Abstract
Background
Test anxiety has been widely found in medical students. Emotion regulation and psychological resilience have been identified as key factors contributing to anxiety. However, studies on relationships were limited. This study investigated the links between psychological resilience, emotion regulation, and test anxiety in addition to exploring the differences about socio-demographic factors.
Methods
A sample of 1266 medical students was selected through cross-sectional survey from a medical university in China during 2019. Data were obtained by network technique using designed questionnaire, which assesses the level of test anxiety, emotion regulation and psychological resilience, respectively.
Results
Medical students experienced test anxiety at different levels, 33.7% of these were seriously. It revealed significant effects of the gender and academic performance on test anxiety. Results of logistic regression indicated that test anxiety was significantly associated with emotion regulation and psychological resilience (p < 0.01). Psychological resilience played a mediating role on the relationship between emotion regulation and test anxiety.
Conclusions
These findings highlight the importance of psychological resilience and emotion regulation in understanding how psychological resilience relates to test anxiety in medical students. Resilience-training intervention may be developed to support students encountering anxiety during the exam.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s12991-021-00360-4.
Keywords: Test anxiety, Emotion regulation, Psychological resilience, Medical students, Academic performance
Background
The term “test anxiety”, is considered as a series of psychological and behavioral responses when individual concern about possible failure on the exam or similar assessment situation [1]. It can occur at any phase of exam. According to psychologists and experts in education [2], an average level of anxiety is useful as an effective motivational factor can enhance one’s performance for more effort. While for some, taking excessive anxiety has an adverse effect on mental health and generates negative feelings for individuals, such as the sense of fear, stress, helplessness, anger, and so on [3]. Notably, test anxiety has a high prevalence all over the world [4–6].
Emotion regulation is defined as the process “by which individuals influence which emotions they have, when they have them, and how they experience and express these emotions” [7]. In general, cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression are two commonly investigated strategies that have been associated with emotional responses and cognition processes [8]. According to the occurring time in which a strategy play a great role in the emotion regulation process, Gross [9] proposed that cognitive reappraisal emphasized antecedent-focused, referring to change the perception and assessment of emotional events and emotional consequence. And expressive suppression referred as a response-focused strategy that individual modify emotional responses through suppressing emotional expression, which appear in the late stage of emotion behaviors [9]. Previous studies [9–11] suggested that cognitive reappraisal, as an adaptive emotion regulation strategy, not only reduced negative emotion experience, but also decreased the sympathetic activity in the limbic brain system. Conversely, expressive suppression possibly was associated with negative emotion, even influenced physiological response including skin conductance level, and heart rate, and these symptoms might persist for some time [12].
Previous works have shown that test anxiety was regulated by psychological resilience [13, 14], which provided a new perspective to explore the intervention on test anxiety. Psychological resilience is recognized as the individual’s ability to effectively maintain psychological and physical health or positively adapt following exposure to the frustrations, difficulties, and adversities [15]. Most researchers study the assessment of two conditions referred in this concept of psychological resilience, including risk or adversity and positive adaptation. Beyond the above assessment, researchers also pay attention to understanding the protective factor of psychological resilience that ameliorate negative effects of threats to one’s functioning. Some studies show that psychological resilience plays a positive role in alleviating stress and arousing positive emotions [16]. In addition, individual with high level of psychological resilience might soon recover from negative emotional as well as bring the higher level of positive emotions [17].
Based on the above literature analysis, emotion regulation and psychological resilience are considered to be protective components for mental health and may have a synergistic effect on anxiety. If the above two factors are proved to influence test anxiety, their improvement could cause better outcomes in the prevention of test anxiety. To the best of our knowledge, there are few studies in this respect. Therefore, this study aims at a deeper understanding about the relation between test anxiety, psychological and emotion regulation. It may provide help to identify the psychological mechanism that lies behind test anxiety and to gain interventions about how effectively students deal with this negative emotion.
Methods
Study sample and procedure
The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) undergraduate students at the Chinese medical university in Guangdong; and (2) respondents need to take an exam this semester. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) anxiety disorder; (2) have a history of other mental disorders; and (3) have a history of psychotropic drug use.
To estimate the sample size in the survey, a formula was taken about simple random sampling.
