Abstract
Background
Aging is associated with declining protective immunity and persistent low-grade inflammatory responses, which significantly contribute to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) pathogenesis. Detecting aging-related cerebral vulnerability associated with deterioration of the immune system requires from non-invasive biomarkers able to detect failures in the brain-immunity connection. Reduced levels of salivary lactoferrin (sLF), an iron-binding protein with immunomodulatory activity, have been related to AD diagnosis. However, it remains unknown whether decreased sLF is associated with increased cortical amyloid-beta (Aβ) load and/or with loss of cortical integrity in normal aging.
Methods
Seventy-four cognitively normal older adults (51 females) participated in the study. We applied multiple linear regression analyses to assess (i) whether sLF is associated with cortical Aβ load measured by 18F-Florbetaben (FBB)-positron emission tomography (PET), (ii) whether sLF-related variations in cortical thickness and cortical glucose metabolism depend on global Aβ burden, and (iii) whether such sLF-related cortical abnormalities moderate the relationship between sLF and cognition.
Results
sLF was negatively associated with Aβ load in parieto-temporal regions. Moreover, sLF was related to thickening of the middle temporal cortex, increased FDG uptake in the posterior cingulate cortex, and poorer memory. These associations were stronger in individuals showing the highest Aβ burden.
Conclusions
sLF levels are sensitive to variations in cortical Aβ load, structural and metabolic cortical abnormalities, and subclinical memory impairment in asymptomatic older adults. These findings provide support for the use of sLF as a non-invasive biomarker of cerebral vulnerability in the general aging population.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s13195-021-00891-8.
Keywords: Saliva, Lactoferrin, Aging, Amyloid-beta PET, FDG-PET, Cortical thickness
Background
Evidence suggests that aging modifies the composition of the oral microbiome favoring dysbiosis, increased infections, and persistent low-grade inflammation, which may ultimately compromise general health [1, 2]. Oral infections increase the morbidity and mortality risk in older individuals [3] and the susceptibility to develop systemic diseases, such as cardiovascular [4] and respiratory diseases [5], diabetes mellitus [6], rheumatoid arthritis [7], and dementia [8].
Lactoferrin (LF) is an iron-binding glycoprotein synthesized by neutrophil granulocytes and released by exocrine glands [9]. Among its multiple functions [10], LF contributes to host defense against infections by sequestering the ionic iron required for microbial growth [11–14]. In saliva, LF is modulated by the inflammatory state of the oral mucosa and plays a central role in regulating the oral microbiome [15].
LF has the capacity to penetrate into the brain parenchyma via receptor-mediated transcytosis [16–18]. Thus, exogenous administration of LF has been associated with improved spatial cognition through reductions in oxidative stress and inflammation in the hippocampus of aged mice [19], with reduced motor deficits in an induced mouse model of Parkinson disease (PD) [20, 21] and with decreased amyloid-beta (Aβ) aggregation and enhanced cognition in transgenic mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [22, 23]. In humans, LF has been shown to accumulate in dopamine neurons of PD patients [24] and in cortical regions affected by AD pathology [25–27], likely to attenuate disease consequences. In line with this, LF administration in AD patients led to a reduction of AD pathology and improvement of cognitive performance [28].
Recent evidence suggests that salivary LF (sLF) could be an early, non-invasive, and cost-effective AD biomarker [29, 30]. In these studies, we showed that sLF levels are reduced in patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD compared to healthy controls [29]. We next demonstrated that low concentrations of sLF were able to differentiate positive Aβ-MCI/AD patients not only from controls but also from patients with frontotemporal dementia [30]. However, it remains unknown whether sLF is related to cortical Aβ load in clinically normal older adults. This aspect deserves attention since Aβ depositions are ubiquitous in the brain of a high proportion of asymptomatic older individuals [31–33], and they are further considered an early pathological event in AD [34, 35]. To shed light into this, we evaluated associations between sLF and cortical Aβ burden measured with 18F-Florbetaben (FBB) amyloid positron emission tomography (PET) imaging. Our hypothesis is that reduced levels of sLF are related to increased Aβ load in clinically normal older adults, mainly in those cortical regions particularly vulnerable to accelerated aging and AD.
Regional patterns of cortical thinning and cortical hypometabolism are well-established signatures of prodromal and clinical AD [36–39]. However, patterns of cortical thickening and cortical hypermetabolism have also been associated with increased cerebral Aβ aggregates in asymptomatic older adults [40–43], likely signaling cerebral vulnerability and/or neural compensation in response to increased Aβ deposition. Based on these findings, we expect that sLF levels are negatively correlated with cortical thickness and cortical glucose metabolism in normal older individuals and that such relationships are stronger in those participants showing higher Aβ load. As previous studies have found associations between LF and improved memory [19, 44], we further predict that sLF-related variations in cortical thickness and/or FDG/FBB binding are associated with subclinical changes in memory function compared to other cognitive domains.
Methods
Subjects
Seventy-four cognitively normal older adults participated in the study (age = 66.5 ± 5.7 years; 51 females). They were recruited from senior citizen’s associations, health-screening programs, and hospital outpatient services. All of them underwent neurological and neuropsychological assessment to discard the presence of early signs of dementia and objective cognitive impairment. Participants met the following inclusion criteria: (i) normal global cognitive status in the MMSE (scores ≥ 26), (ii) normal cognitive performance in the neuropsychological tests relative to appropriate reference values for age and education level, (iii) global score of 0 (no dementia) in the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR), (iv) functional independence as assessed by the Spanish version of the Interview for Deterioration in Daily Living Activities [45], (v) scores ≤ 5 (no depression) in the short form of the Geriatric Depression Scale [46], and (vi) not be on medications that affect cognition. All participants gave informed consent to the experimental protocol approved by the Ethical Committee for Clinical Research of the Junta de Andalucía according to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki. Table 1 shows sample characteristics.
Table 1.
Total sample N = 74 |
Female N = 51 |
Male N = 23 |
|
---|---|---|---|
Age | 66.5 ± 5.7 | 66.0 ± 5.9 | 67.6 ± 5.1 |
Education years | 12.5 ± 4.9 | 12.5 ± 5.2 | 12.4 ± 4.1 |
ApoE4 (yes/no) | 16/58 | 11/40 | 6/17 |
CDR | 0 | 0 | 0 |
MMSE | 29.0 ± 1.2 | 28.9 ± 1.2 | 29.3 ± 1.1 |
Memory Binding Test | |||
Total free recall | 15.5 ± 4.4 | 15.8 ± 4.3 | 14.9 ± 4.7 |
Pairs in free recall | 5.9 ± 2.8 | 6.0 ± 2.7 | 5.8 ± 3.0 |
Total paired recall | 24.6 ± 4.2 | 24.6 ± 3.9 | 24.7 ± 4.8 |
Paired recall pairs | 9.7 ± 3.7 | 9.7 ± 3.8 | 9.7 ± 3.5 |
Total delayed free recall | 16.4 ± 4.9 | 16.6 ± 4.8 | 16.0 ± 5.1 |
Pairs in delayed free recall | 6.3 ± 3.1 | 6.4 ± 3.0 | 6.1 ± 3.3 |
Total delayed paired recall | 24.0 ± 4.6 | 23.9 ± 4.4 | 24.1 ± 5.2 |
Letter-Number Test | 9.0 ± 2.3 | 8.8 ± 2.1 | 9.7 ± 2.7 |
Digit span | 14.3 ± 3.2 | 14.1 ± 3.0 | 14.9 ± 3.4 |
D2 Test of Attention | 364.1 ± 94.0 | 355.6 ± 107.0 | 382.8 ± 52.8 |
TMT-A | 47.0 ± 21.5 | 47.6 ± 21.5 | 45.6 ± 22.0 |
TMT-B | 119.5 ± 67.7 | 123.4 ± 69.6 | 110.8 ± 63.8 |
Tower of London | 319.3 ± 113.2 | 338.6 ± 121.4 | 276.3 ± 78.8 |
Boston Naming Test | 12.1 ± 2.1 | 11.9 ± 2.1 | 12.5 ± 1.9 |
Salivary LF (μg/ml) | 6.2 ± 2.9 | 6.0 ± 2.9 | 6.5 ± 2.8 |
Saliva total protein (μg/ml) | 4.1 ± 1 | 4.1 ± 1.1 | 4.2 ± 1 |
Global Aβ load (SUVR) | 1.2 ± 0.3 | 1.1 ± 0.3 | 1.2 ± 0.3 |
FBB-PET (positive Aβ) | 10 | 7 | 3 |
Results are expressed as mean ± SD, unless otherwise stated. CDR Clinical Dementia Rating, MMSE Mini Mental State Examination, TMT-A and B Trail Making test (forms A and B), LF lactoferrin, SUVR standardized uptake value ratios, FBB Florbetaben
Neuropsychological assessment
The neuropsychological battery included different tests for assessment of memory, working memory, attention, executive function, and language. We computed a composite Z score for each cognitive domain comprising the following tests: (i) the Spanish version of the Memory Binding Test as an index of memory [47], (ii) the letter-number sequencing (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III) and the digit span (Wechsler Memory Scale-III) subtests as measures of working memory, (iii) the D2 test as an index of attention, (iv) the Trail Making test (TMT, forms A and B) and the Tower of London as measures of executive function, and (iv) the short form of the Boston Naming Test as a measure of language.
Quantification of sLF
Saliva samples were collected at 9:00–10:00 am in all subjects using the spitting method. Participants were asked to refrain from any oral stimulation (e.g., food, drink, smoke, chew gum, oral hygiene) or taking medication for at least 8 h prior to saliva collection. They were not allowed to wear lipstick or lip balm. The presence of blood contamination in saliva samples was excluded by visual inspection. Unstimulated whole saliva was collected into 50 ml conical sterile polypropylene tubes previously treated with 2% sodium azide solution. Samples were immediately placed on ice and centrifuged at 365g for 5 min at 4 °C. Supernatant saliva was then transferred into 1.5 ml polypropylene tubes, treated with a protease inhibitor cocktail (cOmplete Ultra Tablets mini, Roche, Basel, Switzerland), and stored at − 80 °C until analysis. Estimation of total protein concentration in saliva samples was performed with a bicinchoninic acid assay (BCA) (Pierce, Rockford, IL), according to the manufacturer’s instructions. We further correlated sLF with saliva protein levels to determine if salivary flow rate was associated with sLF concentration [48].
Levels of sLF were quantified using a commercially available ELISA kit (ab200015, Abcam, Cambridge, UK) according to manufacturer’s instructions. Briefly, we added 50 μL of saliva samples or standard to appropriate wells diluted 1:10,000 into sample diluent NS. All samples were tested in duplicate and the average of the two measurements (μg/ml) was used for statistical analyses. Mean intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation (CV) were 4.6% and 8%, respectively. Intra-assay CV values were below 20% in all subjects (Fig. 1A).
MRI and PET acquisition
Structural brain images were acquired on a Philips Ingenia 3 T MRI scanner equipped with a 32-channel head coil (Philips, Best, Netherlands). A whole-brain T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence was acquired in the sagittal plane using the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 2600 ms, echo time (TE) = 4.7 ms, flip angle = 9°, matrix = 384 × 384, voxel resolution = 0.65 mm3 isotropic, and no gap between slices. Head motion was minimized by using a head restraint system and placing foam padding around the subject’s head. Participants were provided with headphones and foam earplugs to attenuate scanner noise.
PET examinations (2-[18F]fluoro-2-deoxy-D-glucose—FDG—and 18F-Florbetaben—FBB) were performed in a Philips Gemini 16 PET/CT scanner (Philips, Best, Netherlands). FDG-PET assessments were followed by FBB-PET scans two weeks later. Subjects fasted for at least 8 h before FDG-PET examination. Participants were injected with 185 MBq of FDG in a quiet, dimly lit room. FDG-PET images were acquired 30 min after injection with scan duration of 10 min. For FBB-PET imaging, participants were injected with 300 MBq of [18F] FBB (NeuraCeq™, Piramal Pharma) 90 min before acquisition. PET data were corrected for radioactive decay, dead time, attenuation, and scatter. Cerebral PET images were reconstructed iteratively with an isotropic voxel resolution of 2 mm3. Participants underwent a 20-min FBB-PET scan in dynamic mode consisting of 4 frames of 5 min each. Each frame was inspected for excessive motion. As no excessive head motion was detected in scanned images, the four frames were averaged to create a single static FBB brain image used for quantitative analysis. All FBB-PET scans were assessed visually by an expert in nuclear medicine and quantitatively using a standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR) cutoff of 1.43 [49].
