Abstract
It is likely that when using an artificially augmented hand with six fingers, the natural five plus a robotic one, corticospinal motor synergies controlling grasping actions might be different. However, no direct neurophysiological evidence for this reasonable assumption is available yet. We used transcranial magnetic stimulation of the primary motor cortex to directly address this issue during motor imagery of objects’ grasping actions performed with or without the Soft Sixth Finger (SSF). The SSF is a wearable robotic additional thumb patented for helping patients with hand paresis and inherent loss of thumb opposition abilities. To this aim, we capitalized from the solid notion that neural circuits and mechanisms underlying motor imagery overlap those of physiological voluntary actions. After a few minutes of training, healthy humans wearing the SSF rapidly reshaped the pattern of corticospinal outputs towards forearm and hand muscles governing imagined grasping actions of different objects, suggesting the possibility that the extra finger might rapidly be encoded into the user’s body schema, which is integral part of the frontal-parietal grasping network. Such neural signatures might explain how the motor system of human beings is open to very quickly welcoming emerging augmentative bioartificial corticospinal grasping strategies. Such an ability might represent the functional substrate of a final common pathway the brain might count on towards new interactions with the surrounding objects within the peripersonal space. Findings provide a neurophysiological framework for implementing augmentative robotic tools in humans and for the exploitation of the SSF in conceptually new rehabilitation settings.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Motor control
Introduction
Wearable robotics is an emerging field bridging engineering with neuroscience, with great translational impact into neurological and rehabilitative practice1,2. While many technologically advanced prosthetic devices are available for amputees, patients that still have their hand but cannot use it due to motor impairments (or paresis) of various etiologies, cannot benefit from these devices for obvious reasons. Hence, alternative strategies are needed: exoskeletons3 are hardly usable in activities of daily living because of their excessive weight, cumbersome size, limited wearability and lack of accommodating individual anatomical variations due to the motor impairment at the hand, forearm and upper limb. The Soft Sixth Finger (SSF), a wearable robotic additional thumb resting on the wrist that can be activated on user’s demand for grasping actions4,5, represents a novel augmentative/compensatory device that has been favorably accepted by post-stroke patients. Such patients, after having lost grasping abilities due to the paresis, experienced immediate functional gains in recovery of grasping and bimanual cooperation5. Some preliminary behavioral evidence suggests that an extra thumb might be incorporated into the user’s body schema6, but the brain mechanisms underpinnings SSF use are still basically unknown: healthy subjects scanned by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during the use of SSF showed increased activations in brain regions relevant for body space and motor control4, but whether they have been using extant or alternative -newly formed- corticospinal motor synergies to control the hand actions performed with the supernumerary thumb is still unexplored. Understanding these mechanisms is a necessary, still unmet, step before the exploitation of supernumerary fingers in rehabilitation or augmentative scenarios.
Neural assemblies within the primary motor cortex (M1) are connected in a complex way to the periphery of the musculoskeletal system and finely tune corticospinal commands for arm and/or hand movements that require a defined coordinated activation of specific muscles and less activation, up to inhibition, of others. Therefore, M1 exerts its control of movements in terms of goal-directed actions rather than independently on single muscles. Actions are subtended by motor synergies, i.e. “the output patterns of conjoined muscle activity whose timing and amplitude modulation enable the correct production of goal-directed movements”7,8. Few evidences point out that, in humans and non-human primates, motor synergies are implemented within the corticospinal output: in a study combining electromyographic, kinematic and neuroimaging recordings, synergies governing several hand grasping gestures were successfully predicted by neural activation patterns within M19, thus paralleling experimental results in rhesus macaques, in which the electrical microstimulation of M1 evokes complex and highly coordinated movements across multiple joints, resembling common gestures of the monkey's natural actions’ repertoire10,11. A similar organization can be observed in healthy humans thanks to transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), which is a primer12 for assessing corticospinal function non-invasively: for example, it has been shown that TMS-evoked finger movements after stimulation of M1 overlap those derived from grasping movements13.
TMS investigations have consistently revealed the engagement of the motor system in planned14 or executed actions15 capitalizing from corticospinal output changes accompanying these motor tasks resulting from the functional cooperation of M1 and the connected brain regions16. In healthy humans, TMS and neuroimaging investigations converge on the notion that neural networks underpinning imagined and executed actions largely overlap and functionally engage M1 as a final effector area17–21. The different degree of engagement of forearm and hand muscles within the motor plan dispatched, but not executed, during motor imagery can also be disentangled by TMS over M117,22,23. Notably, TMS during motor imagery is more likely of capturing fine modulations of M1 activation patterns than during overt movements, when the whole corticospinal system is operating well above its threshold levels for activation, thus overshadowing local changes for synergies generation.
On these premises, we reasoned that the pattern of TMS-evoked responses in different muscle groups of the forearm/hand during imagined grasping actions of objects could represent the most appropriate approach to verify the specific hypothesis of the study, that was to reveal different motor synergies eventually recruited when using the additional SSF versus naturally performed actions. Understanding mechanisms of these bioartificial synergies is a crucial step before extra fingers (or arms) could be fully exploited into rehabilitation settings for compensation, as well as in forthcoming scenarios of augmentation of physiological human capabilities, that are not so far away to come24.
Methods
The soft sixth finger (SSF)
The SSF (Fig. 1) is a 140 g weighing robotic wearable robotic supernumerary extra finger that acts as an additional thumb4,5,25. As the SSF does not rely on subject’s skeletal structure, anatomical variability and motion restriction are a minor issue. It represents the minimal complexity solution for grasping that also guarantees extreme wearability. After being fasten on the wrist by means of an elastic yet stable band, the system composed of the SSF and the hand/forearm acts like a gripper to hold and manipulate an object. The SSF can stably grasp objects of different shape and size thanks to its intrinsically compliant structure. The device can be used on demand through different kinds of system interfaces26 while resting wrapped around the wrist, as a bracelet, when not in use.
Participants, task description and experimental paradigm
Thirty (twenty-seven fully right-handed) healthy subjects (male: 15; female 15; mean age 29.5 ± 7.5 SD years) participated in the study after having signed an informed consent and having received the approval of the study by the Local Ethic Committee [Ethic Committee Regione Toscana, Area Vasta Sud-Est (CEAVSE)]. All experiments were carried out in accordance with the ICH Guideline for Good Clinical Practice.
The sample size was estimated expecting as significant an amplitude variation of 25%, with an α error probability of 0.05, nonsphericity correction ɛ = 1,within an ANOVA for repeated measures, according to the experimental design (one group of healthy subjects, five conditions): output parameters sorted a sample of 26 subjects to achieve a statistical power of 0.90 and to discriminate significant changes between different conditions (Noncentrality parameter λ: 16.25; Critical F: 2.46; degrees of freedom: 4).
Handedness was assessed by Oldfield’s questionnaire27. All subjects had no contraindications to TMS28, were free from neurological or psychiatric disorders, did not assume drugs for therapeutic purposes and had a quite normal sleeping night the day before the experiment. None declared the assumption of recreative substances in the last ten days.
The main task consisted in performing the mental imagination of grasping actions with or without the SSF. Objects to be grasped were a bottle, a cylinder, a ball, and a foam rubber dice (mean weight: 262.5 ± 116.1 g), without particular affordances able to influence the grasping pre-shaping at cortical level. Participants sat in front of a table where one of the four objects was laying, with their arm resting near it (Fig. 2). All subjects were given the opportunity to train with the grasping tasks and with the SSF use before starting the experiment, until they were able to complete a true grasping action with the SSF and stated to be confident in its use. Then, subjects were trained to perform the imagery task of grasping without producing any electromyographic activity in the four recorded muscles (flexors and extensors at the forearm and two intrinsic muscles of the hand, see Fig. 2). This two-step training required no more than three minutes.
After having located the left motor cortex on the scalp and having found the individual threshold of stimulation for TMS (see the following paragraph), the following experimental conditions were considered:
Basal-pre [20 motor evoked potentials (MEPs) simultaneously recorded from the four muscles], in which subjects were instructed to fully relax, fixing a point in front of them.
20 MEPs simultaneously recorded from the four muscles during motor imagery of grasping actions of the four objects (5 MEPs for each object) [Natural grasping (Nat-grasp)].
-
20 MEPs simultaneously recorded from the four muscles during motor imagery of grasping actions of the four objects (5 MEPs for each object) with the SSF [Augmented grasping (Aug-grasp)].
The participants wear the SSF in this grasping condition only.
20 MEPs from the four muscles during motor imagery of a pinch-to-grip-action involving the index finger and the thumb (Pinch).
20 MEPs simultaneously recorded from the four muscles in a complete rest condition, wearing the SSF (Basal-post).
Conditions 2, 3, and 4 were balanced and randomized between subjects. In conditions 2 and 3, the four objects were randomly presented to the subject, in successive blocks of five trials each. Multiple objects (without definite affordances) were used to simulate an ecological use of the SSF in daily living, where a person with reduced hand movement abilities must interface to various items in terms of shape, compliance and weight. Moreover, the secondary aim was to avoid an habituation effect and a flattening an electrophysiological response caused to a multiple presentation of the same object.
In all motor imagery conditions, the participants imagined the grasping movement they should have done, but without actually doing it, starting at an acoustic “go” signal given by one of the experimenters. The subject was required to imagine the movement in its entirety, from the rest position to the final grasp of the object, but without lifting it. The TMS pulse randomly followed 1–3 s the “go” signal, therefore occurring during the reaching phase, and a random time between 5 and 10 s elapsed between two consecutive "go" signals, aiming to minimize expectancy effects17,21,23. This time served also to monitor the electromyographic (EMG) silence in the target muscles in the time window preceding the brain stimulation. The EMG silence preceding the TMS pulse was also monitored using an acoustic feedback provided by the EMG recorder. The whole experimental paradigm, that lasted between 45 min and one hour, was designed for being as ecological as possible (i.e., as close as possible to everyday life). Figure 2 shows representative MEPs recorded in one of the subjects.
Procedures of brain stimulation and neurophysiological recording
A circular non-focal 90-mm coil connected with a ATES magnetic stimulator (EBNeuro, Italy) was positioned over the vertex with its handle pointing backwards, so that the electric field flowed in a posterior-anterior counterclockwise direction. The cortical representations of the right Extensor Digitorum Communis (EDC), Flexor Digitorum Superficialis (FDS), Abductor Pollicis Brevis (APB) and First Dorsal Interosseous (FDI) were targeted within the left M1. The use of a non-focal coil guaranteed fairly stable simultaneous MEPs from all the considered forearm/hand muscles, even if the coil did not cover evenly the cortical hot-spots of the four muscles. To the same aim, the intensity of each TMS pulse was adjusted to obtain fairly stable MEPs simultaneously from the four muscles (mean 113%, SD 2.9, of the resting motor threshold, corresponding to the 55–75% of the maximal stimulator output among all subjects), defined as the minimal intensity to produce a MEP of 50–100 µV in the APB muscle with a probability of 50%29.
Once identified the best fitting scalp position for the coil for targeting M1, this was maintained throughout the experiment by means of a neuronavigation system (BrainNET, EBNeuro Ltd, Florence, Italy) using infrared cameras (Polaris Vicra, NDI, Waterloo, Canada). An MRI template for each participant was uploaded in the neuronavigation software and a co-registration procedure was performed using scalp landmarks (nasion, vertex and the two preauricular points) and additional landmarks positioned on a plastic glasses frame worn by the subjects. The coil was calibrated using an in-house algorithm based on five landmarks specific for the device. During TMS, the software provided online visual feedback allowing the investigators to keep constant (with a tolerance of 2 mm for each spatial x, y, z axis) the desired coil orientation/rotation/distance during the whole session.
Corticospinal responses (or MEPs) were recorded by means of surface electrodes placed with a belly/negative-tendon/positive montage; analysis time base was 50–100 ms, bandpass acquisition filers were 20–20.000 Hz, the gain of amplifiers was adjusted to entirely show the negative–positive evoked peaks of MEPs, without saturation.
Neurophysiological data analysis
Pre-processing
For each of the four muscles, the peak-to-peak amplitude of the MEPs obtained in each experimental condition were considered. Trials with detectable voluntary EMG activity in the analysis time were discarded from further analysis. Whenever unwanted EMG or other artifactual activity contaminated one of the 4 MEPs simultaneously acquired for each TMS pulse, or if there was a latency jitter of more than 3 ms in one of the four MEPs, all the remaining responses were discarded as well, even if they were artifact-free. Outliers in amplitude (i.e., more than 2 standard deviations from the average value of the considered condition) were also discarded. The final rejection rate ranged 10–20% (summing rejections during the acquisition phase and post-experimental visual inspection) between subjects. After these procedures, a sample of at least 15–18 MEPs/condition remained available for successive processing.
Statistical analysis
Data were analyzed using SPSS Version 20 statistic software package (IBM Corp. Released 2011. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp). Since the assumption of normality as assessed by the Shapiro–Wilk test was violated (p < 0.05), nonparametric Friedman test statistics were adopted for the repeated measures experimental design addressing amplitude changes across conditions.
As a first step, the modulation of MEPs amplitude of each muscle (APB, FDI, FDS, EDC) was separately investigated across the five conditions (Basal-Pre, Nat-grasp, Aug-grasp, Pinch, Basal-post). Three outliers (mean amplitude of participant’s MEPs higher than 2 SD compared to the group average) for APB were excluded. The statistical analysis for this muscle was thus conducted on twenty seven healthy subjects (male 14; female 13).
A second test was carried out to investigate differences of the MEP amplitudes by coupling responses from the intrinsic hand (APB and FDI) and extrinsic forearm hand muscles (FDS and EDC) across conditions (5 levels: Basal-pre, Nat-grasp, Aug-grasp, Pinch, Basal-post). Here, by means of a preliminary analysis of the average values within each condition, one outlier was identified (mean amplitude of participant’s MEPs higher than 2 SD compared to the group average), and thus excluded from the analysis. The statistical analysis was thus conducted on twenty-nine subjects (male: 14; female 15).
Finally, a third test was performed to evaluate modifications of MEPs amplitude between agonist (FDI and FDS) and no-agonist (APB and EDC) muscles in the imagined actions. Amplitude variation of these two groups of muscles were compared (factor Condition: 5 levels: Basal-pre, Nat-grasp, Aug-grasp, Pinch, Basal-post). As the preliminary analysis identified one outlier (mean amplitude of participant’s MEPs higher than 2 SD compared to the group average), it was excluded from the analysis, that was conducted on twenty-nine subjects (male: 15; female 14).
Whenever the Friedman’s test resulted significant, Dunn’s pairwise post-hoc tests were conducted, followed by Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. For all tests, the level of significance was set at p < 0.05. While statistics were carried out on raw MEPs amplitude values, also percentage variations were used for graphical representations (see Supplemental Material) by considering the Basal-pre condition as 100%, irrespective of the absolute average value of MEP amplitudes.
These analyses allowed to verify modifications of the specific corticospinal output independently for each muscle, as well as for distinct corticospinal synergy patterns across the different conditions of movement imagination.
All statistical comparisons are available in the Table S2 of the supplemental material, as well as graphical representations of percentage variations (Figure S1, S2 and S3).
Significance statement
The study shows the emergence of new bioartificial cortico-muscular synergies, as revealed by direct stimulation of the primary motor cortex by transcranial magnetic stimulation, induced by a wearable robotic sixth finger designed for augmentation of grasping in healthy humans and for recovery of grasping in paretic patients.
Results
Table S1 reports MEPs’ mean amplitude values and standard deviations in each individual muscle in the basal condition. Table S2 reports all statistics. Figures S1-S3 additionally show percentual amplitude data variations in all conditions.
Single muscles level
Figure 3 shows the variation of MEPs amplitude versus Baseline-pre in the different conditions. Table S2 reports all statistics. Concerning APB MEPs, a significant difference across conditions (χ2(4) = 28.28; p < 0.001) emerged. Post hoc tests revealed a significant increase of the MEPs amplitude in the conditions Pinch (p < 0.001) and Nat-grasp (p = 0.002) compared to Basal-pre. The condition Nat-grasp differed significantly also from Post (p = 0.019), while Aug-Grasp differed significantly from Nat-grasp (p = 0.048) and from Pinch (p = 0.019). Finally, the MEPs amplitude in Basal-post condition significantly differed from Pinch (p = 0.007) and Nat-grasp (p = 0.019) conditions, but not from Aug-grasp.
A significant difference across conditions (χ2(4) = 34.528; p < 0.001) occurred also for FDI MEPs: post-hoc tests showed a significant increase of the MEP in the condition Pinch versus Basal-pre, Post, Aug-grasp and Nat-grasp (p < 0.001, p < 0.002, p = 0.043 and p = 0.003 respectively).
Differently, for FDS MEPs (χ2(4) = 44.614; p < 0.001), the MEPs amplitude of Basal-pre condition significantly increased during both Nat-grasp and Aug-grasp (respectively: p < 0.006 and p < 0.001); MEP amplitude significantly increased in Nat-grasp and Aug-grasp compared to the Basal-post (both p < 0.001). Additionally, the MEPs amplitude of Pinch condition was higher (p = 0.005) compared to that of Basal-post.
A similar corticospinal modulation was observed for EDC MEPs (χ2(4) = 32.514; p < 0.001), whose variation versus the MEPs amplitude of the Basal-pre condition significantly increased during Nat-grasp and Aug-grasp (both p < 0.001). Again, it was significantly augmented in Nat-grasp and Aug-grasp compared to the Basal-post (both p < 0.001).
Taken together, this first analysis at single-muscle level (Fig. 3) showed that the FDI muscle was involved only in the Pinch condition, the APB muscle was involved in the Pinch and in the Nat-grasp, but not in the Aug-grasp (i.e., when its function was replaced by the SSF), while the two forearm muscles were involved both in the Nat-grasp and Aug-grasp, but not in the precision pinch grip. MEPs amplitude between Basal-Pre and Basal-post conditions did not significantly differ for any muscle.
Intrinsic (“Hand”) versus extrinsic (“Forearm”) hand muscles
Figure 4 shows “Hand” and “Forearm” MEPs amplitude variation versus Baseline-pre in all experimental conditions; Table S2 reports all statistics.
Analyzing the “Hand” MEPs amplitude, a main effect of Condition was found (χ2(4) = 33.112; p < 0.001), the corticospinal output for the intrinsic hand muscles being significantly increased (p < 0.001) versus Basal-Pre, only during the imagery of the pinch grip. Considering comparisons between the other conditions, MEPs amplitude was higher at Pinch compared to Basal-post and to Aug-Grasp (p < 0.001 and p = 0.002). No other significant variations were observed.
Considering the extrinsic hand “Forearm” muscles, a significant main effect of Condition was detected (χ2(4) = 39.965; p < 0.001). Post hoc tests revealed that the corticospinal output for extrinsic forearm muscles significantly increased during the Nat-grasp (p < 0.001) and Aug-grasp (p < 0.001) conditions compared to Basal-pre and Basal-post, but not during Pinch. MEPs amplitude was higher during the Nat-grasp, Aug-grasp and Pinch compared to Basal-post condition (p < 0.001, p < 0.001 and p = 0.024 respectively).
An opposite trend (that did not reach significance) was observed for “Forearm” and “Hand” muscles when engaged in Nat-grasp and Aug-grasp: Aug-grasp increased corticospinal output towards “Forearm” muscles and decreased it versus Nat-grasp in “Hand” muscles.
Agonist versus no-agonist muscles
Figure 5 shows amplitude variation of MEPs amplitude versus Baseline-pre in all experimental conditions; Table S2 reports all statistics.
Considering the corticospinal output in agonist muscles (FDI + FDS), a significant main effect of Condition was found (χ2(4) = 37.492; p < 0.001). The MEP amplitude of these muscles significantly increased in conditions Pinch and Aug-grasp versus the MEPs value of Basal-pre condition (p < 0.001 and p = 0.032 respectively). Analyzing the activity of non-agonist muscles (APB + EDC), a main effect of Condition was again detected (χ2(4) = 30.059; p < 0.001): post-hoc tests revealed a significant increase versus Basal-pre of MEPs recorded during Pinch (p = 0.001) and Nat-grasp (p = 0.005), but not versus Aug-grasp condition. MEPs amplitude recorded during Pinch and Nat-grasp was higher compared to those of Basal-post (p < 0.001 and p = 0.005 respectively).
Discussion
As shown in two families of individuals born with six-fingered hands, a genetic condition known as polydactyly30, the human brain is able to control a body part with over-physiological degrees of freedom, as a supernumerary fingered hand. In these individuals, the innate supernumerary finger augments some manipulation abilities versus a conventional five-fingered hand, but it is controlled neuromechanically by independent muscles and nerves, and its somatotopy at cortical level is distinct from that of the other fingers31. However, brain motor synergies controlling the supernumerary finger, not addressed indeed, remain obscure. Even in conventional five-fingered living humans, “peripheral motor synergies” have been extensively investigated biomechanically, kinematically and in terms of electromyographic activity32–34, but very seldom addressed from a central nervous system control’s perspective23,35–38.
The current study is based on the unique opportunity to model the neural adaptation of healthy humans to an augmentative supernumerary thumb. Results show that using this acquired six-fingered hand, emerging bioartificial corticospinal synergies, different from natural ones, may be disclosed. Unlike individuals with polydactyly, that are “pre-wired” in this sense, these emerging synergies should obviously be implemented by the same muscle groups used for controlling natural hand actions, although recruited with different patterns of excitation/inhibition: these patterns can be reflected in the modulation of muscle responses following an invariant TMS pulse as intensity and location (i.e., a navigated supra-threshold stimulus of the contralateral M1).
This is not surprising, according to previous experimental evidences and to some established physiological notions: (1) in monkeys, a motor cortical region containing neurons coding functional synergies for proximal (extrinsic) and distal (intrinsic) muscles has been identified39,40; (2) within M1, neurons controlling individual hand muscles are widely represented and overlap the distribution of neurons controlling other hand muscles41; (3) under physiological conditions (i.e., without supernumerary fingers) the redundancy of the musculoskeletal system allows the motor system to employ a restricted set of modular commands (i.e., synergies) to accomplish both automatic and goal-directed actions42; (4) this strategy is aimed at reducing the dimensionality of motor commands with consequent flexibility and computational brain’s advantages. Such strategy might rely on common yet divergent cortical outputs43 impinged by descending volleys induced by TMS towards spinal motoneurons: these can control either extrinsic hand muscles via polysynaptic connections (with diverging commands being mediated by propriospinal interneurons44,45) or intrinsic ones, via more direct monosynaptic connections46, during imagined grasping with or without the SSF. So, despite physiological synergies are invariant and hard-wired as patterns of activation across muscle groups11, current results suggest that they can immediately be reshaped by adding a new and unexpected bioartificial effector within the motor plan for grasping. Such a flexibility of motor synergies, even beyond the physiological repertoire (as wearing an extra finger), represents a still unexplored capability of the human brain.
More in detail, during rehearsal of the pinch grip between index finger and thumb, the more involved muscles were the two intrinsic hand muscles (Figs. 3a, b, 4a), with a synergistic contribution of the FDS muscle at the forearm, that is agonist for the index finger displacement in flexion (Fig. 5a). Imagined actions of whole hand grasping did not significantly modulate the corticospinal drive towards hand muscles (Fig. 4a), but a clear-cut facilitation of both flexors and extensor muscles at the forearm level occurred (Figs. 3b, c, 4b), either when imagining the grasping with (Aug-grasp) or without the SSF (Nat-grasp). Crucially, during the augmented grasping, the corticospinal drive towards the APB, prime mover for the thumb, did not change versus the resting condition: this also resulted in a significant difference between Nat-Grasp and Aug-grasp (Table S2 and Fig. 1), according to the exclusion of the natural thumb from the planning of the grasping action performed with the aid of the extra finger. Finally, the two resting conditions in which the extra finger was or was not present, did not differ each other: this suggests that robotic finger was able to modify the corticospinal output only when engaged in the planning of an action to be performed.
Planning a grasping action requires the functional cooperation of a wide premotor-parietal network that identifies the target, determines the action goal and the trajectories of hand/object interactions, while coding the peripersonal space in which the forthcoming action will occur46,47 (for recent reviews on the topic see48–50). All these information finally converge into the M1 as an effector area for muscles51. This network is similarly active either during voluntary actions or imagery of the action itself19. The necessary bi-directional communication within the prefrontal-parietal network for action planning relies on associative intracortical horizontal fibers located in the most superficial layers of the cortex, known to be the ones more prone to be activated by a TMS pulse52 that -if slightly suprathreshold- determines the final trans-synaptic firing of the corticospinal cells and the following muscular response53. As a consequence, any functional change occurring in this network (either at premotor or parietal hubs or within M1 itself) will finally modulate the activity of the corticospinal system, even when activated by the TMS pulse instead by the physiological motor command. Current results show a robust and immediate modulation of corticospinal output for hand and forearm muscles in all the experimental imagery conditions (i.e., pinch grip, grasping with or without the SSF), despite these muscles are supplied by physiologically different descending systems in terms of number of synapsis they are controlled by46. Both muscles groups, however, seem to be equally susceptible to the sum of modulating converging inputs coming from the different nodes of the premotor-parietal network, suggesting that the neural computation of corticospinal synergies represents a robust top-down hierarchical mechanism governing both direct monosynaptic and indirect polysynaptic cortico-muscular connections.
