Abstract
School suspension is a common form of punishment in the United States that is disproportionately concentrated among racial minority and disadvantaged youth. Labeling theories imply that such stigmatized sanctions may lead to interpersonal exclusion from normative others and greater involvement with antisocial peers. I test these propositions in the context of rural schools by (1) examining the association between suspension and discontinuity in same-grade friendship ties, focusing on three mechanisms implied in labeling theories: rejection, withdrawal, and physical separation; (2) testing the association between suspension and increased involvement with antisocial peers; and (3) assessing whether these associations are stronger in smaller schools. Consistent with labeling theories, I find suspension associated with greater discontinuity in friendship ties, based on changes in the respondents’ friendship preferences and self-reports of their peers. Findings are also consistent with changes in perceptual measures of exclusion. Additionally, I find suspension associated with greater involvement with substance-using peers. Some but not all of these associations are stronger in smaller rural schools. Given the disproportionate distribution of suspension, my findings suggest an excessive reliance on this exclusionary form of punishment may foster inequality among these youth.
Keywords: labeling theory, peer networks, school suspension, social exclusion
The emphasis on crime control over much of the past half-century did more than fill our penal institutions; it also left empty desks in our classrooms. School suspension is a common response to classroom misbehavior in the United States and is heavily concentrated among racial minority and disadvantaged youth (Hirschfield, 2018a; Payne & Welch, 2010; Vanderhaar, Petrosko, & Muñoz, 2015; Welch & Payne, 2012). Excessive reliance on suspension is problematic because it excludes students from school activities and puts a mark on their academic records, potentially leading to further disengagement and lower educational attainment (Balfanz, Byrnes, & Fox, 2015; Morris & Perry, 2016; Pyne, 2019).
This weakened institutional attachment following suspension is consistent with labeling theories (Goffman, 1963; Lemert, 1951, 1967; Paternoster & Iovanni, 1989), which imply that stigmatizing sanctions can foster social exclusion, defined as the process of being pushed out of conventional society (Foster & Hagan, 2015). A focus on institutional exclusion is important, but labeling theories make clear that exclusion may also be interpersonal. I refer to interpersonal exclusion as a deterioration of relationships with normative others due to punishment. One of the relationships most relevant to students are friendships with peers they interact with most from year to year—those in their own grade. Such friendships are the medium by which children develop social skills and learn age-graded tasks. Indeed, having normative friends in one’s grade may be an early source of social capital (Coleman, 1988; Stanton-Salazar & Dornbusch, 1995), promoting outcomes like school achievement, emotional wellbeing, and behavioral adjustment (Crosnoe, 2000; Hartup & Stevens, 1997; Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). In contrast, exclusion from such friends may be accompanied by greater involvement with antisocial peers (Dishion, Patterson, Stoolmiller, & Skinner, 1991; Laird et al., 2001).
If suspension is associated with exclusion from same-grade friends, this should be more apparent in settings where it is more stigmatizing. In disadvantaged urban areas, prior research suggests juvenile sanctions are “normalized” experiences (Hirschfield, 2008; Nolan, 2011). Small-town or rural areas, on the other hand, are often characterized by factors that reinforce social norms and may increase costs of a deviant label. These include greater network density or closure, more time spent with neighbors, and a larger share of ties to family and kin (Beggs, Haines, & Hurlbert, 1996; Coleman, 1988; Fischer, 1982; Marsden & Srivastava, 2012; Osgood & Chambers, 2000; Smith, 2003). To capture such a setting, I depart from the urban focus dominating school punishment research (e.g., Mittleman, 2018; Pyne, 2019) by relying on a predominantly rural sample. Rural schools vary in size, but smaller rural schools offer suspended youth less anonymity than larger rural schools can. Therefore, I also assess the extent to which associations between suspension and friendship outcomes are greater in smaller rural schools.
In this study, I extend prior labeling research in school contexts by moving beyond a focus on weakened institutional attachment or behavior outcomes (i.e., secondary deviance; Lemert, 1951; Wolf & Kupchik, 2017) to test whether suspension in a predominantly rural sample is followed by interpersonal exclusion from same-grade friends. I use a unique dataset of self-reports on behaviors and friendship preferences from students and their peers as they move from sixth to ninth grade. First, I examine the association between suspension and discontinuity in friendship ties. In doing so, I focus on three mechanisms of interpersonal exclusion implied in labeling theories: rejection, withdrawal, and separation. Second, I test the association between suspension and increased involvement with antisocial peers. Third, I assess the extent to which these associations are stronger in smaller schools.
BACKGROUND
SCHOOL SUSPENSION AND SOCIAL EXCLUSION
Suspension is not a rare experience in the US. Each year, 2.6 million children and adolescents are temporarily removed from school due to out-of-school suspension, and 2.7 million are excluded from class due to in-school suspension (Office of Civil Rights, 2018). Reform efforts have led to recent declines in some states (Loveless, 2017), but overall rates are still high, particularly for disadvantaged and racial minority students. This is not because juvenile crime rates are high but because suspension is often in response to minor misbehavior like classroom disruptions and attendance problems (Kupchik, 2010; Morris & Perry, 2017; Skiba et al., 2014). This is problematic because a growing body of research suggests suspension may be harmful for child and adolescent development (Cuellar & Markowitz, 2015; Hemphill, Toumbourou, Herrenkohl, McMorris, & Catalano, 2006; Jacobsen, Pace, & Ramirez, 2019; Mittleman, 2018; Morris & Perry, 2016; Perry & Morris, 2014).
One way that suspension may be harmful is through social exclusion. This concept, defined as “the structural condition of being shut out from conventional society” (Foster & Hagan, 2015: 136) is generally conceptualized as weakened attachment to important social institutions following an official sanction. Prior research on this topic has often focused on institutional rejection of those with a criminal history. For example, formerly incarcerated individuals may be barred from housing, legal employment, or adequate healthcare (Geller & Curtis, 2011; Pager, 2003; Lara-Millán, 2014). They may also exclude themselves (institutional withdrawal, or “system avoidance”) by minimizing involvement with schools, hospitals, or other institutions that keep formal records, out of fear of further apprehension or having their record discovered (Brayne, 2014; Goffman, 2009; Haskins & Jacobsen, 2017; Lageson, 2016).
Suspension also involves institutional exclusion. Not only are students physically separated from their classroom or school, they are formally excluded by a mark on their school records. School personnel who become aware of a student’s suspension history may lower their expectations or increase surveillance of the student (Ferguson, 2001; Weissman, 2015). Sensing or fearing these administrative reactions, previously suspended students may lower their trust in school personnel (Pyne, 2019) or disengage from school activities. These exclusionary processes may then be perpetuated into later grades and even beyond secondary school. For example, many high schools send discipline information to colleges (Weissman & NaPier, 2015), and college applications often inquire about suspension history. This may partly explain why suspension is associated with lower likelihood of school completion and postsecondary enrollment (Balfanz et al., 2015; Noltemeyer, Ward, & Mcloughlin, 2015).
INTERPERSONAL EXCLUSION
This institution-focused conceptualization of social exclusion is important for understanding consequences of excessive crime control for social inequality; however, it leaves out another type of exclusion often implied by theorists but rarely examined empirically. Lemert (1967: 252) describes exclusion as a “process that begins with persistent interpersonal difficulties between the individual and … other persons in the community.” Whereas institutional exclusion refers to a person’s weakened bonds to institutions, I refer to interpersonal exclusion as a weakening of ties to members of an individual’s social network following punishment. For example, some research suggests suspension may strain family relationships through stress (e.g., by interrupting parent work schedules) or embarrassment (Dunning-Lozano 2018; Kupchik 2016; Mowen 2017). I extend this work by examining suspended students’ weakened or severed ties to friends in their school. Friends in school are important because they provide the context in which children and adolescents learn social and behavioral skills that are critical for healthy development. Conforming friends encourage school adjustment and achievement (Crosnoe, 2000; Hartup & Stevens, 1997). They may also transmit knowledge from parents and mentors about appropriate classroom behavior, or share information such as how to prepare for college (Coleman, 1988; Stanton-Salazar & Dornbusch, 1995). Such friendships may be particularly important in rural areas where families and schools often have fewer economic or institutional resources (Gottfredson & Gottfredson, 2001; Nelson, 2016; Roscigno, 2006). I focus on friendships among same-grade peers because the youth in one’s grade represent a consistently present audience and pool of potential friends in which to examine changes in ties from year to year. I distinguish three ways that interpersonal exclusion may occur following suspension: rejection, withdrawal, and physical separation.
Rejection
In labeling theories, rejection refers to reactions of conforming individuals toward stigmatized others. It occurs when “normals” circumvent encounters with formerly sanctioned individuals out of uneasiness or to avoid guilt by association (Goffman, 1963). It also occurs when interactions with formerly sanctioned persons become less friendly or more restrictive to protect individual or group values (Lemert, 1967). Consistent with this idea, prior research suggests suspension may be more common among students with lower status among their peers (Kupersmidt & Coie, 1990). Importantly, rejection is defined as a response to the sanctioned individuals, rather than as a reaction to their behavior (Link, Cullen, Frank, & Wozniak, 1987). It would be evident if Classmates A and B respond to Student C’s suspension by no longer considering themselves to be C’s friends because C is a “bad kid,” not because C’s misbehavior was inappropriate.
Withdrawal
The concept of withdrawal characterizes the behavior of a stigmatized individual toward others, either out of fear of rejection or to avoid uncomfortable encounters (Goffman, 1963; Link, 1987; Link, Cullen, Struening, Shrout, & Dohrenwend, 1989). People are often socialized to believe that sanctioned individuals are dangerous or dishonest (Hirschfield & Piquero, 2010; McNulty & Roseboro, 2009). In the context of suspension, children may see how other suspended students have been excluded and come to believe suspension is for troublemakers. These stereotypes likely take on personal significance when students are suspended; they may anticipate strained interactions with normative peers and withdraw as a means of defense. Withdrawal would be evident if, following a suspension, Student C no longer prefers Classmates A or B as friends because C fears they may now view C as a troublemaker.
Rejection and withdrawal from peers may be more likely if the suspension is repeated. Lemert (1951: 76) suggests repeated sanctions for minor misbehavior facilitate “ingrouping and outgrouping” between sanctioned individuals and normative others until “a stigmatizing of the deviant occurs in the form of name calling, labeling, or stereotyping.” Thus, societal reactions accumulate as the sanction is experienced multiple times, amplifying the deviant label (Sampson & Laub, 1997). Most suspensions last no more than a few days (Shollenberger, 2015) but they are often repeated, even within the same year. Indeed, about four in ten suspended students are suspended multiple times in the same year (Office of Civil Rights, 2018). Having multiple suspensions could be a stronger signal that peers should avoid a suspended student, or it may reify the student’s expectations of rejection, increasing their likelihood of withdrawal. Therefore, suspension may be more strongly associated with rejection and withdrawal for students who have experienced it multiple times. This would be similar to prior findings that suspension is more strongly associated with a subsequent arrest when it is repeated (Mowen & Brent, 2016).
Separation
Rejection and withdrawal are responses to stigma, but interpersonal exclusion may also occur due to the physical separation the sanction entails. For example, prior research finds lengthy or repeated prison or jail stays, which involve separation from partners and children, associated with weakened family ties (Massoglia, Remster, & King, 2011; Turney & Wildeman, 2013). Suspension is not jail, but it involves separating students from their peers at school by removing them from school grounds or segregating them in an alternative classroom setting. Long or reoccurring suspensions cause students to miss out on shared experiences with other students. For example, in a qualitative study of the families of suspended youth, one boy reports that the time away from school during his suspensions caused him to lose contact with school peers (Kupchik 2016:58). Suspension may therefore weaken relationships with friends at school, independent of the level of stigma. In sum, suspension should be associated with discontinuity in friendships from the previous year, and some of this may be due to rejection and withdrawal; but part should also be explained by lengthy or repeated separation from friends.
INCREASED INVOLVEMENT WITH ANTISOCIAL PEERS
The processes described thus far imply that stigmatizing forms of punishment push an individual away from normative networks and increase the attractiveness, or pull, of marginalized and delinquent networks (Bernburg, Krohn, & Rivera, 2006). This suggests suspension may be associated with less involvement with peers who promote normative development and more involvement with peers who, according to a long history of criminological research on peer influence, may facilitate delinquency (McGloin, 2009; Ragan, 2014; Sutherland, 1947). Peers who are already more involved in antisocial behavior may be more accepting of the deviant label ascribed by the suspension. These may be current antisocial friends or other suspended peers with whom the student interacts during an in-school suspension, including from other grades. They may also be youth the student spends time with during an out-of-school suspension (Quin & Hemphill, 2014), such as older youth not in school, which some research associates with deviance (Harding, 2009). These changes should be evident in the changes in behavioral composition of networks following punishment. If suspension is followed by interpersonal exclusion from normative peers and greater involvement with other suspended or delinquent students, it should be associated with an increase in the average level of antisocial behavior among friends, such as would occur if more of the suspended student’s friends were involved in antisocial behavior than before.
STRONGER ASSOCIATIONS AMONG STUDENTS IN SMALLER RURAL SCHOOLS
Suspension may not be associated with peer exclusion equally across rural youth. In particular, students who are suspended in smaller schools may be more affected by stigma. Rural schools are, on average, smaller than urban schools (Lippman, Burns, & McArthur, 1996), but within rural settings, schools vary widely in size.1 As the number of students in a school declines, the level of anonymity the school can provide also diminishes. Smaller schools offer less anonymity because more peers know each other as classmates, friends, or in other ways. A small school in a rural school district means that school peers may be relatives, members of the same religious congregation, or their parents may be coworkers (Beggs et al., 1996). Indeed, prior research finds greater connectedness in smaller relative to larger rural schools (Temkin, Gest, & Osgood, 2018; see also Allcott, Karlan, Möbius, Rosenblat, & Szeidl, 2007). More ties among peers may facilitate the spread of news of the suspension and could mean higher social costs for suspended youth in smaller schools. For example, parents may discourage their child from spending time with a peer whom they heard was suspended. In sum, suspension may be more strongly associated with peer exclusion among students in smaller rural schools. It may also be more strongly associated with involvement with antisocial peers. This is because in smaller grades, exclusion from conforming friends leaves suspended youth with even greater constraints in friendship selection, potentially amplifying involvement with antisocial peers.