The sample size was calculated as follows:
where n is sample size, z is confident level, d is the error band and set to 0.2, α is estimate standard deviation = 3.8 (pretest). Setting the inspection level (α) as 0.05, z is 1.96. And the error band (d) is set to 0.2. Screening out research objects that do not meet the requirements, the sample size of the included study was finally determined to be 1300.
For avoiding sampling error, these students were told that the study about a test anxiety aimed at helping individuals. They recognized the value of study and signed informed consent during an initial investigations. Meanwhile, the present study was carried on the exam period from the first to second semesters in 2019 concerning acute anxiety evoked during stress was shown to be worse than usual [18]. Except for sampling error, the wording of questions in survey can result in bias in the collection of data. In order to avoid investigation error, investigators were conducted by uniform and standardized training.
Totally 1300 students were chosen from a medical university of China via simple random sampling in five majors (four in medicine, one in non-medicine). The study was approved by the institutional research ethics committee of Guangdong Medical University (Ref. YJYS2019027). Data were obtained from these participants who completed designed-questionnaires online after were informed of instructions by interview. It took about 15 min to finish responses on these questionnaires.
Measures
Test Anxiety Scale (TAS) was developed in China by Wang [19], based on the Sarason’s Test Anxiety Scale [1]. The TAS is designed to explore the students how often they experience anxiety symptoms before, during, and after taking tests using 37 items. Participants were instructed to answer “Yes” or “No” for each item. If the answer is yes, one point will be counted, and if the answer is no, one point will not be counted, except six items are reversed (e.g., the 3rd, 15th, 26th, 27th, 29th, 33th). Respondents are divided into three different types of test anxiety according to TAS points. Scores greater than or equal to 12 are classified as low-test anxiety, scores falling above 12 and below 20 are classified as a normal level of anxiety, scores greater than or equal to 20 are classified as high level of test anxiety. The internal consistency as measured by Cronbach’s alpha in sample collected from Chinese was 0.77, and the split-half reliability was 0.60 [19]. The Cronbach’s alpha in this sample was 0.884, indicating an acceptable level of internal consistency.
The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) worked out by Gross and John [9] is one of the most widely used instruments for measuring emotion expression. This scale is designed to measure the emotions regulation of participants based on five universal emotions (including disgust, anger, sadness, fear and joy). This scale contains two dimensions, comprising six items each: expressive suppression (e.g., ‘Even when I am happy, I try not to show my feeling’, ‘When I am sad, I will suppress my emotion in order to not let others know how I really feel’), cognitive reappraisal (e.g., ‘I will try to revise my view about the surrounding people and thing to make myself happier’, ‘When faced with a situation that makes me angry, I change the way I look to relieve my anger’) and in addition to two items that whether an individual frequently uses an strategy (e.g., ‘I make sure not to express my emotions’, ‘I change the way I think to regulate my emotions’). Subjects respondent to items on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), indicating that higher scores represent more frequently emotion regulation strategy used. Previous studies have shown that this scale has adequate reliability and validity, the Cronbach’s alpha (α) of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression was 0.83 and 0.77, respectively [20]. The Cronbach’s alpha of ERQ determined in this experiment was 0.903.
For assessment of psychological resilience among students, the Chinese adaptation of Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC) was designed by researchers [21]. In creating the CD-RISC, Connor and Davidson [22] developed the scale that was designed to assess qualities, which was adapted by individuals in face of adversity. Basing on the analysis on data gained from both clinical and populations, the CD-RISC comprised 25 items including three dimensions model: strength, optimism and resilience. Respondents were asked to answer items based on their own situation. Each statement has a 5-point Likert scale anchored by “not true at all” and “true nearly all the time”. The CD-RISC has adequate internal consistency determined by the Cronbach’s alpha (α = 0.89) [23] and there is evidence shown that the reliable of the questionnaire items with college students samples in China was also adequate [21]. In the present study, we examined the internal reliability for the CD-RISC and it was adequate (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.955).
Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS version 21.0 (SPSS Inc., USA). All study data for categorical variables were conducted by frequencies and proportions and data for continuous variables were conducted by means ± standard deviations (SDs). ANOVA and t-tests were used to identify the associated factors among test anxiety, resilience and emotion regulation. Comparisons the correlation between emotion regulation, resilience and test anxiety were performed via correlation analyses and the predictor of test anxiety was analyzed by regression analyses. Bootstrapping [24] was employed to examine the mediation effects of resilience on emotion regulation and test anxiety. Statistical significance was considered to be indicated by p < 0.05.
