Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 Sep 20;10:e68045. doi: 10.7554/eLife.68045

Maturation of Purkinje cell firing properties relies on neurogenesis of excitatory neurons

Meike E van der Heijden 1,2, Elizabeth P Lackey 1,2,3, Ross Perez 2, Fatma S Ișleyen 1,4, Amanda M Brown 1,2,3, Sarah G Donofrio 1,2,3, Tao Lin 1,2, Huda Y Zoghbi 2,3,4,5, Roy V Sillitoe 1,2,3,4,6,
Editors: Vatsala Thirumalai7, K VijayRaghavan8
PMCID: PMC8452305  PMID: 34542409

Abstract

Preterm infants that suffer cerebellar insults often develop motor disorders and cognitive difficulty. Excitatory granule cells, the most numerous neuron type in the brain, are especially vulnerable and likely instigate disease by impairing the function of their targets, the Purkinje cells. Here, we use regional genetic manipulations and in vivo electrophysiology to test whether excitatory neurons establish the firing properties of Purkinje cells during postnatal mouse development. We generated mutant mice that lack the majority of excitatory cerebellar neurons and tracked the structural and functional consequences on Purkinje cells. We reveal that Purkinje cells fail to acquire their typical morphology and connectivity, and that the concomitant transformation of Purkinje cell firing activity does not occur either. We also show that our mutant pups have impaired motor behaviors and vocal skills. These data argue that excitatory cerebellar neurons define the maturation time-window for postnatal Purkinje cell functions and refine cerebellar-dependent behaviors.

Research organism: Mouse

eLife digest

Preterm infants have a higher risk of developing movement difficulties and neurodevelopmental conditions like autism spectrum disorder. This is likely caused by injuries to a part of the brain called the cerebellum. The cerebellum is important for movement, language and social interactions. During the final weeks of pregnancy, the cerebellum grows larger and develops a complex pattern of folds. Tiny granule cells, which are particularly vulnerable to harm, drive this development.

Exactly how damage to granule cells causes movement difficulties and other conditions is unclear. One potential explanation may be that granule cells are important for the development of Purkinje cells in the brain. The Purkinje cells send and receive messages and are very important for coordinating movement.

To learn more, van der Heijden et al. studied Purkinje cells in mice during a period that corresponds with the third trimester of pregnancy in humans. During this time, the pattern of electrical signals sent by the Purkinje cells changed from slow and irregular to fast and rhythmic with long pauses between bursts. However, mice that had been genetically engineered to lack most of their granule cells showed a completely different pattern of Purkinje cell development. The pattern of electrical signals emitted by these Purkinje cells stayed slow and irregular. Mice that lacked granule cells also had movement difficulties, tremors, and abnormal vocalizations.

The experiments confirm that granule cells are essential for normal brain development. Without enough granule cells, the Purkinje cells become stuck in an immature state. This discovery may help physicians identify preterm infants with motor disorders and other conditions earlier. It may also lead to changes in the care of preterm infants designed to protect their granule cells.

Introduction

Abnormal cerebellar development instigates motor diseases and neurodevelopmental disorders including ataxia, dystonia, tremor, and autism. These conditions have a high incidence in premature infants as well as in newborns with cerebellar hemorrhage (Dijkshoorn et al., 2020; Limperopoulos et al., 2007; Steggerda et al., 2009; Zayek et al., 2012). The affected children ultimately attain a smaller cerebellar size compared to children born full-term (Limperopoulos et al., 2005; Volpe, 2009), and they have altered functional connectivity between the cerebellum and the neocortex (Herzmann et al., 2019; Hortensius et al., 2018). Observations from human clinical data indicate a strong correlation between abnormalities in cerebellar size and the subsequent incidence of cognitive disorders, suggesting that the late-term stage of cerebellar development encompasses a critical developmental time-window for establishing not only the motor but also the non-motor circuitry that is required for complex cerebellar functions and behavior (van der Heijden et al., 2021b; Volpe, 2009; Wang et al., 2014).

The cerebellum undergoes a series of dynamic developmental changes during the third trimester and early postnatal stages, a period that corresponds to the first 2 postnatal weeks in mice (Sathyanesan et al., 2019). At the level of gross morphology, the rapid proliferation of granule cell precursors and the integration of granule cells into the cerebellar circuit result in a fivefold enlargement of cerebellar size (Chang et al., 2000), an increase in foliation complexity (Corrales et al., 2006), and the formation of the tri-laminar cerebellar cortex (Miyata et al., 2010). At the cellular level, the differentiation and arrival of mature granule cells into the circuit modifies the laminar localization and synaptic specification of cerebellar afferents that project to Purkinje cells (van der Heijden and Sillitoe, 2021). Despite an extensive literature describing the temporal anatomical changes in the cerebellum and studies postulating that perturbations to the cerebellum during this dynamic period may lead to cognitive, social, and motor deficits (Volpe, 2009; Wang et al., 2014), there is still relatively little known about how the anatomical aspects of cerebellar development correspond to its functional maturation programs that establish animal behavior.

Previous studies show that preterm birth in pigs, as well as postnatal hemorrhage and hypoxia (which mimics cerebellar hemorrhage) in mice, result in decreased granule cell numbers (Iskusnykh et al., 2018; Yoo et al., 2014) and abnormal motor control (Sathyanesan et al., 2018; Yoo et al., 2014). Importantly, postnatal hypoxia causes impairments in the spontaneous firing properties of Purkinje cells in mice (Sathyanesan et al., 2018), and impaired Purkinje cell firing properties are also observed in prematurely born baboons (Barron and Kim, 2020). It is not surprising that decreasing the number of granule cells results in abnormal Purkinje cell function, since each Purkinje cell may integrate inputs from up to two hundred fifty thousand excitatory granule cell synapses (Huang et al., 2014; Napper and Harvey, 1988). Nevertheless, genetic silencing of granule cells causes only modest alterations to the baseline firing properties of Purkinje cells (Galliano et al., 2013), perhaps because the predominant Purkinje cell action potential, called the simple spike, is spontaneously generated by the Purkinje cells (Raman and Bean, 1999). In line with this hypothesis, gross motor control is not affected in mice with impaired granule cell signaling, although they do have impaired motor learning (Galliano et al., 2013). In contrast, some mouse models that lack the majority of granule cells (agranular mice) have such severe motor defects that these mice are named after their phenotype; for example scrambler, weaver, reeler, and staggerer (Bolivar et al., 1996; Falconer, 1951; Heuzé et al., 1997; Le Marec et al., 1997; Sheldon et al., 1997). The discordance between the functional outcomes in mutant mice with reduced granule cell numbers and mice lacking granule cell signaling raises the question whether granule cell neurogenesis, rather than granule cell synaptic signaling, contributes more to the maturation of Purkinje cell firing properties in vivo.

To probe these cellular interactions, we manipulated the mouse cerebellum by genetically blocking neurogenesis of excitatory neurons. We used an En1Cre allele (Wurst et al., 1994) to delete the proneural gene, Atoh1, from the embryonic hindbrain. Atoh1 is necessary for the development of excitatory cerebellar neurons (Ben-Arie et al., 1997; Rose et al., 2009) and is not expressed in the inhibitory Purkinje cells. In this agranular cerebellar model, we test how Purkinje cells develop their structure, connectivity, and function using immunostaining, neural tract tracing, and in vivo electrophysiology performed in the second postnatal week. We also examined motor function and vocal skills in the mutant pups to test how the structural and functional changes in the cerebellum impact the expression of behaviors that are usually acquired during the first two weeks of life.

Results

Atoh1 lineage neurons shape multiple aspects of cerebellar morphogenesis

To test the hypothesis that granule cell neurogenesis is essential for the functional development of Purkinje cells, we first needed to establish an appropriate agranular mouse model. In previously generated models, the mice lack granule cells due to spontaneously occurring mutations in genes with widespread expression patterns (Falconer, 1951; van der Heijden and Sillitoe, 2021; Sheldon et al., 1997). Therefore, one could not differentiate cell-extrinsic from cell-intrinsic effects in Purkinje cells (Dusart et al., 2006; Gold et al., 2007). To impair granule cell neurogenesis in a manner independent of genes expressed in Purkinje cells, we made use of the distinct origins of Purkinje cells and granule cells to establish a new model of agranular mice (Figure 1A–B; Hoshino et al., 2005; Rose et al., 2009). Atoh1 is necessary for the development of excitatory cerebellar neurons, including granule cells (Ben-Arie et al., 1997), which are the most populous cell type in the cerebellum and make up at least 99% of all neurons in the cerebellum (Consalez et al., 2020). En1 is a homeodomain transcription factor that is expressed in the mesencephalon and rhombomere 1 by embryonic day 8 (E8), where it is required for the formation of the cerebellum (Davis and Joyner, 1988; Wurst et al., 1994). The intersection of the Atoh1 and En1 lineages converges on excitatory neurons that originate from the rostral rhombic lip (rRL) (Wang et al., 2005), but does not include inhibitory neurons like Purkinje cells that originate in the ventricular zone (Hoshino et al., 2005). We demonstrate this selectivity by conditional expression of TdTomato, which is expressed by an intersectional reporter allele that is dependent on both Cre- (En1 [Wurst et al., 1994]) and FlpO- (Atoh1 [van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018]) mediated excision of a stop-cassette (Figure 1C). As expected, we observed TdTomato expression in the granule cell layer and molecular layer of the cerebellar cortex, where the axons (parallel fibers) of granule cells reside, but not in the Purkinje cell layer (Figure 1C).

Figure 1. Conditional deletion of Atoh1 from the En1 domain prevents proper size expansion, lamination, and foliation of the cerebellum.

(A) Schematic of an embryonic brain. Inset is the cerebellar anlage. Atoh1 domain (excitatory lineage neurons, including granule cell precursors, pink), Ptf1a domain (inhibitory lineage neurons, including Purkinje cell precursors, green), En1 domain (gray). Orientation is the same for all panels unless otherwise indicated. (B) Schematic of a sagittal section of a P14 cerebellum (left). Cerebellar lobules are labeled with Roman numerals (Larsell, 1952). Schematic of the cerebellar microcircuit with Purkinje cells and granule cells (right). Purkinje cell = green; granule cell = pink; ml = molecular layer, pcl = Purkinje cell layer; gcl = granule cell layer. (C) Intersectional labeling of En1;Atoh1 domain with tdTomato (pink) shows no overlap with Purkinje cells (Calbindin; green). (D) Whole brain images of cerebellum from anterior (left two images) and dorsal view (right two images) from P7 and P14 in control animals. The cerebellum is pseudo-colored in red. (E) Whole brain images of the cerebellum from anterior (left two images) and dorsal views (right two images) from P7 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. The cerebellum is pseudo-colored in red. (F) Outline of cerebellar size in control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice at P7 and P14 showing size expansion in control, but not En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. (G) Sagittal sections of the hindbrain from control mice stained with cresyl violet to visualize gross architecture and regional organization of the brain. (H) Sagittal sections of the hindbrain from En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice stained with cresyl violet showing the general deformities in regional patterning. (I) Outline of cerebellar size in control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice at P7 and P14 showing size expansion in control, but not En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. (J) Sagittal (top) and coronal (bottom) whole section images of cerebella from P7 (left) and P14 (right) control mice. Purkinje cells are stained with Calbindin (green) and DAPI (pink). Note the layered and foliated structure of the cerebellum. (K) Sagittal (top) and coronal (bottom) whole section images of cerebella from P7 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. Purkinje cells are stained with Calbindin (green) and DAPI (pink). All images are presented at the same magnification. Note the lack of layers and foliation. All images in J and K are presented at the same magnification. (L) Outline of cerebellar size in control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice at P7 and P14 showing size expansion in control, but not in the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. All images are representative of N=3 per age and genotype group.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Conditional deletion of Atoh1 from the En1 domain leads to cerebellum-specific morphological abnormalities.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Whole brain sagittal tissue sections from control mice stained using cresyl violet demonstrate the typical anatomical features found throughout the forebrain, midbrain and hindbrain. (B) Whole brain sagittal tissue sections of En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice stained using cresyl violet display cerebellar-specific gross morphological abnormalities. Regions labeled in the control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- brain sections: olfactory bulb, hippocampus, striatum, thalamus, pons, vestibular nuclei, inferior olive, and neocortex. All tissue sections were taken from P14 mice.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Conditional deletion of Atoh1 from the En1 domain reduces the density of excitatory cerebellar cell types, but increases the density of inhibitory cerebellar cell types.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A) En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice lack differentiated granule cells, identified with GABARα6. (B and C) En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have a reduction in unipolar brush cells, identified by Calretinin and Tbr2. (D) En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have dense staining for NFH that marks Purkinje cells and excitatory nuclei (interposed nucleus shown here). (E through H) En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have a high density of inhibitory neurons, revealed with the expression of RORα (E), HCN1 (F), Neurogranin (G), and PV (H). All images are representative for N=3 brains for each genotype and were performed in P14 mice.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Conditional deletion of Atoh1 from the En1 domain reduces the density of En1;Atoh1 lineage neurons.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(A) Genetic strategy to express TdTomato in the En1;Atoh1 lineage of control mice. (B) Genetic strategy to express TdTomato in the En1;Atoh1 lineage of conditional knockout mice. (C) Expression of TdTomato in sagittal tissue sections taken at the midline (vermis) and more medial (para-vermis) regions of cerebella from control (bottom, left) and conditional knockout (top, right) mice. (D) High magnification of TdTomato signal in lobule IX of control mice (i and iii) and the ventro-caudal region of conditional knockout mice (ii and iv) showing signal saturation in control mice and individually countable cells (arrows) in the conditional knockout mice.
Figure 1—figure supplement 4. Conditional deletion of Atoh1 from the En1 domain reduces the number of unipolar brush cells and excitatory nuclei neurons.

Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

(A) Co-expression of Tbr2 (green) and TdTomato (pink) in the En1;Atoh1 lineage (as in Figure 1—figure supplement 3) in control (top) and conditional knockout (bottom) cerebellums. (B) Co-expression of NFH (green) and TdTomato (pink) in the En1;Atoh1 lineage (as in Figure 1—figure supplement 3) in control (top) and conditional knockout (bottom) cerebellums. (C) Whole section image of Tbr2 (grey) expressing cells in cerebellum from control mouse. Cerebellar lobules are labeled with Roman numerals (Larsell, 1952). Insets (i, ii, iii) show lower density of Tbr2+ cells in dorsal lobule VI (i) than in ventral lobules IX (iii) and X (ii). (D) Whole section image of Tbr2 (gray) expressing cells in the cerebellum from an En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mouse. Insets (i, ii) show higher density of Tbr2+ neurons in ventral cerebellum (i) than in dorsal cerebellum (ii). (E) Quantification of Tbr2+ cells (unipolar brush cells) in control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. (F) NFH (pink) and Calbindin (green) staining in the cerebellum of a control mouse. Insets show examples of NFH+/Calbindin- (i, excitatory nuclei cells) and NFH+/Calbindin+ (ii, Purkinje cell). In insets, N = NFH signal; C = Calbindin signal; merge = both. (G) NFH (pink) and Calbindin (green) staining in the cerebellum of an En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mouse. Insets show examples of NFH+/Calbindin- (i, putative excitatory nuclei cell) and NFH+/Calbindin+ (ii, Purkinje cell). In insets, N = NFH signal; C = Calbindin signal; merge = both. (H) Quantification of NFH+/Calbindin- cells (putative excitatory nuclei cells for the mutant) in control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. For (E and H) bar height represents the mean for each genotype (N=3 mice for each). *p<0.05 as determined by a two-tailed, unpaired T-test. The raw data and specific p-values for the comparisons are presented in Figure 1—figure supplement 4—source data 1.
Figure 1—figure supplement 4—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

Our rationale for choosing to impair granule cell neurogenesis by employing a conditional deletion of Atoh1 from the En1 domain was because Atoh1 null mice are neonatal lethal (Ben-Arie et al., 1997). We generated conditional knockout mice by crossing mice that are heterozygous for the En1Cre knock-in allele (Wurst et al., 1994) and heterozygous for Atoh1 with mice that are homozygous for a LoxP-flanked Atoh1 allele (Atoh1fl/fl) (Shroyer et al., 2007). From this cross, we obtain control mice with one of the following three genotypes: Atoh1fl/+ mice, Atoh1FlpO/fl mice, En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+ mice. The different control alleles were analyzed separately for the behavioral data and subsequently grouped together for anatomical and electrophysiological analyses due to the absence of any significant behavioral differences. In addition, the cross generates our experimental En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/FlpO mice that we will hereafter refer to as En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice because the Atoh1FlpO is a functional null-allele (van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018). We previously showed that these En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice are informative for assessing the period of dynamic cerebellar development (P7-P14) but the animals succumb at around weaning age (P21), likely due to a combination of impaired motor coordination and breathing abnormalities (van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018). Using these mice, we have now investigated how the En1-mediated deletion of Atoh1 influences the period of cerebellar growth that usually occurs in the second postnatal week. We found that cerebellar size rapidly expands between P7 and P14 in control mice (Figure 1D, cerebellum is pseudo-colored in red) but that no such size-expansion occurs in the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (Figure 1E). When we overlay the outlines of the cerebellum, we furthermore observe that the size of the cerebellum in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice is markedly smaller at both P7 and P14 compared to that of similar aged controls (Figure 1F). In sagittal tissue sections cut through the hindbrain and then stained with cresyl violet to visualize cell nuclei, we also observe that P7 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- cerebella lack a densely packed granule cell layer, that En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- cerebella do not form cerebellar lobules, and that there is no substantial increase in cerebellar size that takes place during the second postnatal week, which are morphological milestones typically observed in control mice (Figure 1G–I). At P14, sagittal cerebellar tissue sections from En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice measure just 11.3% (±1%, n=12 sets) the size of paired control tissue sections and in addition, coronal-cut cerebellar tissue sections from En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice measure just 20.2% (±3%, n=6 sets) the size of paired control tissue sections. Moreover, as expected based on the expression patterns of the two genes we used in our manipulation, the deletion of Atoh1 from just the En1 domain did not result in any gross morphological abnormalities in brain regions other than the cerebellum, including the olfactory bulb, hippocampus, striatum, thalamus, pons, vestibular nuclei, inferior olive, and neocortex (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

Previous studies on agranular animal models have reported a spectrum of morphological changes in the cerebellum, depending on the degree of granule cell loss; a range of cerebellar size differences compared to controls, thinner to completely absent granule cell layers, and fewer or complete lack of cerebellar foliation (compared to controls, the cerebellum in X-irradiated rats has: smaller size, thinner granule cell layer [Armstrong and Hawkes, 2001; Zanjani et al., 1992]; staggerer and weaver mice: smaller cerebellar size, thinner granule cell layer, less foliation [Armstrong and Hawkes, 2001; Zanjani et al., 1992]; reeler and scrambler mice: smaller cerebellar size, thinner granule cell layer, no foliation [Goldowitz et al., 1997]; Atoh1 chimera mice: smaller cerebellar size, thinner/no granule cell layer, less/no foliation [the phenotype is variable depending on the percentage of chimerism; Jensen et al., 2004]). Our En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice encompass all these malformations, without the variability that was observed in Atoh1 chimera mice (Jensen et al., 2004) or the gross abnormalities observed outside of the cerebellum that was reported for the reeler and scrambler mice (Goldowitz et al., 1997). Therefore, our En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice encompass all of the morphological changes observed in previous agranular models, although at the more extreme end of the severity spectrum, with little experimental variability and high regional specificity.

Atoh1 deletion reduces the number of excitatory lineage neurons in the cerebellum

Next, we used immunohistochemistry to investigate which of the main cellular components of the cerebellum is affected in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. Based on the intersectional strategy we employed in our studies, we would expect a reduction in excitatory lineage cells with minimal changes to inhibitory neurons, including Purkinje cells. We show that deletion of Atoh1 prevents granule cells from reaching differentiation by staining for GABARα6, which marks mature granule cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 1–2A). However, it remained unclear whether this change in signal was due to abnormal maturation or impaired granule cell neurogenesis. To address this question, we decided to employ a genetic strategy to label all En1;Atoh1 lineage neurons in control and conditional knockout mice with an intersectional TdTomato-reporter allele (Figure 1—figure supplement 1–3A–B). In this strategy, control animals are En1Cre/+;Atoh1FlpO/+;Rosalsl-lfl-TdTomato/+ mice (as in Figure 1C) and conditional knockout mice are En1Cre/+;Atoh1FlpO/fl;Rosalsl-lfl-TdTomato/+ mice. Within the granule cell layer of control mice, the TdTomato+ signal saturates due to the dense packing of granule cell bodies. This leads to a signal density of 99.6% (± 0.1%, N=2, n=10) within the granule cell layer of lobule IX in control mice (En1Cre/+;Atoh1FlpO/+;Rosalsl-lfl-TdTomato/+), compared to just 43.4% (± 3.5%, N=3, n=15) in the ventral-caudal regions of cerebella in conditional knockout mice (En1Cre/+;Atoh1FlpO/fl;Rosalsl-lfl-TdTomato/+) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1–3D). Despite the mainly qualitative utility of these data, this reduced signal density does not provide direct numerical insight in the quantity of En1;Atoh1 lineage neurons that escape our genetic manipulation because it does not account for the origins of the signal saturation, the nearly 50-fold size difference between control and conditional knockout cerebella, or the difference in cellular compartments present in the area of interest (only cell bodies and dendrites in control mice, versus cell bodies, dendrites, and axons in conditional knockout mice). Unfortunately, the signal density in the control animals prevented us from manually counting individual En1;Atoh1 lineage neurons in control mice, but we were able to count the total number of TdTomato+, En1;Atoh1 lineage neurons in sagittal tissue sections from conditional knockout mice. We found an average of 925 (± 22, N=3, n=12) TdTomato+ cells in each tissue section (Figure 1—figure supplement 1–3A–BC–D). We usually obtain ~70 sagittal sections from a single conditional knockout mouse brain (N=5), leading us to estimate that the total number of En1;Atoh1 lineage cells that escape the conditional knockout of Atoh1 from the En1 lineage to be in the range of 65 • 103 (/thousand) neurons per mutant mouse brain. The total number of cerebellar neurons (99% of which are granule cells) in control mice has previously been estimated in the range of millions (between 35 • 106 and 70 • 106 neurons [Consalez et al., 2020; Herculano-Houzel et al., 2006; Surchev et al., 2007; Vogel et al., 1989]). We therefore estimate an approximate 103 (thousand)-fold reduction in the total number of En1;Atoh1 lineage neurons in our mutant mice, or less than 1% of the En1;Atoh1 lineage neurons escape the genetic manipulation.

Consistent with the substantially reduced number of En1;Atoh1 lineage neurons in the conditional knockout mice, we observe that En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- conditional knockout mice have a reduced pool of excitatory unipolar brush cells, which also derive from the En1;Atoh1 lineage (Figure 1—figure supplement 1–4A), compared to the density of unipolar brush cells that are localized to lobule IX and X in control mice (Figure 1—figure supplement 2B–D). When we counted the number Tbr2+unipolar brush cells in control and conditional knockout mice, we found an approximately eight-fold reduction in the number of unipolar brush cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 4C–E). Interestingly, none of the Tbr2+ escaper unipolar brush cells in En1Cre/+;Atoh1FlpO/fl;Rosalsl-lfl-TdTomato/+ conditional knockout mice express the TdTomato reporter (Figure 1—figure supplement 4A), suggesting that these cells may derive from a different sub-lineage. We also counted the number of excitatory cerebellar nuclei neurons, that derive from the En1;Atoh1 lineage (Figure 1—figure supplement 4B) and found an approximate fivefold reduction in their number (Figure 1—figure supplement 4F–H). Unlike the escaper Tbr2+ unipolar brush cells, cerebellar nuclei neurons in the conditional knockout mice express the TdTomato reporter, but the NFH signal appears more fibrous. Taken together, we find a significant reduction in the total number of all types of excitatory neurons that are derived from the En1;Atoh1 lineage. We also observed an abnormal localization of GABARα6 expression in granule cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 2A) and NFH expression in excitatory nuclei neurons (Figure 1—figure supplement 4F–G), which is an interesting molecular response to progressing this far into development without Atoh1.

To confirm that our genetic strategy did not prevent the development of inhibitory neurons, specifically Purkinje cells, we stained control cerebella using a combination of Calbindin, which marks Purkinje cells, and DAPI, which binds to DNA and ubiquitously marks cell nuclei. In sagittal and coronal tissue sections cut through the cerebella from control mice, the staining again highlights the increase in size between P7 and P14, as well as the formation of perfectly delineated dorsal-ventral layering of cells and the establishment of the characteristic folding that defines the cerebellar lobules (Figure 1J). These morphological features are not evident in the cerebellum of the mutant mice. In addition to the easily visualized difference in the pattern of rostra-caudal lobules and reduced cerebellar size, the mediolateral morphological divisions of the cerebellum are not apparent and lamination is almost non-existent in P7 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutant mice (Figure 1K). In En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice, we could identify Purkinje cells (Calbindin+ cells, Figure 1K), confirming that conditional deletion of Atoh1 does not block Purkinje cell neurogenesis. However, the cellular organization of Purkinje cells in cerebella from En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice is markedly different from controls. We confirmed the stunted cerebellar size and lack of lobule complexity by comparing the outline of cerebella from control to En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (Figure 1L). In line with our conditional knockout that targets neurons in the excitatory lineage, we further found that the representation of different classes of inhibitory neurons, including molecular layer interneurons, basket cells, and Golgi cells, remains robust and clearly detected by cell-type-specific markers (Figure 1—figure supplement 2E–H). These data indicate that there are simultaneously occurring alterations in cerebellar morphology and cytoarchitecture in the mutant mice, both owing to the principal defect, which is the elimination of excitatory neurons.

Excitatory cerebellar neurons guide the transformation of cerebellar clusters into zones

One of the hallmarks of cerebellar circuit organization are the longitudinal zones, or modules, that have a specific pattern and function (Apps et al., 2018). While Purkinje cells differentiate into distinct molecular subtypes shortly after birth (Wassef et al., 1990), they initially group together into cellular clusters and only form the more precise and sharply delineated longitudinal zones as the cerebellum greatly expands in size and volume (Dastjerdi et al., 2012; Larouche and Hawkes, 2006). Here, we investigated whether Purkinje cell molecular subtypes in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice form distinct zones. As expected, at P14, ZebrinII+ and PLCβ4+ expressing Purkinje cells in control mice are organized in alternating zones (Figure 2A–G). In contrast, ZebrinII+ and PLCβ4+ expression patterns in the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- cerebellum are organized into a series of clusters rather than sharp zones. The overall pattern of clusters in the mutants resembles the architecture that is typically observed in normal neonates (Figure 2H–N; Fujita et al., 2012; Sugihara and Fujita, 2013). Some Purkinje cells, here marked with PLCβ4+, over-migrate beyond the boundaries of the cerebellum and into the inferior colliculus that is immediately adjacent (arrowheads Figure 2K). We also observed co-expression of ZebrinII+ and PLCβ4+ in the same clusters of cells in the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (arrowheads Figure 2M–N). In normal mice, postnatal ZebrinII+ expression sweeps over the whole cerebellum becoming expressed in all Purkinje cells by around P10, before it segments into zones by ~P15 (Tano et al., 1992). The prevalent overlap in what are typically complementary markers at P14 in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice supports the hypothesis of an altered temporal progression of cerebellar development that has affected Purkinje cell patterning.

Figure 2. Atoh1 lineage neurons are necessary for the formation of sharp zonal patterns of Purkinje cell sub-types and spinal cord mossy fiber projections.

Figure 2.

(A) Top view of a control P14 brain. Dotted line shows the position of the schematic in (B) and where the section in (C) was taken from. (B) Schematic of Purkinje cell ZebrinII (pink) and PLCβ4 (green) patterns in a control section illustrating the striped patterns at P14. (C) Staining of ZebrinII (pink) and PLCβ4 (green). (D-G) Higher power magnification images of insets in (C, H). Top view of En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- P14 brain. Dotted line shows position of schematic in (I) and where the section in (J) was taken from. (I) Schematic of Purkinje cell ZebrinII (pink) and PLCβ4 (green) patterns in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice showing a clustered pattern at P14. (J) Staining of ZebrinII (pink) and PLCβ4 (green). (K-N) Higher power magnification images of insets in (J) Arrowhead in K: over-migrated Purkinje cells in the inferior colliculus. Arrowhead in M and N: Purkinje cells co-expressing ZebrinII and PLCβ4. (O) Schematic of ZebrinII staining pattern and WGA-Alexa 555 tracing form the spinal cord to the cerebellum in control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. (P) Representative images of WGA-Alexa 555+ terminals (left: gray; right: pink) in the cerebellum of a control mouse. Dotted lines represent the border between ZebrinII+ (cyan) and ZebrinII- regions. Purkinje cell stained with Calbindin (green) ZebrinII is shown in dark blue. The overlap between Calbindin and ZebrinII is shown cyan. (Q) Representative images of WGA-Alexa 555+ terminals (left: gray; right: pink) in the cerebellum of an En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mouse. Dotted lines represent the border between ZebrinII+ (cyan) and ZebrinII- regions. Purkinje cell stained with Calbindin (green). All images are representative of N=3 brains per genotype.

The zonal organization of Purkinje cells is also essential for the formation of patterned synaptic inputs. For example, spinal cord mossy fibers send their projections to the granular layer below ZebrinII- zones (Sillitoe et al., 2010). During development, these projections directly target Purkinje cells (Kalinovsky et al., 2011; Sillitoe, 2016), but with the integration of granule cells into the circuit, mossy fibers displace their synapses onto granule cells (Arsénio Nunes et al., 1988; Pan et al., 2009). To test how this sequence of events was impacted by the lack of granule cells in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice, we injected the anterograde tracer WGA-Alexa 555 into the lower thoracic-upper lumbar spinal cord of P12 mice (Gebre et al., 2012; Lackey and Sillitoe, 2020). We observed WGA-Alexa 555 labeled fibers and terminals at P14 after two days of tracer transport. In controls, mossy fibers project to the cerebellar cortex in a zonal pattern that reflects the topography of ZebrinII (Figure 2O–P; Brochu et al., 1990; Sillitoe and Hawkes, 2002). As in the controls, we found that spinocerebellar mossy fibers projected mainly to ZebrinII- domains in the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. However, in controls the mossy fibers occupy the domains in the granular layer that are located directly below the Purkinje cell zones, whereas in the mutants the mossy fibers remain intermingled within Purkinje cell clusters (Figure 2O–Q). These results confirm that excitatory cerebellar neurons are not essential for initiating the zonal topography of mossy fiber projections that arise from the spinal cord. However, the organization of mossy fibers into poorly defined clusters in the mutants indicates that integration of excitatory neurons into the circuit is important for the architectural rearrangement and the refinement of both the Purkinje cells and afferents into zones.

Excitatory cerebellar neurons promote Purkinje cell dendritic complexity

Previous studies suggest that decreasing excitatory input alters Purkinje cell dendrite outgrowth (Bradley and Berry, 1976; Pan et al., 2009; Park et al., 2019; Takeo et al., 2021), which results in abnormal dendrite morphology in agranular mice (Sotelo and Dusart, 2009). Therefore, we next investigated the cellular morphology of Purkinje cells in control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice by staining mouse brains using a modified Golgi-Cox method. We found that Purkinje cells in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice had stunted and smaller dendritic arbors compared to the controls (Figure 3A), and unlike in the control cerebella, neighboring Purkinje cells did not orient their arbors in the same direction (Figure 3A). Sholl analysis revealed that the Purkinje cells in the mutants were smaller with less bifurcated dendritic branches (Figure 3B–C). Furthermore, spine density was significantly lower in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice compared to the control mice (Figure 3D–E), which is in agreement with previous work showing that dendrite outgrowth is dependent on the formation of excitatory synapses, the majority of which are contributed from granule cell parallel fibers in control animals (Bradley and Berry, 1976; Pan et al., 2009; Park et al., 2019; Takeo et al., 2021). From these data, we conclude that Purkinje cells in the P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have less morphological complexity. This shows that in addition to causing gross morphological impairments of the cerebellum, loss of Atoh1 and a reduced number of excitatory cerebellar neurons also negatively affects the cellular level morphology of neurons.

Figure 3. Atoh1 lineage neurons are necessary for establishing the dendritic complexity of Purkinje cells.

Figure 3.

(A) Representative images of Golgi-Cox-labeled Purkinje cells in the cerebella of control (top row) and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- (bottom row) mice. (B) Sholl analysis for dendritic complexity. (C) Purkinje cells in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have shorter and less branched Purkinje cell dendrites (n=30/N=3 for each genotype, each animal is indicated with a differentially oriented triangle). (D) Golgi stains showing dendritic spines for control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (examples indicated by green arrowheads). (E) En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice contain a significantly lower number of dendritic spines compared to control mice. *p<0.001 for spine density (n=15/N=3 for each genotype, each animal is indicated with a differentially oriented triangle). Linear mixed model with genotype as the fixed effect and mouse number as the random effect. *p<0.001 for both the distance from the soma, the branch number, and spine density. All images were acquired from the cerebella of P14 mice. The raw data and specific p-values for the comparisons are presented in Figure 3—source data 1.

Figure 3—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 3.

The cerebellar cortex of En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutant mice is largely devoid of Vglut1 synapses

Next, we addressed whether the large reduction of excitatory cerebellar neurons changes the composition of cerebellar cortical afferents. First, we looked how the density of Vglut1+ synapses on Purkinje cells changed during the dynamic period. From P7 to P14, a large population of excitatory (Vglut1+) granule cells integrate into the cerebellar circuit and make direct contacts to Purkinje cells via their parallel fibers in the molecular layer (Figure 4A). While some mossy fibers are also Vglut1+, they do not directly contact mature Purkinje cells in control mice but instead target granule cells in the granule cell layer. Therefore, we only quantified the density of Vglut1+ synapses in the molecular and Purkinje cell layer. We saw an increase in the density of Vglut1+ synapses in the Purkinje cell layer and molecular layer from P7 to P14 in control mice (Figure 4B, top row). Because we impaired the neurogenesis of Vglut1-expressing granule cells, we expected the density of Vglut1+ synapses to be lower in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice and not increase between P7 and P14. Indeed, we saw that the signal of Vglut1+ synapses in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice did not change between P7 and P14 (Figure 4B, bottom row) and quantification showed that this signal was significantly lower at both time points in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice compared to age-matched control mice (Figure 4C). The remaining Vglut1+ synapses in the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice likely originate from mossy fibers (see Figure 2), and the small number of granule cells and unipolar brush cells that escaped our genetic manipulation of rRL-derived cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 2, Figure 1—figure supplement 3, and Figure 1—figure supplement 4). However, the approximately nine-fold reduction in Vglut1+ signal in the cerebella of En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice further confirms that our conditional knockout strategy impairs the integration of granule cells into the cerebellar circuit. Taken together, we show that without excitatory cerebellar neurons, the density of Vglut1+ excitatory synapses in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice is much lower when assessed on individual Purkinje cells (Figure 3D–E) as well as throughout the morphogenetically abnormal mutant cerebellum at large (Figure 4C).

Figure 4. The density of Vglut1 synapses onto Purkinje cells increases between P7 and P14 in control mice and is lower in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice.

Figure 4.

(A) Schematic of Purkinje cell microcircuit in control (left) and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (right) at P7 and P14. Vglut1 is expressed in granule cell (pink) and some mossy fiber (gray) terminals but not in Purkinje cells (green) or climbing fiber (orange) terminals. Abbreviations: ml = molecular layer; pcl = Purkinje cell layer; gcl = granule cell layer. (B) Representative images for Vglut1 density in the cerebellum of control (top) and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (bottom) at P7 and P14. Left panels are gray-scale images of the Vglut1 signal and right images show the merged images of the Vglut1 (pink) and Calbindin (green) signals. (C) Density of Vglut1 synapses as a percentage of area of interest (AOI; AOI comprises the molecular and Purkinje cell layer in cerebella from control animals and the area occupied by Purkinje cells in the cerebella of En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice). Significance was established based on the average of each mouse (N=3 mice per genotype, large circles; n=3–5 tissue sections, small dots) using a two-way ANOVA (genotype*age) followed by a Tukey Kramer post-hoc analysis. *p=0.05. The raw data and specific P-values for the comparisons are presented in Figure 4—source data 1.

Figure 4—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 4.

Excitatory cerebellar neurons shape Purkinje cell simple spike firing patterns

Purkinje cells have a distinct firing profile characterized by spontaneously generated simple spikes and climbing fiber-induced complex spikes. In vitro recordings in rats (Crepel, 1972; McKay and Turner, 2005) and mice (Beekhof et al., 2021) show that Purkinje cell firing properties change significantly during early postnatal development (P7-P14), but it is unclear how firing patterns evolve in vivo during this dynamic period of rewiring. Therefore, we set out to answer two questions: first, how do the in vivo Purkinje cell firing patterns change during the dynamic period? Second, is Purkinje cell firing affected in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice that have a reduced number of Vglut1+ excitatory inputs? We hypothesized that the reduced number of Vglut1+ excitatory inputs affects the modulation of simple spike firing rate. Based on the time points we tested, and in line with the highest Vglut1+ density in P14 control mice, Purkinje cells recorded from these mice had the highest peak firing frequency, whereas Purkinje cells in P7 control mice had an overall lower firing rate (Figure 5A–B). Interestingly, Purkinje cells in P14 controls fired in burst-like patterns that have unique powerfully represented features in their firing frequency and frequency mode (or preferred frequency) (Figure 5B and D), whereas Purkinje cells in P7 control mice fired more continuously, yet at a lower frequency and with higher variability in their inter-spike intervals (Figure 5A and E). We quantified the firing features with six parameters: frequency (total spikes/recording time; Figure 5F), frequency mode (most frequently observed frequency; Figure 5G), CV (a measure for global regularity; Figure 5J), pause percentage (defined as a discrete portion of the trace where no spikes were observed; Figure 5K), rhythmicity index (the oscillatory properties in the auto-correlation of spike times; Figure 5L–N), and CV2 (inter-spike interval irregularity; Figure 5O; Holt et al., 1996). Specifically, a faster firing pattern is represented as a higher frequency and frequency mode (Figure 5D–G). A more burst-like, or globally irregular, firing pattern (Figure 5H–I) can be observed as a larger difference between the observed frequency and frequency mode (Figure 5D–G), a higher CV (Figure 5J), and a larger pause proportion (Figure 5K). Additionally, the higher inter-spike interval (local) irregularity is reflected as a lower rhythmicity index (Figure 5L–N) and higher CV2 (Figure 5O).

Figure 5. Purkinje cell simple spike firing patterns transform from P7 to P14 in control mice and reflect altered maturation in P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice.

(A) Schematic of in vivo Purkinje cell recordings in P7 control mouse. Top left: cerebellum with recording electrode. Top right: schematic with recording electrode extracellular to Purkinje cell body. Bottom: representative 15 s recording from a Purkinje cell, each large vertical line is an action potential. (B) Same as (A) For P14 control mouse. (C) Same as (A) for P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mouse. (D) and (E) Examples of frequency (interspike interval, ISI−1) distributions of simple spike firing rate in a P14 Purkinje cell recorded in a control (D) and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- (E) mouse. Blue line indicates the ‘frequency’ as calculate by the total number of simple spikes/recording time (spikes/s). Red lines indicate ‘frequency mode’ as the frequency most commonly observed in the frequency distribution. (F) Simple spike firing frequency (spikes/recording time). (G) Simple spike frequency mode (peak ISI−1 distribution). (H) and (I) example recordings (25 s) from P14 Purkinje cells in control (H) and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- (I) mouse. The top is the recording. Bottom is the firing frequency averaged over one second. (J) Simple spike CV (global firing irregularity). (K) Pause percentage (proportion of recording with ISI > five times average ISI). (L) and (M) Examples of auto-correlograms of the simple spike ISI in a P14 Purkinje cell recorded in a control (L) and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- (M) mouse. (N) Simple spike rhythmicity index calculated based on auto-correlogram. (O) Simple spike CV2 (local firing irregularity). For F,G, J, K, N, and O, significance was determined using an ANOVA between the four control age groups and two En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- age groups followed by a Tukey-Kramer post-hoc analysis to assess differences between the groups. Significance was accepted at p<0.05. In the figure, the statistical significance between two groups is indicated as a line starting and ending above the two groups. Gray lines indicate differences within control groups of different ages and orange lines indicate differences between control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- groups. N/n-numbers: control: P7-8: n=22 cells from N=13 mice; control P9-10: n=30/N=12; control P11-12: n=35/N=11; control P13-14: n=32/N=14; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- P10: n=15/N=3; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- P14: n=15/N=6. The raw data and specific p-values for all the comparisons are presented in Figure 5—source data 1.

Figure 5—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figures 5 and 6.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Simple spike distributions of single Purkinje cells.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Heatmap of frequency distribution of simple spike firing patterns in Purkinje cells of different ages in control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice.

We calculated these parameters for Purkinje cells recorded from control mice P7-8, P9-10, P11-12, and P13-14. We found that there is a statistically significant increase in frequency and frequency mode from P7-10 to P11-14 (Figure 5F–G). We also found a statistically significant increase in CV and pause proportion from P7-12 to P13-14 (Figure 5J–K). In addition, there was an increase in the rhythmicity index from P7-10 to P11-14 (Figure 5N), but no change in the CV2 (Figure 5O). Together, these results suggest that there are changes in the firing patterns that occur between P7-10 and P13-14, and that these changes in activity involve a higher firing frequency, with more burst-like firing patterns, and a higher rhythmicity within the burst. The changes in firing patterns are further visualized in the firing frequency distributions for each single cell that we recorded for this study (Figure 5—figure supplement 1).

Next, we set out to quantify the firing patterns in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants (Figure 5C). We decided to measure spontaneous Purkinje cell signals at P10 and P14, because P9-10 control cells were never statistically different from P7-8 in any of the parameters included in our study and because P13-14 control cells were different from P7-10 control cells in five out of six parameters we included. We found that spontaneous simple spikes in Purkinje cells recorded in P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants had a lower frequency and frequency mode than those recorded in Purkinje cells in P11-14 control mice (Figure 5D–G). We also found that the CV and pause proportion was significantly lower in Purkinje cells recorded from P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants compared to the P13-14 control Purkinje cells (Figure 5H–K). In addition, we found that Purkinje cells recorded from P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants had a lower rhythmicity index than the P11-14 control Purkinje cells (Figure 5L–N) and a higher CV2 than all control Purkinje cells (Figure 5O). Finally, we found that Purkinje cell firing patterns in P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants never differed from each other in any of the six parameters that we included in our analysis. This observation is further supported by the similarity in firing frequency distributions of the single Purkinje cell recordings in P10 and in P14 in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Together, these results suggest that the changes in Purkinje firing patterns that occur between P7-10 and P13-14 in control animals does not occur in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants, and that Purkinje cells in P10 and in P14 in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants have a firing pattern that is highly similar to Purkinje cells in the P7-10 control mice.

An electrophysiological pseudo-timeline uncovers a developmental transformation in Purkinje cell firing properties

Based on the six parameters quantified in Figure 5, P14 Purkinje cells in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice appear more dissimilar from their age-matched controls than younger Purkinje cells in control mice. This is further supported when looking at representative traces from Purkinje cells recorded in P7 to P14 control mice and P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (Figure 6A). However, no single parameter alone can fully describe the complex temporal dynamics of the transformation that occurs in Purkinje cell firing patterns during the second postnatal week. Therefore, we used several mathematical approaches to compare the firing patterns of the 149 single neuron recordings, based on all six firing properties described in Figure 5. First, we performed an unbiased cluster analysis that groups together neurons that have the most similar firing properties (Figure 6B). When we retrospectively label the age and genotype of each cell after unbiased clustering, we see that older control cells cluster together (P11-14 in difference shades of green, clustered at the top) and younger control cells cluster together (P7-10 in different shades of purple, cluster at the bottom). Interestingly, Purkinje cells from both P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice cluster with the young control cells (orange, at the bottom) (Figure 6B). Second, we performed t-distributed stochastic neighbor embedding (t-SNE) analysis on the aforementioned firing parameters (Figure 6C). After retrospectively color-coding the data points, we present a pseudo-timeline of the developmental transformation of firing patterns, with Purkinje cells from both P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice clustered with the young (P7-10) control cells in the bottom right and older (P11-14) control cells on clustered together on the top left (Figure 6C). Both the unbiased cluster and the tSNE analysis show some cells in an intermediate state (between young and old) (Figure 6B–C, middle cells). This suggests the existence of a transformation that occurs between younger and older cells, but the transformation does not occur at the same time for all Purkinje cells, as is expected based on previous studies showing that the heterogeneity of Purkinje cell function is an inherent property of Purkinje cells that respects and spans the timelines of anatomical and functional development of Purkinje cells (Beekhof et al., 2021; McKay and Turner, 2005; van Welie et al., 2011). To further test whether there are general changes that occur at different time points or a transformation that occurs at a discrete age, we averaged the firing properties of all cells recorded in each group, allowing us to test for the relative contribution of specific firing properties in each age group, and performed an unbiased, hierarchical cluster analysis on the first two principal components of the averaged firing properties (Figure 6D). We find that although the young control cells (P7-10) are most similar to themselves and most dissimilar from older control cells (P11-14), there is a level of variability that likely reflects systematic differences for when the transformation occurs across different Purkinje cells. In agreement with our aforementioned findings, we found that the P10 and P14 Purkinje cells recorded in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice cluster with the young control cells (P7-10) and do not undergo the functional transformation in firing patterns that Purkinje cells in control mice do. Taken together, in a period of dynamic circuit development, Purkinje cells undergo a major developmental transformation in firing patterns between P7-10 and P11-14. Furthermore, in agreement with the absence of change in gross cerebellar morphology or dynamics in Vglut1+ synapses, this developmental transformation does not occur between P10 and P14 in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice.