ALTERNATIVE EXPLANATIONS
Weakened Institutional Attachment
It is possible that discontinuity in friendship ties following suspension is due to weakened institutional attachment instead of the mechanisms I have described. Students may be less likely to maintain ties to conforming peers (and vice versa) not because of the stigma or separation associated with the suspension itself but because of the negative effects suspension may have on school engagement and achievement (Morris & Perry, 2016; Pyne, 2019; but see Kinsler, 2013). Lower achieving students experience more peer exclusion and have fewer same-grade friends, though evidence in rural schools is mixed (Austin & Draper, 1984; Flashman, 2012). Thus, it is important for examinations of interpersonal exclusion following suspension to account for weakened institutional attachment.
Spuriousness
An association between suspension and friendship discontinuity may also be due to spuriousness, rather than to an effect of the former on the latter. Friendship ties change from year to year for many reasons such as transferring schools (Felmlee, McMillan, Rodis, & Osgood, 2018), joining a sports team (Eder & Kinney, 1995), or engaging in certain behaviors. Some behaviors like substance use are associated with popularity, or increases in friendship ties (Moody, Brynildsen, Osgood, Feinberg, & Gest, 2011), but others like delinquency and physical aggression are associated with fewer friends (Dodge, 1983; Rulison, Kreager, & Osgood, 2014) and also increase the risk of suspension. This implies that any discontinuity in friendship ties following suspension may not be due to suspension itself but to characteristics correlated with suspension. Indeed, students who are at risk of suspension already feel less connected or accepted by school peers (Pyne, 2019). They are also more disadvantaged and may have difficulty maintaining ties due to issues like residential mobility (South, Haynie, & Bose, 2007), longer distances to school (particularly in rural communities; Fox, 1996), or other stigmas already present. For example, parental criminal justice involvement is associated with peer exclusion and suspension (Bryan, 2017; Cochran, Siennick, & Mears, 2018; Jacobsen, 2019).
Such spuriousness could also apply to an association between suspension and increased involvement with antisocial peers. Youth prefer friendships with peers who share similar characteristics, including behavior problems (McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Cook, 2001; Osgood, Feinberg, & Ragan, 2015). It may be that suspended youth become more involved with antisocial peers not because of suspension, but because their own behavior problems make them more compatible with antisocial peers or less so with conforming peers. Therefore, in examining the associations of suspension with changes in friendship preferences and friends’ behavioral composition, the ability to account for a wide range of potential confounders is critical.
LONGITUDINAL NETWORK APPROACH
Examining the exclusionary processes I have described requires the ability to identify (1) which peers consider the respondent to be a friend, separately from (2) which peers the respondent considers to be a friend. Discontinuity in the former should capture rejection (as much as it is associated with suspension), and discontinuity in the latter should capture withdrawal. Therefore, such an examination also requires that this information be longitudinal, tracking the same respondents and a consistent body of their peers over time. Most prior studies of labeling or peer exclusion have relied on respondent perceptions of exclusion or on general peer preferences, rather than changes in specific friendships over time (Dodge et al., 2003; Wiley, Slocum, & Esbensen, 2013). Some of this work has focused on suspension specifically and has been longitudinal. Pyne (2019) found that students at risk of suspension perceived lower belongingness or connectedness to peers but little evidence that suspension is associated with changes in such perceptions. I revisit this by focusing on changes in ties among actors in a network, rather than less specific perceptions. Using nomination data (“name your closest friends”), ties are based on nominations of respondents toward peers (outgoing ties) or of peers toward respondents (incoming ties). This provides a more complete picture of student networks (Young et al., 2011) and allows for operationalizing rejection and withdrawal as within-individual changes in nominations (Schaefer, Kornienko, & Fox, 2011).
STUDY CONTRIBUTIONS
This work extends prior research on the consequences of excessive crime control for social exclusion and inequality (Kirk & Wakefield, 2018) by focusing on school suspension, which many suggest is a precursor to criminal justice involvement (Hirschfield, 2018b; Kupchik 2010; Mittleman 2018; Mowen and Brent 2016; Ramey 2016). Moreover, it advances knowledge of the prevalence and outcomes of suspension in rural schools. It also joins others in shifting the focus dominating prior labeling research from diminished institutional attachment and secondary deviance to the micro-level processes of interpersonal exclusion implied in labeling theories (Bryan, 2017; Cochran et al., 2018; Mowen, 2017; Rengifo & DeWitt, 2019). Additionally, it shows how these network processes may be observed over time (e.g., Schaefer et al., 2011). This is a particularly important contribution in the context of suspension due to the limited availability of individual-level discipline information combined with longitudinal network data.
First, I examine descriptive differences in network size and other characteristics between suspended and non-suspended students. Second, I test the association between suspension and the deterioration of incoming ties (rejection) and outgoing ties (withdrawal) from one wave or grade to the next. Third, I assess the extent to which school absence (an indicator of separation from school friends) explains this association. As a sensitivity check, I compare results using alternative outcome measures based on changes in perceptions of peer exclusion. Finding analyses of perceptions consistent with the main results would be evidence of the reliability of my network approach. Fourth, I assess changes in the self-reported behavioral composition (substance use and delinquency) of sets of friends, comprised separately of incoming and outgoing ties. Finally, I test whether the associations of suspension with interpersonal exclusion and increased involvement with antisocial peers are stronger in smaller schools.
DATA AND METHODS
DATA
Predominantly Rural Sample
Data were collected as part of the test of the PROSPER partnership model, a project for delivering community-focused interventions for reducing adolescent substance use and risky behavior (Spoth et al., 2007). The project included all sixth-grade students in 28 predominantly rural public school districts, with 14 each in Iowa and Pennsylvania (about 11,000 participants at baseline, with 162 to 792 per school district). To be eligible, districts had to have between 1,300 and 5,200 students, with no less than 15% eligible for free or reduced-price lunch.2 These enrollment-specific criteria result in grades being considerably larger on average than they are in rural schools nationally, and even larger than US schools overall (Appendix A). Two successive cohorts of students participated, completing baseline surveys in school during fall of sixth grade (2002 and 2003). They then completed follow-up surveys in spring the same year and every year after to ninth grade (five waves).3 As part of this in-school survey, students were asked to list the names of their two closest friends and up to five other close friends in their same grade and school. Ninety-six percent of participating students provided nomination data for at least one wave. It total, 83% of nominations were matched successfully to class rosters, with an average of four names per student-wave. Unsuccessful matches occurred when nominations were not on the class roster (15%) or there were multiple plausible names (2%).
Suspension data were collected from additional in-home questionnaires administered to a randomly selected subset of students from the 2003 cohort and their parents. Interviewers contacted the families via mail and telephone, followed by an in-person recruitment visit. Of 2,267 families invited, 979 (43%) participated (Lippold, Greenberg, & Collins, 2013). This low participation rate resulted in an in-home subsample that has small but statistically significant differences from the larger PROSPER sample. Compared to students in the larger sample (full 2003 cohort), students in the in-home subsample were more likely to be white. They had more friends in school and were somewhat less likely to report delinquency or substance use; however, they were similar in terms of socioeconomic status (free or reduced-price lunch) and proportion male or female (Appendix A). Students as well as their mothers, and fathers when present (70% at baseline), completed written questionnaires concurrently with the five waves of the larger in-school survey. By ninth grade, 75% of students were still participating, with an average participation of about three waves per student (participation rates in Appendix B).
I limit my analytic sample to 2,915 in-home participant observations in the spring of grades six to nine. This excludes follow-up observations of 140 students who continued participating in the in-home survey but moved away from a PROSPER school district, leaving no basis for assessing rejection and withdrawal. I do not include observations from the fall of sixth grade (baseline) as cases in the analyses because my focus is on discontinuity in friendship ties from the prior to the current grade. However, data from fall of sixth grade contribute because control variables are lagged to the previous wave to establish the appropriate temporal ordering with key variables. Of these 2,915 observations I exclude 378 in which the student did not complete the in-school survey due to refusal, incompletion, or absence (about the same number for each reason). I also drop 164 cases due to nonresponse in the suspension items, resulting in an analytic sample of N=2,373 observations from 766 students. At baseline, there are no notable differences between students in my analytic sample and those of the in-home subsample from which they are drawn, but there are disproportionately fewer racial minorities compared to the full PROSPER 2003 cohort (12% vs. 16%) and to the entire US population of rural, regular public-school sixth-graders in the same year (21%; Appendix A). Forty-one percent of the racial minorities in my sample are Hispanic and 17% are non-Hispanic black. Therefore, results of my analyses may not generalize to all racial minority youth in these school districts.
Variables
Outcome Variables.
Friends are defined by respondent nominations of peers (maximum of seven) or peer nominations of the respondent (could be nominated by anyone in grade). I examine disparities in the number of friendship nominations made and received, but my main outcome of interest is a within-individual discontinuity in or loss of nominations from one grade to the next. I focus on same-grade peers because they provide a consistent pool of potential friends as students advance through middle school and into high school. They are also the most relevant audience likely to learn of and respond to suspension. Peers beyond this pool should be considered as well but are not as easily incorporated in a full network analysis. To provide some perspective beyond same-grade peers, in descriptive analyses I also consider the number of close friends the respondent reports to have in other grades and schools.
Discontinuity in friendship nominations is represented in two ways. Withdrawal represents the number of friendship nominations the respondent made in the previous wave but did not repeat in the current wave. Rejection represents the number of friendship nominations the respondent received in the previous wave but lost (did not receive) again in the current wave.4
In examining whether suspension is associated with increased involvement with antisocial peers, I focus on two behaviors of friends: substance use and delinquency. Both are based on self-reports of peers who participated in the in-school survey and were nominated by the respondent, or nominated the respondent as a friend. Substance use is based on four items about the frequency of smoking, drinking alcohol, getting drunk, and using marijuana in the past month (alpha=0.76). Delinquency is based on responses to 12 items about the frequency of a range of behaviors in the past year (e.g., theft, fighting, vandalism; alpha=0.82). A complete list of items for each measure is provided in Appendix C. To address issues with skewness in combining items, each measure is constructed using the graded response model from item response theory (an extension of the two-parameter logistic model; Samejima, 1969). This transforms each into an equal-interval scale with a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1 (Osgood, McMorris, & Potenza, 2002). Principle component analyses for each measure supported the assumption in item response theory that there is a single dominant latent variable underlying the respective items. Final measures represent the mean substance use and the mean delinquency across the respondent’s friends. One version of each pertains to peers whom the respondent nominated as a friend, and the other to peers who nominated the respondent.
Explanatory Variable.
Suspension data represent student self-reports on the number of times they were “suspended from school for disciplinary reasons” in the past 12 months. To minimize underreporting, these are combined with responses from up to two participating parents (“In the past year, has this child ever been suspended from school for disciplinary reasons? How many times?”). The number of suspensions is based on the maximum number of times indicated by any of the three potential reporters. For example, if the student reports two suspensions but the mother and father each report only one, I count this as two suspensions. In my analytic sample, 155 families, or 20%, reported that the student experienced at least one suspension between fifth and ninth grade. Of these, a little more than half reported being suspended more than once, and one-third reported being suspended at least three times. Eight percent were suspended ten or more times, or an average of more than twice per grade. For the multivariable analyses, I collapse these suspension counts into a set of three time-varying dummy variables. The first represents the reference category and includes students who were never suspended between the fifth grade and a given subsequent grade. The second refers to students who were suspended once, and the third to students who were suspended more than once. These are time-varying because students in each category at a given wave could report a new suspension at a subsequent wave. This way, the measure is indicative of a label carried across years—one that should become more salient the more times a student is suspended.
This measure is limited because I do not have data on whether the student was suspended prior to the first wave, meaning my analyses assume suspensions in fifth-grade are first-time suspensions. However, this appears to be a safe assumption because few students or parents (4%) reported that the student was suspended in fifth grade, and by sixth grade, the prevalence of suspension (8%) is consistent with cumulative risk between first and sixth grade for rural students nationally (details in results section). The measure is also limited because it does not allow for assessing other differences in suspension (e.g., in-school or out-of-school, duration, etc.) or other forms of exclusionary discipline such as expulsion. Some forms may have more or less impact than others, but unfortunately few large-scale datasets distinguish between types.
School Absence.
Absence, a measure of separation, is based on in-school self-reports on the number of days the student was absent in the past year (1=none, 2=1 to 2 days, 3=3 to 6 days, 4=7 to 15 days, and 5=16 or more days). Data on the length of suspensions or reasons for missing school are not available. I combine the last two categories (only 5% missed 16 or more days) and construct a set of dummy variables with the reference group representing those who never missed 7 or more days in a year by a given wave or any previous wave. The second group includes students who ever missed 7 or more days (not necessarily consecutively), and the third group refers to students who missed this amount more than once since sixth grade. In analyses not shown, I compare results using measures based on other cutoffs (3 or more, 16 or more), but the 7-day cutoff explains a larger portion of the association between suspension and friendship tie discontinuity than the others do. Among students who missed 7 days in a single year, 42% did this in more than one wave. These may be at greater risk of weakened ties among school peers.
Weakened Institutional Attachment.
Two indicators of weakened institutional attachment include low school attachment and low academic achievement. Low school attachment represents the average of five in-home survey items ranging from 1=not at all true to 5=really true. Examples include “I wish I could move to another school,” “I like being in my school” (reverse coded), and “I wish I could stay home from school” (alpha=0.83). For increased reliability, low achievement is based on the combined reports of the student and participating parents about the grades the student usually gets in school, ranging from 1=mostly A’s to 9=mostly F’s. The final measure represents the average across the standardized (z-score) version of each of the individual reports (alpha=0.91).
School Size and Other Control Variables.
As the most relevant aspect of school size, I use the number of students per grade as a control variable and in examining whether associations with friendship discontinuity are stronger in smaller schools. I also address concerns with selection on observed characteristics by including a rich set of controls from both the in-school and in-home surveys (full list in Table 1). Among these are behavioral antecedents to suspension, each measured prior to the current wave (one-wave lags). These include school misbehavior (disrupting class, talking back to teachers, breaking rules, etc.), delinquency, substance use, and risk and sensation seeking. Controlling for antisocial behaviors eases concerns about reverse causality because labeling theories suggest the effects of peer exclusion on suspension (the reverse relationship) would operate through increases in antisocial behavior. Other controls include indicators of economic disadvantage, residential mobility, parenting behaviors, after-school activities, and other reasons for friendship changes such as when friends drop out, move away, or choose not to participate in the survey (coding described in Appendix D). I also control for current wave (grade) to account for trends in behavior and friendship selection. All but four variables are missing less than 10% of observations. These four include parent self-reports of any prior arrest, asked only at ninth grade (21%), the racial composition of friendship nominations made (15%), special education services (11%), and risk and sensation seeking (11%). I use multiple imputation with chained equations to address missing data (analyses rely on 20 imputed datasets) and to preserve a consistent sample size when comparing across nested models. Supplemental analyses not shown used listwise deletion instead of imputation; with this approach, multivariable regression coefficients were very similar in terms of size and direction but models relied on smaller portions of the sample.
Table 1.
Variable | Suspended during Study |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Never Suspended during Study | Suspended by Sixth Grade | Suspended after Sixth Grade | Full Sample at Sixth Grade | ||||||
| |||||||||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | ||
| |||||||||
Friendship Nominations Lost Since Last Wave | |||||||||
Number of nominations made last wave (0 to 7) | 3.69 | * | 2.57 | 3.44 | 2.62 | 2.83 | 2.41 | 3.58 | 2.57 |
Mean proportion lost since last wavea (0 to 1) | .33 | * | .27 | .46 | .28 | .36 | .33 | .34 | .28 |
Number of nominations received last wave (0 to 16) | 3.47 | *** | 2.72 | 2.62 | 2.40 | 2.42 | 2.65 | 3.29 | 2.71 |
Mean proportion lost since last waveb (0 to 1) | .36 | ** | .32 | .60 | .34 | .38 | .37 | .38 | .33 |
School Absence (ref: Never Missed 7+ days) | |||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school in a year since fall of 6th grade | .19 | .40 | .20 | .40 | .22 | .42 | .20 | .40 | |
Missed 7+ days of school in a year more than once since fall of 6th grade | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | |
Weakened Institutional Attachment | |||||||||
Low school attachment current wave (1 to 5) | 2.11 | ** | .85 | 2.50 | .83 | 2.31 | .97 | 2.16 | .87 |
Low academic achievement current wave (z-score) | −.22 | *** | .81 | .35 | .91 | .19 | .95 | −.13 | .86 |
Control Variables | |||||||||
Male (ref: Female) | .43 | *** | .50 | .76 | .43 | .57 | .50 | .47 | .50 |
Nonwhite (ref: Non-Hispanic white) | .10 | ** | .30 | .20 | .40 | .21 | .41 | .12 | .33 |
Nominations made last wave no longer in school/study (log) | .17 | .34 | .26 | .41 | .20 | .38 | .18 | .35 | |
Nominations received last wave no longer in school/study (log) | .22 | * | .36 | .11 | .27 | .17 | .35 | .21 | .36 |
Racial composition of nominations made last wave all white | .69 | * | .46 | .51 | .50 | .63 | .49 | .67 | .47 |
Racial composition of nominations received last wave all white | .71 | .45 | .62 | .49 | .74 | .44 | .71 | .46 | |
Number of students in grade last wave (6 to 470) | 167.30 | 105.62 | 177.75 | 126.84 | 163.63 | 99.76 | 167.75 | 106.75 | |
Transitioned to new school last wave | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | |
Miles to school last wave (log) | 1.23 | .62 | 1.18 | .59 | 1.12 | .54 | 1.22 | .61 | |
Special education services last wave | .17 | *** | .38 | .44 | .50 | .26 | .44 | .20 | .40 |
Structured activities after school last wave (z-score) | −.09 | 1.02 | −.08 | .85 | −.29 | .78 | −.11 | .98 | |
Unstructured socializing after school last wave (z-score) | −.12 | *** | .97 | .26 | 1.10 | .22 | 1.17 | −.06 | 1.01 |
Any substance use last wave | .05 | ** | .21 | .13 | .34 | .11 | .32 | .06 | .68 |
Delinquency variety score last wave (log) | .25 | *** | .45 | .73 | .67 | .44 | .60 | .31 | .51 |
Frequency of school misbehavior last wave (log) | .27 | *** | .25 | .67 | .33 | .51 | .33 | .33 | .30 |
Risk and sensation seeking last wave (1 to 5) | 1.82 | *** | .82 | 2.29 | .95 | 2.05 | .98 | 1.88 | .86 |
Frequency of bully victimization last wave (log) | .27 | *** | .30 | .38 | .36 | .40 | .38 | .29 | .32 |
Parental discipline last wave (1 to 5) | 3.73 | *** | .89 | 3.20 | 1.12 | 3.52 | .94 | 3.66 | .92 |
Parental monitoring last wave (1 to 5) | 4.61 | *** | .51 | 4.28 | .65 | 4.48 | .51 | 4.57 | .53 |
Parent education last wave (z-score) | −.01 | *** | 1.02 | −.28 | 1.15 | −.47 | .79 | −.07 | 1.02 |
Household income last wave (log) | 10.78 | *** | .79 | 10.42 | .86 | 10.32 | .86 | 10.70 | .82 |
Parent unemployment last wave | .11 | *** | .31 | .16 | .37 | .28 | .45 | .13 | .34 |
Parent ever arrested (ninth grade only) | .14 | *** | .35 | .42 | .50 | .29 | .46 | .18 | .38 |
Mother relationship transitions last wave (0 to 8) | 1.97 | 1.45 | 2.24 | 1.71 | 2.17 | 1.39 | 2.01 | 1.46 | |
Children in household last wave (0 to 8) | 2.45 | 1.03 | 2.75 | 1.32 | 2.31 | .91 | 2.46 | 1.05 | |
Mother depression last wave | .23 | *** | .42 | .40 | .49 | .40 | .49 | .26 | .44 |
Religiosity last wave (1 to 6) | 3.98 | *** | 1.77 | 3.53 | 1.73 | 3.22 | 1.81 | 3.86 | 1.78 |
Years in current residence last wave (0 to 19) | 6.40 | *** | 4.08 | 5.16 | 3.93 | 4.93 | 3.69 | 6.15 | 4.06 |
Community cohesion last wave (z-score) | −.01 | ** | 1.03 | .17 | 1.09 | .39 | 1.04 | .05 | 1.04 |
N of Students (Unweighted) | 561 | 55 | 72 | 688 |
NOTES: PROSPER sample limited to 2,373 observations of 766 in-home survey participants meeting the following criteria: attending participating school district, participated in the in-school survey, valid data on suspension. Only sixth-grade observations are shown here (n=688). Control variable for current grade not shown. Results based on first of 20 multiply imputed datasets. Independent samples t-tests compare ever-suspended students (those in either of the middle two categories) to never-suspended students.
Of those who made nominations last wave (n=444 non-suspended, 42 suspended by sixth, 53 suspended after sixth, 539 total)
Of those who received nominations last wave (n=500 non-suspended, 44 suspended by sixth, 57 suspended after sixth, 601 total)
ABBREVIATIONS: M = mean; SD = standard deviation
p<.001
p<.01
p<.05 (two-tailed)
Analytic Strategy
After examining descriptive statistics by suspension, analyses proceed in the order of my three main hypotheses: (1) interpersonal exclusion, (2) increased involvement with antisocial peers, and (3) stronger associations in smaller rural schools.
Interpersonal Exclusion.
To estimate the risk of losing a specific friend, I rely on generalized estimating equations (Liang & Zeger, 1986), an extension of generalized linear models that provides a semiparametric approach to analyses of panel data with a categorical outcome. I use a logit link function and binomial probability distribution to estimate differences in the odds of losing a friend from one wave to the next. This is similar to a random-effects logistic regression approach, but it estimates a population-averaged effect rather than the effect of a change in suspension status. A key benefit of this strategy for network analysis is that using the binomial probability allows me to account for variation in the number of possible friends to lose across students and waves (the “trials” for this application of the binomial). Analyses estimate the odds of discontinuity in nominations among suspended students compared to their non-suspended counterparts. I test the overall association with friendship discontinuity and then estimate the proportion of that association explained by lengthy or repeated school absence.
After accounting for school absence, a remaining association may be due to rejection or withdrawal but could also be due to unobserved heterogeneity. My focus on discontinuity in friendship ties, a type of within-individual change, should partially address this concern. To address it even further, I respecify my models among students with high levels of school misbehavior, delinquency, or substance use (above median in grade). Next, I check the robustness of my results to alternative measures of peer exclusion based on perceptions, rather than network data. For greater reliability, I use both student self-reports and parent perceptions of the student’s exclusion from school peers. I analyze these outcomes with within-individual, fixed-effects models (Allison, 2009).5 An important benefit of this approach is that it adjusts for all observed and unobserved time-stable differences between suspended and non-suspended students. A limitation is that it is still subject to bias due to unobserved time-varying characteristics and does not provide between-person estimates.
Increased Involvement with Antisocial Peers.
In examining associations between suspension and changes in network behavioral composition, I focus on friends’ past-month substance use and past-year delinquency. I observe changes in the mean levels of substance use and delinquency across peers the respondent nominated as a friend and across those who nominated the respondent. Analyses again use individual fixed-effects and include all time-varying control variables to minimize concerns with unobserved heterogeneity and selection.
Stronger Associations in Smaller Rural Schools.
To examine heterogeneity by school size in these proposed associations, I test for an interaction between suspension and the number of students in the respondent’s grade. I first test for heterogeneity in the association of suspension with friendship discontinuity and then in the association with behavioral composition of student friendship networks. A summary of results (presented as fitted probabilities) is graphed for ease of interpretation.
RESULTS
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS BY SUSPENSION STATUS
Figure 1 illustrates how risk of experiencing a suspension accumulates in my sample as students advance from sixth to ninth grade. For reference, I compare these to a nationally representative sample of rural and urban students in the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1997 (NLSY97). I combine racial minorities (Hispanic or nonwhite) as a single category because there are so few in rural areas relative to non-Hispanic whites (12% in my sample vs. 17% of rural youth in the NLSY97). Little is known about the prevalence of suspension among rural youth. My results suggest cumulative risk is about as high in rural areas as it is in urban areas, particularly after seventh grade. Also striking is the consistency across samples of the difference between whites and racial minorities. Nearly 15% of racial minority youth have already been suspended from school by sixth grade, compared to just over 5% of whites. By about the time they transition into high school, 40% of racial minorities have been suspended, compared to fewer than 20% of whites.
Descriptive statistics in Table 1 compare suspended students with their non-suspended counterparts. Means and standard deviations are presented for the full sample and by three categories of suspension: never suspended during study, suspended by sixth grade, and suspended after sixth grade. Independent samples t-tests compare means between (1) non-suspended students and (2) students in either of the latter two subgroups of students suspended during the study. Splitting the sample this way reveals important correlates of suspension. Students who get suspended (especially if suspended in sixth grade) come from more disadvantaged backgrounds than those who never experience suspension during the study. They are more likely to have experienced stressful life circumstances such as residential mobility, parental unemployment, and maternal depression than students who do not experience suspension. Their parents are also more likely to have experienced criminal justice contact. Perhaps due in part to these family conditions, they exhibit more misbehavior in school and lower levels of achievement. In addition, their parents monitor and discipline their child’s behavior with less consistency, providing more opportunities for unstructured socializing and greater involvement in substance use, delinquency, and other risky behaviors (see Appendix E for more summary statistics, including between- and within-person standard deviations).
I now examine how suspended students differ from their peers in the size of their friendship networks. Figure 2 presents the average number of friends for students who were suspended by a given wave relative to those who were not. In sixth grade, suspended students nominate four peers in their grades as close friends and receive about three nominations. This is about one nomination less than non-suspended students make or receive. By ninth grade, this difference approaches two friends, partly due to a greater decline in same-grade friends among suspended students. Whereas non-suspended students still nominate a little less than five peers as friends and receive nominations from four, students who have been suspended make only three nominations and receive just over two. Some of this difference may be due to suspension, but some may also be due to more disadvantaged students already having fewer friends. Indeed, students with lower household incomes (below median) make and receive fewer nominations than those with higher incomes (3 made and 3 received versus 4 made and 4 received), and racial minorities make and receive fewer nominations than whites (3 made and 3 received versus 4 made and just over 3 received). If suspension is associated with interpersonal exclusion, even if the overall association is not large, it may be especially meaningful for students who already have fewer same-grade friends. It may also provide opportunities for shared experiences with youth in other grades during in-school suspension or youth outside school during out-of-school suspension. Indeed, the bottom panel of Figure 2 suggests that, compared to other students by the end of middle school, suspended youth have more friends in other grades (6 compared to 5) and other schools (6 compared to 4). Supplemental analyses also suggest a greater proportion of their friends are older than they are, as perceived by parents.6
INTERPERSONAL EXCLUSION
Suspended students have fewer friends in their grade than their peers, but assessing the extent to which they are excluded from particular peers requires moving beyond the basic question of network size to examine discontinuity in specific friendships over time. Table 2 presents multivariable results for discontinuity in same-grade nominations that the respondent makes and then for discontinuity in nominations the respondent receives (full models with controls are presented in Appendix F). Because these analyses exclude observations in which students did not make or receive nominations respectively in the preceding wave, the number of students varies from the full sample of 766 (down to n=697 for analyses of nominations made and n=734 for analyses of nominations received). The first column on the left presents models that include suspension variables without controls. Moving right, the second column shows models that include control variables, and the third column adds controls for weakened institutional attachment. The fourth column adds variables that capture school absence. Finally, in the fifth column, I repeat the analyses presented in the fourth column after limiting the sample to observations with the highest levels of antisocial behavior.
Table 2.
Model | Suspension | Add Control Variables | Add Weakened Institutional Attachment | Add School Absence | High Antisocial Observations | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||||||||
b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | ||||||
| |||||||||||||||
Panel A: Friendship Nomination Made | |||||||||||||||
School suspension (ref: Never suspended in study) | |||||||||||||||
Suspended by current wave | .27 | ** | (.09) | .20 | * | (.09) | .19 | * | (.09) | .18 | (.09) | .24 | * | (.11) | |
Suspended more than once by current wave | .39 | *** | (.09) | .25 | * | (.11) | .25 | * | (.11) | .22 | * | (.11) | .29 | * | (.11) |
Weakened institutional attachment | |||||||||||||||
Low school attachment current wave | .06 | * | (.03) | .06 | * | (.03) | .01 | (.04) | |||||||
Low academic achievement current wave | .03 | (.03) | .02 | (.03) | .03 | (.04) | |||||||||
School Absence (ref: Never missed 7+ days in study) | |||||||||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school in a year by current wave | .04 | (.07) | −.01 | (.09) | |||||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school more than once by current wave | .24 | ** | (.07) | .23 | * | (.09) | |||||||||
Observations | 2,022 | 2,022 | 2,022 | 2,022 | 1,110 | ||||||||||
Students | 697 | 697 | 697 | 697 | 497 | ||||||||||
Panel B: Friendship Nomination Received | |||||||||||||||
School suspension (ref: Never suspended in study) | |||||||||||||||
Suspended by current wave | .25 | ** | (.09) | .09 | (.10) | .09 | (.10) | .09 | (.10) | .13 | (.12) | ||||
Suspended more than once by current wave | .46 | *** | (.10) | .25 | * | (.11) | .23 | * | (.11) | .21 | (.11) | .24 | (.13) | ||
Weakened institutional attachment | |||||||||||||||
Low school attachment current wave | −.03 | (.03) | −.03 | (.03) | −.07 | (.05) | |||||||||
Low academic achievement current wave | .02 | (.03) | .01 | (.03) | .04 | (.04) | |||||||||
School Absence (ref: Never missed 7+ days in study) | |||||||||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school in a year by current wave | .06 | (.07) | .08 | (.09) | |||||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school more than once by current wave | .15 | (.08) | .02 | (.10) | |||||||||||
Observations | 2,154 | 2,154 | 2,154 | 2,154 | 1,044 | ||||||||||
Students | 734 | 734 | 734 | 734 | 483 |
NOTES: PROSPER sample limited to observations of in-home survey participants meeting the following criteria: attending participating school district, participated in the in-school survey, valid data on suspension. Models of nominations made exclude 351 observations of students who did not make a nomination last wave. Models of nominations received exclude 219 observations of students who did not receive a nomination last wave. Suspension and absences observed beginning fall of sixth grade. Control variables are not shown for brevity. High antisocial observations have higher than median substance use, delinquency, or school misbehavior. Results combined across 20 multiply imputed datasets.
ABBREVIATIONS: b = log odds coefficient; SE = standard error; ref = reference category
p<.001
p<.01
p<.05 (two-tailed)
I begin at Panel A by examining the likelihood of discontinuing a same-grade friendship nomination made in the previous wave. Results suggest that among students who have been suspended, the odds of discontinuing a nomination are 31% greater [(e0.27 – 1) ∙ 100] than the odds among non-suspended students (p<.01). This association is stronger for students who have experienced multiple suspensions. Among these, the odds of discontinuing a nomination are 47% greater than the odds among non-suspended students (p<.001). When control variables are added, the associations for both groups remain positive and statistically significant. Among students with one suspension, the odds of discontinuing a nomination are 22% greater than the odds for non-suspended students, and among those with multiple suspensions, the odds are 29% greater. I also add controls for low school attachment and achievement to assess whether they explain these results. Their associations with the outcome are weak, and statistically significant only for low school attachment. One unit of low attachment is associated with a 7% greater odds [(e0.06 – 1) ∙ 100] of discontinuing a nomination (p<.05). Accounting for these variables reduces the coefficient for one suspension by only 4% and does not affect the coefficient for multiple suspensions. 7 After adjusting for all of these controls, converting the log odds coefficients for suspension to fitted probabilities reveals an increase of 0.05 (from 0.42 to 0.47) in the probability of discontinuing a nomination made for students with one suspension, and a difference of 0.06 for multiple suspensions.
Next, I add school absence to the model (due to missing school), and the coefficients for suspension decline further, by 5% for students with one suspension and 13% among students with multiple suspensions. Students who missed 7 or more days in a year are no more likely to discontinue a nomination from the previous wave than those who never had this degree of lengthy or repeated absence during the study. However, among students who have accumulated such absences across multiple waves, the odds of discontinuing a nomination are 27% greater [(e0.24 – 1) ∙ 100] than the odds for the reference category (p<.01). Finally, I respecify these models after retaining only cases with above-median antisocial behaviors in the preceding wave (n=1,110). If results to this point have been biased by unobserved heterogeneity, I would expect the association to decrease substantially. However, the final column of Table 2 suggests the opposite—effect sizes increase slightly and remain statistically significant.
I now move to Panel B of Table 2 to examine the association between suspension and the respondent’s likelihood of losing a friendship nomination received in the previous wave. Without accounting for controls, the odds of suspended students losing a nomination are 29% greater [(e0.25 – 1) ∙ 100] than the odds among non-suspended students (p<.01). This association is larger for students with multiple suspensions: 58% greater than the odds among non-suspended students (p<.001). When controls are added, the coefficients decline substantially (by 63% for students suspended once, 46% for students suspended more than once), suggesting much of this association was driven by selection on observed characteristics. The log-odds coefficient for students suspended only once approaches zero, but the coefficient for those carrying multiple suspensions remains positive and statistically significant. Among these students, the odds of losing a nomination are 29% greater than the odds among non-suspended students (p<.05). I also add controls for low school attachment and achievement, this time focusing on associations for students with multiple suspensions, because associations for students suspended only once were not statistically significant in the previous model. Neither low attachment nor low achievement is significantly associated with discontinuity in nominations received, and these variables reduce the size of the coefficient for multiple suspension by only 5%. After adjusting for all of these controls, converting the log odds for multiple suspensions to a fitted probability reveals an increase of 0.06 (from 0.47 to 0.53) in the probability of discontinuing a nomination received in the previous wave.
Next, I add school absence to the model and again find that it explains only a small part (9%) of the association between suspension and the odds of losing a friendship nomination. For students who have accumulated lengthy or repeated absences over multiple waves, the odds of losing a nomination are 16% greater [(e0.15 – 1) ∙ 100] than the odds among students who never missed 7 or more days in a year (p<.10). Adding school absence to the model causes the coefficient for multiple suspensions to decline below statistical significance but remain stable in size (b=0.21; p<.10). I also check the robustness of these models by limiting the analytic sample to observations with above-median antisocial behaviors (n=1,044). Again, the log-odds coefficient for students with multiple suspensions remains stable (b=0.24; p=<.10). In supplemental models not shown,8 I explore variation in these results by racial minority status and gender. I find no significant differences for boys relative to girls or for racial minorities relative to whites in associations with discontinuity in friendship nominations made or received.9
Alternative Measures of Peer Exclusion
I now examine the robustness of my results to three alternative measures based on perceptions of exclusion from friends at school rather than nomination change: loneliness at school, poor relationships with school friends, and parent-perceived rejection by school friends. Loneliness at school represents the mean of three items adapted from the Children’s Loneliness Scale (Asher & Wheeler, 1985), each ranging from 1=not at all true to 5=really true. Examples include “I feel left out of things at school” and “I feel lonely at school” (alpha=0.93). Poor relationships with school friends also represents the mean of three items, each asking about relationships in the past year (1=never true to 5=always true). Examples include “My school friends and I got along well” (reverse coded)” and “I had a hard time making friends at school” (alpha=0.66). Parent-perceived rejection by school friends is constructed from two items asking parents what percentage of their child’s school friends (1) like and accept him or her (reverse coded) and (2) dislike and reject him or her. Responses range from 1=very few (less than 25%) to 5=almost all (more than 75%). Responses are averaged across both items and across reports of both parents (alpha=0.77). To ease concerns about selection, I focus on within-individual change by relying on fixed-effects linear regression models. I also include the same long list of time-varying controls included in my earlier analyses.
Table 3 presents results first without controls and then with their addition. Results reveal no statistically significant associations between suspension and loneliness at school (Panel A) but moderately-sized positive associations with poor relationships with school friends (Panel B) and with parent-perceived rejection (Panel C). Students suspended once have poorer relationships with school friends (b=0.22; p<.05) and higher levels of parent-reported exclusion from school friends (b=0.22; p<.01) after their suspension than they did before. Furthermore, associations are larger with multiple suspensions (b=0.28, p<.05 and b=0.27, p<.01, respectively).
Table 3.
Model | Suspension | Add Control Variables | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||
b | (SE) | b | (SE) | |||
| ||||||
Panel A: Student-Perceived Loneliness at School | ||||||
School suspension (ref: Never suspended in study) | ||||||
Suspended by current wave | .04 | (.13) | −.05 | (.12) | ||
Suspended more than once by current wave | .12 | (.14) | .03 | (.14) | ||
Panel B: Student-Perceived Poor Relationships with School Friends | ||||||
School suspension (ref: Never suspended in study) | ||||||
Suspended by current wave | .26 | ** | (.09) | .22 | * | (.09) |
Suspended more than once by current wave | .36 | ** | (.13) | .28 | * | (.13) |
Panel C: Parent-Perceived Rejection by School Friends | ||||||
School suspension (ref: Never suspended in study) | ||||||
Suspended by current wave | .22 | ** | (.07) | .22 | ** | (.09) |
Suspended more than once by current wave | .30 | ** | (.09) | .27 | ** | (.09) |
NOTES: PROSPER sample limited to observations of in-home survey participants meeting the following criteria: attending participating school district, participated in the in-school survey, valid data on suspension. N = 2,373 observations from 766 students. Suspension observed beginning fall of sixth grade. Results for controls are omitted for brevity. Each outcome variable is measured on a 5-point Likert scale. Results are combined across 20 multiply imputed datasets.
ABBREVIATIONS: b = unstandardized regression coefficient; SE = robust standard error; ref = reference
p<.01
p<.05 (two-tailed)
INCREASED INVOLVEMENT WITH ANTISOCIAL PEERS
To extend these analyses of friendship discontinuity, I test for changes in network behavioral composition. I again rely on within-individual change following suspension to account for time-stable differences between suspended students and their non-suspended counterparts. Fixed-effects models are presented in Table 4. Coefficients represent the expected standard-deviation unit change in the mean level of substance use or delinquency among friends following the respondent’s suspension. Results in Panel A suggest one suspension is associated with almost one standard-deviation unit increase in substance use among peers the respondent nominates (b=0.92; p<.001) and just under half a standard-deviation unit increase in their delinquency (b=0.43; p<.01). However, when time-varying controls are added, these coefficients are reduced by more than 60%, remaining statistically significant for substance use (b=0.37; p<.05) but not delinquency. Results for multiple suspensions are larger for substance use (b=1.69; p<.001) and delinquency (b=0.90; p<.01), but only changes in the former are robust to time-varying controls. Following multiple suspensions, students experience an increase of more than two-thirds a standard-deviation in substance use among peers they nominate as friends (b=0.69; p<.01).
Table 4.
Model | Mean Past-Month Substance Use of Friends | Mean Past-Year Delinquency of Friends | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||||
Suspension | Add Control Variables | Suspension | Add Control Variables | ||||||||
| |||||||||||
b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | ||||
| |||||||||||
Panel A: Friendship Nominations Made | |||||||||||
School suspension (ref: Never suspended in study) | |||||||||||
Suspended by current wave | .92 | *** | (.18) | .37 | * | (.18) | .43 | ** | (.16) | .15 | (.17) |
Suspended more than once by current wave | 1.69 | *** | (.18) | .69 | ** | (.25) | .90 | ** | (.26) | .35 | (.26) |
Observations | 2,237 | 2,237 | 2,237 | 2,237 | |||||||
Students | 744 | 744 | 744 | 744 | |||||||
Panel B: Friendship Nominations Received | |||||||||||
School suspension (ref: Never suspended in study) | |||||||||||
Suspended by current wave | .74 | *** | (.16) | .22 | (.16) | .25 | (.17) | −.05 | (.17) | ||
Suspended more than once by current wave | 1.18 | *** | (.24) | .34 | (.23) | .60 | * | (.27) | .00 | (.27) | |
Observations | 2,175 | 2,175 | 2,175 | 2,175 | |||||||
Students | 744 | 744 | 744 | 744 |
NOTES: PROSPER sample limited to observations of in-home survey participants meeting the following criteria: attending participating school district, participated in the in-school survey, valid data on suspension. Models of nominations made exclude 351 observations of students who did not make a nomination last wave. Models of nominations received exclude 219 cases of students who did not receive a nomination last wave. Suspension observed beginning fall, sixth grade. Controls not shown for brevity. Results combined across 20 multiply imputed datasets.
ABBREVIATIONS: b = unstandardized regression coefficient; SE = robust standard error; ref = reference category
p<.001
p<.01
p<.05 (two-tailed)
Results for behavior among peers who nominate the respondent are weaker for levels of substance use and delinquency (Panel B). There is a positive and statistically significant association with substance use (b=0.74, p<.001) that is larger with multiple suspensions (b=1.18, p<.001) but each of these falls below statistical significance when controls are added. There is a smaller positive association with delinquency, but it only reaches statistical significance after multiple suspensions (b=0.60; p<001) and is rendered null with the addition of controls.
STRONGER ASSOCIATIONS AMONG STUDENTS IN SMALLER RURAL SCHOOLS
In the final stage of my analyses, I test whether associations reported in Tables 2 and 4 are stronger among students in smaller schools. These earlier results (full models in Appendix F) suggested grade size was associated with greater friendship discontinuity, but associations were quite weak; an increase of 10 students was associated with only a 1% increase [(e0.001 – 1) ∙ 100] in the odds of losing a nomination (p<.001). When the interaction terms described previously are added to these full models, results reveal no statistically significant interactions between grade size and suspension in models of discontinuity in nominations made. Additionally, I find no significant interaction in models of nominations received, if students have only been suspended once. However, the interaction term for students with multiple suspensions is statistically significant (p<.01). Figure 3 summarizes these results with a series of fitted probabilities, shown for respondents with grade sizes of 50, 100, 150, 200, and 300 students. These grade sizes correspond roughly to the tenth, twenty-fifth, fiftieth, seventy-fifth, and ninetieth percentiles of the sample. Results suggest that for students attending a grade of only 50 students, the probability of losing a friendship nomination received last year is expected to be 0.13 higher following multiple suspensions than it is for students who have not reported a suspension (0.57 compared to 0.44). This difference declines to 0.07 among students with the median grade size (0.54 for multiple suspensions, 0.47 for no suspension) but remains statistically significant. However, among students in the largest grades, those with multiple suspensions appear similar to other youth in their probability of losing a friendship nomination.