Results
General characteristics
A total of 1266 samples were further analyzed in this study, in which unqualified informations (e.g., too many same responses, answer is blank) had been removed. This scale had a response rate of 97.4%. All descriptive statistics for study participants are presented in Table 1. Of these participants, females held the predominant majority (N = 820, 64.8%). There were quite a few participants (78.3%) that took five to seven exams as the predominant majority. More than half of participants (83.9%) engaged in the club, 26.5% in part-time jobs. Most participants (61.7%) came from rural, while the rest (38.3%) were from city. More than two-thirds of participants (76.7%) were not from single-child family while only 23.3% were from single-child family. Maximum participants (47.8%) were first year, followed by 24.6% were third year.
Table 1.
Variable | Category | Frequency | Percentage |
---|---|---|---|
Gender | Male | 446 | 35.2 |
Female | 820 | 64.8 | |
Grade | First year | 605 | 47.8 |
Second year | 264 | 20.9 | |
Third year | 312 | 24.6 | |
Fourth year | 64 | 5.1 | |
Fifth year | 21 | 1.7 | |
College | Public health | 280 | 22.1 |
Humanities and management | 14 | 1.1 | |
Clinical medicine | 100 | 7.9 | |
Medical technology | 234 | 18.5 | |
Pharmacy | 638 | 50.4 | |
Part-time jobs | Yes | 335 | 26.5 |
No | 931 | 73.5 | |
Members of the club | Yes | 1062 | 83.9 |
No | 204 | 16.1 | |
Academic evaluation | Excellent | 351 | 27.7 |
Moderate | 490 | 38.7 | |
Lower–moderate | 241 | 19.0 | |
Poor | 184 | 14.5 | |
Examination number | Below 5 | 82 | 6.5 |
5 to 7 | 991 | 78.3 | |
8 to 9 | 144 | 11.4 | |
Above 9 | 49 | 3.9 | |
Birthplace | City | 485 | 38.3 |
Rural | 781 | 61.7 | |
Single-child family | Yes | 295 | 23.3 |
No | 971 | 76.7 |
Levels and associated factors of test anxiety, resilience and emotion regulation
In subjects group, 28.6% with mild level of test anxiety, 37.7% with moderate level of test anxiety, 33.7% with high level of test anxiety (Additional file 1). It showed that problematic test anxiety was prevalent among medical students. Meanwhile, students are more likely to opt for cognitive reappraisal compared with expressive suppression (Additional file 2).
The means and standard deviations of all study variables for test anxiety, resilience and emotion regulation are shown in Table 2. Additionally, gender and academic evaluation were significant factors using one sample t-test and one-way ANOVA, respectively (Table 2). Compared with female college students, the effects of psychological resilience and emotion regulation were much stronger, but the effects of test anxiety were much lower among male college students (p < 0.01, p < 0.01, p < 0.01, respectively). In terms of psychological resilience and emotion regulation, those who have excellent academic evaluation performed better than those who have poor academic evaluation, in contrast to the trend in test anxiety (p < 0.01, p < 0.05, p < 0.01, respectively).
Table 2.