Estimation of cortical thickness, FDG, and FBB binding
MRI data were processed using the analysis pipeline of Freesurfer v6.0 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) that involves intensity normalization, registration to Talairach, skull stripping, white matter (WM) segmentation, tessellation of WM boundaries, and automatic correction of topological defects [50]. Pial surface misplacements and erroneous WM segmentation were manually corrected on a slice-by-slice basis to enhance the reliability of cortical thickness measurements. Individual cortical thickness maps were smoothed using non-linear spherical wavelet-based de-noising schemes, which have previously shown greater specificity and sensitivity than Gaussian spatial filters for detecting local and global changes in cortical thickness [51]. Partial volume correction (PVC) of FDG/FBB PET images was performed with PetSurfer, a toolbox for PET analysis implemented in Freesurfer v6.0. We employed the Geometric Transfer Matrix-derived region-based voxel-wise method [52], assuming a uniform 6 mm point spread function. Cerebral FDG/FBB images were first co-registered to T1 scans. Next, PVC-cortical FDG/FBB images were transformed into SUVR using the pons or the grey matter of cerebellum as reference regions, respectively. Resulting PVC-FDG/FBB cortical-to-pons/cerebellum SUVR images were mapped into individual cortical surfaces and smoothed with non-linear spherical wavelet-based de-noising schemes [51]. Additionally, we obtained global cortical Aβ values for each participant using a FBB composite comprising 4 large bilateral cortical regions: frontal (orbitofrontal cortex/inferior frontal gyrus/middle frontal gyrus/superior frontal gyrus/frontal pole), cingulate (anterior/posterior/isthmus cingulate), parietal (precuneus/inferior parietal cortex/superior parietal cortex/supramarginal gyrus), and lateral temporal cortex (middle temporal/superior temporal gyri) [53].
Statistical analysis
Simple linear regression was used to estimate the relationship of sLF with total protein content in saliva samples, while multiple linear regression was used to determine the association of sLF and global Aβ load with age and sex. We also applied multiple-linear regression analysis to test the hypothesis that sLF levels are associated with between-individual variations in cortical thickness and cortical FDG/FBB binding in clinically normal older adults. Cerebral dependent variables were Box-Cox transformed to improve normality and alleviate heteroscedasticity [54]. All statistical models, except the one performed with FBB binding, were adjusted for global Aβ load, age, and sex. The model that included FBB binding as dependent variable was only adjusted by age and sex. To determine whether the association of sLF with either cortical thickness or FDG uptake was moderated by global Aβ load, we added the sLF × global Aβ load interaction as explanatory variable. The two interaction models were adjusted by age, sex, sLF, and global Aβ load.
Vertex-wise regression analyses with cortical thickness and FDG/FBB binding as dependent variables were performed using the SurfStat package (https://www.math.mcgill.ca/keith/surfstat/). Results were corrected for multiple comparisons using a previously validated hierarchical statistical model [55]. This procedure first controls the family-wise error rate in significant clusters over smoothed statistical maps applying random field theory; and it next controls the false discovery rate in vertices of significant clusters over unsmoothed statistical maps. The anatomical location of clusters that survived correction was identified by the location of each cluster’s peak vertex on the Desikan-Killiany atlas [56]. For each regression analysis, we reported the maximum F (i.e., Roy’s largest root) and its associated p value, which was obtained by comparing either the additive or interactive model with a model with no regressors (i.e., a model that only included the intercept). For each post-hoc contrast, we reported the maximum R2 and its associated p value for each significant cluster. After inferential evidence of a main effect, we computed the standardized measure of effect size (i.e., Cohen’s f2), which allows an evaluation of local effect size within the context of a multivariate regression model [57]. To establish the precision of effect sizes, we computed 95% confidence intervals (CI95%) using the normal approximated interval with bootstrapped bias and standard error (N = 10.000 bootstrap samples) through the function bootci implemented in Matlab.
We next assessed associations between levels of sLF and cognition. Five cognitive domains were assessed: memory, working memory, attention, language, and executive function. To improve normality and heteroscedasticity, the model included as dependent variable the Yeo-Johnson transformation of composite Z scores for each cognitive domain [58]. We applied linear mixed modeling as this approach allows for random intercepts across participants, which, in turn, reduces de variance of fixed effect estimates [59]. In particular, we specified a four-step mixed effects model. The main predictors were added in the first step (i.e., cognitive domain, sLF, sLF-related variations in cortical Aβ load, cortical thickness and cortical FDG uptake, age, and sex), whereas the two-, three-, and four-way interactions were sequentially added in subsequent steps. We next applied ANOVAs to compare the different models. These analyses were performed with R Statistical Software v3.0.1 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).
Power analysis
Power analysis was performed with the G*Power software (v3.1.9.6) (https://www.psychologie.hhu.de/arbeitsgruppen/allgemeine-psychologie-und-arbeitspsychologie/gpower.html). We computed an a priori (prospective) power analysis (fixed model, R2 deviation from zero) to achieve a satisfactory power level of 80% given a α-level of 0.05 and a Cohen’s effect size (f) of 0.25. As sex was included as covariate of no interest, the number of predictors ranged between 6 and 10. Taking into account these parameters, a sample size between 62 and 74 participants was required to achieve the desired power.
Results
Demographics, cognitive and cerebral Aβ measures
Table 1 includes demographics, cognitive and cerebral Aβ measures of the total sample and stratified by sex. The sample comprised 74 cognitively normal elderly subjects (51 females and 23 males). Ten of them showed a positive FBB scan (7 females and 3 males). sLF was not correlated with saliva protein levels (r = − 0.03, p = 0.8), arguing against an effect of salivary flow rate on sLF levels in our study (Fig. 1B). Regression analyses indicated that there was no main effect of age or sex on sLF (Fig. 1C, D) or global Aβ load (Fig. 1E, F). The age × sex interaction was also not significant for any of these dependent variables. On the contrary, we found a significant main effect of age on cognition, which was mostly evident for the memory domain, but found no significant differences between sexes or between cognitive domains (see Supplementary material).
Relationship between sLF and cortical Aβ load
Regression analysis revealed a main effect of sLF on cortical FBB binding after adjustment by age and sex (F3,70 = 8.3, p < 10−4). The post-hoc regression analysis showed a negative relationship between sLF levels and FBB binding in areas of the left temporal and inferior parietal lobe ( = 0.22, tmax = 4.50, pcluster = 0.006; f2 = 0.28, CI95%[0.21 0.35]). Results derived from this analysis are shown in Fig. 2A.
Relationship between sLF and cortical thickness/cortical FDG uptake
After adjustment by global Aβ load, age, and sex, we also found a main effect of sLF on cortical thickness (F4,69 = 11,52, p < 10−5) and FDG uptake (F4,69 = 11.2, p < 10−4). Post hoc regression analyses showed a negative association of sLF with cortical thickness in areas of the left middle temporal lobe ( = 0.19, tmax = 4.15, pcluster = 0.008; f2 = 0.32, CI95%[0.25 0.38]) and with FDG uptake in areas of the right posterior cingulate cortex (rPCC) ( = 0.2, tmax = 4.19, pcluster = 10−5; f2 = 0.55, CI95%[0.48 0.62]). Results from these analyses are illustrated in Fig. 2B, C.
Moderating role of global Aβ load on the relationship between sLF and cortical thickness/cortical FDG uptake
To assess the putative role of global Aβ load as a moderator of the relationship between sLF and cortical thickness, we built a new model including the sLF × global Aβ load interaction term. The ANOVA comparing the additive and interaction models showed significant differences over the rPCC (F5,68 = 9.35, p < 10−5, = 0.32, tmax = 5.76, pcluster < 10−6; rho = 6.96, CI95%[3.8 10.1]). Figure 3A shows the results derived from this analysis on cortical surfaces and the scatter plot of the interaction effect. In particular, the correlation between sLF and cortical thickness was most negative for those individuals in the highest tertile of global Aβ load.
The interaction effect was also significant for the FDG uptake at the rPCC (F5,68 = 12.45, p < 10−4, = 0.29, tmax = 5.39, pcluster = 0.002; rho = 6.65, CI95%[4.4 8.9]). As illustrated in Fig. 3B, individuals in the highest tertile of global Aβ load showed a more negative relationship between sLF and cortical FDG uptake compared to those in the lowest tertile.
Moderating role of sLF-related cortical variations on the association of sLF with cognition
We first built a mixed-effect model with cognition as dependent variable, with cognitive domain, sLF, age, sex, and sLF-related changes in Aβ load (i.e., the mean value of the significant Aβ cluster in Fig. 2A), cortical thickness (i.e., the mean value of the significant thickness cluster in Fig. 2B) and FDG uptake (i.e., the mean value of the significant FDG cluster in Fig. 2C) as fixed effects, and with participants as random effect. Results indicated that cognition was negatively associated with sLF (F1,74 = 10.4, p = 0.002), sLF-related changes in Aβ load (F1,74 = 4.3, p = 0.04), and age (F1,74 = 15.8, p = 0.0002).
Next, we built an interaction model with 4 two-way interactions terms (i.e., cognitive domain × sLF, sLF-related changes in Aβ load, cortical thickness and FDG uptake). This model provided a better fit to the data than the additive model (χ2 = 38, p = 0.001). The ANOVA revealed a significant association of cognitive domain with sLF (F4,296 = 7, p = 0.00002) and with sLF-related changes in Aβ load (F4,296 = 4.2, p = 0.002). Post hoc analyses indicated that both sLF and sLF-related changes in Aβ load were negatively associated with memory when compared with the remaining cognitive domains. These results are illustrated in Fig. 4A and Fig. 4B, respectively.
We finally assessed whether these interactions were further moderated by sLF-related changes in either cortical thickness or cortical FDG uptake. But none of these models provided a better fit to the data when compared to the additive model or any of the interaction models mentioned above.
Discussion
In this study, we showed that aging-related sLF is negatively associated with regional Aβ load, particularly in parieto-temporal regions of the left hemisphere. Results further revealed that global Aβ load moderated the association of sLF with increased thickness and FDG uptake in the rPCC. Remarkably, both sLF and sLF-related changes in Aβ load were associated with poorer memory. Together, these findings provide evidence supporting the role of sLF as a biomarker of cerebral vulnerability in normal aging and contribute to extend our understanding of sLF in the continuum aging-AD.
In consonance with previous results obtained in MCI and AD patients [29, 30], decreased levels of sLF were related to increased Aβ burden in left parieto-temporal regions of cognitively normal older adults. We have recently hypothesized that reduced sLF observed in prodromal and clinical stages of AD may indicate early immunological disturbances that eventually increase the risk of AD [60]. Low levels of sLF may assist oral dysbiosis, which, in turn, may produce long-term infections and a pro-inflammatory response that weakens the blood brain barrier, facilitating colonization of brain tissue by periodontal bacteria [61] and accelerating neuroinflammation that contribute to AD pathology [62]. In this vein, previous studies have shown an association between periodontal disease and brain Aβ accumulation in normal aging, suggesting that periodontal inflammation/infection may accelerate brain Aβ deposition [63, 64]. Our results complement these findings, suggesting that reductions of sLF relate to increased regional Aβ burden, which may reflect early immunological alterations potentially associated with higher risk of developing AD [60]. Alternatively, a recent study has shown that levels of LF in either saliva or cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) were not able to differentiate between controls, MCI/AD, and non-AD dementias. Nor did they find an association between sLF and CSF AD biomarkers [65]. These conflicting results do not invalidate our results that were obtained in cognitively normal elderly subjects, emphasizing the potential role of sLF to track cerebral Aβ changes in late life.
The present study further showed that between individual reductions in sLF are associated with thickening of the left middle temporal lobe and increased FDG uptake in the rPCC. Accumulated evidence suggests that cortical thickening of middle temporal regions occurs in cognitively normal elderly subjects before the onset of AD symptoms [42, 43, 66–68]. Accordingly, post mortem human brain studies have shown hypertrophy of neuronal cell bodies, nuclei, and nucleoli in asymptomatic individuals with Aβ aggregates, likely revealing a very early reaction to cerebral Aβ burden and/or the activation of cellular processes in an attempt to prevent the natural progression of AD [69]. Previous works have further identified a hypermetabolic phase in cognitively normal older adults, affecting the posterior cingulate among other cortical regions, which became hypometabolic in later stages of AD [70]. Patterns of increased cortical FDG uptake have also been observed in association with increased accumulation of Aβ deposition in asymptomatic older adults [40] that may result from Aβ-related microgliosis [71], overproduction of inflammatory mediators [72], and/or aberrant hyperactivation of cortical neurons instigated by Aβ burden [73]. Therefore, the link between decreased sLF and abnormal patterns of cortical thickness/cortical FGD uptake may mirror the earliest Aβ-related effects on vulnerable cortical regions in late life.