I also check for interactions with grade size in associations with network behavioral composition. I focus on substance use of peers nominated by the respondent because it was the only statistically significant association after accounting for controls. Results reveal no significant variation by grade cohort size in associations with friends’ substance use.
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
In this paper, I have tested key propositions of labeling theories that move beyond a focus on weakened institutional attachment or secondary deviance (Paternoster & Iovanni, 1989) to examine interpersonal exclusion. Using a sample of predominantly rural students and their same-grade peers, I have assessed: (1) the association between suspension and exclusion from school friends, or discontinuity in friendship nominations from one wave to the next, (2) the association between suspension and increased involvement with antisocial peers, and (3) the extent to which these associations are stronger for students in smaller versus larger rural schools.
INTERPERSONAL EXCLUSION
Overall Findings
In regards to interpersonal exclusion, I find that students who have been suspended experience greater discontinuity in friendship with same-grade peers, based on changes in their own preferences and, for those suspended multiple times, changes in the preferences of their peers. These results are robust to a long list of controls for the student’s antisocial behaviors and other factors that might alter friendship preferences; they also hold up among the most antisocial youth in the sample. The likelihood of a friendship tie discontinuing is greater among suspended students than non-suspended students, and this association is larger for students have been suspended more than once. Furthermore, this association does not appear to be driven by weakened institutional attachment, or student disengagement from school following suspension. Moreover, these results are consistent with models of perceived exclusion, in the form of poor relationships with school friends (student-perceived), and rejection (parent-perceived). The one exception was student-perceived loneliness at school, which does not appear to increase following suspension. This null finding seems consistent with prior research on the association between suspension and feelings of connectedness (Pyne, 2019). It may be that as suspended students lose ties to former friends, they replace them with peers in other grades or schools, perhaps including with other suspended peers. Indeed, suspended youth in eighth and ninth grades report having more friends in other grades and schools than their non-suspended peers. This could explain why suspension is not associated with increases in loneliness, even though it is associated with friendship discontinuity and increased discordance with school friends.
Taken together, the associations with network-based measures are not particularly large but they may be harmful, especially for racial minorities and the poor who already have smaller social networks in these rural schools. I find no significant variation in these results by race or gender, but minority girls and boys are much more likely to be suspended and are thus at greater risk of experiencing the deterioration of a friendship tie. Overall, these findings support labeling theories which suggest formal sanctions may weaken or disrupt ties to non-stigmatized or conforming others (Goffman, 1963; Lemert, 1967) and that multiple sanctions may exacerbate already existing disadvantages (Sampson & Laub, 1997).
Rejection, Withdrawal, and Separation from Friends
Additionally, I find that lengthy or repeated school absences explain a small part (5% to 13%) of the association between suspension and friendship discontinuity. The likelihood of discontinuing a nomination made in the previous wave, and the likelihood of losing a nomination received in the previous wave, are greater for students with multiple periods of lengthy or repeated absence than they are for students who had no such absence. Suspension involves temporarily removing a student from school activities and interactions. Therefore, suspensions that are long or repeated likely limit shared experiences among friends in school and weaken their relationships. These findings suggest separation plays much less of a role in the deterioration of interpersonal ties during suspension than it does during formal sanctions of much greater magnitude, such incarceration (Massoglia et al., 2011; see also Rengifo & DeWitt, 2019).
After accounting for school absence, remaining associations with friendship discontinuity are consistent with my conceptualizations of rejection and withdrawal. The odds of discontinuity in nominations made are 20% greater with one suspension and 25% greater with multiple suspensions (consistent with withdrawal). The odds of discontinuity in nominations received are 23% greater with multiple suspensions (consistent with rejection). Taken together, my findings provide support for both processes but results for withdrawal are more consistent than those for rejection, across categories of suspension and across sensitivity analyses. All of these results should be interpreted with caution because I have not measured rejection or withdrawal directly. Doing so would require such measures as attitudes toward suspended youth, or changes in self-reported attitudes of friends toward the respondent and vice versa. It would also require even greater attention to ruling out potential alternative explanations, including those for which I have not accounted. Nevertheless, my findings are bolstered by my fixed-effects results for student- and parent-perceived exclusion. Future research should apply a similar longitudinal network approach to examining these exclusionary processes following formal sanctions by other institutions, including the criminal justice system (Bryan, 2017; Cochran, et al., 2018).
INCREASED INVOLVEMENT WITH ANTISOCIAL PEERS
In regards to antisocial peers, results suggest students’ friendship networks exhibit greater substance use and delinquency following their suspension. For peers who nominate the suspended student as a friend, much or all of this association appears due to other characteristics, such as behavioral trends that occur with age, or the suspended student’s own antisocial behaviors. However, for peers whom the suspended student nominates as a friend, this association holds even after accounting for these observed characteristics. In particular, suspension is associated with an increase of more than one-third of a standard-deviation, and multiple suspensions are associated with an increase of more than two-thirds of a standard deviation in mean substance use across friends. Average delinquency also increased but the association was not statistically significant after accounting for controls. This latter finding seems inconsistent with prior research suggesting increased involvement with delinquent peers following justice involvement (Bernburg et al., 2006; Wiley et al., 2013), but these studies relied on city-based samples and what may be considered more serious sanctions. Future research should examine whether suspension in urban schools is associated with interpersonal exclusion and greater involvement with delinquent peers.
In my predominantly rural sample, finding suspension associated with increased involvement with substance-using friends seems important given the drug crisis in some rural communities (Keyes, Cerdá, Brady, Havens, & Galea, 2014). Perhaps suspension provides opportunities for students to develop friendships with substance-using peers, either outside school or during in-school suspension. Given that prior delinquency research finds consistent evidence for peer influence (McGloin, 2009; Ragan, 2014), future research should investigate the extent to which this increased involvement with substance-using peers explains associations of suspension with later antisocial behaviors, arrest, and other indicators of secondary deviance (Cuellar & Markowitz, 2015; Hemphill et al., 2006; Mittleman, 2018; Ramey, 2016).
STRONGER ASSOCIATIONS AMONG STUDENTS IN SMALLER RURAL SCHOOLS
In regards to interactions with school size, I find some evidence that the association between suspension and discontinuity in received nominations is stronger among students in smaller grade cohorts, but this does not appear to be the case for nominations made by the respondent. Nor does it appear to be the case if the student has only been suspended once. If suspension were more stigmatizing in smaller schools because of less anonymity, it seems there would be larger effect sizes for one suspension as well, and for discontinuity in nominations made by respondents. It may be that smaller schools are characterized not only be less anonymity but also more social cohesion and trust among peers. In such a setting, it’s possible that friends are forgiving after one suspension, but after multiple suspensions, negative reactions accumulate and shaming becomes more severe (Braithwaite, 1989; Sampson & Laub, 1997). However, supplemental analyses did not reveal interactions by grade size in associations with my perceptual measures of exclusion, so this finding warrants further investigation. In addition, future research should examine the extent to which peer reactions to suspension vary across different contexts. Juvenile justice research finds negative outcomes of justice involvement (Wiley et al., 2013), but some argue that the stigma of juvenile justice involvement may not be as salient in disadvantaged urban areas where it is heavily concentrated (Hirschfield, 2008). I find suspension in this rural sample to be about as common as it is in a national sample, but its effects could vary by contextual-level factors like concentrated disadvantage or school punitiveness. Future research should explore these potential sources of heterogeneity.
CONCLUSION
Before concluding, several limitations should be reiterated. Although prior research suggests the overwhelming majority of suspensions are for minor classroom misbehavior and attendance problems (Skiba et al., 2014), I do not have administrative data or other information on specific incidents that led to suspensions in my sample. In addition, I have focused solely on suspension because this is the only school punishment ascertained in PROSPER. Another exclusionary form of discipline is expulsion, which deserves greater attention in future research. Relatedly, the questionnaire wording prevents me from examining heterogeneity across types of suspension. Perhaps in-school suspension is less influential for peer networks than out-of-school suspension, but few large-scale surveys distinguish between types. Moreover, the results of all of my analyses are based on observational data; I cannot rule out the possibility that they are driven by unobserved heterogeneity between suspended and non-suspended students. Finally, although the use of PROSPER data contributes to knowledge about the prevalence and outcomes of suspension in some rural schools, my results may not generalize beyond these youth. Future work should extend these analyses among youth in more racially diverse communities, more representative samples, and in school districts that do not meet PROSPER’s enrollment criteria, such as smaller schools that are more similar in size to rural schools nationally.
Taken together, these findings are consistent with labeling theories, which imply that stigmatizing sanctions may be followed by more constrained social networks and greater involvement with deviant peers. In my sample of rural youth, suspension is associated with a loss of friends in one’s grade and greater involvement with substance-using peers. Suspended youth also report having more friends in other grades and schools. These findings have implications for school policy. Suspension may be like other youth-based interventions that involve segregating antisocial youth or providing them opportunities to interact with one another, which research finds may have negative behavioral consequences (Dishion, McCord, & Poulin, 1999). Furthermore, racial minority and disadvantaged youth are at much greater risk of suspension already and are already more marginalized in their school networks; therefore, reliance on suspension may foster social inequality by stratifying access to friends who are important for healthy development and academic success (Crosnoe, 2000; Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). As states and school districts consider evidence-based alternatives to suspension, such as school-based substance use interventions or restorative justice programs (Owen, Wettach, & Hoffman, 2015), their students may benefit from alternatives that emphasize inclusion over exclusion.
Acknowledgments
I am grateful to D. Wayne Osgood for his suggestions regarding my methodological approach. I also thank him and the following for their comments on earlier drafts: Michelle L. Frisco, S. Michael Gaddis, Scott D. Gest, Anna R. Haskins, Derek A. Kreager, Janet L. Lauritsen, Jean M. McGloin, David M. Ramey, Sonja E. Siennick, Mei Yang, and multiple anonymous reviewers. Any errors are my own. Grants from the William T. Grant Foundation (8316), National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01-DA018225), and National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R24-HD041025) supported this research. The analyses used data from PROSPER, a project directed by R. L. Spoth, funded by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01-DA013709) and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (AA14702). The content of this article is solely my responsibility and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Appendix A.
PROSPER (Iowa and Pennsylvania) | United States | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|||||||||||
Full 2003 Cohort | In-home Subsample | Analytic Sample | Rural Sixth-Grade Students | PROSPER-Criteriaa Sixth-Grade Students | All Sixth-Grade Students | ||||||
| |||||||||||
Receives free or reduced-price lunch | .34 | .35 | .33 | .37 | b | .47 | b | .41 | b | ||
Racial minority | .16 | .13 | ** | .12 | *** | .21 | .29 | .41 | |||
Male | .49 | .47 | .48 | .47 | .47 | .48 | |||||
Friendship nominations made | 4.14 | 4.32 | * | 4.33 | * | — | — | — | |||
Friendship nominations received | 2.80 | 3.20 | *** | 3.23 | *** | — | — | — | |||
Past-year delinquency | .07 | −.02 | *** | −.03 | *** | — | — | — | |||
Past-month substance use | .07 | .03 | ** | .02 | *** | — | — | — | |||
Sixth-grade students | 6,165 | 977 | 766 | 789,494 | 624,864 | 3,746,944 | |||||
Sixth-grade students per school (median) | 105 | 105 | 104 | 42 | 93 | 77 |
NOTES: PROSPER descriptive statistics shown here are based on baseline respondents only. The median number of PROSPER students per school is based on school roster data (regardless of participation). The analytic sample is limited to observations from in-home survey respondents who met the following criteria: attending participating school district, participated in the in-school survey, valid data on suspension. US data come from the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES) 2003–2004 and include students from regular (not alternative or other) public schools with at least two sixth-grade students (minimum number necessary to be comparable to a peer network study). “Rural” refers to NCES locale codes (census-defined) and is based on school address. PROSPER data are unweighted. Mean comparisons represent dependent-sample t-tests between each of the smaller PROSPER samples and the full 2003 cohort.
Students attending school districts with enrollments between 1,300 and 5,200 students, at least 15% of which are eligible for free or reduced-price lunch (Spoth, et al., 2007).
National data on lunch status are not available by grade. These represent the proportion of all public school students.
p<.001
p<.01
p<.05 (two-tailed)
Appendix B.
Wave | Percent of Baseline Respondents Participating | Percent of Respondents at Wave with Participating Mother | Percent of Respondents at Wave with Participating Father |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Fall, Sixth Grade | 100 | 97 | 70 |
Spring, Sixth Grade | 82 | 97 | 68 |
Spring, Seventh Grade | 81 | 97 | 67 |
Spring, Eighth Grade | 79 | 96 | 66 |
Spring, Ninth Grade | 75 | 95 | 64 |
Appendix C.
Questionnaire Item | Response Options | |
---|---|---|
| ||
Substance use | ||
1. | During the past month, how many times have you smoked any cigarettes? | (1) Not at all (2) One time (3) A few times (4) About once a week (5) More than once a week |
2. | During the past month, how many times have you had beer, wine, wine coolers, or other liquor? | |
3. | During the past month, how many times have you been drunk from drinking wine, wine coolors, or other liquor? | |
4. | During the past month, how many times have you smoked marijuana (pot, reefer, weed, blunts)? | |
| ||
Delinquency | ||
1. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you taken something worth less than $25 that didn’t belong to you? | (1) Never (2) Once (3) Twice (4) Three or four times (5) Five or more times |
2. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you taken something worth $25 or more that didn’t belong to you? | |
3. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you beat up someone or physically fought with someone because they made you angry (other than just playing around)? | |
4. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you purposely damaged or destroyed property that did not belong to you? | |
5. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you broken into or tried to break into a building just for fun or to look around? | |
6. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you thrown objects such as rocks or bottles at people to hurt or scare them? | |
7. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you been picked up by the police for breaking a law? | |
8. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you run away from home? | |
9. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you skipped school or classes without an excuse? | |
10. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you carried a hidden weapon? | |
11. | During the past 12 months, how many tiems have you avoided paying for things such as movies, rides, food, or computer services? | |
12. | During the past 12 months, how many times have you taken something from a store that you did not pay for? |
Appendix D.
Control Variable | Description | Survey |
---|---|---|
Nominations made last wave no longer in school/study | Number of nominations respondent made last wave for friends who are no longer in the study or did not participate in the current wave. | IS |
Nominations received last wave no longer in school/study | Number of nominations respondent received last wave for friends who are no longer in the study or did not participate in the current wave. | IS |
Racial composition of nominations made last wave all white | All nominations made last wave were white based on self-reports. | IS |
Racial composition of nominations received last wave all white | All nominations received last wave were white based on self-reports. | IS |
Number of students in grade last wave | Count of number of students on roster in grade at school. | NA |
Transitioned to new school last wave | Student on rosters of new school since previous wave. | NA |
Miles to school last wave | Parent reports of number of miles from home to youth’s school. | IH |
Special education services last wave | Either parent reported that the child received special education services. | IH |
Structured activities after school last wave | Combination of two scales: (1) structured after-school activities and (2) part-time work. Structured after-school activities based on combined reports of mothers, fathers, and student about student’s frequency of programs, lessons, practices, after school (alpha=0.63). Part-time work based on combined reports of mothers, fathers, and student about student’s frequency of work after school (alpha=0.61). Response options for both scales range from 1=never to 5=always. Each is standardized and then the two are summed. | IH |
Unstructured socializing after school last wave | Mean composite of mother, father, and student reports about frequency at which student spends free time after school hanging out with friends (alpha=0.59). Response options originally ranged from 1=never to 5=always. | IH |
Any substance use last wave | Student reported smoking, drinking, getting drunk, or using marijuana in the past month. Based on dichotomized responses originally ranging from 1=not at all to 5=more than once per week. | IS |
Delinquency variety score last wave | Variety score based on 10 items about frequency of various delinquent behaviors in past year. Responses range from 1=never to 5=five or more times. | IS |
Frequency of school misbehavior last wave | Mean composite of mother, father, and student reports about frequency at which student engages in various forms of misbehavior at school (e.g., disrupting class, talking back to teachers; alpha=0.76). Response options range from 1=never to 5=more than 5 times in past year. | IH |
Risk and sensation seeking last wave | Mean of three items about student’s tendency to engage in risky behaviors for fun (alpha=0.79). Responses range from 1=never to 5=always. | IS |
Frequency of bully victimization last wave | Mean of five items about frequency of student’s victimization in past two months (e.g., pushing or shoving, telling rumors or lies; alpha=0.80). Responses range from 1=never to 4=always. | IH |
Parental discipline last wave | Mean of student reports to five items about more consistent and less harsh discipline parental discipline (alpha=0.83). Response options range from 1=always to 5=never. | IH |
Parental monitoring last wave | Mean of five items about frequency of parental monitoring (alpha=0.85). Response options range from 1=always to 5=never. | IH |
Parent education last wave | Parent reports of highest grade of school completed. Items for each parent are standardized and then averaged together (alpha=0.66). | IH |
Household income last wave | Mean of mother-reported total household income and father-reported total household income, each adjusted for inflation. | IH |
Parent unemployment last wave | Either parent reports being currently unemployed or temporarily laid off, or unemployed past year. | IH |
Parent ever arrested | Mother reports at Wave 5 (ninth grade) of whether either parent had ever been arrested for driving under the influence or anything other than a traffic offense. | IH |
Mother relationship transitions last wave | Count of number of times mother has ever married, cohabited, or divorced. | IH |
Children in household last wave | Parent reports of number of children living in household more than half the time. | IH |
Mother depression last wave | Mother reports of whether she experienced any symptoms of depression (feeling sad, blue, depressed, losing interest) for two continuous weeks or more in past 12 months. | IH |
Religiosity last wave | Student self-reports of frequency of attendance at religious services. Responses range from 1=never to 6=more than once per week. | IH |
Years in current residence last wave | Data from household form about the number of years respondent has lived in current residence. | IH |
Community cohesion last wave | Mean of parent reports on 10 items about community cohesion. Separate scales for fathers (alpha=0.85) and mothers (alpha=0.88) are averaged together (alpha=0.56). | IH |
ABBREVIATIONS: IH = in-home survey; IS = in-school survey; NA = not applicable
Appendix E.
Variable | Overall | Decomposed | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
M | SD | Between SD | Within SD | |
| ||||
School suspension (ref: Never suspended in study) | ||||
Suspended by current wave | 0.07 | 0.26 | 0.23 | 0.15 |
Suspended more than once by current wave | 0.08 | 0.27 | 0.25 | 0.12 |
School Absence (ref: Never Missed 7+ days) | ||||
Missed 7+ days of school in a year since fall of 6th grade | 0.19 | 0.39 | 0.33 | 0.24 |
Missed 7+ days of school in a year more than once since fall of 6th grade | 0.14 | 0.34 | 0.26 | 0.20 |
Weakened Institutional Attachment | ||||
Low school attachment current wave (1 to 5) | 2.25 | 0.86 | 0.73 | 0.48 |
Low academic achievement current wave (z-score) | 0.00 | 1.01 | 0.95 | 0.43 |
Control Variables | ||||
Male (ref: Female) | 0.48 | — | — | — |
Nonwhite (ref: Non-Hispanic white) | 0.12 | — | — | — |
Nominations made last wave no longer in school/study (log) | 0.28 | 0.41 | 0.26 | 0.34 |
Nominations received last wave no longer in school/study (log) | 0.29 | 0.41 | 0.27 | 0.33 |
Racial composition of nominations made last wave all white | 0.68 | 0.47 | 0.35 | 0.32 |
Racial composition of nominations received last wave all white | 0.69 | 0.46 | 0.36 | 0.31 |
Number of students in grade last wave (6 to 470) | 185.12 | 109.46 | 105.41 | 38.91 |
Transitioned to new school last wave | 0.09 | 0.29 | 0.15 | 0.25 |
Miles to school last wave (log) | 1.25 | 0.64 | 0.59 | 0.24 |
Special education services last wave | 0.18 | 0.39 | 0.35 | 0.21 |
Structured activities after school last wave (z-score) | 0.00 | 1.00 | 0.81 | 0.66 |
Unstructured socializing after school last wave (z-score) | 0.00 | 1.00 | 0.90 | 0.54 |
Any substance use last wave | 0.11 | 0.31 | 0.21 | 0.23 |
Delinquency variety score last wave (log) | 0.32 | 0.53 | 0.45 | 0.32 |
Frequency of school misbehavior last wave (log) | 0.34 | 0.31 | 0.29 | 0.13 |
Risk and sensation seeking last wave (1 to 5) | 1.89 | 0.90 | 0.73 | 0.57 |
Frequency of bully victimization last wave (log) | 0.30 | 0.31 | 0.25 | 0.19 |
Parental discipline last wave (1 to 5) | 3.72 | 0.95 | 0.79 | 0.57 |
Parental monitoring last wave (1 to 5) | 4.51 | 0.61 | 0.50 | 0.37 |
Parent education last wave (z-score) | 0.00 | 1.00 | 1.02 | 0.27 |
Household income last wave (log) | 10.74 | 0.84 | 0.79 | 0.37 |
Parent unemployment last wave | 0.19 | 0.39 | 0.30 | 0.27 |
Parent ever arrested (ninth grade only) | 0.17 | 0.38 | 0.37 | 0.13 |
Mother relationship transitions last wave (0 to 8) | 1.96 | 1.45 | 1.42 | 0.44 |
Children in household last wave (0 to 8) | 2.40 | 1.05 | 1.01 | 0.38 |
Mother depression last wave | 0.22 | 0.42 | 0.34 | 0.27 |
Religiosity last wave (1 to 6) | 3.83 | 1.75 | 1.59 | 0.82 |
Years in current residence last wave (0 to 19) | 6.88 | 4.31 | 4.09 | 1.47 |
Community cohesion last wave (z-score) | −0.01 | 1.00 | 0.94 | 0.44 |
Observations | 2,373 | |||
Students | 766 |
NOTES: PROSPER sample limited to observations of in-home survey participants meeting the following criteria: attending participating school district, participated in the in-school survey, valid data on suspension. Control variable for current grade not shown. Results based on first of 20 multiply imputed datasets.
ABBREVIATIONS: M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Appendix F.
Friendship Nominations Made | Friendship Nominations Received | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||||||||||
Explanatory Variable | Suspension and Control Variables | Add School Absence | Suspension and Control Variables | Add School Absence | ||||||||
| ||||||||||||
b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | |||||
| ||||||||||||
School Suspension (ref: Never Suspended in Study) | ||||||||||||
Suspended by current wave | .19 | * | (.09) | .18 | (.09) | .09 | (.10) | .09 | (.10) | |||
Suspended more than once by current wave | .25 | * | (.11) | .22 | * | (.11) | .23 | * | (.11) | .21 | (.11) | |
Control Variables | ||||||||||||
Current grade (ref: 6th) | ||||||||||||
7th | .59 | *** | (.06) | .56 | *** | (.06) | .74 | *** | (.06) | .73 | *** | (.06) |
8th | .36 | *** | (.07) | .31 | *** | (.07) | .53 | *** | (.07) | .50 | *** | (.07) |
9th | .37 | *** | (.07) | .32 | *** | (.07) | .50 | *** | (.07) | .47 | *** | (.08) |
Male (ref: Female) | .01 | (.06) | .02 | (.06) | .08 | (.05) | .09 | (.05) | ||||
Nonwhite (ref: Non–Hispanic white) | .14 | (.08) | .15 | (.08) | .04 | (.08) | .06 | (.08) | ||||
Number of friends last wave | .13 | *** | (.02) | .13 | *** | (.02) | .04 | *** | (.01) | .04 | *** | (.01) |
Nominations last wave no longer in school/study | −.67 | *** | (.05) | −.68 | *** | (.05) | −.71 | *** | (.05) | −.70 | *** | (.05) |
Racial composition of last wave’s friends all white | −.07 | (.05) | −.08 | (.05) | −.09 | (.05) | −.10 | * | (.05) | |||
Number of students in grade last wave | .00 | *** | (.00) | .00 | *** | (.00) | .00 | *** | (.00) | .00 | *** | (.00) |
Transitioned to new school last wave | .18 | * | (.08) | .19 | * | (.08) | .06 | (.08) | .07 | (.08) | ||
Miles to school last wave | .00 | (.04) | −.01 | (.04) | .05 | (.04) | .05 | (.04) | ||||
Special education services last wave | −.08 | (.07) | −.08 | (.07) | .00 | (.07) | .00 | (.07) | ||||
Structured activities after school last wave | −.01 | (.02) | −.01 | (.02) | .00 | (.02) | .00 | (.02) | ||||
Unstructured socializing after school last wave | −.01 | (.03) | −.01 | (.03) | −.01 | (.03) | −.01 | (.03) | ||||
Any substance use last wave | −.04 | (.08) | −.05 | (.07) | .09 | (.08) | .09 | (.08) | ||||
Delinquency variety score last wave | .08 | (.06) | .09 | (.06) | .02 | (.06) | .02 | (.06) | ||||
Frequency of school misbehavior last wave | .14 | (.12) | .13 | (.12) | .26 | * | (.12) | .25 | * | (.12) | ||
Risk and sensation seeking last wave | −.04 | (.03) | −.03 | (.03) | .01 | (.03) | .01 | (.03) | ||||
Frequency of bully victimization last wave | .07 | (.09) | .08 | (.09) | −.09 | (.09) | −.08 | (.09) | ||||
Parental discipline last wave | −.06 | * | (.03) | −.06 | * | (.03) | −.05 | (.03) | −.05 | (.03) | ||
Parental monitoring last wave | −.03 | (.05) | −.03 | (.05) | −.02 | (.05) | −.02 | (.05) | ||||
Parent education last wave | −.04 | (.03) | −.05 | (.03) | −.03 | (.03) | −.03 | (.03) | ||||
Household income last wave | −.07 | * | (.04) | −.07 | (.04) | −.03 | (.04) | −.03 | (.04) | |||
Parent unemployment last wave | .01 | (.06) | .00 | (.06) | −.04 | (.07) | −.05 | (.07) | ||||
Parent ever arrested | .09 | (.08) | .08 | (.08) | .09 | (.09) | .09 | (.09) | ||||
Mother relationship transitions last wave | .03 | (.02) | .03 | (.02) | .01 | (.02) | .01 | (.02) | ||||
Children in household last wave | .03 | (.02) | .02 | (.02) | .04 | (.02) | .04 | (.02) | ||||
Mother depression last wave | −.04 | (.06) | −.06 | (.06) | −.03 | (.06) | −.04 | (.06) | ||||
Religiosity last wave | −.01 | (.01) | −.01 | (.01) | .00 | (.02) | .00 | (.02) | ||||
Years in current residence last wave | .01 | (.01) | .01 | (.01) | −.01 | (.01) | −.01 | (.01) | ||||
Community cohesion last wave | .01 | (.03) | .01 | (.03) | −.01 | (.03) | −.01 | (.03) | ||||
Weakened Institutional Attachment | ||||||||||||
Low school attachment current wave | .06 | * | (.03) | .06 | * | (.03) | −.03 | (.03) | −.03 | (.03) | ||
Low academic achievement current wave | .03 | (.03) | .02 | (.03) | .02 | (.03) | .01 | (.03) | ||||
School Absence (ref: Never Missed 7+ Days in Study) | ||||||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school in a year by current wave | .04 | (.07) | .06 | (.07) | ||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school more than once by current wave | .24 | ** | (.07) | .15 | (.08) | |||||||
Constant | −.38 | (.51) | −.47 | (.51) | −.19 | (.53) | −.22 | (.54) | ||||
N of Students | 697 | 697 | 734 | 734 | ||||||||
N of Observations | 2,022 | 2,022 | 2,154 | 2,154 |
NOTES: PROSPER sample limited to observations of in–home survey participants meeting the following criteria: attending participating school district, participated in the in–school survey, valid data on suspension. Models of friendship nominations made exclude 351 observations of students who did not make a nomination last wave. Models of friendship nominations received exclude 219 observations of students who did not receive a nomination last wave. Suspension and absences observed beginning fall of sixth grade. Results are combined across 20 multiply imputed datasets.
ABBREVIATIONS: b = log odds coefficient; SE = standard error; ref = reference category
p<.001
p<.01
p<.05 (two–tailed)
Appendix G.
Friendship Nominations Made | Friendship Nominations Received | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||||||||||
Explanatory Variable | Full Sample | High Antisocial Observations | Full Sample | High Antisocial Observations | ||||||||
| ||||||||||||
b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | |||||
| ||||||||||||
School Suspension (ref: Never Suspended in Study) | ||||||||||||
Suspended by current wave | .18 | (.12) | .29 | * | (.13) | .03 | (.13) | .06 | (.16) | |||
Suspended more than once by current wave | −.09 | (.22) | −.17 | (.26) | .24 | (.26) | −.01 | (.23) | ||||
Control Variables | ||||||||||||
Current grade (ref: 6th) | ||||||||||||
7th | .61 | *** | (.06) | .53 | *** | (.09) | .74 | *** | (.06) | .73 | *** | (.10) |
8th | .38 | *** | (.07) | .43 | *** | (.11) | .51 | *** | (.07) | .56 | *** | (.11) |
9th | .37 | *** | (.07) | .41 | *** | (.10) | .49 | *** | (.08) | .44 | *** | (.12) |
Male (ref: Female) | .04 | (.06) | −.04 | (.08) | .07 | (.06) | −.01 | (.08) | ||||
Nonwhite (ref: Non–Hispanic white) | .15 | + | (.09) | .15 | (.11) | .05 | (.08) | .07 | (.12) | |||
Number of friends last wave | .14 | *** | (.02) | .13 | *** | (.02) | .04 | *** | (.01) | .05 | ** | (.01) |
Nominations last wave not participating/not in school | −.67 | *** | (.05) | −.71 | *** | (.08) | −.67 | *** | (.06) | −.64 | *** | (.08) |
Racial composition of last wave’s friends all white | −.06 | (.05) | −.06 | (.07) | −.10 | * | (.05) | −.10 | (.08) | |||
Number of students in grade last wave | .00 | *** | (.00) | .00 | ** | (.00) | .00 | *** | (.00) | .00 | ** | (.00) |
Transitioned to new school last wave | .18 | * | (.09) | .19 | (.12) | .08 | (.09) | −.06 | (.12) | |||
Miles to school last wave | .00 | (.05) | −.08 | (.06) | .06 | (.04) | .01 | (.06) | ||||
Special education services last wave | −.07 | (.08) | −.14 | (.11) | −.01 | (.08) | −.15 | (.11) | ||||
Structured activities after school last wave | −.02 | (.02) | −.06 | * | (.03) | .00 | (.02) | .01 | (.04) | |||
Unstructured socializing after school last wave | −.01 | (.03) | −.01 | (.04) | .00 | (.03) | −.03 | (.04) | ||||
Any substance use last wave | −.07 | (.08) | −.15 | + | (.09) | .07 | (.08) | .06 | (.09) | |||
Delinquency variety score last wave | .09 | (.06) | .10 | (.07) | −.01 | (.06) | .01 | (.07) | ||||
Frequency of school misbehavior last wave | .06 | (.12) | −.09 | (.15) | .30 | (.13) | .18 | (.19) | ||||
Risk and sensation seeking last wave | −.05 | (.03) | −.06 | (.04) | .00 | (.04) | .01 | (.05) | ||||
Frequency of bully victimization last wave | .11 | (.10) | .12 | (.12) | −.07 | (.09) | .02 | (.12) | ||||
Parental discipline last wave | −.05 | + | (.03) | −.09 | * | (.04) | −.06 | + | (.03) | −.08 | + | (.04) |
Parental monitoring last wave | −.03 | (.05) | −.07 | (.06) | −.01 | (.05) | .06 | (.06) | ||||
Parent education last wave | −.03 | (.03) | −.01 | (.04) | −.03 | (.03) | .02 | (.04) | ||||
Household income last wave | −.09 | * | (.04) | −.07 | (.05) | −.05 | (.04) | −.06 | (.06) | |||
Parent unemployment last wave | −.04 | (.07) | −.04 | (.09) | −.05 | (.07) | .01 | (.10) | ||||
Parent ever arrested | .12 | (.09) | .08 | (.10) | .08 | (.09) | .04 | (.12) | ||||
Mother relationship transitions last wave | .03 | (.02) | .04 | + | (.02) | .01 | (.02) | .03 | + | (.03) | ||
Children in household last wave | .03 | (.03) | .05 | (.03) | .04 | (.03) | .05 | (.04) | ||||
Mother depression last wave | −.03 | (.06) | −.08 | (.09) | −.06 | (.06) | −.03 | (.10) | ||||
Religiosity last wave | −.01 | (.01) | .01 | (.02) | .00 | (.02) | .00 | (.02) | ||||
Years in current residence last wave | .01 | + | (.01) | .02 | * | (.01) | .00 | (.01) | .01 | (.01) | ||
Community cohesion last wave | .03 | (.03) | .05 | (.04) | −.01 | (.03) | .01 | (.04) | ||||
Weakened Institutional Attachment | ||||||||||||
Low school attachment current wave | .06 | (.03) | −.02 | (.05) | −.03 | (.03) | −.05 | (.05) | ||||
Low academic achievement current wave | .05 | (.04) | .07 | + | (.04) | .01 | (.04) | .04 | (.05) | |||
Constant | −.29 | (.54) | .02 | (.73) | −.05 | (.56) | −.17 | (.81) | ||||
n of Students | 654 | 448 | 694 | 448 | ||||||||
n of Observations | 1,825 | 936 | 1,951 | 936 |
NOTES: PROSPER sample limited to observations of in–home survey participants meeting the following criteria: attending participating school district, participated in the in–school survey, valid data on suspension. Models of nominations made exclude 351 observations of students who did not make a nomination last wave and 75 observations of students suspended in the year leading up to the study. Models of nominations received exclude 219 observations of students who did not receive a nomination last wave and 81 observations of students suspended in the year leading up to the study. Both sets of models also exclude 122 subsequent observations of remaining suspended students. High antisocial observations have higher than median substance use, delinquency, or school misbehavior. Results combined across 20 multiply imputed datasets.
ABBREVIATIONS: b = log odds coefficient; SE = standard error; ref = reference category
p<.001
p<.01
p<.05
p<.10 (two–tailed)
Appendix H.
Model | Suspension | Add Control Variables | Add Weakened Institutional Attachment | Add School Absence | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||||||
b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | b | (SE) | |||||
| ||||||||||||
Panel A: Friendship Nomination Made | ||||||||||||
School suspension (ref: Never suspended in study) | ||||||||||||
Suspended by current wave | −1.12 | + | (.61) | −1.39 | (.85) | −1.52 | + | (.90) | −1.55 | + | (.93) | |
Suspended more than once by current wave | −1.84 | ** | (.56) | −1.64 | * | (.83) | −1.70 | + | (.88) | −1.77 | + | (.92) |
Weakened institutional attachment | ||||||||||||
Low school attachment current wave | .15 | (.29) | .15 | (.30) | ||||||||
Low academic achievement current wave | −.49 | (.34) | −.50 | (.35) | ||||||||
School Absence (ref: Never missed 7+ days in study) | ||||||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school in a year by current wave | −.26 | (.61) | ||||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school more than once by current wave | .28 | (.93) | ||||||||||
Observations | 351 | 351 | 351 | 351 | ||||||||
Students | 288 | 288 | 288 | 288 | ||||||||
Panel B: Friendship Nomination Received | ||||||||||||
School suspension (ref: Never suspended in study) | ||||||||||||
Suspended by current wave | .29 | (.48) | .30 | (.64) | .27 | (.66) | .13 | (.67) | ||||
Suspended more than once by current wave | .12 | (.42) | .03 | (.62) | .01 | (.62) | .04 | (.63) | ||||
Weakened institutional attachment | ||||||||||||
Low school attachment current wave | −.48 | * | (.22) | −.50 | * | (.22) | ||||||
Low academic achievement current wave | .27 | (.21) | .25 | (.22) | ||||||||
School Absence (ref: Never missed 7+ days in study) | ||||||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school in a year by current wave | .05 | (.40) | ||||||||||
Missed 7+ days of school more than once by current wave | 1.21 | (.77) | ||||||||||
Observations | 219 | 219 | 219 | 219 | ||||||||
Students | 154 | 154 | 154 | 154 |
NOTES: PROSPER sample limited to observations of in-home survey participants meeting the following criteria: attending participating school district, participated in the in-school survey, valid data on suspension. Of these, models of nominations made only include observations of students who did not make a nomination last wave. Models of nominations received only include observations of students who did not receive a nomination last wave. Results for controls are omitted for brevity. Results are combined across 20 multiply imputed datasets.
ABBREVIATIONS: b = log odds coefficient; SE = standard error; ref = reference category
p<.01
p<.05
p<.10 (two-tailed)
Footnotes
My own analysis of public school data compiled by the National Center for Education Statistics (https://nces.ed.gov/ccd/elsi/tableGenerator.aspx) suggests that in the 2016–2017 school year, rural schools (NCES locale code based on Census definition) had fewer students than non-rural schools on average but also varied in size. For example, for regular schools with at least one sixth-grade student, the rural mean sixth-grade enrollment was 68 students, with a standard deviation of 79. The non-rural mean enrollment was 135, with a standard deviation of 115.
School districts that were already involved in another prevention research project or where half or more of the population was attending or employed by a college or university were ineligible.
Seventy-four percent of the student body participated in the in-school survey at the first wave, 86% by ninth grade. Seventy-three percent of first-wave respondents participated at ninth grade.
Nine percent of cases (351 observations from 288 students) pertain to students who did not nominate anyone in the previous wave, and 15% (219 observations from 154 students) to students who did not receive nominations. A few were isolates in both directions (68 observations from 57 students). I examine a separate set of models limited to these students in which the outcome variable is a binary measure indicating a change from no nominations in the previous wave, to any nominations in the current wave (described later).
Researchers use the term “fixed effects” in various ways. In this paper, I am referring to this basic equation (Firebaugh, Warner, & Massoglia, 2013): yit = αt + μi + βxit + εit, where yit is the outcome for person i at time t, αt is a fixed effect for time, μi is a fixed effect for individuals, xit is a vector of explanatory variables that vary over time, β is a vector of coefficients, and εit is a within-individual error term.
Parents were asked how many of their child’s friends were older (1=very few to 5=almost all), and in each grade, parents of suspended youth reported a higher level. For example, in eighth grade, suspended youth had a mean of 1.64 versus 1.44 for other students (p<.01).
In additional analyses not shown, I examined the association between suspension and these indicators of weakened institutional attachment in a series of fixed-effects linear regression models with all time-varying controls included. One suspension was associated with lower achievement (b=0.29; p<.001) and the association increased with multiple suspensions (b=0.51; p<.001). I found less evidence for an association with low school attachment (b=0.16, p<.10 for one suspension; coefficient for multiple suspensions not significant).
One set of supplemental analyses addresses concerns with temporal ordering by (1) dropping all 97 cases (33 students) suspended in the year preceding the study and (2) limiting analyses to first-reported suspensions by dropping all 122 subsequent observations of suspended students. Due to lost statistical power and variation in suspension, fewer coefficients are statistically significant, but those for one suspension are comparable in size and direction to those in Table 2, particularly in regards to nominations made (Appendix G).
Appendix H presents results for models described in Footnote 2 (no nominations last year). Similar to findings in Table 2, these results suggest the odds of making any friendship nominations in the current year are lower for students who have been suspended relative to those who have not. In contrast, the association with receiving a nomination is much weaker and not statistically significant.
REFERENCES
- Allcott H, Karlan D, Möbius MM, Rosenblat TS, & Szeidl A (2007). Community size and network closure. The American Economic Review, 97, 80–85. [Google Scholar]
- Allison PD (2009). Fixed effects regression models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Asher SR, & Wheeler VA (1985). Children’s loneliness: A comparison of rejected and neglected peer status. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 500–505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Austin AMB, & Draper DC (1984). The relationship among peer acceptance, social impact, and academic achievement in middle childhood. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 597–604. [Google Scholar]
- Balfanz R, Byrnes V, & Fox JH (2015). Sent home and put off track: The antecedents, disproportionalities, and consequences of being suspended in the 9th grade. In Losen DJ (Ed.), Closing the school discipline gap: Equitable remedies for excessive exclusion (pp. 17–30). New York: Teachers College Press. [Google Scholar]
- Beggs JJ, Haines VA, & Hurlbert JS (1996). Revisiting the rural-urban contrast: Personal networks in nonmetropolitan and metropolitan settings. Rural Sociology, 61, 306–325. [Google Scholar]
- Bernburg JG, Krohn MD, & Rivera CJ (2006). Official labeling, criminal embeddedness, and subsequent delinquency: A longitudinal test of labeling theory. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 43, 67–88. [Google Scholar]
- Braithwaite J (1989). Crime, shame and reintegration. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Brayne S (2014). Surveillance and system avoidance: Criminal justice contact and institutional attachment. American Sociological Review, 79, 367–391. [Google Scholar]
- Bryan B (2017). Paternal incarceration and adolescent social network disadvantage. Demography, 54, 1477–1501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cochran JC, Siennick SE, & Mears DP (2018). Social exclusion and parental incarceration impacts on adolescents’ networks and school engagement. Journal of Marriage and Family, 80, 478–498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman JS (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of Sociology, 94, S95–S120. [Google Scholar]
- Crosnoe R (2000). Friendships in childhood and adolescence: The life course and new directions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63, 377–391. [Google Scholar]
- Cuellar AE, & Markowitz S (2015). School suspension and the school-to-prison pipeline. International Review of Law and Economics, 43, 98–106. [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Patterson GR, Stoolmiller M, & Skinner ML (1991). Family, school, and behavioral antecedents to early adolescent involvement with antisocial peers. Developmental Psychology, 27, 172–180. [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, McCord J, & Poulin F (1999). When interventions harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American Psychologist, 54, 7557–7564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge KA (1983). Behavioral antecedents of peer social status. Child Development, 54, 1386–1399. [Google Scholar]
- Dodge KA, Lansford JE, Burks VS, Bates JE, Pettit GS, Fontaine R, & Price JM (2003). Peer rejection and social information-processing factors in the development of aggressive behavior problems in children. Child Development, 74, 374–393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunning-Lozano JL (2018). Secondary discipline: The unintended consequences of zero tolerance school discipline for low-income black and Latina mothers. Urban Education. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1177/0042085918817343 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Eder D, & Kinney DA (1995). The effect of middle school extracurricular activities on adolescents’ popularity and peer status. Youth & Society, 26, 298–324. [Google Scholar]
- Felmlee D, McMillan C, Rodis PI, & Osgood DW (2018). Falling behind: Lingering costs of the high school transition for youth friendships and grades. Sociology of Education, 91, 159–182. [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson AA. (2001). Bad boys: Public schools in the making of black masculinity. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. [Google Scholar]
- Firebaugh G, Warner C, & Massoglia M (2013). Fixed effects, random effects, and hybrid models for causal analysis. In Morgan SL (Ed.), Handbook for Causal Analysis for Social Research (pp. 113–132). New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Fischer CS (1982). To dwell among friends: Personal networks in town and city. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Flashman J (2012). Academic achievement and its impact on friend dynamics. Sociology of Education, 85, 61–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster H, & Hagan J (2015). Punishment regimes and the multilevel effects of parental incarceration: Intergenerational, intersectional, and interinstitutional models of social inequality and systemic exclusion. Annual Review of Sociology, 41, 135–158. [Google Scholar]
- Fox M (1996). Rural school transportation as a daily constraint on students’ lives. Rural Educator, 17, 22–27. [Google Scholar]
- Geller A & Curtis MA (2011). A sort of homecoming: Incarceration and the housing security of urban men. Social Science Research, 40, 1196–1213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goffman A (2009). On the run: Wanted men in a Philadelphia ghetto. American Sociological Review, 74, 339–357. [Google Scholar]
- Goffman E (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Gottfredson GD, & Gottfredson DC (2001). What schools do to prevent problem behavior and promote safe environments. Journal of Educational and Psychological Consultation, 12, 313–344. [Google Scholar]
- Harding DJ (2009). Violence, older peers, and the socialization of adolescent boys in disadvantaged neighborhoods. American Sociological Review, 74, 445–464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartup WW, & Stevens N (1997). Friendship and adaptation in the life course. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 355–370. [Google Scholar]
- Haskins AR, & Jacobsen WC (2017). Schools as surveilling institutions? Paternal incarceration, system avoidance, and parental involvement in schooling. American Sociological Review, 82, 657–684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hemphill SA, Toumbourou JW, Herrenkohl TI, McMorris BJ, & Catalano RF (2006). The effect of school suspensions and arrests on subsequent adolescent antisocial behavior in Australia and the United States. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, 736–744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirschfield PJ (2008). The declining significance of delinquent labels in disadvantaged urban communities. Sociological Forum, 23, 575–601. [Google Scholar]
- Hirschfield PJ (2018a). The role of schools in sustaining juvenile justice system inequality. The Future of Children, 28, 11–35. [Google Scholar]
- Hirschfield PJ (2018b). Schools and crime. Annual Review of Criminology, 1, 149–169. [Google Scholar]
- Hirschfield PJ & Piquero AR (2010). Normalization and legitimation: Modeling stigmatizing attitudes toward ex-offenders. Criminology, 48, 27–55. [Google Scholar]
- Jacobsen WC (2019). The intergenerational stability of punishment: Paternal incarceration and suspension or expulsion in elementary school. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1177/0022427819829794 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobsen WC, Pace GT, & Ramirez NG (2019). Punishment and inequality at an early age: Exclusionary discipline in elementary school. Social Forces, 97, 973–998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keyes KM, Cerdá M, Brady JE, Havens JR, & Galea S (2014). Understanding the rural-urban differences in nonmedical prescription opioid use and abuse in the United States. American Journal of Public Health, 104, e52–e59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kinsler J (2013). School discipline: A source or salve for the racial achievement gap? International Economic Review, 54, 355–383. [Google Scholar]
- Kirk DS, & Wakefield S (2018). Collateral consequences of punishment: A critical review and path forward. Annual Review of Criminology, 1, 171–194. [Google Scholar]
- Kupchik A (2010). Homeroom security: School discipline in an age of fear. New York: New York University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kupchik A (2016). The real school safety problem: The long-term consequences of harsh school punishment. Oakland, CA: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kupersmidt JB, & Coie JD (1990). Preadolescent peer status, aggression, and school adjustment as predictors of externalizing problems in adolescence. Child Development, 61, 1350–1362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lageson SE (2016). Found out and opting out: The consequences of online criminal records for families. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 665, 127–141. [Google Scholar]
- Laird RD, Jordan KY, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, & Bates JE (2001). Peer rejection in childhood, involvement with antisocial peers in early adolescence, and the development of externalizing behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 337–354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lara-Millán A (2014). Public emergency room overcrowding in the era of mass imprisonment. American Sociological Review, 79, 866–887. [Google Scholar]
- Lemert EM (1951). Social pathology: A systematic approach to the theory of sociopathic behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Lemert EM (1967). Human deviance, social problems, and social control. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Liang K, & Zeger SL (1986). Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika, 73, 13–22. [Google Scholar]
- Link B (1987). Understanding labeling effects in the area of mental disorders: An empirical assessment of the effects of expectations of rejection. American Sociological Review, 52, 96–112. [Google Scholar]
- Link B, Cullen FT, Struening E, Shrout PE, & Dohrenwend BP (1989). A modified labeling theory approach to mental disorders: An empirical assessment. American Sociological Review, 54, 400–423. [Google Scholar]
- Link B, Cullen FT, Frank J, & Wozniak JF (1987). The social rejection of former mental patients: Understanding why labels matter. American Journal of Sociology, 92, 1461–1500. [Google Scholar]
- Lippman L, Burns S, & McArthur E (1996). Urban schools: The challenge of location and poverty. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics. [Google Scholar]
- Lippold MA, Greenberg MT, & Collins LM (2013). Parental knowledge and youth risky behavior: A person oriented approach. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 1732–1744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loveless T (2017). How well are American students learning? With sections on the latest international test scores, foreign exchange students, and school suspensions. Washington, DC: The Brookings Institution. [Google Scholar]
- Marsden PV, & Srivastava SB (2012). Trends in informal social participation: 1974–2008. In Marsden PV (Ed.), Social trends in American life: Findings from the General Social Survey since 1972 (pp. 240–263). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Massoglia M, Remster B, & King RD (2011). Stigma or separation? Understanding the incarceration-divorce relationship. Social Forces, 90, 133–155. [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS, & Coatsworth JD (1998). The development of competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: Lessons from research on successful children. American Psychologist, 53, 205–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGloin JM (2009). Delinquency balance: Revisiting peer influence. Criminology, 47, 439–477. [Google Scholar]
- McNulty CP, & Roseboro DL (2009). “I’m not really that bad”: Alternative school students, stigma, and identity politics. Equity & Excellence in Education, 42, 412–427. [Google Scholar]
- McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, & Cook JM (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415–444. [Google Scholar]
- Mittleman J (2018). A downward spiral? Childhood suspension and the path to juvenile arrest. Sociology of Education, 91, 183–204. [Google Scholar]
- Moody J, Brynildsen WD, Osgood DW, Feinberg ME, & Gest S (2011). Popularity trajectories and substance use in early adolescence. Social Networks, 33, 101–112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris EW & Perry BL (2016). The punishment gap: School suspension and racial disparities in achievement. Social Problems, 63, 68–86. [Google Scholar]
- Morris EW & Perry BL (2017). Girls behaving badly? Race, gender, and subjective evaluation in the discipline of African American girls. Sociology of Education, 90, 127–148. [Google Scholar]
- Mowen TJ (2017). The collateral consequences of “criminalized” school punishment on disadvantaged parents and families. The Urban Review, 49, 832–851. [Google Scholar]
- Mowen TJ & Brent J (2016). School discipline as a turning point: The cumulative effect of suspension on arrest. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 53, 628–653. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson IA (2016). Rural students’ social capital in the college search and application process. Rural Sociology, 81, 249–281. [Google Scholar]
- Nolan K (2011). Police in the hallways: Discipline in an urban high school. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. [Google Scholar]
- Noltemeyer AL, Ward RM, & Mcloughlin C (2015). Relationship between school suspension and student outcomes: A meta-analysis. School Psychology Review, 44, 224–240. [Google Scholar]
- Office of Civil Rights (2018). 2013–2014 State and National Estimations. Department of Education Office of Civil Rights. January 22. http://ocrdata.ed.gov/StateNationalEstimations/Estimations_2013_14. [Google Scholar]
- Osgood DW, & Chambers JM (2000). Social disorganization outside the metropolis: An analysis of rural youth violence. Criminology, 38, 81–116. [Google Scholar]
- Osgood DW, Feinberg ME, & Ragan DT (2015). Social networks and the diffusion of adolescent problem behavior: Reliable estimates of selection and influence from sixth through ninth grades. Prevention Science, 16, 832–843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osgood DW, McMorris BJ, & Potenza MT (2002). Analyzing multiple-item measures of crime and deviance I: Item response theory scaling. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 18, 267–296. [Google Scholar]
- Owen J, Wettach J, & Hoffman KC (2015). Instead of suspension: Alternative strategies for effective school discipline. Durham, NC: Duke Center for Child and Family Policy and Duke Law School. [Google Scholar]
- Pager D (2003). The Mark of a Criminal Record. American Journal of Sociology, 108, 937–975. [Google Scholar]
- Paternoster R, & Iovanni L (1989). The labeling perspective and delinquency: An elaboration of the theory and an assessment of the evidence. Justice Quarterly, 6, 359–394. [Google Scholar]
- Payne AA, & Welch K (2010). Modeling the effects of racial threat on punitive and restorative school discipline practices. Criminology, 48, 1019–1062. [Google Scholar]
- Perry BL, & Morris EW (2014). Suspended progress: Collateral consequences of exclusionary punishment in public schools. American Sociological Review, 79, 1067–1087. [Google Scholar]
- Pyne J (2019). Suspended attitudes: Exclusion and emotional disengagement from school. Sociology of Education, 92, 59–82. [Google Scholar]
- Quin D, & Hemphill SA (2014). Students’ experiences of school suspension. Health Promotion Journal of Australia, 25, 52–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ragan DT (2014). Revisiting “what they think”: Adolescent drinking and the importance of peer beliefs. Criminology, 52, 488–515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramey DM (2016). The influence of early school punishment and therapy/medication on social control experiences during young adulthood. Criminology, 54, 113–1141. [Google Scholar]
- Rengifo AF, & DeWitt SE (2019). Incarceration and personal networks: Unpacking measures and meanings of tie strength. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 35, 393–431. [Google Scholar]
- Roscigno VJ (2006). Education and the inequalities of place. Social Forces, 84, 2121–45. [Google Scholar]
- Rulison K, Kreager DA, & Osgood DW (2014). Delinquency and peer acceptance in adolescence: A within-person test of Moffitt’s hypotheses. Developmental Psychology, 50, 2437–2448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samejima F (1969). Estimation of latent ability using a response pattern of graded scores. Psychometrika Monograph Supplement No. 17. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, & Laub JH (1997). A life-course theory of cumulative disadvantage and the stability of delinquency. In Thornberry TP (Ed.), Developmental Theories of Crime and Delinquency (pp. 133–161). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer DR, Kornienko O, & Fox AM (2011). Misery does not love company: Network selection mechanisms and depression homophily. American Sociological Review, 76, 764–785. [Google Scholar]
- Shollenberger TL (2015). Racial disparities in school suspension and subsequent outcomes: Evidence from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. In Losen DJ (Ed.), Closing the school discipline gap: Equitable remedies for excessive exclusion (pp. 31–43). New York: Teachers College Press. [Google Scholar]
- Skiba RJ, Chung C, Trachok M, Baker TL, Sheya A, & Hughes R (2014). Parsing disciplinary disproportionality contributions of infraction, student, and school characteristics to out-of-school suspension and expulsion. American Educational Research Journal, 51, 640–670. [Google Scholar]
- Spoth R, Redmond C, Shin C, Greenberg M, Chair S, & Feinberg M (2007). Substance-use outcomes at 18 months past baseline. American Journal of Preventative Medicine, 32, 395–402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- South SJ, Haynie DL, & Bose S (2007). Student mobility and school dropout. Social Science Research, 36, 68–94. [Google Scholar]
- Stanton-Salazar RD, & Dornbusch SM (1995). Social capital and the reproduction of inequality: Information networks among Mexican-origin high school students. Sociology of Education, 68, 116–135. [Google Scholar]
- Sutherland E (1947). Principles of Criminology. Philadelphia: Lippincot. [Google Scholar]
- Temkin DA, Gest SD, & Osgood DW (2018). Social network implications of normative school transitions in non-urban school districts. Youth & Society, 50, 462–484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turney K, & Wildeman C (2013). Redefining relationships: Explaining the countervailing consequences of paternal incarceration for parenting. American Sociological Review, 78, 949–979. [Google Scholar]
- Vanderhaar JE, Petrosko JM, & Muñoz MA (2015). Reconsidering the alternatives: The relationship among suspension, placement, subsequent juvenile detention, and the salience of race. In Losen DJ (Ed.), Closing the school discipline gap: Equitable remedies for excessive exclusion (pp. 222–236). New York: Teachers College Press. [Google Scholar]
- Weissman M (2015). Prelude to prison: Student perspectives on school suspension. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Weissman M, & NaPier E (2015). Education suspended: The use of high school disciplinary records in college admissions. New York: The Center for Community Alternatives. [Google Scholar]
- Welch K, & Payne AA (2012). Exclusionary school punishment: The effect of racial threat on expulsion and suspension. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 10, 155–171. [Google Scholar]
- Wiley SA, Slocum LA, Esbensen F (2013). The unintended consequences of being stopped or arrested: An exploration of the labeling mechanisms through which police contact leads to subsequent delinquency. Criminology, 51, 927–966. [Google Scholar]
- Wolf KC, & Kupchik A (2017). School suspensions and adverse experiences in adulthood. Justice Quarterly, 34, 407–430. [Google Scholar]
- Young JTN, Barnes JC, Meldrum RC, & Weerman FM (2011). Assessing and explaining misperceptions of peer delinquency. Criminology, 49, 599–630. [Google Scholar]