Abstract
Background
Individuals with coronary heart disease (CHD) often suffer from severe distress due to diagnosis, hospitalization, surgical procedures, uncertainty of outcome, fear of dying, doubts about progress in recovery, helplessness and loss of control. Such adverse effects put the cardiac patient at greater risk for complications, including sudden cardiac death. It is therefore of crucial importance that the care of people with CHD focuses on psychological as well as physiological needs.
Music interventions have been used to reduce anxiety and distress and improve physiological functioning in medical patients; however its efficacy for people with CHD needs to be evaluated.
Objectives
To update the previously published review that examined the effects of music interventions with standard care versus standard care alone on psychological and physiological responses in persons with CHD.
Search methods
We searched the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL) on The Cochrane Library (2012, Issue 10), MEDLINE (OvidSP, 1950 to October week 4 2012), EMBASE (OvidSP, 1974 to October week 5 2012), CINAHL (EBSCOhost, 1982 to 9 November 2012), PsycINFO (OvidSP, 1806 to October week 5 2012), LILACS (Virtual Health Library, 1982 to 15 November 2012), Social Science Citation Index (ISI, 1974 to 9 November 2012), a number of other databases, and clinical trial registers. We also conducted handsearching of journals and reference lists. We applied no language restrictions.
Selection criteria
We included all randomized controlled trials and quasi‐randomized trials that compared music interventions and standard care with standard care alone for persons with confirmed CHD.
Data collection and analysis
Two review authors independently extracted data and assessed methodological quality, seeking additional information from the trial researchers when necessary. We present results using weighted mean differences for outcomes measured by the same scale, and standardized mean differences for outcomes measured by different scales. We used post‐intervention scores. In cases of significant baseline difference, we used change scores (changes from baseline).
Main results
We identified four new trials for this update. In total, the evidence for this review rests on 26 trials (1369 participants). Listening to music was the main intervention used, and 23 of the studies did not include a trained music therapist.
Results indicate that music interventions have a small beneficial effect on psychological distress in people with CHD and this effect is consistent across studies (MD = ‐1.26, 95% CI ‐2.30 to ‐0.22, P = 0.02, I² = 0%). Listening to music has a moderate effect on anxiety in people with CHD; however results were inconsistent across studies (SMD = ‐0.70, 95% CI ‐1.17 to ‐0.22, P = 0.004, I² = 77%). Studies that used music interventions in people with myocardial infarction found more consistent anxiety‐reducing effects of music, with an average anxiety reduction of 5.87 units on a 20 to 80 point score range (95% CI ‐7.99 to ‐3.75, P < 0.00001, I² = 53%). Furthermore, studies that used patient‐selected music resulted in greater anxiety‐reducing effects that were consistent across studies (SMD = ‐0.89, 95% CI ‐1.42 to ‐0.36, P = 0.001, I² = 48%). Findings indicate that listening to music reduces heart rate (MD = ‐3.40, 95% CI ‐6.12 to ‐0.69, P = 0.01), respiratory rate (MD = ‐2.50, 95% CI ‐3.61 to ‐1.39, P < 0.00001) and systolic blood pressure (MD = ‐5.52 mmHg, 95% CI ‐ 7.43 to ‐3.60, P < 0.00001). Studies that included two or more music sessions led to a small and consistent pain‐reducing effect (SMD = ‐0.27, 95% CI ‐0.55 to ‐0.00, P = 0.05). The results also suggest that listening to music may improve patients' quality of sleep following a cardiac procedure or surgery (SMD = 0.91, 95% CI 0.03 to 1.79, P = 0.04).
We found no strong evidence for heart rate variability and depression. Only one study considered hormone levels and quality of life as an outcome variable. A small number of studies pointed to a possible beneficial effect of music on opioid intake after cardiac procedures or surgery, but more research is needed to strengthen this evidence.
Authors' conclusions
This systematic review indicates that listening to music may have a beneficial effect on anxiety in persons with CHD, especially those with a myocardial infarction. Anxiety‐reducing effects appear to be greatest when people are given a choice of which music to listen to.
Furthermore, listening to music may have a beneficial effect on systolic blood pressure, heart rate, respiratory rate, quality of sleep and pain in persons with CHD. However, the clinical significance of these findings is unclear. Since many of the studies are at high risk of bias, these findings need to be interpreted with caution. More research is needed into the effects of music interventions offered by a trained music therapist.
