Abstract
The hippocampal spatial code’s relevance for downstream neuronal populations—particularly its major subcortical output the lateral septum (LS)—is still poorly understood. Here, using calcium imaging combined with unbiased analytical methods, we functionally characterized and compared the spatial tuning of LS GABAergic cells to those of dorsal CA3 and CA1 cells. We identified a significant number of LS cells that are modulated by place, speed, acceleration, and direction, as well as conjunctions of these properties, directly comparable to hippocampal CA1 and CA3 spatially modulated cells. Interestingly, Bayesian decoding of position based on LS spatial cells reflected the animal’s location as accurately as decoding using the activity of hippocampal pyramidal cells. A portion of LS cells showed stable spatial codes over the course of multiple days, potentially reflecting long-term episodic memory. The distributions of cells exhibiting these properties formed gradients along the anterior–posterior and dorsal–ventral axes of the LS, directly reflecting the topographical organization of hippocampal inputs to the LS. Finally, we show using transsynaptic tracing that LS neurons receiving CA3 and CA1 excitatory input send projections to the hypothalamus and medial septum, regions that are not targeted directly by principal cells of the dorsal hippocampus. Together, our findings demonstrate that the LS accurately and robustly represents spatial, directional as well as self-motion information and is uniquely positioned to relay this information from the hippocampus to its downstream regions, thus occupying a key position within a distributed spatial memory network.
Calcium imaging of neurons in freely behaving mice reveals how the lateral septum, the main output of the hippocampal place cells, effectively represents information about not only location, but also head direction and self-movement, and may be pivotal in sending this information to downstream brain regions.
Introduction
The lateral septum (LS) is an anatomically complex structure uniquely positioned within a broader distributed spatial memory network. The LS consists almost exclusively of highly interconnected inhibitory GABAergic cells [1,2] that are densely innervated by the principal cells of the hippocampus [3,4], in contrast to the medial septum (MS), which receives inputs solely from the inhibitory neurons of the hippocampus [5]. Previous research has implicated the LS in a wide variety of behaviors, including spatial and working memory [6–10], regulation of feeding [11–14], anxiety [15–18], locomotion [19,20], and social behaviors [21–24].
To date, functional characterizations of the LS have often focused on the spatial coding properties of these neurons, primarily due to the extensive excitatory input from positionally tuned hippocampal pyramidal neurons, commonly referred to as “place cells” [4,24–26]. Yet, this body of work has not yet yielded a consensus. Several studies have reported place-like cells in the LS, but the number, information content, and stability vary widely from study to study [20,27–31], while others described a lack of canonical place cells altogether [4]. As previous studies often depended on variable criteria of what defines a place cell, estimates range from 5.3% [4] to 56.0% [20] of LS cells classified as spatially modulated. Strikingly, even estimates by the same authors on data acquired from the same subjects engaging the same task ranged between 26.5% [32] to 56.0% [20]. When reported, LS place cells are typically described as of lesser quality than classic hippocampal place cells, with lower within-session stability and larger place fields [30,31]. Adding to this complexity, the LS is a large structure that is cytoarchitecturally subdivided into dorsal, intermediate, and ventral subregions that spread across its anterior–posterior axis [5,26,33]. This suggests that, in addition to subjective criteria, the striking differences in LS spatial coding characteristics between studies may be in part a product of variations in recording locations within the region. In addition to positional information, LS neurons may also encode direction and self-motion information, including acceleration and velocity [20,30,31].
Beyond their spatial tuning properties, hippocampal place cells are also noted for the rapid reorganization of the place fields across time (on the order of days) and navigational contexts, a phenomenon termed “remapping” [34–38]. If the LS spatial code is directly inherited from its hippocampal inputs, one would expect comparable dynamics over a similar timescale. While the LS, similar to the hippocampus, has been implicated in behaviors that may require a stable representation of space over time, including context-reward [39,40], contextual fear conditioning [16,41–44], spatial learning and memory [32,45,46] remapping dynamics in the LS remain largely understudied. Thus, whether and how the LS spatial code changes across time is crucial to clarifying its relationship to the upstream hippocampal spatial code, with important implications for its involvement in this broader spatial memory network and how it supports behaviors across longer timescales.
To resolve these outstanding questions, we functionally characterize hippocampal connectivity of LS and asked how information-rich LS GABAergic neurons are in relation to their main pyramidal inputs in dorsal CA3 and CA1. To this end, we used head-mounted miniaturized microscopes to record approximately 2,000 LS neurons across the anterior–posterior and dorsal–ventral axes of the LS, and compared their firing characteristics to pyramidal cells recorded in CA1 and CA3, as animals navigated linear track and open field environments. Results demonstrate that the LS accurately and robustly represents spatial, speed, and directional information in an anatomically organized fashion. While the information of LS neurons is generally comparable to CA3 and CA1, our tracing studies suggest that LS information is sent to downstream brain regions such as the MS and hypothalamus that are not directly connected by the principal cells of the dorsal hippocampus.
Results
LS GABAergic neurons exhibit stable spatial activity during alternation on a linear track
Previous electrophysiological studies of the LS disagree on both the proportion of place encoding cells in this region and their spatial content. Cells have been previously described to be of lower quality than the classically described hippocampal place cells [20,30,32] or to be virtually absent [4].
First, to assess the distribution of excitatory pyramidal inputs from each hippocampal subregion to the GABAergic cells of the LS, we injected a monosynaptic retrograde rabies tracer in dorsal LS (Figs 1A and S1, n = 5 mice), utilizing the monosynaptically restricted, Cre-dependent retrograde tracing features of this technique. We observed strong labeling of pyramidal neurons of the hippocampus in areas CA1 and CA3 (Figs 1B and S1). To investigate how the spatial coding properties of the LS compare to these main dorsal hippocampal inputs, we expressed the calcium (Ca2+) indicator GCaMP6f in GABAergic cells of the LS or in the pyramidal cells of the dorsal CA1 (dCA1) and dorsal CA3 (dCA3) of mice (Figs 1C, S2 and S3). We recorded Ca2+ activity from 1,082 LS GABAergic neurons (n = 15 mice), 1,251 dCA1 pyramidal cells (n = 5 mice), and 464 cells from dCA3 (n = 6 mice) from animals alternating on a linear track. We subsequently filtered and binarized the recorded calcium traces (S4A Fig) [47] and excluded periods of immobility (<5 cm.s−1). Using an information-theoretic framework [48–50], we computed the mutual information (MI) between calcium activity and spatial location for each cell and expressed the results in bits (S4B Fig). As we observed a significant correlation between probability of a cell to be active (P(A)) and MI (S4B Fig), we computed the MI value for 30 bootstrap samples (50% data sampling) and compared those to 30 circularly shuffled surrogates, which allowed us to compute significance of the MI value independently of P(A) (S4C–S4E Fig) [47]. We found that LS GABAergic activity was significantly spatially modulated (p < 0.01, Figs 1D and S4H). Indeed, both MI and within-session stability were higher for spatially modulated LS cells compared to nonmodulated cells (Mann–Whitney test, p < 0.0001, S4E and S4F Fig). Using this approach, a total of 43.90% of LS GABAergic cells qualified as spatially modulated (475/1,082 cells), with LS cells similarly distributed along the linear track as place cells recorded from dorsal CA1 and dorsal CA3 (Fig 1E). In contrast to previous reports [4,20,32], we observed a significant difference in the proportion of spatially modulated cells across recording regions (LS: 43.57 ± 4.46%; dCA1: 29.60 ± 2.99%; dCA3: 27.01 ± 7.07%; Kruskal–Wallis, H(3) = 6.212, p = 0.0448; Dunn’s multiple comparisons test: LS-dCA1: p = 0.1757; LS-dCA3: p = 0.1101; Fig 1F). We found that within-session stability of spatially modulated cells (expressed as a correlation where 0 means no stability while 1 means perfectly stable) was significantly different between LS (0.44 ± .0165) and dCA3 (0.73 ± 0.26; Kruskal–Wallis, H(3) = 80.48, p < 0.0001; Dunn’s multiple comparisons test, p < 0.0001), and between dCA1 and dCA3 (p < 0.0001; Fig 1G). Even though LS GABAergic cells are not fast spiking (S5 and S6 Figs) [51], P(A) in LS (0.02005 ± 4.692 × 104) is higher than both dCA1 (0.0094 ± 2.544 × 104, Kruskal–Wallis, H(3) = 502.0, p < 0.0001; Dunn’s multiple comparisons test, p > 0.0001) and dCA3 (0.009225 ± 3.759 × 104, p < 0.0001, S6A and S6B Fig). Therefore, we assessed the MI in bits carried in each binarized event and observed that the LS (5.624 × 105 ± 1.151 × 106 bits/binarized event) carries significantly less information per binarized event than CA1 (1.430 × 104 ± 1.613 × 106 bits/binarized event; Kruskal–Wallis, H(3) = 1,288, p < 0.0001; Dunn’s multiple comparisons test, p < 0.0001); and CA3 (1.130 × 104 ± 2.507 × 106; bits/binarized event; p < 0.0001; Fig 1H). To confirm that the observed differences in proportion of spatially modulated cells and within-session stability were not due to differences in P(A), we used an activity cutoff of P(A) > 0.001 and confirmed our previous results (S6D Fig).
In contrast to their omnidirectional firing properties in open field environments, hippocampal neurons rapidly develop strong directional selectivity on linear tracks [52–54]. To test whether the LS place code developed comparable directional selectivity, we ranked spatially modulated cells as a function of the difference between MI for left and right traversals (S7A–S7D Fig), and found LS cells tuned to the absolute location regardless of direction (7.67%, 83/1,082 cells), while other cells were selective to the left (15.52%, 168/1,082 cells) or right trajectories (22.08%, 239/1,082 cells).
Previous studies have used decoding methods to predict behavioral variables from calcium imaging data recorded in the hippocampus [34,38,55], yielding insights into the amount of information encoded by a neuronal assembly. Here, we asked whether we could reliably estimate the mouse location solely from LS neuronal activity patterns (Fig 2). Using 30 bootstrap samples of 60 cells (Fig 2B and 2C), decoding location using LS neuronal activity significantly outperformed a decoder that was trained using shuffled data (paired t test, t(6) = 13.56, p < 0.0001; Fig 2D), even when decreasing the number of neurons used (S8A and S8B Fig). Similarly, a decoder trained using CA1 or CA3 data also yielded significantly less error than one using shuffled surrogates (CA1: t(4) = 7.558, p = 0.0016; CA3: t(3) = 3.233, p = 0.0481; Figs 2D and S8C and S8D). While using a small bootstrap sample size allowed a fair comparison between recording regions, it also induced higher error rates. To ensure the proper functioning of our decoder, we confirmed that the decoding error decreased significantly when increasing bootstrapped sample size (S8C and S8D Fig).