Variable | Psychological resilience | Resilience | Strength | Optimism | Emotion regulation | Cognitive reappraisal | Expressive suppression | Test anxiety |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total | 58.89 ± 15.28 | 29.68 ± 8.56 | 20.14 ± 5.01 | 9.07 ± 2.70 | 64.56 ± 11.92 | 34.30 ± 6.82 | 30.26 ± 6.82 | 16.75 ± 7.62 |
Gender | ||||||||
Male | 60.96 ± 17.58 | 30.99 ± 9.85 | 20.64 ± 5.71 | 9.33 ± 2.97 | 65.52 ± 13.93 | 33.97 ± 7.72 | 31.55 ± 7.63 | 15.77 ± 7.74 |
Female | 57.77 ± 13.76 | 28.97 ± 7.68 | 19.86 ± 4.56 | 8.93 ± 2.53 | 64.03 ± 10.63 | 33.48 ± 6.28 | 29.55 ± 7.61 | 17.27 ± 7.51 |
T | 3.33** | 3.75** | 2.49* | 2.39* | 1.97* | − 1.184 | 4.66** | − 3.36** |
Grade | ||||||||
First year | 59.87 ± 15.31 | 30.19 ± 8.61 | 20.45 ± 4.97 | 9.22 ± 2.69 | 64.42 ± 11.99 | 34.22 ± 6.88 | 30.20 ± 7.25 | 16.61 ± 7.61 |
Second-year | 59.59 ± 16.70 | 30.36 ± 9.09 | 20.10 ± 5.47 | 9.13 ± 2.92 | 64.91 ± 12.54 | 34.26 ± 6.88 | 30.65 ± 7.17 | 16.74 ± 7.74 |
Third year | 56.26 ± 13.83 | 28.10 ± 7.83 | 19.43 ± 4.69 | 8.73 ± 2.52 | 64.54 ± 11.45 | 34.35 ± 6.69 | 30.19 ± 6.61 | 17.02 ± 7.74 |
F | 3.17* | 3.80** | 2.38* | 1.18 | 0.26 | 0.14 | 0.81 | 0.17 |
Academic evaluation | ||||||||
Excellent | 61.89 ± 13.41 | 31.22 ± 7.59 | 21.26 ± 4.48 | 9.40 ± 2.46 | 65.89 ± 11.08 | 35.28 ± 6.21 | 30.61 ± 6.81 | 15.49 ± 6.67 |
Moderate | 60.22 ± 14.41 | 30.37 ± 8.14 | 20.56 ± 4.61 | 9.29 ± 2.67 | 64.76 ± 11.46 | 34.55 ± 6.57 | 30.21 ± 7.00 | 16.51 ± 7.61 |
Lower–moderate | 56.40 ± 14.73 | 28.37 ± 8.23 | 19.27 ± 4.90 | 8.77 ± 2.58 | 63.46 ± 11.67 | 33.55 ± 6.96 | 29.91 ± 6.61 | 16.85 ± 7.94 |
Poor | 52.92 ± 19.11 | 26.64 ± 10.62 | 18.00 ± 6.17 | 8.28 ± 3.15 | 62.92 ± 14.47 | 32.75 ± 8.01 | 30.17 ± 8.12 | 19.64 ± 8.22 |
F | 17.93** | 15.01** | 21.73** | 9.22** | 3.37* | 6.86** | 0.51 | 12.48** |
Single-child family | ||||||||
Yes | 59.37 ± 16.45 | 29.94 ± 9.14 | 20.19 ± 5.35 | 9.24 ± 2.85 | 64.01 ± 12.75 | 33.49 ± 7.16 | 30.52 ± 7.41 | 17.12 ± 7.88 |
No | 58.75 ± 14.92 | 29.61 ± 8.37 | 20.12 ± 4.90 | 9.02 ± 2.65 | 64.73 ± 11.65 | 34.54 ± 6.70 | 30.18 ± 6.93 | 16.63 ± 7.54 |
T | 0.58 | 0.58 | 0.21 | 1.21 | − 0.90 | − 2.32* | 0.72 | 0.95 |
Part-time jobs | ||||||||
Yes | 60.71 ± 15.44 | 30.84 ± 8.72 | 20.52 ± 5.02 | 9.35 ± 2.75 | 66.00 ± 12.75 | 34.90 ± 6.97 | 31.10 ± 7.30 | 16.37 ± 7.97 |
No | 58.24 ± 15.18 | 29.26 ± 8.46 | 20.00 ± 5.00 | 8.98 ± 2.68 | 64.04 ± 11.56 | 34.08 ± 6.76 | 29.95 ± 6.93 | 16.88 ± 7.49 |
T | 2.54* | 2.91** | 1.62 | 2.18* | 2.59* | 1.88 | 2.56* | − 1.05 |
Members of the club | ||||||||
Yes | 59.26 ± 14.79 | 29.86 ± 8.26 | 20.27 ± 4.88 | 9.12 ± 2.65 | 64.68 ± 11.59 | 34.46 ± 6.67 | 30.22 ± 6.94 | 16.86 ± 7.55 |
No | 57.00 ± 17.53 | 28.74 ± 9.91 | 19.42 ± 5.60 | 8.84 ± 2.93 | 63.92 ± 13.49 | 33.47 ± 7.53 | 30.45 ± 7.59 | 16.17 ± 7.97 |
T | 1.73 | 1.53 | 2.03* | 1.34 | 0.83 | 1.90 | − 0.43 | 1.18 |
Birthplace | ||||||||
City | 59.32 ± 15.94 | 29.86 ± 8.84 | 20.17 ± 5.23 | 9.29 ± 2.82 | 64.74 ± 12.27 | 34.09 ± 6.78 | 30.66 ± 7.34 | 17.41 ± 7.87 |
Country | 58.63 ± 14.87 | 29.57 ± 8.38 | 20.12 ± 4.87 | 8.94 ± 2.62 | 64.44 ± 11.70 | 34.43 ± 6.85 | 30.01 ± 6.85 | 16.33 ± 7.44 |
T | 0.78 | 0.57 | 0.18 | 2.23* | 0.44 | − 0.87 | 1.58 | 2.46* |
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01
The correlation analysis results indicated that test anxiety has a significant negative correlation with psychological resilience (r = − 0.382, p < 0.01) and emotion regulation (r = − 0.158, p < 0.01) (Table 3). Meanwhile, the two dimensions of emotion regulation and the three dimensions of psychological resilience were significantly negative correlations with test anxiety, respectively. In addition, the relation between emotion regulation and psychological resilience is a significant positive correlation (r = 0.507, p < 0.01).