Plain language summary
Music to reduce stress and anxiety for people with coronary heart disease
Individuals with coronary heart disease often suffer from severe distress, putting them at greater risk for complications, including sudden cardiac death. It is therefore important that the care of people with coronary heart disease focuses on psychological as well as physiological needs. Music interventions have been used for many years to reduce anxiety and distress and improve physiological responses such as heart rate and respiratory rate in medical patients.
This review is an update of a previous Cochrane review from 2009 which suggested that music interventions may have a beneficial effect on anxiety and physiological responses in people with coronary heart disease but the quality of the evidence was not strong and the clinical significance unclear.
For this review, we searched for additional trials on the effect of music interventions on stress and anxiety in people with coronary heart disease. We searched for studies published up until November 2012 as well as ongoing studies until November 2012. We considered all studies in which any form of participation in music (e.g. listening to music, singing, playing music) was compared with any form of standard treatment and included persons with confirmed coronary heart disease. We identified four new trials for this update.
This review includes 26 trials with a total of 1369 participants. The trials were small in size. The findings suggest that listening to music may have a beneficial effect on systolic blood pressure and heart rate in people with coronary heart disease. Listening to music also appears to be effective in reducing anxiety in people with myocardial infarction, especially when they are given a choice of which music to listen to. Listening to music may also reduce pain and respiratory rate. However the size of the effects on pain and respiratory rate is small. Therefore, its clinical importance is unclear. Finally, listening to music appears to improve patients' quality of sleep following a cardiac procedure or surgery. We found no evidence of effect for depression or heart rate variability, and inconsistent results for mood. No adverse effects of music interventions were reported. The majority of the studies examined the effects of listening to pre‐recorded music. More research is needed on the effects of music interventions offered by a trained music therapist. Overall, the quality of the evidence is not strong thus the results should be interpreted with caution.
We did not identify any conflicts of interests in the included studies.
Summary of findings
Summary of findings 1. Music versus standard care for coronary heart disease.
Music versus standard care for coronary heart disease | ||||||
Patient or population: people with coronary heart disease Settings: Intervention: music versus standard care | ||||||
Outcomes | Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI) | Relative effect (95% CI) | No of Participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments | |
Assumed risk | Corresponding risk | |||||
Control | Music versus standard care | |||||
Psychological Distress POMS | The mean psychological distress in the intervention groups was 1.26 lower (2.30 to 0.22 lower) | 228 (5 studies) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ low1 | |||
Anxiety (all measures) NRS, VAS, HADS, STAI | The mean anxiety (all measures) in the intervention groups was 0.70 standard deviations lower (1.17 to 0.22 lower) | 353 (10 studies) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ very low1,2,3 | |||
State anxiety (MI patients) STAI | The mean state anxiety (MI patients) in the intervention groups was 5.87 lower (7.99 to 3.75 lower) | 243 (6 studies) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ low1 | |||
Heart rate bpm | The mean heart rate in the intervention groups was 3.62 lower (6.28 to 0.95 lower) | 828 (13 studies) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ very low1,2,3 | |||
Respiratory rate breaths per minute | The mean respiratory rate in the intervention groups was 2.50 lower (3.61 to 1.39 lower) | 442 (7 studies) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ very low1,4 | |||
Systolic blood pressure | The mean systolic blood pressure in the intervention groups was 5.52 lower (7.43 to 3.60 lower) | 775 (11 studies) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ low1 | |||
Pain VAS, NRS | The mean pain in the intervention groups was 0.43 standard deviations lower (0.80 to 0.05 lower) | 562 (8 studies) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ very low1,3,5 | |||
*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI). CI: Confidence interval; | ||||||
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: Further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect. Moderate quality: Further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate. Low quality: Further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate. Very low quality: We are very uncertain about the estimate. |
1The majority of the trials were assessed as being at high risk of bias 2Results were inconsistent across studies as evidenced by I² = 77%. 3Wide confidence interval 4Results were inconsistent across studies as evidenced by I² = 79%. 5Results were inconsistent across studies as evidenced by I² = 81%.
Background
Description of the condition
Coronary heart disease (CHD) is the leading cause of death worldwide. According to the World Health Organization (WHO), 17.3 million people worldwide die of cardiovascular diseases (CVD) each year. By 2030, the WHO predicts 25 million CVD deaths globally (WHO 2012). Heart disease has no geographical, gender, or socioeconomic boundaries (Chockalingam 1999). People with CHD often suffer from severe distress due to diagnosis, hospitalization, surgical procedures, uncertainty of outcome, fear of dying, doubts about progress in recovery, helplessness and loss of control (Barnason 1995; Bolwerk 1990; Guzzetta 1989; Malan 1992). This stress is likely to stimulate the release of epinephrine and norepinephrine, resulting in increased heart rate, respiratory rate, arterial blood pressure, myocardial oxygen demand and anxiety levels. Such adverse effects put the cardiac patient at greater risk for complications, including sudden cardiac death (White 1999). It is therefore of crucial importance that the care of people with CHD focuses on psychological as well as physiological needs.
Description of the intervention
There is a great deal of literature, both quantitative and qualitative, regarding the use of music to reduce stress and anxiety in non‐medical patients, and this provides the context and rationale for its hypothesized effects in people with CHD. Moreover, with non‐medical patients, music is used both alone and as an adjunct to traditional stress‐reduction approaches in therapy and for self‐help procedures. Effects of music for stress reduction have been documented in physiological (e.g. heart rate, blood pressure, hormonal levels), neurological (e.g. electroencephalographic (EEG) readings) and psychological domains (e.g. self report, the Spielberger State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)) (Dileo 2007). In addition, the effects of both music and music therapy interventions have been documented in a range of other medical patients, for example, pre‐surgical, oncology, pediatric, and pre‐procedural patients (Dileo 1999; Dileo 2005). Moreover, anxiety and stress reduction is one of the primary outcomes investigated in music medicine and music therapy research with medical patients. Effects similar to those reported in the current review have been observed, and meta‐analyses of these effects have been conducted (Bradt 2010b; Bradt 2011; Dileo 2005; Standley 1986; Standley 2000).
Dileo 2007 makes a clear distinction between music interventions administered by medical or healthcare professionals (music medicine) and those implemented by trained music therapists (music therapy). Interventions are categorized as 'music medicine' when passive listening to pre‐recorded music is offered by medical personnel. In contrast, music therapy requires the implementation of a music intervention by a trained music therapist, the presence of a therapeutic process, and the use of 'live' music experiences. These music experiences include: (1) listening to live, improvised or pre‐recorded music; (2) performing music on an instrument; (3) improvising music spontaneously using voice or instruments or both; (4) composing music; and (5) music combined with other modalities (e.g. movement, imagery and art).
Several investigators have examined the effects of music on a variety of outcomes in people with CHD, including heart rate (Barnason 1995; Davis‐Rollans 1987), respiratory rate, blood pressure (Barnason 1995), myocardial oxygen demand (White 1999), hormone levels (Vollert 2002), anxiety (Barnason 1995; Bolwerk 1990), and pain (Zimmerman 1996).
How the intervention might work
As discussed in a recently published Cochrane systematic review on the effects of music interventions on pre‐operative anxiety (Bradt 2013), a common theory regarding the anxiety‐reducing effects of music is that music can help people focus their attention away from stressful events to something pleasant and soothing (Mitchell 2003; Nilsson 2008). Even though this is an important mechanism in anxiety reduction, it is important to emphasize that music does more than refocusing people's attention. It provides an aesthetic experience that can offer comfort and peace while awaiting a cardiac procedure or surgery or the outcome of a myocardial infarction. In music interventions provided by a trained music therapist, the music therapist furthermore adapts the live music interactions to the in‐the‐moment needs of the participants. This often provides a deeply humanizing and validating experience for the patient. The act of making music together can provide a strong sense of support. Moreover, the active and creative engagement in music making (e.g. singing songs, improvising music) stands in stark contrast with passively submitting oneself to cardiac procedures or surgery. This may result in an increased sense of control and empowerment.
It has been postulated that music induces relaxation through its impact on automated and central nervous responses (Gillen 2008). More specifically, it is believed that the anxiolytic effect of music is achieved through its suppressive action on the sympathetic nervous system, leading to decreased adrenergic activity and decreased neuromuscular arousal (Chlan 1998; Gillen 2008). Music furthermore triggers the limbic system in the brain to release endorphins; these neurotransmitters play an important role in enhancing a sense of well‐being (Arslan 2008; Lee 2005). However, Gillen 2008 has suggested that more research is needed to examine the physiological mechanisms that explain the anxiolytic effects of music.
Why it is important to do this review
Although there are no hypothesized responses to music unique to this population, the effects of music on heart rate, respiratory rate, blood pressure, and anxiety have been widely studied both in people with or without medical conditions. These outcomes have heightened significance when it comes to cardiac patients, and a systematic review of the existing data is therefore much needed.
The previous version of this review found that music listening may have a beneficial effect for people with CHD and may reduce blood pressure and heart rate. Listening to music also appeared to be effective in reducing anxiety in people with myocardial infarction (MI) upon hospitalization. It has therefore been recommended that music listening be offered as a stress management intervention to people with MI upon hospitalization.
Objectives
To update the previously published review that examined the effects of music interventions with standard care versus standard care alone on psychological and physiological responses in persons with CHD.
Methods
Criteria for considering studies for this review
Types of studies
All randomized clinical trials and quasi‐randomized trials (trials that allocate participants to a treatment by using a method that is not random such as alternate group assignment or date of birth) in any language, published and unpublished, were eligible for inclusion.
Types of participants
The review included studies of men, women, and children, inpatient or outpatient, with confirmed coronary heart disease (CHD). The original review included several studies with participants who did not have confirmed CHD (e.g., cardiac diagnostic procedures). For this update we limited the inclusion criteria to studies where all participants had confirmed CHD. Therefore, studies involving cardiac procedures for diagnostic purposes as well as studies with participants with suspected myocardial infarction were not included in this review. We imposed no restrictions as to age, gender, or ethnicity.
Types of interventions
The review included all studies in which any form of participation in music (e.g. listening to music, singing, playing music) was compared with any form of standard treatment. Studies using music therapy interventions, as defined by the authors, as well as music medicine interventions, as defined by the authors, were considered for inclusion. We did not use length or frequency of music sessions as inclusion criteria for this review.
Types of outcome measures
Primary outcomes
Psychological distress including anxiety, depression, and mood;
Quality of life.
Secondary outcomes
Heart rate;
Respiratory rate;
Systolic blood pressure;
Diastolic blood pressure;
Mean arterial pressure;
Myocardial oxygen demand;
Oxygen saturation;
Peripheral skin temperature;
Hormone levels;
Pain;
Opioid intake;
Quality of sleep;
Length of hospitalization;
Duration of cardiac procedure.
Where more than one measure per outcome was used for psychological distress, quality of life, and pain, we gave preference to measures taken using validated instruments. Primary outcomes and pain were rated by the participant. Physiological responses were recorded by an observer who may or may not have been blinded.
Search methods for identification of studies
Electronic searches
We updated the previously‐run searches from 2008 (Appendix 1) and searched the following databases between 5 November and 15 November 2012:
Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL) on The Cochrane Library (2012, Issue 10);
MEDLINE (OvidSP, 1950 to October week 4 2012);
EMBASE (OvidSP, 1974 to October week 5 2012);
CINAHL (EBSCOhost, 1982 to 9 November 2012);
PsycINFO (OvidSP, 1806 to October week 5 2012);
LILACS (Virtual Health Library, 1982 to 15 November 2012);
Social Science Citation Index (ISI, 1974 to 9 November 2012);
the specialist music therapy research database at www.musictherapyworld.net (on June 16 2007; database has not been maintained after this date);
CAIRSS for Music (Webvoyage, to February 2005; database has not been maintained after this date);
Proquest Digital Dissertations (1861 to 9 November 2012).
We also searched the following clinical trials registers:
ClinicalTrials.gov (www.clinicaltrials.gov) (11 November 2012);
Current Controlled Trials (www.controlled-trials.com) (11 November 2012);
National Research Register (http://www.nihr.ac.uk/Pages/NRRArchiveSearch.aspx) (2000 to September 2007).
The search strategies are listed in Appendix 2. For this update, the search results for the databases were limited from 2008 until November 2012.
We also searched the Internet (www.google.com) to find scholars and research centers that have focused on the use of music for cardiac care.
Searching other resources
In addition, we handsearched relevant journals, see Appendix 3 for full details. For this update, the journals were handsearched from 2008 until November 2012.
We checked the bibliographies of relevant studies or reviews and contacted relevant experts for the identification of unpublished trials. There were no language restrictions for either searching or trial inclusion.
Data collection and analysis
Selection of studies
One review author (JB) scanned the titles and abstracts of each record retrieved from the searches for the original review, while another review author (NB) did this for the update. If information in the abstract clearly indicated that the trial did not meet the inclusion criteria, we rejected the trial. When a title or abstract could not be rejected with certainty, we obtained the full‐text article, and two review authors (JB and CD for original review; JB and NP for update) independently inspected it. The review authors used an inclusion criteria form to assess the trial's eligibility for inclusion. If we excluded a trial, we recorded both the article citation and the reason for exclusion.
Data extraction and management
Two review authors (JB and CD in original review; JB and NP for the update) independently extracted data from the selected trials using a standardized coding form. We discussed and collaboratively resolved any differences in data extraction. We extracted the following data (where applicable):
General information
Author; Year of publication; Title; Journal (title, volume, pages); If unpublished, source; Duplicate publications; Country; Language of publication.
Trial information
Study design (parallel group, cross‐over); Randomization; Randomization method; Allocation concealment; Allocation concealment method; Level of blinding.
Intervention information
Type of intervention (e.g. listening, singing, playing music). Music selection (music style, detailed information on music selection, live music, recorded music); Music preference (patient‐preferred, researcher‐selected); Administrator of music intervention (music therapist, medical personnel); Length of intervention; Intensity of intervention; Comparison intervention.
Participants information
Total sample size; N of experimental group; N of control group; Gender; Age; Ethnicity; Diagnosis; Setting; Inclusion criteria.
Outcomes
Heart rate; Respiratory rate; Systolic blood pressure; Diastolic blood pressure; Myocardial oxygen demand; Hormone levels; Anxiety; Depression; Mood (e.g. Profile of Mood States (POMS)); Pain; Other.
Assessment of risk of bias in included studies
At least two review authors (JB and CD in original review; JB, CD, and NP for update) assessed all included trials, blinded to each other's assessment for trial quality. In case of disagreements, JB was the arbiter. We used the following criteria for quality assessment, following Cochrane Collaboration guidance in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Cochrane Handbook: Higgins 2011):
Random sequence generation
Low risk;
Unclear risk;
High risk.
We rated random sequence generation as being at low risk if every participant had an equal chance to be selected for either condition, and if the investigator was unable to predict to which treatment group the participant would be assigned. Use of date of birth, date of admission or alternation was rated as being at high risk of bias.
Allocation concealment
Low risk ‐ methods to conceal allocation include:
central randomization;
serially numbered, opaque, sealed envelopes;
other descriptions with convincing concealment.
Unclear risk ‐ authors did not adequately report on method of concealment;
High risk (e.g. alternation methods were used).
Blinding of participants and personnel
Low risk;
Unclear risk;
High risk.
Since participants cannot be blinded in a music intervention trial, we did not downgrade studies for not blinding the participants. As for personnel, in music therapy studies music therapists cannot be blinded because they are actively making music with the participants. In contrast, in music medicine studies, blinding of personnel is possible by providing control group participants with headphones but no music (e.g. a blank CD). Downgrading for not blinding personnel was therefore only applied in studies that used listening to pre‐recorded music.
Blinding of outcome assessors
Low risk;
Unclear risk;
High risk.
Incomplete outcome data
We recorded the proportion of participants whose outcomes were analyzed. We coded loss to follow‐up for each outcome as:
Low risk: if fewer than 20% of participants were lost to follow‐up and reasons for loss to follow‐up were similar in both treatment arms;
Unclear risk: if loss to follow‐up was not reported;
High risk: if more than 20% of participants were lost to follow‐up or reasons for loss to follow‐up differed between treatment arms.
Selective reporting
Low risk: reports of the study were free of suggestion of selective outcome reporting;
Unclear risk;
High risk: reports of the study suggest selective outcome reporting.