In order to compare decoding accuracy for LS, CA1, and CA3, a decoding score was computed (Fig 2E; see Methods), and we found that LS significantly outperformed CA3 (one-way ANOVA, F(2,13) = 6.277, p = 0.0124; Holm–Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, p < 0.05), but not CA1. Overall, spatially modulated cells recorded in LS were significantly more active than those recorded in the hippocampus (H(3) = 155.1, p < 0.0001; S5A and S5B Fig), which could be a contributing factor to the overall higher decoding accuracy using LS cells. To confirm that the differences observed in decoding error are not due to the inclusion of a large number of pseudo-silent cells in the dCA1 or dCA3 bootstrapped samples, we confirmed our findings using an activity cutoff of P(A) > 0.001 (S8E and S8F Fig). Similarly, to confirm that the observed differences in decoding error are not due to the use of a temporal smoothing window, we replicated our results using a decoder that omitted temporal filtering (S8G and S8H Fig).
LS GABAergic neurons exhibit stable spatial activity during free exploration in a 2D environment
In order to assess spatial coding during free exploration in a 2D environment, we recorded Ca2+ activity of 1,899 GABAergic neurons in 28 mice implanted in distinct subregions of the LS, while the animals were freely exploring a novel open field. Out of 1,899 recorded cells, 37.80% (718/1,899 cells, n = 28 mice) of LS cells displayed significant spatial information (p < 0.01; Figs 3A and 3B and S9), as compared to 31.33% (323/1,031 cells, n = 6 mice) in CA1 and 25.84% (138/534 cells, n = 8 mice) CA3 cells. Despite the LS’ known involvement in feeding-related behaviors [11–14], we did not observe any increased number of cells with firing fields around food zones, nor did we observe overrepresentation of objects or walls (S10 Fig). To assess reward-modulation in the context of increased task demand, we assessed spatial firing of LS GABAergic neurons recorded while animals performed a delayed nonmatching to place task in a T-maze but did not observe any significant reward code compared to shuffled surrogates (S11 Fig).
With theta-rhythmic cells in the LS having higher firing rates than theta-rhythm-independent cells [56], and the suggestion that theta-rhythmic cells could receive direct hippocampal inputs (though see [57,58]), we asked whether LS cells with higher firing rates were also more likely to be spatially modulated. Indeed, spatially modulated cells were significantly more active than nonmodulated cells (Mann–Whitney test, U = 342,219, p < 0.0001; S9G and S9H Fig) and displayed a higher bursting index, defined as the probability of a cell being active, given it was already in an active state, or P(At|At-1), than nonspatially modulated cells (Mann–Whitney test, U = 368,276, p < 0.0001; S9H Fig).
We compared the place field properties of cells recorded in LS to those of dorsal CA1 and CA3 (Fig 3). For each animal, we computed the portion of significantly positionally modulated cells per region, which was not significantly different across LS (36.71 ± 2.16%), dCA1 (38.32 ± 9.36%), and dCA3 (28.39 ± 6.73%; one-way ANOVA, F(2,39) = 1.105, p = 0.3414; Fig 3B). We computed the information for each binarized event and observed that the LS (1.020 × 104 ± 7.56 × 107 bits/binarized event) carries significantly less information per binarized event than CA1 (1.200 × 104 ± 1.067 × 106 bits/binarized event; Kruskal–Wallis, H(3) = 264.6, p < 0.0001; Dunn’s multiple comparisons test, p < 0.0001) and CA3 (1.199 × 104 ± 1.605 × 106 bits/binarized event; p < 0.0001). As expected on the basis of prior results [4,28,30,31], within-session stability of LS cells (0.31 ± 0.011) was significantly lower than that of spatially modulated cells recorded in CA1 (0.44 ± 0.017; Kruskal–Wallis, H(3) = 89.52, p < 0.0001; Dunn’s multiple comparisons test, p < 0.0001) and CA3 (0.56 ± 0.027; p < 0.0001; Fig 3D), and place fields were more dispersed (LS: 14.47 ± 0.13 cm; CA1: 12.00 ± 0.207 cm; CA3: 10.61 ± 0.35 cm; Kruskal–Wallis, H(3) = 155.7, p < 0.0001; Fig 3E). Similar to recordings in a 1D environment, we observed large differences in P(A) between spatially modulated cells recorded from LS and those from dorsal CA1 and CA3 (S12A Fig). Therefore, we also assessed for each region the proportion of spatially modulated cells, split-half stability and mean-dispersion using an activity cutoff of P(A) > 0.001, and confirmed our results (S12B Fig).
When decoding the location of the animals using bootstrap samples of 80 cells from each region (Fig 3F), decoding error was significantly lower in LS (20.16 ± 1.48 cm) compared to shuffled surrogates (26.03 ± 0.84 cm; two-way RM ANOVA, F(1,13) = 89.09, p < 0.0001 for main effect of shuffle), even when decreasing the number of neurons used to only 40 neurons (S13A and S13B Fig). Omission of temporal filtering recapitulated these results (S13E Fig). The same held true for decoders trained using CA1 or CA3 data, respectively (S13C and S13D Fig). Strikingly, a decoder trained on data from LS does not perform significantly worse than a decoder trained on CA1 or CA3 data (F(2,13) = 2.186, p = 0.2384 for main effect of region; Fig 3H). Similarly to recordings on the linear track, we used an activity cutoff of P(A) > 0.001 to confirm that the differences observed in decoding error between regions are not due to the inclusion of a large number of pseudo-silent cells in the bootstrapped sample (S13F Fig).
A subset of LS cells has stable spatial representations up to 8 days, similar to CA1
To test whether the LS and the hippocampus exhibit comparable evolutions of their spatial code over time, we recorded from animals implanted in LS and dorsal CA1 in a novel open field over both short (3 days) and longer timescales (8 days; Figs 4 and S14). We assessed the stability of the spatial map across sessions and observed a subset of LS spatially modulated cells that were stable over time (Fig 4B). Correlating the tuning maps of aligned LS cell pairs over days lead to a significant increase in mean pairwise correlation value over days (two-way ANOVA, F(2,808) = 3.259, p = 0.0389, main effect of time) with LS cells being statistically more stable than shuffled comparisons (F(2,808) = 277.0, p < 0.0001, main effect of shuffling; Figs 4C and S14C). We observed the opposite pattern for pyramidal cells recorded from dorsal CA1, with a significant decrease in mean pairwise correlation value over days (two-way ANOVA, F(2,766) = 33.47, p < 0.0001 main effect of time, F(1,766) = 319.8, p < 0.0001 main effect of shuffle; Fig 4C). For cells recorded in the LS, we observed the strongest within-session stability on day 3 (Kruskal–Wallis, H(4) = 19.16, p = 0.0003; S14E Fig), as well as an increase in spatial information (Kruskal–Wallis, H(4) = 13.27, p = 0.0041; S14B Fig). There was no significant increase in the proportion of stable cell pairs over subsequent days (one-way ANOVA, F(3) = 0.400, p = 0.9537; S14D Fig), or the proportion of spatially modulated cells per animal for each days (one-way ANOVA, F(3,16) = 1.618, p = 0.2247; S14E Fig).
We next assessed the significant tuning map correlations on subsequent day pairs for those cells that were significantly spatially modulated on day 1 and were subsequently found on all recording days (Fig 4D), as well as those that passed our criteria for spatial modulation on all days (Fig 4E). We then compared the place field correlation for different day pairs (F(5,363) = 2.625, p = 0.0239, interaction effect), and we found that on shorter timescales, tuning maps for LS cells (0.316 ± 0.0507) are significantly less correlated than CA1 (0.497 ± 0.0411; Fisher’s LSD day 1 to 2, p = 0.0153; Fig 4F). On the other hand, the correlation for day pairs 3 to 8 is higher for the LS (0.369 ± 0.0513) compared to dorsal CA1 (0.174 ± 0.0669; Fisher’s LSD, p = 0.0098; Fig 4F). Together, this suggests that a subset of LS spatial cells encodes spatial information over longer periods of time.
LS represents direction, velocity, and acceleration information
In CA1, place cells have been found to carry directional [59–61] and speed-related information [61–64]. Previous studies found that subsets of LS neurons show some degree of modulation by direction of travel [31] as well as velocity and acceleration [20] through correlative measures in spatial alternation or T-maze navigation tasks, where the relationship between cell activity patterns and movement-associated variables may be confounded with task-dependent variables. Here, we again employed an information-theoretic approach to compute the MI between calcium activity and each of these self-motion correlates in a subset of mice recorded during a 15-minute free foraging task in the open field. We found that 28.13% of LS cells are significantly modulated by head-direction (346/1,230 cells, n = 19 mice; Figs 5A and S15), 18.70% by velocity (230/1,230 cells; Fig 5C) and 24.63% by acceleration (303/1,230 cells; Fig 5E). We assessed the stability of LS directional and self-motion tuning over short (3 days) and longer timescales (8 days; S16 Fig), and we observed a subset of LS directionally modulated cells that were stable over time (S16A Fig). Mean tuning vectors correlation of aligned LS cell pairs significantly increased over days (two-way ANOVA, F(2,774) = 3.682, p = 0.0256, main effect of time) with LS cells being statistically more stable than shuffled surrogates (F(1,774) = 54,42, p < 0.0001, main effect of shuffling; S16C Fig). For velocity or acceleration tuning, we did not observe such stable tuning over time (two-way ANOVA, velocity: F(1,506) = 2.609, p = 0.1069, acceleration: F(1,320) = 0.1986, p = 0.6561, main effect of shuffle; S16D and S16E Fig).
