Skip to main content
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience logoLink to Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
. 2021 Apr 16;16(9):926–936. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab047

Neurophysiological contributors to advantageous risk-taking: an experimental psychopharmacological investigation

Jennifer K MacCormack 1,2, Emma Armstrong-Carter 3, Kathryn L Humphreys 4, Keely A Muscatell 5,6,7,
PMCID: PMC8421704  PMID: 33860790

Abstract

The ability to learn from experience is critical for determining when to take risks and when to play it safe. However, we know little about how within-person state changes, such as an individual’s degree of neurophysiological arousal, may impact the ability to learn which risks are most likely to fail vs succeed. To test this, we used a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled design to pharmacologically manipulate neurophysiological arousal and assess its causal impact on risk-related learning and performance. Eighty-seven adults (45% female, Mage = 20.1 ± 1.46 years) took either propranolol (n = 42), a beta-adrenergic receptor blocker that attenuates sympathetic nervous system–related signaling, or a placebo (n = 45). Participants then completed the Balloon Emotional Learning Task, a risk-taking task wherein experiential learning is necessary for task success. We found that individuals on propranolol, relative to placebo, earned fewer points on the task, suggesting that they were less effective risk-takers. This effect was mediated by the fact that those on propranolol made less optimal decisions in the final phase of the task on trials with the greatest opportunity for advantageous risk-taking. These findings highlight that neurophysiological arousal supports risk-related learning and, in turn, more advantageous decision-making and optimal behavior under conditions of risk.

Keywords: beta-adrenergic blockade, propranolol, learning, risk-taking, arousal


Everyday life is filled with situations in which we must decide whether to take a risk or play it safe. Should we ask that attractive stranger for their number, try out that new restaurant, or risk not getting a health concern examined? Effective risky decision-making does not just involve estimations of chance but also requires learning from prior information and experience in order to predict the likelihood of positive or negative outcomes (Denrell, 2007; Ben-Elia et al., 2008). One classic illustration of learning-informed risk-taking is how drivers learn over time which roads have the least risk of traffic given the time of the day, weather conditions, etc. For example, during rush hour, drivers might risk using a shortcut but discover that this risky choice proved worse than their typical route, reducing their likelihood to risk similar shortcuts in the future during comparable traffic conditions.

Ultimately, what factors contribute to people’s ability to learn from experience in order to optimize when to play it safe vs take a risk? Most prior literature investigates the role of trait-based factors such as impulsivity or sensation-seeking in predicting risk-taking (e.g. Nigg, 2017; Khurana et al., 2018). This focus on trait-based predictors means we still know little about how state factors within the individual impact risk-taking, especially in contexts where experiential learning is critical to success. One long-standing state factor of interest has been arousal, with prior theory suggesting that some arousal is beneficial or facilitative for decision-making, especially when decisions are more intuitive, uncertain, ambiguous or risky (e.g. Bechara et al., 2000; Storbeck and Clore, 2008). Furthermore, a certain degree of arousal can support effective learning, as arousal helps sustain the attention needed for noticing and encoding information while also potentially promoting the acquisition of feedback for learning via exploration and experimentation (e.g. Yerkes and Dodson, 1908; Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Eldar et al., 2013). Arousal can be operationalized in several ways—subjectively (e.g. self-report), behaviorally (e.g. pupil dilation) or neurophysiologically [e.g. sympathetic nervous system (SNS) signaling]. Herein, we manipulated SNS-related neurophysiological arousal using the beta-blocker drug propranolol and investigated subsequent effects on risk-taking behavior in a task that requires learning from experience, with the key prediction that propranolol would ultimately impair advantageous risk-taking.

Advantageous risk-taking involves learning from experience

Building more accurate predictions from past experience (i.e. learning) is key for guiding advantageous risk decisions in real life outside the laboratory (Lo and Repin, 2002; Denrell, 2007; Ben-Elia et al., 2008). However, most prior laboratory research examines risk-taking in the context of gambling-based chance games wherein there is little opportunity for learning (Lejuez et al., 2002; Rogers et al., 2004; FeldmanHall et al., 2016). When learning-guided risk-taking is studied in the laboratory, the Iowa Gambling Task (Bechara et al., 1994) is often used. In this task, participants choose cards from four decks with different—initially unknown—average reward and punishment contingencies. As an implicit learning task, participants must learn from successive trials which decks produce advantageous vs disadvantageous outcomes. Although the Iowa Gambling Task allows for a behavioral test following implicit learning, the choices are forced choice (i.e. participants must choose a card) and categorical, resulting in fewer opportunities to learn and explore within each trial.

To address the need for a more dimensional, learning-driven risk-taking task, Humphreys and colleagues (Humphreys et al., 2013) created the Balloon Emotional Learning Task (BELT). In the BELT, individuals have more opportunity within each trial to explore the bounds of risk-taking (e.g. via balloon pumps) while learning across multiple trials which conditions afford more advantageous vs disadvantageous risks. The BELT thus offers an improvement over other implicit learning tasks (e.g. Iowa Gambling Task), as it captures more dimensional decision-based processes in contexts that support greater exploration within each trial rather than forced-choice decisions. Initial studies using the BELT suggested that a combination of dispositional factors is associated with maximal task performance (Humphreys et al., 2013). However, less research has examined the intraindividual mechanisms that contribute to learning about advantageous risk-taking. As such, we know little about how within-person state fluctuations influence learning about when it is most effective to take risks.

Theoretical role of arousal in risk-taking

Both theory and empirical research identify arousal as one fundamental intraindividual pathway that facilitates learning and effective risk-taking. Arousal supports diverse functions such as wakefulness, motivational states, attention to salient or evocative stimuli, encoding and retrieval in learning and memory, and affect-based perceptions and decisions (see discussion in Satpute et al., 2019). Arousal is derived from the integration of afferent autonomic signals from the periphery (e.g. the SNS) alongside signals from other neuromodulating pathways such as the adrenergic/noradrenergic, serotoninergic and dopaminergic systems (e.g. Robbins and Everitt, 1995; Coull et al., 1997; Critchley et al., 2000; Berridge, 2008; Kleckner et al., 2017; Satpute et al., 2019).

More generally, theories of arousal such as affect-as-information theory posit that individuals implicitly use their momentary feelings (e.g. arousal rooted in afferent physiological signals) to evaluate contextual cues and make decisions that drive behavior (Schachter and Singer, 1962; Clore et al., 2001; Storbeck and Clore, 2008; Schwarz, 2010). Similarly, the somatic marker hypothesis suggests that physiological sensations during and after decisions help individuals better determine whether it will be advantageous to make that decision again in future (Damasio, 1994, 1999; Bechara et al., 1999). Finally, predictive inference models of affect argue that the brain uses both a priori knowledge and ongoing afferent physiological signals (including arousal) to interpret contextual cues and inform behavior (Barrett and Simmons, 2015; Barrett, 2017; MacCormack and Lindquist, 2017). Ultimately, these theories suggest that the predictions built through experiential learning should interact with the neurobiology underpinning arousal to improve decision-making under conditions of uncertainty (e.g. risk). Risky decisions, as assessed in this study with a learning task rather than a gambling task and combined with the power of pharmacological blockade, provide a valuable model for testing these theory-driven hypotheses.

Neurobiological evidence for arousal in learning and risk-taking

Consistent with the theoretical insights above, a long history of studies in animals and humans show that arousal and arousal-related neurobiology are key mediators of effective learning and risk-taking. For instance, economic traders who exhibited greater autonomic responses during market trades and those who were more interoceptively aware of their physiological sensations make more advantageous decisions compared with their colleagues (Lo and Repin, 2002; Kandasamy et al., 2016). Arousal also appears to influence gamblers’ ability to judge situations and risks effectively (Tranel, 2000). Conversely, blunted arousal or the impaired ability to perceive physiological signaling may hinder learning about advantageous risk-taking (Critchley et al., 2001). For example, during the Iowa Gambling Task, individuals with medical conditions that weaken afferent peripheral signals selected riskier options and performed worse compared with healthy individuals (Busemeyer and Stout, 2002; Yechiam et al., 2005). Neurophysiological arousal may be particularly important for guiding decisions in ambiguous contexts when more information is needed to perform optimally (Zink et al., 2004; FeldmanHall et al., 2016).

Not only is there promising behavioral evidence for the role of arousal in facilitating advantageous risk-taking, but also there is compelling neurobiological evidence that the SNS and adrenergic/noradrenergic systems matter for both learning and risk-taking (Sara, 2009). The SNS is a fast-acting branch of the autonomic nervous system that helps initiate changes across the cardiovascular system and other modalities (e.g. pupil dilation, sweat) in response to environmental stimuli. As such, the SNS facilitates heightened action-readiness and vigilance to environmental cues, providing richer information when making decisions (Ruffolo, 1991; Blascovich et al., 2011). SNS activation itself is largely instigated by the adrenergic/noradrenergic systems via binding of the catecholamines epinephrine and norepinephrine to beta-adrenergic receptors throughout the body and brain.

Classic rodent experiments demonstrate that knockout, lesioning or blockade of SNS-related neurobiology reduces learning across multiple domains (e.g. motor, spatial, taste, affective), while increasing reactivity to novel stimuli and modulating arousal-driven memory consolidation and reconsolidation (Decker et al., 1990; Heron et al., 1996; Cahill et al., 2000; Clayton and Williams, 2000; Spreng et al., 2001; Myers and Rinaman, 2002; Dębiec and Ledoux, 2004; Miranda et al., 2008; Gazarini et al., 2013; Giustino and Maren, 2018). SNS-related signaling further appears to regulate learning through trial-and-error (Amemiya et al., 2016), which underscores how arousal-related neurobiological systems may drive learning through the accumulation of priors. More recently, parallel evidence has been observed in humans, wherein SNS-related signaling can alter learning and memory across many domains, including in the affective contexts of reward, threat, and uncertainty (Coull et al., 1997; Kroes et al., 2010; Mihov et al., 2010; Soeter and Kindt, 2011; Marshall et al., 2016; Chae et al., 2019). For instance, recent evidence suggests that the same neurobiology also helps regulate prediction updating in humans during learning tasks (Jepma et al., 2018).

In addition to arousal-related neurobiology supporting learning, these systems are firmly implicated in the computation of risk and resultant decisions and behaviors. Prior experiments suggest that pharmacologically attenuating SNS activation using beta-blockers such as propranolol to disrupt beta-adrenergic signaling can impair cognitive processes related to advantageous risk-taking. Yet, most of this work has been conducted in the context of chance-based gambling tasks. For example, individuals randomly assigned to take propranolol were less able to discriminate large potential losses and gains from small ones, in order to guide advantageous gambling decisions (Rogers et al., 2004). Propranolol has also been shown to reduce the ability to track and refer to recent experiences (Lempert et al., 2017), reduce aversion to monetary loss (Sokol-Hessner et al., 2015b) and diminish amygdala-driven modulation of memory in contexts of chance (Phelps, 2006). Despite this work suggesting that beta-adrenergic signaling causally influences decision-making during chance-based gambling, we understand little about whether this same pathway impacts risk-taking during situations in which learning from experience is crucial for success.

Present study

The present study thus used propranolol, a beta-blocker that blocks the SNS-related effects of epinephrine and norepinephrine at the sites of Inline graphic1 and Inline graphic2 adrenoceptors (Turner et al., 1965) in order to blunt neurophysiological arousal. Specifically, in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled mechanistic trial, participants took a single 40 mg dose of propranolol or a placebo and completed the BELT to examine beta-adrenergic impacts on learning and advantageous risk-taking. We hypothesized that individuals on propranolol (vs placebo) would learn the task parameters less effectively and thus take fewer advantageous risks, due to blunted access to neurophysiological arousal.

Methods

Participants

Data presented here were collected as part of a larger project examining how beta-adrenergic receptor blockade impacts reactivity to stress (MacCormack et al., 2021; MacCormack et al., in press). None of the data herein are published elsewhere. Participants were recruited from the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill and its surrounding community via flyers, class announcements, and email listservs, and then screened for eligibility via telephone interview and an in-person visit. Individuals were excluded if they: reported prior/current use of beta-blockers, cigarettes, substances or prescription medications; had a history of/current physical or mental illness, a pacemaker, known cardiac irregularities, body mass index (BMI) over 33 or if they exhibited low resting diastolic blood pressure (DBP < 80 Hg/ml) or heart rate (HR < 60 bpm), given that low BP/HR are contraindications for propranolol. Of the 90 total participants enrolled in the study, three had missing BELT data due to computer error. The remaining 87 participants (45% female; Mage = 20.1 ± 1.46 years, 18–25 years; 56% White, 25% Asian, 9% Black, 7% bi- or multiracial and 2% other) are included herein, with n = 42 randomly assigned to take propranolol and n = 45 randomly assigned to take placebo. Drug groups were randomized such that they were matched on sex [t(85) = .074, P = 0.942] and race/ethnicity [Inline graphic2(4, N = 87)= 1.25, P= 0.870]. See Table 1 for full participant characteristics and the supplementary materials (SMs) for details on statistical power.