Table 3.
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Test anxiety | ||||||||
2. Emotion regulation | − 0.158** | |||||||
3. Cognitive reappraisal | − 0.191** | 0.854** | ||||||
4. Expressive suppression | − 0.082** | 0.864** | 0.476** | |||||
5. Resilience | − 0.382** | 0.507** | 0.559** | 0.316** | ||||
6. Resilience | − 0.374** | 0.500** | 0.537** | 0.326** | 0.972** | |||
7. Strength | − 0.364** | 0.492** | 0.562** | 0.288** | 0.946** | 0.867** | ||
8. Optimism | − 0.304** | 0.371** | 0.420** | 0.221** | 0.827** | 0.723** | 0.723** |
*p< 0.05; **p < 0.01
Testing the mediating role of resilience
Analysis was performed to find whether psychological resilience and emotion regulation has an effect on test anxiety. The multiple regression analysis results indicated that cognitive reappraisal of emotion regulation negatively predicted test anxiety (β = − 0.191, p < 0.01) (Table 4). Similarly, as a predictor, resilience and strength negatively predicted test anxiety (β = − 0.235, p < 0.01; β = -0.160, p < 0.01, respectively).
Table 4.
Independent variable | Equation 1 (dependent variables: test anxiety) | Equation 2 (dependent variables: test anxiety) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ba | SEb | βc | t | B | SE | β | t | |
Constant | 24.046 | 1.079 | 22.290** | 27.879 | 0.821 | 33.941** | ||
Cognitive reappraisal | − 0.213 | 0.031 | − 0.191 | − 6.900** | ||||
Resilience | − 0.209 | 0.046 | − 0.235 | − 4.506** | ||||
Strength | − 0.244 | 0.079 | − 0.160 | − 3.078 | ||||
R2 d | 0.036 | 0.146 | ||||||
F | 47.611** | 108.230** |
**p < 0.01, which present significant differences
aRegression coefficient was presented in B
bStandard error was presented in SE
cStandardized regression coefficient was presented in β
dCoefficient of determination was presented in R2
Furthermore, regression analyses found that dimensions of psychological resilience might be taken to have a mediating role. To test the mediation, total effects model was built by taking cognitive reappraisal as independent variable X, resilience and strength as mediating variables M1 and M2, respectively, and test anxiety as dependent variable Y. The bootstrapping method was adopted to examine mediation effects and 95% confidence interval (CI) level was used. When the value calculated by CI did not include 0, it could be determined that mediating effects have occurred. Results presented that the ab value was statistically significant, indicating that psychological resilience played a mediating role between emotion regulation and test anxiety. Therefore, the constructed model about emotion regulation—resilience—test anxiety was fit. The indirect effect of resilience between emotion regulation and test anxiety did not include 0 (95% CI − 0.189, − 0.091) (Table 5), the indirect effect of strength between emotion regulation and test anxiety also did not include 0 (95% CI − 0.162, − 0.048) (Table 5), indicating resilience and strength as mediators. And on this basis the mediating variables being controlled, it was found that the direct effect of cognitive reappraisal on test anxiety included 0 (95% CI − 0.027, 0.112). These results further suggest that resilience and strength performed a complete mediating role between emotion regulation and test anxiety.
Table 5.
Roadmap | Category | Effect ratio | 95% CI (lower, upper) | Relative mediation effect (%) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Cognitive reappraisal → test anxiety | Total effect | − 0.213 | − 0.273, − 0.152 | 100 |
Cognitive reappraisal → test anxiety | Direct effect | 0.043 | − 0.027, 0.112 | 14.40 |
Cognitive reappraisal → resilience → test anxiety | Indirect effect | − 0.146 | − 0.189, − 0.091 | 48.76 |
Cognitive reappraisal → strength → test anxiety | − 0.110 | − 0.162, − 0.048 | 36.84 |
Discussion
The population characteristics of medical students’ test anxiety, emotion regulation, and psychological resilience
In China, high test anxiety has become the serious health problem to disturb the college students with the ascending tendency, which increased from 27.52 [25] to 35.7% [26] for nearly a decade. The present cross-sectional study conducted in a medical university among the undergraduate students showed that the test anxiety was prevalent, even more (33.7%) of the students experienced unhealthy test anxiety. The results were consistent with recent trends. Overall, summaries of test anxiety research from around the world have shown that students who major in medicine tend to have higher test anxiety compared with other majors. That is partly because the subjects with future careers related to human life take more time to pass the exam because of strict requirements for knowledge and skills [4, 6, 27].
In the present study, we found interesting results with regard to the effects of academic performance and gender which were the main focus on the test anxiety. Many researches have provided strong support for the fact that women in trouble tend to adopt negative emotion regulation and have significantly higher levels of the cognitive component on anxiety than man, due to their emotional and psychological characteristics [28, 29]. The academic performance findings suggested that students with excellent academic performance have significantly higher levels of the emotion regulation and psychological resilience, and were less affected by test anxiety. This was in line with previous findings that there was a negative relation between test anxiety and educational achievement [30]. In part, students with excellent academic performance have developed learning abilities and strategies to easily cope with examinations, while poor students suffered from unrealistic expectations which may increase excessive anxiety in exam. Although the present findings may be considered preliminary, it suggests that individual differences on test anxiety should be regarded with some care in practice.
The relationship between emotional regulation and test anxiety
Emotional regulation is a process in which individuals consciously manage and change their emotions, as well as closely related to mental health. The study found that emotion regulation had a significantly negative correlation with test anxiety, and test anxiety was negatively predicted by emotion regulation. These findings were consistent with previous studies. In both Moltrecht [31] and Aldao [32] studies, emotional regulation can reduce the anxiety level of individuals caused by stressful events, and promote good psychological adaptation of individuals. These results revealed that students who adopt the emotion regulation strategy will have more positive and healthy mental state, which is conducive in reducing the occurrence of exam anxiety.
In addition, the results showed students chose more cognitive reappraisal in the adoption of emotion regulation strategy. Interestingly, the situation is similar in different cultural background. Studies with college students in America, Australia, and Belgium [33, 34] found that subjects believed cognitive reappraisal can improve anxiety to mental flexibility. That may be because the cognitive reappraisal strategy with long-term effects is more adaptive in emotion regulation strategy selection than expressive suppression. Some research point out that adolescents with anxiety disorders can reduce their negative emotion after using cognitive reappraisal [35–37].
To cultivate positive emotions and cope with negative emotions (anxiety), active intervention is necessary before emotional reaction, such as choosing favorable situations, cognitive reappraisal strategies.
The relationships between test anxiety, emotion regulation, and psychological resilience
Recent studies have indicated that test anxiety makes the individual prone to negative emotions (e.g., the sense of fear, stress, helplessness and anger) [3, 38], even more leads to the emotion disorders and subsequent psychological problems in groups [39]. However, extant research has shown that test anxiety, emotion regulation and psychological resilience are related, but has yet to examine the interaction with each other. The present study will address this gap.
As previously expected, there is a significant negative correlation between test anxiety and psychological resilience. There is a significant positive correlation between emotion regulation and psychological resilience. These results were consistent with previous studies [40–42], which reported that psychological resilience is associated with positive emotions and might be used to predict anxiety. Moreover, several recent studies have indicated psychological resilience as the protective factor can cope with the negative emotion, which arises in individuals in the face of adversities or anxiety [43]. Based on this finding, the mediating effect analysis was used in the current study to examine the relationship between test anxiety, emotion regulation, and psychological resilience. Consequently, a relationship between emotion regulation and test anxiety acts via the psychological resilience. Much of the recent work in anxiety [13, 14], psychological resilience has been focused more and more by researchersbecause of its protective effect on anxiety. These findings provided initial evidence that psychological resilience may have positive effects on emotion regulation working on a test anxiety. This may provide insight for clinical psychologists on cultivating the psychological resilience to alleviate test anxiety among medical students.
Conclusions
The aim of this study was a deeper understanding about the relation between test anxiety, emotion regulation, and psychological resilience to identify the psychological mechanism that lies behind test anxiety and to gain anxiety interventions about how effectively students deal with this emotion. Our empirical research suggests that emotion regulation indirectly affected test anxiety through the mediating effect of psychological resilience. However, investigation on the continuous psychological development of subjects was limited due to the cross-sectional study design. The background of the subjects and irrelevant variables that could impair data reliability, such as environmental factors, need to be explored further to improve reliability in future research.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Abbreviations
- TAS
Test Anxiety Scale
- ERQ
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire
- CD-RISC
Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale
- CI
Confidence interval
Authors’ contributions
YL conceived and designed the study, wrote the paper. XW, JR and XZ performed the investigation and carried out data collection. RY and HP contributed to analysis of the data. YL and CP drafted and edited the manuscript. HP reviewed the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
This work is supported by the Social Science and Technology Development Project of Dongguan (No. 2019507152236); Guangdong Graduate Education Innovation Program (No. 2018JGXM54); Provincial key platforms and major scientific research projects of Guangdong colleges and universities-(educational research) project (No. 2017GXJK062); Provincial key platforms and projects of Guangdong colleges (No. 2018KQNCX096); Key scientific research platforms and research projects of ordinary universities in Guangdong Province (No. 2018KQNCX088).
Availability of data and materials
The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Guangdong Medical University (Ref. YJYS2019027). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the survey administration.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
Authors declare no conflict of interests for this article.
Footnotes
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Yuguo Liu and Haiyan Pan contributed equally to this work
References
- 1.Sarason IG, Sarason BR. Test anxiety: handbook of social and evaluation anxiety. New York: Plenum; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Arbabisarjou A, et al. Analyzing test anxiety among medical sciences students of Zahedan in 2015. Int J Med Res Health Sci. 2016;5(7):334–337. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Duan HX, et al. Anticipatory processes under academic stress: an ERP study. Brain Cogn. 2015;94:60–67. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kulsoom B, et al. Stress, anxiety, and depression among medical students in a multiethnic setting. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:1713–1722. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S83577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Yusoff MSB, et al. Prevalence and associated factors of stress, anxiety and depression among prospective medical students. Asian J Psychiatr. 2017;53(1):77–84. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2012.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Wahed WYA, Hassan SK. Prevalence and associated factors of stress, anxiety and depression among medical Fayoum University students. Alexandria J Med. 2017;53(1):77–84. doi: 10.1016/j.ajme.2016.01.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gross JJ. Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74(1):224–237. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Brewer SK, Zahniser E, Conley CS. Longitudinal impacts of emotion regulation on emerging adults: variable-and person-centered approaches. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2016;47:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2016.09.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85(2):348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mauss IB, et al. Individual differences in cognitive reappraisal: experiential and physiological responses to an anger provocation. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;66(2):116–124. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Goldin PR, et al. Neural bases of social anxiety disorder: emotional reactivity and cognitive regulation during social and physical threat. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(2):170–180. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Larsen JK, et al. Social coping by masking parental support and peer victimization as mediators of the relationship between depressive symptoms and expressive suppression in adolescents. J Youth Adolesc. 2012;41(12):1628–1642. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9782-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wu D, et al. Organizational stressors predict competitive trait anxiety and burnout in young athletes: testing psychological resilience as a moderator. Curr Psychol. 2021 doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01633-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Li LB, et al. The mediating and moderating roles of resilience in the relationship between anxiety, depression, and post-traumatic growth among breast cancer patients based on structural equation modeling: an observational study. Medicine. 2020;99(50):23273. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000023273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Haeffel GJ, Vargas I. Resilience to depressive symptoms: the buffering effects of enhancing cognitive style and positive life events. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2011;42(1):13–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gloria CT, Steinhardt MA. Relationships among positive emotions, coping, resilience and mental health. Stress health. 2016;32(2):145–156. doi: 10.1002/smi.2589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tugade MM, Fredrickson BL. Regulation of positive emotions: emotion regulation strategies that promote resilience. J Happiness Stud. 2007;8(3):311–333. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9015-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Lushene R. State-trait anxiety inventory, manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologist Press; 1970. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wang CK. Reliability and validity of test anxiety scale. Chin Mental Health J. 2001;2:96–97. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wang L, et al. Test of difficulties in emotion regulation scale in Chinese People. China J Health Psychol. 2007;4:336–340. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Yu XN, et al. Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Connor-Davidson resilience scale among Chinese adolescents. Compr Psychiatry. 2011;52(2):218–224. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Connor KM, Davidson JR. Development of a new resilience scale: the Connor-Davidson resilience scale (CD-RISC) Depress Anxiety. 2003;18(2):76–82. doi: 10.1002/da.10113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Nishi D, et al. Culturally sensitive and universal measure of resilience for Japanese populations: Tachikawa resilience scale in comparison with resilience scale 14-item version. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;67(3):174–181. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(4):422–445. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Chen R, Liu XN, Zhou RL. The attentional bias to threat stimuli in test-anxious students. Psychol Sci. 2011;34(1):151–154. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Jiang HT. Study on the influencing factors and intervention for college students' test anxiety. Master thesis: Anhui Normal Univ. 2020.
- 27.Guo XX, et al. Investigation on students’ test anxiety in medical college. Chin J Health Psychol. 2016;24(12):1877–1880. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Afzal H, et al. Measures used by medical students to reduce test anxiety. J Pak Med Assoc. 2012;62(9):982–986. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Putwain D, Daly AL. Test anxiety prevalence and gender differences in a sample of English secondary school students. Educ Stud. 2014;40:554–570. doi: 10.1080/03055698.2014.953914. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.von der Embse N, et al. Test anxiety effects, predictors, and correlates: a 30-year meta-analytic review. J Affect Disord. 2018;227:483–493. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Moltrecht B, et al. Effectiveness of current psychological interventions to improve emotion regulation in youth: a meta-analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021;30(6):829–848. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01498-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(2):217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Butler EA, Lee TL, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation and culture: are the social consequences of emotion suppression culture-specific? Emotion. 2007;7(1):30–48. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Brans K, et al. The regulation of negative and positive affect in daily life. Emotion. 2013;13(5):926–939. doi: 10.1037/a0032400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Sheppes G, et al. Emotion regulation choice: a conceptual framework and supporting evidence. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014;143(1):163–181. doi: 10.1037/a0030831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hay AC, et al. Choosing how to feel: emotion regulation choice in bipolar disorder. Emotion. 2015;15(2):139–145. doi: 10.1037/emo0000024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Carthy T, et al. Emotional reactivity and cognitive regulation in anxious children. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(5):384–393. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Owens M, et al. Anxiety and depression in academic performance: an exploration of the mediating factors of worry and working memory. Sch Psychol Int. 2012;33:433–449. doi: 10.1177/0143034311427433. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Putwain DW, et al. The development and validation of a new multidimensional test anxiety scale (MTAS) Eur J Psychol Assess. 2020 doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000604. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Tugade MM, Fredrickson BL, Barrett LF. Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health. J Pers. 2004;72(6):1162–1190. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00294.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Waugh CE, Thompson RJ, Gotlib IH. Flexible emotional responsiveness in trait resilience. Emotion. 2011;11(5):1059–1067. doi: 10.1037/a0021786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sudom KA, Lee JE, Zamorski MA. A longitudinal pilot study of resilience in Canadian military personnel. Stress Health. 2014;30(5):377–385. doi: 10.1002/smi.2614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hjemdal O, et al. The relationship between resilience and levels of anxiety, depression, and obsessive–compulsive symptoms in adolescents. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2011;18:314–321. doi: 10.1002/cpp.719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.