Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Brain Behav Immun. 2021 Jul 25;97:186–192. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2021.07.011

Immune Markers Are Associated with Cognitive Performance in a Multiethnic Cohort: the Northern Manhattan Study

Mitchell S V Elkind 1,2, Michelle Moon 1, Tatjana Rundek 3, Clinton B Wright 4, Ken Cheung 5, Ralph L Sacco 6, Mady Hornig 2
PMCID: PMC8453105  NIHMSID: NIHMS1730867  PMID: 34320382

Abstract

OBJECTIVE:

To determine whether immune protein panels add significant information to correlates of cognition.

BACKGROUND:

Immune mechanisms in vascular cognitive aging are incompletely characterized.

DESIGN/METHODS:

A subsample of the prospective Northern Manhattan Study underwent detailed neuropsychological testing. Cognitive scores were converted into Z-scores and categorized into four domains (memory, language, processing speed, and executive function) based on factor analysis. Blood samples were analyzed using a 60-plex immunoassay. We used least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO) procedures to select markers and their interactions independently associated with cognitive scores. Linear regression models assessed cross-sectional associations of known correlates of cognition with cognitive scores, and assessed model fit before and after addition of LASSO-selected immune markers.

RESULTS:

Among 1179 participants (mean age 70±8.9 years, 60% women, 68% Hispanic), inclusion of LASSO-selected immune markers improved model fit above age, education, and other risk factors (p for likelihood ratio test<0.005 for all domains). C-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 11 (CCL 11, eotaxin), C-X-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 9 (CXCL9), hepatocyte growth factor (HGF), and serpin E1 (plasminogen activator inhibitor-1) were associated with each of the domains and with overall cognitive function. Immune marker effects were comparable to conventional risk factors: for executive function, each standard deviation (SD) increase in CCL11 was associated with an effect equivalent to aging three years; for memory, HGF had twice the effect of aging.

CONCLUSIONS:

Immune markers associate with cognitive function in a multi-ethnic cohort. Further work is needed to validate these findings and determine optimal treatment targets.

Keywords: Cognition, Cohort Studies, Epidemiology, Immune Function, Innate immunity, Vascular dementia

Introduction

Communication between the brain and immune system is increasingly recognized as critical for brain health and neuroprotection after cerebrovascular, metabolic and microbial challenges.1,2,3,4 Questions remain, however, regarding the role of inflammation in vascular and neurodegenerative mechanisms of cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia; the specific inflammatory pathways that are most important;5,6 and the role of inflammation in populations, particularly Hispanics, with a high burden of vascular risk factors but that have been underrepresented in research.7,8,9 Innate immunity, activation of complement and microglia, and humoral and cellular immunity may all contribute.10,11,12,13

There is evidence, for example, that breakdown of the blood brain barrier at the time of small vessel stroke permits peripheral blood proteins, such as fibrinogen, to enter the brain and facilitate inflammation through activation of the innate immune receptor molecule Toll-Like Receptor 4 and cellular immune pathways.14,15 At the same time, brain proteins may be released into the periphery, provoking humoral immune responses against brain antigens. Levels of antibodies against myelin basic protein and other neural-derived molecules, and a Th1 pro-inflammatory cellular response, are associated with subsequent cognitive decline in animal and human studies.16,17 Much of this work, however, is based on preclinical models or small sample sizes in humans.

We hypothesized that a multiplex immunoassay measuring 60 immune molecules would provide insight into pathways involved in cognitive decline. We explored this hypothesis in a population-based cohort study, the Northern Manhattan Study (NOMAS), a sample of Hispanic and non-Hispanic participants from New York City.

METHODS

Study Population

NOMAS is a racially/ethnically diverse prospective cohort study consisting of 3,298 stroke-free participants enrolled between 1993 and 2001. The study was initially designed to evaluate effects of medical, socio-economic, and vascular risk factors on incidence of stroke and other vascular outcomes. Eligible participants were individuals residing in northern Manhattan for ≥ 3 months in a household with a telephone, with no prior history of stroke, and age ≥40 years old at the time of enrollment. Subjects were identified by random-digit dialing, and in-person interview and assessment were conducted by trained bilingual research assistants. A subcohort of 1290 participants included 1091 subjects remaining clinically stroke-free from the original cohort as well as a sample of unrelated household members (n=199). Participants with cognitive impairment severe enough to preclude giving of consent, or who were unwilling or unable to undergo magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), were excluded. Detailed neuropsychological assessments were conducted, and blood samples were collected; methods of recruitment and characteristics of this sample have been described previously.18 The study was approved by the Columbia University and University of Miami Institutional Review Boards, and all participants provided informed consent.

Data collection

Demographics and risk factors were collected through in-person interviews, as described previously.18 Race-ethnicity was determined by self-identification. Insurance status was defined as no insurance/Medicaid versus private insurance/Medicare. Educational achievement was self-reported as number of years in school and degree achieved. Physical activity was evaluated by in-person questionnaire and defined dichotomously. Smoking was categorized as current (within the past year), former, or never smoker of cigarettes, cigars or pipes. Standardized questions adapted from the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System assessed hypertension and diabetes. Hypertension was defined either as participant self-report of hypertension, blood pressure measurement of 140/90 mmHg or greater, or use of anti-hypertensive medication. Diabetes was defined either as participant self-report of diabetes, fasting glucose of 126 mg/dL or greater, or use of insulin or oral anti-diabetic medications.

Cognitive assessment

Neuropsychological assessments were administered by trained bilingual research staff in English or Spanish, as previously described.19 Specific neuropsychological tests representing each domain are listed in Table e-1.20 Because of lack of population norms for older cohorts, we used baseline neuropsychological scores as our cohort norms.21 Normative values for each neuropsychological test were obtained by calculating means and standard deviations. The cohort-specific norms were used to normalize individual neuropsychological test scores by multiplying by the test specific mean and dividing by the test-specific standard deviation. For timed tests (Color Trail 1, Color Trail 2 and Grooved Pegboard), because some participants did not complete tasks within the maximum time allowed, we calculated their new “imputed” time by adding penalty time to the maximum time allowed. The penalty time was obtained by multiplying the number of incomplete items by “expected time per correct items completed” estimated using Kaplan-Meier methods.

To enhance interpretability of cognitive performance, we developed cognitive domain-specific scores using individual neuropsychological test scores. The standardized scores for individual tests were then categorized into four key cognitive domains (memory, language, processing speed and executive function) based on an exploratory factor analysis and prior findings, as previously described.21 In brief, we used confirmatory factor analysis to choose a theoretical model that fit the data best. Model fit was evaluated with several fit statistics, including chi-square (X2), critical ratio (X2/df), and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation, for which values closer to zero indicate a better fit. Models were selected a priori based on evidence from the literature as to what each variable is hypothesized to measure. Modeling led to a four-factor model comprising executive function, memory, language function, and psychomotor speed domains. Thereafter, cognitive domain-specific scores were calculated by taking the mean of the individual standardized test scores selected for each domain. Cognitive outcomes were domain-specific z scores (memory, language, processing speed and executive function) at baseline and a composite cognitive function score obtained by averaging those four domain scores. Deficits in memory and language are often associated with primary neurodegenerative dementias, while deficits in executive function and processing speed are often considered typical of vascular or subcortical dementias.

Multiplex immunoassay assessment

The plasma samples for immune marker measurements were collected on the same day as neuropsychological testing for 87% of the cohort; an additional 10% of the cohort had blood collected within 1-7 days of neuropsychological testing. The distribution of the time difference between blood collection and neuropsychological testing has a mean of 0.002 (± 0.067) days.

We completed assays of 60 immune molecules in 1179 participants from stored fasting plasma drawn at the time of the detailed cognitive assessment. We used a customized, highly multiplexed, magnetic bead-based immunoassay similar to one used in previous studies in our laboratory. 22,23 Our assay (Table e-2) included molecules integral to inflammatory responses, reflecting relative activation of Th1 (pro-inflammatory), Th2 (counter-regulatory, autoimmunity-promoting), Th17 (innate mucosal anti-pathogen defenses, including in gut), T regulatory, B (antibody-producing) and NK cells, and, more generally, innate and adaptive (memory) arms of the immune system; chemoattractants (chemokines); growth factors (including those vital to vascular remodeling); neurotrophic factors; cell adhesion markers; and adipokines.

Plasma samples were coded, randomized and assayed in duplicate. Median fluorescence intensities (MFI) of each analyte-specific immunoassay bead set were detected by the flow- and fluorescence-based Luminex 200™ detection platform (Luminex Corporation, Austin, TX).24 Data were processed using a quality control (QC) algorithm that calibrates performance of an expanded set of serial standard curves and an in-house plasma control run on every assay plate, monitoring intra- and inter-plate covariance (CV) and bead counts. Only plates with mean intra-assay %CV <15% were accepted. Samples failing to meet QC criteria were designated for re-run when feasible (%CVs >25%, bead counts <30). MFI values exceeding machine limits of reliable detection (>25,000) were excluded. Because interpolated concentrations can introduce bias for samples with very low or high values in relation to the serial standard curves, averaged MFI values for all 60 cytokines were used in the final statistical analyses rather than absolute concentrations.25,26 The final data set (test samples) had mean intra-assay %CV of 4.6% (standard deviation [SD] 2.2%) and 0.5% of all possible intra-assay %CV values were >25%, across all cytokines. Per-cytokine intra-assay %CV values ranged from 1.9% to 7.9%.

Statistical analyses

Distributions of socio-demographic characteristics and vascular risk factors for the study cohort were calculated using descriptive statistics.

Association between cognitive outcomes and immune markers was assessed using a two-step hybrid Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) approach27: first, we performed the LASSO technique to select a subset among the 60 immune markers (logarithm base 2-transformed and standardized) and all possible two-way interactions that linearly predicted each cognitive domain; variable selection was performed using 5-fold cross validation based on the sum of squared error criterion. Then associations of selected markers with cognitive function, and added effects of immune markers beyond other predictors of cognition, were assessed using a series of 5 linear regression models. Model 1 adjusted for immune markers only, Model 2 adjusted for age only, and Model 3 adjusted for demographics (age, sex, education, race-ethnicity, education, and insurance status), and risk factors (diabetes, smoking status, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, high-density lipoprotein, low-density lipoprotein, and estimated glomerular filtration rate). Model 4 adjusted for age and immune markers, and Model 5 (the full model) adjusted for immune markers, demographics and risk factors. The independent association of the immune marker ensemble was tested using the likelihood ratio test (LRT) to compare models with and without the LASSO-selected markers and interactions. R-squared (R2) was used to quantify the improvement of variance in the cognitive function explained by the LASSO-selected immune marker ensemble. We also created models including quadratic terms in the regression. We used LASSO as a variable selection procedure in order to avoid screening all immune markers (and their interactions) marginally. LASSO provides an inherent control of multiplicity. LASSO was performed using R3.6.3 (R Core team, 2020) with the package glmnet,28 and linear regression analyses were conducted using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc. Cary, NC)..

RESULTS

Description of cohort

There were 1179 participants with immune marker data available (Table 1). Mean age was 70±8.9 years and 60% were women. There was a high proportion of Hispanics (68%); mean education level was 10±5 years. There was a high burden of diabetes (28%) and other cardiovascular risk factors.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the Cohort

Overall
N=1179
Age at visit 1, mean (±SD) 70 (±8.9)
Men, n (%) 473 (40.1%)
Years of Education, mean (±SD) 9.7 (±5.1)
Race-ethnicity, n (%) Hispanic 785 (66.6%)
Non-Hispanic Black 198 (16.8%)
Non-Hispanic White 171 (14.5%)
Medical Insurance, n (%) Medicaid or no Insurance 560 (47.5%)
Medicare or Private Insurance 619 (52.5%)
Current Smokers, n (%) 182 (15.4%)
Moderate or Heavy Physical Activity, n (%) 119 (10.2%)
Diabetes, n (%) 324 (27.5%)
Systolic blood pressure in mm Hg, mean (±SD) 137 (±17.6)
Diastolic blood pressure in mm Hg, mean (±SD) 78 (±9.6)
High-density lipoprotein, mg/dl, mean (±SD) 53.3 (±16.9)
Low-density lipoprotein, mg/dl, mean (±SD) 115.6 (±35.1)
eGFR, mean (±SD) 73.6 (±18.0)

SD=standard deviation; eGFR=estimated glomerular filtration rate

Associations of LASSO-selected immune markers with cognitive function

Inclusion of LASSO-selected immune markers collectively improved model fit over age alone (p-value for LRT< 0.0001 for global score and for all domains) and over age, demographics including education, and traditional risk factors for each cognitive outcome (p-value <0.0001 for global score, memory, language, and executive function: p = 0.0081 for processing speed; Table 2). For global cognitive function, the variance in cognition explained by the immune markers was similar to that explained by age (model 0 and model 1-1), and the addition of immune markers to a model that included age alone led to almost a doubling of the R2 (model 1-1 and model 1-2), implying that the effect of the immune markers was independent and comparable in size to that of the effect of age. The effect of age alone was greatest for the domain of processing speed (R2 =0.17) and least for language (R2 =0.02). The impact of adding immune markers to age was greatest for executive function, for which R2 increased from 0.04 for a model including only age to 0.17 when the immune markers were added.

Table 2.

Impact of immune markers and other factors on explanatory models for cognitive test scores

Adjusted R2 values for models containing the listed variables
Cognitive function Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
Immune
markers*
only
Age only Age,
demographics
and risk
factors**
Immune
markers* and
age
Immune
markers*, age,
demographics,
and risk
factors**
Global Score 0.13 0.12 0.48 0.21ǂ 0.50¥
Cognitive Domains
  Memory 0.12 0.11 0.31 0.18ǂ 0.33¥
  Language 0.09 0.02 0.39 0.10ǂ 0.41¥
  Processing Speed 0.11 0.17 0.31 0.22ǂ 0.32¥¥
  Executive Function 0.14 0.04 0.39 0.17ǂ 0.42¥
*

Immune markers refers to those immune markers and their interactions that were selected from the Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) procedure as described in Methods.

**

Demographics and risk factors: Sex, race-ethnicity, education, medical insurance type, diabetes, smoking status, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, high-density lipoprotein, low-density lipoprotein, estimated glomerular filtration rate

ǂ

p<0.0001 for likelihood ratio test for effect of addition of immune markers selected by LASSO to model adjusted only for age (comparing Model 2 and Model 4)

¥

p<0.0001 for likelihood ratio test for effect of addition of immune markers selected by LASSO to model adjusted for age, demographics and risk factors (comparing Model 3 and Model 5)

¥¥

p=0.0081for likelihood ratio test for effect of addition of immune markers selected by LASSO to model adjusted for age, demographics and risk factors (comparing Model 3 and Model 5)

Approximately 48% of the variance in global cognition was explained by a model containing age, other demographics, and risk factors; the model fit improved by approximately 4.0% (from 48% to 50%) after inclusion of the immune markers.

In models including quadratic terms, we found no significant improvement of model fit over a model without quadratic terms and the R2 for two nested models were almost equivalent.