Within this framework, however, it should be noted that it is exactly the phylogenetic development of the corticospinal system that has allowed the finest shaping of hand movements, as well as the acquisition of new complex hand motor skills only in those species with fully developed monosynaptic connections for hand muscles control (i.e., from non-human primates onwards)54. This is clearly evident by the selective modulation of FDI and APB intrinsic hand muscles during the imagination of the pinch-grip, in which they act as the main effectors of the precision action. Such a fine modulation of corticospinal output also inherently shows that subjects were indeed able to correctly perform the imagery tasks.
A large part of the premotor-parietal network for actions control in humans is devoted to the representation of the own’s body schema, a hierarchical high-level construct indicating a non-conscious process, continuously updated during movements, through which the individual registers his posture (or body part position) in relation to the peripersonal space55. Body schema is intrinsically highly flexible and adaptable: for example, the level of dexterity in tools use is reached once the tool has been incorporated, or embodied, into the body schema56 and that repeated tool use (as a mechanical grabber extending the space around the arm) may carry after effects on subsequent free-hand grasping and pointing movements, likely altering the individual’s body schema57. Emerging behavioural data indicate that a supernumerary finger, which is conceptually similar to a common tool, can actually be embodied into the user’s body schema6, even when presented as an avatar in virtual reality scenarios58. This implies that the brain may readily adapt to various configurations of the hand and to new action strategies59 at least when they occur within the peripersonal space. The reshaping of corticospinal motor synergies (or plasticity of motor synergies), which would hardly happen without the SSF being incorporated into the own body schema, is the neurophysiological mechanism that could allow such a flexibility.
An intriguing issue to explore in future studies is to directly test the functional connectivity between parietal or premotor hubs of the grasping network with the M1: this could be done by paired, appropriately spaced in time, TMS pulses60,61 delivered in concomitance with different timing of the (imagined) grasping action. Such paired TMS approach (a conditioning stimulus on premotor or parietal targets preceding the test pulse on M1), has been proven useful in verifying the role of the ventral premotor cortex in precision60 or object-driven62 grasping, of the dorsal premotor cortex in planning a grasp63, or in disentangling the role of different sub-regions of the parietal cortex in action planning/execution61,64 or movement observation65. In the frame of such paired-pulse paradigm, with the MEPs simultaneously recorded by several intrinsic and extrinsic upper limb muscles, it could be directly verified whether the observed changes of bioartificial motor synergies depend mainly by cortical (either premotor or parietal) activity upstream to M1, or take place directly at M1 level.
In conclusion, current results show how the human motor system is capable of quickly welcoming augmentative bioartificial corticospinal grasping strategies, in the frame of a still unexplored system-level form of brain plasticity. This ability might represent the functional substrate of a final common pathway the brain might count on towards new interactions with the peripersonal space, once the supernumerary finger has been embodied into the user’s body schema. Understanding these immediate plastic brain changes is a necessary step towards the exploitation of supernumerary fingers in conceptually new home-based rehabilitation settings for grasping compensation or reacquisition of bimanual task cooperation in patients with hand paresis (i.e., post stroke and of other origin)5. More generally, current results disclose the neuroscientific foundations needed for correctly approaching the emerging field of “human augmentation”, a scenario that refers to a new class of wearable technologies aimed to resemble -or even surpass- human limb functions, thereby facilitating the integration of supernumerary fingers -or even arms- with user’s natural abilities2,24.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
Authors thank Dr. Carmelo Smeralda, Mattia Pomes, and Zubar Iqbal for experimental help, and Prof. Patrizio Pasqualetti for fruitful discussions on statistics.
Author contributions
S.R., D.P., G.S.: conceived the study; interpreted results; wrote the paper. F.N., S.R., T.L.B., A.C.: performed experiments; analyzed data; prepared figures. C.S., E.S., M.U.: made critical revisions and helped interpretation. All Authors reviewed the paper.
Funding
The research was partly granted by the EU Project SOFTPRO (Synergy-based Open-source Foundations and Technologies for Prosthetics and RehabilitatiOn), European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program, Grant Agreement No. 688857.
Data availability
All data used to support the findings of this study are included within the article. Additional data is available upon request to the Corresponding Author.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-97876-2.
