Introduction
The COVID-19 emergency brought out the role of online digital technologies. The increase in online social interactivity was accelerated by social distancing, which has been recognized to have adverse effects due to physical and emotional isolation (Canet-Juric et al., 2020).
Body language is central to social interactions, and its role is clearly diminished when going online, but the relevance of this change is still not clear. This transition toward online could affect the wellness of the people, especially the population with specific fragilities, e.g., young people and seniors (Beam and Kim, 2020; Canet-Juric et al., 2020; Fernández Cruz et al., 2020).
We here briefly present our viewpoint on some issues concerning changes in body interactions in online interpersonal communication. Our aim is to encourage constructive discussion and raise awareness about these very topical issues.
Online Communication Platforms: Toward New Communication Strategies?
Progress in digital technologies is having a profound impact on interpersonal communication. The natural face-to-face modality is nowadays often replaced by interactions through video-mediated online communication platforms (VMOCPs) (mediatized communication). Furthermore, the duration of restrictions related to the COVID-19 emergency determined, in a very short time, an acceleration in the diffusion and an intensification in the use of VMOCPs.
In fact, VMOCPs are now ubiquitously used for meetings and courses in different contexts, such as work environments, education, and, in general, for whatever activity involving social interaction, thus, determining rapid changes in the everyday lives of the people (Chan et al., 2020; Dorn, 2021).
Interestingly, also the seniors, which were only marginal users of these technologies, were forced to use them as their only chance for social contacts (López et al., 2020; Pelicioni and Lord, 2020).
The new form of communication has brought great improvement in communication possibilities, by overcoming the limitations of time and space. However, VMOCPs have also modified the communication rules, e.g., those related to proxemics (Hall, 1966). Proxemics assumes a direct proportionality between the geometrical peripersonal/extrapersonal space and different types of interpersonal acquaintanceship: intimate, personal, social, and public. When people communicate through VMOCPs, the geometrical distance separating the screen image and the real interlocutor is a few tens of centimeters, which is smaller than the distance between persons involved in a social/public face-to-face conversation. Then, when passing online, does a short “perceived” distance imply a personal/intimate level? In our opinion, the answer to this question is not obvious as going online likely brings non-trivial changes in the proxemics rules.
For example, such a physical closeness would presuppose an intimacy between persons and a mutual disposition to the potential use of the tactile channel (handshake, hug, and tap on the shoulder). However, despite this virtual closeness, we cannot touch (Drag, 2020).
Another important change concerns the communicative role of eye gaze. In fact, in agreement with the “fractured ecologies” concept (Heath and Luff, 1992; Luff et al., 2003), the eye contact and the meaning it conveys (Drag, 2020), when mediated by the camera, is unrecoverable by the interacting participants.
In summary, the above discussed issues suggest that changes from live to online communication are complex, as the direct proportionality between (apparent) geometrical distance and interpersonal acquaintanceship is deeply changed both quantitatively and qualitatively.
Body Motion and Video-Mediated Online Communication Platforms: A Focus on Online Education and Therapeutic Intervention
Body movements and language are crucial in both emotion-based non-verbal communication and cognitive-based social interactions. It is foreseeable that extensive use of online technologies could have important effects on cognitive processes, not only those involving learning/educational/training activities but also those related to emotion-driven relationships in social living. To date, however, the role of body movements in inter-subject online interaction has been scarcely investigated (Zuo et al., 2021).
Let us discuss two different examples: online education and dance movement therapy (DMT).
Serious games are interactive virtual simulations whose goal is to train while entertaining. They were proposed as useful tools to improve learning performances (Hanus and Fox, 2015) and, with the COVID-19 emergency, have been thought as a possible mainstream solution to mitigate the problem of social distancing in online teaching (Koivisto and Hamari, 2019).
This approach, although certainly useful, needs to be reviewed considering the concept of “embodied education,” which refers to the embodied cognition (EC) theory (Kiverstein and Miller, 2015; Shapiro, 2019). According to it, cognition is formed by the processing of informative stimuli from and through the body. Some authors found a positive correlation between learning performances and movement synchronization in the teacher–learner interaction (Sacheli et al., 2018; Shapiro and Stolz, 2019; Madsen et al., 2020; Pan et al., 2021). Furthermore, mobility strategies of the teachers in the classroom (classroom proxemics) at various stages of a lesson and according to the task of the students have been found to have positive effects on engagement (Chin et al., 2017), motivation (Fernandes et al., 2011), and disruptive behavior of the students (Gunter et al., 1995).
DMT is a complementary therapy where body movements are employed to promote personal and social wellbeing. This is reached by eliciting the harmonization of mental, somatic, and relational manifestations of the individual through the creative use of movement improvization and dance (ADTA—American Dance Therapy Association, 2014). DMT emphasizes the communicational aspect of dance (Karkou and Sanderson, 2006). In fact, the DMT setting presupposes a social component triggered by interpersonal interactions mediated by body language and, in particular, movements. Indeed, the social component through body interaction has a crucial role in DMT functioning: the game of distances, perspectives, and reciprocity creates the communicative context in which movement takes place. The circle, for example, is a DMT basic figure (Karampoula and Panhofer, 2018) in which all group members can see everyone else, thus, having direct access to verbal and nonverbal cues of the participants. The basic elements of a circle include mirroring, echoing of emotional states, containment and holding, and physical contact through the holding of hands. Mirroring consists of matching/echoing the movements of the person (Tortora, 2006) and, in a circle, is multiple (multimirror). This technique has been shown to be effective in strengthening the self-confidence and physical resilience of the group members but also social integration and affiliation by promoting empathy. The “motor theory of empathy,” in fact, proposes that the human mirror system may participate to the understanding of the intentions and feelings of others (Rizzolatti and Fabbri-Destro, 2008) and that empathy may stem from the link between perception and action (Iacoboni, 2009; Zardi et al., 2021). In brief, understanding of action may promote empathizing with others (Carr et al., 2003). Despite this theory being criticized (Hickok, 2014), phenomena based on motor resonance (i.e., a direct link between the perception of an action and its execution), namely, mimicry, synchrony, and automatic imitation, are considered involved in higher social cognition, including empathy, and in promoting positive social effects both in the adult and during development (Rauchbauer and Grosbras, 2020).
What Is Missing Online?
In DMT, the dyadic relationship between the conductor/participant is at least partially preserved (APID—Associazione professionale italiana danzamovimentoterapia, 2021). The conductor can stimulate the participant with the voice, music, and gestures also through the screen. The participant, by observing the conductor on the screen, can be accompanied in the experimentation of his own movement aimed at creating an internal/external dialog. Indeed, evidence from trials of online meditation (Cavalera et al., 2019; Yang et al., 2019) supports the idea that online, it is possible to work on the “vertical dimension of energy” that is on the individual depth of feelings. In contrast, the transversal level, which includes all the non-verbal interactions between participants, is greatly impaired. Even with groups of three to four persons, most of the interpersonal and transpersonal components of movement cannot be reproduced.
The example of DMT shows that online sensory interaction is very different from a live one. The visual and auditory sensory channels are essentially the only online communication modalities. Actually, images on the screen are two-dimensional (2D), thus, reducing tridimensional visual perception to a quasi 2D one1. Other modalities such as the tactile and olfactory ones cannot be directly employed. Human touch has been suggested to play a large role in establishing a sense of “proximity” between persons and to facilitate affiliative behavior and social bonding (Morrison et al., 2010). Previous studies have demonstrated a close association between pleasant social touch and the release of oxytocin, which is a crucial modulator of social behavior and emotions across species (Olff et al., 2013; Walker et al., 2017; Kendrick et al., 2018; Tang et al., 2020).
The sense of smell is also involved in the non-verbal social communication of humans; in fact, through smell, humans can involuntarily convey personal information (de Groot et al., 2017; Parma et al., 2017; Pause, 2017; Roberts et al., 2020).
Some authors claim that chemosignal communication critically contributes to the formation and maintenance of social groups and has a role in the evolution of the social brain (Dunbar and Shultz, 2007; Parma et al., 2017).
A new multidisciplinary discipline involving psychology and chemistry (sociochemistry) is studying the chemical basis of olfactory communication emphasizing the role of psychological states and traits in modulating body odorant composition (de Groot et al., 2020). Interestingly, data from patients with no or reduced olfactory capabilities show that the loss of smell severely affects the richness of social relationships over the lifespan from the early developmental stages to the old age (Boesveldt et al., 2017).
Final Discussion
We can argue that in online social interactions:
(i) smell and touch are absent;
(ii) visual is limited to a quasi 2D perception;
(iii) auditory is almost unchanged;
-
(iv) changes in the relationship between perceived geometrical distances and acquaintanceship
are expected, but still unclear;
(v) there are no direct bodily interactions.
According to the “fractured ecologies” concept, when online, interacting persons cannot recover most of the relevant features of the environment and bodily behavior of others (gesture, eye contact) and behave accordingly (Luff et al., 2003).
These changes may undermine the emotional and empathetic aspects of interpersonal communication. However, cooperation is still possible through the auditory and the reduced quasi 2D visual perception.
A better understanding of these aspects could need a partial revision of classical communication theories (McLuhan, 1964; Hall, 1966; Bolter and Grusin, 1999; Jensen, 1999) in order to consider the new modalities of communication introduced by online interactions. Thus, the consequences of passing online probably include remediation mechanisms related to the use of the new digital technologies (Bolter and Grusin, 1999). These mechanisms are compatible with the self-organization paradigm of complexity: cooperative social dynamics between different individuals/groups trigger the emergence of a new dynamical equilibrium in a relatively short time due to an environmental change (in this case, the new digital technologies) (Zeleny, 1977; Santos et al., 2006; Paradisi et al., 2015; Paradisi and Allegrini, 2017; Mahmoodi et al., 2018). The equilibrium is constrained to the optimization of social interactivity mediated by the new technology, the optimum possibly being a condition as nearest as possible to live interactivity. Thus, the question that should be answered should not only be “what is lost?” but also “what is new?,” possibly involving the development of virtual proxemics.
An open question that deserves further investigations is the quantification of perceived virtual distances in online interactions. For example, the distance between the screen and the individual could be used, but the effect of the image size on the screen should also be clarified.
Figure 1 reports a graphical summary of our discussion. Panel (A) is a scheme of self-organizing live interaction between two individuals. Panel (B) sketches the passage from live to online interactions.
To address the challenges of online communications and to mitigate the effect of emotional isolation, an interdisciplinary research is needed that would have to (i) monitor the social change, (ii) develop new communication models, and (iii) develop strategies and technologies to partially fill the online-live interactivity gap.
Author Contributions
PP and LS wrote the paper and carried out the bibliographic search with many insights from MR regarding DMT and body language in social communication. All authors equally discussed and developed the idea of the paper.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
1Here “quasi” is related to a partial mental reconstruction of vision depth that the brain can operate on the basis of past live experiences, by elaborating what is actually a two-dimensional image on the screen.
References
- ADTA—American Dance Therapy Association (2014). What is Dance/Movement Therapy. Available online at: https://adta.memberclicks.net/what-is-dancemovement-therapy (accessed April 2021).
- APID—Associazione professionale italiana danzamovimentoterapia (2021). Available online at: https://www.apid.it/ (accessed April, 2021).
- Beam C. R., Kim A. J. (2020). Psychological sequelae of social isolation and loneliness might be a larger problem in young adults than older adults. Psychol. Trauma 12, S58–S60. 10.1037/tra0000774 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boesveldt S., Yee J. R., McClintock M. K., Lundström J. N. (2017). Olfactory function and the social lives of older adults: a matter of sex. Sci. Rep. 7:45118. 10.1038/srep45118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolter J. D., Grusin R. (1999). Remediation: Understanding New Media. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Canet-Juric L., Andrés M. L., del Valle M., López-Morales H., Po,ó F., Galli J. I., et al. (2020). A longitudinal study on the emotional impact cause by the COVID-19 pandemic quarantine on general population. Front. Psychol. 11:565688. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565688 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carr L., Iacoboni M., Dubeau M. C., Mazziotta J. C., Lenzi G. L. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 100, 5497–5502. 10.1073/pnas.0935845100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavalera C., Rovaris M., Mendozzi L., Pugnetti L., Garegnani M., Castelnuovo G., et al. (2019). Online meditation training for people with multiple sclerosis: a randomized controlled trial. Mult. Scler. J. 25, 610–617. 10.1177/1352458518761187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan A. K. M., Nickson C. P., Rudolph J. W., Lee A., Joynt G. M. (2020). Social media for rapid knowledge dissemination: early experience from the COVID-19 pandemic. Anaesthesia 75, 1579–1582. 10.1111/anae.15057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chin H. B., Mei C. C. Y., Taib F. (2017). Instructional proxemics and its impact on classroom teaching and learning. Int. J. Mod. Lang. Appl. Ling. 1, 1–20. 10.24191/ijmal.v1i1.7637 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- de Groot J. H. B., Croijmans I., Smeets M. A. M. (2020). More data, please: machine learning to advance the multidisciplinary science of human sociochemistry. Front Psychol. 