Abstract
Cognitive reserve (CR) is known to reduce or even protect against the negative effects of aging on cognitive functioning. Nonetheless, little is known about how CR influences the relationship between different cognitive abilities and age in the old–old. The goal of the present study was, therefore, to test the hypothesis whether, in the old–old, CR still modifies the relationship between age and cognitive functioning. Eighty-three adults (aged 71–94) without mild cognitive impairment or dementia residing in residential care facilities completed a detailed neuropsychological test battery. CR was estimated using a combination of educational attainment and an estimation of verbal intelligence. Moderation analyses revealed a significant effect for fluency and a trend for flexibility, showing that the negative relationship between age and cognitive performance is reduced as the level of CR increases. These results demonstrate that CR still influences the relationship between age and executive functions in adults of advanced age.
Keywords: Cognitive reserve, Executive functions, Information processing speed, Episodic memory, Aging
Introduction
Cognitive reserve (CR) has become one of the most studied constructs in understanding the major variability in aging-related cognitive decline across individuals [1]. It has been hypothesized that CR increases the adaptability of cognitive or functional brain processes, thus acting as a buffer against the negative effects of aging-related brain pathology on cognition [1]. Although there are currently no established methods to capture CR directly, proxy measures of CR involve sociodemographic variables reflective of experiences that may promote CR (e.g., educational attainment and verbal intelligence) [1, 2]. Studies have indeed shown that higher levels of CR are associated with diminished cognitive decline in healthy aging [1, 2]. However, what remains less understood is the potential protective effect of CR at advanced ages (e.g., mean age over 80).
Previous studies that examined CR in the old–old have several limitations. First, although executive functions are considered the core functions underlying the compensatory mechanisms of CR [3], an intensive evaluation of diverse executive control processes in relation to CR in very old individuals is lacking. Most studies in the old–old examined CR in relation to global cognitive functioning or only a limited number of cognitive functions such as memory and processing speed [2, 4]. Second, studies that did examine multiple cognitive domains, including executive functions, mostly examined simple associations between CR and cognition [5, 6]. Therefore, it is unclear to what extent these findings may not merely represent main effects of, for instance, educational attainment [1]. Moreover, simple associations such as zero-order correlations cannot disentangle how CR attenuates the negative effects of aging and aging-related brain changes on cognition [1].
The present study aims to overcome these limitations and gain further insight into the effects of CR on cognition in the old–old. We examined how CR relates to cognitive test performance, primarily focusing on the different executive functions. Moreover, we examined how CR moderates the relationship between age and cognition. Due to the importance of executive functions in compensating for aging-related cognitive decline [3], we hypothesized that CR particularly influences the relationship between age and executive functions.
Methods
Participants
Eighty-three participants (Mage = 84.9, SD = 5.2, range 71–94; 29 men) were recruited in cooperation with four different homes for the elderly in Amsterdam, the Netherlands (see [7] for more details). We screened medical records of residents admitted to somatic wards for the following inclusion criteria: no neurological disease (e.g., dementia, Parkinson’s disease, stroke), no psychiatric disorder (e.g., schizophrenia, major depression), and no substance use disorder. To exclude individuals with potential cognitive impairments, the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) [8] was administered with a score of ≥ 24 as a requisite for participation (see Table 1 for descriptives).
Table 1.
Variable | N | Value |
---|---|---|
Age | 83 | 84.9 (5.2) |
Education level | 83 | |
< Primary education | 1 | 1.2% |
Primary education | 21 | 25.3% |
Incomplete lower secondary education | 13 | 15.7% |
Lower secondary education | 16 | 19.3% |
Vocational education | 19 | 22.9% |
Higher secondary/professional education | 7 | 8.4% |
University degree | 6 | 7.2% |
NART IQ | 80 | 98.7 (12.4) |
MMSE | 83 | 27.0 (1.7) |
Memory | ||
RAVLT immediate recall | 78 | 32.0 (9.4) |
RAVLT delayed recall | 78 | 5.7 (3.0) |
Pattern recognition memory | 76 | 18.9 (3.0) |
Psychomotor speed | ||
TMT-A | 81 | 98.4 (60.1) |
Stroop Color-Word test W card | 79 | 72.1 (19.3) |
Stroop Color-Word test C card | 79 | 59.6 (13.8) |
Executive function | ||
Letter fluency | 83 | 26.6 (10.9) |
Category fluency | 82 | 25.7 (9.5) |
TMT-B | 68 | 236.0 (125.6) |
Stroop Color-Word test CW card | 79 | 25.7 (12.3) |
Digit Span total | 83 | 10.8 (2.4) |
SWM no. of between-search errors | 75 | 66.1 (18.8) |
IED no. stages completed | 75 | 7.0 (2.4) |
SOC no. of problems solved | 68 | 6.2 (1.9) |
All values represent means ± standard deviations, with the exception of education, which represents percentages. Stroop scores represent the number of correct responses within 45 s for a certain condition
C Color, CW Color Word, IED Intra-Extradimensional Set Shift, MMSE Mini Mental State Examination, NART National Adult Reading Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task, SOC Stockings of Cambridge, SWM Spatial Working Memory, TMT Trail Making Test, W Word
Neuropsychological assessment
A comprehensive battery of tests tapping memory, speed and attention, and executive functioning was administered, including the following tests [see 7, 8].