Interestingly, associations between decreased sLF and cortical thickening/cortical hypermetabolism were mostly evident in those individuals showing the highest global Aβ load. While it is increasingly clear that accumulation of Aβ plaques is not sufficient to cause AD [74], the mere presence of Aβ aggregates has adverse effects that might increase the risk for AD such as impaired microvascular integrity, unbalanced glucose homeostasis, failure in neuronal cell cycle control, and inflammatory responses regulated by microglia and astrocytes [75]. The moderating role of global Aβ burden on the relationship between sLF and cortical thickening/cortical hypermetabolism may reveal aggravated aging-related immunity deficits in individuals with the highest global Aβ concentration. Whether this association may eventually increase the likelihood of developing AD has to be determined in future studies. In the context of aging, substantial cerebral Aβ load could lead to chronic neuroinflammation favoring deregulation of the immune system and, consequently, altering the intrinsic immunomodulatory role of LF [76].
In line with this hypothesis, sLF-related increases in parieto-temporal Aβ load predicted lower cognition, particularly affecting memory. However, this relationship was not moderated by sLF-associated changes in either cortical thickness or FDG uptake. Previous studies have found greater LF deposition in brain regions enriched with Aβ plaques [25, 27], which may be interpreted as an attempt to minimize the consequences of cerebral amyloidosis. On the other hand, exogenous administration of LF has shown to stimulate the non-amyloidogenic processing of amyloid precursor protein and α-secretase activity, with a consequent reduction in Aβ deposition that ameliorates cognitive decline in mouse models of AD [22]. Beneficial effects of LF for the maintenance of cellular and tissue homeostasis may be due to an increase of autophagy activity via AMPK signaling through the LRP1 receptor [77].
Limitations
The results of the current study are purely correlational, and we cannot infer any causal relationship between sLF, cerebral changes, and/or cognitive deficits. Furthermore, our findings were obtained with a relatively small sample and therefore they should be considered as preliminary and replicated in further experiments. Finally, additional studies should be carried out to determine if variations in sLF levels predict AD progression.
Conclusions
In summary, these findings provided first evidence relating sLF to regional Aβ load, cortical integrity, and poorer memory in clinically normal older adults. In particular, our results suggested that global Aβ load plays a major role in boosting associations between sLF and abnormal patterns of cortical thickness and cortical glucose metabolism, likely signaling early dysregulation of the immune system. Although these findings are preliminary, they represent a step further into the utilization of sLF as a non-invasive biomarker of cerebral vulnerability in the general aging population.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Abbreviations
- Aβ
Amyloid-β
- AD
Alzheimer’s disease
- FDG
2-[18F]fluoro-2-deoxy-D-glucose
- FBB
Florbetaben
- MCI
Mild cognitive impairment
- MMSE
Mini Mental State Examination
- MRI
Magnetic resonance imaging
- PET
Positron emission tomography
- PET-CT
Positron emission tomography-computed tomography
- PVC
Partial volume correction
- sLF
Salivary lactoferrin
- SUVR
Standardized uptake value ratios
- TMT
Trail making test
Authors’ contributions
JLC conceived the study and wrote the manuscript. JLC, MA, MFA, and LR contributed to the data acquisition, data analysis, and preparation of figures. EC performed lactoferrin determinations. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by research grants from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (PID2020-119978RB-I00 to JLC, PSI2017-85311-P and PID2020-118825GB-I00 to MA), Junta de Andalucía (PY20_00858 to JLC) Research Program for a Long-Life Society (grant number 0551_PSL_6_E to JLC), and CIBERNED (CB06/05/1111 to JLC).
Availability of data and materials
The datasets employed in the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All participants gave informed consent to the experimental protocol approved by the Ethical Committee for Clinical Research of the Junta de Andalucía according to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
Dr. Carro is co-founder of GEROA Diagnostics.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Garcia-Peña C, Alvarez-Cisneros T, Quiroz-Baez R, Friedland RP. Microbiota and aging. A review and commentary. Arch Med Res. 2017;48(8):681–689. doi: 10.1016/j.arcmed.2017.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Santoro A, Zhao J, Wu L, Carru C, Biagi E, Franceschi C. Microbiomes other than the gut: inflammaging and age-related diseases. Semin Immunopathol. 2020;42(5):589–605. doi: 10.1007/s00281-020-00814-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Garcia RI, Krall EA, Vokonas PS. Periodontal disease and mortality from all causes in the VA Dental Longitudinal Study. Ann Periodontol. 1998;3(1):339–349. doi: 10.1902/annals.1998.3.1.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Beck JD, Slade G, Offenbacher S. Oral disease, cardiovascular disease and systemic inflammation. Periodontol 2000. 2000;23:110–120. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0757.2000.2230111.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Scannapieco FA, Papandonatos GD, Dunford RG. Associations between oral conditions and respiratory disease in a national sample survey population. Ann Periodontol. 1998;3(1):251–256. doi: 10.1902/annals.1998.3.1.251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ritchie CS. Mechanistic links between type 2 diabetes and periodontitis. J Dent. 2009;37(8):S578–S579. doi: 10.1016/j.jdent.2009.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bingham CO, Moni M. Periodontal disease and rheumatoid arthritis: the evidence accumulates for complex pathobiologic interactions. Curr Opin Rheumatol. 2013;25(3):345–353. doi: 10.1097/BOR.0b013e32835fb8ec. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ming Y, Hsu SW, Yen YY, Lan SJ. Association of oral health-related quality of life and Alzheimer disease: a systematic review. J Prosthet Dent. 2020;124(2):168–175. doi: 10.1016/j.prosdent.2019.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Metz-Boutigue MH, Jolles J, Mazurier J, Schoentgen F, Legrand D, Spik G, et al. Human lactotransferrin: amino acid sequence and structural comparison with the other transferrins. Eur J Biochem. 1984;145(3):659–676. doi: 10.1111/j.1432-1033.1984.tb08607.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mayeur S, Spahis S, Pouliot Y, Levy E. Lactoferrin, a pleiotropic protein in health and disease. Antioxid Redox Signal. 2016;24(14):813–836. doi: 10.1089/ars.2015.6458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Arnold RR, Cole MF, McGhee JR. A bactericidal effect for human lactoferrin. Science. 1977;197(4300):263–265. doi: 10.1126/science.327545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bullen JJ. The significance of iron in infection. Rev Infect Dis. 1981;3(6):1127–1138. doi: 10.1093/clinids/3.6.1127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dominique L, Elizabeth E, Mathireu C, Joel M. Interactions of lactoferrin with cells involved in immune function. Biochem Cell Biol. 2006;84:282–290. doi: 10.1139/o06-045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Drago-Serrano ME, Campos-Rodriguez R, Carrero JC, de la Garza M. Lactoferrin: balancing ups and downs of inflammation due to microbial infections. Int J Mol Sci. 2017;18(3):501. doi: 10.3390/ijms18030501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Conneely OM. Antiinflammatory activities of lactoferrin. J Am Coll Nutr. 2001;20(sup5):389S–395S. doi: 10.1080/07315724.2001.10719173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kamemori N, Takeuchi T, Sugiyama A, Miyabayashi M, Kitagawa H, Shimizu H, et al. Trans-endothelial and trans-epithelial transfer of lactoferrin into the brain through BBB and BCSFB in adult rats. J Vet Med Sci. 2008;70(3):313–315. doi: 10.1292/jvms.70.313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hayashi T, To M, Saruta J, Sato C, Yamamoto Y, Kondo Y, et al. Salivary lactoferrin is transferred into the brain via the sublingual route. Biosci Biotechnol Biochem. 2017;81(7):1300–1304. doi: 10.1080/09168451.2017.1308241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kopaeva Y, Cherepov AB, Zarayskaya IY, Nesterenko MV. Transport of human lactoferrin into mouse brain: administration routes and distribution. Bull Exp Biol Med. 2019;167(4):561–567. doi: 10.1007/s10517-019-04572-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Zheng J, Xie Y, Li F, Zhou Y, Qi L, Liu L, Chen Z. Lactoferrin improves cognitive function and attenuates brain senescence in aged mice. J Funct Foods. 2020;65:103736. doi: 10.1016/j.jff.2019.103736. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Xu SF, Zhang YH, Wang S, Pang ZQ, Fan YG, Li JY, et al. Lactoferrin ameliorates dopaminergic neurodegeneration and motor deficits in MPTP-treated mice. Redox Biol. 2019;21:101090. doi: 10.1016/j.redox.2018.101090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Liu H, Wu H, Zhu N, Xu Z, Wang Y, Qu Y, Wang J. Lactoferrin protects against iron dysregulation, oxidative stress, and apoptosis in 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP)-induced Parkinson’s disease in mice. J Neurochem. 2020;152(3):397–415. doi: 10.1111/jnc.14857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Guo C, Yang ZH, Zhang S, Chai R, Xue H, Zhang YH, Li JY, Wang ZY. Intranasal lactoferrin enhances α-secretase-dependent amyloid precursor protein processing via the ERK1/2-CREB and HIF-1α pathways in an Alzheimer’s disease mouse model. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42(13):2504–2515. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Abdelhamid M, Jung CG, Zhou C, Abdullah M, Nakano M, Wakabayashi H, Abe F, Michikawa M. Dietary lactoferrin supplementation prevents memory impairment and reduces amyloid-β generation in J20 mice. J Alzheimers Dis. 2020;74(1):245–259. doi: 10.3233/JAD-191181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rousseau E, Michel PP, Hirsch EC. The iron-binding protein lactoferrin protects vulnerable dopamine neurons from degeneration by preserving mitochondrial calcium homeostasis. Mol Pharmacol. 2013;84(6):888–898. doi: 10.1124/mol.113.087965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kawamata T, Tooyama I, Yamada T, Walker DG, McGeer PL. Lactotransferrin immunocytochemistry in Alzheimer and normal human brain. Am J Pathol. 1993;142(5):1574–1585. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Leveugle B, Spik G, Perl DP, Bouras C, Fillit HM, Hof PR. The iron-binding protein lactotransferrin is present in pathologic lesions in a variety of neurodegenerative disorders: a comparative immunohistochemical analysis. Brain Res. 1994;650(1):20–31. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90202-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wang L, Sato H, Zhao S, Tooyama I. Deposition of lactoferrin in fibrillar-type senile plaques in the brains of transgenic mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett. 2010;481(3):164–167. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.06.079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mohamed WA, Salama RM, Schaalan MF. A pilot study on the effect of lactoferrin on Alzheimer’s disease pathological sequelae: Impact of the p-Akt/PTEN pathway. Biomed Pharmacother. 2019;111:714–723. doi: 10.1016/j.biopha.2018.12.118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Carro E, Bartolome F, Bermejo-Pareja F, Villarejo-Galende A, Molina JA, Ortiz P, et al. Early diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease based on salivary lactoferrin. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2017;8:131–138. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2017.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gonzalez-Sanchez M, Bartolome F, Antequera D, Puertas-Martin V, Gonzalez P, Gomez-Grande A, et al. Decreased salivary lactoferrin levels are specific to Alzheimer’s disease. EBioMedicine. 2020;57:102834. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2020.102834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Knopman DS, Parisi JE, Salviati A, Floriach-Robert M, Boeve BF, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, Dickson DW, Johnson KA, Petersen LE, McDonald WC, Braak H, Petersen RC. Neuropathology of cognitively normal elderly. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2003;62(11):1087–1095. doi: 10.1093/jnen/62.11.1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Dugger BN, Hentz JG, Adler CH, Sabbagh MN, Shill HA, Jacobson S, Caviness JN, Belden C, Driver-Dunckley E, Davis KJ, Sue LI, Beach TG. Clinicopathological outcomes of prospectively followed normal elderly brain bank volunteers. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2014;73(3):244–252. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0000000000000046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gordon BA, Blazey T, Su Y, Fagan AM, Holtzman DM, Morris JC, Benzinger TLS. Longitudinal β-amyloid deposition and hippocampal volume in preclinical Alzheimer disease and suspected non-Alzheimer disease pathophysiology. JAMA Neurol. 2016;73(10):1192–1200. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2016.2642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Jack CR, Jr, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Rocca WA, Knopman DS, Mielke MM, Lowe VJ, Senjem ML, Gunter JL, Preboske GM, Pankratz VS, Vemuri P, Petersen RC. Age-specific population frequencies of cerebral β-amyloidosis and neurodegeneration among people with normal cognitive function aged 50-89 years: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(10):997–1005. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70194-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jansen WJ, Ossenkoppele R, Knol DL, Tijms BM, Scheltens P, Verhey FR, Visser PJ, Amyloid Biomarker Study Group. Aalten P, Aarsland D, Alcolea D, Alexander M, Almdahl IS, Arnold SE, Baldeiras I, Barthel H, van Berckel B, Bibeau K, Blennow K, Brooks DJ, van Buchem M, Camus V, Cavedo E, Chen K, Chetelat G, Cohen AD, Drzezga A, Engelborghs S, Fagan AM, Fladby T, Fleisher AS, van der Flier W, Ford L, Förster S, Fortea J, Foskett N, Frederiksen KS, Freund-Levi Y, Frisoni GB, Froelich L, Gabryelewicz T, Gill KD, Gkatzima O, Gómez-Tortosa E, Gordon MF, Grimmer T, Hampel H, Hausner L, Hellwig S, Herukka SK, Hildebrandt H, Ishihara L, Ivanoiu A, Jagust WJ, Johannsen P, Kandimalla R, Kapaki E, Klimkowicz-Mrowiec A, Klunk WE, Köhler S, Koglin N, Kornhuber J, Kramberger MG, van Laere K, Landau SM, Lee DY, de Leon M, Lisetti V, Lleó A, Madsen K, Maier W, Marcusson J, Mattsson N, de Mendonça A, Meulenbroek O, Meyer PT, Mintun MA, Mok V, Molinuevo JL, Møllergård HM, Morris JC, Mroczko B, van der Mussele S, Na DL, Newberg A, Nordberg A, Nordlund A, Novak GP, Paraskevas GP, Parnetti L, Perera G, Peters O, Popp J, Prabhakar S, Rabinovici GD, Ramakers IH, Rami L, Resende de Oliveira C, Rinne JO, Rodrigue KM, Rodríguez-Rodríguez E, Roe CM, Rot U, Rowe CC, Rüther E, Sabri O, Sanchez-Juan P, Santana I, Sarazin M, Schröder J, Schütte C, Seo SW, Soetewey F, Soininen H, Spiru L, Struyfs H, Teunissen CE, Tsolaki M, Vandenberghe R, Verbeek MM, Villemagne VL, Vos SJ, van Waalwijk van Doorn L, Waldemar G, Wallin A, Wallin ÅK, Wiltfang J, Wolk DA, Zboch M, Zetterberg H. Prevalence of cerebral amyloid pathology in persons without dementia: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2015;313(19):1924–1938. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.4668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Drzezga A, Lautenschlager N, Siebner H, Riemenschneider M, Willoch F, Minoshima S, Schwaiger M, Kurz A. Cerebral metabolic changes accompanying conversion of mild cognitive impairment into Alzheimer’s disease: a PET follow-up study. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging. 2003;30(8):1104–1113. doi: 10.1007/s00259-003-1194-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Singh V, Chertkow H, Lerch JP, Evans AC, Dorr AE, Kabani NJ. Spatial patterns of cortical thinning in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2006;129(11):2885–2893. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Dickerson BC, Bakkour A, Salat DH, Feczko E, Pacheco J, Greve DN, Grodstein F, Wright CI, Blacker D, Rosas HD, Sperling RA, Atri A, Growdon JH, Hyman BT, Morris JC, Fischl B, Buckner RL. The cortical signature of Alzheimer’s disease: regionally specific cortical thinning relates to symptom severity in very mild to mild AD dementia and is detectable in asymptomatic amyloid-positive individuals. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(3):497–510. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mosconi L, Mistur R, Switalski R, Tsui WH, Glodzik L, Li Y, Pirraglia E, de Santi S, Reisberg B, Wisniewski T, de Leon MJ. FDG-PET changes in brain glucose metabolism from normal cognition to pathologically verified Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging. 2009;36(5):811–822. doi: 10.1007/s00259-008-1039-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Oh H, Habeck C, Madison C, Jagust W. Covarying alterations in Aβ deposition, glucose metabolism, and gray matter volume in cognitively normal elderly. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(1):297–308. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Yi D, Lee DY, Sohn BK, Choe YM, Seo EH, Byun MS, Woo JI. Beta-amyloid associated differential effects of APOE ε4 on brain metabolism in cognitively normal elderly. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014;22(10):961–970. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2013.12.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cantero JL, Atienza M, Sanchez-Juan P, Rodriguez-Rodriguez E, Vazquez-Higuera JL, Pozueta A, Gonzalez-Suarez A, Vilaplana E, Pegueroles J, Montal V, Blesa R, Alcolea D, Lleo A, Fortea J. Cerebral changes and disrupted gray matter cortical networks in asymptomatic older adults at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2018;64:58–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cantero JL, Atienza M, Lage C, Zaborszky L, Vilaplana E, Lopez-Garcia S, Pozueta A, Rodriguez-Rodriguez E, Blesa R, Alcolea D, Lleo A, Sanchez-Juan P, Fortea J, Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Atrophy of basal forebrain initiates with tau pathology in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Cereb Cortex. 2020;30(4):2083–2098. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Madi NM, El-Saka MH. The possible protective effect of lactoferrin on lipopolysaccharide-induced memory impairment in albino rats. Alexandria J Med. 2018;54(4):295–298. doi: 10.1016/j.ajme.2018.05.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Böhm P, Peña-Casanova J, Aguilar M, Hernandez G, Sol JM, Blesa R. Clinical validity and utility of the interview for deterioration of daily living in dementia for Spanish-speaking communities. Int Psychogeriatr. 1998;10(3):261–270. doi: 10.1017/s1041610298005377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sheikh JL, Yesavage JA. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin Gerontol. 1986;5:165–173. doi: 10.1300/J018v05n01_09. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Gramunt N, Sanchez-Benavides G, Buschke H, Dieguez-Vide F, Peña-Casanova J, Masramon X, et al. The memory binding test: Development of two alternate forms into Spanish and Catalan. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;52(1):283–293. doi: 10.3233/JAD-151175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Corey-Bloom J, Haque AS, Park S, Nathan AS, Baker RW, Thomas EA. Salivary levels of total huntingtin are elevated in Huntington’s disease patients. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):7371. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25095-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bullich S, Seibyl J, Catafau AM, Jovalekic A, Koglin N, Barthel H, Sabri O, de Santi S. Optimized classification of 18F-Florbetaben PET scans as positive and negative using an SUVR quantitative approach and comparison to visual assessment. Neuroimage Clin. 2017;15:325–332. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.04.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97(20):11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Bernal-Rusiel JL, Atienza M, Cantero JL. Detection of focal changes in human cortical thickness: spherical wavelets versus Gaussian smoothing. Neuroimage. 2008;41(4):1278–1292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Greve DN, Salat DH, Bowen SL, Izquierdo-Garcia D, Schultz AP, Catana C, Becker JA, Svarer C, Knudsen GM, Sperling RA, Johnson KA. Different partial volume correction methods lead to different conclusions: an 18 F-FDG-PET study of aging. Neuroimage. 2016;132:334–343. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Mormino EC, Betensky RA, Hedden T, Schultz AP, Ward A, Huijbers W, Rentz DM, Johnson KA, Sperling RA, Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Australian Imaging Biomarkers and Lifestyle Flagship Study of Ageing. Harvard Aging Brain Study Amyloid and APOE ε4 interact to influence short-term decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2014;82(20):1760–1767. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Box GEP, Cox DR. An analysis of transformations. J R Stat Soc Ser B. 1964;26:211–252. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Bernal-Rusiel JL, Atienza M, Cantero JL. Determining the optimal level of smoothing in cortical thickness analysis: a hierarchical approach based on sequential statistical thresholding. Neuroimage. 2010;52(1):158–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31(3):968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Cohen JE. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Yeo IK, Johnson RA. A new family of power transformations to improve normality or symmetry. Biometrika. 2000;87(4):954–959. doi: 10.1093/biomet/87.4.954. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Clark TS, Linzer DA. Should I use fixed or random effects? Pol Sci Res Meth. 2015;3:399–408. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bermejo-Pareja F, Del Ser T, Valenti M, de la Fuente M, Bartolome F, Carro E. Salivary lactoferrin as biomarker for Alzheimer’s disease: brain-immunity interactions. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(8):1196–1204. doi: 10.1002/alz.12107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Sweeney MD, Zhao Z, Montagne A, Nelson AR, Zlokovic BV. Blood-brain barrier: from physiology to disease and back. Physiol Rev. 2019;99(1):21–78. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00050.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kamer AR, Dasanayake AP, Craig RG, Glodzik-Sobanska L, Bry M, de Leon MJ. Alzheimer’s disease and peripheral infections: the possible contribution from periodontal infections, model and hypothesis. J Alzheimers Dis. 2008;13(4):437–449. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2008-13408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kamer AR, Pirraglia E, Tsui W, Rusinek H, Vallabhajosula S, Mosconi L, Yi L, McHugh P, Craig RG, Svetcov S, Linker R, Shi C, Glodzik L, Williams S, Corby P, Saxena D, de Leon MJ. Periodontal disease associates with higher brain amyloid load in normal elderly. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36(2):627–633. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.10.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kamer AR, Pushalkar S, Gulivindala D, Butler T, Li Y, Annam KRC, et al. Periodontal dysbiosis associates with reduced CSF Aβ42 in cognitively normal elderly. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2021;13:e12172. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Gleerup HS, Jensen CS, Høgh P, Hasselbalch SG, Simonsen AH. Lactoferrin in cerebrospinal fluid and saliva is not a diagnostic biomarker for Alzheimer's disease in a mixed memory clinic population. EBioMedicine. 2021;67:103361. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2021.103361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Chetelat G, Villemagne VL, Pike KE, Baron JC, Bourgeat P, Jones G, et al. Larger temporal volume in elderly with high versus low beta-amyloid deposition. Brain. 2010;133(11):3349–3358. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Fortea J, Sala-Llonch R, Bartres-Faz D, Llado A, Sole-Padulles C, Bosch B, et al. Cognitively preserved subjects with transitional cerebrospinal fluid ß-amyloid 1-42 values have thicker cortex in Alzheimer’s disease vulnerable areas. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70(2):183–190. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Johnson SC, Christian BT, Okonkwo OC, Oh JM, Harding S, Xu G, Hillmer AT, Wooten DW, Murali D, Barnhart TE, Hall LT, Racine AM, Klunk WE, Mathis CA, Bendlin BB, Gallagher CL, Carlsson CM, Rowley HA, Hermann BP, Dowling NM, Asthana S, Sager MA. Amyloid burden and neural function in people at risk for Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35(3):576–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.09.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Iacono D, O’Brien R, Resnick SM, Zonderman AB, Pletnikova O, Rudow G, et al. Neuronal hypertrophy in asymptomatic Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2008;67(6):578–589. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e3181772794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Benzinger TL, Blazey T, Jack CR, Jr, Koeppe RA, Su Y, Xiong C, et al. Regional variability of imaging biomarkers in autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(47):E4502–E4509. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1317918110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Fellin T, Pascual O, Gobbo S, Pozzan T, Haydon PG, Carmignoto G. Neuronal synchrony mediated by astrocytic glutamate through activation of extrasynaptic NMDA receptors. Neuron. 2004;43(5):729–743. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Vezzani A, Granata T. Brain inflammation in epilepsy: experimental and clinical evidence. Epilepsia. 2005;46(11):1724–1743. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2005.00298.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Palop JJ, Mucke L. Amyloid-beta-induced neuronal dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: from synapses toward neural networks. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(7):812–818. doi: 10.1038/nn.2583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Drachman DA. The amyloid hypothesis, time to move on: amyloid is the downstream result, not cause, of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(3):372–380. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Herrup K. The case for rejecting the amyloid cascade hypothesis. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(6):794–799. doi: 10.1038/nn.4017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Actor JK, Hwang SA, Kruzel ML. Lactoferrin as a natural immune modulator. Curr Pharm Des. 2009;15(17):1956–1973. doi: 10.2174/138161209788453202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Aizawa S, Hoki M, Yamamuro Y. Lactoferrin promotes autophagy via AMP-activated protein kinase activation through low-density lipoprotein receptor-related protein 1. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2017;493(1):509–513. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2017.08.160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets employed in the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.