11:581701. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.581701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Groot J. H. B., Semin G. R., Smeets M. A. M. (2017). On the communicative function of body odors: a theoretical integration and review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 306–324. 10.1177/1745691616676599 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dorn S. D. (2021). Backslide or forward progress? Virtual care at U.S. healthcare systems beyond the COVID-19 pandemic. NPJ Dig. Med. 4, 1–3. 10.1038/s41746-020-00379-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drag K. (2020). Revaluation of the proxemics code in mediatized communication. Soc. Commun. 1, 93–105. 10.2478/sc-2020-0010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dunbar R. I. M., Shultz S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science 317, 1344–1347. 10.1126/science.1145463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandes A. C., Huang J., Rinaldo V. (2011). Does where a student sits really matter? The impact of seating locations on student classroom learning. Int. J. Appl. Educ. Stud. 10, 66–77. [Google Scholar]
- Fernández Cruz M., Álvarez Rodríguez J., Ávalos Ruiz I., Cuevas López M., de Barros Camargo C., Díaz Rosas F., et al. (2020). Evaluation of the emotional and cognitive regulation of young people in a lockdown situation due to the Covid-19 pandemic. Front. Psychol. 11:565503. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565503 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunter P. L., Shores R. E., Jack S. L., Rasmussen S. K., Flowers J. (1995). On the move using teacher/student proximity to improve students' behavior. Teach. Except. Child. 28, 12–14. 10.1177/004005999502800103 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hall E. T. (1966). The Hidden Dimension (1st ed.). New York, NY: Doubleday and Co. [Google Scholar]
- Hanus M. D., Fox J. (2015). Assessing the effects of gamification in the classroom: a longitudinal study on intrinsic motivation, social comparison, satisfaction, effort, and academic performance. Comput. Educ. 80, 152–161. 10.1016/j.compedu.2014.08.019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Heath C., Luff P. (1992). Media space and communicative asymmetries: preliminary observations of video-mediated interaction. Hum. Comput. Interact. 7, 315–346. 10.1207/s15327051hci0703_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hickok G. (2014). The Myth of Mirror Neurons: The Real Neuroscience of Communication and Cognition. New York, NY: W. W. Norton and Company. [Google Scholar]
- Iacoboni M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 653–670. 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163604 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen J. F. (1999). Interactivity, in Tracking a New Concept in Media and Communication Studies, Computer Media and Communication, ed. Mayer P. (Oxford: Oxford University Press; ). [Google Scholar]
- Karampoula E., Panhofer H. (2018). The circle in dance movement therapy: a literature review. Arts Psychother. 58, 27–32. 10.1016/j.aip.2018.02.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Karkou V., Sanderson P. (2006). Arts Therapies: A Research-Based Map of the Field. Edinburgh: Elsevier [Google Scholar]
- Kendrick K. M., Guastella A. J., Becker B. (2018). Overview of human oxytocin research. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 35, 321–348. 10.1007/7854_2017_19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiverstein J., Miller M. (2015). The embodied brain: towards a radical embodied cognitive neuroscience. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:237. 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00237 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koivisto J., Hamari J. (2019). The rise of motivational information systems: a review of gamification research. Int. J. Inf. Manag. 45, 191–210. 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018.10.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- López J., Perez-Rojo G., Noriega C., Carretero I., Velasco C., Martinez-Huertas, et al. (2020). Psychological well-being among older adults during the COVID-19 outbreak: a comparative study of the young-old and the old-old adults. Int. Psychogeriatr. 32, 1365–1370. 10.1017/S1041610220000964 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luff P., Heath C., Kuzuoka H., Hindmarsh J., Yamazaki K. (2003). Fractured ecologies: creating environments for collaboration. Hum. Comput. Interact. 18, 51–84. 10.1207/S15327051HCI1812_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Madsen K. L., Aggerholm K., Jensen J.-O. (2020). Enactive movement integration: results from an action research project. Teach. Teach. Educ. 95, 103139. 10.1016/j.tate.2020.103139 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mahmoodi K., West B. J., Grigolini P. (2018). Self-organized temporal criticality: bottom-up resilience versus top-down vulnerability. Complexity 2018:8139058. 10.1155/2018/8139058 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McLuhan M. (1964). Understanding Media. The Extensions of Man. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Morrison I., Löken L., Olausson H. (2010). The skin as a social organ. Exp. Brain Res. 204:305–314 10.1007/s00221-009-2007-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olff M., Frijling J. L., Kubzansky L. D., Bradley B., Ellenbogen M. A., Cardoso C., et al. (2013). The role of oxytocin in social bonding, stress regulation and mental health: an update on the moderating effects of context and interindividual differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1883–1894. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.06.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pan Y., Novembre G., Song B., Zhu Y., Hu Y. (2021). Dual brain stimulation enhances interpersonal learning through spontaneous movement synchrony. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 210–221. 10.1093/scan/nsaa080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paradisi P., Allegrini P. (2017). Intermittency-driven complexity in signal processing, in Complexity and Nonlinearity in Cardiovascular Signals, ed Barbieri R. (Cham: Springer; ), 161–195. 10.1007/978-3-319-58709-7_6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Paradisi P., Kaniadakis G., Scarfone A. M. (2015), The emergence of self-organization in complex systems. Preface Chaos Solitons Fractals 81, 407–411. 10.1016/j.chaos.2015.09.017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Parma V., Gordon A. R., Cecchetto C., Cavazzana A., Lundström J. N. (2017). Processing of human body odors, in Springer Handbook of Odor, ed. Buettner A. (New York, NY: Springer; ), 963–986. 10.1007/978-3-319-26932-0_51 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pause B. M. (2017). Human chemosensory communication, in Springer Handbook of Odor, ed. Buettner A. (New York, NY: Springer; ), 987–1010. 10.1007/978-3-319-26932-0_52 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pelicioni P. H. S., Lord S. R. (2020). COVID-19 will severely impact older people's lives, and in many more ways than you think! Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 24, 293–294. 10.1016/j.bjpt.2020.04.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauchbauer B., Grosbras M.-H. (2020). Developmental trajectory of interpersonal motor alignment: positive social effects and link to social cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 118, 411–425. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G., Fabbri-Destro M. (2008). The mirror system and its role in social cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 179–184. 10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts S. C., Havlíček J., Schaal B. (2020). Human olfactory communication: current challenges and future prospects. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 375:20190258. 10.1098/rstb.2019.0258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sacheli L. M., Arcangeli E., Paulesu E. (2018). Evidence for a dyadic motor plan in joint action. Sci. Rep. 8:027. 10.1038/s41598-018-23275-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santos F. C., Pacheco J. M., Lenaerts T. (2006). Cooperation prevails when individuals adjust their social ties. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2:e140. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro L. (2019). Embodied Cognition. New York, NY: Routledge (Taylor and Francis). ISBN: 978-1-315-18038-0 [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro L., Stolz S. A. (2019). Embodied cognition and its significance for education. Theory Res. Educ. 17, 19–39. 10.1177/1477878518822149 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tang Y., Benusiglio D., Lefevre A., Hilfiger L., Althammer F., Bludau A., et al. (2020). Social touch promotes interfemale communication via activation of parvocellular oxytocin neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 1125–1137. 10.1038/s41593-020-0674-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tortora S. (2006). The Dancing Dialogue: Using the Communicative Power of Movement with Young Children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Pub. [Google Scholar]
- Walker S. C., Trotter P. D., Swaney W. T., Marshall A., Mcglone F. P. (2017). C-tactile afferents: cutaneous mediators of oxytocin release during affiliative tactile interactions? Neuropeptides 64, 27–38. 10.1016/j.npep.2017.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang M., Jia G., Sun S., Ye C., Zhang R., Yu X. (2019). Effects of an online mindfulness intervention focusing on attention monitoring and acceptance in pregnant women: a randomized controlled trial. J. Midwifery Womens Health 64, 68–77. 10.1111/jmwh.12944 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zardi A., Carlotti E. G., Pontremoli A., Morese R. (2021). Dancing in your head: an interdisciplinary review. Front. Psychol. 12:649121. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.649121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeleny M. (1977). Self-organization of living systems: a formal model of autopoiesis. Int. J. General Syst. 4, 13–28. 10.1080/03081077708960651 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zuo Y., Ma Y., Zhang M., Wu X., Ren Z. (2021). The impact of sharing physical activity experience on social network sites on residents' social connectedness: a cross-sectional survey during COVID-19 social quarantine. Glob. Health 17:10. 10.1186/s12992-021-00661-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]