Episodic memory tests
The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) was used as a measure of verbal episodic memory (immediate and delayed recall) and the Pattern Recognition Test (PRM; total correct) of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) as a measure of visual episodic memory.
Executive function tests
To fully grasp the complexity of executive functioning, we administered a wide array of tests to measure the following domains: flexibility, working memory, inhibition, fluency and planning. For flexibility, the Trail Making Test (TMT, using the TMT-B/TMT-A ratio score) and the Intra-Extradimensional Set Shift (IED, number of stages completed) task of the CANTAB were used. Working memory was assessed using the Digit Span (DS, forward and backward total correct) and CANTAB Spatial Working Memory (SWM, total between-search errors). Inhibition was measured with the 45 s version of the Stroop Colour-Word test. The interference score was used as primary outcome, which is based on the total correctly named words/colours on the Word (W), Colour (C) and Colour-Word (CW) cards, and calculated using the following formula: . Fluency was assessed using category and letter fluency (for both total number correct productions). Finally, planning was assessed with CANTAB Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) (number of problems solved in minimal moves).
Information processing speed tests
We used the TMT part A (completion time) and the W and C cards (number of word/colours correctly named) of the Stroop test to measure processing speed.
Cognitive reserve
Whereas previous studies mostly only used educational attainment to capture CR [2], we combined the level of educational attainment with verbal intelligence to obtain a more comprehensive and reliable proxy measure of CR [9]. For this, educational attainment was assessed with an ordinal rating scale based on the Dutch educational system that distinguishes between levels of education (rather than years of education): 1 = less than primary education, 2 = primary education, 3 = incomplete lower secondary education, 4 = lower secondary education, 5 = vocational education, 6 = higher secondary and professional education, 7 = university degree. The Dutch version of the National Adult Reading Test (NART) was used to estimate verbal intelligence.
Analyses
All CR and neuropsychological test scores were z standardized. As we used two outcome measures of the RAVLT for episodic memory (immediate and delayed recall), z scores of immediate and delayed recall were first averaged into one score. A similar procedure was used for the two outcomes of the Stroop Colour-Word test for processing speed (C and W cards). Next, we used the z-standardized scores to calculate cognitive domain scores that covered flexibility (TMT-B/TMT-A, IED), working memory (DS, SWM), fluency (letter, category), information processing speed (TMT-A, Stroop Colour-Word) and episodic memory (RAVLT, PRM), as well as a CR score (education, NART). For inhibition and planning, the single test score was used. In case of missing data, we used the available scores to calculate the domain. If necessary, test scores were multiplied by − 1 such that a higher score indicates better performance.
Moderation analyses (5000 Bootstraps [10]) were performed to determine if CR moderates the relationship between age and the cognitive domain scores. The domain scores were used as dependent variable, age as main predictor, CR as moderator, and sex as covariate. We report both uncorrected and false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected p values to account for multiple comparisons. To estimate the effect sizes, we reported R2 and calculated Cohen’s f2 statistics using the following formula: Cohen’s . We interpreted 0.02 as a small effect size, 0.15 as a medium effect size and 0.35 as a large effect size.
Results
Occasional missing data were present, most pronouncedly for the computerized CANTAB tests and the TMT-B, primarily due to the observed inability to complete or comprehend the (display of the computerized) test.
Results from the moderation analyses (see Table 2), after applying FDR corrections, showed main effects of CR for fluency, information processing speed, and working memory, indicating that a higher CR is associated with better cognitive performance. Furthermore, CR moderated the relationship of age with fluency performance (corrected p = 0.02, R2 change = 0.08), whereas a trend (uncorrected p = 0.06, R2 change = 0.04) was observed for flexibility. To further explore these effects, the relationship between age and these cognitive scores was plotted as a function of lower (− 1 SD), average (0 SD), and higher (+ 1 SD) CR (see Table 3 for the slope statistics). For both fluency and flexibility, a significant negative association with age was found only in individuals with a lower CR (all corrected p < 0.05), but not in individuals with an average or high CR (Fig. 1).
Table 2.
Cognitive domain | Episodic memory | Working memory | Fluency | Flexibility | Planning | Inhibition | Information processing speed |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | − 0.06 (− 0.09– − 0.02)****† | − 0.01 (− 0.04–0.02) | − 0.02 (− 0.05–0.02) | − 0.04 (− 0.07–0.00)* | − 0.05 (− 0.15–0.04) | − 0.31 (− 0.69–0.07) | − 0.04 (− 0.08– − 0.01)** |
CR | 0.09 (− 0.10–0.28) | 0.32 (0.13–0.50)****† | 0.55 (0.36–0.74)****† | 0.15 (− 0.07–0.38) | 0.14 (− 0.42–0.70) | 0.38 (-1.87–2.63) | 0.32 (0.12–0.52)***† |
Sex | 0.36 (0.01–0.71)** | 0.12 (− 0.21–0.46) | 0.15 (− 0.20–0.50) | 0.41 (0.00–0.83)* | 0.05 (− 0.98–1.08) | 1.55 (− 2.62–5.71) | 0.10 (− 0.27–0.47) |
Age*CR | 0.02 (− 0.02–0.05) | 0.02 (-0.01–0.06) | 0.05 (0.02–0.09)***† | 0.04 (0.00–0.08)* | 0.02 (− 0.08–0.12) | 0.16 (− 0.25–0.56) | 0.01 (− 0.03–0.05) |
Finteraction | F(1,76) = 1.06 | F(1,78) = 1.93 | F(1,78) = 8.86 | F(1,75) = 3.55 | F(1,63) = 0.14 | F(1,73) = 0.58 | F(1,76) = 0.24 |
∆R2interaction | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.00 |
R2total | 0.22 | 0.15 | 0.32 | 0.16 | 0.02 | 0.06 | 0.19 |
Cohen’s f2total | 0.28 | 0.17 | 0.47 | 0.19 | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.23 |
B values are reported, together with lower and upper borders of the 95% confidence interval. Higher cognitive domain scores indicate better performance. Cohen’s f2 calculations are based on the unrounded R2 values
CR cognitive reserve (compound score of educational attainment and verbal intelligence estimate)
*Uncorrected p < 0.07
**Uncorrected p < 0.05
***Uncorrected p < 0.01
****Uncorrected p < 0.001
†Survived FDR corrections
Table 3.
Cognitive domain | Fluency | Flexibility |
---|---|---|
Slopes | ||
Low CR | − 0.07 (− 0.11– − 0.02)*† | − 0.07 (− 0.13–-0.02)*† |
Average CR | − 0.01 (− 0.05–0.02) | − 0.03 (− 0.07–0.00) |
High CR | 0.03 (− 0.01–0.07) | 0.00 (− 0.05–0.05) |
B values are reported, together with lower and upper borders of the 95% confidence interval. Slopes represent the relation between age and the cognitive domain score at a lower (− 1 SD), average (0 SD) and higher (+ 1 SD) level of CR
CR cognitive reserve (compound score of educational attainment and verbal intelligence estimate)
*p < 0.01
†Survived FDR corrections
The effect sizes (see Table 2) for the entire model ranged from small (planning, inhibition) to medium (episodic memory, working memory, flexibility, information processing speed) and large (fluency) effect sizes. A sensitivity analysis showed that, given a sample size of 83 with α = 0.05, the study was sufficiently powered (1 − β = 0.80) to detect a medium effect size (f2 = 0.16).
Discussion
The goal of the present study was to examine whether CR influences the relationship between age and cognitive performance, with a particular focus on executive functioning, in the old–old. The results show that CR moderates the relationship of age with performance on fluency and, potentially, flexibility tasks. Corresponding to the notion that CR reflects one’s ability to alleviate the cognitive effects of aging-related brain pathology [1], these findings revealed that the negative correlation between age and cognitive performance levels is less pronounced as the level of CR increases. This study, therefore, clearly illustrates that even in a very old population, CR attenuates cognitive test performance. As cognitive, and particularly executive, functions play a profound role in functional independence in geriatric populations [11], these findings are of clinical relevance.
Our findings corroborate and extend previous studies showing that CR relates to cognitive functions in very old populations [2, 5, 6]. Our study provides new insights as we tested this for multiple executive functions, and analysed how CR modulates the relationship between age and cognition to focus on the hypothesized CR effects rather than the main effects of education [1]. Very little is known about how CR relates to the negative relationship between age and cognition in the old–old. From a theoretical point of view, one could hypothesize that CR may protect across the full life span, that is, as long as the aging-associated cognitive decline continues. Our study results corroborate this, by showing that even in very old individuals, CR still attenuates the negative age–cognition relationship for two executive function domains, although the moderation effect of CR on flexibility requires replication in larger studies.
The current study findings contrast those from a recent longitudinal study showing that education (as a proxy for CR) did not influence cognitive decline in these old populations [12]. As the current study relied on cross-sectional data, our findings may be confounded by cohort effects [13], although it has been reported that cross-sectional data may actually provide a very accurate indication of the true cognitive aging that occurs across the adult lifespan [13]. Moreover, previous work confirmed the interaction between CR and age, suggesting that the effects of CR actually increase with advancing age [14]. Likewise, the current study indeed revealed that with increasing age, the effect of CR becomes more pronounced (see Fig. 1).
The precise mechanisms via which CR exerts its protective role is still a topic of debate. According to one of the most prevailing theories, part of the protective role of CR may be exerted via increased capacity, efficiency or flexibility of existing neural networks [1]. From this perspective, CR may have a general effect on diverse cognitive functions that is supported by the already involved networks of brain regions. On the other hand, CR may work via compensatory mechanisms that are supported by alternative neural networks [1], mostly via increased involvement of prefrontal cortex processes and associated executive control processes [3]. However, a very recent study in unilateral frontal and non-frontal stroke patients failed to support this notion, showing that CR similarly influences cognitive performance in both patient groups [15]. The extent to which existing versus compensatory mechanisms are involved in the current study findings as well as in maintaining cognition in aging in general, requires further investigation.
To conclude, the current study suggests that CR attenuates the effects of age on executive functioning in old to very old adults. Considering the current worldwide double aging of the population, future studies are needed that examine the complex dynamics between age, neurodegenerative processes, and CR, as well as the functional and cognitive mechanisms in these old–old individuals that allow them to compensate for the effects of aging-related pathologies.
Author contributions
JMO and EJAS contributed to the study conception and design. JMO performed data collection and analysis. The first draft of the manuscript was written by JMO and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
There are no conflicts of interest to declare.
Ethics approval
The study was approved by the local ethics committee of the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, and the study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent
All participant gave informed consent.
Footnotes
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Stern Y, Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Bartrés-Faz D et al. the Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors PIA Empirical Definitions and Conceptual Frameworks Workgroup Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16:1305–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Seblova D, Berggren R, Lövdén M. Education and age-related decline in cognitive performance: systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2020;58:101005. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2019.101005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Tucker MA, Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in aging. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2011;8:354–360. doi: 10.2174/156720511795745320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lavrencic LM, Richardson C, Harrison SL, et al. Is there a link between cognitive reserve and cognitive function in the oldest-old? J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2018;73:499–505. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kaplan RF, Cohen RA, Moscufo N, et al. Demographic and biological influences on cognitive reserve. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2009;31:868–876. doi: 10.1080/13803390802635174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lindenberger U, Baltes PB. Intellectual functioning in old and very old age: cross-sectional results from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychol Aging. 1997;12:410–432. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.3.410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Oosterman JM, de Vries K, Scherder EJA. Executive ability in relation to blood pressure in residents of homes for the elderly. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 2007;22:731–738. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, et al. Neuropsychological assessment. 5. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Caffò AO, Lopez A, Spano G, et al. The role of pre-morbid intelligence and cognitive reserve in predicting cognitive efficiency in a sample of Italian elderly. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2016;28:1203–1210. doi: 10.1007/s40520-016-0580-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. New York: Guilford publications; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Overdorp EJ, Kessels RPC, Claassen JA, et al. The combined effect of neuropsychological and neuropathological deficits on instrumental activities of daily living in older adults: a systematic review. Neuropsych Rev. 2016;26:92–106. doi: 10.1007/s11065-015-9312-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wilson RS, Yu L, Lamar M, et al. Education and cognitive reserve in old age. Neurology. 2019;92:e1041–e1050. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000007036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Salthouse TA. Trajectories of normal cognitive aging. Psychol Aging. 2019;34:17–24. doi: 10.1037/pag0000288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Opdebeeck C, Martyr A, Clare L. Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2016;23:40–60. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2015.1041450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.MacPherson SE, Allerhand M, Gharooni S, et al. Cognitive reserve proxies do not differentially account for cognitive performance in patients with focal frontal and non-frontal lesions. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2020;26:739–748. doi: 10.1017/S1355617720000326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.