Table 1.

Participant characteristics

Demographics Placebo Propranolol Total
Sex, n (%)
    Female 20 (23.0) 19 (21.8) 39 (44.8)
    Male 25 (28.7) 23 (26.4) 48 (55.2)
Race, n (%)
    Asian descent 11 (12.6) 11 (12.6) 22 (25.3)
    African descent 5 (5.7) 3 (3.4) 8 (9.2)
    European descent 26 (30.0) 23 (26.4) 49 (56.3)
    Bi- or multiracial 2 (2.3) 4 (4.6) 6 (6.9)
    Other 1 (1.1) 1 (1.1) 2 (2.3)
Age, mean ± SD 20.49 ± 1.59 20.07 ± 1.30 20.28 ± 1.45
BMI, mean ± SD 22.96 ± 2.38 22.47 ± 2.52 22.72 ± 2.45
Objective socioeconomic status (SES), mean ± SD 16.48 ± 1.95 16.24 ± 1.88 16.36 ± 1.92

Frequency counts show percentages of total sample. The “Total” column for Age, BMI, and SES represents the mean and SD for the full sample. Objective SES was operationalized as the mean years of education that both parents completed. There were no significant differences between drug groups (as tested by Pearson’s chi-square and independent samples t-tests) in sex, race, age, BMI or objective SES (all P values > 0.10).

Procedures

Participants received either a visually identical propranolol (40 mg) or placebo tablet, which they self-administered orally under supervision. A single propranolol dose of 40 mg was chosen given that higher doses may have lowered HR/BP to the point of causing fainting in our healthy, young adult sample, and given that 40 mg is a common clinical dosage administered for one-time performance anxiety situations (e.g. Currie et al., 1988; Alexander et al., 2007; Ernst et al., 2016). Given that this was part of a larger study examining stress (see Open Science Framework (OSF)), all participants in both conditions first completed a standard laboratory paradigm designed to elicit social stress (Kirschbaum et al., 1993), reported their affective responses, and provided biological samples. Two hours after completing the stressor and 3.5 h after ingesting the propranolol or placebo, participants completed the BELT. Given that the half-life of propranolol is 5 h after oral administration (Paterson et al., 1970; Williams et al., 1986), propranolol was still in effect during this task. Participants were compensated $100 USD and discharged after confirming that their HR and BP had returned to baseline levels. Procedures were approved by the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill’s Human Subjects Protection Committee (IRB #16-2498).

Measures

BELT.

To measure risk-taking and learning, participants completed the BELT, a computer task in which participants make decisions about how much to pump up three different colored balloons in order to obtain the highest score. Participants were told that the more points they earned in the game, the more money they would receive as an extra reward.

Participants pumped three different types of balloons that differed by color (blue, pink and orange). Each successful pump was worth one point regardless of balloon color, but each balloon color exploded after a different number of pumps. Specifically, certain-long balloons always exploded at 20 pumps, certain-short balloons always exploded at 8 pumps and uncertain balloons were unpredictable, exploding at 8, 14, or 20 pumps, depending on the trial. Participants were not told that balloon colors signified different explosion points, but they were explicitly told that not all balloons explode at the same point. Thus, to perform well on the task, participants needed to learn the strength of each balloon type (i.e. color). To make the most advantageous decisions on when to continue vs stop pumping, participants had to learn that certain-long balloons could be pumped the most and would yield the greatest number of points, certain-short balloons could only be pumped a few times and yielded fewer points but were still predictable, whereas uncertain balloons could sometimes be pumped many times and thus yield many points, but were risky because they would sometimes explode quickly.

To track learning effects, the BELT is divided into three separate task phases (Humphreys et al., 2013). Participants first complete an early phase (first 1/3 of trials), wherein they know little about which balloons are the least vs most risky. This is when we would expect participants to experiment and learn through trial-and-error. The second or mid-phase allows individuals to continue learning and fine-tuning their risk predictions based on the early phase. Finally, the third or late phase is where individuals can most fully apply whatever information they gained from the prior phases (if they learned effectively) in order to make the most advantageous risk decisions. Ultimately, we expected that if individuals are effectively learning about risk throughout the task, then by the late phase, they should be at their most effective in judging when to vs not to pump up balloons further.

There were 18 trials per balloon type across the entire task (54 trials in total), and for each third of the task, there was an equal number of trials of each balloon type. This task was identical to that used in prior work (Humphreys et al., 2013), except that we doubled the number of trials, allowing us to examine learning over a longer period of time and providing more opportunities for participants to explore and learn the different balloon contingencies. Participants pressed the spacebar to ‘pump up’ balloons. After the first pump, participants could press another button to ‘cash in’ their pumps for points, or they could continue pumping the balloons. Points accumulated across the course of the entire task. If participants pumped beyond a balloon’s limit, an explosion occurred, resulting in the loss of all points for that trial. We examined two primary outcomes from the BELT: (1) number of points, which served as our measure of overall task performance and (2) number of pumps, which served as our measure of risk-taking. Finally, as a secondary measure of risk-taking—and more specifically, untempered risk-taking, we examined (3) the number of explosions that an individual incurred. Given that we doubled the number of trials compared with prior work (e.g. Humphreys et al., 2013), in analyses, we first replicated prior findings with this lengthened task in the placebo group to confirm that participants effectively learned task parameters (see SMs).

Covariates.

Both negative, high arousal affect post-stressor and BMI were examined as covariates, to assess whether the stressor from 2-h previously had any lingering effects on BELT performance, and whether BMI altered dosage effects of propranolol. There were no main effects or interactions of either covariate with propranolol or the BELT task parameters in predicting outcomes (see SMs for full details and results).

Data analyses

Following prior analytical approaches with the BELT (Humphreys et al., 2013), we examined task outcomes (i.e. points, pumps, explosions) by balloon type (i.e. certain-long, certain-short, uncertain) and by task phase (i.e. early, mid, late). Table 2 displays descriptive statistics for the BELT outcomes split by drug. We conducted three separate mixed ANOVAs (with points, pumps and explosions as the outcome, respectively), with balloon type (certain-long, certain-short, uncertain) × task phase (early, mid, late) as within-subjects predictors and drug (0 = placebo, 1 = propranolol) as a between-subjects predictor. Significant interactions were probed via ANOVAs within each specific task phase and balloon type, to minimize the inflation of a Type 1 error due to multiple pairwise comparison testing (Kao and Green, 2008). Results presented herein are the main effects of drug, and interactions of drug with the within-subjects variables (e.g. task phase, balloon type). Full results are presented in the SMs.

Table 2.

Mean points, pumps and explosions by balloon condition, drug and task phase

Task phase
Early phase Mid-phase Late phase
Outcome Balloon Drug M SD M SD M SD
Pointsa Certain-long Placebo 55.02 18.34 66.98 23.53 73.69 27.54
Propranolol 49.38 15.07 60.83 25.37 59.95 23.53
Certain-short Placebo 16.31 8.86 24.18 11.02 26.07 11.21
Propranolol 16.14 6.57 19.57 9.81 22.14 11.63
Uncertain Placebo 37.07 13.35 35.40 13.17 39.29 11.25
Propranolol 26.69 10.26 34.10 13.61 29.02 13.21
Pumpsb Certain-long Placebo 59.60 21.94 71.44 27.49 79.13 29.93
Propranolol 55.69 23.22 64.26 28.99 66.29 29.72
Certain-short Placebo 42.31 4.96 41.82 4.34 40.73 4.86
Propranolol 40.19 7.03 40.52 7.25 39.90 7.25
Uncertain Placebo 48.53 12.02 48.53 12.03 49.89 10.34
Propranolol 50.45 15.26 49.79 13.56 51.05 16.06
Explosionsb Certain-long Placebo 0.33 0.56 0.38 0.65 0.42 0.69
Propranolol 0.41 0.73 0.24 0.43 0.52 0.77
Certain-short Placebo 3.38 1.50 2.24 1.69 1.96 1.68
Propranolol 3.07 1.40 2.69 1.81 2.38 1.96
Uncertain Placebo 1.67 1.07 1.56 1.39 1.20 1.31
Propranolol 1.38 1.08 1.83 1.53 1.31 1.28

Points represent performance on the BELT, pumps represent risk-taking and explosions represent untempered risk-taking. The total task contained 54 trials, with 18 trials (six of each balloon type) in each phase of the task.

a

In each task phase, participants could earn a maximum of 114 points from the certain-long balloons, 42 points from the certain-short balloons, and variable number of points from the uncertain balloons.

b

In each task phase, the certain-long balloon exploded on the 20th pump (with a maximum number of 114 possible safe pumps and up to six explosions), the certain-short balloons exploded on the 8th pump (with a maximum number of 42 possible safe pumps and up to six explosions) and uncertain balloons exploded on the 8th, 14th or 20th pumps (with a variable number of maximum possible safe pumps and up to six explosions). For any trial where a balloon exploded, all points on that trial were lost.

After testing main effects of drug and interactions, we ran a mediation model using SPSS PROCESS Model 4 (Hayes, 2012), in order to model a simple mediation between (a) the predictor of drug (0 = placebo, 1 = propranolol), (b) the primary mediator of interest, pumps made in the late phase with the certain-long balloon, and (c) the primary outcome, total number of points overall achieved across the entire BELT. To assess the indirect effect (a*b), we used a nonparametric boot-strap procedure with replacement (N = 5000) with 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals (CIs). If the CIs did not include zero, the indirect effect was considered statistically significant.

Results

Beta-adrenergic blockade reduces overall task performance

To examine whether SNS signaling via beta-adrenergic receptors affects the overall ability to perform well on the BELT, we assessed the effects of propranolol on the number of points earned (Tables 3–4). As shown in Figure 1, there was a main effect of drug, F(1, 84) = 4.86, P= 0.030, partial η2 = 0.055, such that participants on propranolol (M = 337.74, SD= 72.50) earned fewer points overall in the task overall relative to those on placebo (M = 373.96, SD= 82.44). This suggests that attenuated beta-adrenergic signaling impaired overall task performance. There were no two- or three-way interactions of task phase or balloon type with drug on total points earned across the task (see Table 3).

Table 3.

Mixed effects ANOVAs assessing overall effects of propranolol, balloon type and BELT task phase on BELT points earned, pumps made and explosions, controlling for negative, high arousal affect

Points model Pumps model Explosions model
Predictors df F P partial η2 F P partial η2 F P partial η2
Between-subject effects
Intercept 1 224.34 0.000 0.728 192.41 0.000 0.699 56.63 0.000 0.403
Drug 1 4.86 0.030 0.055 1.90 0.172 0.022 0.16 0.688 0.002
Affect 1 0.22 0.641 0.003 0.55 0.463 0.007 0.56 0.456 0.007
Error 84
Within-subject effects
Balloon 2 35.26 0.000 0.296 11.94 0.000 0.124 20.43 0.000 0.196
Balloon × Drug 2 2.65 0.092 0.031 2.22 0.135 0.026 0.23 0.728 0.003
Balloon × Affect 2 0.38 0.613 0.004 0.19 0.713 0.002 0.14 0.808 0.002
Balloon (Error) 168
Task phase 2 3.08 0.051 0.035 2.36 0.115 0.027 0.70 0.491 0.008
Task phase × Drug 2 1.57 0.213 0.018 0.38 0.616 0.004 2.43 0.094 0.028
Task phase × Affect 2 0.01 0.984 0.000 0.40 0.599 0.005 0.88 0.412 0.010
Task phase (Error) 168
Balloon × Task 4 2.72 0.034 0.031 6.53 0.000 0.072 2.86 0.031 0.033
Balloon × Task × Drug 4 1.32 0.264 0.015 3.91 0.011 0.044 1.52 0.203 0.018
Balloon × Task × Affect 4 0.70 0.584 0.008 0.78 0.500 0.009 1.40 0.239 0.016
Balloon × Task (Error) 336

Drug was coded 0 = Placebo, 1 = Propranolol. Affect refers to post-stressor mean negative, high arousal affect. Balloon included three types: certain-long, certain-short and uncertain. Task included three phases or averaged timepoints: the early task phase, mid-task phase, and the late task phase. Given that the repeated measures of balloon type and task phase were significant in Mauchly’s test of sphericity (P< 0.000), model P-values reported use the Greenhouse–Geisser correction. Significant effects are bolded.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Points earned by placebo and propranolol groups across the task. There was a main effect of drug, F(1, 84) = 4.85, P= 0.030, partial η2 = 0.055, such that participants on propranolol (M = 337.74, SD= 72.50) earned fewer points across the task than those on placebo (M = 373.96, SD= 82.44). Error bars are standard errors.

Beta-adrenergic blockade reduces advantageous risk-taking

To examine how SNS signaling via beta-adrenergic receptors impacts advantageous risk-taking, we assessed the effects of propranolol on the number of pumps made. There was no main effect of drug, although participants on propranolol on average made fewer pumps overall (M = 457.14, SD= 116.02) relative to those on placebo (M = 487.36, SD= 98.93). There was, however, a significant three-way interaction between drug, balloon type and task phase, F(4, 336) = 3.91, P= 0.011, partial η2 = 0.044 (see Table 3). To probe this interaction, we ran mixed ANOVAs within each task phase (early, mid, late) to examine the main effects and interaction of balloon type and drug. Within the late task phase (but not the early or mid-phases), we also found a significant interaction of balloon type × drug, F(2, 168) = 4.52, P= 0.027, partial η2 = 0.051 (see Table S1 in SMs).

To probe this interaction further, we conducted three separate ANOVAs within the late task phase for each balloon type (Table 4). In each of these three models, drug was the independent variable (i.e. between-subjects factor) and pumps in the late phase was the dependent variable. As shown in Figure 2, the difference in pumps in the late task phase between drug groups was only significant for the certain-long balloon (i.e. the balloon type with the greatest opportunity for advantageous risk-taking), F(1, 84) = 4.39, P= 0.039, η2 = 0.050. Specifically, individuals on propranolol pumped the certain-long balloon less (M = 66.29, SD= 29.72) than those on placebo (M = 79.13, SD= 29.93) in the late task phase. This effect of drug in the late task phase was not observed for the certain-short nor uncertain balloons.

Table 4.

Univariate ANOVAs probing overall effects of propranolol on BELT pumps in the late task phase split by balloon type, controlling for negative, high arousal affect

Long-certain balloon model Short-certain balloon model Uncertain balloon model
Predictors df F P η2 F P η2 F P η2
Between-subject effects
Intercept 1 72.19 0.000 0.462 479.18 0.000 0.851 153.86 0.000 0.647
Drug 1 4.39 0.039 0.050 0.58 0.448 0.007 0.11 0.744 0.001
Affect 1 0.61 0.438 0.007 0.92 0.339 0.011 0.22 0.639 0.003
Error 84

Drug was coded 0 = Placebo, 1 = Propranolol. Affect refers to post-stressor mean negative, high arousal affect. Significant effects are bolded.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Pumps across task phases, split by drug group, within the certain-long balloon. The difference in pumps between drug groups was only significant for the certain-long balloon in the final phase of the task, F(1, 84) = 4.39, P= 0.039, η2 = 0.050, and not the first two task phases (P> 0.05), such that the propranolol group (M = 66.29, SD= 29.72) pumped less in the final third of the task than did the placebo group (M = 79.13, SD= 29.93). Error bars are standard errors.

As a secondary measure of risk-taking—and more specifically, untempered risk-taking, we examined the number of explosions. Although the propranolol group exploded more balloons on average (M = 13.07, SD= 6.32) than those on placebo (M = 12.38, SD= 5.82), there was no main effect of drug, nor any two-way or three-way interactions between drug, balloon type and task phase (Table 3). These findings suggest that people in both the propranolol and placebo conditions exploded balloons at a similar rate.

Mediation linking propranolol with reduced task performance

Finally, in a mediation model, we examined if decreased pumping of the certain-long balloon during the final phase explained why individuals on propranolol scored fewer points overall relative to those on placebo. As shown in Figure 3, all paths were significant (P< 0.00–0.03), with a significant total effect (c = −37.40, SE = 16.95, P = 0.028). The indirect (a*b) effect was also significant, 95% CIs [−61.78, −2.17], demonstrating mediation. This suggests that blunted neurophysiological arousal (i.e. via SNS-related beta-adrenergic signaling) among those on propranolol disrupted optimal performance in part because it decreased effective learning about which risks were advantageous, particularly in the task condition with the most opportunity for risk-taking.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Mediation model. The link between drug group and higher overall BELT performance scores (total accumulated points across the entire task) was mediated via a significant indirect (a*b) effect of pumps made in the late phase with the certain-long balloon, estimated between 95% CIs [−61.78, −2.17]. Because zero was not within the 95% CI, the indirect effect was significantly different from zero at P< 0.05. Note all paths are significant but see “Results” for specific details.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to examine how in-the-moment neurophysiological arousal impacts learning which risks are likely to be rewarded vs detrimental. In a sample of healthy young adults, we pharmacologically manipulated SNS-related beta-adrenergic signaling, a key contributor to neurophysiological arousal, and examined consequent effects on risk-taking during a task in which learning from experience is critical for success. We found that individuals randomly assigned to take propranolol earned fewer points in the task than those on placebo, suggesting that blockade of beta-adrenergic signaling impaired performance. Moreover, mediation analysis suggested that attenuated beta-adrenergic signaling impaired performance in part because it reduced learning about which risks (i.e. balloon pumps) were advantageous. Together, these results suggest that a certain amount of neurophysiological arousal can help individuals more effectively learn over time which risks are advantageous, ultimately optimizing decision-making performance.

Specifically, we found that individuals with full access to their neurophysiological signals (i.e. those on a placebo) took more risks compared with those with attenuated neurophysiological arousal (i.e. those on propranolol), but only in the task condition that allowed the most risk-related exploration (i.e. the balloon that exploded the slowest) and only toward the end of the task (i.e. in the last phase of the trials). Indeed, mediation analysis showed that the placebo group’s greater pumping during this condition partially explained their greater overall task performance. We take these findings as evidence that SNS-related beta-adrenergic signaling helped facilitate more effective information gathering and risk-related learning, leading those on placebo to ultimately take more advantageous risks. In contrast, there were no arousal effects on BELT performance in the early phase, as presumably both groups (both placebo and propranolol) were gathering information about the risky nature of each balloon type. Likewise, there were no effects of propranolol in the mid-phase of the task, suggesting that beta-adrenergic facilitation of risk-related learning may take time to unfold. Beta-adrenergic signaling also did not impact risk-taking behavior within the certain-short balloon type (i.e. balloons that quickly exploded consistently). Indeed, it appears that all participants quickly mastered the meaning of certain-short balloons, perhaps because quickly exploding balloons may be more surprising or easy to detect. Similarly, beta-adrenergic signaling did not impact risk-taking in the uncertain condition, in which balloons exploded seemingly at random, consistent with the notion that arousal could not facilitate effective learning when there were no predictable rules or parameters that could be inferred from the context.