Figure 6. Purkinje cell simple spike firing patterns undergo a developmental transformation between P7-P10 and P11-P14 that does not occur in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice.

(A) Representative electrophysiological recordings (each 10 s long) from Purkinje cells recorded in P7-P14 control mice and P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. In some recordings, we observed action potentials from a second cells in the background, typically observed as spikes with a smaller amplitude (for example, in the representative P12 trace). We only include the large amplitude spikes of the single predominant cell in our analysis. (B) Unbiased clusterogram based on the firing parameters summarized in Figure 5. Each row represents the Z-scored firing parameters of a single neuron. The rows were retroactively color-coded according to the colors in panel A. (C) tSNE analysis of the firing patterns summarized in Figure 5. (D) Unbiased cluster analysis on average firing patterns of Purkinje cells recorded in P7-P14 control mice and P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Simple spike firing properties do not normalize by P18 in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(A) Simple spike firing frequency (spikes/recording time). (B) Simple spike frequency mode (peak ISI−1 distribution). (C) Pause percentage (proportion of recording with ISI > five times average ISI). (D) Simple spike rhythmicity index calculated based on auto-correlogram. (E) Simple spike CV (global firing irregularity). (F) Simple spike CV2 (local firing irregularity). (G) Unbiased clusterogram based on the firing parameters summarized in Figure 5 and A-F. Each row represents the Z-scored firing parameters of a single neuron. The rows were retroactively color-coded according to the colors in panel of the legend. (H) tSNE analysis of the firing patterns summarized in Figure 5 and A-F. (I) Unbiased cluster analysis on average firing patterns of Purkinje cells recorded in P7-P14 and P18 control mice, and P10, P14, and P18 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. For A-F, significance was determined using a t-test between Purkinje cells in P18 control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. Significance was accepted at p<0.05. N/n-numbers: control P18: n=17 cells from N=4 mice; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- P18: n=15/N=3. The raw data and specific p-values for all the comparisons are presented in Figure 5—source data 1.

Next, we tested whether the altered neural activity reflects a delay in maturation of the firing patterns. We therefore recorded Purkinje cells in P18 control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice, which is a week after the changes in the electrophysiological signature occur in control mice and the latest timepoint at which we are confident that the general health of the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice does not confound the quality of our recordings. We find that at P18, Purkinje cells recorded in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice still exhibit a lower firing frequency (Figure 6—figure supplement 1A) and preferred firing frequency (Figure 6—figure supplement 1A), (Figure 6—figure supplement 1B), no difference in pause proportion (Figure 6—figure supplement 1C), lower rhythmicity index (Figure 6—figure supplement 1D), lower global firing rate variability (Figure 6—figure supplement 1E), and higher local firing rate variability (Figure 6—figure supplement 1F) compared to controls. We then reran the unbiased clustering analysis and tSNE analysis on all 179 single-cell recordings, and the hierarchical cluster analysis on the all group means to investigate whether P18 Purkinje cells in control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice more closely relate to Purkinje cells at the start (P7-10) or end (P11-14) of the dynamic developmental period. We found that Purkinje cells recorded in P18 control mice cluster with Purkinje cells recorded in P11-14 control mice, but Purkinje cells recorded in P18 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice cluster with the younger Purkinje cells that were recorded at P7-10 in control mice, which were also similar to Purkinje cells recorded in P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (Figure 6—figure supplement 1G–I). These analyses further underscore that developing Purkinje cells rely on the neurogenesis of their surrounding excitatory neurons to make a dynamic functional transformation in their firing properties and that Purkinje cells developing in an environment largely devoid of granule cells are maintained in a functionally immature state, rather than acquiring a new, pathophysiological firing pattern.

Excitatory cerebellar neurons are necessary for pruning away excess Vglut2 synapses from Purkinje cells

Our results argue that excitatory neurons are essential for shaping the spontaneous firing properties of Purkinje cells. However, a subset of Purkinje cell action potentials is dynamically induced by excitatory inputs from climbing fibers originating in the inferior olive. Interestingly, during early postnatal development, multiple climbing fibers contact one Purkinje cell, but in a parallel-fiber/granule cell-dependent process, the additional climbing fibers are pruned away during the second and third weeks of life (Crepel and Delhaye-Bouchaud, 1979; Hashimoto et al., 2009; Kano and Hashimoto, 2012). Immature Vglut2+/climbing fiber synapses are located around the Purkinje cell body in controls, but at around P9 only the ‘winner’ among these synapses translocates to the Purkinje cell dendrites in the molecular layer (Kano et al., 2018). In addition to this change in Vglut2+-expressing climbing fibers, mossy fibers temporarily innervate Purkinje cells bodies (Kalinovsky et al., 2011; Sillitoe, 2016) and granule cells may transiently express Vglut2 (Miyazaki et al., 2003). We therefore expected a reduction in Vglut2+-positive synapses around the Purkinje cell (Figure 7A). Indeed, we observed a decrease in the density of Vglut2+ synapses in P14 control cerebella compared to P7, likely due to a combined reduction in Vglut2+ expression in granule cells, displacement of mossy fibers to granule cells, and pruning of climbing fibers (Figure 7B–C). At both timepoints, however, we observed a high density of Vglut2+ synapses in the cerebella of En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (Figure 7B–C). Given the large reduction in the total number of granule cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 3), it is likely that this high density of Vglut2+ synapses stems from mossy fibers and climbing fibers directly innervating Purkinje cell bodies. We tested this hypothesis by counting the number of Purkinje cells that had Vglut2+ puncta on their cell bodies. As expected, the number of soma-associated Vglut2+ contacts on Purkinje cells decreased between P7 and P14 in control mice, whereas all Purkinje cell somata in the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice remained enriched with Vglut2+ synapses (Figure 7D). These data show that the En1;Atoh1 lineage, excitatory cerebellar neurons are necessary for shaping the regional localization and cellular targeting of immature mossy fibers and climbing fibers that project to the Purkinje cells.

Figure 7. The density of Vglut2 synapses onto Purkinje cells decreases between P7 and P14 in control mice and is higher in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants.

Figure 7.

(A) Schematic of Purkinje cell microcircuit in control (left) and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (right) at P7 and P14. Vglut2 is expressed in climbing fibers (orange) and some mossy fibers (gray) but not in Purkinje cells (green) or granule cell (pink) terminals at P14, but in some granule cells at P7 (Miyazaki et al., 2003). Abbreviations: ml = molecular layer; pcl = Purkinje cell layer; gcl = granule cell layer. (B) Representative images showing the density of Vglut2+ profiles in the cerebella of control (top) and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (bottom) at P7 and P14. Left panels are gray-scale images of the Vglut1 signal and right panels are merged to show the Vglut2 (pink) and Calbindin (green) signals. Arrowheads point to Purkinje cells that have direct Vglut2 synapses on their cell bodies. (C) Density of Vglut2 synapses as a percentage of the area of interest (AOI; AOI comprises the molecular and Purkinje cell layer in cerebella from control animals and the area occupied by Purkinje cells in the cerebella of En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice). (D) Number of Purkinje cells with direct Vglut2+ puncta on their cell bodies. Significance in C and D was established based on the average of each mouse (N=3 mice per genotype, large circles; n=3–5 tissue sections, small dots) using a two-way ANOVA (genotype*age) followed by a Tukey Kramer post-hoc analysis. *p=0.05. The raw data and specific p-values for all the comparisons are presented in Figure 7—source data 1.

Figure 7—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 7.

Loss of excitatory cerebellar neurons alters Purkinje cell complex spike firing patterns

Based on our findings that En1;Atoh1 lineage excitatory cerebellar neurons are necessary for the architectural maturation of mossy fibers and climbing fibers, we next wanted to investigate how climbing fiber-induced complex spikes changed between P7 and P14 control mice and if they were altered in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (Figure 8A–C). Interestingly, the predominant shape of the complex spike was different between P7 and P14. Purkinje cells in P14 control mice fired classical complex spikes, which are defined by a large sodium spike followed by a train of three to five smaller spikelets (Davie et al., 2008; Zagha et al., 2008; Figure 8B). In contrast, Purkinje cells in P7 control mice fire ‘doublets’, which are characterized by an initial simple spike-like action potential, followed by a smaller action potential that occurs within 20 ms (a similar profile of doublets was previously reported in neonatal rats [Puro and Woodward, 1977; Sokoloff et al., 2015, Figure 8A]). Similar to Purkinje cells in P7 control mice, the Purkinje cells in P10 and P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice also predominantly fired doublets instead of true complex spikes, suggesting that the extrinsically-induced action potentials in Purkinje cells are maintained in an immature state just like the firing patterns of spontaneous simple spikes (Figures 5 and 6). Furthermore, we found that some Purkinje cells fired both complex spikes and doublets, which may indicate two separate climbing fibers promoting distinct types of post-synaptic spikes (example Figure 8D). We observed heterogeneity in complex spike type in about half of the Purkinje cells recorded in the youngest P7-10 control mice (P7-8: 10/21 cells; P9-10: 19/30 cells) and in about one third of Purkinje cells in the older P11-14 control mice (P11-12: 9/35 cells; P13-14: 12/32 cells). However, the majority of Purkinje cells recorded from P10 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (P10: 12/15 cells) and all Purkinje cells recorded from P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (P14: 15/15 cells) showed complex spike heterogeneity. These data may collectively indicate a heterogeneity in climbing fiber innervation that is reduced within the second postnatal week and dependent on the integration of excitatory neurons within the cerebellar circuit.

Figure 8. Purkinje cell complex spike firing patterns change from P7 to P14 in control mice and are altered in P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice.

Figure 8.

(A) Schematic of in vivo Purkinje cell recordings in P7 control mouse. Top left: cerebellum with recording electrode. Top right: schematic showing the recording electrode extracellular and adjacent to the Purkinje cell body. Bottom right: representative 1 s recording from Purkinje cell, each large vertical line is an action potential. *indicates a doublet/complex spike. Bottom left: doublet indicated on the right. (B) Same as (A) For P14 control mouse. * = a complex spike. (C) Same as (A) for P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mouse, * is a doublet. (D) Example of a Purkinje cell recording with a classical complex spike and doublet occurring in the same cell (trace is 1 s long). Bottom are expanded views of (i) simple spike, (ii) complex spike, and (iii) doublet in top trace. The trace is an example from a Purkinje cell recording in a P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mouse. (E) Example of a Purkinje cell recording with a train of classical complex spikes (trace is 1 s long). Bottom are expanded views for the three complex spikes occurring in rapid succession of one another (i, ii, iii). (F) Frequency of ‘classical’ complex spikes. (G) Frequency of doublets. (H) Frequency of combined complex spikes and doublets. For F-H, significance was determined using an ANOVA between the four control age groups and two En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- age groups followed by a Tukey-Kramer post-hoc analysis to assess differences between the groups. Significance was accepted at p<0.05. In the figure, the statistical significance between two groups is indicated as a line starting and ending above the two groups. Gray lines indicate differences within control groups of different ages and orange lines indicate differences between control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- groups. N/n-numbers: control P7-8: n=22 cells from N=12 mice; control P9-10: n=30/N=12; control P11-12: n=35/N=11; control P13-14: n=32/N=14; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- P10: n=15/N=3; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- P14: n=15/N=6. The raw data and specific p-values for all the comparisons are presented in Figure 8—source data 1.

Figure 8—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 8.

Another interesting observation we made was the occurrence of ‘complex spike trains’, which we defined as three or more complex spikes that followed each other in close succession (Figure 8E). These complex spike trains were observed in Purkinje cells recorded in control mice of all ages (P7-8: 13/22 cells; P9-10: 8/30 cells; P11-12: 8/35 cells; P13-14: 9/32 cells) as well as En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (P10: 5/15; P14: 15/15 cells) and may be a necessary property that helps establish, stabilize, and/or maintain inferior olive to Purkinje cell climbing fiber synapses.

When we quantified the number of different types of complex spikes, we found that classical complex spikes occurred less often in Purkinje cells of young control animals and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants. Specifically, we found that Purkinje cells recorded in P11-14 control mice had a statistically significant higher frequency of complex spikes than Purkinje cells recorded from P7 to P10 control mice and P10 to P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants (Figure 8F). Conversely, the frequency of doublets was significantly higher in Purkinje cells recorded from P10 to P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants than in Purkinje cells in control animals of all ages (Figure 8G). In addition, the number of all combined complex spikes was higher in Purkinje cells recorded from in P14 En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants than that observed in Purkinje cells recorded in P7-8, P9-10, and P13-14 control animals (Figure 8H). Together, these results indicate that the Purkinje cells in the mutant mice do not acquire a similar maturation in overall complex spike shape that the control Purkinje cells do and they have a high number of immature-like doublets, which may indicate an altered pruning process of immature climbing fibers.

The proper establishment of cerebellar-dependent behaviors requires the presence of a full repertoire of excitatory cerebellar neurons

Finally, we wanted to investigate how the observed changes in cerebellar function are translated into behavioral abnormalities. Purkinje cells send projections to the cerebellar nuclei, which consists of both excitatory and inhibitory neurons, as well as some direct projections to the vestibular nuclei (Sillitoe and Joyner, 2007). These circuits are important for motor coordination and balance as well as social behaviors including ultrasonic vocalization (USV) in neonatal pups (Fujita et al., 2008; Lalonde and Strazielle, 2015). Interestingly, the contribution of the cerebellum to these behaviors is initiated before circuit rewiring is completed (Lalonde and Strazielle, 2015). Therefore, we were curious to know whether the immature circuit of En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice was sufficient for performing a substantial repertoire of cerebellar-dependent behaviors. Observations of control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutant mice showed overt phenotypic differences in motor control (Video 1 and Figure 9A) similar to what was previously observed in other agranular mice (Sidman et al., 1962; Sidman et al., 1965; Woodward et al., 1974). At P14, control mice explore an open arena with smooth intentional motions, whereas En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice often fall on their backs. The frequent falling over onto their backs prevented us from performing classical assays of motor function such as rotarod or foot printing. Instead, we assayed the righting reflex, open field exploration, and USVs. We also tested for clinically relevant features such as tremor and dystonia-like postures, which often arise with cerebellar dysfunction. We found that En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice perform poorly compared to control littermates during the righting reflex. They were significantly slower in returning onto four paws when compared to Atoh1fl/+ control mice at P8 and P10, and they were slower than heterozygous En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+mice only at P10 (Figure 9B). Because all mice attempted to turn right-side-up immediately after being placed on their backs, it is likely that this delay in righting is the result of impaired motor coordination rather than an abnormal sense of gravity. Next, we tested whether En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice showed abnormal USVs when briefly separated from their mothers (Figure 9C). We found that call time in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice was shorter than those observed in control littermates and that En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice called less frequently than Atoh1fl/+ mice (Figure 9C–E). We next quantified how En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice moved in an open field (Figure 9F). The distance traveled or movement time in a 15-min-period was not significantly impaired (total distance (cm): Atoh1fl/+: 58.1±11.7; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+: 35.7±9.9; Atoh1fl/-: 44.4±6.5; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/-: 43.1±12.4; Kruskal-Wallis test p=0.32; movement time (s): Atoh1fl/+: 55.9±5.9; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+: 42.1±7.2; Atoh1fl/-: 50.1±7.4; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/-: 83.5±17.7; Kruskal-Wallis test p=0.14). However, En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutant mice traveled slower than Atoh1fl/- control mice and the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+ mice made more isolated movements during their trajectory compared to all their littermate controls (Figure 9F–H). Finally, we observed a tremor in the mutants and measured the severity with our custom-made tremor monitor (Figure 9I; Brown et al., 2020). We found that En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice had a higher power tremor in the 12–16 Hz frequency range (Figure 9J). This range corresponds to physiological tremor and indicates the presence of a pathophysiological defect that emerges from a rise in baseline values. The mutant mice also had a higher peak tremor power compared to all control littermates (Figure 9K). Together, we uncovered that the lack of rRL-derived excitatory cerebellar neurons in developing En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice leads to multiple abnormal cerebellar-dependent behaviors that are observed in the first 2 weeks of postnatal life.

Figure 9. Postnatal En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have abnormal motor coordination, enhanced tremor and altered vocalization behavior.

Figure 9.

(A) Timed-series photos of Atoh1fl/+ (gray) and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- (orange) mice. En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have a wide stance (blue arrows) and fall on their backs (red arrows). (B) Time to right in the righting reflex of P6, P8, and P10 mice (top) and percentage of failed trials (bottom). (C) Representative ultrasonic vocalization traces with intensity of the black line representing the power of the vocalization frequency (filled circles in D and E). (D) Duration of vocalizations. (E) Frequency of vocalizations. (F) Representative tracks of mice in the open field (filled circles in G and H). Box measures 40x40 cm. (G) Movement speed. (H) Number of movements. (I) Representative power spectra of tremor recordings. (J) Tremor power at different frequencies. (K) Peak tremor power. N-numbers: Atoh1fl/+ (gray): N=10–15 mice; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+ (green): N=9–15 mice; Atoh1fl/- (pink): N=11–15 mice; En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- (orange): N=11–14 mice. Significance was determined using a non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test followed by a Tukey-Kramer post-hoc test. *p<0.05. The raw data and specific p-values for all the comparisons are presented in Figure 9—source data 1.

Figure 9—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 9.

Video 1. En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have a spectrum of visible motor impairments.

Download video file (4.6MB, mp4)

At P14, control mice explore the open box smoothly, whereas En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have a visible tremor, often fall on their backs, and have dystonia-like postures and movements in their hindlimbs.

Discussion

In this paper, we used En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice as a model of cerebellar agranularity to test how cell-to-cell interactions impact the formation of functional circuits. Using this model with circuit-wide loss of granule cell neurogenesis, we uncovered how these late-born cells influence the functional development of their downstream synaptic partners, the Purkinje cells. We find that granule cell elimination stalls the anatomical and functional maturation of postnatal Purkinje cells. In humans, granule cell neurogenesis is impaired in premature infants with cerebellar hemorrhages, which is expected as proliferating granule cell precursors are highly vulnerable to the hemorrhage, likely because of their high metabolic demand (Dobbing, 1974; Gano and Barkovich, 2019; Hortensius et al., 2018). Moreover, even a localized lesion that occurs during early cerebellar genesis can have major repercussions; loss of just a few precursors has exponential effects on the number of granule cells that integrate into the cerebellar circuit (Corrales et al., 2004; Corrales et al., 2006). Our findings show that manipulating granule cell neurogenesis not only causes major anatomical defects, but it also obstructs the remodeling and wiring of circuits that establish cerebellar cortical function during a dynamic postnatal period. Our data add to our understanding of how temporally defined insults to the developing cerebellum may result in downstream circuit dysfunctions that are associated with specific neurological disorders observed in premature infants (Dijkshoorn et al., 2020).

In our model of agranularity, we show that deletion of Atoh1 from the En1 domain impairs the development of the majority of granule cells. Even though some granule cells escape our genetic manipulation, we are confident that we impair the neurogenesis of the majority of granule cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Granule cells are the major driver for increasing cerebellar size, foliation, and lamination. Accordingly, the cerebella of En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice do not undergo proper expansion, and are devoid of lobules and a densely packed granule cell layer (Figure 1D–J). In addition, the granule cell marker, GABARα6, is not reliably expressed in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (Figure 1—figure supplement 2A) and we find an approximate thousand-fold reduction in total En1;Atoh1 lineage neurons (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Furthermore, the density of Vglut1+ terminals is decreased nearly nine-fold, with the remaining Vglut1 signal mainly representing mossy fiber terminals since any granule cell and unipolar brush cell escapees would only contribute minimally to the existing circuit in the mutants (Figure 4). Regardless, even if some granule cells are present, they are not sufficient to drive the cellular circuit reorganizations that are known to depend on granule cells (Figure 2, Figure 7).

In addition to impaired granule cell neurogenesis, we observe a lower number of unipolar brush cells and excitatory nuclei cells in our En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice, which could, in theory, contribute to our findings of abnormal Purkinje cell function by causing a lack of feedforward and feedback signals, respectively (van Dorp and De Zeeuw, 2015; Gao et al., 2016). However, both excitatory nuclei cells and unipolar brush cells signal through the granule cells, so loss of granule cells alone would in theory have a similar effect on Purkinje cell function as the combined loss of all three subtypes. In addition, a lower number of excitatory nuclei cells can reduce the number of Purkinje cells (Willett et al., 2019) and a lower number of Purkinje cells was also noted in models of chimeric Atoh1 knockouts (Jensen et al., 2004); therefore, the reduction in cerebellar size that we reported in Figure 1 could be, at least in part, enhanced by a lower number of Purkinje cells. Regardless, the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- cerebellum is filled with Purkinje cells, the predominant neuronal type comprising this massively altered cerebellum, making it an ideal environment for identifying and measuring synapse densities and easily isolating single-units during the in vivo recordings.

Nevertheless, there are several caveats to using an agranular model to investigate circuitry. For instance, loss of morphogenetic processes that determine cerebellar architecture including its size (Dahmane and Ruiz i Altaba, 1999), foliation (Corrales et al., 2006), and layering (Miyata et al., 2010) complicate interpretations of how Purkinje cells directly respond to granule cells. A previous study showed that impaired granule cell migration can also impair Purkinje cell morphology and monolayer formation, which in turn could change Purkinje cell function (Adams et al., 2002). However, here, we observed that the firing patterns of young Purkinje cells (P7-P10) do not differ between control and experimental mice, even though the defects in monolayer formation and foliation have already occurred by this time. This suggests that our observations of abnormal maturation in Purkinje cell firing patterns cannot be solely attributed to gross abnormalities in the anatomical structure of the cerebellum during morphogenesis in the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. In addition, changes in cerebellar shape and size could influence the function of forebrain regions (Kuemerle et al., 2007), and the vast connectivity of the cerebellum with forebrain regions such as the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex could contribute to the non-motor vocalization defects that we observed (Liu et al., 2020; McAfee et al., 2019). The regional specificity of cerebellar circuitry that mediates non-motor connectivity is thus likely obscured in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice (Badura et al., 2018; Stoodley and Limperopoulos, 2016).

Despite these caveats, much can be learned from our genetically precise agranular mouse model. Specifically, our mouse model has several advantages over previously described agranular mice when it comes to investigating the contribution of granule cell neurogenesis to cerebellar development and function. Because our model takes advantage of a conditional genetic strategy that only targets the rhombic lip lineage, our manipulation does not affect cell intrinsic developmental Purkinje cell programs (Herrup, 1983; Miyata et al., 2010; Sheldon et al., 1997). And unlike previous models, our approach is independent of procedural variations (Altman and Anderson, 1971; Sathyanesan et al., 2018; Yoo et al., 2014), targets the entire Atoh1 lineage in the cerebellum (Ben-Arie et al., 1997; Jensen et al., 2002; Jensen et al., 2004), and has allowed us to study postnatal development. As a result, mutant mice with the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- genotype have highly penetrant, consistent, and reliable anatomical and functional phenotypes, all of which have provided us with key insights into how cerebellar lineages shape circuit development and behavior.

The cerebellum controls motor and non-motor behaviors (Hull, 2020; Wagner and Luo, 2020). Regardless of the specific behavior, Purkinje cells are always at the center of the responsible circuit. Interestingly, during the first 2 weeks of life in mice, motor control becomes more precise (Lalonde and Strazielle, 2015), in concert with the refinement of the cerebellar circuitry (White and Sillitoe, 2013). During this period, Purkinje cell innervation switches from climbing fibers and mossy fibers to climbing fibers and parallel fibers (Mason and Gregory, 1984), climbing fibers are pruned and a ‘winner’ establishes a single Purkinje cell target (Kano et al., 2018), and Purkinje cell zones are sharpened (White et al., 2014). Accompanying these changes are emergent properties of the two Purkinje cell spike profiles, simple spikes and complex spikes. In addition to the increased complexity of intrinsic cellular properties that have been defined using slice electrophysiology approaches (McKay and Turner, 2005), we postulated using our in vivo recordings that intercellular interactions between granule cells and Purkinje cells during development may support the maturation of Purkinje cell firing properties, both by providing direct inputs and by supporting anatomical maturation, including the outgrowth of Purkinje cell dendrites. In control mice, we observed dynamic changes in normal Purkinje cell firing between P7 and P14. We found an increase in firing rate that was not uniformly acquired but was present during bursts of rapid firing that were interspersed with frequent pauses without Purkinje cell action potentials. We previously reported burst-like Purkinje cell firing from P15 to P19, although by P30 the pattern acquires the regularity that is characteristic of adults (Arancillo et al., 2015). Thus, burst-like firing occurs at intermediate stages of normal Purkinje cell development. Interestingly, bursting Purkinje cell firing patterns are also observed in mouse models of ataxia, tremor, and dystonia (Brown et al., 2020; Fremont et al., 2014; LeDoux and Lorden, 2002; Miterko et al., 2019; Miterko et al., 2021; White and Sillitoe, 2017; White et al., 2016). The dynamically adapting circuit in control mice and the range of disease severities in disease models with bursting Purkinje cells raise the possibility that Purkinje cell firing is differentially decoded by downstream neurons based on the age of the mice. The data also indicate that the intermediate stages of Purkinje cell development not only highlight a developmental phase characterized by erratic neuronal activity, but that this mode of firing represents a pathophysiological hallmark that could be a default network state in different diseases. The higher level of baseline tremor in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice suggests the possibility that perhaps the process of stabilizing neuronal activity is affected in our mutants. In contrast, alterations in other behaviors such as the USVs indicates that behaviors required during the postnatal period, such as communication with the dams, are also dependent of stage-specific neuronal properties. Therefore, the presence of granule cells in the cerebellar cortex impacts behaviors used at different ages as a consequence of how they influence Purkinje cell firing.

The behavioral abnormalities in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice are likely caused by a combined effect of Purkinje cell dysfunction and a reduction of excitatory neurons in the cerebellar nuclei. Given the broad network effects of our manipulation, it is not surprising that En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have such severe behavioral abnormalities. However, our findings are very interesting given what we know about cerebellar function. First, while En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice often rotate onto their backs and have a slower righting reflex (Figure 9A–B, Video 1), they immediately initiate righting in both cases, suggesting that these mice have normal perception of gravitational force and that the observed phenotypes are mainly due to motor impairments. Second, because some extra-cerebellar structures are involved in airway pressure and breathing (Bautista and Dutschmann, 2014; Chamberlin, 2004; Dutschmann and Herbert, 2006), specifically the parabrachial nucleus and Kölliker Fuse (van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018), the defects in USVs could ultimately be due to alterations in multiple brain regions with particular effects on the duration of vocalizations (Figure 9C–D). Yet, the respiratory cell types we have manipulated are unlikely to drive the initiation and number of vocalizations (Figure 9E), suggesting that these abnormalities are cerebellum-driven. Finally, loss of Purkinje cell signaling is sufficient to reduce baseline and harmaline-induced tremor (Brown et al., 2020), whereas we observe that loss of excitatory cerebellar neurons causes an enhanced tremor phenotype (Figure 9I–K). These data show that unlike Purkinje cells, excitatory cerebellar neurons are not necessary for propagating tremor but they are nevertheless required for modulating it to achieve adequate control of the muscles.

The severity of motor impairments and electrophysiological changes in Purkinje cells in our model and previously reported agranular mice remains in contrast to the modest changes in Purkinje cell firing after silencing granule cell synapses (Galliano et al., 2013). However, a noteworthy distinction should be made that in our mice, granule cells are removed altogether, whereas Galliano et al., 2013 blocked granule cell neurotransmitter release – the difference in functional outcomes could therefore include unique cell non-autonomous impacts on development induced by each type of manipulation and not solely depend on the primary manipulated cell type, the granule cells. Furthermore, in multiple models, impairing parallel fiber synapses results in motor impairments that can be assessed with the rotor rod assay (Aiba et al., 1994; Park et al., 2019), which we could not do due to the severity of motor impairment in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. Taking these results together, from both the technical and conceptual standpoints, when one seeks to resolve developmental mechanisms, we must not only consider what is manipulated, but also how it is manipulated. As such, the timing of neurogenesis is a primary consideration. Purkinje cells are generated between E10 and E13 (Hashimoto and Mikoshiba, 2003) and granule cell progenitors from ~E13 onwards (Machold and Fishell, 2005; Rose et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2005). Whereas Purkinje cells migrate into the core of the cerebellar anlage upon their birth, granule cell precursors first migrate over the surface of the developing cerebellum and proliferate extensively in the external granular layer to increase the precursor pool (Wingate and Hatten, 1999). Only after this phase do they migrate radially past Purkinje cells, the first potential opportunity for direct cell-to-cell interactions. Based on our data, we argue that the initial communication between Purkinje cells and granule cells sets the efficiency of Purkinje cell function and the establishment of Purkinje cell spike properties through structural as well as synaptic signals. Thus, insults to granule cell proliferation and an obstruction of granule cell neurogenesis may have different, and perhaps more severe effects on downstream Purkinje cell function, compared to lesions of mature granule cells. This may explain why relatively mild but early lesions and structural changes, as observed in preterm infants with and without cerebellar hemorrhage, have large effects on cerebellar function.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type (species) or resource Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional information
Antibody Anti-Calbindin (guinea-pig polyclonal) Synaptic Systems RRID:AB_2619902 IF: (1:1,000)
Antibody Anti-GABARα6 (rabbit polyclonal) Millipore Sigma RRID:AB_91935 IF: (1:500)
Antibody Anti-Tbr2 (rabbit polyclonal) Abcam RRID:AB_778267 IF: (1:500)
Antibody Anti-Calretinin (mouse monoclonal) Swant RRID:AB_10000320 IF: (1:500)
Antibody Anti-NFH (mouse monoclonal) Biolegend RRID:AB_2564642 IF: (1:1,000)
Antibody Anti-HCN1 (rabbit polyclonal) Alomone Lab RRID:AB_2039900 IF: (1:500)
Antibody Anti-RORα (goat polyclonal) Santa Cruz RRID:AB_2301066 IF: (1:250)
Antibody Anti-PV (rabbit polyclonal) Swant RRID:AB_10000344 IF: (1:1,000)
Antibody Anti-neurogranin (rabbit polyclonal) Millipore RRID:AB_91937 IF: (1:500)
Antibody Anti-ZebrinII (mouse monoclonal) Gift from Dr. Hawkes IF: (1:500)
Antibody Anti-Vglut1 (rabbit polyclonal) Synaptic Systems RRID:AB_887877 IF: (1:500)
Antibody Anti-Vglut2 (rabbit polyclonal) Synaptic Systems RRID:AB_887883 IF: (1:500)
Antibody Anti-PLCβ4 (rabbit polyclonal) Santa Cruz Biotechnology RRID:AB_654082 IF: (1:150)
Chemical compound, drug WGA-Alexa 555 Thermo Fisher Scientific
Chemical compound, drug Fast green Sigma-Aldrich
Chemical compound, drug PBS Sigma-Aldrich
Commercial assay or kit Golgi-Cox staining kit FD Neurotechnologies
Strain, strain background (M. musculus) Mouse: Atoh1-FlpO Gift from Dr. Zoghbi van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018
Strain, strain background (M. musculus) Mouse: Rosa-FSF-LSL-tdTomato (Ai65(RCFL-tdT), Gt(ROSA)26Sortm65.1(CAG-tdTomato)Hze) The Jackson Laboratory RRID:IMSR_JAX:021875
Strain, strain background (M. musculus) Mouse: En1-Cre (En1tm2(cre)Wrst/J) The Jackson Laboratory RRID:IMSR_JAX:007916
Strain, strain background (M. musculus) Mouse: Atoh1-Flox (Atoh1tm3Hzo) The Jackson Laboratory RRID:MGI:4420944
Software, algorithm MATLAB MathWorks RRID:SCR_001622
Software, algorithm ImageJ National Institutes of Health RRID:SCR_003070
Software, algorithm Photoshop Adobe RRID:SCR_014199
Software, algorithm Illustrator Adobe RRID:SCR_010279
Software, algorithm Spike2 CED RRID:SCR_010279
Other Tungsten electrodes Thomas Recording
Other VetBond 3M

Animals

All mice used in this study were housed in a Level 3, AALAS-certified facility. All experiments and studies that involved mice were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of Baylor College of Medicine (BCM). The following transgenic mouse lines were used for the experiments carried out in this study: Atoh1FlpO (van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018); En1Cre (En1tm2(cre)Wrst/J, JAX:007916); Ai65 (Gt(ROSA)26Sortm65.1(CAG-tdTomato)Hze, JAX:021875); Atoh1Flox (Atoh1tm3Hzo, MGI:4420944). Conditional knockout mice were generated by crossing En1Cre/+;Atoh1+/FlpO double heterozygote mice (which can be functionally defined as En1Cre/+;Atoh1+/- mice) with homozygote Atoh1fl/fl mice. En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice were considered experimental conditional knockout mice and all littermates with a different genotype were considered control mice. Specifically, we used Atoh1fl/+, Atoh1FlpO/fl, and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+ mice as controls. Ear tissue or tail clips were collected before weaning and used for genotyping and identification of the different alleles. For all experiments, we bred mice using standard timed pregnancies, noon on the day a vaginal plug was detected was considered embryonic day E0.5 and P0 was defined as the day of birth. Pups of both sexes were used in all experiments.

Tissue processing

Brain tissue was collected as described in our previous publications (Zhou et al., 2020). First, we anesthetized mice with Avertin. Once the mice did not respond to toe or tail pinch, we accessed the chest cavity and then penetrated the heart with a butterfly needle for perfusions. The mice were perfused with 1M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS pH 7.4) to remove blood from the tissue and 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) to fix the tissue. The tissue was concomitantly post-fixed overnight in 4% PFA at 4°C. Tissue was cryoprotected in a sucrose gradient (10% → 20% → 30% sucrose in PBS) at 4°C, each step lasting until the tissue sank to the bottom of a 15 mL tube. Tissue was frozen in optimal cutting temperature (OCT) solution and stored at −80°C until it was cut. All tissue were cut into 40 µm free-floating tissue sections and stored in PBS at 4°C until it was used for immunohistochemistry.

Immunohistochemistry

Free floating tissue sections were stained according to the following protocol. Free floating tissue sections were blocked in 10% normal goat or donkey serum and 0.1% Triton-X in PBS (PBS-T) for two hours. Next, tissue sections were incubated overnight in primary antibodies in blocking solution. Tissue was washed three times for 5 min in PBS-T. For fluorescent staining, the tissue was incubated for 2 hr in PBS-T with the preferred secondary antibodies conjugated to an Alexa fluorophore. Finally, tissue sections were washed three times in PBS-T and mounted on electrostatically coated slides with hard-set, DAPI-containing mounting medium. Alternatively, for DAB staining, we incubated the tissue for 2 hr in PBS-T with the preferred secondary antibodies that was conjugated to horseradish peroxidase (HRP). After washing three times in PBS-T, the tissue was incubated with diaminobenzidine (DAB) solution until the desired color intensity was reached. The DAB color reaction was stopped by washing tissue three times with PBS-T. The tissue was then mounted on electrostatically coated glass slides, dehydrated in an ethanol series (70% → 90% → 100%) and then mounted using Xylene or histoclear. All steps of immunohistochemistry were performed at room temperature. All mounted slides were stored at 4°C until they were imaged.

The following primary antibodies were used for the data described in this manuscript: guinea pig (gp)-α-Calbindin (1:1,000; SySy; #214004); rabbit (rb)-α-gamma-aminobutyric acid receptor α6 (GABARα6; 1:500; Millipore Sigma; #AB5610), rb-α-T-box brain protein 2 (Tbr2; 1:500; Abcam; #AB23345), mouse (ms)-α-Calretinin (1:500; Swant; #6B3); ms-α-Neurofilament Heavy (NFH; 1:1,000; Biolegend; #801701); rb-α-Hyperpolarization Activated Cyclic Nucleotide Gated Potassium Channel 1 (HCN1; 1:500; Alomone Lab; #APC-056); goat (gt)-α-RAR-related orphan receptor alpha (RORα; 1:250; Santa Cruz; #F2510); rb-α-parvalbumin (PV; 1:1,000; Swant; #PV25); rb-α-neurogranin (1:500; Millipore Sigma; #AB5620); ms-α-ZebrinII (1:500; kind gift from Dr. Richard Hawkes, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada); rb-α- PLCβ4 (1:150; Santa Cruz Biotechnology; catalog #sc-20760); rb-α-Vglut1 (1:500; SySy; #135302); rb-α-Vglut2 (1:500; SySy; #135403). The following secondary antibodies were used for immunohistochemistry: HRP-conjugated goat (gt)-α-mouse; gt-α-rabbit; and donkey (dk)-α-goat (1:200; DAKO). The following secondary antibodies were used for immunofluorescence: dk-α-mouse IgG Alexa Fluor 488 (1;1,500; Thermo Fisher Scientific; #A21202) and gt-α-gp IgG Alexa Fluor 488 (1;1,500; Thermo Fisher Scientific;#A11073).

Cresyl violet staining

Brain tissue sections were mounted on glass slides and then dried overnight. Slides were submerged in 100% histoclear and rehydrated in an ethanol series (100% → 90% → 70%). Then, the slides were submerged in cresyl violet solution for staining until sufficiently dark and then dehydrated in an ethanol series (70% → 90% → 100%). Finally, the slides were sealed with a coverslip using Cytoseal mounting media. All steps were performed at room temperature and the mounted slides were stored at 4°C until they were imaged.

Neuroanatomical anterograde tracing

Anterograde neuroanatomical tracing of mossy fibers to the cerebellum was performed as described previously (Lackey and Sillitoe, 2020; Sillitoe, 2016). P12 pups were anesthetized with isoflurane on a surgery rig. Hair was removed and an incision was made in the skin over the lower thoracic/upper lumbar spinal cord, using the curvature of the spine as a guide. We used a Nanoject II to inject 0.2–1 µl of 2% WGA-Alexa Fluor 555 (Thermo Fisher Scientific; #W32464) and 0.5% Fast Green (Sigma-Aldrich; #F7252, used for visualization) diluted in 0.1 M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS; Sigma-Aldric; #P4417; pH 7.4). Tracer was injected 1 mm below the surface of the spinal cord, on the right side of the dorsal spinal vein. After tracer injection, we applied antibiotic ointment and closed the incision using VetBond (3M; #1469 SB) and wound clips (Fine Science Tools; #12032–07). Pups were placed back with the mom after waking up from anesthesia. We placed soft food and hydrogel on the cage floor and monitored behavior closely for whether the mom accepted the pups back into the litter. Tissue was collected (see section on Tissue processing above) for tracer visualization 2 days after the surgery, at P14.

Golgi-Cox staining

Golgi-Cox staining was performed according to previously described protocols (Brown et al., 2019) and the manufacturer’s instructions (FD Neurotechnologies; #PK401). Brains were dissected from the skulls and immediately emerged in the staining solution. After staining, tissue sections were cut at a thickness of 10 µm and directly mounted onto electrostatically coated glass slides. The tissue was then dehydrated in an ethanol series (70% → 90% → 100%), cleared with Xylene, and mounted with cytoseal. All slides were dried overnight before imaging and were kept at 4°C for storage.

Microscopy and image processing

Photomicrographs of stained whole mount cerebella and DAB stained cerebellar tissue sections were acquired using Leica cameras DPC365FX and DMC2900, respectively, attached to a Leica DM4000 B LED microscope. Photomicrographs of Golgi-Cox stained tissue sections and WGA-Alexa 555 tracing were captured using Zeiss cameras AxioCam MRc5 and AxiaCam Mrm, respectively, attached to a Zeiss Axio Imager.M2 microscope. Whole mount images were stitched together using Adobe Photoshop (Adobe Systems 2020) Photomerge function. Color brightness and contrast were adjusted using ImageJ (Schneider et al., 2012). Photomicrographs of images were cropped to the desired size using Adobe Illustrator (Adobe Systems 2020).

Image quantification – TdTomato signal density

We obtained high-magnification images from lobule IX in control animals or the ventrocaudal region of conditional knockout mice. We quantified the percentage of the area of interest occupied by the signal by transforming the signal into a binary signal with TdTomato pixel brightness cut off at 80/255.

Image quantification – unipolar brush cell and excitatory nuclei cell count

We serially sectioned brains and kept every sixth (coronal) or eight (sagittal) section in an individual well of a 24-well plate. This allowed us to have an unbiased tissue collection system that spanned the entire cerebellum. We then stained all tissue sections collected in a single well for anti-Tbr2 (unipolar brush cells) or anti-NFH (excitatory nuclei neurons and Purkinje cells) plus anti-Calbindin (Purkinje cells). Co-staining for NFH and Calbindin was necessary to distinguish between Purkinje cells (Calbindin+ and NFH+) and excitatory nuclei cells (Calbindin- and NFH+) in primarily the conditional knockout mice, as nuclei neurons and Purkinje cells did not have clear anatomical segregation in these mice. After staining our tissue sections as described above, we imaged whole tissue sections at a magnification that allowed us to identify and count single cells. Images were merged together in photoshop and all Tbr2+ cells and NFH+/Calbindin- cells were counted in all tissue sections from a single animal in ImageJ. The final cell-count was multiplied by six (coronal) or eight (sagittal) to obtain an estimate of the total number of unipolar brush cells or excitatory nuclei cells for each animal. A t-test was performed to investigate whether there was a statistically significant difference between cell number in control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice.

Image quantification – Purkinje cell morphology analysis

Brightfield images of Golgi-Cox stains were imported into ImageJ and then converted into a binary image with brightness and contrast adjusted so that cellular processes were clearly visible. Then, Sholl analysis was performed using the build-in Sholl analysis module in ImageJ (Ferreira et al., 2014) and false positive intersections were manually subtracted from the counts. Next, we used a Zeiss Laser Scanner confocal Microscope model 710 to take high-magnification Z-stacked images (0.2 μm). We imported the images to imageJ/Fiji and used the Simple Neurite Tracer plug-in to track the length of a stained dendrite. We manually counted the number of synaptic boutons along the dendrite and reported this number as a density per dendrite length.

Image quantification – Vglut1 and Vglut2 density

We stained tissue sections obtained from P7 and P14 control and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice with anti-Calbindin and anti-Vglut1 or Vglut2 antibodies as described above. Next, we imaged 3–5 tissue sections from three mice per age, per genotype. We imported tissue sections into ImageJ and defined our area of interest as the area of the image where Vglut1+ and Vglut2+ synapses could directly innervate Purkinje cells. This included the molecular and Purkinje cell layers in control tissue and the area occupied by Purkinje cell bodies in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- tissue; both were identified as the area of the image positive for the Calbindin signal. We adjusted the brightness and contrast in the Vglut1 or Vglut2 channels so that individual synapses were visible and then transformed the image into a binary (black and white) image using the Threshold function in ImageJ. We quantified the percentage of area of interest covered by Vglut1 or Vglut2 synapses using the Analyze Particles tool in ImageJ. We also manually counted the number of Purkinje cells that received direct Vglut2+ inputs on their somas. Finally, we averaged the values of the area covered by Vglut1+ or Vglut2+ synapses across all tissue sections (n=3–5) per animal (N=3) and performed a two-way ANOVA (genotype*age) followed by a Tukey-Kramer post-hoc analysis to identify the statistical significance of observed differences in the density of Vglut1+ and Vglut2+ synapses in mice of different genotypes and ages.

In vivo electrophysiology

All in vivo, anesthetized experiments were performed as described in previous publications (Arancillo et al., 2015; White and Sillitoe, 2017). Specifically, we anesthetized mice using a mixture of ketamine 80 (mg/kg) and dexmedetomidine (16 mg/kg). We held mice on a heated surgery pad. We removed hair from skull and made an incision in the skin over the anterior part of the skull. We stabilized the heads of our mice using ear bars and a mouth mount when animals were large enough (most P11-P14 mice) and otherwise fixed the mouse skull (P7-P10 mice) to a plastic mount that was attached to ear bars on our stereotaxic surgery rig to stabilize the head during recordings. Using a sharp needle or dental drill, we made a craniotomy in the interparietal bone plate, ~3 mm dorsal from lambda and ~3 mm lateral from the midline, with a diameter of ~3 mm. We kept our surgical coordinates consistent based on the distance from lambda across mice of all ages, as the skull undergoes significant growth during the ages at which we measured neural activity. After making a craniotomy, we recorded neural activity using tungsten electrodes (Thomas Recording, Germany) and then the digitized the signals into Spike2 (CED, England). We recorded neural activity from cells that were 0–2 mm below the brain surface.

Analysis of in vivo electrophysiological recordings

All electrophysiological recording data were spike sorted in Spike2. We sorted out three types of spikes: simple spikes, complex spikes, and doublets. Complex spikes were characterized by their large amplitude, and post-spike depolarization and smaller spikelets that follow. Doublets were characterized as action potentials that were followed by one or more smaller action potentials within 20 ms after the initial action potential. All other action potentials were characterized as simple spikes (see examples in Figures 5, 6 and 8). We only included traces with clearly identifiable complex spikes or doublets and analyzed only cells from which we could obtain a sufficiently long and stable recording (186 ± 6.5 s; minimum = 75 s) with an optimal signal to noise ratio.

After spike sorting our traces in Spike 2, we analyzed the frequency and regularity of firing patterns in MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc, version R2018a). For this study, we defined ‘frequency’ as number of all spikes observed in the total analyzed recording time (spikes/s). We determined the ‘frequency mode’ by populating the inverse of all observed interspike intervals (ISI−1, representing the frequency of a single spike) into 2.5 Hz bins and defining the center of the bin with the largest proportion of spikes as the ‘frequency mode’ (see Figure 5D and E for examples). Pause Percent was the proportion of the recording time during which the ISI was longer than five times the mean ISI for each independent cell, defined as the following: (sum(ISI>5*mean(ISI)))/(total recording time). Our measures of global regularity or burstiness (CV) and regularity (CV2) were based on the interspike intervals (ISI) between two adjacent spikes (in seconds). CV = stdev(ISI)/mean(ISI), and CV2 = mean(2*|ISIn-ISIn-1|/ (ISIn+ISIn-1)) (Holt et al., 1996).

The rhythmicity index was based on oscillatory properties represented by the auto-correlogram of the ISI (see Figure 5L and M for examples) and based on previously described quantifications with minor adjustments (Lang et al., 1997; Sugihara et al., 1995; White et al., 2016). The ‘rhythmicity index’ was calculated as the sum of the difference in peak and trough of the proportion of spikes at each delay from spike onset, divided by the baseline proportion of spikes. Baseline = (total spike number)2/(recording time/bin width). We used a bin width of 5 ms. The first peak of each oscillation was determined as the highest bin between 10 ms and 1.5 times the mean ISI for a given cell, and the time of the peak was denoted as a1. Each subsequent peak was determined as the highest bin between the delay-time of the previous trough and an+a1+10 ms, where an is the time of the previous peak. The first trough was determined as the lowest bin between the first peak (a1) and an+a1. Peaks and troughs were only accepted if their sum was higher than four times the standard deviation of auto-correlation between 0.96 and 1 s lag-time, or if the peak was higher than baseline + two times the standard deviation and the trough was lower than baseline – two times the standard deviation.

Cluster and dimension reduction analysis on simple spikes

To investigate and visualize how firing patterns changed over time, we performed additional computational analysis of the firing properties of single neurons (n=179 neurons). We included the parameters summarized in Figure 5 in our analysis (frequency, frequency mode, pause percent, rhythmicity index, CV, and CV2). First, we performed an unbiased cluster analysis on the firing properties of single neurons using the MATLAB function ‘clusterogram,’ which previously allowed us to visualize genotype-specific changes in Purkinje cell firing patterns (van der Heijden et al., 2021a). After analysis, we de-identified the cells and labelled them by age and genotype (Figure 6B). Next, we performed a t-distributed stochastic neighbor embedding (tSNE) dimension reduction analysis to visualize the relative similarities of firing patterns between cells using the MATLAB function ‘tsne’ based on Euclidean distance. We plotted tSNE component 1 and 2 against each other (tSNE(1) and tSNE(2)) and color-coded the coordinates based on age and genotype of the cell recording to generate an electrophysiological pseudotimeline (Figure 6C). Finally, we used the MATLAB function ‘linkage’ on the first two components of the principal component analysis on the average firing properties per age and genotype group to define how the firing patterns of each group clustered together. We visualized this unbiased linkage analysis using the MATLAB function ‘dendrogram’ (Figure 6D).

Behavioral analyses

Righting reflex was measured on P6, P8, and P10 as follows. The mouse was placed on its back in a clean cage without bedding. One finger was used to stabilize the mouse on its back. The timer was set the moment the experimenter removed their finger, and time was recorded until the mouse righted itself up onto its four paws. All mice were tested twice at each age. A ‘failed’ trial was defined as when the mouse did not right itself within one minute (sixty seconds).

At P7, we recorded pup vocalizations as described previously (Yin et al., 2018). Pups were placed in an anechoic, sound-attenuating chamber (Med Associates Inc). The pup was placed in a round plastic tub that was positioned near a CM16 microphone (Avisoft Bioacoustics) that was located in the center of the chamber. Sound was amplified and digitized using UltraSoundGate 416H at a 250 kHz sampling rate and bit depth of 16. Avisoft RECORDER software was used to collect the recordings. Ultrasonic vocalizations were monitored for 2 min for each animal.

We also performed an open-field assay at P13, as previously described (Alcott et al., 2020). Mice were habituated to a room with the light set to 200 lux and ambient white noise to 60 dB. We placed each mouse in the center of an open field (40x40x30 cm chamber). The chamber has photobeams that records movement. Each mouse was tested for 15 min and activity was recorded using Fusion software (Accuscan Instruments). We used the standard settings for movement segmentation. We analyzed the data for total distance traveled, movement time, speed, and total movements during the 15-min test period.

We measured tremor using our custom-built tremor monitor (Brown et al., 2020). Each mouse was placed in the tremor chamber, which is a translucent box with an open top that is suspended in the air by eight elastic cords that are attached to four metal rods. An accelerometer is attached to the bottom of the box. Mice were allowed to habituate to the chamber for 120 s prior to initiating the tremor recordings. The mice are free to move around in the box. Power spectra of the tremor recordings were assessed using Fast Fourier transform (FFT) with a Hanning window in Spike2 software as previously described (Brown et al., 2020). FFT frequency was targeted to ~1 Hz per bin.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed in MATLAB. For the Purkinje cell morphology data, we performed a linear mix-model analysis with genotype as the fixed variable and mouse number as the random variable and accepted p<0.05 as statistically significant. For Vglut1/2 quantification, we averaged measurements from three to five tissue sections per mouse and performed a two-way ANOVA (genotype*age) followed by a Tukey-Kramer post-hoc analysis to define statistical significance between independent groups. For electrophysiology data (frequency (complex spikes and simple spikes), frequency mode, pause percent, rhythmicity index, CV, CV2, and doublet frequency), analysis was performed using an ANOVA with six total groups (four control groups, two mutant groups), followed by a Tukey-Kramer post-hoc test. For behavioral data, we performed a Kruskal-Wallis test followed by a Tukey-Kramer post-hoc test to define significance between independent groups. For these tests, we accepted p<0.05 as statistically significant.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by Baylor College of Medicine (BCM), Texas Children’s Hospital, The Hamill Foundation, and the National Institutes of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) R01NS089664, R01NS100874, and R01NS119301 to RVS. Research reported in this publication was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number P50HD103555 for use of the Cell and Tissue Pathogenesis Core and the Mouse Neurobehavioral Core facilities. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Support was also provided by a Dystonia Medical Research Foundation (DMRF) postdoctoral award to MEvdH and an F31NS101891 to AMB. HYZ is supported by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute (HHMI).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Roy V Sillitoe, Email: sillitoe@bcm.edu.

Vatsala Thirumalai, National Centre for Biological Sciences, India.

K VijayRaghavan, National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, India.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Dystonia Medical Research Foundation to Meike E van der Heijden.

  • Howard Hughes Medical Institute to Huda Y Zoghbi.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke R01NS089664 to Roy V Sillitoe.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke R01NS100874 to Roy V Sillitoe.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke R01NS119301 to Roy V Sillitoe.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke F31NS101891 to Amanda M Brown.

Additional information

Competing interests

Reviewing editor, eLife.

Senior editor, eLife.

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Formal analysis, Writing - review and editing.

Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All animals were housed in an AALAS-certified facility on a 14hr light cycle. Husbandry, housing, euthanasia, and experimental guidelines were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of Baylor College of Medicine (protocol number: AN-5996).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for Figures 1, 3–9.

References

  1. Adams NC, Tomoda T, Cooper M, Dietz G, Hatten ME. Mice that lack astrotactin have slowed neuronal migration. Development. 2002;129:965–972. doi: 10.1242/dev.129.4.965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aiba A, Kano M, Chen C, Stanton ME, Fox GD, Herrup K, Zwingman TA, Tonegawa S. Deficient cerebellar long-term depression and impaired motor learning in mGluR1 mutant mice. Cell. 1994;79:377–388. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alcott CE, Yalamanchili HK, Ji P, van der Heijden ME, Saltzman A, Elrod N, Lin A, Leng M, Bhatt B, Hao S, Wang Q, Saliba A, Tang J, Malovannaya A, Wagner EJ, Liu Z, Zoghbi HY. Partial loss of CFIm25 causes learning deficits and aberrant neuronal alternative polyadenylation. eLife. 2020;9:e50895. doi: 10.7554/eLife.50895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Altman J, Anderson WJ. Irradiation of the cerebellum in infant rats with low-level x-ray: histological and cytological effects during infancy and adulthood. Experimental Neurology. 1971;30:492–509. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(71)90150-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Apps R, Hawkes R, Aoki S, Bengtsson F, Brown AM, Chen G, Ebner TJ, Isope P, Jörntell H, Lackey EP, Lawrenson C, Lumb B, Schonewille M, Sillitoe RV, Spaeth L, Sugihara I, Valera A, Voogd J, Wylie DR, Ruigrok TJH. Cerebellar modules and their role as operational cerebellar processing units: a consensus paper [corrected] Cerebellum. 2018;17:654–682. doi: 10.1007/s12311-018-0952-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Arancillo M, White JJ, Lin T, Stay TL, Sillitoe RV. In vivo analysis of purkinje cell firing properties during postnatal mouse development. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2015;113:578–591. doi: 10.1152/jn.00586.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Armstrong C, Hawkes R. Selective Purkinje cell ectopia in the cerebellum of the weaver mouse. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2001;439:151–161. doi: 10.1002/cne.1339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Arsénio Nunes ML, Sotelo C, Wehrlé R. Organization of spinocerebellar projection map in three types of agranular cerebellum: Purkinje cells vs. granule cells as organizer element. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1988;273:120–136. doi: 10.1002/cne.902730110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Badura A, Verpeut JL, Metzger JW, Pereira TD, Pisano TJ, Deverett B, Bakshinskaya DE, Wang SS. Normal cognitive and social development require posterior cerebellar activity. eLife. 2018;7:e36401. doi: 10.7554/eLife.36401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barron T, Kim JH. Preterm birth impedes structural and functional development of cerebellar purkinje cells in the developing baboon cerebellum. Brain Sciences. 2020;10:897. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10120897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bautista TG, Dutschmann M. Inhibition of the pontine Kölliker-Fuse nucleus abolishes eupneic inspiratory hypoglossal motor discharge in rat. Neuroscience. 2014;267:22–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.02.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Beekhof GC, Osório C, White JJ, van Zoomeren S, van der Stok H, Xiong B, Nettersheim IH, Mak WA, Runge M, Fiocchi FR, Boele HJ, Hoebeek FE, Schonewille M. Differential spatiotemporal development of Purkinje cell populations and cerebellum-dependent sensorimotor behaviors. eLife. 2021;10:e63668. doi: 10.7554/eLife.63668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ben-Arie N, Bellen HJ, Armstrong DL, McCall AE, Gordadze PR, Guo Q, Matzuk MM, Zoghbi HY. Math1 is essential for genesis of cerebellar granule neurons. Nature. 1997;390:169–172. doi: 10.1038/36579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bolivar VJ, Manley K, Fentress JC. The development of swimming behavior in the neurological mutant weaver mouse. Developmental Psychobiology. 1996;29:123–137. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199603)29:2&#x0003c;123::AID-DEV3&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-U. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bradley P, Berry M. The effects of reduced climbing and parallel fibre input on Purkinje cell dendritic growth. Brain Research. 1976;109:133–151. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90384-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brochu G, Maler L, Hawkes R. Zebrin II: a polypeptide antigen expressed selectively by Purkinje cells reveals compartments in rat and fish cerebellum. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1990;291:538–552. doi: 10.1002/cne.902910405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brown AM, Arancillo M, Lin T, Catt DR, Zhou J, Lackey EP, Stay TL, Zuo Z, White JJ, Sillitoe RV. Molecular layer interneurons shape the spike activity of cerebellar purkinje cells. Scientific Reports. 2019;9:1742. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-38264-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brown AM, White JJ, van der Heijden ME, Zhou J, Lin T, Sillitoe RV. Purkinje cell misfiring generates high-amplitude action tremors that are corrected by cerebellar deep brain stimulation. eLife. 2020;9:e51928. doi: 10.7554/eLife.51928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chamberlin NL. Functional organization of the parabrachial complex and intertrigeminal region in the control of breathing. Respiratory Physiology & Neurobiology. 2004;143:115–125. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2004.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chang CH, Chang FM, Yu CH, Ko HC, Chen HY. Assessment of fetal cerebellar volume using three-dimensional ultrasound. Ultrasound in Medicine & Biology. 2000;26:981–988. doi: 10.1016/S0301-5629(00)00225-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Consalez GG, Goldowitz D, Casoni F, Hawkes R. Origins, development, and compartmentation of the granule cells of the cerebellum. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2020;14:611841. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2020.611841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Corrales JD, Rocco GL, Blaess S, Guo Q, Joyner AL. Spatial pattern of sonic hedgehog signaling through Gli genes during cerebellum development. Development. 2004;131:5581–5590. doi: 10.1242/dev.01438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Corrales JD, Blaess S, Mahoney EM, Joyner AL. The level of sonic hedgehog signaling regulates the complexity of cerebellar foliation. Development. 2006;133:1811–1821. doi: 10.1242/dev.02351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Crepel F. Maturation of the cerebellar Purkinje cells. Experimental Brain Research. 1972;14:472–479. doi: 10.1007/BF00236589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Crepel F, Delhaye-Bouchaud N. Distribution of climbing fibres on cerebellar Purkinje cells in X-irradiated rats. An electrophysiological study. The Journal of Physiology. 1979;290:97–112. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dahmane N, Ruiz i Altaba A. Sonic hedgehog regulates the growth and patterning of the cerebellum. Development. 1999;126:3089–3100. doi: 10.1242/dev.126.14.3089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dastjerdi FV, Consalez GG, Hawkes R. Pattern formation during development of the embryonic cerebellum. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy. 2012;6:10. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2012.00010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Davie JT, Clark BA, Häusser M. The origin of the complex spike in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:7599–7609. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0559-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Davis CA, Joyner AL. Expression patterns of the homeo box-containing genes En-1 and En-2 and the proto-oncogene int-1 diverge during mouse development. Genes & Development. 1988;2:1736–1744. doi: 10.1101/gad.2.12b.1736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dijkshoorn ABC, Turk E, Hortensius LM, van der Aa NE, Hoebeek FE, Groenendaal F, Benders M, Dudink J. Preterm infants with isolated cerebellar hemorrhage show bilateral cortical alterations at term equivalent age. Scientific Reports. 2020;10:5283. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-62078-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dobbing J. The later growth of the brain and its vulnerability. Pediatrics. 1974;53:2–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dusart I, Guenet JL, Sotelo C. Purkinje cell death: differences between developmental cell death and neurodegenerative death in mutant mice. The Cerebellum. 2006;5:163–173. doi: 10.1080/14734220600699373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dutschmann M, Herbert H. The Kölliker-Fuse nucleus gates the postinspiratory phase of the respiratory cycle to control inspiratory off-switch and upper airway resistance in rat. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;24:1071–1084. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04981.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Falconer DS. Two new mutants, 'trembler' and 'reeler', with neurological actions in the house mouse (Mus musculus L.) Journal of Genetics. 1951;50:192–205. doi: 10.1007/BF02996215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ferreira TA, Blackman AV, Oyrer J, Jayabal S, Chung AJ, Watt AJ, Sjöström PJ, van Meyel DJ. Neuronal morphometry directly from bitmap images. Nature Methods. 2014;11:982–984. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fremont R, Calderon DP, Maleki S, Khodakhah K. Abnormal high-frequency burst firing of cerebellar neurons in rapid-onset dystonia-parkinsonism. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:11723–11732. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1409-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fujita E, Tanabe Y, Shiota A, Ueda M, Suwa K, Momoi MY, Momoi T. Ultrasonic vocalization impairment of Foxp2 (R552H) knockin mice related to speech-language disorder and abnormality of Purkinje cells. PNAS. 2008;105:3117–3122. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0712298105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Fujita H, Morita N, Furuichi T, Sugihara I. Clustered fine compartmentalization of the mouse embryonic cerebellar cortex and its rearrangement into the postnatal striped configuration. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:15688–15703. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1710-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Galliano E, Gao Z, Schonewille M, Todorov B, Simons E, Pop AS, D'Angelo E, van den Maagdenberg AM, Hoebeek FE, De Zeeuw CI. Silencing the majority of cerebellar granule cells uncovers their essential role in motor learning and consolidation. Cell Reports. 2013;3:1239–1251. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gano D, Barkovich AJ. Cerebellar hypoplasia of prematurity: causes and consequences. Handb Clin Neurol. 2019;162:201–216. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-64029-1.00009-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gao Z, Proietti-Onori M, Lin Z, Ten Brinke MM, Boele HJ, Potters JW, Ruigrok TJ, Hoebeek FE, De Zeeuw CI. Excitatory cerebellar nucleocortical circuit provides internal amplification during associative conditioning. Neuron. 2016;89:645–657. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gebre SA, Reeber SL, Sillitoe RV. Parasagittal compartmentation of cerebellar mossy fibers as revealed by the patterned expression of vesicular glutamate transporters VGLUT1 and VGLUT2. Brain Structure and Function. 2012;217:165–180. doi: 10.1007/s00429-011-0339-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gold DA, Gent PM, Hamilton BA. ROR alpha in genetic control of cerebellum development: 50 staggering years. Brain Research. 2007;1140:19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Goldowitz D, Cushing RC, Laywell E, D'Arcangelo G, Sheldon M, Sweet HO, Davisson M, Steindler D, Curran T. Cerebellar disorganization characteristic of Reeler in scrambler mutant mice despite presence of reelin. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:8767–8777. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-22-08767.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hashimoto K, Yoshida T, Sakimura K, Mishina M, Watanabe M, Kano M. Influence of parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapse formation on postnatal development of climbing fiber-Purkinje cell synapses in the cerebellum. Neuroscience. 2009;162:601–611. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.12.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hashimoto M, Mikoshiba K. Mediolateral compartmentalization of the cerebellum is determined on the "birth date" of Purkinje cells. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:11342–11351. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-36-11342.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Herculano-Houzel S, Mota B, Lent R. Cellular scaling rules for rodent brains. PNAS. 2006;103:12138–12143. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604911103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Herrup K. Role of staggerer gene in determining cell number in cerebellar cortex. I. granule cell death is an indirect consequence of staggerer gene action. Developmental Brain Research. 1983;11:267–274. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(83)90225-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Herzmann CS, Snyder AZ, Kenley JK, Rogers CE, Shimony JS, Smyser CD. Cerebellar functional connectivity in term- and very Preterm-Born infants. Cerebral Cortex. 2019;29:1174–1184. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Heuzé P, Féron C, Baudoin C. Early behavioral development of mice is affected by staggerer mutation as soon as postnatal day three. Developmental Brain Research. 1997;101:81–84. doi: 10.1016/S0165-3806(97)00049-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Holt GR, Softky WR, Koch C, Douglas RJ. Comparison of discharge variability in vitro and in vivo in cat visual cortex neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1996;75:1806–1814. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.5.1806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hortensius LM, Dijkshoorn ABC, Ecury-Goossen GM, Steggerda SJ, Hoebeek FE, Benders M, Dudink J. Neurodevelopmental consequences of preterm isolated cerebellar hemorrhage: a systematic review. Pediatrics. 2018;142:e20180609. doi: 10.1542/peds.2018-0609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hoshino M, Nakamura S, Mori K, Kawauchi T, Terao M, Nishimura YV, Fukuda A, Fuse T, Matsuo N, Sone M, Watanabe M, Bito H, Terashima T, Wright CV, Kawaguchi Y, Nakao K, Nabeshima Y. Ptf1a, a bHLH transcriptional gene, defines GABAergic neuronal fates in cerebellum. Neuron. 2005;47:201–213. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Huang C, Gammon SJ, Dieterle M, Huang RH, Likins L, Ricklefs RE. Dramatic increases in number of cerebellar granule-cell-Purkinje-cell synapses across several mammals. Mammalian Biology. 2014;79:163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.mambio.2013.12.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hull C. Prediction signals in the cerebellum: beyond supervised motor learning. eLife. 2020;9:e54073. doi: 10.7554/eLife.54073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Iskusnykh IY, Buddington RK, Chizhikov VV. Preterm birth disrupts cerebellar development by affecting granule cell proliferation program and Bergmann Glia. Experimental Neurology. 2018;306:209–221. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2018.05.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Jensen P, Zoghbi HY, Goldowitz D. Dissection of the cellular and molecular events that position cerebellar Purkinje cells: a study of the math1 null-mutant mouse. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:8110–8116. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-18-08110.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Jensen P, Smeyne R, Goldowitz D. Analysis of cerebellar development in math1 null embryos and chimeras. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:2202–2211. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3427-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kalinovsky A, Boukhtouche F, Blazeski R, Bornmann C, Suzuki N, Mason CA, Scheiffele P. Development of axon-target specificity of ponto-cerebellar afferents. PLOS Biology. 2011;9:e1001013. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kano M, Watanabe T, Uesaka N, Watanabe M. Multiple phases of climbing fiber synapse elimination in the developing cerebellum. The Cerebellum. 2018;17:722–734. doi: 10.1007/s12311-018-0964-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kano M, Hashimoto K. Activity-dependent maturation of climbing fiber to Purkinje cell synapses during postnatal cerebellar development. The Cerebellum. 2012;11:449–450. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0337-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kuemerle B, Gulden F, Cherosky N, Williams E, Herrup K. The mouse engrailed genes: a window into autism. Behavioural Brain Research. 2007;176:121–132. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lackey EP, Sillitoe RV. Eph/ephrin function contributes to the patterning of spinocerebellar mossy fibers into parasagittal zones. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2020;14:7. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2020.00007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lalonde R, Strazielle C. Behavioral effects of neonatal lesions on the cerebellar system. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 2015;43:58–65. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2015.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lang EJ, Sugihara I, Llinás R. Differential roles of apamin- and charybdotoxin-sensitive K+ conductances in the generation of inferior olive rhythmicity in vivo. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:2825–2838. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-08-02825.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Larouche M, Hawkes R. From clusters to stripes: the developmental origins of adult cerebellar compartmentation. The Cerebellum. 2006;5:77–88. doi: 10.1080/14734220600804668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Larsell O. The morphogenesis and adult pattern of the lobules and fissures of the cerebellum of the white rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1952;97:281–356. doi: 10.1002/cne.900970204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Le Marec N, Stelz T, Delhaye-Bouchaud N, Mariani J, Caston J. Effect of cerebellar granule cell depletion on learning of the equilibrium behaviour: study in postnatally X-irradiated rats. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;9:2472–2478. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01664.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. LeDoux MS, Lorden JF. Abnormal spontaneous and harmaline-stimulated Purkinje cell activity in the awake genetically dystonic rat. Experimental Brain Research. 2002;145:457–467. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1127-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Limperopoulos C, Soul JS, Gauvreau K, Huppi PS, Warfield SK, Bassan H, Robertson RL, Volpe JJ, du Plessis AJ. Late gestation cerebellar growth is rapid and impeded by premature birth. Pediatrics. 2005;115:688–695. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Limperopoulos C, Bassan H, Gauvreau K, Robertson RL, Sullivan NR, Benson CB, Avery L, Stewart J, Soul JS, Ringer SA, Volpe JJ, duPlessis AJ. Does cerebellar injury in premature infants contribute to the high prevalence of long-term cognitive, learning, and behavioral disability in survivors? Pediatrics. 2007;120:584–593. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Liu Y, McAfee SS, Sillitoe RV, Heck DH. Cerebellar modulation of gamma coherence between prefrontal cortex and Hippocampus during spatial working memory decision making. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.03.16.994541. [DOI]
  73. Machold R, Fishell G. Math1 is expressed in temporally discrete pools of cerebellar rhombic-lip neural progenitors. Neuron. 2005;48:17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Mason CA, Gregory E. Postnatal maturation of cerebellar mossy and climbing fibers: transient expression of dual features on single axons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1984;4:1715–1735. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-07-01715.1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. McAfee SS, Liu Y, Sillitoe RV, Heck DH. Cerebellar lobulus simplex and crus I differentially represent phase and phase difference of prefrontal cortical and hippocampal oscillations. Cell Reports. 2019;27:2328–2334. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.04.085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. McKay BE, Turner RW. Physiological and morphological development of the rat cerebellar purkinje cell. The Journal of Physiology. 2005;567:829–850. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.089383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Miterko LN, White JJ, Lin T, Brown AM, O'Donovan KJ, Sillitoe RV. Persistent motor dysfunction despite homeostatic rescue of cerebellar morphogenesis in the Car8 waddles mutant mouse. Neural Development. 2019;14:6. doi: 10.1186/s13064-019-0130-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Miterko LN, Lin T, Zhou J, van der Heijden ME, Beckinghausen J, White JJ, Sillitoe RV. Neuromodulation of the cerebellum rescues movement in a mouse model of ataxia. Nature Communications. 2021;12:1295. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-21417-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Miyata T, Ono Y, Okamoto M, Masaoka M, Sakakibara A, Kawaguchi A, Hashimoto M, Ogawa M. Migration, early axonogenesis, and Reelin-dependent layer-forming behavior of early/posterior-born Purkinje cells in the developing mouse lateral cerebellum. Neural Development. 2010;5:23. doi: 10.1186/1749-8104-5-23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Miyazaki T, Fukaya M, Shimizu H, Watanabe M. Subtype switching of vesicular glutamate transporters at parallel fibre-Purkinje cell synapses in developing mouse cerebellum. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;17:2563–2572. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02698.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Napper RM, Harvey RJ. Number of parallel fiber synapses on an individual Purkinje cell in the cerebellum of the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1988;274:168–177. doi: 10.1002/cne.902740204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Pan N, Jahan I, Lee JE, Fritzsch B. Defects in the Cerebella of conditional Neurod1 null mice correlate with effective Tg(Atoh1-cre) recombination and granule cell requirements for Neurod1 for differentiation. Cell and Tissue Research. 2009;337:407–428. doi: 10.1007/s00441-009-0826-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Park H, Kim T, Kim J, Yamamoto Y, Tanaka-Yamamoto K. Inputs from sequentially developed parallel fibers are required for cerebellar organization. Cell Reports. 2019;28:2939–2954. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Puro DG, Woodward DJ. Maturation of evoked climbing fiber input to rat cerebellar purkinje cells (I.) Experimental Brain Research. 1977;28:85–100. doi: 10.1007/BF00237088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Raman IM, Bean BP. Ionic currents underlying spontaneous action potentials in isolated cerebellar Purkinje neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:1663–1674. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-05-01663.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Rose MF, Ahmad KA, Thaller C, Zoghbi HY. Excitatory neurons of the proprioceptive, interoceptive, and arousal hindbrain networks share a developmental requirement for Math1. PNAS. 2009;106:22462–22467. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911579106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sathyanesan A, Kundu S, Abbah J, Gallo V. Neonatal brain injury causes cerebellar learning deficits and Purkinje cell dysfunction. Nature Communications. 2018;9:3235. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05656-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Sathyanesan A, Zhou J, Scafidi J, Heck DH, Sillitoe RV, Gallo V. Emerging connections between cerebellar development, behaviour and complex brain disorders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2019;20:298–313. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0152-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Schneider CA, Rasband WS, Eliceiri KW. NIH image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nature Methods. 2012;9:671–675. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Sheldon M, Rice DS, D'Arcangelo G, Yoneshima H, Nakajima K, Mikoshiba K, Howell BW, Cooper JA, Goldowitz D, Curran T. Scrambler and yotari disrupt the disabled gene and produce a reeler-like phenotype in mice. Nature. 1997;389:730–733. doi: 10.1038/39601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Shroyer NF, Helmrath MA, Wang VY, Antalffy B, Henning SJ, Zoghbi HY. Intestine-specific ablation of mouse atonal homolog 1 (Math1) reveals a role in cellular homeostasis. Gastroenterology. 2007;132:2478–2488. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2007.03.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Sidman RL, Lane PW, Dickie MM. Staggerer, a new mutation in the mouse affecting the cerebellum. Science. 1962;137:610–612. doi: 10.1126/science.137.3530.610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Sidman RL, Green MC, Appel SH. Catalog of the Neurological Mutants of the Mouse. London, England: Harvard University Press; 1965. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  94. Sillitoe RV, Vogel MW, Joyner AL. Engrailed homeobox genes regulate establishment of the cerebellar afferent circuit map. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:10015–10024. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0653-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Sillitoe RV. Mossy fibers terminate directly within purkinje cell zones during mouse development. The Cerebellum. 2016;15:14–17. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0712-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Sillitoe RV, Hawkes R. Whole-mount immunohistochemistry: a high-throughput screen for patterning defects in the mouse cerebellum. Journal of Histochemistry & Cytochemistry. 2002;50:235–244. doi: 10.1177/002215540205000211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Sillitoe RV, Joyner AL. Morphology, molecular codes, and circuitry produce the three-dimensional complexity of the cerebellum. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology. 2007;23:549–577. doi: 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.23.090506.123237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Sokoloff G, Plumeau AM, Mukherjee D, Blumberg MS. Twitch-related and rhythmic activation of the developing cerebellar cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2015;114:1746–1756. doi: 10.1152/jn.00284.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Sotelo C, Dusart I. Intrinsic versus extrinsic determinants during the development of Purkinje cell dendrites. Neuroscience. 2009;162:589–600. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.12.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Steggerda SJ, Leijser LM, Wiggers-de Bruïne FT, van der Grond J, Walther FJ, van Wezel-Meijler G. Cerebellar injury in preterm infants: incidence and findings on US and MR images. Radiology. 2009;252:190–199. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2521081525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Stoodley CJ, Limperopoulos C. Structure-function relationships in the developing cerebellum: evidence from early-life cerebellar injury and neurodevelopmental disorders. Seminars in Fetal and Neonatal Medicine. 2016;21:356–364. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2016.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Sugihara I, Lang EJ, Llinás R. Serotonin modulation of inferior olivary oscillations and synchronicity: a Multiple-electrode study in the rat cerebellum. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;7:521–534. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1995.tb00657.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Sugihara I, Fujita H. Peri- and postnatal development of cerebellar compartments in the mouse. The Cerebellum. 2013;12:325–327. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0450-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Surchev L, Nazwar TA, Weisheit G, Schilling K. Developmental increase of total cell numbers in the murine cerebellum. The Cerebellum. 2007;6:315–320. doi: 10.1080/14734220601169699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Takeo YH, Shuster SA, Jiang L, Hu MC, Luginbuhl DJ, Rülicke T, Contreras X, Hippenmeyer S, Wagner MJ, Ganguli S, Luo L. GluD2- and Cbln1-mediated competitive interactions shape the dendritic arbors of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Neuron. 2021;109:629–644. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Tano D, Napieralski JA, Eisenman LM, Messer A, Plummer J, Hawkes R. Novel developmental boundary in the cerebellum revealed by zebrin expression in the lurcher (Lc/+) mutant mouse. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1992;323:128–136. doi: 10.1002/cne.903230111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. van der Heijden ME, Kizek DJ, Perez R, Ruff EK, Ehrlich ME, Sillitoe RV. Abnormal cerebellar function and tremor in a mouse model for non-manifesting partially penetrant dystonia type 6. The Journal of Physiology. 2021a;599:2037–2054. doi: 10.1113/JP280978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. van der Heijden ME, Gill JS, Sillitoe RV. Abnormal cerebellar development in autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Neuroscience. 2021b;43:181–190. doi: 10.1159/000515189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. van der Heijden ME, Sillitoe RV. Interactions between purkinje cells and granule cells coordinate the development of functional cerebellar circuits. Neuroscience. 2021;462:4–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. van der Heijden ME, Zoghbi HY. Loss of Atoh1 from neurons regulating hypoxic and hypercapnic chemoresponses causes neonatal respiratory failure in mice. eLife. 2018;7:e38455. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. van Dorp S, De Zeeuw CI. Forward signaling by unipolar brush cells in the mouse cerebellum. The Cerebellum. 2015;14:528–533. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0693-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. van Welie I, Smith IT, Watt AJ. The metamorphosis of the developing cerebellar microcircuit. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2011;21:245–253. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Vogel MW, Sunter K, Herrup K. Numerical matching between granule and Purkinje cells in lurcher chimeric mice: a hypothesis for the trophic rescue of granule cells from target-related cell death. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1989;9:3454–3462. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-10-03454.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Volpe JJ. Cerebellum of the premature infant: rapidly developing, vulnerable, clinically important. Journal of Child Neurology. 2009;24:1085–1104. doi: 10.1177/0883073809338067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Wagner MJ, Luo L. Neocortex-Cerebellum circuits for cognitive processing. Trends in Neurosciences. 2020;43:42–54. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2019.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Wang VY, Rose MF, Zoghbi HY. Math1 expression redefines the rhombic lip derivatives and reveals novel lineages within the brainstem and cerebellum. Neuron. 2005;48:31–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Wang SS, Kloth AD, Badura A. The cerebellum, sensitive periods, and autism. Neuron. 2014;83:518–532. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Wassef M, Sotelo C, Thomasset M, Granholm AC, Leclerc N, Rafrafi J, Hawkes R. Expression of compartmentation antigen zebrin I in cerebellar transplants. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1990;294:223–234. doi: 10.1002/cne.902940207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. White JJ, Arancillo M, Stay TL, George-Jones NA, Levy SL, Heck DH, Sillitoe RV. Cerebellar zonal patterning relies on purkinje cell neurotransmission. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:8231–8245. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0122-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. White JJ, Arancillo M, King A, Lin T, Miterko LN, Gebre SA, Sillitoe RV. Pathogenesis of severe ataxia and tremor without the typical signs of neurodegeneration. Neurobiology of Disease. 2016;86:86–98. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. White JJ, Sillitoe RV. Development of the cerebellum: from gene expression patterns to circuit maps. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Developmental Biology. 2013;2:149–164. doi: 10.1002/wdev.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. White JJ, Sillitoe RV. Genetic silencing of olivocerebellar synapses causes dystonia-like behaviour in mice. Nature Communications. 2017;8:14912. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Willett RT, Bayin NS, Lee AS, Krishnamurthy A, Wojcinski A, Lao Z, Stephen D, Rosello-Diez A, Dauber-Decker KL, Orvis GD, Wu Z, Tessier-Lavigne M, Joyner AL. Cerebellar nuclei excitatory neurons regulate developmental scaling of presynaptic Purkinje cell number and organ growth. eLife. 2019;8:e50617. doi: 10.7554/eLife.50617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Wingate RJ, Hatten ME. The role of the rhombic lip in avian cerebellum development. Development. 1999;126:4395–4404. doi: 10.1242/dev.126.20.4395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Woodward DJ, Hoffer BJ, Altman J. Physiological and pharmacological properties of Purkinje cells in rat cerebellum degranulated by postnatal x-irradiation. Journal of Neurobiology. 1974;5:283–304. doi: 10.1002/neu.480050402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Wurst W, Auerbach AB, Joyner AL. Multiple developmental defects in Engrailed-1 mutant mice: an early mid-hindbrain deletion and patterning defects in forelimbs and sternum. Development. 1994;120:2065–2075. doi: 10.1242/dev.120.7.2065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Yin J, Chen W, Chao ES, Soriano S, Wang L, Wang W, Cummock SE, Tao H, Pang K, Liu Z, Pereira FA, Samaco RC, Zoghbi HY, Xue M, Schaaf CP. Otud7a knockout mice recapitulate many neurological features of 15q13.3 Microdeletion Syndrome. The American Journal of Human Genetics. 2018;102:296–308. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2018.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Yoo JY, Mak GK, Goldowitz D. The effect of hemorrhage on the development of the postnatal mouse cerebellum. Experimental Neurology. 2014;252:85–94. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2013.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Zagha E, Lang EJ, Rudy B. Kv3.3 channels at the Purkinje cell soma are necessary for generation of the classical complex spike waveform. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:1291–1300. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4358-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Zanjani HS, Mariani J, Delhaye-Bouchaud N, Herrup K. Neuronal cell loss in heterozygous staggerer mutant mice: a model for genetic contributions to the aging process. Developmental Brain Research. 1992;67:153–160. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(92)90216-J. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Zayek MM, Benjamin JT, Maertens P, Trimm RF, Lal CV, Eyal FG. Cerebellar hemorrhage: a major morbidity in extremely preterm infants. Journal of Perinatology. 2012;32:699–704. doi: 10.1038/jp.2011.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Zhou J, Brown AM, Lackey EP, Arancillo M, Lin T, Sillitoe RV. Purkinje cell neurotransmission patterns cerebellar basket cells into zonal modules defined by distinct pinceau sizes. eLife. 2020;9:e55569. doi: 10.7554/eLife.55569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Vatsala Thirumalai1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

Cerebellar granule cells provide extensive excitatory inputs to Purkinje neurons. Using genetic tools to abolish granule cells, this study explores how granule cells could regulate the development of structural, synaptic and intrinsic firing properties of Purkinje neurons. The results from this study contribute to our understanding of cerebellar development and how developmental insults to the cerebellum may lead to motor disorders.

Decision letter after peer review:

[Editors’ note: the authors submitted for reconsideration following the decision after peer review. What follows is the decision letter after the first round of review.]

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "Maturation of Purkinje cell firing properties relies on granule cell neurogenesis" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

Our decision has been reached after consultation between the reviewers. Based on these discussions and the individual reviews below, we regret to inform you that your work will not be considered further for publication in eLife.

The reviewers appreciate the authors for collecting this data set recording the various effects of loss of granule cells on Purkinje cells and motor behaviors. However, reviewers also pointed out several weaknesses in the manuscript. Primarily, the manuscript feels like a collection of unrelated observations as no clear causal link is established between the various cellular/behavioral phenotypes. Secondly, some of the phenotypes reported have also been seen in other models of agranularity and the results presented are along expected lines, taking away from the novelty of their study. Thirdly, several of the claims need stronger support and quantification with accompanying statistical testing. Please see the individual reviews appended below for more detail on these comments. We grappled with whether we could consider a revision that addressed these concerns. In the event, they are rather substantive and attending to them will take time and experimental effort, This will not be feasible in the time we usually expect. Should the authors be able to address all concerns fully, they may want to consider a fresh submission to eLife.

Reviewer #1:

In this manuscript, Sillitoe and colleagues use a novel conditional knockout mouse line to ask how the absence of granule cells affects Purkinje neuron development and function. By knocking out the Atoh1 gene in the Engrailed1 lineage, they generate mice that are 'agranular' – i.e. lacking cerebellar granule cells. This mouse has previously been studied by two of the authors with respect to respiratory networks (van der Heijden and Zoghbi, eLife, 2018) where also they report the loss of granule cells. The main focus of this paper is the effect of agranularity on Purkinje cells and motor behaviors.

Using immunohistochemistry, authors show that Purkinje neurons are present, but have stunted dendritic arbors and lack clear organization into ZebrinII stripes as compared to controls. They also seem to receive more vGluT2 positive synapses (presumably from mossy fibers and climbing fibers), while lacking the vGluT1 positive parallel fiber synapses. Next authors record extracellularly from Purkinje cells of control mice during the first two post-natal weeks and from the agranular mice at P14. Purkinje neurons show a less bursty and irregular simple spike firing pattern in the agranular mice compared to control, which seems to be similar to the immature activity pattern seen at P8. Complex spikes are also less frequent. Finally authors show that the agranular mice exhibit several severe motor deficits including deficits in righting reflex, ultrasonic vocalizations and the presence of tremor.

These results, documenting severe impact on Purkinje neuron development, physiology and behavior are in contrast to earlier studies which showed mild effects on Purkinje neuron physiology after acute silencing of granule cell input. Thus the authors argue that the developmental presence of granule cells is more important in sculpting the Purkinje cell layer rather than just the excitatory synaptic input they provide.

While these ideas are compelling, I was left somewhat disappointed by this paper as there aren't any clear threads emerging from the various lines of investigation, nor are there links that connect them directly. What is the connection between data presented in figures 2,3 and 4? It is unclear which of these deficits reported in these figures are a direct result of not having granule cells and which are due to secondary effects arising out of compensatory and/or other pathways that may be affected. For instance, with granule cells being absent, feed forward inhibition in the molecular layer is also likely to be affected. With the behavioral readouts, effect of the mutations on extracerebellar structures cannot be ruled out either.

Another point is that it is also not clear whether any or all of these changes are merely a delay in development and whether they persist into adulthood. The authors should consider repeating some of their assays in the adults.

Figure 2 needs quantitative measures, especially with statements about the density of synapses. The argument that the increased vGlut2 synapses could be from retained mossy fibers is not investigated further, but needs to be.

For figure 3, there are a number of methods available to better estimate the regularity of neural activity, such as looking at power spectral density. Peak frequency and power at that frequency should be estimated for controls and mutants.

Are granule cells completely absent? The staining in Figure supplement 1A for the mutants appears weak and diffuse, such as would be seen if some granule cells were indeed present, but not organized into a layer. Authors should consider using another marker to show the agranularity as this is a major point of their paper. Likewise quantifications are necessary for making points such as "reduced number of UBCs". Are the numbers of Purkinje cells normal?

The discussion tries to place the study in the context of cerebellar pathologies in pre-term infants however, some of the statements are very strong and need to be toned down (such as that in lines 255-258). Authors should also try to address how they think the agranular state is influencing the properties of Purkinje neurons as described in the figures. In addition they should clearly state how the different results are related or not. Authors mention seeing lower number of complex spikes in mutants, but what they think this means is not discussed at all. Same goes for the doublets and for the zebrin stripes.

Reviewer #2:

This manuscript by van der Heijden et al. presents an interesting and thorough analysis of the cerebellar deficits in a novel mouse model, in which the transcription factor Atoh1 is conditionally deleted in Engrailed 1-expressing cells. Given what we know about cerebellar development and the consequences of disrupting this complex process from previous mouse models, the results of the study were largely to be expected. However, as the authors elegantly demonstrate, their new model is highly specific and reproducible and allows the interrogation of effects mostly specific to granule cell neurogenesis.

Reviewer #3:

In this article, van der Heijden et al. use mouse genetics to generate a model in which the transcription factor Atoh1 is deleted from the En1 domain and the cerebellum lacks granule cells. They analyze the consequences on cerebellar development, Purkinje cell firing properties and mouse behavior. They conclude that "granule cell neurogenesis sets the maturation time window for Purkinje cell function and refines cerebellar-dependent behavior." The mouse model generated by the authors provides a controlled way to generate agranular mutants and its analysis can provide new information on the mechanisms controlling the development of the cerebellar circuit. The morphological and behavior analysis confirms observations and conclusions made previously by several other groups through comparisons of different agranular models. The role of granule cells in the maturation of PC firing properties is an interesting new observation that deserves to be documented. However more work is needed to confirm that the phenotype is a blockade of maturation and to determine how granule cells control the development of this property.

1. The first conclusion of the manuscript is that lack of granule cells due to deletion of Atoh1 in the En1 domain leads to abnormal cerebellar development, Purkinje cell dendritic differentiation and circuit defects. While figure 1 clearly illustrates the major effect on cerebellar morphogenesis, the effects on the different neuronal populations deserves better documentation. The results of some of the immunostainings are not sufficient to document the lack of neurons (for example GABA-Ra6 or HCN1), and quantifications of the effects on cell numbers are not provided. In particular since unipolar brush cells are affected by the mutation, the effect should be quantified. The result on cerebellar development and Purkinje cell differentiation and connectivity is expected from previous research using x-irradiation and genetic mutants (these need to be cited in the Van der Heijden manuscript). Pan et al. (Cell Tissue Res (2009) 337:407-428) have shown that lack of granule cell death (induced by NeuroD1 knockout in the Atoh1 lineage) leads to defects in PC dendritogenesis and deficient mossy fiber targeting. Granule cell depletion using X-irradiation during early postnatal development in the rat has shown in addition defects in climbing fiber and mossy fiber connectivity both by electrophysiological and morphological analysis (cf. not only the cited reference Altman and Anderson 1971, but also for example Puro and Woodward 1978, Crepel and Delhaye-Bouchaud 1979, Arsenio Nunes et al. 1988).

2. The second conclusion of the manuscript is that lack of granule cells blocks the maturation of Purkinje cell firing. The result is interesting: the Purkinje cell firing properties are not similar to controls at two weeks and resemble those that are found in controls at the end of the first postnatal week. However a major issue is that measurements of PC firing were done only at P14 in the mutant and compared to several ages in controls (cf. figure 3). This leads to a problem in interpretation : is the phenotype really the result of blockade of maturation?

3. The third conclusion of the manuscript is that cerebellum dependent behaviors are deficient in these agranular mutants. Given the morphological and circuit phenotype, this is expected. The authors argue that "the contribution of the cerebellum to these behaviors is established before circuit rewiring is completed" to justify their analysis. However, what they mean by this is unclear and references to support this affirmation are not provided.

[Editors’ note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Maturation of Purkinje cell firing properties relies on neurogenesis of excitatory neurons" for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by K VijayRaghavan as the Senior Editor.

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed, as outlined below:

The revised version of this manuscript includes new quantification and analysis and is a significant improvement. But, there are several points that remain to be clarified in the text or addressed with experiments and analysis. Please see the reviews below for a detailed description of these points.

Reviewer #1:

In this manuscript, authors generate mice lacking cerebellar granule cells using intersectional genetics and analyze the phenotype of these mice at the anatomical, physiological and behavioral levels. The major findings are that Purkinje neuron morphological and physiological development is impaired when granule cells are absent. In addition, motor coordination and ultrasonic vocalizations are also affected. These deficits are much more severe than the effects observed after acute silencing of granule cells in other studies. Thus, in addition to the synaptic influence of granule cells, their developmental presence at the correct stages seems to be critical for proper Purkinje cell development.

The manuscript is a significant improvement over the previous version that was submitted last year. Several results have now been properly quantified and add to the weight of their arguments. Nevertheless, some key points remain to be addressed:

1. The key result that the entire paper rests on is the agranular state of the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. The authors acknowledge that some granule cells and UBCs may escape and be present. The only data to show a lack of granule cells is the GABARa6 staining shown in Supp Figure 1-1A. As pointed out in the earlier review, given the criticality of knowing the extent of agranularity, authors should provide additional substantiation of this result. The ISH does not clarify whether the lower level of staining seen is non-specific background or remaining granule cells. This point needs to be clarified thoroughly. When the authors mention that there are escapees, how many cells do they see per animal?

2. Another point regarding genetic intervention is whether brain areas other than the cerebellum are affected. This is especially critical for understanding the basis of the behavioral phenotypes observed. Images showing ventricles and nuclear staining in key brain regions (cortex, hippocampus, striatum, etc) will help sort out this issue.

3. The authors claim that PCs in mutants are not bursty while those in controls at P14 are. The representative traces also show this clearly. Nevertheless, all of the analysis done is on spike regularity and do not reflect burstiness. The pause percent could reflect burstiness, but it will also depend on baseline firing rates, which are lower for the mutant. The autocorrelogram shows spike regularity within a +/- 100 ms window while the burst intervals are much longer and therefore are likely not reflected in the peaks of the autocorrelogram. The authors should consider Fourier analysis and show a heatmap power spectrum reflecting the various frequency bands found in PCs of P14 animals.

4. The authors' claims regarding Vglut1 and Vglut2 positive terminals onto PCs need more bolstering. Regarding Vglut1, the images shown in Figure 4 show diffuse weak staining of Vglut1 puncta. Can we rule out the existence of more widespread Vlglut1 synapses from escapee GCs and mossy fibers? In line 254, the authors state, "Taken together, we show that without excitatory cerebellar neurons, Purkinje cells do not acquire a large number of excitatory synapses during the second postnatal week." I do not believe that such a statement can be made based on these data on Vglut1 staining alone. To be able to say this, either mEPSC recordings or quantification of PF synapses using GFP positive lines must be done.

Similarly, for the Vglut2 staining, the authors state in line 368, "These data show that the rRL-derived excitatory cerebellar neurons are necessary for shaping the regional localization and cellular targeting of climbing fiber afferents that project from the inferior olive to the Purkinje cells." However, since Vglut2 is also present in mossy fibers, and it is not clear if the increased staining arises from an increased number of CFs projecting to single PCs, this is a strong interpretation of the results. For both vglut1 and vglut2 staining data, without additional experiments, effects on numbers or placement of CF/PF synapses cannot be unequivocally concluded.

Reviewer #2:

The authors have fully addressed my comments.

Reviewer #3:

This manuscript describes a smart genetic design to generate mice with agranular cerebella in a very reproducible manner and will be of interest to the field of cerebellar development. This mouse mutant recapitulates many previously described phenotypes for agranular mutants. A detailed in vivo analysis shows that Purkinje cells do not acquire mature firing properties, indicating a contribution of excitatory inputs to their development.

The manuscript from van der Heijden et al. describes the analysis of mice with an invalidation of the Atoh1 gene in the Engrailed 1 domain (mesencephalon and rhombomere 1) generated by breeding En1Cre mice with Atoh1Flox mice (En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/-). This should result in the invalidation of Atoh1 in excitatory neurons generated by the rostral rhombic lip, and thus impair the generation of cerebellar granule cells. The authors use a broad range of techniques to provide a thorough analysis of the phenotype of these mice at the morphological, functional and behavioral levels.

Using immunolabeling with different cellular markers and quantification for some of the cell types, the authors demonstrate that indeed the majority of cerebellar granule cells are not generated and that the number of unipolar brush cells and excitatory cerebellar nuclei neurons is significantly decreased. The dendritogenesis of cerebellar PCs, as well as excitatory synapse formation on these neurons, are profoundly affected. These mutant mice display behavioral deficits: tremor, motor deficits and alterations of the pattern of ultrasonic vocalizations.

The main novelty is in the analysis of PC activity during postnatal development. The authors perform a detailed analysis during the 3 first postnatal weeks in control mice and analyze the properties of mutant PCs at P14 and P18. They show that during the second postnatal week a change occurs in the spontaneous activity of PCs in control mice. In En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants the properties of PCs at P14 and P18 more closely resemble those of Purkinje cells in control mice until P10. The authors thus propose that the switch in Purkinje cell activity is under the control of excitatory neurons in the cerebellum, and most likely granule cells.

1. The manuscript should clearly mention the fact that many of the phenotypes found in the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants have already been described in other agranular models. While those models are less specific (genetic mutants) or less controlled (x-irradiation in neonates), their study has nonetheless led to similar findings. For example, in the X-irradiation model, the consequences have been described for PC dendritic development (cf. for review Sotelo and Dusart 2009); for circuit and synaptic maturation (multi-innervation by climbing fibers for example in Woodward, Hoffer and Altman, 1974 and Crepel, Delhaye-Bouchaud and Dupont 1981), ataxic behavior and tremor (Woodward, Hoffer and Altman, 1974) and Purkinje cell activity (also Woodward, Hoffer and Altman 1974). The similarities and differences between the phenotypes described in these papers and those in the van der Heijden manuscript should be discussed.

2. The mutant mice are called En1Cre/+/Atoh1fl/- and the analysis is done by comparing to "control" mice in all the manuscript except in figure 9 where 3 different controls are used in the behavioral analysis. How were the mutants generated? Is there a cross with an Atoh1-/-(not mentioned in the methods)? What genotype is "control" mice in the manuscript?

3. The authors describe that there is a change in PC spiking during the second postnatal week in controls and that mutant PCs do not acquire the characteristics of control PCs. The study of the developmental pattern of PC spontaneous firing in vivo adds to previously obtained results by the team (Arancillo et al; 2015) and others (Crepel, 1972). The analysis done by van der Heijden et al. is detailed and measures many parameters. Their data confirm the observation of a switch during the second postnatal week (Crepel 1972 should be cited, in particular line 260). However, in figure 5, no statistical test is performed to determine which data points are significantly different between the different control stages analyzed. The use of clustering and t-SNE analysis is helpful and defines two broad stages: until P10 and after. But there seem to be a large heterogeneity amongst Purkinje cells at each postnatal day, and it is not exactly clear what drives this heterogeneity.

4. For the mutant PCs, the authors state that "this developmental switch does not occur by P14 in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/-mice". I do not think this can be stated since there is no data on the mutant PCs during the first postnatal week. It could be that the PCs in the agranular cerebellum never behave like control ones. The authors should record from PCs in the mutant at P7 if they want to state that the phenotype is due to the non-occurrence of a developmental switch.

5. There is sometimes confusion between intrinsic spiking properties and spontaneous spiking in Purkinje cells. For example, line 82 in Sathyanesan et al. 2018, the authors study spontaneous and evoked firing properties of PCs, not intrinsic properties. In the discussion, it is not very clear to me what the authors mean by "In addition to the increased complexity of intrinsic cellular properties (McKay and Turner, 2005), we postulated that intercellular interactions during development may support the maturation of Purkinje cell firing"(line 532/533). Could actually granule cells change the intrinsic spiking properties of PCs ? Slice recordings would be helpful to study this.

6. Line 358: There is a switch between Vglut2 and vglut1 at Parallel fiber PC synapses during development (cf. Miyazaki EJN 2003). The decrease in VGLUT2 puncta density observed in the molecular layer is most likely due to this switch rather that CF synaptic pruning. It is thus difficult to interpret the result in panel 7C.

[Editors’ note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Maturation of Purkinje cell firing properties relies on neurogenesis of excitatory neurons" for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been reviewed by 1 peer reviewer and the evaluation has been overseen by K VijayRaghavan as the Senior Editor, and a Reviewing Editor.

In this revised version, the authors have addressed most of the points raised by the reviewers. However, a few points remain and these are detailed below.

Reviewer #3:

The authors have answered many of my concerns and the manuscript is significantly improved. There remain however several points that need to be addressed before publication.

1. If I understand well the explanation about the control mice, the controls are either Atoh1fl/+, En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+ or Atoh1fl/FlpO and the data are grouped together except in the behavioral analysis. This should be stated clearly not only in the methods section, but at the beginning of the Results section (line 138 and line 146), as it is important for the interpretation of the results (some of the variability in the "control" data could be due to the difference in genotype). As it is, the description is misleading. How was the Atoh1FlpO line characterized (I can only find genotyping data in a previous paper)? Is there any data showing that the three control genotypes are not different from each other and from wild-type animals?

2. Result section page 10 lines 342 to 345: Crepel had described in 1972 (Experimental Brain Research) in vivo recordings of Purkinje cell activity in rat neonates and showed increased frequency with maturation (see figure 1 of that paper). The Crepel paper should be cited in this part of the result section, as well as in the discussion page 19. Line 343, the Beekhof et al. article describes not only in vitro but also in vivo recordings of Purkinje cell activity. The sentence as it is in line 343 is misleading.

3. The last paragraph of the discussion should be simplified. Granule cells influence the maturation of Purkinje cell spiking (line 728-729), but also many other parameters of Purkinje cell development and maturation, as described in this manuscript but also previous data on agranular models. As the relationship between the different deficits is difficult to disentangle, this part of the discussion should be toned down (line 728 for example "direct influence").

eLife. 2021 Sep 20;10:e68045. doi: 10.7554/eLife.68045.sa2

Author response


[Editors’ note: the authors resubmitted a revised version of the paper for consideration. What follows is the authors’ response to the first round of review.]

Reviewer #1:

[…] I was left somewhat disappointed by this paper as there aren't any clear threads emerging from the various lines of investigation, nor are there links that connect them directly. What is the connection between data presented in figures 2,3 and 4?

In our revised manuscript, we have clarified the connections between the cellular anatomy and electrophysiology data as well as network-level behavioral abnormalities by focusing on the changes that occur between P7 and P14. In control animals, there are highly dynamic changes in cerebellar size (figure 1), density in Vglut1 and Vglut2 synapses (figure 4 and 7), and, as reported before, electrophysiological properties in Purkinje cells (figure 5, 6, and 8). None of these changes occur in our agranular mice.

It is unclear which of these deficits reported in these figures are a direct result of not having granule cells and which are due to secondary effects arising out of compensatory and/or other pathways that may be affected. For instance, with granule cells being absent, feed forward inhibition in the molecular layer is also likely to be affected. With the behavioral readouts, effect of the mutations on extracerebellar structures cannot be ruled out either.

We appreciate that the lack of granule cells has additional, secondary effects. We have included a paragraph in our discussion outlining the caveats of our model (Line 451-474) and the considerations that readers should take when interpreting the behavioral data (Line 499-547). Thus, we present the strengths and new information that our model has provided, but we now also provide a more balanced assessment of the data and interpretations.

Another point is that it is also not clear whether any or all of these changes are merely a delay in development and whether they persist into adulthood. The authors should consider repeating some of their assays in the adults.

This is a great question. Unfortunately, our mouse model does not survive post weaning age, likely due to a combination of breathing impairments and severe motor impairments (see Van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018). However, we have addressed this issue by performing additional experiments. In lieu of performing additional recordings in adult mice, therefore, we have provided additional analysis to show that in control animals there is a striking functional switch in Purkinje cell function that does not occur in the same ages of our mutant mice (Figure 6). The temporal coincidence of this switch, with rearrangement of synapses at the peak of granule cell integration, further suggests that this maturation is driven by granule cells entering the circuit, which never occurs in the agranular mice. Additionally, we recorded from P18 conditional knockout and control mice. By this time point, the functional switch has still not occurred. These data are included in Figure 6 – supplement 1. Again, we could not reliably record from mice older than P18 as the health of the mutants rapidly declines towards the time of death, which happens consistently within one to three days.

Figure 2 needs quantitative measures, especially with statements about the density of synapses. The argument that the increased vGlut2 synapses could be from retained mossy fibers is not investigated further, but needs to be.

Thank you, this is a good idea. We have now quantified the density of Vglut1 and Vlgut2 synapses and have adjusted our text accordingly.

For figure 3, there are a number of methods available to better estimate the regularity of neural activity, such as looking at power spectral density. Peak frequency and power at that frequency should be estimated for controls and mutants.

We already include the peak frequency as the frequency mode in our figure. We decided to include a second measure of regularity by calculating the rhythmicity index, which is computed by the oscillation properties of the autocorrelation simple spikes in our Purkinje cells. High oscillatory properties in the autocorrelation means highly rhythmic/regular firing patterns, thus a high rhythmicity index is means high regularity. This computation is included in Figure 5I-K.

Are granule cells completely absent? The staining in Figure supplement 1A for the mutants appears weak and diffuse, such as would be seen if some granule cells were indeed present, but not organized into a layer. Authors should consider using another marker to show the agranularity as this is a major point of their paper. Likewise quantifications are necessary for making points such as "reduced number of UBCs". Are the numbers of Purkinje cells normal?

We quantified the number of UBCs and excitatory nuclei cells and included this quantification in Supplemental Figure 1-2. It is likely that the number of Purkinje cells is reduced, as we have discussed in Lines 469-474. However, our investigations are based on density of excitatory synapses and single cell recordings, neither of which are likely affected by the total number of Purkinje cells.

Finally, it remains a possibility that a small number granule cells escape our genetic manipulation. However, this number is likely very low because (1) the granule cells are the major cause for cerebellar size expansion, which we do not see in our En1-Cre;Atoh1-fl/- mice; (2) we see no systematic expression of the granule cell marker, GABARα6, in our En1-Cre;Atoh1-fl/- mice; (3) we see a nearly 9-fold decrease in Vglut1 density, which is overwhelmingly due to the presence of mossy fiber projections, as the remaining granule cells provide only a minor contribution. We have included these points in our discussion Lines 451-461.

The discussion tries to place the study in the context of cerebellar pathologies in pre-term infants however, some of the statements are very strong and need to be toned down (such as that in lines 255-258). Authors should also try to address how they think the agranular state is influencing the properties of Purkinje neurons as described in the figures. In addition they should clearly state how the different results are related or not. Authors mention seeing lower number of complex spikes in mutants, but what they think this means is not discussed at all. Same goes for the doublets and for the zebrin stripes.

We have added in the text more explanations about the relationship between agranularity, changes in excitatory synapses onto Purkinje cells, and observed electrophysiological changes. We have vastly expanded our analysis of Vglut1/2 densities onto Purkinje cells, that show that lower Vglut1 density is paired with lower simple spike firing rate, whereas higher and more immature-like Vglut2 density is paired with lower complex spike number and higher number of doublets. We have also revised and expanded our discussion about the ZebrinII zones.

Reviewer #3:

[…] 1. The first conclusion of the manuscript is that lack of granule cells due to deletion of Atoh1 in the En1 domain leads to abnormal cerebellar development, Purkinje cell dendritic differentiation and circuit defects. While figure 1 clearly illustrates the major effect on cerebellar morphogenesis, the effects on the different neuronal populations deserves better documentation. The results of some of the immunostainings are not sufficient to document the lack of neurons (for example GABA-Ra6 or HCN1), and quantifications of the effects on cell numbers are not provided. In particular since unipolar brush cells are affected by the mutation, the effect should be quantified. The result on cerebellar development and Purkinje cell differentiation and connectivity is expected from previous research using x-irradiation and genetic mutants (these need to be cited in the Van der Heijden manuscript). Pan et al. (Cell Tissue Res (2009) 337:407-428) have shown that lack of granule cell death (induced by NeuroD1 knockout in the Atoh1 lineage) leads to defects in PC dendritogenesis and deficient mossy fiber targeting. Granule cell depletion using X-irradiation during early postnatal development in the rat has shown in addition defects in climbing fiber and mossy fiber connectivity both by electrophysiological and morphological analysis (cf. not only the cited reference Altman and Anderson 1971, but also for example Puro and Woodward 1978, Crepel and Delhaye-Bouchaud 1979, Arsenio Nunes et al. 1988).

We now quantified the number of unipolar brush cell, excitatory nuclei cells, and Vglut1/2 density. Please also see our responses above to Reviewer #1 and Reviewer #2.

We appreciate that this reviewer recognizes that our mouse model recapitulates many aspects of previously reported agranular mice. This was the intention of some of our experiments, as we needed to validate our new model for cerebellar agranularity. We specifically choose this model of agranularity because it was reliable, independent of experimental variability, and independent of genes expressed in Purkinje cells. The last was specifically important because we wanted to exclude the possibility that our results were confounded by Purkinje cell autonomous effects. The fact that many of the anatomical findings are similar to those reported in other models of agranularity, underscores the effectiveness of our new genetic approach. We have now included more references to previous models of agranularity to highlight the similarities to our new models. Specifically, we have added the excellent references the reviewer has suggested – thank you.

2. The second conclusion of the manuscript is that lack of granule cells blocks the maturation of Purkinje cell firing. The result is interesting: the Purkinje cell firing properties are not similar to controls at two weeks and resemble those that are found in controls at the end of the first postnatal week. However a major issue is that measurements of PC firing were done only at P14 in the mutant and compared to several ages in controls (cf. figure 3). This leads to a problem in interpretation : is the phenotype really the result of blockade of maturation?

This is an excellent point, and one that was also raised by Reviewer #1 above. We noted above that unfortunately, our mouse model does not survive post weaning age, likely due to a combination of breathing impairments and severe motor impairments (see Van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018). However, we have addressed this issue by performing additional experiments. In lieu of performing additional recordings in adult mice, therefore, we have provided additional analysis to show that in control animals there is a striking functional switch in Purkinje cell function that does not occur in the same ages of our mutant mice (Figure 6). The temporal coincidence of this switch, with rearrangement of synapses at the peak of granule cell integration, further suggests that this maturation is driven by granule cells entering the circuit, which never occurs in the agranular mice. Additionally, we recorded from P18 conditional knockout and control mice. By this time point, the functional switch has still not occurred. These data are included in Figure 6 – supplement 1. Again, we could not reliably record from mice older than P18 as the health of the mutants rapidly declines towards the time of death, which happens consistently within one to three days.

3. The third conclusion of the manuscript is that cerebellum dependent behaviors are deficient in these agranular mutants. Given the morphological and circuit phenotype, this is expected. The authors argue that "the contribution of the cerebellum to these behaviors is established before circuit rewiring is completed" to justify their analysis. However, what they mean by this is unclear and references to support this affirmation are not provided.

We included a more in-depth discussion of our findings in the behavioral essays in our discussion:

“The behavioral abnormalities in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice are likely caused by a combined effect of Purkinje cell dysfunction and a reduction of excitatory neurons in the cerebellar nuclei. Given the broad network effects of our manipulation, it is not surprising that En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice have severe behavioral abnormalities. […] This shows that unlike Purkinje cells, excitatory cerebellar neurons are not necessary for propagating tremor but are required for modulating it.”

[Editors’ note: what follows is the authors’ response to the second round of review.]

Reviewer #1:

[…] The manuscript is a significant improvement over the previous version that was submitted last year. Several results have now been properly quantified and add to the weight of their arguments. Nevertheless, some key points remain to be addressed:

1. The key result that the entire paper rests on is the agranular state of the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice. The authors acknowledge that some granule cells and UBCs may escape and be present. The only data to show a lack of granule cells is the GABARa6 staining shown in Supp Figure 1-1A. As pointed out in the earlier review, given the criticality of knowing the extent of agranularity, authors should provide additional substantiation of this result. The ISH does not clarify whether the lower level of staining seen is non-specific background or remaining granule cells. This point needs to be clarified thoroughly. When the authors mention that there are escapees, how many cells do they see per animal?

To address this comment, we decided to employ a genetic strategy to circumvent the potential ISH issues, as pointed out by the reviewer. In our genetic strategy, we marked all En1;Atoh1 lineage neurons with TdTomato using an intersectional allele (see Supp. Figure 1-3). In this method, the marker signal of granule cells in the granule cell layer of control animals is near saturation (which was fully expected), due the density of their cell bodies (99.6% ± 0.4% of the area of interest measured is occupied by the TdTomato signal). However, we were able to distinguish individual TdTomato+ neurons in the conditional knockout mice and estimated that ~65 thousand neurons escaped our genetic manipulation. This is an approximate thousand-fold reduction in neurons compared to what has previously been reported for control animals in the literature (between 35 and 70 million neurons using density estimations or isotropic fractionation (Vogel et al. 1989; Herculano-Houzel et al. 2006; Surchev et al. 2007; Consalez et al. 2020)).

We now provide several lines of evidence that all indicate and describe the significant reduction of granule cells in our conditional knockout mice:

1) The total number of Atoh1;En1 lineage neurons that are left in the mutant is in the range of tens of thousands of cells (versus tens of millions reported in the literature for control animals).

2) There is a nearly 50-fold reduction in cerebellar size (10X in sagittal plane, 5X in coronal plane), and the cerebellum does not have a visible granule cell layer or obvious cerebellar foliation. These morphological phenotypes encompass the most experimentally consistent and profound cerebellar malformations as a result of granule cell agenesis, and together culminate into a severe phenotype compared to previously reported manipulations. (X-irradiated rats: small cerebellar size, thin GCL (Zanjani et al. 1992; Armstrong and Hawkes 2001); staggerer and weaver mice: small cerebellar size, thin GCL, less foliation (Zanjani et al. 1992; Armstrong and Hawkes 2001); reeler and scrambler mice: small cerebellar size, thin GCL, no foliation (Goldowitz et al. 1997); Atoh1 chimera mice: small cerebellar size, thin/no GCL, less/no foliation (phenotype is variable depending on degree of chimerism) (Jensen et al. 2004)). Our mutants exhibit each of these phenotypes, but with increased severity.

3) The total density of Vglut1 signal is reduced 9-fold in the mutant mice (Figure 4). However, this number does not only account for the total escaper granule cells because the density analysis does not distinguish the granule cell parallel fiber synapses from either Vglut1 expression in mossy fibers or escaper UBCs, and in addition this estimate does not account for the overall size difference of the cerebellum.

4) Purkinje cell monolayer formation and the establishment of mature stripe/zonal patterns do not occur (Figure 1 and 2).

5) VGluT2+ climbing fibers and/or mossy fiber synapse displacement from the Purkinje cell bodies is severely impaired (Figure 2 and 7).

Based on these different but complementary lines of evidence, we conclude that our genetic model of agranularity significantly reduces granule cell neurogenesis to ultimately result in the cerebellum having only a fraction of differentiated granule cells. The manipulation therefore massively obstructs but does not completely eliminate granule cell neurogenesis.

2. Another point regarding genetic intervention is whether brain areas other than the cerebellum are affected. This is especially critical for understanding the basis of the behavioral phenotypes observed. Images showing ventricles and nuclear staining in key brain regions (cortex, hippocampus, striatum, etc) will help sort out this issue.

This is an excellent suggestion. We have generated a new supplemental Figure (Supp Figure 1-1 A-B) showing that the overt gross morphological changes only occur in the cerebellum.

3. The authors claim that PCs in mutants are not bursty while those in controls at P14 are. The representative traces also show this clearly. Nevertheless, all of the analysis done is on spike regularity and do not reflect burstiness. The pause percent could reflect burstiness, but it will also depend on baseline firing rates, which are lower for the mutant. The autocorrelogram shows spike regularity within a +/- 100 ms window while the burst intervals are much longer and therefore are likely not reflected in the peaks of the autocorrelogram. The authors should consider Fourier analysis and show a heatmap power spectrum reflecting the various frequency bands found in PCs of P14 animals.

Apologies for creating confusion in our descriptions of the “burstiness” in the parameters quantified in Figure 5. We refer to “bursty” cells as cells that have many high-frequency (short inter-spike interval (ISI)) spikes occurring in bursts that are interspersed by a few low frequency (long ISI) pauses. The vast majority of the spike trains thus have a short ISI, whereas a small proportion in our cells have a very long ISI. The reviewer is correct that these bursts are not reflected in the regularity within a +/- 1 s window (as is reflected in Figure 5L), because the bursts occur at a longer interval, have variable length, and only a small proportion of all the spikes have a very long ISI, despite their clear contribution to the burst. Therefore, we would not see a peak for the long ISI spikes in the auto correlogram even if we extended the time window beyond 1 s.

Our parameters define two types of irregularities: global and local. Global irregularity is caused by the occurrence of pauses and bursts (as in the Purkinje cells in P14 control mice). Local irregularity is caused by irregularity of inter spike intervals (as in the Purkinje cells of P14 mutant mice).

To examine the burstiness (or global irregularity in firing patterns) in our Purkinje cells we used a number of other parameters that are very effective for detecting global changes in neuronal firing patterns:

1. Frequency is calculated as the total number of spikes per second. Frequency mode (or preferred firing frequency) is calculated as the inverse of the most common ISI. We are able to detect the presence of “bursty” features because the presence of very long ISIs alters the relationship between Frequency and Frequency mode since the long ISIs reduce the average Frequency that is calculated over the entire duration of the recording. For example, the Purkinje cells in P13-14 control animals have an average Frequency of ~13.7 Hz, whereas the Frequency mode is ~26.2 Hz (nearly 2-fold difference). In contrast, in the P14 conditional knockout mice the Purkinje cell Frequency is ~4.35 Hz and the Frequency mode is ~5.25 Hz (about 1.2-fold difference). Therefore, Frequency-based relationships can reflect the pattern.

2. As the reviewer states, the pause proportion reflects burstiness. We defined a pause as an ISI that was five times as long as the most common ISI, to control for the differences in firing rate that the reviewer has mentioned. This calculation allowed us to measure the more reliable breaks in between spikes instead of long ISI that can happen in slowly firing neurons. If we would define the pause as an ISI of pre-set duration, it would miss pauses in fast-firing neurons or designate regularly occurring long ISI in slow firing neurons a pause. For example, in Purkinje cells in the P13-14 control animals have a pause proportion of ~0.35 whereas Purkinje cells in P14 conditional knockout mice have a pause proportion of ~0.14.

3. Finally, we included the CV. This parameter is calculated as the ratio of the standard deviation and mean of the ISI. As mentioned before, in the bursty cells, there are a few ISI that are much longer than the most common ISI (namely the pauses). Thus, the range, and therefore the standard deviation, of ISIs in bursty cells is very large. By dividing this large standard deviation by the mean ISI we normalize this variability to the firing rate to give a proportional variation. As a result, a more bursty cell will have a higher CV. For example, in Purkinje cells in the P13-14 control animals the CV is ~4.0 whereas in P14 conditional knockout mice, Purkinje cells in have a CV of ~1.3.

Even though we find that Purkinje cells in the P13-14 control animals have more bursty firing patterns, we observe that the simple spikes within the burst are very rhythmic. This is the reason we also included measures of local spike irregularity namely, the rhythmicity index (low in irregular cells) and CV2 (high in irregular cells). We now define in the text that these are measures for local irregularity in firing patterns.

We liked the idea that this reviewer has raised. The suggestion was to show a heatmap of the various frequencies at which the cells fire. Unfortunately, performing a Fourier analysis on single cell in vivo recordings would not provide us with an easily interpretable heatmap. Even though our single cell recordings are continuous, the signal information in our continuous recording is binary; spike or no spike. As a result, the Fourier analysis would detect the sinusoidal shape of single spikes, but not the frequency at which these spikes occur. Performing a Fourier analysis on the frequency averaged over small time-windows (for example 1 s as is presented in Figure 5H and I) would not provide a satisfactory power frequency plot because the pauses and bursts occur at random intervals. Instead, to visualize a heatmap of various frequency bands, as the reviewer suggested, we included heatmaps of spike frequency distributions of all 149 cells included in Figure 5 and 6 as Figure 5 – supplement 1. In this figure, some cells show a second, small peak, at a lower frequency, which indicates pause-events (and therefore bursts). However, the firing frequency is much higher within the burst than within the pause (per definition) and the duration of the pause is not the same for each pause. Therefore, the relative proportion of spikes that occur at a low frequency is so small that this second peak in not visible for all cells.

4. The authors' claims regarding Vglut1 and Vglut2 positive terminals onto PCs need more bolstering. Regarding Vglut1, the images shown in Figure 4 show diffuse weak staining of Vglut1 puncta. Can we rule out the existence of more widespread Vlglut1 synapses from escapee GCs and mossy fibers? In line 254, the authors state, "Taken together, we show that without excitatory cerebellar neurons, Purkinje cells do not acquire a large number of excitatory synapses during the second postnatal week." I do not believe that such a statement can be made based on these data on Vglut1 staining alone. To be able to say this, either mEPSC recordings or quantification of PF synapses using GFP positive lines must be done.

We agree with the reviewer. It is an excellent suggestion to bolster our statement that the number of excitatory synapses on Purkinje cells is reduced. Therefore, to address this comment, we quantified the spine density on Golgi-Cox stained images of Purkinje cells from control and mutant cerebellar. These quantifications are now included in Figure 5D-E.

Similarly, for the Vglut2 staining, the authors state in line 368, "These data show that the rRL-derived excitatory cerebellar neurons are necessary for shaping the regional localization and cellular targeting of climbing fiber afferents that project from the inferior olive to the Purkinje cells." However, since Vglut2 is also present in mossy fibers, and it is not clear if the increased staining arises from an increased number of CFs projecting to single PCs, this is a strong interpretation of the results. For both vglut1 and vglut2 staining data, without additional experiments, effects on numbers or placement of CF/PF synapses cannot be unequivocally concluded.

This is a good point. We reworded the cited sentence to include mossy fibers. The new sentence now reads:

“These data show that the En1;Atoh1 lineage, excitatory cerebellar neurons are necessary for shaping the regional localization and cellular targeting of immature mossy fibers and climbing fiber afferents that project to the Purkinje cells.”

Reviewer #3:

[…] 1. The manuscript should clearly mention the fact that many of the phenotypes found in the En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mutants have already been described in other agranular models. While those models are less specific (genetic mutants) or less controlled (x-irradiation in neonates), their study has nonetheless led to similar findings. For example, in the X-irradiation model, the consequences have been described for PC dendritic development (cf. for review Sotelo and Dusart 2009); for circuit and synaptic maturation (multi-innervation by climbing fibers for example in Woodward, Hoffer and Altman, 1974 and Crepel, Delhaye-Bouchaud and Dupont 1981), ataxic behavior and tremor (Woodward, Hoffer and Altman, 1974) and Purkinje cell activity (also Woodward, Hoffer and Altman 1974). The similarities and differences between the phenotypes described in these papers and those in the van der Heijden manuscript should be discussed.

At the beginning of our Results section, we have now included a paragraph describing the gross anatomical malformations in agranular animal models, as this is the most consistently reported phenotype described across all such models.

We have also included additional citations throughout the text to indicate the similarities and differences between the cellular-level phenotypes we observe in our mice and those reported in previous publications on models of cerebellar agranularity.

2. The mutant mice are called En1Cre/+/Atoh1fl/- and the analysis is done by comparing to "control" mice in all the manuscript except in figure 9 where 3 different controls are used in the behavioral analysis. How were the mutants generated? Is there a cross with an Atoh1-/-(not mentioned in the methods)? What genotype is "control" mice in the manuscript?

We did not do the null cross because Atoh1-/- mice are neonatal lethal. We have now clarified this in the methods as follows:

“Conditional knockout mice were generated by crossing En1Cre/+;Atoh1+/FlpO double heterozygote mice (which can be functionally defined as En1Cre/+;Atoh1+/- mice) with homozygote Atoh1fl/fl mice. En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice were considered experimental conditional knockout mice and all littermates with a different genotype were considered control mice.”

3. The authors describe that there is a change in PC spiking during the second postnatal week in controls and that mutant PCs do not acquire the characteristics of control PCs. The study of the developmental pattern of PC spontaneous firing in vivo adds to previously obtained results by the team (Arancillo et al; 2015) and others (Crepel, 1972). The analysis done by van der Heijden et al. is detailed and measures many parameters. Their data confirm the observation of a switch during the second postnatal week (Crepel 1972 should be cited, in particular line 260). However, in figure 5, no statistical test is performed to determine which data points are significantly different between the different control stages analyzed. The use of clustering and t-SNE analysis is helpful and defines two broad stages: until P10 and after. But there seem to be a large heterogeneity amongst Purkinje cells at each postnatal day, and it is not exactly clear what drives this heterogeneity.

This is an excellent suggestion. To attain the necessary power to make the statistical comparisons between all the age groups, we grouped together our data from P7-8, P9-10, P11-12, and the P13-14 mice. We next performed an ANOVA between all groups (4 control groups, 2 experimental groups) and did a post-hoc analysis to identify which groups have a statistically significant different from each other.

The use of clustering and t-SNE analysis is helpful and defines two broad stages: until P10 and after. But there seem to be a large heterogeneity amongst Purkinje cells at each postnatal day, and it is not exactly clear what drives this heterogeneity.

Heterogeneity in the maturation of anatomical properties have been reported previously (McKay and Turner, 2005; van Welie et al., 2011). Furthermore, it is known that adult Purkinje cells that are recorded in vivo in awake animals have a large degree of variability in their firing frequency and regularity (see for example our previous publications: Brown et al., 2020; Miterko et al., 2021; van der Heijden et al., 2021). Other groups have found that this heterogeneity can be in part attributed to the molecular identity (patterning) of Purkinje cells (Zebrin+ vs. Zebrin- cells) (Zhou et al., 2014; Wu et al., 2019). It was also recently reported that the patterned identity of Purkinje cells within specific zones arises according to the development of these patterns and ultimately contributes to the heterogeneity of Purkinje cell functional properties observed in adults (Beekhof et al., 2021). Taken together, the heterogeneity in Purkinje cell function is an expected observation. We address this point in the text as follows:

“This suggests the existence of a transformation that occurs between younger and older cells, but the transformation does not occur at the same time for all Purkinje cells, as is expected based on previous studies showing that the heterogeneity of Purkinje cell function is an inherent property of Purkinje cells that respects and spans the timelines of anatomical and functional development of Purkinje cells (Beekhof et al. 2021; van Welie et al. 2011; McKay and Turner 2005).”

Additionally, we account for the expected inter- and intra-animal heterogeneity of Purkinje cell development by recording from a large number of single neurons (10-20) and from multiple mice (3-7) on each of the 8 developmental days that fall within our analysis time-window (P7 to P14).

4. For the mutant PCs, the authors state that "this developmental switch does not occur by P14 in En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/-mice". I do not think this can be stated since there is no data on the mutant PCs during the first postnatal week. It could be that the PCs in the agranular cerebellum never behave like control ones. The authors should record from PCs in the mutant at P7 if they want to state that the phenotype is due to the non-occurrence of a developmental switch.

This is another excellent suggestion. We decided to perform additional recordings in P10 mutant mice. We choose this time point for several reasons:

1: Our mutant mice lack several excitatory pontine respiratory nuclei (Van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018), which makes them more sensitive to the respiratory repression that results from anesthetics such as ketamine, which we use in our studies for anesthetizing the mice during our recordings. In our experience, P10 mutant mice were more resilient to the anesthesia than P7 mutant mice. Using the P10 timepoint allowed us to record mice using the same dose as all other mice in the study, which ultimately helped us avoid having to change the anesthesia concentration, which would have been a confounding factor in our analysis.

2: The initial ANOVA results as described in the first part of this reviewer comment showed no statistically significant difference between P7 and P10 control mice.

3: Our clustering analysis showed that P14 experimental mice were most similar to P10 control mice (Figure 6D).

When we performed these experiments, we found that Purkinje cells in P10 mutant mice were not different from Purkinje cells in P14 mutant mice in any of the parameters included in our study, confirming our hypothesis that the developmental transformation that typically occurs at this stage does not occur in our mutant mice.

5. There is sometimes confusion between intrinsic spiking properties and spontaneous spiking in Purkinje cells. For example, line 82 in Sathyanesan et al. 2018, the authors study spontaneous and evoked firing properties of PCs, not intrinsic properties. In the discussion, it is not very clear to me what the authors mean by "In addition to the increased complexity of intrinsic cellular properties (McKay and Turner, 2005), we postulated that intercellular interactions during development may support the maturation of Purkinje cell firing"(line 532/533). Could actually granule cells change the intrinsic spiking properties of PCs ? Slice recordings would be helpful to study this.

The reviewer is correct. We did not mean to draw too many conclusions with reference to intrinsic properties as our in vivo recordings did not measure such firing features. To avoid this confusion, we changed the wording from intrinsic to spontaneous. We have also toned down the extent of comparisons that we make to the previous slice physiology data from the Turner lab. We have now made the intent of our comparison clearer:

“In addition to the increased complexity of intrinsic cellular properties that have been defined using slice electrophysiology approaches (McKay and Turner 2005), we postulated using our in vivo recordings that intercellular interactions between granule cells and Purkinje cells during development may support the maturation of Purkinje cell firing properties, both by providing direct inputs and by supporting anatomical maturation, including the outgrowth of Purkinje cell dendrites.”

6. Line 358: There is a switch between Vglut2 and vglut1 at Parallel fiber PC synapses during development (cf. Miyazaki EJN 2003). The decrease in VGLUT2 puncta density observed in the molecular layer is most likely due to this switch rather that CF synaptic pruning. It is thus difficult to interpret the result in panel 7C.

This is a good point. We included this citation in the text. We clarified the interpretation of figure panel 7C as followed:

“Immature VGluT2+/climbing fiber synapses are located around the Purkinje cell body in controls, but at around P9 only the “winner” among these synapses translocates to the Purkinje cell dendrites in the molecular layer (Kano et al. 2018). […] These data show that the En1;Atoh1 lineage, excitatory cerebellar neurons are necessary for shaping the regional localization and cellular targeting of immature mossy fibers and climbing fiber afferents that project to the Purkinje cells.”

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

In this revised version, the authors have addressed most of the points raised by the reviewers. However, a few points remain and these are detailed below.

Reviewer #3:

The authors have answered many of my concerns and the manuscript is significantly improved. There remain however several points that need to be addressed before publication.

1. If I understand well the explanation about the control mice, the controls are either Atoh1fl/+, En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+ or Atoh1fl/FlpO and the data are grouped together except in the behavioral analysis. This should be stated clearly not only in the methods section, but at the beginning of the Results section (line 138 and line 146), as it is important for the interpretation of the results (some of the variability in the "control" data could be due to the difference in genotype). As it is, the description is misleading.

Thank you for pointing out this confusion, we addressed this concern as followed in the start of the result section:

“We generated conditional knockout mice by crossing mice that are heterozygous for the En1Cre knock-in allele (Wurst et al., 1994) and heterozygous for Atoh1 (van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018) with mice that are homozygous for a LoxP-flanked Atoh1 allele (Atoh1fl/fl) (Shroyer et al., 2007). From this cross we obtain mice with four genotypes: Atoh1fl/+ mice, Atoh1FlpO/fl mice, and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+ mice that we hereafter will refer to as control mice, and our experimental En1Cre/+;Atoh1FlpO/+ mice that we hereafter will refer to as En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/- mice because the Atoh1FlpO is a functional null-allele (van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018).”

How was the Atoh1FlpO line characterized (I can only find genotyping data in a previous paper)?

The Atoh1FlpO line is characterized and validated by (1) PCR validation of correct insertion in the endogenous Atoh1-locus and loss of the coding region for the Atoh1 gene; (2) expression of the Atoh1FlpO-allele in the brainstem in this and our previous paper (Van der Heijden and Zoghbi, 2018); (3) extensive outcrossing of the founder mice to avoid potential other genomic rearrangements confounding our expression; (4) validation that the Atoh1FlpO/+ mice have no unexpected behavioral phenotypes included those discussed in Figure 9 of the current paper and extensive breathing assays in our previous paper.

Is there any data showing that the three control genotypes are not different from each other and from wild-type animals?

We would expect that if there were any biologically meaningful differences between either of the control groups it would first show up at the mouse behavioral level as the Atoh1FlpO/+ mice are heterozygote for Atoh1 in all cells, and En1Cre/+;Atoh1fl/+ are heterozygote for En1 in all cells and Atoh1 only in the En1-lineage. Nevertheless, we find no significant differences in any of the behavioral assays described in Figure 9 (see also the raw P-values in extended data for Figure 9).

2. Result section page 10 lines 342 to 345: Crepel had described in 1972 (Experimental Brain Research) in vivo recordings of Purkinje cell activity in rat neonates and showed increased frequency with maturation (see figure 1 of that paper). The Crepel paper should be cited in this part of the result section, as well as in the discussion page 19. Line 343, the Beekhof et al. article describes not only in vitro but also in vivo recordings of Purkinje cell activity. The sentence as it is in line 343 is misleading.

Thank you for pointing out that we should include the Crepel 1972 in our references. As for the Beekhof et al., 2021 citation, all in vivo recordings were performed in mice with earliest timepoint P12 through P17, but not earlier. To clarify, and avoid any potential confusion for the reading, we included the timepoints relevant to our paper in this sentence. The sentence now reads:

“in vitro recordings in rats (Crepel, 1972; McKay and Turner, 2005) and mice (Beekhof et al., 2021) show that Purkinje cell firing properties change significantly during early postnatal development (P7-P14), but it is unclear how firing patterns evolve in vivo during this dynamic period of rewiring.”

3. The last paragraph of the discussion should be simplified. Granule cells influence the maturation of Purkinje cell spiking (line 728-729), but also many other parameters of Purkinje cell development and maturation, as described in this manuscript but also previous data on agranular models. As the relationship between the different deficits is difficult to disentangle, this part of the discussion should be toned down (line 728 for example "direct influence").

We reworded this sentence as followed:

“Based on our data, we argue that the initial communication between Purkinje cells and granule cells sets the efficiency of Purkinje cell function and the establishment of Purkinje cell spike properties through structural as well as synaptic signals.”

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—figure supplement 4—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 1—figure supplement 4.
    Figure 3—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 3.
    Figure 4—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 4.
    Figure 5—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figures 5 and 6.
    Figure 7—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 7.
    Figure 8—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 8.
    Figure 9—source data 1. Source data and specific p-values for representative graphs in Figure 9.
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for Figures 1, 3–9.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES