Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Sep 8.
Published in final edited form as: Cell Host Microbe. 2021 Aug 5;29(9):1378–1392.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.chom.2021.07.004

Alterations in the gut microbiota contribute to cognitive impairment induced by the ketogenic diet and hypoxia

Christine A Olson 1,*, Alonso J Iñiguez 1, Grace E Yang 1, Ping Fang 1, Geoffrey N Pronovost 1, Kelly G Jameson 1, Tomiko K Rendon 1, Jorge Paramo 1, Jacob T Barlow 2, Rustem F Ismagilov 2, Elaine Y Hsiao 1,*,3
PMCID: PMC8429275  NIHMSID: NIHMS1731552  PMID: 34358434

SUMMARY

Many genetic and environmental factors increase susceptibility to cognitive impairment (CI), and the gut microbiome is increasingly implicated. However, the identity of gut microbes associated with CI risk, their effects on CI, and their mechanisms remain unclear. Here we show that a carbohydrate-restricted (Ketogenic) diet and intermittent hypoxia in mice potentiates CI and alters the gut microbiota. Depleting the microbiome prevents CI, whereas transplantation of the risk-associated microbiome or monocolonization with Bilophila wadsworthia confers CI in mice fed a standard diet. B. wadsworthia and the risk-associated microbiome disrupt hippocampal synaptic plasticity, neurogenesis, and gene expression. The CI is associated with microbiome-dependent increases in intestinal interferon-gamma (IFNg)-producing Th1 cells. Inhibiting Th1 cell development abrogates the adverse effects of both B. wadsworthia and environmental risk factors on CI. Together, these findings identify select gut bacteria that contribute to environmental risk for CI in mice by promoting inflammation and hippocampal dysfunction.

eTOC

Alterations in the gut microbiome are increasingly implicated in cognitive dysfunction. Olson et al. reveal that ketogenic diet and hypoxia synergistically impair cognitive behavior and modify the gut microbiota in mice. Select risk-associated bacteria are sufficient to disrupt hippocampal function and cognitive behavior via immune-mediated pathways.

Graphical Abstract

graphic file with name nihms-1731552-f0001.jpg

INTRODUCTION

Cognitive impairment (CI) is characterized by deficient attention, causal reasoning, and learning and memory, and afflicts a reported 3–19% of people over 65 years old worldwide (Mavrodaris et al., 2013). In addition to its prevalence in aging and aging-related neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease, CI is also associated with a wide variety of chronic metabolic, immunological, neuropsychiatric and sleep disorders, making CI a pressing global health concern. Genetic, environmental, and behavioral factors together predispose to CI, and many biological pathways are implicated, including neuroinflammation, mitochondrial dysfunction, and blood brain barrier disruptions (Han et al., 2020; Sweeney et al., 2018). In particular, both hypoxia-associated physiological stressors, such as high altitude, strenuous activity, sleep apnea, vascular dysfunction (Akinyemi et al., 2013; Das et al., 2018; Duncombe et al., 2017; Pun et al., 2019; Solis et al., 2020; Zhao and Gong, 2015) and the high fat, high sugar “Western” diet (Kanoski and Davidson, 2011a; Noble et al., 2017; Pistell et al., 2010; Wei et al., 2018) are well-known to increase risk for CI, while the high fat, low carbohydrate ketogenic diet is being explored for treatment of CI, with variable results (Bahr et al., 2020; Lauritzen et al., 2016; Nagpal et al., 2019; Ota et al., 2019; Phillips et al., 2018; van Berkel et al., 2018; Zhao et al., 2004). Despite many studies on environmental risk for CI, exactly how environmental factors, such as diet and physical stress, modify susceptibility for CI remains poorly understood.

The gut microbiome is emerging as an important mediator of environmental contributions to host health and disease. External factors, including diet, stress, and age can shape the composition and function of the gut microbiota (Marques et al., 2010; O’Toole and Jeffery, 2015; Rea et al., 2016). Alterations in the gut microbiome can, in turn, mediate effects of environmental challenges on various behavioral abnormalities, including impaired communication in response to maternal immune activation, anxiety-related behavior in response to stress, and reduced sociability in response to high-fat diet (Bravo et al., 2011; Buffington et al., 2016; Gareau et al., 2011; Hsiao et al., 2013; Savignac et al., 2015). Moreover, depleting the microbiome by germ-free rearing or antibiotic treatment disrupts cognitive performance across various tasks for working and spatial memory, when compared to controls raised with conventional microbiomes (Chu et al., 2019; D’Amato et al., 2020; Frohlich et al., 2016; Gareau et al., 2011; Hoban et al., 2016; Sampson and Mazmanian, 2015; Vuong et al., 2017; Yu et al., 2019). These animal studies provide fundamental proof-of-concept that investigating interactions between the gut microbiome and environmental risk factors for CI may reveal previously uncharacterized cellular and molecular signaling pathways that regulate CI.

Herein, we examine the effects of two environmental modifiers of CI —intermittent hypoxia and the ketogenic diet and — on the gut microbiota, neurophysiology and cognitive behavior. We evaluate causal roles for environmentally-induced changes in the gut microbiome in promoting CI, and further identify specific microbial taxa that contribute to environmental risk for impaired hippocampal function and cognitive deficits. Finally, we interrogate functional effects of risk-associated microbes to reveal that microbial stimulation of type 1 immune responses contribute to microbiome-mediated environmental susceptibility to CI.

RESULTS

The Ketogenic Diet Exacerbates Hypoxia-Induced Cognitive Impairment

Hypoxia is an environmental risk factor for CI associated with high altitude exposure, sleep apnea, vascular dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease, among many other pathological conditions (Biswal et al., 2016; de Aquino Lemos et al., 2012; Desbonnet et al., 2014; Giuliani et al., 2019; Qaid et al., 2017). To determine the impact of acute intermittent hypoxia on cognitive behavior, conventional mice (specific pathogen-free, SPF) consuming a control diet (CD, Table S1) were subjected to 6 hours of restricted 12% oxygen (normobaric hypoxia, Hyp) or ambient 21% oxygen (normoxia, Mock) daily for five days, a regimen within the range of hypoxic treatments sufficient to induce cognitive impairment in mice (Deguil et al., 2016). Following a 4-day recovery period, mice were then evaluated for cognitive and spatial learning and memory behavior in the Barnes maze task (Figure 1A). Consistent with prior literature (Aubrecht et al., 2015; Mei et al., 2020) mice exposed to Hyp exhibited impaired cognitive behavior in the Barnes maze, as indicated by increased latency to enter the escape box, errors made, and use of random search strategy as compared to Mock controls (Figures 1B1F). These impairments were observed even on the first trial of testing, suggesting CI that can include disrupted learning and memory, in addition to deficits in other underlying cognitive processes (Li et al., 2013; Shen et al., 2020). There were no significant differences in velocity or total distance travelled in the Barnes maze (Figures S1A and S1F), suggesting no confounding abnormalities in motor function. There were also no differences in performance in the open field test and prepulse inhibition task, suggesting no overt alterations in stress-induced exploration or sensorimotor gating (Figure S1N). These results indicate that acute intermittent hypoxia impairs cognitive behavior in mice, which is consistent with previous reports (Aubrecht et al., 2015; Mei et al., 2020).

Figure 1: The Ketogenic Diet Potentiates Hypoxia-Induced Impairments in Cognitive Behavior.

Figure 1:

A) Experimental timeline B) Representative Barnes maze traces for SPF mice fed the CD and exposed to Mock or Hyp. White lines indicate movement trajectories, whereas blue hues denote increasing durations of time spent at a specific location. Orange circles indicate the escape hole. C) Latency to enter the escape hole of the Barnes maze across six 300-second trials. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=13–17). D) Errors made as measured by number of incorrect nose pokes. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=13–17). E) Errors made during the final trial (probe). (Unpaired two-tailed Students t-test, n=13–17). F) Search strategy used during the probe trial. (n=13–17). G) Experimental timeline. (n=11–13). H) Representative Barnes maze traces for SPF mice fed the KD and exposed to Mock or Hyp. I) Latency to enter the escape hole across six 300-second trials. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=11–13). J) Errors made. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=11–13). K) Errors made during the probe trial. (Unpaired two-tailed Students t-test, n=11–13). L) Search strategy used during the probe trial. (n=11–13). Data are presented as mean ± S.E.M. * p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001. n.s.=not statistically significant. SPF=specific pathogen-free (conventionally-colonized), CD=control diet, KD=ketogenic diet, Mock=intermittent normoxia exposure, Hyp=intermittent hypoxia exposure.

High dietary fat intake modifies cognitive behavior in humans and animal models. For example, consumption of the high-fat, high-sugar “Western” diet leads to deficits in memory speed, flexibility, prospective memory, and spatial learning and memory (Beilharz et al., 2015; Cordner and Tamashiro, 2015; Kanoski and Davidson, 2011a; Noble et al., 2017; Pistell et al., 2010; Wei et al., 2018), particularly in response to physical or psychosocial stress (Arcego et al., 2018; Kesby et al., 2015). In contrast, the ketogenic diet (KD), which is also based on high fat intake, but includes carbohydrate restriction, is used clinically for its neuroprotective effects in refractory epilepsy, and is now increasingly applied to human neurodegenerative and cognitive disorders, despite limited experimental studies in this context (McDonald and Cervenka, 2019; Vinciguerra et al., 2020). While some studies suggest that the KD may protect against CI, other studies report that it may be detrimental (Lauritzen et al., 2016; Zhao et al., 2004). To examine effects of the KD on cognitive behavior, SPF mice were pre-treated with the ketogenic chow, used commonly to model the classical clinical KD (Dutton et al., 2011; Samala et al., 2008), and subjected to Mock or Hyp as described above (Figure 1G). Mice fed the KD exhibited no significant differences in cognitive behavior in the Barnes maze, as compared to mice fed the vitamin and mineral-matched CD (Figure 1, SPF CD Mock vs. SPF KD Mock), indicating that the KD alone has no overt effect on cognitive behavior in the Barnes maze. Notably, however, mice fed the KD and exposed to Hyp (SPF KD Hyp) exhibited a substantial increase in latency to enter the escape box, errors made, and random search strategy as compared to Mock controls fed the KD (SPF KD Mock) (Figures 1H1L). The Hyp-induced behavioral impairment was significantly more severe in KD-fed mice than in CD-fed mice (Figures 1 and S1KM). There was no significant difference across experimental groups in velocity or total distance traveled in the Barnes maze (Figures S1B and S1G), or in performance in the open field and prepulse inhibition tasks (Figure S1NR). These results indicate that the KD potentiates the adverse effects of Hyp on cognitive behavior in mice, and further highlight synergistic interactions between diet and hypoxic stress as environmental risk factors for CI.

Ketogenic Diet- and Hypoxia-Associated Alterations in the Gut Microbiota Impair Cognitive Behavior

Environmental factors, including diet and stress, play important roles in shaping the composition and function of the gut microbiota (David et al., 2014; Lobionda et al., 2019; Sbihi et al., 2019; Tripathi et al., 2018; van de Wouw et al., 2018). To gain insight into whether the gut microbiota contributes to KD- and Hyp-induced disruptions in cognitive behavior, SPF mice were pre-treated with broad-spectrum antibiotics (Abx) to deplete the gut microbiota prior to KD and Hyp exposure (Figure 2A) (Reikvam et al., 2011). Compared to vehicle-treated controls, KD- and Hyp-exposed mice that were pre-treated with Abx exhibited improved cognitive performance, as indicated by decreased latency to enter the escape box, errors made, and use of random search strategy (Figures 2B2F, SPF KD Hyp vs. Abx KD Hyp). Results for each of these behavioral parameters were comparable to those seen in Mock controls fed the KD (Figures 2B2F, S1C and S1H; Abx KD Hyp vs. SPF KD Mock), suggesting that depletion of the microbiota abrogated KD and Hyp-induced impairments in cognitive behavior. To determine whether mice reared in the absence of the microbiome exhibit similar protection against KD- and Hyp-induced CI, germ-free (GF) mice were fed the KD and exposed to Hyp under sterile conditions, and then tested in the Barnes maze using aseptic technique. qPCR for fecal loads of the 16S rRNA gene after the last day of Barnes maze testing revealed that the behaviorally-tested GF mice exhibit 16S rRNA gene levels comparable to those seen in untested GF mice, suggesting no overt contamination during behavioral testing (Figure S2I). However, given the possibility that there could be low-level contamination within typical variation seen in fecal 16S rRNA gene levels from GF mice, we refer to behaviorally tested GF mice as “ex-GF”. Consistent with results from Abx treatment, ex-GF mice fed the KD were resistant to Hyp-induced CI in the Barnes maze (Figures S2AH). Compared to KD-fed SPF mice, however, ex-GF mice fed the KD exhibited impaired cognitive behavior at baseline, as indicated by increased latency to enter and increased errors made in the Barnes maze. This suggests detrimental effects of KD particularly in mice raised GF (Gareau et al., 2011). To additionally evaluate potential off-target effects of antibiotic treatment, GF mice were pre-treated with Abx to determine whether antibiotics can impact hypoxia-induced impairments in Barnes maze behavior in the absence of the microbiome. Abx treatment had no significant effect on latency to enter in the Barnes and significantly increased errors made by KD-fed ex-GF mice, regardless of Mock or Hyp treatment (Figure S2JO). This indicates that Abx pre-treatment has microbiome-independent effects that increase errors made by ex-GF mice in the Barnes maze, and further suggests that microbiome-dependent effects drive the Abx-induced reductions in errors made by SPF KD Hyp mice in the Barnes maze (Figures 2DE). Altogether, these data suggest that depletion of the gut microbiota prevents the synergistic effects of KD and Hyp on CI in mice.