References
- 1.Beckerle P, et al. A human–robot interaction perspective on assistive and rehabilitation robotics. Front. Neurorobot. 2017 doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2017.00024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Prattichizzo, D., Pozzi, M., Lisini Baldi, T., Malvezzi, M., Rossi, S., & Salvietti, G. Human augmentation by wearable supernumerary robotic limbs: review and perspectives. Progr. Biomed. Eng. in press (2021)
- 3.Bogue R. Exoskeletons and robotic prosthetics: a review of recent developments. Ind. Robot Int. J. 2009;36:421–427. doi: 10.1108/01439910910980141. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hussain I, et al. A magnetic compatible supernumerary robotic finger for functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) acquisitions: Device description and preliminary results. IEEE Int. Conf. Rehabil. Robot. 2017;2017:1177–1182. doi: 10.1109/ICORR.2017.8009409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Salvietti G, et al. Compensating hand function in chronic Stroke patients through the robotic sixth finger. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 2017;25:142–150. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2016.2529684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Segura Meraz N, Sobajima M, Aoyama T, Hasegawa Y. Modification of body schema by use of extra robotic thumb. ROBOMECH J. 2018;5:3. doi: 10.1186/s40648-018-0100-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.d’Avella A, Lacquaniti F. Control of reaching movements by muscle synergy combinations. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 2013 doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Santello M, et al. Hand synergies: Integration of robotics and neuroscience for understanding the control of biological and artificial hands. Phys. Life Rev. 2016;17:1–23. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2016.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Leo A, et al. A synergy-based hand control is encoded in human motor cortical areas. Elife. 2016 doi: 10.7554/eLife.13420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Graziano MSA, Taylor CSR, Moore T, Cooke DF. The cortical control of movement revisited. Neuron. 2002;36:349–362. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01003-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Overduin SA, d’Avella A, Carmena JM, Bizzi E. Microstimulation activates a handful of muscle synergies. Neuron. 2012;76:1071–1077. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hallett M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation: a primer. Neuron. 2007;55:187–199. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gentner R, Classen J. Modular organization of finger movements by the human central nervous system. Neuron. 2006;52:731–742. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cattaneo L, Caruana F, Jezzini A, Rizzolatti G. Representation of goal and movements without overt motor behavior in the human motor cortex: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:11134–11138. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2605-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Lemon RN, Johansson RS, Westling G. Corticospinal control during reach, grasp, and precision lift in man. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:6145–6156. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-09-06145.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Rossi S, Rossini PM. TMS in cognitive plasticity and the potential for rehabilitation. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2004;8:273–279. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Fadiga L, et al. Corticospinal excitability is specifically modulated by motor imagery: a magnetic stimulation study. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:147–158. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00089-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Gerardin E, et al. Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. Cereb. Cortex. 2000;10:1093–1104. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.11.1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hardwick RM, Caspers S, Eickhoff SB, Swinnen SP. Neural correlates of action: Comparing meta-analyses of imagery, observation, and execution. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018;94:31–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lotze M, et al. Activation of cortical and cerebellar motor areas during executed and imagined hand movements: an fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1999;11:491–501. doi: 10.1162/089892999563553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Rossini PM, Rossi S, Pasqualetti P, Tecchio F. Corticospinal excitability modulation to hand muscles during movement imagery. Cereb. Cortex. 1999;9:161–167. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.2.161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fourkas AD, Bonavolontà V, Avenanti A, Aglioti SM. Kinesthetic imagery and tool-specific modulation of corticospinal representations in expert tennis players. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:2382–2390. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Rossi S, et al. Cross-modal audiovisual modulation of corticospinal motor synergies in professional piano players: a TMS study during motor imagery. Neural Plast. 2019;2019:1328453. doi: 10.1155/2019/1328453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Dominijanni, G., Shokur, S., Salvietti, G., Buehler, S., Palmerini, E., Rossi, S., De Vignemont, F., D’Avella, A., Makin, T., Prattichizzo, D., & Micera, S. Enhancing human bodies with extra arms and fingers: a unified vision for engineers and neuroscientists. Nat. Mach. Intell. In press (2021)
- 25.Hussain I, Spagnoletti G, Salvietti G, Prattichizzo D. An EMG interface for the control of motion and compliance of a supernumerary robotic finger. Front. Neurorobot. 2016 doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2016.00018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hussain I, Spagnoletti G, Salvietti G, Prattichizzo D. Toward wearable supernumerary robotic fingers to compensate missing grasping abilities in hemiparetic upper limb. Int. J. Robot. Res. 2017;36:1414–1436. doi: 10.1177/0278364917712433. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A. Screening questionnaire before TMS: an update. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2011;122:1686. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.12.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Rossini PM, et al. Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: Basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. An updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2015;126:1071–1107. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.McCarroll HR. Congenital anomalies: a 25-year overview. J. Hand Surg. Am. 2000;25:1007–1037. doi: 10.1053/jhsu.2000.6457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Mehring C, et al. Augmented manipulation ability in humans with six-fingered hands. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-10306-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.d’Avella A, Portone A, Fernandez L, Lacquaniti F. Control of fast-reaching movements by muscle synergy combinations. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:7791–7810. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0830-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Latash M, Aruin AS, Zatsiorsky VM. The basis of a simple synergy: Reconstruction of joint equilibrium trajectories during unrestrained arm movements. Hum. Mov. Sci. 1999;18:3–30. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(98)00029-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Prattichizzo D, Malvezzi M, Gabiccini M, Bicchi A. On motion and force controllability of precision grasps with hands actuated by soft synergies. IEEE Trans. Robot. 2013;29:1440–1456. doi: 10.1109/TRO.2013.2273849. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.D’Ausilio A, Bartoli E, Maffongelli L. Grasping synergies: a motor-control approach to the mirror neuron mechanism. Phys Life Rev. 2015;12:91–103. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2014.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Devanne H, Cohen LG, Kouchtir-Devanne N, Capaday C. Integrated motor cortical control of task-related muscles during pointing in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;87:3006–3017. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.87.6.3006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ejaz N, Hamada M, Diedrichsen J. Hand use predicts the structure of representations in sensorimotor cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:1034–1040. doi: 10.1038/nn.4038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Mathew J, Kübler A, Bauer R, Gharabaghi A. Probing corticospinal recruitment patterns and functional synergies with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2016;10:175. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2016.00175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Park MC, Belhaj-Saïf A, Gordon M, Cheney PD. Consistent features in the forelimb representation of primary motor cortex in rhesus macaques. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:2784–2792. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-08-02784.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rathelot J-A, Strick PL. Muscle representation in the macaque motor cortex: An anatomical perspective. PNAS. 2006;103:8257–8262. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602933103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Foley J. The co-ordination and regulation of movements: By N. Bernstein, xii + 196 pages, 31 illustrations, 7 tables, Pergamon Press, Oxford, 1967, 50s net; $ 8.00. J. Neurol. Sci. 1968;7:403–404. doi: 10.1016/0022-510X(68)90166-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Schieber MH, Rivlis G. Partial reconstruction of muscle activity from a pruned network of diverse motor cortex neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:70–82. doi: 10.1152/jn.00544.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Isa T, Ohki Y, Alstermark B, Pettersson L-G, Sasaki S. Direct and indirect cortico-motoneuronal pathways and control of hand/arm movements. Physiology (Bethesda) 2007;22:145–152. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00045.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Alstermark B, et al. Motor command for precision grip in the macaque monkey can be mediated by spinal interneurons. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:122–126. doi: 10.1152/jn.00089.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Winges SA, Santello M. Common input to motor units of digit flexors during multi-digit grasping. J. Neurophysiol. 2004;92:3210–3220. doi: 10.1152/jn.00516.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gentner R, et al. Encoding of motor skill in the corticomuscular system of musicians. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:1869–1874. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Georgopoulos AP. Current issues in directional motor control. Trends Neurosci. 1995;18:506–510. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(95)92775-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Andersen RA, Cui H. Intention, action planning, and decision making in parietal-frontal circuits. Neuron. 2009;63:568–583. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gallivan JP, Culham JC. Neural coding within human brain areas involved in actions. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2015;33:141–149. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fattori P, Breveglieri R, Bosco A, Gamberini M, Galletti C. Vision for prehension in the medial parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27:1149–1163. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Russo AA, et al. Motor cortex embeds muscle-like commands in an untangled population response. Neuron. 2018;97:953–966.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Rothwell JC. Techniques and mechanisms of action of transcranial stimulation of the human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. Methods. 1997;74:113–122. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(97)02242-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Di Lazzaro V, et al. The physiological basis of transcranial motor cortex stimulation in conscious humans. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004;115:255–266. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lemon RN, Griffiths J. Comparing the function of the corticospinal system in different species: organizational differences for motor specialization? Muscle Nerve. 2005;32:261–279. doi: 10.1002/mus.20333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Holmes NP, Spence C. The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space. Cogn. Process. 2004;5:94–105. doi: 10.1007/s10339-004-0013-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Shokur S, et al. Expanding the primate body schema in sensorimotor cortex by virtual touches of an avatar. PNAS. 2013;110:15121–15126. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308459110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Cardinali L, et al. Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Curr. Biol. 2009;19:R478–R479. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hoyet L, Argelaguet F, Nicole C, Lécuyer A. “Wow! i have six fingers!”: would you accept structural changes of your hand in VR? Front. Robot. AI. 2016 doi: 10.3389/frobt.2016.00027. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kieliba P, Clode D, Maimon-Mor RO, Makin TR. Robotic hand augmentation drives changes in neural body representation. Sci. Robot. 2021;6:eabd7935. doi: 10.1126/scirobotics.abd7935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Davare M, Lemon R, Olivier E. Selective modulation of interactions between ventral premotor cortex and primary motor cortex during precision grasping in humans. J. Physiol. 2008;586:2735–2742. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.152603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Koch G, et al. In vivo definition of parieto-motor connections involved in planning of grasping movements. Neuroimage. 2010;51:300–312. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Davare M, Montague K, Olivier E, Rothwell JC, Lemon RN. Ventral premotor to primary motor cortical interactions during object-driven grasp in humans. Cortex. 2009;45:1050–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Vesia M, et al. Functional interaction between human dorsal premotor cortex and the ipsilateral primary motor cortex for grasp plans: a dual-site TMS study. NeuroReport. 2018;29:1355–1359. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000001117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Vesia M, et al. Human dorsomedial parieto-motor circuit specifies grasp during the planning of goal-directed hand actions. Cortex. 2017;92:175–186. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Koch G, et al. Resonance of cortico-cortical connections of the motor system with the observation of goal directed grasping movements. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:3513–3520. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data used to support the findings of this study are included within the article. Additional data is available upon request to the Corresponding Author.