We compared the proportion of LS cells significantly modulated by each of these variables with the proportion of those cell types found in dCA1 and dCA3. The proportion of directionally modulated cells was significantly different across regions (LS: 27.89 ± 2.16, dCA1: 46.11 ± 4.98, dCA3: 28.92 ± 4.52; Kruskal–Wallis, H(3) = 6.586, p = 0.0371, Fig 5B) with a higher proportion of directionally modulated cells in CA1 as compared to LS (Dunn’s multiple comparisons test, p = 0.0340; Fig 5B). The proportion of velocity encoding cells was not significantly different between LS (20.43 ± 2.598), CA1 (24.38 ± 7.73), or CA3 (20.04 ± 3.62; Kruskal–Wallis, H(3) = 0.1016, p = 0.9504, Fig 5D). Similarly, no difference was found in the proportion of acceleration encoding cells between LS (25.11 ± 2.34), CA1 (24.14 ± 4.13), or CA3 (22.60 ± 2.98, Kruskal–Wallis, H(3) = 0.3337, p = 0.8463; Fig 5F). We found a proportion of cells that were significantly tuned to more than one of these variables in both LS as well as dCA1 and dCA3 (Figs 5G, S15A–S15D and S17), with 22.03% of LS cells (271/1,230 cells) encoding more than one modality (Fig 5H). Together, this conjunctive coding for location, directionality, and velocity indicates that LS cells fire in response to more complex navigational features, similarly to the hippocampus.
To understand the function of this downstream copy of the hippocampal spatial code, we assessed the downstream targets of the dCA1/dCA3 to LS projection. First, we used a Cre-dependent, anterograde tracing approach to confirm LS projections to the hypothalamus and ventral tegmental area (VTA) in the mouse (S18 Fig), as it was initially described in the rat [3,5,25]. Next, we leveraged the anterograde transsynaptic properties of Cre-expressing AAV1 viral vector [65] and assessed the targets of the dorsal CA1-lateral septum (dCA1-LS) as well as dorsal CA3-LS (dCA3-LS) projections (Fig 6). For either dCA1 or dCA3 injections (Figs 6A and S19), we observed a direct pathway from the principal cells of the hippocampus, via the LS, leading to dense innervation of the MS (Fig 6B) and the hypothalamic regions (Fig 6C). We observed that dCA1-LS preferentially targets the LH, and dCA3-LS targets the hypothalamus more broadly as well as the nuclei of the medial zone. Whether the GABAergic cells of the LS form functional synapses at the level of the MS/diagonal band complex is debated [5,26,66–68]. Thus, in order to determine whether the projections observed at the level of the MS with both our anterograde tracing approaches constituted passing fibers or synaptic connections, we used an AAV-Flex-Synaptag [69], a Cre-dependent viral construct to express mRuby in the cytosol and eGFP bound to synaptophysin, a protein primarily expressed in the presynaptic endings of cells participating in synaptic transmission (Fig 6D). For this, we used a CaMKIIα-Cre mouse line, expressing Cre only in CAMKII-positive cells, a marker that is abundant in the GABAergic cells of the LS but absent in the MS, thereby preventing any unspecific labeling. After expressing AAV-Flex-Synaptag in the intermediate LS, we observed significant synaptophysin-bound projections in the MS (Fig 6E), confirming the existence of a synaptic interface between the LS and the MS. Together, our work shows that LS receives direct inputs from dorsal CA1 and CA3 and, in turn, projects to regions that are not directly receiving inputs from the hippocampus itself (Fig 6F).
LS cells coding for space, direction, and self-motion are nonuniformly distributed
To map out the monosynaptic connections between hippocampal and LS neurons, we injected the anterograde transsynaptic Cre-expressing AAV1 viral vector [65], in either dCA1 or dCA3 (S19 and S20A Figs). We subsequently injected a Cre-dependent eYFP expressing virus in the LS to visualize cells that receive direct hippocampal input (S20B Fig). Importantly, this approach allowed us to quantify the cells along the LS anterior–posterior and dorsal–ventral axis receiving direct hippocampal inputs and assess their distribution density within the anterior regions of the LS (Bregma +1.0 to 0.0 anterior–posterior). Because we can only observe secondary labeled cell bodies at locations where there was infusion of the secondary, Cre-dependent virus, we used a relatively large injection volume (total 800 nL, targeted at the intermediate LS). Both dorsal dCA1 and dCA3 injections resulted in a majority of labeled LS cell bodies in dorsal regions compared to intermediate and ventral LS (S20D–S20F Fig). Hence, LS neurons receiving direct inputs from dorsal CA1 and CA3 appear to be localized at more dorsal–posterior regions as compared to more ventral–anterior regions of LS.
Next, we examined whether spatially modulated cells recorded in LS had spatial properties that were dependent on the localization of their recording. To measure this, we classified the location of gradient refractive index (GRIN) lens implants along the dorsal–ventral and anterior–posterior axis (Fig 7A). Postmortem histological verification of GRIN lens implant combined with within-animal analysis of the location of recorded cells enabled us to approximate the location of each cell (Fig 7B). While we observed no differences in cell activity characteristics, such as bursting index or activity probability along the anterior–posterior axis, medial–lateral axis, or dorsal–ventral axis (S21 Fig), we found a pronounced increase in the portion of stable spatially modulated cells (within-session stability > 0.5) along the anterior–posterior axis (linear regression, R2 = 0.1071, p = 0.0022, the proportion of stable cells per 0.2 mm bin per animal, n = 24 mice; Fig 7C). We observed a similar gradient along the dorsal–ventral axis, with a larger proportion of stable spatial cells found at the more dorsal regions of the LS, although this trend failed to reach significance (linear regression, R2 = 0.1399, p = 0.0718, n = 24 mice; Fig 7C). Taken together, these data strongly suggest that regions of the LS that receive stronger innervation from the dorsal hippocampus have a larger proportion of cells that reliably encode space. This suggests that this information could be directly inherited from the hippocampus, which should be tested using targeted inactivation of the hippocampal pyramidal inputs to LS.
We also assessed the topographical distribution of direction and self-motion tuned cells within the LS. We observed a relatively strong dorsal–ventral gradient in the distribution of stable (within-session stability > 0.5) directionally modulated cells (linear regression, R2 = 0.3837, p = 0.0061, n = 17 mice; Fig 7C), with most directionally modulated cells located at the more dorsal regions of the LS, but we did not observe such a gradient along the anterior–posterior axis (linear regression, R2 = 0.0544, p = 0.0615, n = 24 mice; Fig 7C). Velocity-modulated cells were primarily found at the ventral pole of the LS (linear regression, R2 = 0.1836, p = 0.0367, n = 24 mice), but no such gradient was found along the anterior–posterior axis (linear regression, R2 = 0.00348 p = 0.0874, n = 24 mice). Acceleration-modulated cells were found evenly distributed throughout the LS (DV: R2 = 0.0133, p = 0.2928; AP: R2 = 0.01332, p = 0.2929, n = 24 mice).
Discussion
While spatial coding has been extensively characterized in the hippocampal formation, how downstream regions integrate this information has only recently begun to receive attention. The LS has reemerged as a critical region implicated in a space encoding network, although electrophysiological recordings of the LS in rats have led to disparate estimates of the quantity and quality of place cells in the region [4,20,28,32]: LS place cells recorded on a circular track were described to be almost absent [4] but were found to be abundant in reward-seeking tasks [20,32]. Using a large calcium imaging dataset and unbiased information metric and decoding approaches, we found that 37.80% of GABAergic LS cells robustly code for space during free exploration in an open field and 43.90% of cells during linear track alternation. One of the major features of the linear track is the induction of directional place fields in LS, which may underlie some of the differences we observed in 1D versus 2D spatial coding characteristics.
In addition to spatial information, we found that the LS also reliably encodes velocity, acceleration, and directional information, suggesting that the LS encodes more complex navigational features than previously thought. Neuronal representations in the hippocampus have been found to change over time, with place code ensembles changing rapidly: An estimated approximately 40% [34] to approximately 75% to 85% [38] of CA1 neurons were found to remap over days. The stability of the LS spatial code has received little attention to date. In the current study, we found a subpopulation of cells in the posterior LS that display stable place fields over 8 days. The functional role of such stability remains to be elucidated. One possibility is that this ensemble may mediate the stable encoding of contexts over longer durations of time, which could account for the critical importance of the LS for a number of spatial behaviors, including contextual fear conditioning [16,41–44], context-reward associations [39,40], and radial-maze navigation tasks [45,46].
Similar to the change in properties of hippocampal place cells along the dorsal–ventral axis [70,71], a previous work has hinted at a dorsal to ventral organization of the LS place code: Electrophysiology studies recorded a slightly larger proportion of LS place cells in the more dorsal regions [30]. A complementary anatomical microstructure has been previously described for the rate-independent phase code in the LS, where the strength of the phase code increased as a function of recording depth along the dorsal–ventral axis [4]. Here, we find that lateral septal spatially modulated cells are arranged along an anterior–posterior gradient similar to the gradient of hippocampal inputs into the LS—a finding that helps to reconcile the variation in previously reported estimates of spatial cells in the LS, which ranged between 5.3% of cells recorded along the dorsal–ventral axis [4] up to 56% recorded at the most dorsal level of the LS [20]. This pattern suggests that the inhibitory neurons of the LS may inherit a place code from the hippocampus, with subregions receiving dorsal hippocampal inputs being most similar to the classical hippocampal place cells. One potential caveat of using miniaturized microscope when comparing hippocampus and LS coding properties is that of circuit damage due to GRIN lens implantation, which may be particularly the case for CA3 implants due to the occasional damage to CA1. Interestingly, the LS place code described here is not the only case of GABAergic neurons inheriting some spatial properties from hippocampal pyramidal cells: Interneurons within CA1 have been shown to exhibit significant spatial modulation and comparable information as pyramidal place cells, although with a higher firing rate and greater spatial dispersion [72]. It should be considered that, although the hippocampus seems a likely source of the positional information encoded by the LS, this information could also arise from incoming projections from any other region projecting to LS, such as the entorhinal cortex [5,25], which could account for part of head direction and velocity tuning.
In addition to spatial modulation of LS cells, we observed a significant number of velocity- and acceleration-modulated cells in the LS. These cells have been previously described by others on the basis of correlative measures, although estimated proportions vary widely: For velocity tuning in reward-seeking tasks, estimates range from almost absent [4] to almost 60% of cells [20]. For acceleration, approximately 45% of LS cells were found to show some degree of correlation, with almost 30% of recorded cells correlated with both speed and acceleration [20]. Here, using information metrics and a free exploration task in which firing characteristics are not confounded with task parameters, we find that 18.70% of cells were modulated by velocity and 24.63% by acceleration. Cells reliably coding for both velocity and acceleration were relatively rare (2%). Velocity and acceleration cells were distributed dissimilarly in the LS, with velocity cells being more abundant toward the ventral pole, whereas acceleration-modulated cells were found to be distributed equally. This raises the question whether velocity and acceleration information are inherited from different areas upstream of the LS. Interestingly, cells coding for direction were more abundant toward the dorsal portion of the LS but distributed equally along the anterior–posterior axis. Previous studies have identified classical head direction cells in multiple, well-described circuits, including the postsubiculum [73,74], anterodorsal thalamic nucleus [75], mammillary bodies [76], entorhinal cortex [77], retrosplenial cortex [78,79], and parasubiculum [80,81]. A likely source of directional information to the LS is the entorhinal cortex [82]. Our work supports the perspective that LS neurons combine a wide range of modalities and may form a complex representation of context over longer timescales than previously reported. In addition to space, velocity, and reward information, hippocampal pyramidal cells have been shown to encode variables such as time [83,84], odor [85,86], and sound frequency [87]. Whether this information is also relayed to the LS remains to be elucidated. Despite this interconnectedness with the LH and previous reports of LS place cells being skewed toward reward in a spatial navigation task [32], we did not observe any overrepresentation of LS spatially modulated cells around food zones during free exploration or around the reward zone in a T-maze non-match-to-place paradigm.
The question arises of what the function could be of having a region downstream of the hippocampus that expresses such seemingly similar coding characteristics. Previous work estimated the convergence of hippocampal efferents onto LS neurons to be 20 to 800 times denser than to any of its cortical targets [4]. Understanding how individual LS neurons integrate the thousands of synaptic inputs they receive from these hippocampal pyramidal neurons will be critical to understanding how the hippocampal map is processed downstream. Through the process of synaptic integration, a target neuron can fire an action potential upon receiving sufficient temporally coincidental excitatory input on its dendrites. The activation of an LS neuron could thus require multiple hippocampal pyramidal cells to spike simultaneously or in close temporal proximity. Hippocampal pyramidal cells will fire concomitantly when their place fields are in overlapping regions or in close vicinity of another. Due to the high interconnectivity of LS GABAergic neurons [88,89], the activation of one LS cell could subsequently lead to the inhibition of other neurons in the nucleus [24,90], thereby reducing noise. Thus, through this process of coincidence detection and recurrent inhibition, the spatial map could converge to be represented by a much lower number of cells. As such, the LS could accurately convey hippocampal information to downstream regions using a much more distributed code and thereby support more effective information transmission and associative learning [91,92].
Finally, our tracing work shows LS cells receiving hippocampal inputs from principal cells in dorsal CA1 and CA3 in turn project directly to the LH and the MS. Although the existence of an LS–MS projection has long been debated [5,26,66,68,93], we observed both dense innervation at the level of the MS after transduction of dCA1-input and dCA3-input receiving LS cells, as well as synaptic densities between the LS and MS. This suggests that principal cells of the hippocampus send indirect projections to the MS through LS neurons. This circuit is complementary to the well-described projections of hippocampal GABAergic interneurons directly to the MS, which may play a key role in theta-rhythm generation [94,95]. The MS plays a role in generating and propagating theta-rhythms throughout the hippocampal formation [96–98], which, in turn, organizes hippocampal place cell activity [99], as well as a behavioral role in the initiation and velocity of locomotion [100,101]. One hypothesis is that the information on locomotion-related information reported in specific MS neurons [101] may originate from the LS. Moreover, we observed a direct projection from CA1 and CA3, via the LS, to the hypothalamus. This pathway was previously shown by previously using nonspecific monosynaptic anterograde tracing [3], with similar unilateral projection patterns for CA1 and bilaterally for CA3 as described here. Here, we show for the first time that the same LS neurons that receive inputs from dorsal CA1 and CA3 project directly to the hypothalamus. The hypothalamus is a highly connected region known for its role in regulating feeding behaviors [102], arousal and motivation [103], and, more recently, learning and memory [104–107]. Additionally, the LH is crucial for the control of locomotion [108–111] and thought to mediate motivational and goal-directed processes underlying feeding [12,102,112,113].
Together, our findings show that GABAergic cells of the LS may provide the hypothalamus and MS with information about location, direction, and speed, and therefore constitute a core node within a distributed spatial memory network. Within this network, the LS may be necessary for the translation of spatial information to goal-directed or environmentally appropriate actions necessary for survival.
Methods
Ethics statement
All procedures were approved by the McGill University and Douglas Hospital Research Centre Animal Use and Care Committee and in accordance with the Canadian Council on Animal Care (protocol 2015–7650).
Animals
Naive mice (8 to 16 weeks old) were housed individually on a 12-hour light/dark cycle at 22°C and 40% humidity with food and water ad libitum. All experiments were carried out during the light portion of the light/dark cycle. Both male and female mice were used for this study.
Virus injections
Mice, 8 to 16 weeks old, were anesthetized with isoflurane (5% induction, 0.5% to 2% maintenance) and placed in a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments). Body temperature was maintained with a heating pad, and eyes were hydrated with gel (Optixcare). Carprofen (10 ml/kg) was administered subcutaneously. All viral injections were performed using a glass pipette connected to a Nanoject III (Drummond) injector at a flow rate of 1 nL/second. All stereotaxic coordinates are taken from Bregma (in mm). After surgery, animals were continuously monitored until recovery. For 3 days after surgery, mice were provided with a soft diet supplemented with Carprofen for pain management (MediGel CPF, approximately 5 mg/kg/day).
Virus-mediated expression of genetically encoded calcium indicator
Adeno-associated virus (AAV) of serotype 9 containing the genetically encoded calcium indicator (GECI) 6 fast (GCaMP6f) under a flex promoter was obtained from the University of Pennsylvania Vector Core (AAV2/9.Syn.Flex.GCaMP6f.WPRE.SV40, CS0641, Penn Vector Core via Addgene). LS viral injections were targeted at the following coordinates: Anterior LS, AP: 0.86; ML: 0.35; DV: −3.0; Intermediate LS, AP: 0.38; ML: 0.35; DV: −2.7, Posterior LS, AP: 0.10; ML: 0.35; DV: −2.5, with DV + or − 0.2 to adjust for more dorsal or ventral placement of injection. LS injections were done in VGAT-IRES-Cre transgenic mice (Slc32a1tm2(cre)Lowl/J, JAX stock #016962). For hippocampal principal cell imaging, C57Bl/6J mice (JAX stock #000664) were injected with a viral vector specifically targeting GCaMP6f expression to CaMKII-positive cells (AAV5.CamKII.GCaMP6f.WPRE.SV40, Penn Vector Core via Addgene, CS1024). GCaMP6f injections were targeted at the following coordinates: CA1, AP: −1.8; ML: 1.5; DV: −1.5; CA3, AP: −2.1; ML: 2.3; DV: −2.2. Three of CA3 animals were Grik4-Cre transgenic mice (C57BL/6-Tg(Grik4-cre)G32-4Stl/J, JAX stock #006474), restricting GCaMP6f expression to the pyramidal cells of CA3 specifically.
Retrograde tracing
Retrograde tracing studies were done using a Cre-dependent retrograde Rabies tracing approach. Briefly, 100 nL of helper virus (AAVdj.hSyn.Flex.TVA.P2A.eGFP.2A.oG, NeuroPhotonics Center, Laval, Canada; [114,115]) was injected in the LSd (coordinates as above) of VGAT-IRES-Cre transgenic mice (n = 5 animals, Slc32a1tm2(cre)Lowl/J, JAX stock #016962). Cre specificity was tested by the NeuroPhotonics center. After 4 weeks of incubation time to allow for the complete expression of the helper virus, 200 nL of inactivated EnvA pseudotyped rabies virus coupled to mCherry was injected (RABV-EnvA-deltaG-mCherry, NeuroPhotonics Center) at the same coordinates as the helper virus injection, and brains were collected 7 days later.
Anterograde transsynaptic tracing
To distinguish synaptic targets from passing projection fibers of dCA1 and dCA3 within the LS, C57Bl/6J mice (JAX stock #000664) received a bilateral injection of transsynaptic Cre-inducing anterograde virus (AAV1-hSyn-Cre-WPRE-hGH, Penn Vector Core via Addgene, V24784) mixed with a Cre-dependent tdTom-expressing reporter virus to visualize injection sites (AAV2-Ef1a-flex-tdTomato, NeuroPhotonics Center, AAV602) in a 3:1 ratio, 80 nL total injection volume per hemisphere (n = 5 mice per group). Injections were targeted at the following coordinates: CA1: AP: −1.86; ML: 1.5; DV: −1.5; CA3: AP: −2.1; ML: 2.3; DV: −2.2. Following 1 week of incubation time, animals received a bilateral injection of a Cre-dependent anterograde vector in LS expressing enhanced yellow fluorescent protein (400 nL, bilateral, AP: 0.5; ML: 0.35; DV: −2.6, AAVdj-DIO-Cheta-eYFP, NeuroPhotonics Center, AAV938). Brains were collected and processed 3 weeks postinjection.
Synaptag tracing
To verify presence of synaptic connections at the level of the LS−MS projection, AAVdj-hSyn-flex-mRuby2-syp-eGFP construct (lot AAV799, NeuroPhotonics Center; Oh and colleagues, 2014) was injected in the LS of CaMKIIα-Cre mice (B6.Cg-Tg(Camk2a-cre)T29-1Stl/J, JAX stock #005359), with 100 nL total injection volume injected at 1 nL/second (unilateral, AP: 0.5; ML: 0.35; DV: −2.7). After 4 weeks of incubation time, animals were perfused, and brains were processed as described below.
GRIN lens implant and baseplate
Two to 4 weeks post-GCaMP6f injection, a 0.5-mm diameter gradient refractive index (GRIN, Inscopix, Palo Alto, California, United States of America) lens was implanted above LS, dorsal CA1 or CA3, or a 1.8-mm GRIN (Edmund Optics, Barrington, New Jersey, United States of America) lens was implanted above dorsal CA1. For a 0.5-mm LS lens implant, the lens was glued to an aluminum baseplate prior to the surgery, which provided 2 main advantages: (1) it allowed us to implant the baseplate/lens assembly in one surgery (instead of separate surgeries); and (2) during the surgery, the miniscope (V3, miniscope.org) was used to image for fluorescence while lowering the lens. The mouse was anesthetized with isoflurane, and the skull was cleared. An approximately 0.6-mm diameter hole was drilled in the skull at the level of the injection site. An anchor screw was placed above the contralateral cerebellum to stabilize the implant. The baseplate and GRIN combination were attached to the miniscope and together secured to the stereotaxic frame. After making a leading track using a 0.5-mm diameter needle, the implant was lowered in place. While lowering the GRIN lens into the tissue, an increase in fluorescent signal indicates that the injection site has been reached. Both the GRIN lens and baseplate were permanently secured using C&B-Metabond (Patterson Dental, Montreal, Quebec, Canada). A plastic cap was used to protect the GRIN lens from scratching during the recovery period and in between recording sessions. The animal was given 4 to 6 weeks of recovery time before experiments started. LS implants were targeted at the following coordinates: Anterior LS, AP: 0.86; ML: 0.35; DV: −2.9; Intermediate LS, AP: 0.38; ML: 0.35; DV: −2.65; Posterior LS, AP: 0.10; ML: 0.35; DV: −2.45; with DV + or − 0.2 to adjust for dorsal, ventral, or intermediate targets; CA3 implants were targeted at AP: −2.1; ML: 2.2; DV: −2.2.
For 1.8-mm GRIN lens implantation, an approximately 2-mm diameter cranial window was prepared. Again, an anchor screw was used to secure the implant in place. After removing the dura, a portion of the cortex above the injection site was gently aspirated using a vacuum pump without applying pressure on the underlying hippocampal tissue. The 1.8-mm GRIN lens was lowered at the following coordinates: AP: −1.8; ML: 1.5; DV: −1.8. The GRIN lens was permanently attached to the skull using Metabond, and Kwik-Sil (World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, Florida, United States of America) silicone adhesive was placed on the GRIN to protect it. On average 4 weeks postimplant, the silicone cap was removed, and CA1 was imaged using a miniscope mounted with an aluminum base plate while the mouse was under light anesthesia (approximately 0.5% isoflurane) to allow the visualization of cell activity. When a satisfying field of view was found (large number of neurons, visible landmarks), the baseplate was cemented above the GRIN lens, and a protective cap was used to protect the GRIN from scratches. Imaging would start approximately 1 week after baseplating.
Miniscope recordings
The animals were habituated by being gently handled for approximately 5 minutes per day for 3 days. Animals were then water or chow scheduled (2-hour access per day). Miniscope recordings performed at 30 Hz for 15 minutes, with just one recording session per day to minimize photobleaching. For open field recordings, animals were freely foraging for randomly placed 10% sucrose or 10% sweetened condensed milk in water rewards.
Recording environments consisted of a 45 × 45 cm dark gray open field with visual cues, or a 49 × 49 cm white plexiglass open field, which was placed in different recording chambers. For linear track recordings, rewards (30 μl of 10% sucrose water) were placed at each end of the 100-cm linear track, or a 130-cm linear track, and the mouse had to consume one reward before getting the next one delivered. To ensure that all animals received the same amount of exposure, for analysis of spatial and self-motion modulation, only recordings were included from the first time the animal spent time in the open field.
For analysis of modulation by food zones or objects, animals were placed again in the, now familiar, open field on days 1, 3, and 5. On days 2 and 4, food zones or objects were added in opposing corners of the familiar open field without changing any of the existing visual cues. Food zones or objects were visible to the animal. Time spent in the region of interest was computed by taking all timestamps in which animals had their nose within less than 2 cm from the edge of the food zone or the object.
Nonmatch to place paradigm: Mice were water scheduled (2 hours per day) and trained in an automatized T-maze (MazeEngineers, Skokie, Illinois, United States of America) to a nonmatch to place task. Briefly, each trial was divided into 2 distinct phases: sample and test. In the initial sample phase, a randomly selected arm was blocked, which forced mice to explore the opposite arm where the animal received a 10% sucrose water reward. After consuming the reward, animals would return to the start box for the test phase, during which both arms could be explored. In the test phase, only the previously unexplored arm is baited, so that mice have to alternate locations between sample and test phases. Mice were subjected to 10 trials (sample + test) per day, for 10 consecutive days, and calcium activity in LS was recorded on all days. Data provided in S11 Fig are from day 10 of training.
Miniscope and behavior video acquisition
Miniscopes (V3 and V4, miniscope.org) were assembled using open-source plans as described previously (miniscope.org) [116,117]. Imaging data were acquired using a CMOS imaging sensor (Aptina, MT9V032) and multiplexed through a lightweight coaxial cable. Data were acquired using a data acquisition (DAQ) box connected via a USB host controller (Cypress, CYUSB3013). Animal behavior was recorded using a webcam mounted above the environment. Calcium and behavioral data were recorded using Miniscope custom acquisition software. The DAQ simultaneously acquired behavioral and cellular imaging streams at 30 Hz as uncompressed.avi files (1,000 frames per file), and all recorded frames were timestamped in order to perform the subsequent alignment.
Histology
After completion of all experiments, animals were deeply anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine/acepromazide (100, 16, 3 mg/kg, respectively, intraperitoneal injection). Mice were then transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS (PFA). Brains were extracted and postfixed in PFA at 4°C for a minimum of 48 hours. Brains were sectioned at 50 μm using a vibratome and cryoprotected in a solution of 30% ethylene glycol, 30% glycerol, and 40% PBS until used.
Immunohistochemistry
Sections were washed 3 × 20 minutes in PGT (0.45% Gelatin and 0.25% Triton in 1× PBS) at room temperature. Next, primary antibodies (1:1,000 goat anti-GFP from Novus Biologicals (Littleton, Colorado, United States of America) or 1:10,000 rabbit anti-RFP from VWR (Rockland)) were incubated with PGT overnight at 4°C. Following 10-, 20-, and 40-minute washes in PGT, sections were incubated with secondary antibodies (1:1,000 donkey anti-goat coupled to A488 or 1:1,000 donkey anti-rabbit coupled to A555, both from Molecular Probes (Eugene, Oregon, United States)) in PGT at room temperature for 1 hour. Sections were subsequently washed for 10 and 20 minutes in PGT and 30 minutes in PBS. Sections were then mounted on glass slides and permanently coverslipped with Fluoromount that contained DAPI.
In vitro patch clamp electrophysiology
Mice were deeply anesthetized, and acute brain slices were obtained following the protective recovery method [118]. Briefly, mice were transcardially perfused with N-methyl-D-glutamine (NMDG-based) solution containing (in mM) 93 NMDG, 93 HCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 30 NaHCO3, 20 HEPES, 25 glucose, 5 sodium ascorbate, 2 thiourea, 3 sodium pyruvate, 10 MgSO4, and 0.5 CaCl2, (pH 7.4, oxygenated with carbogen). The brain was then quickly extracted, and coronal slices (300 μm) were cut using a vibrating microtome (Leica-VT1000S). Slices were incubated for 10 to 12 minutes in 32°C NMDG solution before being transferred to a holding chamber filled with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF), kept at room temperature and containing (in mM) 124 NaCl, 24 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 5 HEPES, 12.5 glucose, 2 MgSO4, and 2 CaCl2, (pH 7.4, oxygenated with carbogen). For electrophysiology, slices were transferred to a submerged recording chamber perfused with aCSF (3 ml/min flow rate, 30°C). Patch-clamp recordings were obtained from LS GABAergic (VGAT-positive) neurons expressing the fluorescent marker GFP and visualized using a 40× water immersion objective on an upright Olympus microscope. Recordings were performed and analyzed using an Axon Multiclamp 700B amplifier and the Clampfit10 software (Molecular Devices, San Jose, California, United States of America). The intrapipette solution contained (in mM) 126 K-gluconate, 4 KCl, 10 HEPES, 4 Mg2ATP, 0.3 Na2GTP, and 10 PO-Creatine, adjusted to pH 7.25 with KOH (272 mosm). Pipette resistance was 4 to 6 MΩ.
Calcium imaging analysis
Calcium imaging videos were analyzed using the Miniscope Analysis pipeline (https://github.com/etterguillaume/MiniscopeAnalysis) as described previously [47]. Nonrigid motion correction was applied using NoRMCorre [119], and videos were spatially downsampled (3×) before concatenation. Calcium traces were extracted using CNMFe [120] using the following parameters: gSig = 3 pixels (width of Gaussian kernel), gSiz = 20 pixels (approximate neuron diameter), background_model = “ring,” spatial_algorithm = “hals,” min_corr = 0.8 (minimum pixel correlation threshold), min_PNR = 8 (minimum peak-to-noise ratio threshold). After extraction, cells and traces were visually inspected, and cells with low signal to noise ratio were removed. Raw calcium traces were filtered to remove high-frequency fluctuations and binarized (normalized amplitude > 2 SD and the first-order derivative > 0) [47].
Mouse position and head orientation was tracked using a DeepLabCut [121,122] model trained on several mouse pose markers including head centroid (for position) and nose tip (used to compute head direction angle; see below). Velocity was extracted by dividing Δd/Δt where d is the distance and t is time and subsequently smoothed using a Gaussian filter with sigma = 33 ms to remove movement artifacts. Acceleration was computed by differentiating (Matlab function diff) velocity. Head direction was computed as the angle between the vertical axis and the line formed by the head centroid and nose tip. Location data were interpolated to calcium imaging sampling frequency using linear interpolation. Head directions were interpolated using the same method, but direction data were first unwrapped, interpolated, and converted back into radians.
Activity and bursting index
Following the binarization of raw calcium traces, we compute the probability of a neuron to be active P(A) using the following formula:
The bursting index was computed as the probability of a cell being active, given it was already in an active state, or P(Active, t | Active, t-1). The activity index is computed as the probability of a cell becoming active when it is in an inactive state, or P(Active, t | Inactive, t-1).
Spatial modulation
Before processing, input variables were binned (Position in open field: 17 × 17 bins of 3 × 3 cm; Position in linear track: 34 bins of 3 cm; velocity, min = 2.5 cm/s, max = 30 cm/s, 20 bins; acceleration: min = −2 cm/s2, max = +2 cm/s2, 20 bins; head direction: 40 bins of 9°). Velocity was smoothed over 30 frames to reduce discontinuities. For head direction and positional information, all frames where velocity was <5 cm/s were excluded. The probability of being active P(A) informs on the activity rate of a neuron. Next, we compute the probability of spending time in a given bin i (spatial, velocity, acceleration, or head direction bin).
Next, we compute the joint probability:
And the probability that a cell is active given the animal is in a bin:
After plotting the binarized activity in space, we perform n = 1,000 random circular permutations. Circular permutations are used to remove the temporal relationship between neuronal activity and behavior, but it preserves the temporal structure of calcium transients and therefore leads to more conservative results (as opposed to complete randomization of every data point, which inflates the significance value of results). Because shuffled surrogates were not systematically normally distributed, we used a nonparametric approach where the p-value corresponds to the number of data points from the shuffled distribution that are greater than the actual data for each bin, divided by the number of permutations [123]. A threshold of p < 0.01 is used to determine the significant data points that make up a significant place field. In our case, for a cell to be considered spatially modulated, at least one of the spatial bins must contain activity that is significantly nonrandom. To compute within-session stability, the halfway point in the recording was determined by taking the timestamps where half of the total number of binarized events for the session was reached. Tuning maps were computed for both the first and second half of the session using the methodology described above. A Gaussian smoothing filter (σ = 3) was used to smooth the tuning maps before computing a correlation between the two. A within-session correlation value of >0.5 was considered a stable field.
Calculating mutual information content
MI is used to describe the amount of information about one variable (spatial location, head direction, velocity, or acceleration) through the observation of neuronal activity and was calculated using the following formula:
where M is the total number of possible behavioral states, P(Si∩Aj) is the joint probability of the animal being in bin i concurrently with activity level j. As we are using binarized activity, j can only be active or inactive. MI was calculated using 30 bootstrapped surrogates using 50 percent of the data (randomly chosen data points) with replacement, to allow for computation of mean MI and SEM. Next, the trace was circularly shuffled in order to calculate a shuffled MI and SEM. Cells were then sorted along with the magnitude of the difference between mean MI and mean shuffled MI, and a two-way ANOVA with a p < 0.01 threshold was used to determine whether the difference between the 2 numbers was significant in order for the cell to be deemed significantly spatially modulated. In case the assumptions for parametric ANOVA are not met (normal distribution, variance homogeneity), the significance value was computed by taking the sum of the shuffled MI values of greater magnitude than actual MI values, divided by the number of shuffles.
Bayesian decoding
A Bayesian decoder was used to evaluate how well LS neural activity estimated the animal’s location as compared to neural activity recorded from dorsal CA1 and CA3. We used decoding methods specifically adapted for calcium imaging data with binarized calcium transients [47]. Using only epochs with velocity >5 cm/s, a training dataset was generated using 50% of the data. The remaining 50% of the data was used for testing. Decoding efficiency (as measured by a decoding error and a decoding agreement score as outlined below) was calculated using 30 bootstrapped surrogates using randomly chosen data points with replacement, to allow for computation of mean MI and SEM. In order to allow for fair comparison between recordings with different numbers of cells recorded, this approach was done using either 40, 60, 80, or 100 randomly chosen cells with replacement for each bootstrapped sample. The posterior probability density function, which is the probability that an animal is in a particular bin Si, given the neural activity A, is calculated using the following equation:
P(S|A) is the posterior probability distribution of states given neuronal activity. No prior assumption is made about the location of the mouse on the linear track; P(S) is kept uniform to attribute equal probability for each location. Every neuron is assumed to be independent of each other. To construct the tuning curves from multiple neurons, we use the following equation:
With P(S|A) a vector of a posteriori behavioral states and N corresponding to the number of neurons used. For every neuron k, the tuning curves are constructed, and corresponding posterior location probability can be derived.
To reconstruct the position of the mouse, we will consider the location associated with the maximum a posteriori:
With the estimated state among all possible states S. We used a temporal filtering window in the open field of 1.5 seconds and in the linear track of 0.5 seconds to remove erratic jumps in the decoded position of the mouse. Decoding accuracy was measured as (1) decoding error and (2) decoding agreement. In 1D space, decoding error was assessed as
In 2D space, decoding error was assessed as
Decoding score was computed as
Decoding agreement was defined as the portion of time where the exact location or head direction bin was successfully decoded:
Tracking cells across sessions
Neurons were tracked over multiple days using a probabilistic method as previously described [124]. Briefly, spatial footprints were aligned using nonrigid alignment (transformation smoothness = 2) to correct for brain tissue displacements or miniscope placement. After alignment, we considered candidate sets of cells to be the same neuron if their maximal distance was <12 μm, and the spatial correlation between sets of cells was >0.65. Next, we visually identified candidate cell pairs across sessions and manually deleted those cell pairs that were erroneously selected (<1% of cases). We assessed the stability of the spatial representation in LS using simple field correlation, which was assessed by correlating smoothed place fields of cell pairs. To generate the null hypothesis for place fields’ displacements between pairs of days, we used the place fields tuning maps but shuffled cell identities over days (30 shuffles).
Materials availability
This study did not generate new unique reagents.
Statistics
Statistical analyses were performed using Matlab (MathWorks) and GraphPad Prism version 6.00 (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, California, USA). All data are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM), and statistical test details are described in the corresponding results. All t tests are two-tailed. Normality distribution of each group was assessed using the Shapiro–Wilk normality test, and parametric tests were used only when distributions were found normal, using Student t tests, one-way ANOVA, or two-way ANOVA. For nonnormal distributions, nonparametric tests Mann–Whitney or Kruskal–Wallis tests were performed and described where applicable. Significant main effects or interactions were followed up with appropriate post hoc testing using Bonferroni corrections were applicable. p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant, or p < 0.01 and described where applicable *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01; ***, p < 0.001, ****, p < 0.0001.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
We thank Fernanda Sosa for help with behavioral recording experiments, training of the animals, immunohistological experiments, and quantifications; Alexandra T. Keinath for valuable comments on a previous version of the manuscript; and Ke Cui for help with colony maintenance and mice perfusions. We also thank Daniel Aharoni (UCLA) for guidance in using the UCLA miniscope and Bruno Rivard for help building the miniscopes. The present study used the services of the Molecular and Cellular Microscopy Platform at the DHRC.
Abbreviations
- GRIN
gradient refractive index
- LH
lateral hypothalamus
- LS
lateral septum
- MI
mutual information
- MS
medial septum
- VTA
ventral tegmental area
Data Availability
All relevant quantitative data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files. Code for cell extraction is available at https://github.com/etterguillaume/MiniscopeAnalysis. Code for behavior extraction is available at https://github.com/etterguillaume/PIMPN. Code for extraction of tuning curves and decoding can be downloaded at the following address: https://github.com/etterguillaume/CaImDecoding. All other code is available at https://github.com/suzannevdveldt/spatialcodingLS.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by funding from the Canadian Institutes for Health Research (CIHR) Foundation Program FDN-148478, the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Discovery Grant RGPIN-2020-06717, and a Tier 1 Canada Research Chair to SW. SVDV is supported by a Vanier Canada Graduate Scholarship and the Richard H. Tomlinson Doctoral Fellowship. C-AM is supported by a FRQS postdoctoral fellowship. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Zhao C, Eisinger B, Gammie S. Characterization of GABAergic neurons in the mouse lateral septum: a double fluorescence in situ hybridization and immunohistochemical study using tyramide signal amplification. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:1–24. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lin W, McKinney K, Liu L, Lakhlani S, Jennes L. Distribution of vesicular glutamate transporter-2 messenger ribonucleic acid and protein in the septum-hypothalamus of the rat. Endocrinology. 2003;144:662–70. doi: 10.1210/en.2002-220908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Risold PY, Swanson LW. Structural evidence for functional domains in the rat hippocampus. Science. 1996;272:1484–6. doi: 10.1126/science.272.5267.1484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Tingley D, Buzsáki G. Transformation of a spatial map across the hippocampal-lateral septal circuit. Neuron. 2018;98: 1229–1242.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Swanson LW, Cowan WM. The connections of the septal region in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1979;186:621–56. doi: 10.1002/cne.901860408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Brito GNO, Thomas GJ. T-maze alternation, response patterning, and septo-hippocampal circuitry in rats. Behav Brain Res. 1981;3:319–40. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(81)90003-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Rawlins JNP, Olton DS. The septo-hippocampal system and cognitive mapping. Behav Brain Res. 1982;5:331–58. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(82)90039-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Stanton ME, Thomas GJ, Brito GNO. Posterodorsal septal lesions impair performance on both shift and stay working memory tasks. Behav Neurosci. 1984;98:405–15. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.98.3.405 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Thomas GJ. Comparison of effects of small lesions in posterodorsal septum on spontaneous and rerun correction (contingently reinforced) alternation in rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1979;93:685–94. doi: 10.1037/h0077608 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Thomas GJ, Brito GN. Recovery of delayed alternation in rats after lesions in medial frontal cortex and septum. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1980;94:808–18. doi: 10.1037/h0077834 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Calderwood MT, Tseng A, Glenn SB. Lateral septum mu opioid receptors in stimulation of feeding. Brain Res. 1734;2020:146648. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2020.146648 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Carus-Cadavieco M, Gorbati M, Ye L, Bender F, van der Veldt S, Kosse C, et al. Gamma oscillations organize top-down signalling to hypothalamus and enable food seeking. Nature. 2017;542:232–6. doi: 10.1038/nature21066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sweeney P, Yang Y. An inhibitory septum to lateral hypothalamus circuit that suppresses feeding. J Neurosci. 2016;36:11185–95. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2042-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Xu Y, Lu Y, Cassidy RM, Mangieri LR, Zhu C, Huang X, et al. Identification of a neurocircuit underlying regulation of feeding by stress-related emotional responses. Nat Commun. 2019;10. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11399-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Anthony TE, Dee N, Bernard A, Lerchner W, Heintz N, Anderson DJ. Control of stress-induced persistent anxiety by an extra-amygdala septohypothalamic circuit. Cell. 2014;156:522–36. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.12.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Besnard A, Gao Y, Kim MT, Twarkowski H, Reed AK, Langberg T, et al. Dorsolateral septum somatostatin interneurons gate mobility to calibrate context-specific behavioral responses. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22:436–46. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0330-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sparks P, Ledoux J. The septal complex as seen through the context of fear. In: the behavioral neuroscience of the septal region. Springer-Verlag; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Trent NL, Menard JL. The ventral hippocampus and the lateral septum work in tandem to regulate rats’ open-arm exploration in the elevated plus-maze. Physiol Behav. 2010;101:141–52. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.04.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Bender F, Gorbati M, Cadavieco MC, Denisova N, Gao X, Holman C, et al. Theta oscillations regulate the speed of locomotion via a hippocampus to lateral septum pathway. Nat Commun. 2015;6:8521. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9521 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wirtshafter HS, Wilson MA. Locomotor and hippocampal processing converge in the lateral septum. Curr Biol. 2019;29: 3177–3192.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bredewold R, Schiavo JK, van der Hart M, Verreij M, Veenema AH. Dynamic changes in extracellular release of GABA and glutamate in the lateral septum during social play behavior in juvenile rats: Implications for sex-specific regulation of social play behavior. Neuroscience. 2015;307:117–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chee S-SA, Menard JL, Dringenberg HC. The lateral septum as a regulator of hippocampal theta oscillations and defensive behavior in rats. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113:1831–41. doi: 10.1152/jn.00806.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Clemens AM, Wang H, Brecht M. The lateral septum mediates kinship behavior in the rat. Nat Commun. 2020;11:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13993-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Leroy F, Park J, Asok A, Brann DH, Meira T, Boyle LM, et al. A circuit from hippocampal CA2 to lateral septum disinhibits social aggression. Nature. 2018;564:213–8. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0772-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Risold PY, Swanson LW. Connections of the rat lateral septal complex. Brain Res Rev. 1997;24:115–95. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00009-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Swanson LW, Cowan WM. An autoradiographic study of the organization of the efferent connections of the hippocampal formation in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1977;172:49–84. doi: 10.1002/cne.901720104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kita T, Nishijo H, Eifuku S, Terasawa K, Ono T. Place and contingency differential responses of monkey septal neurons during conditional place-object discrimination. J Neurosci. 1995;15:1683–703. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-03-01683.1995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Leutgeb S, Mizumori S. Context-specific spatial representations by lateral septal cells. Neuroscience. 2002;112:655–63. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00101-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Nishijo H, Kita T, Tamura R, Uwano T, Terasawa K. Septal neuronal responses related to spatial representation in monkeys. Hippocampus. 1997;464:460–4. doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Takamura Y, Tamura R, Zhou TL, Kobayashi T, Tran AH, Eifuku S, et al. Spatial firing properties of lateral septal neurons. Hippocampus. 2006;16:635–44. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20196 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zhou TL, Tamura R. Kuriwaki J, Ono T. Comparison of medial and lateral septal neuron activity during performance of spatial tasks in rats. 1999;234:220–34. doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wirtshafter HS, Wilson MA. Differences in reward biased spatial representations in the lateral septum and hippocampus. elife. 2020;9:1–20. doi: 10.7554/eLife.55252 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Risold PY, Swanson LW. Chemoarchitecture of the rat lateral septal nucleus. Brain Res Rev. 1997;24:91–113. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00008-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gonzalez WG, Zhang H, Harutyunyan A, Lois C. Persistence of neuronal representations through time and damage in the hippocampus. Science. 2019;365:821–5. doi: 10.1126/science.aav9199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Howard MW, Kahana MJ. A distributed representation of temporal context. J Math Psychol. 2002;46:269–99. doi: 10.1006/jmps.2001.1388 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Mankin EA, Diehl GW, Sparks FT, Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK. Hippocampal CA2 activity patterns change over time to a larger extent than between spatial contexts. Neuron. 2015;85:190–202. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rubin A, Geva N, Sheintuch L, Ziv Y. Hippocampal ensemble dynamics timestamp events in long-term memory. elife. 2015;4:1–16. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Ziv Y, Burns LD, Cocker ED, Hamel EO, Ghosh KK, Kitch LJ, et al. Long-term dynamics of CA1 hippocampal place codes. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:264–6. doi: 10.1038/nn.3329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Luo AH, Tahsili-Fahadan P, Wise RA, Lupica CR, Aston-Jones G. Linking context with reward: a functional circuit from hippocampal CA3 to ventral tegmental area. Science. 2011;333:353–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1204622 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.McGlinchey EM, Aston-Jones G. Dorsal hippocampus drives context-induced cocaine seeking via inputs to lateral septum. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43:987–1000. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Butler CW, Wilson YM, Gunnersen JM, Murphy M. Tracking the fear memory engram: Discrete populations of neurons within amygdala, hypothalamus, and lateral septum are specifically activated by auditory fear conditioning. Learn Mem. 2015;22:370–84. doi: 10.1101/lm.037663.114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Garcia R, Vouimba RM, Jaffard R. Contextual conditioned fear blocks the induction but not the maintenance of lateral septal LTP in behaving mice. J Neurophysiol. 1997;78:76–81. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.1.76 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Reis DG, Scopinho AA, Guimarães FS, Corrêa F, Resstel LBM. Involvement of the lateral septal area in the expression of fear conditioning to context. Learn Mem. 2010;17:134–8. doi: 10.1101/lm.1534710 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Vetere G, Kenney JW, Tran LM, Xia F, Steadman PE, Parkinson J, et al. Chemogenetic interrogation of a brain-wide fear memory network in mice. Neuron. 2017;94:363–374.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Leutgeb S, Mizumori S. Excitotoxic septal lesions result in spatial memory deficits and altered flexibility of hippocampal single-unit representations. J Neurosci. 1999;19:6661–72. Available. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10414995. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-15-06661.1999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.M’Harzi M, Jarrard LE. Effects of medial and lateral septal lesions on acquisition of a place and cue radial maze task. Behav Brain Res. 1992;49:159–65. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(05)80160-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Etter G, Manseau F, Williams S. A probabilistic framework for decoding behavior from in vivo calcium imaging data. Front Neural Circuits. 2020;14:1–16. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2020.00001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Shannon CE. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst Tech J. 1948;27:379–423. doi: 10.1145/584091.584093 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Gothard KM. An information-theoretic approach to deciphering the hippocampal code. In: Hanson SJ, Giles CL, Cowan JD, editors. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems; 5; 1993. p. 1030–1037. doi: 10.1109/EUMC.2007.4405137 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Souza BC, Pavão R, Belchior H, Tort A. On information metrics for spatial coding. Neuroscience. 2018;375:62–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.01.066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Gallagher JP, Zheng F, Hasuo H, Shinnick-Gallagher P. Activities of neurons within the rat dorsolateral septal nucleus (DLSN). Prog Neurobiol. 1995;45:373–95. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(95)98600-a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Markus EJ, Qin YL, Leonard B, Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA. Interactions between location and task affect the spatial and directional firing of hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci. 1995;15:7079–94. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-11-07079.1995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, O’Keefe J. The contributions of position, direction and velocity to single unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats. Exp Brain Res. 1983;52. doi: 10.1007/BF00237147 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Navratilova Z, Hoang LT, Daniela Schwindel C, Tatsuno M, McNaughton BL. Experience-dependent firing rate remapping generates directional selectivity in hippocampal place cells. Front Neural Circuits 2012;6:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Mau W, Sullivan DW, Kinsky NR, Hasselmo ME, Howard MW, Eichenbaum H. The same hippocampal CA1 population simultaneously codes temporal information over multiple timescales. Curr Biol. 2018;28:1499–1508.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.03.051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Pedemonte M, Barrenechea C, Nuñez A, Gambini JP, García-Austt E. Membrane and circuit properties of lateral septum neurons: Relationships with hippocampal rhythms. Brain Res. 1998;800:145–53. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)00517-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Twery MJ, Gallagher JP. Somatostatin depresses GABA receptor-mediated inhibition in the rat dorsolateral septal nucleus. Brain Res. 1990;519:277–86. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90089-t [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Twery MJ, Phelan KD, Gallagher JP. Spontaneous bursting and non-bursting activity in morphologically identified neurons of the rat dorsolateral septal nucleus, in vitro. Neuroscience. 1992;46:669–79. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(92)90153-s [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Acharya L, Aghajan ZM, Vuong C, Moore JJ, Mehta MR. Causal influence of visual cues on hippocampal directional selectivity. Cell. 2016;164:197–207. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.12.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Leutgeb S, Ragozzino KE, Mizumori S. Convergence of head direction and place information in the CA1 region of hippocampus. Neuroscience. 2000;100:11–9. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00258-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Muller RU, Bostock E, Taube JS, Kubie J. On the directional firing properties of hippocampal place cells. J Neurosci. 1994;14:7235–51. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-12-07235.1994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Czurko A, Hirase H, Csicsvari J, Buzsáki G. Sustained activation of hippocampal pyramidal cells by “space clamping” in a running wheel. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:344–52. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00446.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Góis ZHTD, Tort ABL. Characterizing speed cells in the rat hippocampus. Cell Rep. 2018;25:1872–84. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.10.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Iwase M, Kitanishi T, Mizuseki K. Cell type, sub-region, and layer-specific speed representation in the hippocampal–entorhinal circuit. Sci Rep. 2020;10:1–23. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56847-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Zingg B. Chou X lin, Zhang Z gang, Mesik L, Liang F, Tao HW, et al. AAV-mediated anterograde transsynaptic tagging: mapping corticocollicular input-defined neural pathways for defense behaviors Neuron. 2017;93:33–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.11.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Leranth C, Deller T, Buzsáki G. Intraseptal connections redefined: lack of a lateral septum to medial septum path. Brain Res. 1992;583:1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(10)80004-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Staiger JF, Nürnberger F. The efferent connections of the lateral septal nucleus in the guinea pig: projections to the diencephalon and brainstem. Cell Tissue Res. 1991;264:391–413. doi: 10.1007/BF00319031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Witter MP, Daelmans HE, Jorritsma-Byham B, Staiger JF, Wouterlood FG. Restricted origin and distribution of projections from the lateral to the medial septal complex in rat and guinea pig. Neurosci Lett. 1992;148:164–168. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=1284441 doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(92)90830-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Beier KT, Steinberg EE, Deloach KE, Kremer EJ, Malenka RC, Luo L, et al. Circuit architecture of VTA dopamine neurons revealed by systematic input-output mapping. Cell. 2015;162:622–34. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.07.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Jung MW, Wiener SI, McNaughton BL. Comparison of spatial firing characteristics of units in dorsal and ventral hippocampus of the rat. J Neurosci. 1994;14:7347–56. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-12-07347.1994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Kjelstrup KB, Solstad T, Brun VH, Hafting T, Leutgeb S, Witter MP, et al. Finite scale of spatial representation in the hippocampus. Science. 2008;321:140–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1157086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Maurer AP, Cowen SL, Burke SN, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Phase precession in hippocampal interneurons showing strong functional coupling to individual pyramidal cells. J Neurosci. 2006;26:13485–92. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2882-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Taube J, Muller R, Ranck J. Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis. J Neurosci. 1990;10:420–35. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-02-00420.1990 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Taube JS, Muller RU, Ranck JB. Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations. J Neurosci. 1990;10:436–47. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-02-00436.1990 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Taube JS. Head direction cells recorded in the anterior thalamic nuclei of freely moving rats. J Neurosci. 1995;15:70–86. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-01-00070.1995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Stackman RW, Taube JS. Firing properties of rat lateral mammillary single units: Head direction, head pitch, and angular head velocity. J Neurosci. 1998;18:9020–37. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-21-09020.1998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Sargolini F, Fyhn M, Hafting T, McNaughton BL, Witter MP, Moser MB, et al. Conjunctive representation of position, direction, and velocity in entorhinal cortex. Science. 2006;312:758–62. doi: 10.1126/science.1125572 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Chen LL, Lin LH, Green EJ, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Head-direction cells in the rat posterior cortex. I. Anatomical distribution and behavioral modulation. Exp Brain Res. 1994;101:8–23. doi: 10.1007/BF00243212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Sharp PE, Blair HT, Cho J. The anatomical and computational basis of the rat head-direction cell signal. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:289–94. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01797-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Boccara CN, Sargolini F, Thoresen VH, Solstad T, Witter MP, Moser EI, et al. Grid cells in pre-and parasubiculum. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:987–94. doi: 10.1038/nn.2602 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Cacucci F, Lever C, Wills TJ, Burgess N, O’Keefe J. Theta-modulated place-by-direction cells in the hippocampal formation in the rat. J Neurosci. 2004;24:8265–77. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2635-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Oh SW, Harris JA, Ng L, Winslow B, Cain N, Mihalas S, et al. A mesoscale connectome of the mouse brain. Nature. 2014;508:207–14. doi: 10.1038/nature13186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Eichenbaum H. Time cells in the hippocampus: A new dimension for mapping memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15:732–44. doi: 10.1038/nrn3827 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Pastalkova E, Itskov V, Amarasingham A, Buzsáki G. Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus. Science. 2008;321:1322–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1159775 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Deshmukh SS, Bhalla US. Representation of odor habituation and timing in the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2003;23:1903–15. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-05-01903.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.MacDonald CJ, Carrow S, Place R, Eichenbaum H. Distinct hippocampal time cell sequences represent odor memories in immobilized rats. J Neurosci. 2013;33:14607–16. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1537-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Aronov D, Nevers R, Tank DW. Mapping of a non-spatial dimension by the hippocampal-entorhinal circuit. Nature. 2017;543:719–22. doi: 10.1038/nature21692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Alonso JR, Frotscher M. Organization of the septal region in the rat brain: a Golgi EM study of lateral septal neurons. J Comp Neurol. 1989;286:472–87. doi: 10.1002/cne.902860406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Staiger JF, Nürnberger F. Pattern of afferents to the lateral septum in the guinea pig. Cell Tissue Res. 1989;257:471–90. doi: 10.1007/BF00221457 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Phelan KD, Hasuo H, Twery MJ, Gallagher JP. Projection neurons in the rat dorsolateral septal nucleus possess recurrent axon collaterals. Neurosci Lett. 1989;97:259–65. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(89)90607-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Isely G, Hillar CJ, Sommer FT. Deciphering subsampled data: Adaptive compressive sampling as a principle of brain communication.Adv Neural Inf Process Syst 23 24th Annu Conf Neural Inf Process Syst 2010. NIPS. 2010;2010:1–9. [Google Scholar]
- 92.Palm G, Sommer FT. Associative data storage and retrieval in neural networks. In: Domany E, van Hemmen JL, Schulten K, editors. Models of Neural Networks III. Physics of Neural Networks. New York, NY: Springer; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 93.Staiger JF, Nürnberger F. The efferent connections of the lateral septal nucleus in the guinea pig: intrinsic connectivity of the septum and projections to other telencephalic areas. Cell Tissue Res. 1991;264:415–26. doi: 10.1007/BF00319032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Tóth K, Borhegyi Z, Freund TF. Postsynaptic targets of GABAergic hippocampal neurons in the medial septum-diagonal band of broca complex. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3712–24. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-09-03712.1993 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Manseau F, Goutagny R, Danik M, Williams S. The hippocamposeptal pathway generates rhythmic firing of GABAergic neurons in the medial septum and diagonal bands: an investigation using a complete septohippocampal preparation in vitro. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4096–107. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0247-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Leão RN, Targino ZH, Colom LV, Fisahn A. Interconnection and synchronization of neuronal populations in the mouse medial septum/diagonal band of Broca. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113:971–80. doi: 10.1152/jn.00367.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Patel J, Schomburg EW, Bere A. Fujisawa S. Local generation and propagation of ripples along the septotemporal axis of the hippocampus. 2013;33:17029–41. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2036-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Robinson J, Manseau F, Ducharme G, Vigneault E, El Mestikawy S, Williams S. Optogenetic activation of septal glutamatergic neurons drive hippocampal theta rhythms. 2016;36:3016–23. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2141-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Buzsáki G. Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2002;33:325–40. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00586-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Fuhrmann F, Justus D, Sosulina L, Kaneko H, Beutel T, Friedrichs D, et al. Locomotion, theta oscillations, and the speed-correlated firing of hippocampal neurons are controlled by a medial septal glutamatergic circuit. Neuron. 2015;86:1253–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Justus D, Dalügge D, Bothe S, Fuhrmann F, Hannes C, Kaneko H, et al. Glutamatergic synaptic integration of locomotion speed via septoentorhinal projections. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20:16–9. doi: 10.1038/nn.4447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Stuber GD, Wise RA. Lateral hypothalamic circuits for feeding and reward. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19:198–205. doi: 10.1038/nn.4220 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Burdakov D, Peleg-Raibstein D. The hypothalamus as a primary coordinator of memory updating. Physiol Behav. 2020;223:112988. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112988 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Aitta-Aho T, Pappa E, Burdakov D, Apergis-Schoute J. Cellular activation of hypothalamic hypocretin/orexin neurons facilitates short-term spatial memory in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2016;136:183–8. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.10.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Izawa S, Chowdhury S, Miyazaki T, Mukai Y, Ono D, Inoue R, et al. REM sleep-active MCH neurons are involved in forgetting hippocampus-dependent memories. Science. 2019;365:1308–13. doi: 10.1126/science.aax9238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Kosse C, Burdakov D. Natural hypothalamic circuit dynamics underlying object memorization. Nat Commun. 2019;10:1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07882-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Petrovich GD. Lateral hypothalamus as a motivation-cognition interface in the control of feeding behavior. Front Syst Neurosci. 2018;12:1–7. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Gladfelter WE, Brobeck JR. Decreased spontaneous locomotor activity in the rat induced by hypothalamic lesions. Am J Phys. 1962;203:811–7. doi: 10.1152/ajplegacy.1962.203.5.811 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Grillner S, Wallén P, Saitoh K, Kozlov A, Robertson B. Neural bases of goal-directed locomotion in vertebrates-An overview. Brain Res Rev. 2008;57:2–12. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.06.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Kosse C, Schöne C, Bracey E, Burdakov D. Orexin-driven GAD65 network of the lateral hypothalamus sets physical activity in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017;114:4525–30. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1619700114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Sinnamon HM. Preoptic and hypothalamic neurons and the initiation of locomotion in the anesthetized rat. Prog Neurobiol. 1993;41:323–44. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(93)90003-b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Mahler SV, Moorman DE, Smith RJ, James MH, Aston-Jones G. Motivational activation: A unifying hypothesis of orexin/hypocretin function. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:1298–303. doi: 10.1038/nn.3810 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Mogenson GJ, Jones DL, Yim CY. From motivation to action: Functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Prog Neurobiol. 1980;14:69–97. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(80)90018-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Callaway EM. Transneuronal circuit tracing with neurotropic viruses. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008;18:617–23. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.03.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Wickersham IR, Finke S, Conzelmann KK, Callaway EM. Retrograde neuronal tracing with a deletion-mutant rabies virus. Nat Methods. 2007;4:47–9. doi: 10.1038/nmeth999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Cai DJ, Aharoni D, Shuman T, Shobe J, Biane J, Song W, et al. A shared neural ensemble links distinct contextual memories encoded close in time. Nature. 2016;534:115–8. doi: 10.1038/nature17955 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Ghosh KK, Burns LD, Cocker ED, Nimmerjahn A, Ziv Y, El Gamal A, et al. Miniaturized integration of a fluorescence microscope. Nat Methods. 2011;8:871–8. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Ting JT, Daigle TL, Chen Q, Feng G. Acute brain slice methods for adult and aging animals: Application of targeted patch clamp analysis and optogenetics. Methods Mol Biol. 2014;1183:221–42. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-1096-0_14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Pnevmatikakis EA, Giovannucci A. NoRMCorre: An online algorithm for piecewise rigid motion correction of calcium imaging data. J Neurosci Methods. 2017;291:83–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.07.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Zhou P, Resendez SL, Rodriguez-Romaguera J, Jimenez JC, Neufeld SQ, Stuber GD, et al. Efficient and accurate extraction of in vivo calcium signals from microendoscopic video data. 2017. http://arxiv.org/abs/1605.07266 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Mathis A, Mamidanna P, Cury KM, Abe T, Murthy VN, Mathis MW, et al. DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:1281–9. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Nath T, Mathis A, Chen AC, Patel A, Bethge M, Mathis MW. Using DeepLabCut for 3D markerless pose estimation across species and behaviors. Nat Protoc. 2019;14:2152–76. doi: 10.1038/s41596-019-0176-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Cohen MX. Analyzing neural time series data: Theory and practice. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 124.Sheintuch L, Rubin A, Brande-Eilat N, Geva N, Sadeh N, Pinchasof O, et al. Tracking the same neurons across multiple days in Ca2+ imaging data. Cell Rep. 2017;21:1102–15. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.10.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]