Figure 2: Alterations in the Gut Microbiota Contribute to Ketogenic Diet and Hypoxia-Induced Impairments in Cognitive Behavior.

Figure 2:

A) Experimental timeline. B) Representative Barnes maze traces for SPF or Abx mice fed the KD and exposed to Hyp. White lines indicate movement trajectories, whereas blue hues denote increasing durations of time spent at a particular location. Orange circles indicate the escape hole. C) Latency to enter the escape hole of the Barnes maze across six 300-second trials. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=8 for Abx groups; SPF data are as in Fig. 1). D) Errors made as measured by number of incorrect nose pokes. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=8 for Abx groups; SPF data are as in Fig. 1). E) Errors made during the probe trial. (One-way ANOVA with Dunnett, n=8 for Abx groups; SPF data are as in Fig. 1). F) Search strategy used during probe trial. (n=8). G) Experimental timeline. H) Representative Barnes maze traces for GF transplanted with fecal microbiota from SPF KD Mock or SPF KD Hyp donors. Transplanted recipient mice receiving SPF KD Mock microbiota are denoted GF+Mock. Transplanted recipient mice receiving SPF KD Hyp microbiota are denoted GF+Hyp. I) Latency to enter the escape hole. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=23). J) Errors made. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=23). K) Errors made during the probe trial. (Unpaired two-tailed Students t-test, n=23). L) Search strategy used during probe trial. (n=14–15). Data are presented as mean ± S.E.M. * p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****P < 0.0001. n.s.=not statistically significant. SPF=specific pathogen-free (conventionally-colonized), Abx= treated with antibiotics (ampicillin, vancomycin, metronidazole, neomycin), KD=ketogenic diet, Mock=intermittent normoxia exposure, Hyp=intermittent hypoxia exposure, GF=germ-free, GF+Mock = GF mice transplanted with microbiota from SPF mice fed KD and exposed to Mock, GF+Hyp = GF mice transplanted with microbiota from SPF mice fed KD and exposed to Hyp.

To further test whether microbiota alterations in response to KD and Hyp contribute to disruptions in cognitive behavior, GF mice were transplanted with fecal microbiota from donor SPF mice fed KD and exposed to either Hyp or Mock (GF + Hyp and GF + Mock, respectively) (Figure 2G). Compared to controls colonized with the KD and Mock-associated microbiota, mice colonized with KD and Hyp-associated microbiota exhibited poor cognitive performance (Figures 2H2L, S1D and S1I), akin to that seen in mice fed KD and exposed to Hyp (Figures 1H1L). Relative to SPF mice, transplanted control mice exhibited impaired performance on the first trial that improved with subsequent trials of the task, which may reflect initial confounding effects of GF status on behavior. Taken together, these results indicate that i) the KD potentiates Hyp-induced impairments in cognitive behavior (Figures 1 and S1KM), ii) depletion of the microbiota prevents the adverse synergistic effects of KD and Hyp on cognitive behavior (Figures 2A2F), and iii) transplantation of the KD- and Hyp-associated microbiota into naïve GF mice impairs cognitive behavior (Figures 2G2L). These results strongly suggest that changes in the gut microbiota contribute to KD- and Hyp-induced impairments in cognitive behavior in mice.

Bilophila is Enriched by the Ketogenic Diet and Hypoxia and Impairs Cognitive Behavior

To identify candidate microbial taxa that may be responsible for promoting KD- and Hyp-induced abnormalities in cognitive behavior, fecal microbiota were sequenced from SPF mice fed KD and exposed to Hyp or Mock (Figures 1G1L), as well as from the mice transplanted with the corresponding microbiota from those SPF mice (Figures 2G2L). While there were no global alterations in the microbiota of mice fed KD and exposed to Hyp (Figures 3A and S3), select bacterial taxa were significantly altered in the Hyp group, compared to Mock controls (Figures 3B and 3C, Table S2). In particular, the relative abundances of Clostridium cocleatum were reduced in Hyp-exposed animals fed KD, while Bilophila species were elevated in Hyp-exposed animals fed KD (Figures 3B and 3C). The absolute abundance of Bilophila species was enriched particularly in mice exposed to both KD and Hyp, but not in mice exposed to either KD or Hyp alone (Figure S3E). This aligns with the observed synergistic effects of KD and Hyp on CI (Figures 1 and S1KM), and with previous reports that dietary fat favors the growth of Bilophila, an obligate anaerobic pathobiont, in mice (Devkota et al., 2012). Similar reductions in C. cocleatum and increases in Bilophila were seen in mice transplanted with KD and Hyp-associated microbiota (Figures 3D3F, Table S2). Overall, these results reveal that KD and Hyp together enrich Bilophila and also impair cognitive behavior in mice.

Figure 3: Bilophila is Enriched by the Ketogenic Diet and Hypoxia, and Sufficiently Impairs Cognitive Behavior.

Figure 3:

A) Principal coordinates analysis of weighted UniFrac distance based on 16S rRNA gene profiling of feces from SPF mice fed KD and exposed to Hyp or Mock. (n=4 cages). B) Average taxonomic distributions of low abundance bacteria. (n=13 cages). C) Relative abundances of Clostridium cocleatum (left) and Bilophila spp. (right) in fecal microbiota. (Kruskal-Wallis with Bonferroni, n=13 cages). D) Principal coordinates analysis of weighted UniFrac distance based on 16S rRNA gene profiling of feces from GF mice transplanted with fecal microbiota from SPF KD Mock or SPF KD Hyp mice (in panels A-C). (n=4–5 cages) E) Average taxonomic distributions of low abundance bacteria (n=9 cages). F) Relative abundances of C. cocleatum (left) and Bilophila spp. (right) in fecal microbiota. (Kruskal-Wallis with Bonferroni, n=9 cages). G) Experimental timeline. H) Representative Barnes maze traces for GF mice monocolonized with Clos or Bilo. White lines indicate movement trajectories, whereas blue hues denote increasing durations of time spent at a particular location. Orange circles denote the escape hole. I) Latency to enter the escape hole across six 300-second trials for ex-GF mice and GF mice monocolonized with Clos or Bilo. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=15, 24, 6). J) Errors made. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=15, 24, 6). K) Errors made during the probe trial. (Unpaired two-tailed Students t-test, n=15, 24, 6). L) Search strategy used during probe trial. (n=15, 24, 6). M) Effect size of hypoxia on latency to enter the escape hole during the probe trial, as measured by the difference between Hyp groups and respective Mock controls for SPF, Abx, ex-GF, microbiota-transplanted (GF+Hyp-Mock), or monocolonized (GF+Bilo-Clos) mice fed CD or KD. (Two-way ANOVA with Dunnett, n=8–24). Data are presented as mean ± S.E.M. * p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. n.s.=not statistically significant. SPF=specific pathogen-free (conventionally-colonized), KD=ketogenic diet, Mock=intermittent normoxia exposure, Hyp=intermittent hypoxia exposure, GF=germ-free, GF+Mock = GF mice transplanted with SPF KD Mock microbiota, GF+Hyp = GF mice transplanted with SPF KD Hyp microbiota, GF+Clos = GF mice monocolonized with C. cocleatum. GF+Bilo = GF mice monocolonized with B. wadsworthia, Abx= treated with antibiotics (ampicillin, vancomycin, metronidazole, neomycin), CD= control diet, ex-GF = behaviorally tested GF mice.

To test whether Bilophila may contribute to the CI induced by KD and Hyp exposure, GF mice fed standard chow were monocolonized with Bilophila wadsworthia and tested for cognitive behavior, relative to GF mice and GF mice monocolonized with C. cocleatum (Figure 3G). B. wadsworthia was selected because it exhibited the highest sequence identity to the Bilophila operational taxonomic units elevated in mice fed KD and exposed to Hyp (Figures 3B and 3C). Mice colonized with B. wadsworthia exhibited impaired cognitive behavior compared to ex-GF and C. cocleatum-colonized controls, with no overt deficits in motor ability (Figures 3H3L, S1E and S1J). Notably, there were no statistically significant differences between ex-GF and C. cocleatum-colonized mice in latency to enter the escape box or errors made during the probe trial of the Barnes maze test, suggesting detrimental effects of B. wadsworthia, rather than beneficial effects of C. cocleatum, on cognitive behavior (Figures 3H3L, S1E and S1J). The effect of B. wadsworthia on CI during the probe trial of the Barnes maze assay was comparable to that seen with exposure to KD and Hyp and with transplantation of the KD- and Hyp-associated microbiota (Figure 3M). Overall, these data reveal that monocolonization with B. wadsworthia phenocopies the adverse effects of the KD- and Hyp-associated microbiota on cognitive behavior in mice.

The Gut Microbiota, and Bilophila in Particular, Modulates Hippocampal Activity

The hippocampus is sensitive to alterations in diet and hypoxic stress and is a critical site for learning and memory (Goldbart et al., 2006; Gozal et al., 2002; Kanoski and Davidson, 2011b; Kanoski et al., 2010; Titus et al., 2007). To study effects of the KD and Hyp, and potential roles for the microbiota, on hippocampal physiology, field potential recordings were acquired from acute hippocampal slices from SPF mice fed KD and exposed to Hyp or Mock, as well as from GF mice colonized with B. wadsworthia or C. cocleatum. Compared to Mock-exposed controls, KD-fed mice that were exposed to Hyp exhibited significant reductions in hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP, Figures 4A and 4B), population spike relative to field excitatory postsynaptic potential (fEPSP) slope, as a measure of excitatory postsynaptic potential- spike coupling (Figure 4C) and paired-pulse facilitation (Figure 4D). Pre-treatment with Abx diminished these abnormalities in hippocampal synaptic physiology (Figure S4AD), suggesting that the gut microbiome contributes to the adverse effects of KD and Hyp on hippocampal physiology. Consistent with this, mice monocolonized with B. wadsworthia exhibited reduced hippocampal LTP (Figures 4E and 4F), reduced fiber volley amplitude vs. fEPSP slope (Figure 4G) and reduced paired-pulse facilitation (Figure 4H) when compared to C. cocleatum-colonized controls. The disruptions in hippocampal activity induced by B. wadsworthia were comparable to those seen in mice exposed to KD and Hyp, suggesting that colonization with B. wadsworthia phenocopies the adverse effects of KD and Hyp on hippocampal synaptic transmission and plasticity. The microbiota-dependent alterations in hippocampal activity were further associated with microbiota-dependent changes in hippocampal gene expression (Figure S4EJ, Table S3). In particular, colonization with B. wadsworthia resulted in widespread transcriptomic alterations in the hippocampus relative to C. cocleatum-colonized controls (Figure 4I). Particular genes that were differentially expressed in response to B. wadsworthia colonization included subsets related to neuronal excitation (CBLN1, CC2D1A, GRIK4, SYT2, SYT9), mitochondrial processes (DNAJA3, SLC25A16, ALDH4A1, NDUFAF1), neuronal interactions (KCNN3, SEMA3C, TTR, MOV10, PLK5), ubiquitination (BAP1, COPS6, FBXO4, FBXO42, NDUFAF5, USP53), and immune response (LCK, HMGB1, NFKBID, TRAFD1, SPG21) (Figure 4J). Moreover, immunofluorescence staining for doublecortin (DCX), a marker for the central phase of hippocampal neurogenesis (von Bohlen und Halbach, 2011) revealed reduced density of DCX-positive cells in the dentate gyrus of mice colonized with B. wadsworthia compared with C. cocleatum-colonized controls (Figure 4K), adding to prior results demonstrating the microbiota regulates hippocampal neurogenesis (Ogbonnaya et al., 2015). Overall, these results indicate that colonization with B. wadsworthia alters hippocampal physiology, and further suggest that microbiota-dependent impairments in cognitive behavior may be due, at least in part, to microbiota-dependent disruptions in hippocampal function.

Figure 4: Bilophila Colonization Phenocopies Ketogenic Diet and Hypoxia-Induced Impairments in Hippocampal Activity.

Figure 4:

A) Hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) as indicated by fEPSP slope in response to 100 Hz tetanus, expressed as a percentage of 20-minute baseline signal, from slice electrophysiology of brains from SPF mice fed KD and exposed to Hyp or Mock. (n=7–8). B) Average fEPSP slope during the last 5 minutes of hippocampal LTP recording. (Unpaired two-tailed Students t-test, n=7–8). C) Hippocampal population spike amplitude versus fEPSP slope. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=7–8). D) Hippocampal paired pulse facilitation. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=7–8). E) Hippocampal LTP from GF mice monocolonized with Clos or Bilo. (n=12). F) Average fEPSP slope during the last 5 minutes of hippocampal LTP recording. (Unpaired two-tailed Students t-test, n=12). G) Hippocampal population spike amplitude versus fEPSP slope. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=12). H) Hippocampal paired pulse facilitation. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=12). I) Principal components analysis of all differentially regulated genes from RNA sequencing of CA3 subfields of the hippocampus from GF mice monocolonized with Clostridium cocleatum (Clos) or Bilophila wadsworthia (Bilo). (n=6). J) Volcano plot labeling genes with high fold change of differential expression (Wald test, n=6). K) Representative image of doublecortin (DCX)-positive neurons in the dentate gyrus (left). Quantitation of DCX density per area of the dentate gyrus (right). (Unpaired two-tailed Students t-test, n=4–5). Data are presented as mean ± S.E.M. * p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. n.s.=not statistically significant. SPF=specific pathogen-free (conventionally-colonized), Abx= treated with antibiotics (ampicillin, vancomycin, metronidazole, neomycin), GF=germ-free, KD=ketogenic diet, Hyp=intermittent hypoxia exposure, Mock=intermittent normoxia exposure, GF+Clos = GF mice monocolonized with C. cocleatum. GF+Bilo = GF mice monocolonized with B. wadsworthia, DCX=doublecortin, LTP=long-term-potentiation, fEPSP=field excitatory post-synaptic potential.

Th1 Cell Expansion Contributes to Bilophila-Induced Impairments in Cognitive Behavior

B. wadsworthia colonization promotes the expansion of IFNg-producing T helper type I (Th1) cells, and IFNg is associated with impairments in cognitive behavior and hippocampal physiology during homeostasis and in response to chronic stress (Devkota et al., 2012; Kim et al., 2011; Litteljohn et al., 2014). We hypothesized that Th1 induction may contribute to the detrimental effects of B. wadsworthia and of environmental risk factors, KD and Hyp, on cognitive behavior. To first confirm that B. wadsworthia increases Th1 cells, GF mice were monocolonized with either C. cocleatum or B. wadsworthia, and Th1 levels were measured in the intestine. Indeed, B. wadsworthia-colonized mice exhibited increased levels of CD3+CD4+IFNg+IL17a− Th1 cells in the colonic lamina propria relative to GF and C. cocleatum-colonized controls (Figures S5A and S5B). Similar increases in levels of Th1 cells were seen by colonization of Abx-treated mice with B. wadsworthia, indicating that the Th1-promoting effects of B. wadsworthia are observed in response to bacterial enrichment, as well as bacterial monocolonization (Figures S5C and S5D). To determine if Th1 cells are required for the adverse effects of B. wadsworthia on cognitive behavior, mice were reared in the absence of Th1 cells (T-bet knockout, T-bet−/−) or as genetically wildtype (WT) and colonized with B. wadsworthia (Figures 5A, S5C and S5D). Consistent with prior monocolonization experiments conducted with GF Swiss Webster mice (Figure 3), Abx-treated WT C57Bl/6J mice colonized with B. wadsworthia exhibited impaired cognitive behavior in the Barnes maze, as denoted by elevated errors made and increased random search strategy relative to non-colonized Abx-treated WT controls (Figures 5B5F, Abx+Bilo WT vs. Abx WT). In contrast to results from prior monocolonization experiments (Figure 3I), there were no statistically significant differences in latency to enter the escape box (Figure 5C), which may point to effects of B. wadsworthia on this particular behavioral parameter that are dependent on bacterial load, prior colonization status, and/or genetic background of the host. Notably, preventing Th1 induction via T-bet deficiency abrogated the adverse effects of B. wadsworthia on cognitive behavior, as denoted by reductions in errors made and use of random search strategy to levels comparable to those seen in colonized WT controls (Figures 5DF, Abx+Bilo Tbet−/− vs. Abx+Bilo WT). There is no striking difference in latency to enter or errors made in the Barnes maze by Abx-treated Tbet−/− mice relative to WT controls (Figures 5BF, Abx Tbet−/− vs. Abx WT), suggesting that the ability of Tbet deficiency to prevent detrimental effects of B. wadsworthia colonization are not confounded by baseline impairments in Tbet−/− mice. These results support previous findings that B. wadsworthia promotes IFNg-producing Th1 cells in the intestine, and further reveal that Th1 cells are necessary for the ability of B. wadsworthia to impair cognitive behavior in mice.

Figure 5. Th1 cell Expansion Contributes to Bilophila-induced Impairments in Cognitive Behavior.

Figure 5.

A) Experimental timeline. B) Representative Barnes maze traces for Abx+WT mice compared to Abx mice colonized with B. wadsworthia with either WT or Tbet−/− background. White lines indicate movement trajectories, whereas blue hues denote increasing durations of time spent at a particular location. Orange circles denote the escape hole. C) Latency to enter the escape hole. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=7–11). D) Errors made. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=7–11). E) Errors made during the probe trial. (Unpaired two-tailed Students t-test, n=7–11). F) Search strategy used during the probe trial. (n=7–11). G) Experimental timeline. H) Representative Barnes maze traces for SPF KD Mock WT mice compared to SPF KD Hyp mice with either WT or Tbet −/− background. I) Latency to enter the escape hole. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=10). J) Errors made. (Two-way ANOVA with Sidak, n=10). K) Errors made during the probe trial. (Unpaired two-tailed Students t-test, n=10). L) Search strategy used during the probe trial. (n=10). Data are presented as mean ± S.E.M. * p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. n.s.=not statistically significant. SPF=specific pathogen-free (conventionally-colonized), Abx (conventionally-colonized mice treated with broad-spectrum antibiotics), WT= wildtype, Tbet −/−= knockout line for Tbet transcription factor, KD=ketogenic diet, Mock=intermittent normoxia exposure, Hyp=intermittent hypoxia exposure.

To further determine if Th1 cells contribute to the adverse effects of KD and Hyp on cognitive behavior, mice were reared as T-bet−/− or genetically WT, then exposed to both KD and Hyp (Figure 5G). Consistent with prior experiments using Swiss Webster mice (Figure 1), C57Bl/6J WT SPF mice exposed to both KD and Hyp exhibited impaired performance in the Barnes maze relative to Mock-exposed controls (Figures 5H5L). Notably, CI induced by KD and Hyp was associated with significant increases in serum IFNg levels (Figure S5G), and modest, but not statistically significant, increases in levels of IFNg-producing Th1 cells in the colonic lamina propria (Figures S5EF). We additionally find that mice exposed to KD and Hyp display elevated levels of serum IL-2 relative to controls (Figure S5H), which may contribute to the increased differentiation of naïve CD4+ cells to Th1 cells (Liao et al., 2013). Notably, we observe no differences in levels of classical pro-inflammatory cytokines IL-6, IL-1b, TNFa, KC/GRO or in regulatory cytokines IL-10 and IL-4 (Figures S5IO), suggesting no evidence of overt inflammation. Inhibiting Th1 induction through T-bet deficiency abrogated the adverse effects of KD and Hyp on cognitive behavior, as denoted by decreases in latency to enter the escape box, errors made and use of random search strategy, relative to exposed WT controls (Figures 5H5L). Altogether, these results implicate that B. wadsworthia impairs cognitive behavior by increasing IFNg-producing Th1 cells, and further suggest that microbiota-dependent increases in Th1 cells and IFNg contribute to the adverse effects of KD and Hyp on cognitive behavior.

DISCUSSION

Proof-of-concept studies have reported that complete absence or severe depletion of the microbiota results in widespread alterations in complex animal behaviors, including learning and memory (Chu et al., 2019; Desbonnet et al., 2014; Frohlich et al., 2016; Gareau et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2020; Mohle et al., 2016; Vuong and Hsiao, 2017; Yu et al., 2019). However, whether the microbiota is altered by physiologically relevant risk factors for cognitive dysfunction and whether there are select microbial species that are causally linked to CI has been poorly understood. Results from this study reveal that the gut microbiota is shaped by synergistic interactions between environmental risk factors for CI. They further demonstrate that select diet- and hypoxic stress-responsive bacteria from the gut microbiota disrupt hippocampal function and cognitive behavior, likely via the induction of pro-inflammatory immune cells.

In particular, we find that the high-fat, low carbohydrate KD exacerbates the detrimental effects of acute intermittent Hyp on cognitive behavior. This aligns with the so-called “two-hit” or “multiple-hit” hypotheses for neurological and neurodegenerative diseases (Heinemann et al., 2016), wherein multiple genetic and/or environmental risk factors interact to accelerate or predispose to symptoms of disease, including age-related cognitive decline (Zhu et al., 2007). Indeed, both high-fat diet and hypoxia are associated with CI across studies of humans and animal models (Arias-Cavieres et al., 2020; Beilharz et al., 2015; Jha et al., 2018). However, the KD and select ketone bodies, such as beta-hydroxybutyrate, have been recently explored as potential treatments for CI with variable results (Fortier et al., 2021; Park et al., 2020; Reger et al., 2004; Zhao et al., 2004). Findings from this study suggest that when combined with other environmental stressors, the KD can be detrimental to hippocampal function and cognitive behavior. This is consistent with one recent report that KD administration in a rat model of Alzheimer’s disease worsened cognitive performance in the Morris water maze (Park et al., 2020). Further research is warranted to uncover the molecular bases for interactions between varied genetic and environmental risk factors for CI.

Prior studies have implicated various physiological pathways in mediating the neurological effects of the KD, such as adenosine receptor activation, mitochondrial biogenesis, TCA anaplerosis, and altered ion channel expression (Elamin et al., 2020; Masino and Rho, 2012). Data from this study indicate that alterations in the gut microbiota contribute to the ability of the KD to potentiate the adverse effects of Hyp on cognitive behavior in the Barnes maze task. One important consideration is the biological bases of the abnormalities observed in the behavioral task. Adverse effects of the KD and Hyp are seen even during the first trial of the Barnes maze assay that persist through the sixth and last trial. While we observe no overt differences in stress-induced exploration in the open field, acoustic startle response, and sensorimotor gating in the prepulse inhibition task, the early abnormalities in the Barnes maze suggest that the KD and Hyp together induce cognitive issues that extend beyond disruptions in spatial learning and working memory. Indeed, depletion of the risk-associated microbiota diminishes the cognitive behavioral abnormalities, even in the first trial of testing, and also improves hippocampal LTP, suggesting that the microbiome may modify hippocampal-dependent learning and memory as well other aspects of cognitive performance. Overall, while the data reveal an influence of the gut microbiota in modulating cognitive behavior, the complexity of the behavioral phenotypes leave open the likely possibility that there are multiple microbiota-independent effects of diet and hypoxic stress that also contribute to the observed cognitive phenotypes.

In addition to identifying a synergistic effect of KD and Hyp on impairing cognitive behavior, we also reveal an interaction between KD and Hyp in enriching Bilophila species in the gut microbiota. While the exact mechanisms remain unknown, one study reported particular alterations in the gut microbiota that were seen when rats were exposed to both the high-fat, high-sugar diet and hypoxia in a model of obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) (Durgan et al., 2016). A separate paper studying intermittent hypoxia as a model of OSA found that fecal microbiome transplantation of hypoxia-associated samples sufficiently promoted murine sleep disturbances and that the microbial family Desulfovibrionaceae (OTU147), of which Bilophila is a member, was increased in hypoxia-associated samples and in children with OSA (Badran et al., 2020; Valentini et al., 2020). In a similar paradigm where atherosclerosis-prone Ldlr−/− mice were fed a high-fat diet, additional exposure to hypoxia and hypercapnia increased levels of select bile acids, particularly taurodeoxycholic acid (Tripathi et al., 2018).This may be relevant, as a separate study reported that a high-saturated fat diet promoted taurine conjugation of bile acids, and that taurocholic acid in particular promoted levels of B. wadsworthia in mice (Devkota et al., 2012). Based on these studies and the finding that B. wadsworthia is an obligate anaerobe (Devkota et al., 2012; Summanen et al., 1995), we speculate that the KD and Hyp together enrich Bilophila through combined modulation of bile acid profiles and increased anaerobicity of the intestinal microenvironment. Future co-culture and metabolic modeling experiments are needed to dissect how various environmental factors differentially influence microbial community structures.

Consistent with the finding that KD and Hyp together impair cognitive behavior and enrich Bilophila in the gut microbiota, we observe that colonization with B. wadsworthia leads to disrupted hippocampal physiology and cognitive deficits. These findings align with studies linking B. wadsworthia to increases in IFNg (Devkota et al., 2012), and separately, increases in IFNg to CI (Monteiro et al., 2016). While the molecular mechanisms by which Bilophila stimulates Th1 cell expansion remain unclear, prior research reported that lysates from Bilophila cultures are sufficient induce Th1 cell proliferation and IFNg production, as well as IL-12 production by dendritic cells (Devkota et al., 2012). These data suggest that acute exposure to molecular factors derived from Bilophila stimulate IL-12 from dendritic cells, which drives Th1 cell expansion (Devkota et al., 2012). Future advancements and continued research are needed to clearly identify the bacterial factors that enable Bilophila to stimulate IFNg-producing Th1 cells and cognitive impairment.

Notably, while results from our study highlight a likely role for Bilophila in contributing to environmental risk for CI (as induced by KD and Hyp in mice), they do not the preclude the possibility that other members of the microbiota may also modify cognitive behavior. In particular, 16S rRNA gene sequences from Bilophila are reported to comprise approximately 0.004–0.3% of 16S rRNA loads from the fecal microbiota of SPF mice (Caesar et al., 2015). In our experiments, KD and Hyp together (but not individually) increase the absolute abundance of Bilophila 16S rRNA gene copies in feces by ~1000X. As such, the physiological loads of B. wadsworthia in the presence of a complex SPF microbiota, even after increases seen in response to KD and Hyp, are much lower than those achieved by subsequent experiments involving bacterial monocolonization of GF mice or enrichment in Abx-treated mice. Results from our study indicate that gnotobiotic colonization with B. wadsworthia largely phenocopies the cognitive behavioral impairment, hippocampal dysfunction and Th1 induction seen in response to KD and Hyp (and their associated physiological increases in Bilophila). While we did not observe other bacterial taxa, aside from Bilophila, that were significantly elevated in response to both KD and Hyp, future studies of various risk factors for CI may reveal additional microbial taxa and signaling pathways that modify hippocampal function and cognitive behavior.

Consistent with previous literature indicating that B. wadsworthia promotes Th1 cell expansion (Devkota et al., 2012), we observe that T-bet deficiency prevents B. wadsworthia-induced increases in IFNg-producing Th1 cells, as well as the cognitive behavioral impairments seen in B. wadsworthia-colonized mice. Precisely how IFNg-producing Th1 cells promote cognitive behavioral abnormalities remains poorly understood. However, increases in Th1 cells and Th1 responses are commonly associated with aging-associated cognitive decline and CI in Alzheimer’s disease (Browne et al., 2013; Dulken et al., 2019). Moreover, prior literature reports that IFNg and its receptor (IFNGR) are necessary for mediating memory impairment in mice (Monteiro et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2020), and that IFNg is sufficient to drive hippocampal dysfunction and abnormal cognitive behavior in mice (Zhang et al., 2020). IFNg applied to hippocampal slices reduces gamma oscillations, which are critical to higher brain functioning and may be one explanation for how Bilophila manipulates hippocampal activity (Colgin and Moser, 2010; Ta et al., 2019). IL-4, which suppresses Th1 cell differentiation, also alters cognitive function (Derecki et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2020). The precise signaling mechanisms remain unclear as both neurons and microglia are known to express IFNGR and respond to IFNg (Mizuno et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2020). IFNg production by Th1 cells promotes microglial activation (Mount et al., 2007; Takeuchi et al., 2006; Zhou et al., 2015) and hippocampal-dependent cognitive dysfunction (Litteljohn et al., 2014; Monteiro et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2020). One study reported that IFNg induces neurotoxicity by reducing neuronal ATP production via IFNg-mediated GluR1 phosphorylation (Mizuno et al., 2008). IFNg is also reported to have the capacity to increase blood brain barrier permeability (Rahman et al., 2018). In addition to direct effects of IFNg in the brain, peripheral IFNg may act through as yet unknown indirect mechanisms to impact central neurocircuits underlying cognitive function. Peripheral and hippocampal IFNg corresponds with impaired Barnes maze performance due to chronic mild stress (Palumbo et al., 2018). In aged murine brains, T cell infiltration occurs in neurogenic niches and IFNg correlates with reduced neural stem cell function and proliferation (Dulken et al., 2019). Indeed, IFNg directly suppresses hippocampal neural stem/precursor cell proliferation and provokes neuronal apoptosis (Zhang et al., 2020). B. wadsworthia has also been identified in several human microbiome association studies as associating with aging (Shenghua et al., 2020) and aging-associated neurodegenerative disorders (Baldini et al., 2020; Lin et al., 2019; Vogt et al., 2017). Further studies are needed to determine how microbiota-dependent neuroimmune interactions may influence cognitive health.

As the prevalence of cognitive dysfunction continues to increase (Hale et al., 2020), identifying early and modifiable risk factors is critical to enabling early detection and intervention for CI. Results from this study reveal that the gut microbiota is altered by modeling select environmental risk factors for CI in mice and that changes in the gut microbiota, and Bilophila in particular, contribute to disruptions in hippocampal physiology and cognitive behavior. We propose that understanding the biological bases for how complex genetic, environmental and psychosocial factors together predispose to CI requires consideration of the gut microbiome, as an important interface between host genetics and environmental exposures and an integral regulator of nutrition, immunity, metabolism and behavior.

LIMITATIONS OF STUDY

Foundational prior research has revealed that the absence or depletion of the gut microbiota in rodents results in abnormalities in learning and memory across various behavioral paradigms (Chu et al., 2019; Desbonnet et al., 2014; Frohlich et al., 2016; Gareau et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2020; Mohle et al., 2016; Vuong and Hsiao, 2017; Yu et al., 2019). Herein we extend these works by modeling two environmental factors for CI—the high fat, low carbohydrate KD and intermittent hypoxia. We find that that these particular environmental factors interact to elicit select alterations in the gut microbiota. Furthermore, we find that these changes in the complex gut microbiota, which are characterized by increases in Bilophila in our particular study, contribute to hippocampal dysfunction and impaired cognitive behavior in the Barnes maze. Notably, mouse KDs with varying fat to carbohydrate ratios have been used in the existing literature with similar effects on host phenotypes like behavior and neurotransmission, but can also show some nuanced differences (Murphy et al., 2005; Ruskin et al., 2017). We implemented one common form of the mouse KD, based on prior studies (Olson et al., 2018; Samala et al., 2008). In addition, a wide range of hypoxia regimens have been reported to induce varying degrees of CI in mice (Deguil et al., 2016), and this study employs only one version of intermittent hypoxia (12% oxygen, 6 hours per day for 5 days) followed by one spatial learning and memory task—the Barnes maze-- at 4–7 days after the last day of hypoxia exposure. Importantly, this study models pre-exposure to the KD followed by challenge with Hyp, and finds that consumption of the KD exacerbates Hyp-induced CI in mice. Additional research is needed to determine if temporal differences in KD consumption would yield different results and in particular, to assess effects of the KD when supplied after Hyp exposure. Overall, whether the findings in this study will be generalizable across all variations of KD and hypoxia regimens and all cognitive behavioral paradigms in mice remains to be tested.

Similarly, we observe that Bilophila is enriched by the KD and hypoxia regimen used in this study, which aligns with reported increases in B. wadsworthia in response to high fat diet and elevations in taurocholic acid (Devkota et al., 2012; Summanen et al., 1995). This evidence suggests that KD- and Hyp- induced changes in bile acid metabolism and anaerobicity of the intestinal microenvironment may be responsible for enriching Bilophila. However, microbiota composition is known to vary across species, backgrounds, vendors, facilities, cages, sexes and ages, among many other factors, raising the question of whether KD- and Hyp-induced enrichment of Bilophila in particular will be seen universally despite variations in the composition of the SPF mouse microbiota. Nonetheless, we find that monocolonization with or enrichment of B. wadsworthia alone impairs hippocampal function and cognitive behavior in mice. This suggests that the findings herein may be relevant to other environmental risk factors or conditions that elevate B. wadsworthia.

Despite the caveats regarding the particular parameters used for KD, Hyp, SPF microbiota, and behavioral testing, we find that B. wadsworthia colonization impairs hippocampal LTP, gene expression and markers of neurogenesis, as well as cognitive behavior in the Barnes maze, in a manner that is dependent upon IFNg-producing Th1 cells. This aligns with existing literature linking IFNg and Th1 induction to abnormalities in hippocampal physiology, learning, memory and cognition (Litteljohn et al., 2014; Monteiro et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2020). Altogether, these findings support a role for microbiota-induced pro-inflammatory responses in contributing to brain and behavioral endophenotypes of CI.

STAR★METHODS

RESOURCE AVAILABILITY

Lead Contact

Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Elaine Hsiao (ehsiao@g.ucla.edu)

Materials Availability

This study did not generate new unique reagents.

Data and Code Availability

16S rRNA gene sequencing data and metadata are available through QIITA repository (https://qiita.ucsd.edu/) with the study accession # 13510. Hippocampal transcriptomic data are available on through Gene Expression Omnibus repository with the identification number # GSE163099.

EXPERIMENTAL MODELS AND SUBJECT DETAILS

Mice

4–6-week-old SPF wild-type Swiss Webster mice (Taconic Farms), GF wild-type Swiss Webster mice (Taconic Farms), SPF wild-type C57BL6/J mice (Jackson Laboratories), GF wild-type C57BL6/J (Jackson Laboratories), and SPF T-bet TBX21 knockout mice (B6.129S6-Tbx21tm1Glm/J, Jackson Laboratories) were bred in UCLA’s Center for Health Sciences Barrier Facility. Mice were randomly assigned to an experimental group. Experiments include age- and sex-matched cohorts of males and females. Mice were housed in autoclaved cages with irradiated food and sterile water and handled aseptically in a BSL2 biosafety cabinets with autoclaved gloves and sterile consumables. All animal experiments were approved by the UCLA Animal Care and Use Committee.

Bacteria

Bilophila wadsworthia (strain WAL7959), generously provided by Drs. Connie Ha and Suzanne Devkota (Cedars-Sinai Medical Center, Los Angeles, CA), was cultured under anoxic conditions (2–3% H2, 20% CO2, and the balance N2) at 37°C in Modified Brucella media supplemented with 1% taurine, iron, hemin, and vitamin K (Hardy Diagnostics). Clostridium cocleatum (DSMZ 1551) was grown under anoxic conditions (2–3% H2, 20% CO2, and the balance N2) at 37°C in Sweet E. Broth for Anaerobes (ATCC medium 1004). Cultures were authenticated by full-length 16S rRNA gene sequencing (Laragen, Inc.).

METHOD DETAILS

Dietary Treatment

Breeding GF mice were fed sterile “breeder” chow (Lab Diets 5K52). Experimental animals were either fed sterile standard chow (Lab Diets 5010), 6:1 ketogenic diet (Harlan Teklad TD.1150300), or vitamin- and mineral- matched control diet (Harlan Teklad TD.150300).

Acute Intermittent Hypoxia

Mice housed in the home cage were placed in an O2 Control InVivo cabinet (Coy Laboratories) and exposed to 12% oxygen for 6 hours per day for 5 consecutive days. Mock-treated mice were placed in the same chamber and exposed to ambient 21% oxygen for 6 hours per day for 5 consecutive days.

Barnes Maze Testing

Mice were tested for cognitive behavior in the Barnes maze (92 cm diameter, 5 cm hole diameter, 95 cm height; Noldus) using aseptic technique and procedures adapted from a previously published protocol (Attar et al., 2013). For GF mice, the absence of colonization was confirmed at the end of Barnes maze testing by fecal microbial culture under aerobic and anaerobic conditions, 16S rRNA gene qPCR relative to untested GF controls, and visual confirmation of enlarged cecal size consistent with GF status. Briefly, mice were acclimated for at least 1 hour before testing to the behavioral room, which featured consistent visual cues that varied in shape, color and placement. The maze was cleaned before and after each testing trial with 70% ethanol, followed by Accel disinfectant. All sessions were recorded using a Basler Gig3 camera and EthoVision XT (Noldus). For the habituation phase (day 1), mice were placed in a clear glass beaker in the center of the maze for 30 seconds, then slowly guided to the target hole and gently pushed into the escape box if they did not enter on their own accord. Mice were kept in the escape box for 1 minute and then allowed to explore the maze freely for 5 minutes before being returned to their home cages. During the training phase (Day 2–3), mice were tested for 3 trials on the first day, and 2 trials for the second day. For each trial, mice were first placed under an opaque cup in the center of the maze for 15 seconds. Then, the cylinder was removed, and mice were allowed to explore the maze for 5 minutes. Latency to enter was defined as the time elapsed for mice to identify the target hole correctly for the first time. Errors made were defined as nose pokes over incorrect holes. Distance traveled, velocity, and time in each quadrant were other recorded for every trial. Search strategy was also analyzed, where a “random” strategy was coded as greater than 3 errors in non-consecutive holes, a “serial” strategy was coded as errors occurring in consecutive holes, and a “spatial” strategy was coded as less than or equal to 3 hole errors. The probe trial (Day 4) was performed 24 hours after the final training trial. The escape box was removed, and mice were allowed to explore the maze for 5 minutes while latency to enter, distance traveled, errors made, velocity, search strategy and time in target were recorded. Search strategy data for all Barnes maze trials for all experimental groups are displayed in Figure S11. Relative differences in search strategy by pairwise comparison of relevant experimental groups relative to controls are shown in Figure S12.

Open Field Testing

The open field test is widely used to measure anxiety-like and locomotor behavior in rodents. Mice were placed in the center of a 50 cm x 50 cm arena for 10 min, during which an overhead Basler Gig3 camera and EthoVision XT (Noldus) software was used to measure distance traveled, and the number of entries and duration of time spent in the central 17 cm square area. The boxes were cleaned with 70% ethanol and Accel disinfectant before and after each session.

Prepulse Inhibition Testing

Prepulse inhibition is used to measure sensorimotor gating and acoustic startle and was performed according to protocols adapted from (Hsiao, 2013; Swerdlow and Geyer, 1998) in the UCLA Behavioral Testing Core (Franz Hall). Mice were acclimated to an SR-LAB testing chamber (SD Instruments) for 5 min, presented with six 120-dB pulses of white noise (startle stimulus), and then subjected to 14 randomized blocks of either no startle, 5-dB prepulse + startle, or 15-dB prepulse + startle. The startle response was recorded by a piezo-electric sensor and prepulse inhibition was defined as (startle stimulus only −5 or 15 dB prepulse + startle)/ startle stimulus only x 100.

Antibiotic Treatment

SPF mice were gavaged every 12 hours daily for 7 consecutive days with a solution of vancomycin (50mg/kg), neomycin (100 mg/kg) and metronidazole (100 mg/kg), as previously described (Reikvam et al., 2011). Ampicillin (1 mg/ml) was provided ad libitum in sterile drinking water. For mock treatment, mice were gavaged with normal drinking water every 12 hours daily for 7 days. Antibiotic-treated mice were maintained in sterile caging with sterile food and water and handled aseptically for the remainder of the experiments.

Fecal Microbiota Transplant

Fresh fecal samples were obtained from adult SPF Swiss Webster homogenized in 1 mL pre-reduced phosphate-buffered saline (PBS, pH = 7.4) per pellet. 100 μL of the suspension was immediately administered via oral gavage to recipient GF mice. For mock treatment, mice were gavaged with pre-reduced PBS.

16S rRNA Gene Sequencing

Total bacterial genomic DNA was extracted from mouse fecal samples using the Qiagen DNeasy PowerSoil Kit, where sample n reflects separate cages containing 2 mice per cage to reduce cage-dependent effects of variation and focus on biological variation. The library was prepared following previously published and validated methods (Caporaso et al., 2011). The V4 regions of the 16S rDNA gene were PCR amplified using individually barcoded universal primers and 30 ng of the extracted genomic DNA. The PCR reaction was set up in triplicate, and the PCR products were purified using the Qiaquick PCR purification kit (Qiagen). The purified PCR product was pooled in equal molar concentrations quantified by nanodrop and sequenced by Laragen, Inc. using the Illumina MiSeq platform and 2 × 250bp reagent kit for paired-end sequencing. Amplicon sequence variants (ASVs) were chosen after denoising with Deblur (Amir et al., 2017). Taxonomy assignment and rarefaction were performed using QIIME2–2018.6 (Bolyen et al., 2019)

Gnotobiotic Colonization and Antibiotic Enrichment

109 cfu bacteria were suspended in 200 μL pre-reduced PBS and orally gavaged into GF mice or Abx-treated mice. For mock treatment, mice were gavaged with pre-reduced PBS. Mice were maintained in microisolator cages and handled aseptically. Mice were behaviorally tested 7 days post-colonization. Colonization was confirmed via fecal plating on modified Brucella agar supplemented with 0.5 g/L ferric ammonium citrate.

Hippocampal Electrophysiology

For long-term potentiation, paired-pulse-facilitation, and population spike versus fEPSP slopes, we used protocols as previously described (Babiec et al., 2017; Ziehn et al., 2012). Based on prior literature the detrimental effects of Hyp on cognitive behavior primarily in male mice (Aubrecht et al., 2015, males were used for hippocampal electrophysiology. Mice were first deeply anesthetized with isoflurane, and following cervical dislocation, the brain was rapidly removed and submerged in ice-cold, oxygenated (95% O2/5% CO2) artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing (in mM) as follows: 124 NaCl, 4 KCl, 25 NaHCO3, 1 NaH2PO4, 2 CaCl2, 1.2 MgSO4, and 10 glucose (Sigma-Aldrich). While iced, the brain was hemisected, and the hippocampi removed. Slice were made using a manual tissue chopper in 400 μM sections and maintained at 30°C in interface-type chambers that were continuously perfused (2–3 ml/min) with oxygenated (95% O2/5% CO2) ACSF and allowed to recover in the interface chambers for at least 2 h before recordings. For all experiments, a bipolar nichrome wire stimulating electrode was placed in stratum radiatum of the CA1 region and used to activate Schaffer collateral fiber synapses. For paired pulse facilitation and population spike measurements, the recording electrode was placed in the pyramidal cell body layer. All recordings (LTP, paired pulse facilitation, and population spike measurements) were performed using borosilicate glass microelectrodes (5–10 M Ω) filled with ACSF and using an Ag/AgCl electrode. The stimulating and recording electrodes were placed approximately 0.75–1.0 mm apart for each assay. For LTP recordings, the initial maximal fEPSP amplitude was determined and the intensity of stimulation was adjusted to produce fEPSPs with an amplitude 50% of the maximal amplitude for all recorded responses to stimulus. Baseline recordings were taken for at least 20 minutes, followed by two trains of 1 second long 100 Hz stimulation with 10 seconds inter-train interval, then 60 minutes recording post-tetanus. The last five minutes of the 60 minute recording post-tetanus were used for statistical comparison. Paired-pulse facilitation was measured at impulse distances of 10, 20, 30, 40 and 50 ms by measuring the height of the population spike. Acquisition and analysis of data was performed with the pClamp family of programs from Axon Instruments Inc (Burlingame, CA). Slices were included if they showed a stable baseline, with fEPSP slides within 5% error of each other, for 20 minutes. Separate slices were used for recording LTP vs. paired pulse facilitation.

Hippocampal Transcriptomic Profiling

Hippocampi were microdissected and RNA extracted using the Qiagen RNEasy Mini Kit. RNA quality was assessed to be RIN>8.9 using the 4200 Tapestation (Agilent). RNA libraries were prepared using the QuantSeq FWD’ mRNA-Seq Library Prep Kit (Lexogen) and sequenced via the Illumina HiSeq platform by the UCLA Neuroscience Genomics Core. Sequences were filtered using FastQC v. 0.11.9 (Andrews, 2010) for quality control, followed by Trimmomatic (Bolger et al., 2014) to remove barcodes and reads with an average phred score of 33 (parameters: illuminaclip:2:30:6, slidingwindow:5:30, leading:30, trailing:30, crop:65, minlen:20). Parsed reads were then aligned to the mouse genome mm10 using HISAT2 (Kim et al., 2019). Read counts were obtained using HTSeq-count (Anders et al., 2015). Differential gene expression was determined using DESeq2 (Love et al., 2014). Heatmaps were constructed using the R package (Team, 2013) pheatmap (Kolde, 2015), GO term enrichment analysis was conducted using DAVID (Huang da et al., 2009a, b) and Protein-Protein network analysis using STRING (Szklarczyk et al., 2019).

Hippocampal Immunofluorescence Staining and Imaging

Hippocampi were microdissected, post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 24 hours, cryopreserved in 30% sucrose for 24 hours, embedded and frozen in OCT, and cryosectioned using a Leica CM1950 cryostat. 25 μm coronal sections were collected within a span of 200 μm and distributed between two slides beginning at the site of the hippocampal formation, determined in accordance to the Mouse P56 Coronal Reference Atlas of the Allen Institute (Lein et al., 2007). Slides were incubated in DAKO antigen retrieval solution (Agilent) at 90 °C for two minutes, washed, and then blocked (0.3%PBS-T, 5% BSA, 10% normal goat serum) for one hour at room temperature. Sections were incubated at 4°C for 48 hours using anti-DCX (Guinea Pig Polyclonal, 1:500, Millipore AB2253), washed and then incubated with Alexa Fluor secondaries (1:1000) for two hours at room temperature before being washed and mounted. Sections were imaged using a Zeiss LSM 780 confocal microscope at 20X magnification with 1.5 zoom across 8.4 μm section widths across 7 Z-stacks. Image optimization and orthogonal projections were performed in Zen Blue (Zeiss) and background removal was done in ImageJ (Schneider et al., 2012). Quantification of DCX was performed by tracing the granule cell layers of the DG and quantitating DCX+ within enclosed area using ImageJ (NIH) particle analysis (Schneider et al., 2012).

Lymphocyte Isolation and Flow Cytometry

Single-cell suspensions were prepared from colonic lamina propria using procedures adapted from (Takahara et al.). Colons were dissected, cut longitudinally with gentle fat removal, and contents washed away in 1 X PBS. Epithelial cell stripping was achieved through two 45-minute incubations shaking at 37 °C HBSS with 0.5M EDTA and 1M HEPES (ThermoFisher). Colonic tissue was then digested using two 45-minute incubations shaking at 37 °C in RPMI supplemented with 4% FCS, 0.5 mg/mL collagenase D (Sigma), 0.25 mg/mL DNAseI, grade II (Sigma), and 0.5 mg/mL dispase (Gibco). All lymphocytes were then collected and stimulated for two hours with 500 ng/mL PMA and 500 ng/mL ionomycin in RPMI with 10% FCS, 2% MEM NEAA, 2% Pen/strep, 2% sodium pyruvate, followed by 3-hour incubation with 500 ng/mL Brefeldin A. Cells were incubated on ice and in darkness with 1:600 viability dye for 20 min, followed by 30 min incubation on ice and in darkness with CD3-APC (Biolegend), CD4-PE (ThermoFisher), CD45-BV605 (Biolegend) at 1:200 in FACS buffer. Cells were next incubated with 100 μL of fixation buffer for 30 minutes in darkness, washed with 1X PBS, and stored in PBS at 4 °C until the next day. On the following day, cells were incubated with permeabilization buffer for 15 minutes in darkness at room temperature, followed by intracellular antibodies IL-17A- FITC (ThermoFisher) and IFNg-PE (ThermoFisher) for 30 minutes on ice in darkness at a 1:200 ratio in permeabilization buffer. Data were acquired on FACSCalibur (BD Biosciences) or the Attune NxT Flow Cytometer. All data contained within a graph were acquired on the same flow cytometer (GF data on the FACSCalibur, and Abx and SPF data on the Attune NxT, respectively). Data were analyzed using FlowJo (TreeStar) software.

Digital PCR

Briefly, each reaction was set up with 92.0 μL of DNA sample, 10 μL of ddPCR master mix (QX200 ddPCR EvaGreen Supermix, Bio Rad Laboratories), forward (UN00F2, 5′-CGCCGGTATCGAAATCGTGACAGCMGCCGCGGTAA 3′) and reverse (UN00R0, 5′-ATTCGCGGAAGGAGCGAGAG GGACTACHVGGGTWTCTAAT 3′ [1, 3]) primers (Integrated DNA Technologies) at the final concentration of 500 nM each, and ultrapure water (Thermo Fisher Scientific) to the final volume of 20 μL. In some experiments, additional DNA intercalating dye (EvaGreen, Biotium) was added to the reactions up to ×1 final concentration (to achieve up to ×2 overall concentration). Each reaction volume was converted to droplets using a QX200 droplet generator (Bio Rad Laboratories). Droplet samples were amplified on a thermocycler (C1000 Touch, Bio Rad Laboratories) with the following conditions according to the program: initial denaturation at 95°C for 5 min. followed by 40 cycles each consisting of denaturation at 95°C for 30 sec., annealing at 6552°C for 30 sec., and extension at 68°C for 60 sec.; followed by the dye stabilization step consisting of 5 min incubation at 4°C, 5 min incubation at 90°C, and incubation at 12°C for at least 5 min. Droplet samples were quantified on a QX200 Droplet Digital PCR System (Bio Rad Laboratories). The raw data were analyzed and the target molecule Sample concentrations were extracted using the accompanying software (QuantaSoft Software, Bio Rad Laboratories) with auto-thresholding conditions. Sample concentrations were then normalized to sample extraction mass and corrected for volume losses during extraction protocol. Lower limit of quantification of the assay was determined as 3X the no template control concentration normalized to the minimum sample extraction mass.

IFNg Quantification

Blood samples were collected by cardiac puncture and spun through serum separation tubes (SST vacutainers, Becton Dickinson). Serum IFNg concentrations were quantified using the V-PLEX Proinflammatory Panel 1 Mouse Kit (Meso Scale Diagnostics), according to the manufacturer’s instructions.

QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

Statistical analysis was performed using Prism software version 8.2.1 (GraphPad). Data were assessed for normal distribution and plotted in the figures as mean ± SEM. For each figure, n = the number of independent biological replicates. No samples or animals were excluded from the analyses. Differences between two treatment groups were assessed using two-tailed, unpaired Student t test with Welch’s correction. Differences among >2 groups with only one variable were assessed using one-way ANOVA with Sidak post hoc test. Taxonomic comparisons from 16S rDNA gene sequencing analysis were analyzed by Kruskal-Wallis test with Bonferroni post hoc test. Two- post-hoc test was used for ≥ 2 groups with two variables (e.g., Barnes maze latency to enter, errors made over 6 trials). Significant differences emerging from the above tests are indicated in the figures by *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001. Notable non-significant (and non-near significant) differences are indicated in the figures by “n.s.”.

Supplementary Material

1
2

Data S1: Source Data for All Figures, Related to Figures 15, S1S5

3

Table S1: Dietary Information, Related to Figure 1

4

Table S2: 16S rRNA Gene Sequencing Taxonomic Information, Related to Figure 3

5

Table S3: Hippocampal CA3 Relative Gene Expression, Related to Figure 4

KEY RESOURCES TABLE.

REAGENT or RESOURCE SOURCE IDENTIFIER
Antibodies
anti-DCX (Guinea Pig Polyclonal) Millipore Cat#AB2253
IL-17A Monoclonal Antibody (eBio17B7), FITC ThermoFisher Cat#11-7177-81
IFN gamma Monoclonal Antibody, (XMG1.2), PE ThermoFisher Cat#12-7311-81
CD3 Monoclonal Antibody, (17A2), APC BioLegend Cat#100236
CD4 Monoclonal Antibody, (GK1.5), PE-eFluor 610 ThermoFisher Cat#61-0041-80
CD45 Monoclonal Antibody, (30-F11), BV605 BioLegend Cat#103140
Bacterial and Virus Strains
Clostridium cocleatum Deutsche Sammlung von Mikroorganismen und Zellkulturen (DSMZ) DSMZ# 1551
Bilophila wadsworthia Murine-associated strain gifted from Drs. Connie Ha and Suzanne Devkota Strain WAL7959
Chemicals, Peptides, and Recombinant Proteins
Vancomycin hydrochloride Chem-Impex International Cat#00315
Neomycin trisulfate salt hydrate Sigma-Aldrich Cat#N1876
Metronidazole Sigma-Aldrich Cat#M1547
Ampicillin sodium salt Sigma-Aldrich Cat#A9518
NaCl Sigma-Aldrich Cat#S7653
KCl Sigma-Aldrich Cat#P3911
NaHCO3 Sigma-Aldrich Cat#S5761
NaH2PO4 Sigma-Aldrich Cat#S0751
CaCl2 Sigma-Aldrich Cat#C1016
MgSO4 Sigma-Aldrich Cat#M7506
Glucose Sigma-Aldrich Cat#47829
Brucella media Hardy Diagnostics Cat#C5311
Ferric ammonium citrate Fisher Scientific Cat#172-500
DAKO antigen retrieval solution Agilent Cat#S1699
RPMI 1640 Sigma-Aldrich Cat#11875093
EDTA ThermoFisher Cat#15575020
HEPES ThermoFisher Cat#15630080
HBSS ThermoFisher Cat#14170112
Collagenase D Sigma Aldrich Cat#11088866001
DNAse I, Grade II Sigma Aldrich Cat#10104159001
Dispase Gibco Cat#17105041
Ionomycin calcium salt Sigma Aldrich Cat#I3909
PMA Sigma Aldrich Cat#P1585
Brefeldin A Sigma Aldrich Cat#B7651
MEM NEAA ThermoFisher Cat#10370021
Penicillin-streptomycin (Pen/strep) ThermoFisher Cat#15070063
Ultrapure water ThermoFisher Cat#10977015
DNA intercalating dye (EvaGreen) Biotium Cat#31000
Sodium pyruvate Gibco Cat#11360070
Critical Commercial Assays
DNeasy PowerSoil Kit Qiagen Cat#12888-50
Qiaquick PCR purification kit Qiagen Cat#28104
RNEasy Mini Kit Qiagen Cat#74104
QuantSeq FWD’ mRNA-Seq Library Prep Kit Lexogen N/A
LIVE/DEAD™ Fixable Aqua Dead Cell Stain Kit ThermoFisher Cat#L34957
Intracellular Fixation & Permeabilization Buffer Set ThermoFisher Cat#88-8824-00
QX200 ddPCR EvaGreen Supermix Bio-Rad Laboratories Cat#186-4033
V-PLEX Proinflammatory Panel 1 Mouse Kit Meso Scale Diagnostics Cat#K15048D
Deposited Data
16S rRNA gene sequencing https://qiita.ucsd.edu Study ID: 13510
Hippocampal transcriptomic data Gene Expression Omnibus Study ID: GSE163099
Experimental Models: Organisms/Strains
Swiss Webster mice Taconic Farms Cat#Tac:SW
C57BL6/J mice Jackson Laboratories Cat#000664
T-bet TBX21 knockout mice (B6.129S6-Tbx21tm1Glm/J) Jackson Laboratories Cat#004648
Oligonucleotides
Forward primer for digital PCR: UN00F2, 5′-CAGCMGCCGCGGTAA-3 Integrated DNA Technologies N/A
Reverse primer for digital PCR: UN00R0, 5′-GGACTACHVGGGTWTCTAAT-3′ [1, 3]) Integrated DNA Technologies N/A
Software and Algorithms
EthoVision XT Noldus
Deblur https://github.com/biocore/deblur Amir et al., 2017
QIIME2-2018.6 https://qiime2.org/ Bolyen et al.
FastQC v. 0.11.9 https://github.com/s-andrews/FastQC/releases/tag/v0.11.9 Andrews, 2010
Trimmomatic https://github.com/timflutre/trimmomatic Bolger et al.
HISAT2 http://daehwankimlab.github.io/hisat2/ Kim et al.
HTSeq-count https://github.com/htseq/htseq Anders et al.
DESeq2 https://bioconductor.org/packages/release/bioc/html/DESeq2.html Love et al.
R package https://www.r-project.org/ Team, 2013
DAVID https://david.ncifcrf.gov/gene2gene.jsp (Huang da et al., 2009a, b
STRING https://string-db.org/ Szklarczyk et al., 2019
QuantaSoft Software Bio-Rad Laboratories Cat#1864011
Prism software version 8.2.1 GraphPad
Other
“Breeder” chow Lab Diets Cat#5K52
Standard chow Lab Diets Cat#5010
Ketogenic diet Harlan Teklad Cat#TD.1150300
Control diet Harlan Teklad Cat#TD.150300
O2 Control InVivo cabinet Coy Laboratories Cat#Model 30
GigE camera Basler Cat#acA1280-60gc
4200 Tapestation System Agilent Cat#G2991AA
QX200 Droplet Generator Bio-Rad Laboratories Cat#1864002

HIGHLIGHTS.

  • The ketogenic diet and hypoxia synergistically impair cognitive behavior in mice.

  • Depleting or transplanting the gut microbiota modulates cognitive behavior in mice.

  • Bilophila wadsworthia disrupts cognitive behavior and hippocampal physiology.

  • IFNg-producing Th1 cells contribute to microbiota-mediated cognitive impairment.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We thank members of the Hsiao laboratory for their critical review of the manuscript; Dr. Alcino Silva helpful advice regarding behavioral testing; Drs. Thomas O’Dell and Walter Babiec for critical training and advice on hippocampal electrophysiology; Irina Zhuravka of the UCLA Behavioral Testing Core for behavioral assay training; Dr. Matteo Pellegrini (UCLA) for helpful advice regarding analysis of RNA sequencing data; Drs. Suzanne Devkota and Connie Ha (Cedars Sinai) for generously supplying Bilophila wadsworthia; Dr. Said Bogatryev for assistance with experiments for digital PCR and serum IFNg measurements; and Dr. Timothy O’Sullivan for allowing usage of his Attune NxT flow cytometer. This work was supported by funds from an NIH Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (#F31 AG064844) and UCLA Dissertation Year Fellowship to C.A.O., Weston Family Foundation Fellowship to P.F., the Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (#F31 HD101270) to G.N.P., the Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (#F31 NS118966) to K.G.J., Army Research Office Multidisciplinary University Research Initiative (W911NF-17-1-0402 to E.Y.H. and R.F.I.). E.Y.H. is a New York Stem Cell Foundation - Robertson Investigator. This research was supported in part by the New York Stem Cell Foundation. This project has been made possible in part by grant number 2018-191860 from the Chan Zuckerberg Initiative DAF, an advised fund of Silicon Valley Community Foundation.

Footnotes

DECLARATION OF INTERESTS

The authors declare no competing interests.

DIVERSITY AND INCLUSION

We worked to ensure sex balance in the selection of non-human subjects. One or more of the authors of this paper self-identifies as an underrepresented ethnic minority in science. While citing references scientifically relevant for this work, we also actively worked to promote gender balance in our reference list.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

REFERENCES

  1. Akinyemi RO, Mukaetova-Ladinska EB, Attems J, Ihara M, and Kalaria RN (2013). Vascular risk factors and neurodegeneration in ageing related dementias: Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Curr Alzheimer Res 10, 642–653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amir A, McDonald D, Navas-Molina JA, Kopylova E, Morton JT, Zech Xu Z, Kightley EP, Thompson LR, Hyde ER, Gonzalez A, et al. (2017). Deblur Rapidly Resolves Single-Nucleotide Community Sequence Patterns. mSystems 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anders S, Pyl PT, and Huber W (2015). HTSeq--a Python framework to work with high-throughput sequencing data. Bioinformatics 31, 166–169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andrews S (2010). FastQC: a quality control tool for high throughput sequence data. Available online at: http://www.bioinformatics.babraham.ac.uk/projects/fastqc
  5. Arcego DM, Toniazzo AP, Krolow R, Lampert C, Berlitz C, Dos Santos Garcia E, do Couto Nicola F, Hoppe JB, Gaelzer MM, Klein CP, et al. (2018). Impact of High-Fat Diet and Early Stress on Depressive-Like Behavior and Hippocampal Plasticity in Adult Male Rats. Molecular neurobiology 55, 2740–2753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Arias-Cavieres A, Khuu MA, Nwakudu CU, Barnard JE, Dalgin G, and Garcia AJ 3rd (2020). A HIF1a-Dependent Pro-Oxidant State Disrupts Synaptic Plasticity and Impairs Spatial Memory in Response to Intermittent Hypoxia. eNeuro 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Attar A, Liu T, Chan W-TC, Hayes J, Nejad M, Lei K, and Bitan G (2013). A Shortened Barnes Maze Protocol Reveals Memory Deficits at 4-Months of Age in the Triple-Transgenic Mouse Model of Alzheimer’s Disease. PloS one 8, e80355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Aubrecht TG, Jenkins R, Magalang UJ, and Nelson RJ (2015). Influence of gonadal hormones on the behavioral effects of intermittent hypoxia in mice. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 308, R489–499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Babiec WE, Jami SA, Guglietta R, Chen PB, and O’Dell TJ (2017). Differential Regulation of NMDA Receptor-Mediated Transmission by SK Channels Underlies Dorsal-Ventral Differences in Dynamics of Schaffer Collateral Synaptic Function. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 37, 1950–1964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Badran M, Khalyfa A, Ericsson A, and Gozal D (2020). Fecal microbiota transplantation from mice exposed to chronic intermittent hypoxia elicits sleep disturbances in naïve mice. Exp Neurol 334, 113439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bahr LS, Bock M, Liebscher D, Bellmann-Strobl J, Franz L, Prüß A, Schumann D, Piper SK, Kessler CS, Steckhan N, et al. (2020). Ketogenic diet and fasting diet as Nutritional Approaches in Multiple Sclerosis (NAMS): protocol of a randomized controlled study. Trials 21, 3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Baldini F, Hertel J, Sandt E, Thinnes CC, Neuberger-Castillo L, Pavelka L, Betsou F, Krüger R, and Thiele I (2020). Parkinson’s disease-associated alterations of the gut microbiome predict disease-relevant changes in metabolic functions. BMC Biol 18, 62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Beilharz JE, Maniam J, and Morris MJ (2015). Diet-Induced Cognitive Deficits: The Role of Fat and Sugar, Potential Mechanisms and Nutritional Interventions. Nutrients 7, 6719–6738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Biswal S, Sharma D, Kumar K, Nag TC, Barhwal K, Hota SK, and Kumar B (2016). Global hypoxia induced impairment in learning and spatial memory is associated with precocious hippocampal aging. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 133, 157–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bolger AM, Lohse M, and Usadel B (2014). Trimmomatic: a flexible trimmer for Illumina sequence data. Bioinformatics 30, 2114–2120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bolyen E, Rideout JR, Dillon MR, Bokulich NA, Abnet CC, Al-Ghalith GA, Alexander H, Alm EJ, Arumugam M, Asnicar F, et al. (2019). Reproducible, interactive, scalable and extensible microbiome data science using QIIME 2. In Nat Biotechnol, pp. 852–857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bravo JA, Forsythe P, Chew MV, Escaravage E, Savignac HM, Dinan TG, Bienenstock J, and Cryan JF (2011). Ingestion of Lactobacillus strain regulates emotional behavior and central GABA receptor expression in a mouse via the vagus nerve. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108, 16050–16055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Browne TC, McQuillan K, McManus RM, O’Reilly JA, Mills KH, and Lynch MA (2013). IFN-γ Production by amyloid β-specific Th1 cells promotes microglial activation and increases plaque burden in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. J Immunol 190, 2241–2251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Buffington SA, Di Prisco GV, Auchtung TA, Ajami NJ, Petrosino JF, and Costa-Mattioli M (2016). Microbial Reconstitution Reverses Maternal Diet-Induced Social and Synaptic Deficits in Offspring. Cell 165, 1762–1775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Caesar R, Tremaroli V, Kovatcheva-Datchary P, Cani PD, and Bäckhed F (2015). Crosstalk between Gut Microbiota and Dietary Lipids Aggravates WAT Inflammation through TLR Signaling. Cell metabolism 22, 658–668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Caporaso JG, Lauber CL, Walters WA, Berg-Lyons D, Lozupone CA, Turnbaugh PJ, Fierer N, and Knight R (2011). Global patterns of 16S rRNA diversity at a depth of millions of sequences per sample. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 Suppl 1, 4516–4522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chu C, Murdock MH, Jing D, Won TH, Chung H, Kressel AM, Tsaava T, Addorisio ME, Putzel GG, Zhou L, et al. (2019). The microbiota regulate neuronal function and fear extinction learning. Nature 574, 543–548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Colgin LL, and Moser EI (2010). Gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. Physiology (Bethesda) 25, 319–329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cordner ZA, and Tamashiro KL (2015). Effects of high-fat diet exposure on learning & memory. Physiol Behav 152, 363–371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. D’Amato A, Di Cesare Mannelli L, Lucarini E, Man AL, Le Gall G, Branca JJV, Ghelardini C, Amedei A, Bertelli E, Regoli M, et al. (2020). Faecal microbiota transplant from aged donor mice affects spatial learning and memory via modulating hippocampal synaptic plasticity- and neurotransmission-related proteins in young recipients. Microbiome 8, 140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Das SK, Dhar P, Sharma VK, Barhwal K, Hota SK, Norboo T, and Singh SB (2018). High altitude with monotonous environment has significant impact on mood and cognitive performance of acclimatized lowlanders: Possible role of altered serum BDNF and plasma homocysteine level. J Affect Disord 237, 94–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. David LA, Maurice CF, Carmody RN, Gootenberg DB, Button JE, Wolfe BE, Ling AV, Devlin AS, Varma Y, Fischbach MA, et al. (2014). Diet rapidly and reproducibly alters the human gut microbiome. Nature 505, 559–563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. de Aquino Lemos V, Antunes HK, dos Santos RV, Lira FS, Tufik S, and de Mello MT (2012). High altitude exposure impairs sleep patterns, mood, and cognitive functions. Psychophysiology 49, 1298–1306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Deguil J, Ravasi L, Lanteaume L, Lamberty Y, and Bordet R (2016). Translational Challenge Models in Support of Efficacy Studies: Effect of Cerebral Hypoxia on Cognitive Performances in Rodents. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets 15, 765–776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Derecki NC, Cardani AN, Yang CH, Quinnies KM, Crihfield A, Lynch KR, and Kipnis J (2010). Regulation of learning and memory by meningeal immunity: a key role for IL-4. J Exp Med 207, 1067–1080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Desbonnet L, Clarke G, Shanahan F, Dinan TG, and Cryan JF (2014). Microbiota is essential for social development in the mouse. Mol Psychiatry 19, 146–148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Devkota S, Wang Y, Musch MW, Leone V, Fehlner-Peach H, Nadimpalli A, Antonopoulos DA, Jabri B, and Chang EB (2012). Dietary-fat-induced taurocholic acid promotes pathobiont expansion and colitis in Il10−/− mice. Nature 487, 104–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dulken BW, Buckley MT, Navarro Negredo P, Saligrama N, Cayrol R, Leeman DS, George BM, Boutet SC, Hebestreit K, Pluvinage JV, et al. (2019). Single-cell analysis reveals T cell infiltration in old neurogenic niches. Nature 571, 205–210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Duncombe J, Kitamura A, Hase Y, Ihara M, Kalaria RN, and Horsburgh K (2017). Chronic cerebral hypoperfusion: a key mechanism leading to vascular cognitive impairment and dementia. Closing the translational gap between rodent models and human vascular cognitive impairment and dementia. Clin Sci (Lond) 131, 2451–2468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Durgan DJ, Ganesh BP, Cope JL, Ajami NJ, Phillips SC, Petrosino JF, Hollister EB, and Bryan RM Jr. (2016). Role of the Gut Microbiome in Obstructive Sleep Apnea-Induced Hypertension. Hypertension 67, 469–474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dutton SB, Sawyer NT, Kalume F, Jumbo-Lucioni P, Borges K, Catterall WA, and Escayg A (2011). Protective effect of the ketogenic diet in Scn1a mutant mice. Epilepsia 52, 2050–2056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Elamin M, Ruskin DN, Sacchetti P, and Masino SA (2020). A unifying mechanism of ketogenic diet action: The multiple roles of nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide. Epilepsy Res 167, 106469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Fortier M, Castellano CA, St-Pierre V, Myette-Côté É, Langlois F, Roy M, Morin MC, Bocti C, Fulop T, Godin JP, et al. (2021). A ketogenic drink improves cognition in mild cognitive impairment: Results of a 6-month RCT. Alzheimers Dement 17, 543–552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Frohlich EE, Farzi A, Mayerhofer R, Reichmann F, Jacan A, Wagner B, Zinser E, Bordag N, Magnes C, Frohlich E, et al. (2016). Cognitive impairment by antibiotic-induced gut dysbiosis: Analysis of gut microbiota-brain communication. Brain Behav Immun 56, 140–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gareau MG, Wine E, Rodrigues DM, Cho JH, Whary MT, Philpott DJ, Macqueen G, and Sherman PM (2011). Bacterial infection causes stress-induced memory dysfunction in mice. Gut 60, 307–317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Giuliani A, Sivilia S, Baldassarro VA, Gusciglio M, Lorenzini L, Sannia M, Calzà L, and Giardino L (2019). Age-Related Changes of the Neurovascular Unit in the Cerebral Cortex of Alzheimer Disease Mouse Models: A Neuroanatomical and Molecular Study. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 78, 101–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Goldbart AD, Row BW, Kheirandish-Gozal L, Cheng Y, Brittian KR, and Gozal D (2006). High fat/refined carbohydrate diet enhances the susceptibility to spatial learning deficits in rats exposed to intermittent hypoxia. Brain research 1090, 190–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gozal E, Gozal D, Pierce WM, Thongboonkerd V, Scherzer JA, Sachleben LR Jr., Brittian KR, Guo SZ, Cai J, and Klein JB (2002). Proteomic analysis of CA1 and CA3 regions of rat hippocampus and differential susceptibility to intermittent hypoxia. Journal of neurochemistry 83, 331–345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hale JM, Schneider DC, Gampe J, Mehta NK, and Myrskylä M (2020). Trends in the Risk of Cognitive Impairment in the United States, 1996–2014. Epidemiology 31, 745–754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Han B, Chen H, Yao Y, Liu X, Nie C, Min J, Zeng Y, and Lutz MW (2020). Genetic and non-genetic factors associated with the phenotype of exceptional longevity & normal cognition. Sci Rep 10, 19140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Heinemann SD, Posimo JM, Mason DM, Hutchison DF, and Leak RK (2016). Synergistic stress exacerbation in hippocampal neurons: Evidence favoring the dual-hit hypothesis of neurodegeneration. Hippocampus 26, 980–994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hoban AE, Moloney RD, Golubeva AV, McVey Neufeld KA, O’Sullivan O, Patterson E, Stanton C, Dinan TG, Clarke G, and Cryan JF (2016). Behavioural and neurochemical consequences of chronic gut microbiota depletion during adulthood in the rat. Neuroscience 339, 463–477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hsiao EY (2013). Immune dysregulation in autism spectrum disorder. International review of neurobiology 113, 269–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hsiao EY, McBride SW, Hsien S, Sharon G, Hyde ER, McCue T, Codelli JA, Chow J, Reisman SE, Petrosino JF, et al. (2013). Microbiota modulate behavioral and physiological abnormalities associated with neurodevelopmental disorders. Cell 155, 1451–1463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Huang da W, Sherman BT, and Lempicki RA (2009a). Bioinformatics enrichment tools: paths toward the comprehensive functional analysis of large gene lists. Nucleic Acids Res 37, 1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Huang da W, Sherman BT, and Lempicki RA (2009b). Systematic and integrative analysis of large gene lists using DAVID bioinformatics resources. Nature protocols 4, 44–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Jha NK, Jha SK, Sharma R, Kumar D, Ambasta RK, and Kumar P (2018). Hypoxia-Induced Signaling Activation in Neurodegenerative Diseases: Targets for New Therapeutic Strategies. Journal of Alzheimer’s disease : JAD 62, 15–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kanoski SE, and Davidson TL (2011a). Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity. Physiol Behav 103, 59–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kanoski SE, and Davidson TL (2011b). Western Diet Consumption and Cognitive Impairment: Links to Hippocampal Dysfunction and Obesity. Physiology & behavior 103, 59–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kanoski SE, Zhang Y, Zheng W, and Davidson TL (2010). The effects of a high-energy diet on hippocampal function and blood-brain barrier integrity in the rat. Journal of Alzheimer’s disease : JAD 21, 207–219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kesby JP, Kim JJ, Scadeng M, Woods G, Kado DM, Olefsky JM, Jeste DV, Achim CL, and Semenova S (2015). Spatial Cognition in Adult and Aged Mice Exposed to High-Fat Diet. PloS one 10, e0140034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kim D, Paggi JM, Park C, Bennett C, and Salzberg SL (2019). Graph-based genome alignment and genotyping with HISAT2 and HISAT-genotype. Nat Biotechnol 37, 907–915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kolde R (2015). pheatmap: Pretty heatmaps. ftp://cran.r-project.org/pub/R/web/packages/pheatmap/pheatmap.pdf.
  59. Lauritzen KH, Hasan-Olive MM, Regnell CE, Kleppa L, Scheibye-Knudsen M, Gjedde A, Klungland A, Bohr VA, Storm-Mathisen J, and Bergersen LH (2016). A ketogenic diet accelerates neurodegeneration in mice with induced mitochondrial DNA toxicity in the forebrain. Neurobiol Aging 48, 34–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lein ES, Hawrylycz MJ, Ao N, Ayres M, Bensinger A, Bernard A, Boe AF, Boguski MS, Brockway KS, Byrnes EJ, et al. (2007). Genome-wide atlas of gene expression in the adult mouse brain. Nature 445, 168–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Liao W, Lin JX, and Leonard WJ (2013). Interleukin-2 at the crossroads of effector responses, tolerance, and immunotherapy. Immunity 38, 13–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lin CH, Chen CC, Chiang HL, Liou JM, Chang CM, Lu TP, Chuang EY, Tai YC, Cheng C, Lin HY, et al. (2019). Altered gut microbiota and inflammatory cytokine responses in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of neuroinflammation 16, 129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Litteljohn D, Nelson E, and Hayley S (2014). IFN-γ differentially modulates memory-related processes under basal and chronic stressor conditions. Front Cell Neurosci 8, 391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Liu Z, Dai X, Zhang H, Shi R, Hui Y, Jin X, Zhang W, Wang L, Wang Q, Wang D, et al. (2020). Gut microbiota mediates intermittent-fasting alleviation of diabetes-induced cognitive impairment. Nature communications 11, 855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lobionda S, Sittipo P, Kwon HY, and Lee YK (2019). The Role of Gut Microbiota in Intestinal Inflammation with Respect to Diet and Extrinsic Stressors. Microorganisms 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Love MI, Huber W, and Anders S (2014). Moderated estimation of fold change and dispersion for RNA-seq data with DESeq2. Genome Biol 15, 550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Marques TM, Wall R, Ross RP, Fitzgerald GF, Ryan CA, and Stanton C (2010). Programming infant gut microbiota: influence of dietary and environmental factors. Curr Opin Biotechnol 21, 149–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Masino SA, and Rho JM (2012). Mechanisms of Ketogenic Diet Action. In Jasper’s Basic Mechanisms of the Epilepsies, Noebels JL, Avoli M, Rogawski MA, Olsen RW, and Delgado-Escueta AV, eds. (National Center for Biotechnology Information (US), Bethesda, MD: ). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Mavrodaris A, Powell J, and Thorogood M (2013). Prevalences of dementia and cognitive impairment among older people in sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review. Bull World Health Organ 91, 773–783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. McDonald TJW, and Cervenka MC (2019). Lessons learned from recent clinical trials of ketogenic diet therapies in adults. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 22, 418–424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Mei X, Tan G, and Qing W (2020). AMPK activation increases postoperative cognitive impairment in intermittent hypoxia rats via direct activating PAK2. Behavioural brain research 379, 112344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Mizuno T, Zhang G, Takeuchi H, Kawanokuchi J, Wang J, Sonobe Y, Jin S, Takada N, Komatsu Y, and Suzumura A (2008). Interferon-gamma directly induces neurotoxicity through a neuron specific, calcium-permeable complex of IFN-gamma receptor and AMPA GluR1 receptor. Faseb j 22, 1797–1806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Mohle L, Mattei D, Heimesaat MM, Bereswill S, Fischer A, Alutis M, French T, Hambardzumyan D, Matzinger P, Dunay IR, et al. (2016). Ly6C(hi) Monocytes Provide a Link between Antibiotic-Induced Changes in Gut Microbiota and Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis. Cell reports 15, 1945–1956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Monteiro S, Ferreira FM, Pinto V, Roque S, Morais M, de Sá-Calçada D, Mota C, Correia-Neves M, and Cerqueira JJ (2016). Absence of IFNγ promotes hippocampal plasticity and enhances cognitive performance. Translational psychiatry 6, e707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Mount MP, Lira A, Grimes D, Smith PD, Faucher S, Slack R, Anisman H, Hayley S, and Park DS (2007). Involvement of interferon-gamma in microglial-mediated loss of dopaminergic neurons. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 27, 3328–3337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Murphy P, Likhodii SS, Hatamian M, and McIntyre Burnham W (2005). Effect of the ketogenic diet on the activity level of Wistar rats. Pediatr Res 57, 353–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Nagpal R, Neth BJ, Wang S, Craft S, and Yadav H (2019). Modified Mediterranean-ketogenic diet modulates gut microbiome and short-chain fatty acids in association with Alzheimer’s disease markers in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. EBioMedicine 47, 529–542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Noble EE, Hsu TM, and Kanoski SE (2017). Gut to Brain Dysbiosis: Mechanisms Linking Western Diet Consumption, the Microbiome, and Cognitive Impairment. Front Behav Neurosci 11, 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. O’Toole PW, and Jeffery IB (2015). Gut microbiota and aging. Science (New York, NY) 350, 1214–1215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Ogbonnaya ES, Clarke G, Shanahan F, Dinan TG, Cryan JF, and O’Leary OF (2015). Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis Is Regulated by the Microbiome. Biological Psychiatry 78, e7–e9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Olson CA, Vuong HE, Yano JM, Liang QY, Nusbaum DJ, and Hsiao EY (2018). The Gut Microbiota Mediates the Anti-Seizure Effects of the Ketogenic Diet. Cell 173, 1728–1741 e1713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Ota M, Matsuo J, Ishida I, Takano H, Yokoi Y, Hori H, Yoshida S, Ashida K, Nakamura K, Takahashi T, et al. (2019). Effects of a medium-chain triglyceride-based ketogenic formula on cognitive function in patients with mild-to-moderate Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett 690, 232–236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Palumbo ML, Di Rosso ME, Simon EH, Gonzalez Murano MR, and Genaro AM (2018). Altered interferon-γ expression in lymphocytes as a potential peripheral marker of chronic stress-induced cognitive deficit. Cytokine 107, 26–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Park S, Zhang T, Wu X, and Yi Qiu J (2020). Ketone production by ketogenic diet and by intermittent fasting has different effects on the gut microbiota and disease progression in an Alzheimer’s disease rat model. J Clin Biochem Nutr 67, 188–198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Phillips MCL, Murtagh DKJ, Gilbertson LJ, Asztely FJS, and Lynch CDP (2018). Low-fat versus ketogenic diet in Parkinson’s disease: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Mov Disord 33, 1306–1314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Pistell PJ, Morrison CD, Gupta S, Knight AG, Keller JN, Ingram DK, and Bruce-Keller AJ (2010). Cognitive impairment following high fat diet consumption is associated with brain inflammation. J Neuroimmunol 219, 25–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Pun M, Guadagni V, Drogos LL, Pon C, Hartmann SE, Furian M, Lichtblau M, Muralt L, Bader PR, Moraga FA, et al. (2019). Cognitive Effects of Repeated Acute Exposure to Very High Altitude Among Altitude-Experienced Workers at 5050 m. High Alt Med Biol 20, 361–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Qaid E, Zakaria R, Sulaiman SF, Yusof NM, Shafin N, Othman Z, Ahmad AH, and Aziz CA (2017). Insight into potential mechanisms of hypobaric hypoxia-induced learning and memory deficit - Lessons from rat studies. Hum Exp Toxicol 36, 1315–1325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Rahman MT, Ghosh C, Hossain M, Linfield D, Rezaee F, Janigro D, Marchi N, and van Boxel-Dezaire AHH (2018). IFN-γ, IL-17A, or zonulin rapidly increase the permeability of the blood-brain and small intestinal epithelial barriers: Relevance for neuro-inflammatory diseases. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 507, 274–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Rea K, Dinan TG, and Cryan JF (2016). The microbiome: A key regulator of stress and neuroinflammation. Neurobiol Stress 4, 23–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Reger MA, Henderson ST, Hale C, Cholerton B, Baker LD, Watson GS, Hyde K, Chapman D, and Craft S (2004). Effects of beta-hydroxybutyrate on cognition in memory-impaired adults. Neurobiol Aging 25, 311–314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Reikvam DH, Erofeev A, Sandvik A, Grcic V, Jahnsen FL, Gaustad P, McCoy KD, Macpherson AJ, Meza-Zepeda LA, and Johansen FE (2011). Depletion of murine intestinal microbiota: effects on gut mucosa and epithelial gene expression. PloS one 6, e17996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Ruskin DN, Fortin JA, Bisnauth SN, and Masino SA (2017). Ketogenic diets improve behaviors associated with autism spectrum disorder in a sex-specific manner in the EL mouse. Physiol Behav 168, 138–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Samala R, Willis S, and Borges K (2008). Anticonvulsant profile of a balanced ketogenic diet in acute mouse seizure models. Epilepsy Res 81, 119–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Sampson TR, and Mazmanian SK (2015). Control of brain development, function, and behavior by the microbiome. Cell host & microbe 17, 565–576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Savignac HM, Tramullas M, Kiely B, Dinan TG, and Cryan JF (2015). Bifidobacteria modulate cognitive processes in an anxious mouse strain. Behavioural Brain Research 287, 59–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Sbihi H, Boutin RC, Cutler C, Suen M, Finlay BB, and Turvey SE (2019). Thinking bigger: How early-life environmental exposures shape the gut microbiome and influence the development of asthma and allergic disease. Allergy 74, 2103–2115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Schneider CA, Rasband WS, and Eliceiri KW (2012). NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat Meth 9, 671–675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Shenghua P, Ziqin Z, Shuyu T, Huixia Z, Xianglu R, and Jiao G (2020). An integrated fecal microbiome and metabolome in the aged mice reveal anti-aging effects from the intestines and biochemical mechanism of FuFang zhenshu TiaoZhi(FTZ). Biomed Pharmacother 121, 109421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Solis E, Hascup KN, and Hascup ER (2020). Alzheimer’s Disease: The Link Between Amyloid-β and Neurovascular Dysfunction. J Alzheimers Dis 76, 1179–1198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Summanen PH, Jousimies-Somer H, Manley S, Bruckner D, Marina M, Goldstein EJ, and Finegold SM (1995). Bilophila wadsworthia isolates from clinical specimens. Clin Infect Dis 20 Suppl 2, S210–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Sweeney MD, Sagare AP, and Zlokovic BV (2018). Blood-brain barrier breakdown in Alzheimer disease and other neurodegenerative disorders. Nat Rev Neurol 14, 133–150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Swerdlow NR, and Geyer MA (1998). Using an animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating to study the pathophysiology and new treatments of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 24, 285–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Szklarczyk D, Gable AL, Lyon D, Junge A, Wyder S, Huerta-Cepas J, Simonovic M, Doncheva NT, Morris JH, Bork P, et al. (2019). STRING v11: protein-protein association networks with increased coverage, supporting functional discovery in genome-wide experimental datasets. Nucleic Acids Res 47, D607–d613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Ta TT, Dikmen HO, Schilling S, Chausse B, Lewen A, Hollnagel JO, and Kann O (2019). Priming of microglia with IFN-γ slows neuronal gamma oscillations in situ. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116, 4637–4642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Takahara M, Takaki A, Hiraoka S, Adachi T, Shimomura Y, Matsushita H, Nguyen TTT, Koike K, Ikeda A, Takashima S, et al. Berberine improved experimental chronic colitis by regulating interferon-γ- and IL-17A-producing lamina propria CD4+ T cells through AMPK activation. Sci Rep 9, 11934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Takeuchi H, Wang J, Kawanokuchi J, Mitsuma N, Mizuno T, and Suzumura A (2006). Interferon-gamma induces microglial-activation-induced cell death: a hypothetical mechanism of relapse and remission in multiple sclerosis. Neurobiol Dis 22, 33–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Titus AD, Shankaranarayana Rao BS, Harsha HN, Ramkumar K, Srikumar BN, Singh SB, Chattarji S, and Raju TR (2007). Hypobaric hypoxia-induced dendritic atrophy of hippocampal neurons is associated with cognitive impairment in adult rats. Neuroscience 145, 265–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Tripathi A, Melnik AV, Xue J, Poulsen O, Meehan MJ, Humphrey G, Jiang L, Ackermann G, McDonald D, Zhou D, et al. (2018). Intermittent Hypoxia and Hypercapnia, a Hallmark of Obstructive Sleep Apnea, Alters the Gut Microbiome and Metabolome. mSystems 3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Valentini F, Evangelisti M, Arpinelli M, Di Nardo G, Borro M, Simmaco M, and Villa MP (2020). Gut microbiota composition in children with obstructive sleep apnoea syndrome: a pilot study. Sleep Med 76, 140–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. van Berkel AA, DM IJ, and Verkuyl JM (2018). Cognitive benefits of the ketogenic diet in patients with epilepsy: A systematic overview. Epilepsy Behav 87, 69–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. van de Wouw M, Boehme M, Lyte JM, Wiley N, Strain C, O’Sullivan O, Clarke G, Stanton C, Dinan TG, and Cryan JF (2018). Short-chain fatty acids: microbial metabolites that alleviate stress-induced brain-gut axis alterations. The Journal of physiology 596, 4923–4944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Vinciguerra F, Graziano M, Hagnäs M, Frittitta L, and Tumminia A (2020). Influence of the Mediterranean and Ketogenic Diets on Cognitive Status and Decline: A Narrative Review. Nutrients 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Vogt NM, Kerby RL, Dill-McFarland KA, Harding SJ, Merluzzi AP, Johnson SC, Carlsson CM, Asthana S, Zetterberg H, Blennow K, et al. (2017). Gut microbiome alterations in Alzheimer’s disease. Sci Rep 7, 13537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. von Bohlen und Halbach O (2011). Immunohistological markers for proliferative events, gliogenesis, and neurogenesis within the adult hippocampus. Cell Tissue Res 345, 1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Vuong HE, and Hsiao EY (2017). Emerging Roles for the Gut Microbiome in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biological Psychiatry 81, 411–423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Vuong HE, Yano JM, Fung TC, and Hsiao EY (2017). The Microbiome and Host Behavior. Annual review of neuroscience 40, 21–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Wei L, Yao M, Zhao Z, Jiang H, and Ge S (2018). High-fat diet aggravates postoperative cognitive dysfunction in aged mice. BMC Anesthesiol 18, 20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Yu F, Han W, Zhan G, Li S, Xiang S, Zhu B, Jiang X, Yang L, Luo A, Hua F, et al. (2019). Abnormal gut microbiota composition contributes to cognitive dysfunction in streptozotocin-induced diabetic mice. Aging (Albany NY) 11, 3262–3279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Zhang J, He H, Qiao Y, Zhou T, Yi S, Zhang L, Mo L, Li Y, Jiang W, and You Z (2020). Priming of microglia with IFN-γ impairs adult hippocampal neurogenesis and leads to depression-like behaviors and cognitive defects. Glia 68, 2674–2692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Zhao Q, Stafstrom CE, Fu DD, Hu Y, and Holmes GL (2004). Detrimental effects of the ketogenic diet on cognitive function in rats. Pediatr Res 55, 498–506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Zhao Y, and Gong CX (2015). From chronic cerebral hypoperfusion to Alzheimer-like brain pathology and neurodegeneration. Cell Mol Neurobiol 35, 101–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Zhou X, Zöller T, Krieglstein K, and Spittau B (2015). TGFβ1 inhibits IFNγ-mediated microglia activation and protects mDA neurons from IFNγ-driven neurotoxicity. Journal of neurochemistry 134, 125–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Zhu X, Lee HG, Perry G, and Smith MA (2007). Alzheimer disease, the two-hit hypothesis: an update. Biochimica et biophysica acta 1772, 494–502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Ziehn MO, Avedisian AA, Dervin SM, O’Dell TJ, and Voskuhl RR (2012). Estriol preserves synaptic transmission in the hippocampus during autoimmune demyelinating disease. Lab Invest 92, 1234–1245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1
2

Data S1: Source Data for All Figures, Related to Figures 15, S1S5

3

Table S1: Dietary Information, Related to Figure 1

4

Table S2: 16S rRNA Gene Sequencing Taxonomic Information, Related to Figure 3

5

Table S3: Hippocampal CA3 Relative Gene Expression, Related to Figure 4

Data Availability Statement

16S rRNA gene sequencing data and metadata are available through QIITA repository (https://qiita.ucsd.edu/) with the study accession # 13510. Hippocampal transcriptomic data are available on through Gene Expression Omnibus repository with the identification number # GSE163099.

RESOURCES