Specific immune markers predicting cognitive functions

A subset of immune markers and their two-way interactions that predicted each cognitive function were selected using LASSO (Table e-2). C-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 11 (CCL11, or eotaxin), C-X-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 9 (CXCL9), hepatocyte growth factor (HGF) and serpin E1 (or plasminogen activator inhibitor-1) were consistently selected as being associated with overall cognitive function (Table 3) and with each of the neuropsychological domains (table e-3-6). In addition, sFasL, SCF, leptin, CCL3, CSF3 and CCL2 contributed to multiple cognitive domains. Interactions between CCL5 and serpin E1, between CCL3 and TNFβ, between CCL11 and IL1RA, between HGF and leptin, between TRAIL and IL12p40, between CCL11 and CCL5, and between CXCL10 and CXCL9 were also found in 2 or more domains.

Table 3:

Association of immune markers and interactions with global cognitive score*

Immune markerǂ Estimate Standard Error p
CCL11 −0.036 0.017 0.033
HGF −0.066 0.020 0.001
CXCL9 −0.029 0.017 0.092
SCF −0.008 0.018 0.669
Leptin 0.027 0.019 0.160
sFasL 0.043 0.018 0.015
CCL3 0.024 0.019 0.193
Serpin_E1 0.037 0.016 0.022
Serpin_E1*CCL5 −0.021 0.014 0.123
HGF*Leptin −0.036 0.017 0.031
CCL11*IL1RA −0.008 0.012 0.485
CCL11*SCF −0.033 0.015 0.025
sFasL*PDGFBB −0.030 0.015 0.042
CCL5*CCL4 −0.001 0.020 0.962
CCL4*VCAM1 −0.029 0.022 0.186
CXCL10*IFNb 0.009 0.031 0.761
HGF*TRAIL 0.014 0.017 0.411
CXCL10*TGFa 0.011 0.035 0.745
CCL3*TNFb 0.007 0.014 0.599
TRAIL*IL12p40 0.025 0.019 0.179
CXCL10*IL5 −0.010 0.021 0.643
CCL3*LIF −0.006 0.003 0.670
CCL11*sICAM1 0.020 0.017 0.243
CXCL10*VEGFD 0.013 0.016 0.432
*

Adjusted for age, sex, race-ethnicity, education, medical insurance type, diabetes, smoking status, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, high-density lipoprotein, low-density lipoprotein, estimated glomerular filtration rate

ǂ

Immune markers were log 2 transformed and standardized

CCL11 = C-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 11; CXCL9 = C-X-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 9; HGF = hepatocyte growth factor; sFasL = soluble Fas ligand; SCF = stem cell factor; CSF2 = Colony stimulating factor 2; CCL2 = C-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 2; CCL3 = C-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 3; CCL4 = C-C Motif Chemokine 4; IL12p40 = interleukin-12 p40; IL21 = interleukin 21; IL1a = interleukin 1a; CCL4 = C-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 4; IFNb = interferon beta; PDGFBB = Platelet derived growth factor-BB; TGFa = transforming growth factor α; TRAIL = Tumor necrosis factor-related apoptosis-inducing ligand

Magnitude of immune marker effects

The magnitude of immune marker effects was comparable to that of conventional risk factors. For example, for executive function, each SD increase in CCL11 levels (β=−0.052, p=0.016) was associated with an effect size equivalent to aging 3 years (3-year β=−0.048, p<0.0001). HGF had twice the effect on memory function (β per SD=−0.06, p=0.012) as aging (β per year =−0.03, p<0.0001).

Discussion

We found several immune molecules are cross-sectionally associated with cognitive function in a predominantly Hispanic, but ethnically diverse, population. Many immune markers were correlated, prompting us to use the LASSO technique to perform data reduction, thereby extracting those molecules that were independently and most informatively associated with cognition. As a group, those markers that were selected using LASSO had an explanatory impact comparable and independent to that of age. After accounting for sociodemographic and vascular risk factors, as well, the immune panel significantly improved model fit for the association with cognition by approximately 4%.

These results suggest that plasma immune markers have an independent association with cognition beyond that due to traditional risk factors for cognition and vascular disease. While the collective magnitude of these markers on cognition was modest, the magnitude of association for several markers was comparable to that of other well-accepted risk markers of cognitive impairment, including age. Cognitive decline is a complex, multifactorial phenomenon, and it is unlikely that any single risk factor, immune pathway or marker accounts for a substantial proportion of the variance in cognition. The total variance explained by models not including the immune panel did not exceed 50%. Our results suggest that several immune pathways may be involved and that approaches that affect multiple pathways simultaneously will be most likely to have an impact, although further research is needed.

Individual markers most consistently associated with cognition were the chemokines CCL11 and CXCL9, the neurotrophic factor HGF, and serpin E1. Chemokines are a large family of small protein molecules that play an important role in immune functions. CCL11 (eotaxin-1) primarily binds to the CCR3 receptor and is important in eosinophil recruitment, allergic responses, and skewing toward a T helper (Th)-2 immune response. Its levels increase with age, it can cross the blood brain barrier, and it has been postulated to promote aging-accelerated neurodegeneration.29 CCL11, produced by activated astrocytes, induces microglial activation and production of reactive oxygen species, promoting excitotoxic neuronal death in animal models.30 CCL11 concentrations are increased after stroke in mice; in humans CCL11 concentrations were lower after stroke than in controls without stroke, however, and lower levels correlated with increased stroke severity.31

CXCL9 is a member of the subfamily of CXC chemokines. They all share the CXCR3 receptor, and participate in Th1-type immune responses, involving tissue infiltration by T cells as part of the innate immune response. CXCL9 is upregulated by IFN-γ, induces activation of extracellular signal-regulated kinases in cortical neurons in mice, and is involved in neuronal–glial interactions.32 The CXC chemokines increase with inflammation and infection of the CNS,33 and are also up-regulated in human brains with Alzheimer pathology.34

HGF is a neurotrophic factor with anti-apoptotic and angiogenesis effects that facilitates dendritic arborization and has been associated with small artery disease among patients with cognitive impairment and AD.35 Consistent with our findings, these associations were independent of other vascular risk factors, suggesting that the mechanisms by which HGF contributes to cognitive impairment may be independent of traditional vascular risk factors. It is increasingly evident that vascular and neurodegenerative contributions to cognitive aging and dementia overlap, and that vascular mechanisms contribute to neurodegeneration. Immune mechanisms that lead to small vessel disease may thus enhance the degenerative pathways that lead to AD, either by impairing blood flow, enhancing inflammation, reducing regenerative capacity, or inhibiting amyloid clearance.

Serpin E1 is the major inhibitor of the serine proteases tissue plasminogen activator and urokinase, and thus acts as an inhibitor of fibrinolysis. Elevated levels potentially contribute to increased atherosclerosis, and risk of coronary artery events and stroke.36 In addition, serpin E1 inhibits activity of matrix metalloproteinases, which play a role in blood-brain barrier breakdown after ischemia.37 Serpin E1 also plays a role in regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which must be converted from a pro-protein by plasmin. BDNF plays an important role in neuronal activity and memory.38 A small study showed that serpin E1 is elevated in peripheral blood with mild cognitive impairment;39 however, our findings of higher levels of serpin E1 in association with cognitive performance, and without interaction with BDNF, suggest that serpin E1 production may exert a small protective effect.

Associations of immune markers with cognition were present for each of the individual domains of cognitive function investigated, although several markers, as well as a global cognitive measure, were present across all areas. Other markers selected by our data reduction technique as contributing to two or more individual cognitive domains included sFasL (memory, language function, processing speed, and global cognition), SCF (memory, processing speed, and global cognition), CSF2 (memory and processing speed), leptin (language, executive function, and global cognition), and the chemokines CCL2 (processing speed, executive function) and CCL3 (language, executive function, and global cognition).

Evidence that levels of the chemokines CCL2, CCL3, CCL11 and CXCL9 are associated with cognitive decline in our cohort support the hypothesis that the innate immune system contributes to cognitive impairment and dementia.40,41 Recent studies provide evidence that increased amyloid-beta peptide activates the innate immune system via pattern recognition receptors on microglia and astrocytes, contributing to hyperphosphorylation of tau. Sterile inflammation can also be provoked by recognition of intracellular contents released from necrotic cells through damage-associated molecular patterns. Elevations of these chemokines could reflect chronic activation of innate immunity in brains of patients with cognitive impairment. There is also evidence that infections, such as herpes simplex virus-1, provoke innate immunity, cause tau phosphorylation, and contribute to neurodegeneration.42 Previous studies in our cohort demonstrate that serologies against herpesviruses are associated with lower cognitive test scores.43,44

Our results could have clinical implications since inflammation is potentially modifiable. IL1β inhibition diminishes residual inflammatory risk for cardiovascular events,45,46 and some antiviral agents are being tested for efficacy against AD, with variable results.47,48,49 Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors also decreased IFNγ, TNFα, IL1β and IL6 secretion by immune cells and enhanced innate resistance to viral infection in vitro.50 Furthermore, the timing of intervention also appears to be critical, underscoring the significance of long-term prospective investigations such as NOMAS.

Our study has limitations. First, measures of immune molecules and cognition were obtained at a single time point, limiting our ability to make causal determinations. The immune response is complex and changes over time, moreover, due to aging as well as due to other intercurrent events, including illness and development of risk factors, during an individual’s lifetime. Our study explores the associations of immune markers at a single timepoint with cognition measured at a single timepoint, and thus addresses mean effects over an extended period of time, rather than being responsive to short-term changes in immune markers. It is likely that superimposed on these long-term effects are shorter term effects due to changes in the immune response, due to acute infections, surgery, or other inflammatory disorders. Clinically, cognition often declines at the time of surgery, or after hospitalization for other acute illness.51,52 We continue to follow our cohort over time, however, to provide longitudinal data. Second, immune markers were not assessed in the cerebrospinal fluid or brain tissue, the site where injury occurs. Nonetheless, blood biomarkers are increasingly recognized as indicators of neurodegenerative activity and predictors of AD. Third, though our panel included 60 different immune-related markers, many others were not included, and we did not include apolipoprotein E genotype. Fourth, we did not include MRI-derived biomarkers in this study, nor did we have positron emission tomography to assess amyloid, tau, or microglial activations. Our outcomes, however, were cognitive test scores, which may reflect multiple pathologies and may ultimately have greater clinical relevance for patients. Our assay does not provide a clinically useful panel of markers for the prediction of dementia currently. Fifth, some of the participants had some degree of cognitive impairment, as only those with cognitive impairment so severe that they could not give informed consent were excluded. It is possible that these participants may have driven the effects we found. Our findings require confirmation in other cohorts, and in longitudinal analyses, before they can be used for clinical purposes.

Our study also has strengths. Our cohort represents a racially and ethnically diverse, elderly cohort at highest risk for cognitive decline and dementia. Many prior studies have not included large numbers of Black and Hispanic/Latino people. Our population was also well-characterized for demographic and medical risk factors. Our multiplex immune assay included a large number of molecules that may contribute to cognitive decline and dementia.

In conclusion, peripheral blood levels of several immune-related molecules, representing pathways related to innate immunity, neural repair, and thrombosis, may play a role in cognitive decline and dementia. As a group these factors modestly contribute to cognitive impairment, independently of vascular risk factors associated with cognitive decline. These results speak to the ability of a multiplex immune assay to add information regarding pathways that may be involved in neurodegeneration in a diverse population. These findings also indicate the need for further research on immune mechanisms in cognitive aging.

Supplementary Material

1

Highlights:

  • Immune molecules associate with cognition in an ethnically diverse population.

  • They add information about cognition beyond traditional risk factors like age and vascular risk.

  • Chemokines CCL11 and CXCL9, the neurotrophic factor HGF, and serpin E1 most consistently associated with global cognition, as well as individual cognitive domains.

  • These data provide potential immune pathways as targets to reduce cognitive decline.

Source of funding:

NIH/NINDS R01 NS029993

Non-standard Abbreviations and Acronyms

95% CI

95% confidence interval

AD

Alzheimer Disease

CCL2

C-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 2

CCL3

C-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 3

CCL5

C-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 5

CCL11

C-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 11

CSF3

Colony stimulating factor 3

CRP

C-reactive protein

CXCL9

C-X-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 9

CXCL10

C-X-C Motif Chemokine Ligand 10

HGF

hepatocyte growth factor

IL12p40

interleukin-12 p40

IL1RA

interleukin-1 receptor antagonist

LASSO

Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator

LRT

likelihood ratio test

MCI

mild cognitive impairment

NOMAS

Northern Manhattan Study

SCF

stem cell factor

sFasL

soluble Fas ligand

TNFβ

tumor necrosis factor β

TRAIL

tumor necrosis factor-related apoptosis-inducing ligand

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest:

Dr. Elkind receives royalties from UpToDate for chapters on stroke and COVID-19.

Ms. Moon declares no conflicts of interest.

Dr. Cheung declares no conflicts of interest.

Dr. Sacco declares no conflict of interest.

Dr. Hornig declares no conflict of interest.

Dr. Rundek declares no conflict of interest.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Initially presented at American Academy of Neurology Annual Meeting May 2018.

References

  • 1.Schwartz M, Peralta Ramos JM, Ben-Yehuda H. A 20-Year Journey from Axonal Injury to Neurodegenerative Diseases and the Prospect of Immunotherapy for Combating Alzheimer's Disease. Journal of immunology (Baltimore, Md : 1950) 2020;204:243–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Esenwa CC, Elkind MS. Inflammatory risk factors, biomarkers and associated therapy in ischaemic stroke. Nat Rev Neurol 2016;12:594–604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Novellino F, Saccà V, Donato A, et al. Innate Immunity: A Common Denominator between Neurodegenerative and Neuropsychiatric Diseases. International journal of molecular sciences 2020;21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Itzhaki RF, Golde TE, Heneka MT, Readhead B. Do infections have a role in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer disease? Nat Rev Neurol 2020;16:193–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Park JC, Han SH, Mook-Jung I. Peripheral inflammatory biomarkers in Alzheimer's disease: a brief review. BMB reports 2020;53:10–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bostancıklıoğlu M. An update on the interactions between Alzheimer's disease, autophagy and inflammation. Gene 2019;705:157–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Forrester SN, Gallo JJ, Whitfield KE, Thorpe RJ. A Framework of Minority Stress: From Physiological Manifestations to Cognitive Outcomes. The Gerontologist 2019;59:1017–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Daulatzai MA. Role of stress, depression, and aging in cognitive decline and Alzheimer's disease. Current topics in behavioral neurosciences 2014;18:265–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Amariglio RE, Buckley RF, Rabin JS, et al. Examining Cognitive Decline Across Black and White Participants in the Harvard Aging Brain Study. Journal of Alzheimer's disease: JAD 2020;75:1437–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rosenberg GA. Extracellular matrix inflammation in vascular cognitive impairment and dementia. Clinical science (London, England : 1979) 2017;131:425–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Fu Y, Yan Y. Emerging Role of Immunity in Cerebral Small Vessel Disease. Front Immunol 2018;9:67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Noz MP, Ter Telgte A, Wiegertjes K, et al. Trained Immunity Characteristics Are Associated With Progressive Cerebral Small Vessel Disease. Stroke 2018;49:2910–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Poggesi A, Pasi M, Pescini F, Pantoni L, Inzitari D. Circulating biologic markers of endothelial dysfunction in cerebral small vessel disease: A review. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2016;36:72–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Smiley ST, King JA, Hancock WW. Fibrinogen stimulates macrophage chemokine secretion through toll-like receptor 4. J Immunol. 2001;167:2887–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ryu JK, Petersen MA, Murray SG, Baeten KM, Meyer-Franke A, Chan JP, Vagena E, Bedard C, Machado MR, Rios Coronado PE, Prod'homme T, Charo IF, Lassmann H, Degen JL, Zamvil SS, Akassoglou K. Blood coagulation protein fibrinogen promotes autoimmunity and demyelination via chemokine release and antigen presentation. Nat Commun. 2015;6:8164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Becker KJ, Tanzi P, Zierath D, Buckwalter MS. Antibodies to myelin basic protein are associated with cognitive decline after stroke. J Neuroimmunol. 2016;295-296:9–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Zierath D, Kunze A, Fecteau L, Becker K. Promiscuity of autoimmune responses to MBP after stroke. J Neuroimmunol. 2015August15;285:101–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Wright CB, Moon YP, Paik MC, Brown TR, Rabbani LR, Yoshita M, DeCarli C, Sacco RL, Elkind MSV. Inflammatory Biomarkers of Vascular Risk as Correlates of Leukoariosis. Stroke 2009;40:3466–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wright CB, Gardener H, Dong C, Yoshita M, DeCarli C, Sacco RL, Stern Y, Elkind MS. Infectious Burden and Cognitive Decline in the Northern Manhattan Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63(8):1540–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wright CB, DeRosa JT, Moon MP, Strobino K, DeCarli C, Cheung YK, Assuras S, Levin B, Stern Y, Sun X, Rundek T, Elkind MSV, Sacco RL. Race/Ethnic Disparities in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia: The Northern Manhattan Study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;80(3):1129–1138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Siedlecki KL, Rundek T, Elkind MSV, Sacco RL, Stern Y, Wright CB. Using contextual analyses to examine the meaning of neuropsychological variables across samples of English-speaking and Spanish-speaking older adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2011;12:1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hornig M, Montoya JG, Klimas NG, et al. Distinct plasma immune signatures in ME/CFS are present early in the course of illness. Sci Adv 2015;1:e1400121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hornig M, Gottschalk G, Peterson DL, et al. Cytokine network analysis of cerebrospinal fluid in myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome. Mol Psychiatry 2016;21:261–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Vignali DA. Multiplexed particle-based flow cytometric assays. J Immunol Methods 2000;243:243–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Breen EJ, Tan W, Khan A. The Statistical Value of Raw Fluorescence Signal in Luminex xMAP Based Multiplex Immunoassays. Sci Rep. 2016;6:26996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Breen EJ, Polaskova V, Khan A. Bead-based multiplex immuno-assays for cytokines, chemokines, growth factors and other analytes: median fluorescence intensities versus their derived absolute concentration values for statistical analysis. Cytokine. 2015;71:188–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Efron B, Hastie T, Johnstone I, Tibshirani R. Least angle regression. Ann Statist 2004;32:407–499. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Friedman J, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. Regularization Paths for Generalized Linear Models via Coordinate Descent. J Stat Softw. 2010;33:1–22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Villeda SA, Luo J, Mosher KI, Zou B, Britschgi M, Bieri G, et al. The ageing systemic milieu negatively regulates neurogenesis and cognitive function. Nature. 2011;477 (7362): 90–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Parajuli B, Horiuchi H, Mizuno T, Takeuchi H, Suzumura A. CCL11 enhances excitotoxic neuronal death by producing reactive oxygen species in microglia. Glia. 2015;63:2274–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Roy-O'Reilly M, Ritzel RM, Conway SE, Staff I, Fortunato G, McCullough LD. CCL11 (eotaxin-1) Levels Predict Long-Term Functional Outcomes in Patients Following Ischemic Stroke. Transl Stroke Res. 2017;8:578–584. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Xia MQ, Bacskai BJ, Knowles RB, Qin SX, Hyman BT. Expression of the chemokine receptor CXCR3 on neurons and the elevated expression of its ligand IP-10 in reactive astrocytes: In vitro ERK1/2 activation and role in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neuroimmunol. 2000;108(1–2):227–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Watson AES, Goodkey K, Footz T, Voronova A. Regulation of CNS precursor function by neuronal chemokines. Neurosci Lett. 2020;715:134533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Galimberti D, Schoonenboom N, Scarpini E, Scheltens P; Dutch-Italian Alzheimer Research Group. Chemokines in serum and cerebrospinal fluid of Alzheimer’s disease patients. Ann Neurol. 2003;53:547–548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Zhu Y, Hilal S, Chai YL, et al. Serum Hepatocyte Growth Factor Is Associated with Small Vessel Disease in Alzheimer's Dementia. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Liu Y, Cheng J, Guo X, Mo J, Gao B, Zhou H, Wu Y, Li Z. The roles of PAI-1 gene polymorphisms in atherosclerotic diseases: A systematic review and meta-analysis involving 149,908 subjects. Gene. 2018October5;673:167–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Keric N, Maier GS, Samadani U, Kallenberg K, Dechent P, Brueck W, Heuer J, Rohde V. Tissue plasminogen activator induced delayed edema in experimental porcine intracranial hemorrhage: reduction with plasminogen activator inhibitor-1 administration. Transl Stroke Res. 2012;3(Suppl l):88–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Angelucci F, Čechová K, Průša R, Hort J. Amyloid beta soluble forms and plasminogen activation system in Alzheimer's disease: Consequences on extracellular maturation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and therapeutic implications. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2019;25:303–313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Shen XN, Lu Y, Tan CTY, Liu LY, Yu JT, Feng L, Larbi A. Identification of inflammatory and vascular markers associated with mild cognitive impairment. Aging (Albany NY). 2019;11:2403–2419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Heneka MT, Carson MJ, El Khoury J, Landreth GE, Brosseron F, Feinstein DL, Jacobs AH, Wyss-Coray T, Vitorica J, Ransohoff RM, et al. Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol. 2015;14:388–405 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Salminen A, Ojala J, Kauppinen A, Kaarniranta K, Suuronen T. Inflammation in Alzheimer's disease: amyloid-beta oligomers trigger innate immunity defence via pattern recognition receptors. Prog Neurobiol. 2009;87:181–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wozniak MA, Frost AL, Itzhaki RF. Alzheimer's disease-specific tau phosphorylation is induced by herpes simplex virus type 1. J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;16:341–350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Katan M, Moon YP, Paik MC, Sacco RL, Wright CB, Elkind MS. Infectious burden and cognitive function: the Northern Manhattan Study. Neurology. 2013;80:1209–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Zhao C, Strobino K, Moon YP, Cheung YK, Sacco RL, Stern Y, Elkind MSV. APOE ϵ4 modifies the relationship between infectious burden and poor cognition. Neurol Genet. 2020. ;6:e462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ridker PM, Everett BM, Thuren T, et al. Antiinflammatory Therapy with Canakinumab for Atherosclerotic Disease. New Engl J Med 2017;377:1119–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Ridker PM. Anticytokine Agents: Targeting Interleukin Signaling Pathways for the Treatment of Atherothrombosis. Circulation Research 2019;124:437–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ali MM, Ghouri RG, Ans AH, Akbar A, Toheed A. Recommendations for Anti-inflammatory Treatments in Alzheimer's Disease: A Comprehensive Review of the Literature. Cureus 2019;11:e4620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Tzeng NS, Chung CH, Lin FH, et al. Anti-herpetic Medications and Reduced Risk of Dementia in Patients with Herpes Simplex Virus Infections-a Nationwide, Population-Based Cohort Study in Taiwan. Neurotherapeutics : the journal of the American Society for Experimental NeuroTherapeutics 2018;15:417–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Dominy SS, Lynch C, Ermini F, et al. Porphyromonas gingivalis in Alzheimer's disease brains: Evidence for disease causation and treatment with small-molecule inhibitors. Science advances 2019;5:eaau3333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Kokras N, Stamouli E, Sotiropoulos I, et al. Acetyl Cholinesterase Inhibitors and Cell-Derived Peripheral Inflammatory Cytokines in Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease. Journal of clinical psychopharmacology 2018;38:138–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Eriksson LI, Lundholm C, Narasimhalu K, et al. Hospitalization, surgery, and incident dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15:534–542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Schulte PJ, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, et al. Association between exposure to anaesthesia and surgery and long-term cognitive trajectories in older adults: report from the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Br J Anaesth 2018; 121: 398–405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.D’Elia L, Satz P, Uchiyama CL, White T (1994) Color Trails Test Professional Manual, Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Beery K (1989) The VMI, Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration, Modern Curriculum Press, Cleveland, OH. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Ruff RM, Parker SB. Gender- and age-specific changes in motor speed and eye-hand coordination in adults: Normative values for the Finger Tapping and Grooved Pegboard Tests. Percept Mot Skills 1993;76:1219–1230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Weschler D (1997) Wechsler adult intelligence scale - third edition, Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hoppe CD, Muller UD, Werheid KD, Thone AD, von Cramon YD. Digit Ordering Test: Clinical, psychometric, and experimental evaluation of a verbal working memory test. Clin Neuropsychol 2000;14:38–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Goodglass H, Kaplan E (1983) The assessment of aphasia and related disorders, Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Benton AL, Hamsher K, Sivan AB (1994) Manual for the Multiligual Aphasia Examination, AJA Associates, Inc., 3rd Ed., Iowa City. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Smith A (1982) Symbol digit modalities test: Manual. Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Dunn LM, Dunn LM (1997) Peabody picture vocabulary test, American Guidance Service, Circle Pines. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Stone M, Jastak S, Wilkinson G (1995) Wide Range Achievement Test. Rasch Measurement Transactions; 1995;8: 403. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Del Ser T , Gonzalez-Montalvo JI , Martinez-Espinosa S , Delgado-Villapalos C , Bermejo F. Estimation of premorbid intelligence in Spanish people with the Word Accentuation Test and its application to the diagnosis of dementia. Brain Cogn 1997;33:343–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES