Abstract
The climate crisis poses a serious threat to the health and well-being of individuals. For many, climate change knowledge is derived from indirect exposure to information transmitted through the media. Such content can elicit a variety of emotional responses, including anger, sadness, despair, fear, and guilt. Worry and anxiety are especially common responses, usually referred to as “climate anxiety”. The main objectives of this study were to analyze how exposure to climate change through the media relates to climate anxiety and individual and collective self-efficacy, and to evaluate the relationship between climate anxiety and efficacy beliefs. A total of 312 Italian university students (aged 18–26 years) participated in the research by filling out an anonymous questionnaire. Participants reported being exposed several times per week to information about climate change, especially from social media, newspapers, and television programs. Moreover, the results showed that the attention paid to information about climate change was not only positively related to climate anxiety, but also to individual and collective self-efficacy. Most notably, participants’ efficacy beliefs were found to be positively related to climate anxiety. This somewhat controversial finding stresses that, in the context of pro-environmental behavior changes, a moderate level of anxiety could engender feelings of virtue, encouraging people to rethink actions with negative ecological impacts.
Keywords: climate anxiety, media exposure, self-efficacy
1. Introduction
The climate crisis poses a serious threat to the health and well-being of individuals, to which societies must urgently respond and adapt [1]. Multiple studies have investigated the effects of climate change on health [2,3,4,5]. These effects include an increase in mortality rates and injuries resulting from natural disasters and other extreme weather events, and an increased risk of malnutrition, physical illness, and psychological distress [6,7,8]. The relationship between acute meteorological disasters, such as floods, heat waves, and cyclones, and the subsequent anxiety experienced, is particularly well-documented [9,10,11,12], evidencing the threat of climate change to the mental health of individuals. Ogunbode et al. [13] showed that mental health and psycho-social well-being can be influenced both directly, for example, through life experiences related to extreme weather events or natural disasters, and indirectly, through exposure to climate change news. Both have been found to produce a general increase in mental illness and emotional difficulties. Similarly, Fritze et al. [1] argued that exposure to extreme weather events, including prolonged indirect exposure to, and awareness of, the effects, have substantial repercussions in terms of both morbidity and psychological disorders. These repercussions can be either moderated or exacerbated according to a variety of psycho-social factors [14], including individual and community-wide vulnerabilities [15,16,17]. In their review of the relevant literature, Doherty and Clayton [18] identified, among the effects of indirect climate change exposure, intense emotional arousal, including anxiety around both current and future risks to humans and other species.
For many, climate change knowledge is derived from indirect exposure to information transmitted through the media and social media networks. Such content can elicit a variety of emotional responses, predominantly negative, including anger, sadness, despair, fear, and guilt. Worry and anxiety are especially common responses. This phenomenon is commonly referred to as “eco-anxiety” or “climate anxiety,” and is associated with symptoms such as panic attacks, loss of appetite, irritability, weakness, and sleep disturbance [8]. From this perspective, climate change may be seen as a threat to the mental health of populations on a potentially global scale.
1.1. Media Exposure and Climate Anxiety
The terms “climate anxiety” and “eco-anxiety” are used to describe the psychological discomfort that results from knowledge and/or experience of environmental degradation and the associated risks [19,20]. Maibach et al. [21] found that 18% of their sample of 2129 U.S. citizens felt alarmed by climate change and were experiencing feelings such as sadness, anger, and fear. According to an AXA-IPSOS survey that involved 13,000 people from three continents [22], one-third of the respondents said that climate change could have an impact on their personal well-being. Moreover, an Italian survey conducted in 2019 involving a representative national sample of 800 young adults indicated that, for 51% of them, climate change represents the primary source of their distress [23]. In recent years, discourse on climate change has described it as “catastrophic,” “rapid,” “urgent,” “irreversible”, and “chaotic” [24]. Such language is characteristic of a new way of narrating the climate crisis that Risbey [17] terms “alarming” [25,26]. However, according to Vu, Liu, and Tran [27], media coverage of climate change (particularly given the media outlets’ political and/or ideological agendas) is a controversial topic, and many media outlets are criticized for inadequately covering the climate crisis or for not doing enough to highlight its severity (see University of Kansas [28]). Politicians and other influential public figures have contributed to this new narrative (either by over- or underestimating the crisis), as have media and social media networks [29,30,31,32,33]. Investigations by Stamm et al.. [34], Carvalho and Burgess [29], and Olausson [35] have demonstrated the agenda-setting role of the media. Consisting of the creators and distributors of images and narratives circulated among the public, the media is largely responsible for the transmission of scientific knowledge and its incorporation into daily experiences [35]. Through the application of standard principles and procedures, the media determines which events will become “news”, presenting interpretations of an event’s causes and consequences in accordance with their own ideological and political purposes and preferences [29]. A study by Brulle, Carmichael, and Jenkins [36] identified various factors responsible for public anxiety in the United States between 2002 and 2010. Using data from 74 different surveys conducted over nine years, the authors highlighted media coverage as among the primary factors influencing the extent to which participants experienced anxiety around climate change. Climate change information in the media seems to have a significant impact on levels of public anxiety. A positive correlation was found between the two variables [37].
1.2. Media Exposure, Self-Efficacy, and Collective Efficacy
Albrecht [38] identified the perceived inability of individuals to respond to environmental challenges as a form of anxiety termed “ecoparalysis”. Moser and Dilling [39] and Lorenzoni et al. [40], identified various barriers to modifying attitudes and behaviors at two interrelated levels: individual and social. The climate change issue acts as a global narrative that connects and contextualizes multiple events on a global scale, influencing judgments about responsibilities and the effectiveness of interventions. The data analyzed by Lorenzoni et al. [40] were taken from three studies of the U.K. population. These studies, although not originally designed for this purpose, identified the key obstacles and barriers to pro-environmental action: lack of knowledge, uncertainty and skepticism, the outsourcing of responsibilities, apathy [41], and the mistrust of information sources. The outsourcing of responsibilities and skepticism concerning the reality of climate change and the effectiveness of mitigation efforts may be coping mechanisms that moderate negative emotional responses, including anxiety, through denial.
Moreover, the media coverage of climate change’s impacts and responses tends to be saturated with threat information undermining perceived self-efficacy [42]. A study by Kellstedt et al. [43] found that individuals who reported high exposure to information about global warming also reported feeling less able to effectively address the issue. Hibberd and Nguyen [44] reported that young people (aged 18–26 years) have a strong sense of behavioral inefficacy about climate change that is impacted by messages in the media and social media networks. Therefore, there is a growing body of research evidencing that exposure to information about climate change through sources such as these can reduce efficacy beliefs and increase climate anxiety [45].
1.3. Climate Anxiety, Self-Efficacy, and Collective Efficacy
The relationship between climate change, anxiety, and self-efficacy is a subject of ongoing debate. Nabi et al. [46] claimed that self-efficacy could be affected by the negative emotions evoked through exposure to climate change information. According to Lowe et al. [47], “research has shown that people feel overwhelmed by shocking images and, although it heightens their concern, it also reduces their self-efficacy to take action and lessen these events through personal action” (p. 451). However, other studies show that individuals experiencing a certain degree of climate anxiety tend to experience higher levels of self-efficacy [48,49,50]. This response may represent a form of mitigation adopted by individuals who feel more capable of intervening to reduce the impacts of environmental degradation or, at least, of minimizing the damage to the environment [51]. Collective self-efficacy is the belief that a group, or groups, can successfully perform a specific task [52,53,54]. In the context of climate change, collective efficacy focuses on the effectiveness of group mitigative action, the impact on future generations, and a sense of collective responsibility for the health of the planet [55]. A sense of collective self-efficacy may therefore decrease climate anxiety by restoring the confidence of individuals in the belief that, working together, they can effectively address the climate crisis [56,57,58,59].
The primary objective of this study was to evaluate the effects of media exposure to climate change information in a sample of university students in terms of attention paid and the source type. The second aim of this study was to analyze how exposure to climate change through the media relates to climate anxiety and perceived individual and collective self-efficacy. On the basis of the literature referenced above, we hypothesized a positive correlation between attention paid to climate change information and climate anxiety (the greater the attention paid, the more anxiety one experiences), and a negative correlation between climate change information exposure and self-efficacy beliefs (the greater the exposure, the less individual and collective self-efficacy one experiences). The third and final aim of this study was to analyze the relationship between climate anxiety and efficacy beliefs. Given the aforementioned inconsistencies between the results of previous studies, we proceeded without a specific hypothesis, instead adopting an exploratory perspective.
We chose to limit the scope of the investigation to young adults aged 18–28 years. This choice was made in accordance with Wu et al.’s [60] findings indicating that young people are more prone than adults to the psychological, emotional, and physical effects of climate anxiety given their development stage and the consequent vulnerability to anxiety.
This study will help clarify the somewhat controversial relationship between climate change information exposure, efficacy beliefs and climate anxiety.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
A total of 312 students enrolled in undergraduate and postgraduate humanities courses at the University of Turin participated in the research (25% men and 74% women; 1% of the sample opted not to specify their gender). Their ages ranged from 18 to 28 years (mean = 21.58; SD = 1.96). Participants took part in the research on a voluntary basis, and no compensation (or extra credit) for their participation was provided.
2.2. Measures
The data were collected with an anonymous questionnaire. The items and data are from the MECAMH project (English version of the questionnaire available at https://osf.io/6n4rb/?view_only=d708efc61e6945a1bc02e037a3ccac96 (accessed on 30 October 2019); [61]).
Media exposure to climate change information was investigated in relation to the following variables:
-
(a).
Attention paid to information about climate change, measured through one item; possible responses were: none (coded as 0), a little (coded as 1), some (coded as 2), and a lot (coded as 3).
-
(b).
Information source: the frequency of reading or listening to climate change information from different sources measured through 9 items, one for each source (such as TV, YouTube, books, friends etc.). Possible responses ranged from never (coded as 0) to more than 10 times per day (coded as 8).
Climate anxiety was investigated through a scale modelled on the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI Y1) [62,63]. It included 7 items assessing the participants’ present feelings around climate change. The feelings included in the scale were: calm (reverse-coded), tense, relaxed (reverse-coded), anxious, peaceful (reverse-coded), worried, and terrified. Possible responses ranged from not at all (coded as 1), to somewhat (coded as 2), moderately (coded as 3), very much (coded as 4), and extremely (coded as 5). The range of the scale was from 7–35. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was 0.89.
Climate self-efficacy (individual and collective) was investigated through the Perceived Climate Self-Efficacy Scale [64,65,66]. It includes 5 items pertaining to individual self-efficacy, and 5 items pertaining to collective self-efficacy. Participants were asked to express the extent to which they agreed with a variety of statements. For example, “I trust that I can do my part to solve the climate crisis” or “I trust that we, as young people, can contribute to solving the climate crisis”. Possible responses ranged from strongly disagree (coded as 1), to disagree (coded as 2), neither disagree nor agree (coded as 3), agree (coded as 4), and strongly agree (coded as 5). The range of each scale was from 5–25. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha for individual climate self-efficacy was 0.73; the Cronbach’s alpha for collective climate self-efficacy was 0.79.
Statistical analyses were performed with IBM SPSS (IBM Corp. Armonk, NY, USA), release 25. Descriptive measures (means ± SD) were calculated for all test variables. Correlations were calculated to examine the relations between attention paid to information about climate change, climate anxiety, and individual and collective climate self-efficacy.
2.3. Procedure
The local ethical committee of the University of Turin approved this research project (protocol code 437036). The data were collected during the 2019/2020 academic year by research assistants specifically trained by the researchers. In accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki [67], both an information letter and an informed consent form were distributed among the participants alongside the questionnaire. The participants completed the questionnaire in their classroom at the university before the beginning of a lesson and returned it immediately (response rate 98%). Completing the questionnaire required approximately 20 min.
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics
Most participants stated they paid either some or a lot of attention to information about climate change (89%), while only 11% stated they paid little or no attention.
For each of the nine items measuring the frequency with which the participants read or listened to climate information from various sources, a mean of the scores was calculated (range 0: never to 8: more than 10 times per day). For results, see Table 1 (in decreasing order).
Table 1.
M | SD | |
---|---|---|
4.62 | 2412 | |
Printed and online newspapers | 4.03 | 1984 |
TV news and other TV programs | 3.94 | 1927 |
Family, friends and colleagues | 3.87 | 1791 |
Scientific articles/journals/blogs | 3.02 | 2362 |
Radio news and radio programs | 2.33 | 1948 |
YouTube | 2.31 | 2364 |
Books and magazines | 2.16 | 2102 |
1.59 | 2615 |
Note: M = mean; SD = standard deviation.
As shown in Table 1, the top three sources of information were a social media network (Facebook), newspapers, and television programs. In Table 2, the results indicate that, on average, collective self-efficacy had a higher score than individual self-efficacy.
Table 2.
M | SD | Range | |
---|---|---|---|
Climate anxiety | 23.85 | 5.13 | 7–35 |
Individual self-efficacy | 17.42 | 2.92 | 5–25 |
Collective self-efficacy | 19.23 | 2.93 | 5–25 |
Note: M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Range = lowest and highest values of the scale.
Table 2 details the descriptive statistics on climate anxiety and individual and collective self-efficacy.
3.2. Correlation Analysis
Table 3 shows the correlation analysis between the variables. With regard to media exposure, attention paid to information about climate change was considered. As evidenced, attention paid to information about climate change was positively related to climate anxiety, and both individual and collective self-efficacy. Moreover, climate anxiety was positively related to individual and collective self-efficacy.
Table 3.
AI | CA | ISE | CSE | |
---|---|---|---|---|
AI | - | |||
CA | 0.364 ** | - | ||
ISE | 0.291 ** | 0.304 ** | - | |
CSE | 0.278 ** | 0.211 ** | 0.675 ** | - |
** Correlation is significant at 0.01. * Correlation is significant at 0.05. Note: AI = attention paid to information about climate change; CA = climate anxiety; ISE = individual self-efficacy; CSE = collective self-efficacy.
4. Discussion
The present study sought to assess the effects of exposure to climate change information in the media in a sample of 18–26-year-old Italian university students. In particular, the relationship between climate change media information exposure, climate anxiety, and individual and collective self-efficacy was studied. The first interesting result of the investigation concerned media and social media network exposure. Participants reported being exposed several times per week to information about climate change. The results underlined that attention paid to information is positively related to climate anxiety. These findings confirm those of earlier investigations by Risbey [24], Olausson [35], Brulle et al. [36], and Marlon et al. [37], highlighting that media exposure is one of the main factors influencing climate anxiety and vice versa.
Concerning the positive relationship between media exposure and individual and collective self-efficacy, the results show that when the attention paid to climate change increases, there is an increase in efficacy beliefs. These results may have been impacted by the information content. For example, information about how one might intervene as an individual, or mitigate best practices adopted by governments, may provide examples of pro-environmental behavior increasing efficacy beliefs and decreasing anxiety [68]. Moreover, as suggested by Moser [69,70,71], in communication about climate change, it is important to underline the reasons for those changes to improve people’s knowledge about the phenomenon and how to cope with it. Future research could consider how the content of the information to which participants are exposed influences its effects on climate anxiety, individual self-efficacy, and collective self-efficacy.
The findings regarding individual and collective self-efficacy are somewhat controversial. Correlational analysis revealed a positive relationship between individual and collective self-efficacy and climate anxiety, suggesting that an increase in self-efficacy (individual and collective) corresponds to an increase in anxiety, and vice versa. Thus, the levels of climate anxiety in our sample were not affected by eco-paralysis, as defined by Albrecht [38]. Moreover, our investigation demonstrated that participants experienced increased self-efficacy in response to opportunities to mitigate climate change, both as individuals and as a part of a group, even if they were experiencing climate anxiety. Interestingly, this result confirms those of Homburg and Solberg [48], Verplanken and Roy [72], Howell et al. [49], and Helm et al. [50]. In accordance with Pihkala [20], the results show that anxiety can play a positive role as “an emotion which engenders information-seeking and problem-solving” (p. 12). Kurth [73] termed this “practical anxiety” and, in the context of pro-environmental behavior changes, it provokes ethical and practical deliberation about what kind of behavior is needed to encourage people to rethink actions with negative ecological impacts. This “practical” climate anxiety can, for example, motivate individuals to make more sustainable lifestyle choices and increase their pro-environmental activity (for example, involving the neighborhood in shared gardens). Further research is needed to clarify the relationship between climate anxiety, individual and collective self-efficacy, and the willingness to undertake pro-environmental behaviors. Closer study of the behaviors enacted may prove useful as a means of measuring and better understanding how climate anxiety influences people’s willingness to engage in mitigative climate action (see, for example, Lorenzoni et al. [40], and Hart and Feldman [42]). According to Bradley et al. [74], engaging in pro-environmental behaviors may be an attempt to cope with and alleviate climate anxiety and may, with further study, prove useful for moderating the associated distress. This investigation’s findings contribute to a better understanding of how mitigative climate actions may function as effective coping strategies.
Nevertheless, this study is not without limitations. First, because of the difficulty, novelty, and interdisciplinarity of the subject matter, not all relevant sources could be engaged within the article. Moreover, the research is cross-sectional and, therefore, the direction of associations and causality between constructs could not be established. Additionally, all participants were university students with a high level of education, potentially impacting their ability to evaluate the information sources and moderate their responses accordingly. Furthermore, the sample was not sex-balanced; it was composed primarily of women. In the meta-analysis by Su et al. [75], it emerged that women are more prone to experience higher levels of social media addiction than men. Therefore, the extent to which these findings apply to both men and women cannot be conclusively determined. The investigation did not take into consideration socio-demographic variables, such as family situation, income stability, or parental support. Specifically, the age and social group sampled may (more likely than others) display increased anxiety levels for different reasons, be it financial insecurity, class performance, the novelty of the university environment, and countless others. Anxieties may be transferred to a readily available explanation, climatic in this case, without deeper substantiation. Future investigation and statistical control for such confounding factors would be welcomed. Moreover, these variables may provide further insight into the participants’ perceived self-efficacy and/or collective self-efficacy in relation to climate change mitigation. For example, Darawshy et al. [76,77], in their investigation into the psychological consequences of community violence, found that collective self-efficacy moderated the relationship between witnessing community violence and the symptoms of post-traumatic stress. Further research could include variations in the participants’ levels of education and other socio-demographic characteristics. Social-desirability bias [78], that is, the participants’ tendency to provide responses they believe present a favorable image of themselves, may also have impacted the study’s findings. The divisive nature of this hotly debated topic, evident in the media, but also within family and friendship groups, arguably increases the likelihood of this bias, as does the topic’s overtly ethical dimensions. As such, it is difficult to determine the extent to which participants’ responses reflect their desire to appear concerned because they believe this is expected of them. With this in mind, future research would benefit from tools intended to assess and account for social-desirability bias in self-reporting [79]. Finally, the nature and extent of the participants’ climate anxiety were not evaluated and warrants further investigation. Verplanken and Roy [72] find worry about the environment to be a constructive form of concern. They distinguish between “habitual ecological worrying and pathological worry”, evidencing a near-zero correlation between the two. In their study of participants from Europe and the United States (n = 132), they identified a significant correlation between ecological worrying, environmentally conscious attitudes and behaviors, and the perceived opportunity for pro-environmental intervention. Similarly, the results of this study support show that some people who self-report climate anxiety also report efficacy beliefs. Nevertheless, according to Verplanken et al. [80], further investigation might clarify whether ecological worry is a precursor to climate anxiety on the spectrum of concern, and if there is a point at which constructive habitual ecological worry becomes destructive, pathological worry.
5. Conclusions
In conclusion, this investigation demonstrates that, in line with previous research, the exposure of young adults to climate change information in the media and through social media networks is related to negative emotions, including anxiety, and to increased individual and collective self-efficacy beliefs about climate change. Most notably, the participants’ efficacy beliefs were found to be positively related to climate anxiety. It should be noted that the anxiety level of our sample was quite low. For this reason, it would be interesting to investigate this same relationship in a sample of subjects experiencing more severe climate anxiety (perhaps related to the direct experience of an adverse climate event) and evaluate the effects on self-efficacy in relation to this variable, and whether self-efficacy could play a different role contributing to the mitigation of anxiety. Since the literature provides different measures for climate anxiety, it could be interesting to make a comparison between them to better understand how to investigate the construct of climate anxiety (for example [81]).
Climate anxiety could be mitigated by constructive and accurate climate change narratives that underline how to cope with it [10]. The risk of overexposure and the existence of excessive climate disaster content could result in eco-paralysis, as described by Albrecht [38]. There is the possibility of inaction relating to inaccurate information about risk exposure. Media coverage of best practices in pro-environmental behavior (both at the individual and societal level) may mitigate this unfavorable result, alongside published data about differentiated waste collection, new inventions, and policy and structural reforms that could help to improve the efficiency of electrical appliances, cars, and other objects of daily use [69]. The dissemination of news may encourage the uptake of pro-environmental behaviors and begin a virtuous cycle in line with Verplanken and Roy’s [72] account of the “usual worry” about climate change coinciding with positive ecological attitudes and behaviors and modeling a positive example for others to follow. This is particularly important for young people whose lives are already being impacted by climate anxiety, and who, without urgent mitigative action, will see the effects of climate change worsen considerably during their lifetimes.
Acknowledgments
Authors wish to thank Riccardo Grollero, Marzia Martella, and Sara Traldi for their support in administering questionnaires.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, D.A.M.; methodology, T.B.; formal analysis, T.B.; investigation, D.A.M. and T.B.; resources, D.A.M.; data curation, D.A.M. and T.B.; writing—original draft preparation, D.A.M. and T.B.; writing—review and editing, D.A.M. and T.B.; project administration, D.A.M. Both authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research received no external funding.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of Università di Torino (protocol code 437036, 25 September 2019).
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Fritze J.G., Blashki G.A., Burke S., Wiseman J. Hope, despair and transformation: Climate change and the promotion of mental health and wellbeing. Int. J. Ment. Health Syst. 2008;2:13. doi: 10.1186/1752-4458-2-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Berry H.L., Bowen K., Kjellstrom T. Climate change and mental health: A causal pathways framework. Int. J. Public Health. 2010;55:123–132. doi: 10.1007/s00038-009-0112-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Barnett J., Adger W.N. Climate dangers and atoll countries. Clim. Chang. 2003;61:321–337. doi: 10.1023/B:CLIM.0000004559.08755.88. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gleditsch N.P. Whither the weather? Climate change and conflict. J. Peace Res. 2012;49:3–9. doi: 10.1177/0022343311431288. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Weissbecker I., editor. Climate Change and Human Well-Being: Global Challenges and Opportunities. Springer Science & Business Media; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Clayton S. Climate anxiety: Psychological responses to climate change. J. Anxiety Disord. 2020;74:102263. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Taylor S. Anxiety disorders, climate change, and the challenges ahead: Introduction to the special issue. J. Anxiety Disord. 2020;76:102313. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Clayton S., Manning C. Psychology and Climate Change: Human Perceptions, Impacts, and Responses. Academic Press; Cambridge, UK: 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Clayton S. Climate Change and Mental Health. Curr. Environ. Health Rep. 2021;8:1–6. doi: 10.1007/s40572-020-00303-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ojala M., Cunsolo A., Ogunbode C.A., Middleton J. Anxiety, Worry, and Grief in a Time of Environmental and Climate Crisis: A Narrative Review. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 2021;46 doi: 10.1146/annurev-environ-012220-022716. Review in Advance first posted online on 11 June 2021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Galea S., Nandi A., Vlahov D. The epidemiology of post-traumatic stress disorder after disasters. Epidemiol. Rev. 2005;27:78–91. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxi003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Salcioglu E., Basoglu M., Livanou M. Post-traumatic stress disorder and comorbid depression among survivors of the 1999 earthquake in Turkey. Disasters. 2007;31:115–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7717.2007.01000.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ogunbode C.A., Pallesen S., Böhm G., Doran R., Bhullar N., Aquino S., Lomas M.J. Negative emotions about climate change are related to insomnia symptoms and mental health: Cross-sectional evidence from 25 countries. Curr. Psychol. 2021;16:1–10. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Willis H.H., DeKay M.L. The roles of group membership, beliefs, and norms in ecological risk perception. Risk Anal. 2007;27:1365–1380. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2007.00958.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Satterfield T.A., Mertz C.K., Slovic P. Discrimination, vulnerability, and justice in the face of risk. Risk Anal. Int. J. 2004;24:115–129. doi: 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00416.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Cunsolo A., Ellis N.R. Ecological grief as a mental health response to climate change-related loss. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2018;8:275. doi: 10.1038/s41558-018-0092-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bourque F., Cunsolo Willox A. Climate change: The next challenge for public mental health? Int. Rev. Psychiatry. 2014;26:415–422. doi: 10.3109/09540261.2014.925851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Doherty T.J., Clayton S. The psychological impacts of global climate change. Am. Psychol. 2011;66:265. doi: 10.1037/a0023141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Pikhala P. Climate Anxiety. MIELI Mental Health Finland; Helsinki, Finland: 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Pikhala P. Anxiety and the ecological crisis: An analysis of eco-anxiety and climate anxiety. Sustainability. 2020;12:7836. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Maibach E., Roser-Renouf C., Leiserowitz A. Global Warming’s Six Americas 2009: An Audience Segmentation Analysis. Yale University and George Mason University; New Haven, CT, USA: 2009. Yale Project on Climate Change. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Individual Perception of Climate Risks–Survey Axa/Ipsos. [(accessed on 23 June 2021)];2012 Available online: http://www.axa.com/lib/axa/uploads/cahiersaxa/Survey-AXAIpsos_climate-risks.pdf.
- 23.SWG Lotta Contro i Cambiamenti Climatici. [(accessed on 24 August 2021)];2019 Available online: https://www.swg.it/politicapp?id=yedv.
- 24.Risbey J.S. The new climate discourse: Alarmist or alarming? Glob. Environ. Chang. 2008;18:26–37. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2007.06.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ruiu M.L. Representation of climate change consequences in British newspapers. Eur. J. Commun. 2020:0267323120978727. doi: 10.1177/0267323120978727. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Molek-Kozakowska K. Popularity-driven science journalism and climate change: A critical discourse analysis of the unsaid. Discourse Context Media. 2018;21:73–81. doi: 10.1016/j.dcm.2017.09.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Vu H.T., Liu Y., Tran D.V. Nationalizing a global phenomenon: A study of how the press in 45 countries and territories portrays climate change. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2019;58:101942. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2019.101942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.University of Kansas How Media around the World Frame Climate Change News: Rich Countries Politicize Issue, While Poor Countries Present Climate Change as International Concern. ScienceDaily. Aug 13, 2019. [(accessed on 22 June 2021)]. Available online: www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/08/190813130431.htm.
- 29.Carvalho A., Burgess J. Cultural circuits of climate change in UK broadsheet newspapers, 1985–2003. Risk Anal. Int. J. 2005;25:1457–1469. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2005.00692.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.O’Neill S., Boykoff M. The role of new media in engaging individuals with climate change. In: Whitmarsh L., O’Neill S.J., Lorenzoni I., editors. Engaging the Public with Climate Change: Communication and Behaviour Change. Earthscan; London, UK: 2011. pp. 233–251. [Google Scholar]
- 31.O’Neill S.J., Boykoff M., Niemeyer S.M., Day S.A. On the use of imagery for climate change engagement. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2013;23:413–421. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2012.11.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.O’Neill S., Williams H.T., Kurz T., Wiersma B., Boykoff M. Dominant frames in legacy and social media coverage of the IPCC Fifth Assessment Report. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2015;5:380. doi: 10.1038/nclimate2535. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Schäfer M.S., Schlichting I. Media representations of climate change: A meta-analysis of the research field. Environ. Commun. 2014;8:142–160. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2014.914050. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Stamm K.R., Clark F., Eblacas P.R. Mass communication and public understanding of environmental problems: The case of global warming. Public Underst. Sci. 2000;9:219–238. doi: 10.1088/0963-6625/9/3/302. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Olausson U. “We’re the ones to blame”: Citizens’ representations of climate change and the role of the media. Environ. Commun. J. Nat. Cult. 2011;5:281–299. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2011.585026. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Brulle R.J., Carmichael J., Jenkins J.C. Shifting public opinion on climate change: An empirical assessment of factors influencing concern over climate change in the US, 2002–2010. Clim. Chang. 2012;114:169–188. doi: 10.1007/s10584-012-0403-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Marlon J.R., van der Linden S., Howe P.D., Leiserowitz A., Woo S.L., Broad K. Detecting local environmental change: The role of experience in shaping risk judgments about global warming. J. Risk Res. 2019;22:936–950. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2018.1430051. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Albrecht G. Climate Change and Human Well-Being. Springer; New York, NY, USA: 2011. Chronic environmental change: Emerging ‘psychoterratic’syndromes; pp. 43–56. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Moser S.C., Dilling L., editors. Creating a Climate for Change. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lorenzoni I., Nicholson-Cole S., Whitmarsh L. Barriers perceived to engaging with climate change among the UK public and their policy implications. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2007;17:445–459. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2007.01.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hall C. Beyond gloom and doom or hope and possibility. In: Deserai A.C., Maxwell T.B., editors. Culture, Politics and Climate Change: How Information Shapes Our Common Future. Routledge; London, UK: 2014. pp. 23–38. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hart P.S., Feldman L. Threat without efficacy? Climate change on US network news. Sci. Commun. 2014;36:325–351. doi: 10.1177/1075547013520239. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kellstedt P.M., Zahran S., Vedlitz A. Personal efficacy, the information environment, and attitudes toward global warming and climate change in the United States. Risk Anal. Int. J. 2008;28:113–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01010.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hibberd M., Nguyen A. Climate change communications & young people in the Kingdom: A reception study. Int. J. Media Cult. Politics. 2013;9:27–46. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Helferich M., Hanss D., Doran R., Köhler J., Salmela-Aro K., Ogunbode C. Associations between Climate Change-Related Efficacy Beliefs, Social Norms, and Climate Anxiety among Young People in Germany. [(accessed on 4 June 2021)]; Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Marvin_Helferich/publication/344123524_Associations_between_climate_changerelated_efficacy_beliefs_social_norms_and_climate_anxiety_among_young_people_in_Germany/links/5f589295458515e96d3b61ad/Associations-between-climate-changerelated-efficacy-beliefs-social-norms-and-climate-anxiety-among-young-people-inGermany.pdf.
- 46.Nabi R.L., Gustafson A., Jensen R. Framing climate change: Exploring the role of emotion in generating advocacy behavior. Sci. Commun. 2018;40:442–468. doi: 10.1177/1075547018776019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lowe T., Brown K., Dessai S., de França Doria M., Haynes K., Vincent K. Does tomorrow ever come? Disaster narrative and public perceptions of climate change. Public Underst. Sci. 2006;15:435–457. doi: 10.1177/0963662506063796. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Homburg A., Stolberg A. Explaining pro-environmental behavior with a cognitive theory of stress. J. Environ. Psychol. 2006;26:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.03.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Howell R.A., Capstick S., Whitmarsh L. Impacts of adaptation and responsibility framings on attitudes towards climate change mitigation. Clim. Chang. 2016;136:445–461. doi: 10.1007/s10584-016-1627-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Helm S.V., Pollitt A., Barnett M.A., Curran M.A., Craig Z.R. Differentiating environmental concern in the context of psychological adaption to climate change. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2018;48:158–167. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2017.11.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Steg L., Vlek C. Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: An integrative review and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009;29:309–317. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Sora B., Höge T., Caballer A., Peiró J.M. Employment contract, job insecurity and employees’ affective well-being: The role of self-and collective efficacy. Econ. Ind. Democr. 2019;40:193–214. doi: 10.1177/0143831X18804659. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hornsey M.J., Chapman C.M., Oelrichs D.M. Why it is so hard to teach people they can make a difference: Climate change efficacy as a non-analytic form of reasoning. Think. Reason. 2021:1–19. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2021.1893222. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Swim J.K., Geiger N., Lengieza M.L. Climate change marches as motivators for bystander collective action. Front. Commun. 2019;4:4. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2019.00004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Dickinson J.L., Crain R., Yalowitz S., Cherry T.M. How framing climate change influences citizen scientists’ intentions to do something about it. J. Environ. Educ. 2013;44:145–158. doi: 10.1080/00958964.2012.742032. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Ojala M. Regulating Worry, Promoting Hope: How Do Children, Adolescents, and Young Adults Cope with Climate Change? Int. J. Environ. Sci. Educ. 2012;7:537–561. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ojala M. How do children cope with global climate change? Coping strategies, engagement, and well-being. J. Environ. Psychol. 2012;32:225–233. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.02.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Ojala M. Geographies of Global Issues: Change and Threat. Springer Science+Business Media B.V.; Berlin, Germany: 2016. Young people and global climate change: Emotions, coping, and engagement in everyday life; pp. 1–19. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ojala M. Adolescents’ worries about environmental risks: Subjective well-being, values, and existential dimensions. J. Youth Stud. 2005;8:331–347. doi: 10.1080/13676260500261934. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wu J., Snell G., Samji H. Climate anxiety in young people: A call to action. Lancet Planet. Health. 2020;4:e435–e436. doi: 10.1016/S2542-5196(20)30223-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Ogunbode C.A., Doran R., Hanss D., Ytre-Arne B., Moe H., Ojala M., Salmela-Aro K. Investigating Associations between Media Exposure, Climate Anxiety and Mental Health (MECAMH) [(accessed on 30 October 2019)];2019 Available online: https://osf.io/6n4rb/
- 62.Spielberger C.D. STAI State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults form Y: Review Set; Manual, Test, Scoring Key. Mind Garden; Redwood City, CA, USA: 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Pedrabissi L., Santinello M. Verifica della validità dello STAI forma Y di Spielberger. [Verification of the validity of the STAI, Form, Y., by Spielberger.] Giunti Organ. Spec. 1989;191–192:11–14. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Doran R., Hanss D., Larsen S. Attitudes, efficacy beliefs, and willingness to pay for environmental protection when travelling. Tour. Hosp. Res. 2015;15:281–292. doi: 10.1177/1467358415580360. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Doran R., Larsen S. The relative importance of social and personal norms in explaining intentions to choose eco-friendly travel options. Int. J. Tour. Res. 2016;18:159–166. doi: 10.1002/jtr.2042. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Doran R., Hanss D., Larsen S. Intentions to make sustainable tourism choices: Do value orientations, time perspective, and efficacy beliefs explain individual differences? Scand. J. Hosp. Tour. 2017;17:223–238. doi: 10.1080/15022250.2016.1179129. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.General Assembly of the World Medical Association World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. J. Am. Coll. Dent. 2014;81:14–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Stankuniene G., Streimikiene D., Kyriakopoulos G.L. Systematic literature review on behavioral barriers of climate change mitigation in households. Sustainability. 2020;12:7369. doi: 10.3390/su12187369. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Moser S.C. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Climate Science. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2017. Communicating climate change adaptation and resilience. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Moser S.C. Communicating adaptation to climate change: The art and science of public engagement when climate change comes home. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Clim. Chang. 2014;5:337–358. doi: 10.1002/wcc.276. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Moser S.C. Reflections on climate change communication research and practice in the second decade of the 21st century: What more is there to say? Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Clim. Chang. 2016;7:345–369. doi: 10.1002/wcc.403. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Verplanken B., Roy D. “My worries are rational, climate change is not”: Habitual ecological worrying is an adaptive response. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e74708. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kurth C. The Anxious Mind: An Investigation into the Varieties and Virtues of Anxiety. The MIT Press; Cambridge, UK: 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 74.Bradley G.L., Babutsidze Z., Chai A., Reser J.P. The role of climate change risk perception, response efficacy, and psychological adaptation in pro-environmental behavior: A two nation study. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020;68:101410. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101410. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Su W., Han X., Yu H., Wu Y., Potenza M.N. Do men become addicted to internet gaming and women to social media? A meta-analysis examining gender-related differences in specific internet addiction. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2020;113:106480. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106480. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Darawshy N.A., Gewirtz A., Marsalis S. Psychological intervention and prevention programs for child and adolescent exposure to community violence: A systematic review. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2020;25:1–4. doi: 10.1007/s10567-020-00315-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Darawshy N.A., Haj-Yahia M.M. Internalizing and externalizing symptoms among Palestinian adolescents from Israel as consequences of their exposure to community violence: Are they moderated by their self-efficacy and collective efficacy? Child Abus. Negl. 2018;79:61–73. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Paulhus D.L., Reid D.B. Enhancement and denial in socially desirable responding. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1991;60:307. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.307. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Van de Mortel T.F. Faking it: Social desirability response bias in self-report research. Aust. J. Adv. Nurs. 2008;25:40–48. [Google Scholar]
- 80.Verplanken B., Marks E., Dobromir A.I. On the nature of eco-anxiety: How constructive or unconstructive is habitual worry about global warming? J. Environ. Psychol. 2020;72:101528. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101528. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Clayton S., Karazsia B.T. Development and validation of a measure of climate change anxiety. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020;69:101434. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101434. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.