Collectively, these findings are consistent with classic work wherein an optimal amount of arousal can be facilitative for performance (Yerkes and Dodson, 1908) whereas too little arousal (e.g. when blunted by beta-blockade) can impair performance. We did not test what might happen when there is a high degree of neurophysiological arousal (e.g. on administration of epinephrine or norepinephrine), but it is likely that this would likewise impair performance on the BELT, given that high arousal states and related neurophysiology can impair several domains of performance, memory and cognition (e.g. Wichary et al., 2016; Maran et al., 2017; Marko and Riečanský, 2018).

One possible psychological mechanism underlying these findings is that optimal neurophysiological arousal heightens attention and salience of low-level perceptual cues that augment performance. Supporting this interpretation, affect-as-information theory and related work posit that arousal provides valuable insight in part by heightening attention to important environmental stimuli (Storbeck and Clore, 2008; Critchley and Garfinkel, 2018). Indeed, SNS-derived physiological activation is known to amplify the sensitivity of sensory modalities associated with vigilance, such as pupil dilation (Bradley et al., 2008; Lempert et al., 2017). Supporting this, a recent study found that propranolol led individuals to commit to an early decision in an information sampling task, rather than continue to gather more information (Hauser et al., 2018). Moreover, there is emerging evidence that visceral afferent signals and interoceptive awareness thereof can more broadly enhance evaluations of risk and learning more generally (Sokol-Hessner et al., 2009, 2015a; Kandasamy et al., 2016; Pfeifer et al., 2017). Given that neurophysiological arousal facilitates the saliency of and attention to low-level perceptual cues in the environment, an optimal amount of arousal could, as part of learning, increase attention to the success vs failure of past and ongoing risky decisions, thus guiding effective decision-making.

A second possible psychological mechanism is that individuals on propranolol may have been less cognitively alert compared with those on placebo, which may have reduced their capacity to learn from task feedback (e.g. explosions or tracking of point gains). As propranolol lowers heart rate and blood pressure and can contribute to feelings of lethargy (Ko, 2002), individuals on propranolol may have exerted less effort in the task. In future studies, one way to assess this ‘effort’ hypothesis would be to collect trial-by-trial reaction times, as these could provide implicit measures of participant effort and risky decision deliberation. Unfortunately, we did not collect reaction time data during the BELT and thus can only speculate that propranolol-induced lethargy and/or a lack of alertness could be one pathway contributing to these effects.

This study had limitations. Although propranolol’s bioavailability peaks 1 h after ingestion, we did not administer the BELT until 3.5 h after participants took the medication. Although this is within the 5-h half-life of propranolol (Paterson et al., 1970; Williams et al., 1986), our effects may have differed or been stronger if the BELT was completed when the effect of propranolol was at their peak. Furthermore, we did not collect physiological measures proximal to BELT completion, which would have provided further confirmation that propranolol was still active. It is also possible that the stress task completed as part of the larger study influenced the present results, although we controlled for post-stressor negative, high arousal affect in analyses to reduce this possibility. Future replications and extensions wherein the BELT is completed at the peak of propranolol bioavailability and without preceding tasks would provide a more precise estimate of the effect of propranolol on advantageous risk-taking. In addition, future research should clarify the extent to which laboratory-based tasks such as the BELT generalize to real-world contexts wherein optimal performance is contingent upon higher-stakes learning, such as in classroom, health, and personal finance settings.

In sum, the present study adds to the growing literature on the role of arousal and SNS-related beta-adrenergic signaling as a key neurophysiological pathway subserving successful risk-taking and learning. These findings are important given that real-world risk-taking is often predicated upon experiential, adaptive learning processes (e.g. using predictions gained through trial and error) that support optimal risk-related decisions (Denrell, 2007; Pleskac, 2008). To our knowledge, this constitutes the first known causal evidence in humans that neurophysiological arousal instantiated by beta-adrenergic signaling contributes to our ability to learn when to take advantageous risks that lead to desired outcomes. More generally, these findings contribute to the growing understanding that physiology influences cognitive and behavioral processes (e.g. Eisenberger et al., 2017; Critchley and Garfinkel, 2018; MacCormack and Lindquist, 2018).

Supplementary Material

nsab047_Supp

Acknowledgements

We thank Dr Samantha Meltzer-Brody for prescribing propranolol and monitoring participant safety and the UNC Investigational Drug Service for providing blinding and dispensing medication. Finally, we thank Tristen Inagaki, Meghan Meyer and members of the Carolina Social Neuroscience and Health Laboratory for their comments on prior versions of this manuscript.

Contributor Information

Jennifer K MacCormack, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA; Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA.

Emma Armstrong-Carter, Graduate School of Education, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA.

Kathryn L Humphreys, Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, USA.

Keely A Muscatell, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA; Lineberger Comprehensive Cancer Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA; Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA.

Funding

This work was supported by the North Carolina Translational and Clinical Sciences Institute, which is supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health, through grant award number UL1TR002489. Additionally, Jennifer K. MacCormack received support from a Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award predoctoral fellowship from the National Institute on Aging (1F31AG055265-01A1) as well as a T32 postdoctoral fellowship from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (5T32HL007560-37) via the Department of Psychiatry of the University of Pittsburgh.

Conflict of interest

We have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Supplementary data

Supplementary data are available at SCAN online.

References

  1. Alexander J.K., Hillier A., Smith R.M., Tivarus M.E., Beversdorf D.Q. (2007). Beta-adrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility during stress. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 468–78. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amemiya S., Kubota N., Umeyama N., Nishijima T., Kita I. (2016). Noradrenergic signaling in the medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala differentially regulates vicarious trial-and-error in a spatial decision-making task. Behavioural Brain Research, 297, 104–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aston-Jones G., Cohen J.D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus-coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 403–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrett L.F. (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1–23. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barrett L.F., Simmons W.K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 419–29. doi: 10.1038/nrn3950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bechara A., Damasio A. R., Damasio H., & Anderson S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-13-05473.1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bechara A., Damasio H., Damasio A.R., Lee G.P. (1999). Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 5473–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-13-05473.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bechara A., Damasio H., Damasio A.R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295–307. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Ben-Elia E., Erev I., Shiftan Y. (2008). The combined effect of information and experience on drivers’ route-choice behavior. Transportation, 35, 165–77. doi: 10.1007/s11116-007-9143-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Berridge C.W. (2008). Noradrenergic modulation of arousal. Brain Research Reviews, 58, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.10.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Blascovich J., Vanman E., Mendes W., Dickerson S. (2011). Social Psychophysiology for Social and Personality Psychology. London, UK: SAGE Publications. doi: 10.4135/9781446287842. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bradley M.M., Miccoli L., Escrig M.A., Lang P.J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45, 602–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Busemeyer J.R., Stout J.C. (2002). A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: decomposing performance on the Bechara gambling task. Psychological Assessment, 14, 253–62. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.14.3.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cahill L., Pham C.A., Setlow B. (2000). Impaired memory consolidation in rats produced with β-adrenergic blockade. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 74, 259–66. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1999.3950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chae W.R., Metz S., Weise J., et al. (2019). Effects of glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activity on spatial learning and spatial memory in healthy young adults. Behavioural Brain Research, 373, 112072. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Clayton E.C., Williams C.L. (2000). Noradrenergic receptor blockade of the NTS attenuates the mnemonic effects of epinephrine in an appetitive light–dark discrimination learning task. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 74, 135–45. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1999.3946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Clore G.L., Gasper K., Garvin E. (2001). Affect as information. In: Forgas, J.P., editor. Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associate Publishers, 121–14. [Google Scholar]
  18. Coull J.T., Frith C.D., Dolan R.J., Frackowiak R.S.J., Grasby P.M. (1997). The neural correlates of the noradrenergic modulation of human attention, arousal and learning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 589–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01635.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Critchley H.D., Corfield D.R., Chandler M.P., Mathias C.J., Dolan R.J. (2000). Cerebral correlates of autonomic cardiovascular arousal: a functional neuroimaging investigation in humans. The Journal of Physiology, 523, 259–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00259.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Critchley H.D., Mathias C.J., Dolan R.J. (2001). Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation. Neuron, 29, 537–45. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00225-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Critchley H.D., Garfinkel S.N. (2018). The influence of physiological signals on cognition. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 13–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.08.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Currie D., Lewis R., McDevitt D., Nicholson A., Wright N. (1988). Central effects of beta-adrenoceptor antagonists. I-Performance and subjective assessments of mood. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 26, 121–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.1988.tb03378.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Damasio A.R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Putnam. [Google Scholar]
  24. Damasio A.R. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. San Diego, CA: Harcourt. [Google Scholar]
  25. Dębiec J., Ledoux J.E. (2004). Disruption of reconsolidation but not consolidation of auditory fear conditioning by noradrenergic blockade in the amygdala. Neuroscience, 129, 267–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Decker M.W., Gill T.M., McGaugh J.L. (1990). Concurrent muscarinic and β-adrenergic blockade in rats impairs place-learning in a water maze and retention of inhibitory avoidance. Brain Research, 513, 81–5. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91091-T. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Denrell J. (2007). Adaptive learning and risk taking. Psychological Review, 114, 177–87. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Eisenberger N.I., Moieni M., Inagaki T.K., Muscatell K.A., Irwin M.R. (2017). In sickness and in health: the co-regulation of inflammation and social behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42, 242–53. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Eldar E., Cohen J.D., Niv Y. (2013). The effects of neural gain on attention and learning. Nature Neuroscience, 16, 1146–53. doi: 10.1038/nn.3428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ernst M., Lago T., Davis A., Grillon C. (2016). The effects of methylphenidate and propranolol on the interplay between induced-anxiety and working memory. Psychopharmacology, 233, 3565–74. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4390-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. FeldmanHall O., Glimcher P., Baker A.L., Phelps E.A. (2016). Emotion and decision-making under uncertainty: physiological arousal predicts increased gambling during ambiguity but not risk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 1255–62. doi: 10.1037/xge0000205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gazarini L., Stern C.A.J., Carobrez A.P., Bertoglio L.J. (2013). Enhanced noradrenergic activity potentiates fear memory consolidation and reconsolidation by differentially recruiting 1- and -adrenergic receptors. Learning and Memory, 20, 210–9. doi: 10.1101/lm.030007.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Giustino T.F., Maren S. (2018). Noradrenergic modulation of fear conditioning and extinction. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 43. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hauser T.U., Moutoussis M., Purg N., Dayan P., Dolan R.J. (2018). Beta-blocker propranolol modulates decision urgency during sequential information gathering. The Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 7170–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0192-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hayes A.F. (2012). PROCESS: a versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling. University of Kansas, KS. [Google Scholar]
  36. Heron C., Gould T.J., Bickford P. (1996). Acquisition of a runway motor learning task is impaired by a beta adrenergic antagonist in F344 rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 78, 235–41. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00252-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Humphreys K.L., Lee S.S., Tottenham N. (2013). Not all risk taking behavior is bad: associative sensitivity predicts learning during risk taking among high sensation seekers. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 709–15. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jepma M., Brown S.B.R.E., Murphy P.R., et al. (2018). Noradrenergic and cholinergic modulation of belief updating. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30, 1803–20. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kandasamy N., Garfinkel S.N., Page L., et al. (2016). Interoceptive ability predicts survival on a London trading floor. Scientific Reports, 6, 32986. doi: 10.1038/srep32986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kao L.S., Green C.E. (2008). Analysis of variance: is there a difference in means and what does it mean? Journal of Surgical Research, 144, 158–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jss.2007.02.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Khurana A., Romer D., Betancourt L.M., Hurt H. (2018). Modeling trajectories of sensation seeking and impulsivity dimensions from early to late adolescence: universal trends or distinct sub-groups? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47, 1992–2005. doi: 10.1007/s10964-018-0891-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kirschbaum C., Pirke K.M., Hellhammer D.H. (1993). The ‘trier social stress test’—a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kleckner I.R., Zhang J., Touroutoglou A., et al. (2017). Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 1–14. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ko D.T. (2002). β-blocker therapy and symptoms of depression, fatigue, and sexual dysfunction. JAMA, 288, 351. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.3.351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kroes M.C.W., Strange B.A., Dolan R.J. (2010). Beta-adrenergic blockade during memory retrieval in humans evokes a sustained reduction of declarative emotional memory enhancement. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 3959–63. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5469-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lejuez C.W., Read J.P., Kahler C.W., et al. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 75–84. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lempert K.M., Lackovic S.F., Tobe R.H., Glimcher P.W., Phelps E.A. (2017). Propranolol reduces reference-dependence in intertemporal choice. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1394–401. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lo A.W., Repin D.V. (2002). The psychophysiology of real-time financial risk processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 323–39. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. MacCormack J.K., Armstrong-Carter E.L., Gaudier-Diaz M.M., et al. (2020). Beta-adrenergic contributions to emotion and physiology during an acute psychosocial stressor. Psychosomatic Medicine. Under review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. MacCormack J.K., Gaudier-Diaz M.M., Armstrong-Carter E.L., et al. (2021). Beta-adrenergic blockade blunts inflammatory and antiviral/antibody gene expression responses to acute psychosocial stress. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46, 756–62. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00897-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. MacCormack J.K., Lindquist K.A. (2017). Bodily contributions to emotion: Schechter’s legacy for a psychological constructionist view on emotion. Emotion Review, 9, 36–45. doi: 10.1177/1754073916639664. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. MacCormack J.K., Lindquist K.A. (2018). Feeling hangry? When hunger is conceptualized as emotion. Emotion, 19, 301–19. doi: 10.1037/emo0000422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Maran T., Sachse P., Martini M., et al. (2017). Lost in time and space: states of high arousal disrupt implicit acquisition of spatial and sequential context information. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 206. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Marko M., Riečanský I. (2018). Sympathetic arousal, but not disturbed executive functioning, mediates the impairment of cognitive flexibility under stress. Cognition, 174, 94–102. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Marshall L., Mathys C., Ruge D., et al. (2016). Pharmacological fingerprints of contextual uncertainty. PLOS Biology, 14, e1002575. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mihov Y., Mayer S., Musshoff F., Maier W., Kendrick K.M., Hurlemann R. (2010). Facilitation of learning by social-emotional feedback in humans is beta-noradrenergic-dependent. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3168–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Miranda M.I., Rodríguez-García G., Reyes-López J.V., Ferry B., Ferreira G. (2008). Differential effects of β-adrenergic receptor blockade in basolateral amygdala or insular cortex on incidental and associative taste learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 90, 54–61. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Myers E., Rinaman L. (2002). Viscerosensory activation of noradrenergic inputs to the amygdala in rats. Physiology and Behavior, 77, 723–9. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00925-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Nigg J.T. (2017). On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 361–83. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Paterson J.W., Conolly M.E., Dollery C.T., Hayes A., Cooper R.G. (1970). The pharmacodynamics and metabolism of propranolol in man. Pharmacologia Clinica, 2, 127–33. doi: 10.1007/BF00420338. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pfeifer G., Garfinkel S.N., Gould V.P., Sahota C.D., Betka K., Critchley H.D. (2017). Feedback from the heart: emotional learning and memory is controlled by cardiac cycle, interoceptive accuracy and personality. Biological Psychology, 126, 19–29. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Phelps E.A. (2006). Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 27–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Pleskac T.J. (2008). Decision making and learning while taking sequential risks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 34, 167–85. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Robbins T.W., Everitt B.J. (1995). Arousal systems and attention. In: Gazzaniga, M.S., editor. The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 703–20. [Google Scholar]
  65. Rogers R.D., Lancaster M., Wakeley J., Bhagwagar Z. (2004). Effects of beta-adrenoceptor blockade on components of human decision-making. Psychopharmacology, 172, 157–64. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1641-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Ruffolo R.R. (1991). Chirality in α- and β-adrenoceptor agonists and antagonists. Tetrahedron, 47, 9953–80. doi: 10.1016/S0040-4020(01)96045-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sara S.J. (2009). The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 211–23. doi: 10.1038/nrn2573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Satpute A.B., Kragel P.A., Barrett L.F., Wager T.D., Bianciardi M. (2019). Deconstructing arousal into wakeful, autonomic and affective varieties. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 19–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.01.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Schachter S., Singer J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Schwarz N. (2010). Feelings-as-information theory. In: Van Lange P., Kruglanski A., & Higgins E.T., editors. Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology. London: SAGE, 289–308. [Google Scholar]
  71. Soeter M., Kindt M. (2011). Noradrenergic enhancement of associative fear memory in humans. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 96, 263–71. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sokol-Hessner P., Hsu M., Curley N.G., Delgado M.R., Camerer C.F., Phelps E.A. (2009). Thinking like a trader selectively reduces individuals’ loss aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 5035–40. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0806761106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Sokol-Hessner P., Hartley C.A., Hamilton J.R., Phelps E.A. (2015a). Interoceptive ability predicts aversion to losses. Cognition and Emotion, 29, 695–701. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.925426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Sokol-Hessner P., Lackovic S.F., Tobe R.H., Camerer C.F., Leventhal B.L., Phelps E.A. (2015b). Determinants of propranolol’s selective effect on loss aversion. Psychological Science, 26, 1123–30. doi: 10.1177/0956797615582026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Spreng M., Cotecchia S., Schenk F. (2001). A behavioral study of alpha-1b adrenergic receptor knockout mice: increased reaction to novelty and selectively reduced learning capacities. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 75, 214–29. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2000.3965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Storbeck J., Clore G.L. (2008). Affective arousal as information: how affective arousal influences judgments, learning, and memory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1824–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00138.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Tranel D. (2000). Electrodermal activity in cognitive neuroscience: neuroanatomical and neuropsychological correlates. In: Lane, R.D., Nadel, L., editors. Series in Affective Science. Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion. New York: Oxford University Press, 192–224. [Google Scholar]
  78. Turner P., Granville-Grossman K.L., Smart J.V. (1965). Effect of adrenergic receptor blockade on the tachycardia of thyrotoxicosis and anxiety state. The Lancet, 286, 1316–8. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(65)92340-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Wichary S., Mata R., Rieskamp J. (2016). Probabilistic inferences under emotional stress: how arousal affects decision processes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29, 525–38. doi: 10.1002/bdm.1896. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Williams F.M., Leeser J.E., Rawlins M.D. (1986). Pharmacodynamics and pharmacokinetics of single doses of ketanserin and propranolol alone and in combination in healthy volunteers. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 22, 301–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Yechiam E., Busemeyer J.R., Stout J.C., Bechara A. (2005). Using cognitive models to map relations between neuropsychological disorders and human decision-making deficits. Psychological Science, 16, 973–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01646.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Yerkes R.M., Dodson J.D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18, 459–82. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. Zink C.F., Pagnoni G., Martin-Skurski M.E., Chappelow J.C., Berns G.S. (2004). Human striatal responses to monetary reward depend on saliency. Neuron, 42, 509–17. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00183-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

nsab047_Supp

Articles from Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES