188–200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grotheer M, Zhen Z, Lerma-Usabiaga G, Grill-Spector K, 2019. Separate lanes for adding and reading in the white matter highways of the human brain. Nat. Commun 10, 420216. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11424-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U, Levy I, Behrmann M, Hendler T, Malach R, 2002. Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas. Neuron 34, 479–490. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00662-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI, 2000. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn. Sci doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirshorn EA, Li Y, Ward MJ, Richardson RM, Fiez JA, Ghuman AS, 2016. Decoding and disrupting left midfusiform gyrus activity during word reading. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 113, 8162–8167. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1604126113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jonas J, Descoins M, Koessler L, Colnat-Coulbois S, Sauvée M, Guye M, Vignal JP, Vespignani H, Rossion B, Maillard L, 2012. Focal electrical intracerebral stimulation of a face-sensitive area causes transient prosopagnosia. Neuroscience 222, 281–288. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Julian JB, Ryan J, Hamilton RH, Epstein RA, 2016. The occipital place area is causally involved in representing environmental boundaries during navigation. Curr. Biol 26, 1104–1109. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanwisher N, 2010. Functional specificity in the human brain: a window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 107, 11163–11170. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005062107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM, 1997. The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci 17, 4302–4311. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.17-11-04302.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay KN, Yeatman JD, 2017. Bottom-up and top-down computations in word- and face-selective cortex. Elife 6. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kellner E, Dhital B, Kiselev VG, Reisert M, 2016. Gibbs-ringing artifact removal based on local subvoxel-shifts. Magn. Reson. Med 76, 1574–1581. doi: 10.1002/mrm.26054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konen CS, Behrmann M, Nishimura M, Kastner S, 2011. The functional neuroanatomy of object agnosia: a case study. Neuron 71, 49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraft I, Schreiber J, Cafiero R, Metere R, Schaadt G, Brauer J, Neef NE, Müller B, Kirsten H, Wilcke A, Boltze J, Friederici AD, Skeide MA, 2016. Predicting early signs of dyslexia at a preliterate age by combining behavioral assessment with structural MRI. Neuroimage 143, 378–386. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kubota EC, Joo SJ, Huber E, Yeatman JD, 2019. Word selectivity in high-level visual cortex and reading skill. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci 36. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lerma-Usabiaga G, Carreiras M, Paz-Alonso PM, 2018. Converging evidence for functional and structural segregation within the left ventral occipitotemporal cortex in reading. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 115, E9981–E9990. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1803003115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorenz S, Weiner KS, Caspers J, Mohlberg H, Schleicher A, Bludau S, Eickhoff SB, Grill-Spector K, Zilles K, Amunts K, 2017. Two new cytoarchitectonic areas on the human mid-fusiform gyrus. Cereb. Cortex 27, 373–385. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lutti A, Dick F, Sereno MI, Weiskopf N, 2014. Using high-resolution quantitative mapping of R1 as an index of cortical myelination. Neuroimage 93, 176–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malach R, Levy I, Hasson U, 2002. The topography of high-order human object areas. Trends Cogn. Sci doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01870-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKone E, Crookes K, Jeffery L, Dilks DD, 2012. A critical review of the development of face recognition: Experience is less important than previously believed. Cogn. Neuropsychol 29, 174–212. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2012.660138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mezer A, Yeatman JD, Stikov N, Kay KN, Cho NJ, Dougherty RF, Perry ML, Parvizi J, Hua LH, Butts-Pauly K, Wandell BA, 2013. Quantifying the local tissue volume and composition in individual brains with magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Med 19, 1667–1672. doi: 10.1038/nm.3390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Natu VS, Gomez J, Barnett M, Jeska B, Kirilina E, Jaeger C, Zhen Z, Cox S, Weiner KS, Weiskopf N, Grill-Spector K, 2019. Apparent thinning of human visual cortex during childhood is associated with myelination. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 116, 20750–20759. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1904931116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestares O, Heeger DJ, 2000. Robust multiresolution alignment of MRI brain volumes. Magn. Reson. Med 43, 705–715. doi:. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niogi SN, McCandliss BD, 2006. Left lateralized white matter microstructure accounts for individual differences in reading ability and disability. Neuropsychologia 44, 2178–2188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nordt M, Gomez J, Natu V, Rezai A, Finzi D, Kular H, Grill-Spector K, 2020. Cortical recycling in high-level visual cortex during childhood development. bioRxiv 2020.July.18.209783. doi: 10.1101/2020.07.18.209783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nordt M, Gomez J, Natu V, Jeska B, Barnett M, Grill-Spector K, 2019. Learning to read increases the informativeness of distributed ventral temporal responses. Cereb. Cortex 29, 3124–3139. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osher DE, Saxe RR, Koldewyn K, Gabrieli JDE, Kanwisher N, Saygin ZM, 2016. Structural connectivity fingerprints predict cortical selectivity for multiple visual categories across cortex. Cereb. Cortex 26, 1668–1683. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papagno C, Gallucci M, Casarotti A, Castellano A, Falini A, Fava E, Giussani C, Carrabba G, Bello L, Caramazza A, 2011a. Connectivity constraints on cortical reorganization of neural circuits involved in object naming. Neuroimage 55, 1306–1313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papagno C, Miracapillo C, Casarotti A, Romero Lauro LJ, Castellano A, Falini A, Casaceli G, Fava E, Bello L, 2011b. What is the role of the uncinate fasciculus? Surgical removal and proper name retrieval. Brain 134, 405–414. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parvizi J, Jacques C, Foster BL, Withoft N, Rangarajan V, Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K, 2012. Electrical stimulation of human fusiform face-selective regions distorts face perception. J. Neurosci 32, 14915–14920. doi: 10.1523/JNEU-ROSCI.2609-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peelen MV, Downing PE, 2005. Selectivity for the human body in the fusiform gyrus. J. Neurophysiol 93, 603–608. doi: 10.1152/jn.00513.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polk TA, Park J, Smith MR, Park DC, 2007. Nature versus nurture in ventral visual cortex: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of twins. J. Neurosci 27, 13921–13925. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4001-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell HWR, Parker GJM, Alexander DC, Symms MR, Boulby PA, Wheeler-Kingshott CAM, Barker GJ, Noppeney U, Koepp MJ, Dun-can JS, 2006. Hemispheric asymmetries in language-related pathways: a combined functional MRI and tractography study. Neuroimage 32, 388–399. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rangarajan V, Hermes D, Foster BL, Weiner KS, Jacques XC, Grill-Spector K, Parvizi J, 2014. Electrical stimulation of the left and right human fusiform gyrus causes different effects in conscious face perception. J. Neurosci 34, 12828–12836. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0527-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossion B, Caldara R, Seghier M, Schuller AM, Lazeyras F, Mayer E, 2003. A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing. Brain 126, 2381–2395. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saygin ZM, Osher DE, Koldewyn K, Reynolds G, Gabrieli JDE, Saxe RR, 2012. Anatomical connectivity patterns predict face selectivity in the fusiform gyrus. Nat. Neurosci 15, 321–327. doi: 10.1038/nn.3001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saygin ZM, Osher DE, Norton ES, Youssoufian DA, Beach SD, Feather J, Gaab N, Gabrieli JDE, Kanwisher N, 2016. Connectivity precedes function in the development of the visual word form area. Nat. Neurosci 19, 1250–1255. doi: 10.1038/nn.4354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiltz C, Sorger B, Caldara R, Ahmed F, Mayer E, Goebel R, Rossion B, 2006. Impaired face discrimination in acquired prosopagnosia is associated with abnormal response to individual faces in the right middle fusiform gyrus. Cereb. Cortex 16, 574–586. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shum J, Hermes D, Foster BL, Dastjerdi M, Rangarajan V, Winawer J, Miller KJ, Parvizi J, 2013. A brain area for visual numerals. J. Neurosci 33, 6709–6715. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4558-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM, 2004. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23 (Suppl 1), S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith RE, Tournier JD, Calamante F, Connelly A, 2012. Anatomically-constrained tractography: Improved diffusion MRI streamlines tractography through effective use of anatomical information. Neuroimage 62, 1924–1938. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW, 2003. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat. Neurosci 6, 309–315. doi: 10.1038/nn1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Leonard CM, Welcome SE, Kan E, Toga AW, 2004. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J. Neurosci 24, 8223–8231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stigliani A, Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K, 2015. Temporal processing capacity in high-level visual cortex is domain specific. J. Neurosci 35, 12412–12424. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4822-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Su M, Zhao J, de Schotten MT, Zhou W, Gong G, Ramus F, Shu H, 2018. Alterations in white matter pathways underlying phonological and morphological processing in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci 31, 11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takemura H, Rokem A, Winawer J, Yeatman JD, Wandell BA, Pestilli F, 2016. A major human white matter pathway between dorsal and ventral visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 26, 2205–2214. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JSH, Davis MH, Rastle K, 2019. Mapping visual symbols onto spoken language along the ventral visual stream. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 116, 17723–17728. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1818575116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tournier JD, Smith RE, Raffelt DA, Tabbara R, Dhollander T, Pietsch M, Christiaens D, Jeurissen B, Yeh C-H, Connelly A, 2019. MRtrix3: A Fast, Flexible and Open Software Framework for Medical Image Processing and Visualisation. Neuroimage 202, 116137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tournier JD, Calamante F, Connelly A, 2012. MRtrix: diffusion tractography in crossing fiber regions. Int. J. Imaging Syst. Technol 22, 53–66. doi: 10.1002/ima.22005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tournier JD, Calamante F, Connelly A, 2007. Robust determination of the fibre orientation distribution in diffusion MRI: Non-negativity constrained super-resolved spherical deconvolution. Neuroimage 35, 1459–1472. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tustison NJ, Avants BB, Cook PA, Zheng Y, Egan A, Yushkevich PA, Gee JC, 2010. N4ITK: Improved N3 bias correction. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 29, 1310–1320. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2010.2046908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderauwera J, Wouters J, Vandermosten M, Ghesquière P, 2017. Early dynamics of white matter deficits in children developing dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci 27, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandermosten M, Boets B, Poelmans H, Sunaert S, Wouters J, Ghesquière P, 2012. A tractography study in dyslexia: Neuroanatomic correlates of orthographic, phonological and speech processing. Brain 135, 935–948. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veraart J, Fieremans E, Novikov DS, 2016a. Diffusion MRI noise mapping using random matrix theory. Magn. Reson. Med 76, 1582–1593. doi: 10.1002/mrm.26059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veraart J, Novikov DS, Christiaens D, Ades-Aron B, Sijbers J, Fieremans E, 2016b. Denoising of diffusion MRI using random matrix theory. Neuroimage 142, 394–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinckier F, Dehaene S, Jobert A, Dubus JP, Sigman M, Cohen L, 2007. Hierarchical coding of letter strings in the ventral stream: dissecting the inner organization of the visual word-form system. Neuron 55, 143–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wandell BA, Le RK, 2017. Diagnosing the neural circuitry of reading. Neuron 96, 298–311. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Mauer MV, Raney T, Peysakhovich B, Becker BLC, Sliva DD, Gaab N, 2017. Development of tract-specific white matter pathways during early reading development in at-risk children and typical controls. Cereb. Cortex 27, 2469–2485. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiner KS, Barnett MA, Lorenz S, Caspers J, Stigliani A, Amunts K, Zilles K, Fischl B, Grill-Spector K, 2017. The cytoarchitecture of domain-specific regions in human high-level visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 27, 146–161. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiner KS, Barnett MA, Witthoft N, Golarai G, Stigliani A, Kay KN, Gomez J, Natu VS, Amunts K, Zilles K, Grill-Spector K, 2018. Defining the most probable location of the parahippocampal place area using cortex-based alignment and cross-validation. Neuroimage 170, 373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiner KS, Golarai G, Caspers J, Chuapoco MR, Mohlberg H, Zilles K, Amunts K, Grill-Spector K, 2014. The mid-fusiform sulcus: a landmark identifying both cytoarchitectonic and functional divisions of human ventral temporal cortex. Neuroimage 84, 453–465. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K, 2010. Sparsely-distributed organization of face and limb activations in human ventral temporal cortex. Neuroimage 52, 1559–1573. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiner KS, Yeatman JD, Wandell BA, 2016. The posterior arcuate fasciculus and the vertical occipital fasciculus. Cortex doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiskopf N, Mohammadi S, Lutti A, Callaghan MF, 2015. Advances in MRI-based computational neuroanatomy: from morphometry to in-vivo histology. Curr. Opin. Neurol 28, 313–322. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiskopf N, Suckling J, Williams G, Correia M, M. M, Inkster B, Tait R, Ooi C, Bullmore T, E. T, Lutti A, 2013. Quantitative multi-parameter mapping of R1, PD∗, MT, and R2∗ at 3T: a multi-center validation. Front. Neurosci doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White AL, Palmer J, Boynton GM, Yeatman JD, 2019. Parallel spatial channels converge at a bottleneck in anterior word-selective cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 116, 10087–10096. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1822137116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yablonski M, Rastle K, Taylor JSH, Ben-Shachar M, 2019. Structural properties of the ventral reading pathways are associated with morphological processing in adult English readers. Cortex 116, 268–285. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeatman JD, Dougherty RF, Ben-Shachar M, Wandell BA, 2012a. Development of white matter and reading skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 109, E3045–E3053. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206792109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeatman JD, Dougherty RF, Myall NJ, Wandell BA, Feldman HM, 2012b. Tract profiles of white matter properties: automating fiber-tract quantification. PLoS One 7. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeatman JD, Dougherty RF, Rykhlevskaia E, Sherbondy AJ, Deutsch GK, Wandell BA, Ben-Shachar M, 2011. Anatomical properties of the arcuate fasciculus predict phonological and reading skills in children. J. Cogn. Neurosci 23, 3304–3317. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeatman JD, Rauschecker AM, Wandell BA, 2013. Anatomy of the visual word form area: adjacent cortical circuits and long-range white matter connections. Brain Lang 125, 146–155. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeatman JD, Wandell BA, Mezer AA, 2014a. Lifespan maturation and degeneration of human brain white matter. Nat. Commun 5, 4932. doi: 10.1038/ncomms5932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeatman JD, Weiner KS, Pestilli F, Rokem A, Mezer A, Wandell BA, 2014b. The vertical occipital fasciculus: a century of controversy resolved by in vivo measurements. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 111, E5214–E5223. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418503111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao J, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Altarelli I, Dubois J, Ramus F, 2016. Altered hemispheric lateralization of white matter pathways in developmental dyslexia: evidence from spherical deconvolution tractography. Cortex 76, 51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The fMRI and qMRI data were analyzed using the open source mrVista software (available in GitHub: http://github.com/vistalab/vistasoft) and mrQ software (available in GitHub: https://github.com/mezera/mrQ) packages, respectively. The dMRI data were analyzed using open source software, including MRtrix3 (Tournier et al., 2012) (http://www.mrtrix.org/) and AFQ (Yeatman et al., 2012b) (https://github.com/yeatmanlab/AFQ). We make the entire pipeline freely available; custom code for dMRI preprocessing, tractography and further analyses are available in GitHub (https://github.com/VPNL/fat). Code for reproducing all figures and statistics are made available in GitHub as well (https://github.com/VPNL/predictFuncFromStructCode). The data generated in this study will be made available by the corresponding author upon reasonable request.