Other sources of bias • Low risk; • Unclear risk; • High risk. Information on potential financial conflicts of interest was considered as a possible source of additional bias.
The above criteria were used to give each article an overall quality rating based on the Cochrane Handbook, section 8.5.3 (Higgins 2011):
Low risk of bias: all seven criteria met;
Moderate risk of bias: one or more of the criteria only partly met;
High risk of bias: one or more criteria not met.
Dealing with missing data
We did not impute missing outcome data. We analyzed data on an end point basis, including only participants for whom we had final data point measurement (available case analysis). We did not assume that participants who dropped out after randomization had a negative outcome (i.e. intention‐to‐treat analysis)..
Assessment of heterogeneity
We investigated statistical heterogeneity using the I² test, taking a value greater than 50% to indicate significant heterogeneity (Higgins 2003; Higgins 2011).
Assessment of reporting biases
We tested for publication bias visually in the form of funnel plots (Higgins 2011).
Data synthesis
We entered all trials included in the systematic review into Review Manager 5 (Revman 2012). The main outcomes in this review were physiological responses and psychological responses (anxiety, pain, mood) presented as continuous variables. We used post‐intervention scores for the meta‐analysis. In the case of multiple music sessions, we used post‐intervention data from the last session. In the case of statistically significant baseline differences, we computed change scores (i.e. changes from baseline) according to the guidelines provided by the Cochrane Handbook (Higgins 2011). We calculated standardized mean differences (SMDs) for outcome measures using results from different scales, and weighted mean differences for results using the same scales. Studies for which change scores were used were not included in standardized mean difference analyses. For cross‐over trials, we used only data from the first phase of the trials. We calculated pooled estimates using the fixed‐effect model unless there was significant heterogeneity (I² > 50%), in which case we used the random‐effects model to obtain a more conservative estimate. We calculated the 95% confidence interval for each effect size estimate.
This review does not include any categorical variables.
We conducted the following treatment comparison: music versus standard care.
Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity
We had planned the following subgroup analyses a priori, but not all could be carried out because of an insufficient number of studies: 1. Music medicine versus music therapy (as defined by the authors): could not be conducted because only three music therapy studies were included in this review. 2. Different levels of engagement in music experiences (listening, singing, playing instruments): could not be conducted because the majority of the studies used music listening as the intervention. 3. Patient‐selected music versus researcher‐selected music: was conducted for those outcome variables for which the pooled estimate was heterogeneous.
Patient‐type: The included studies presented three distinct population groups: (a) myocardial infarction patients, (b) surgical or procedural patients, and (c) rehabilitation patients. Although we did not determine this subgroup analysis a priori, the review authors decided it was important to conduct a subgroup analysis comparing the effect on these three groups of studies for those outcome variables for which we found significant heterogeneity.
Sensitivity analysis
We examined the impact of sequence generation by comparing the results of including and excluding trials that used inadequate or unclear randomization methods.
Results
Description of studies
Results of the search
The database searches and handsearching of conference proceedings and journals of the original review (2009) resulted in 702 citations. One review author (JB) examined the titles and abstracts, and retrieved 77 references for possible inclusion. Two review authors then independently screened them, resulting in 29 references to 22 studies that met all the inclusion criteria. Twenty‐one references to 20 studies appeared to meet the inclusion criteria but were excluded upon further examination (see Characteristics of excluded studies). A further twenty‐seven references turned out not to be relevant to this review as they were program descriptions, review articles, and case studies, or used a combination of treatments (e.g. music and aroma therapy).
The 2012 update of the search resulted in 216 extra citations. One review author (NP) examined the titles and abstracts and retrieved full‐text articles where necessary. This resulted in the addition of five new references to four studies (Cutshall 2011; Jafari 2012; Leist 2011; Ryu 2011). Four additional cardiac catheterization studies were considered for inclusion but not all study participants had confirmed coronary heart disease (CHD) (Chang 2011; Ghetti 2011; Goertz 2011; Taylor‐Piliae 2002). These studies therefore needed to be excluded. In addition, we decided to include quasi‐randomized controlled trials in this update, with the result that two studies (Barnes 1987; Blankfield 1995) that had been excluded from the original review are included in this update. It is important to note that four studies that were included in the original review are excluded from this update (Argstatter 2006; Guzzetta 1989; Robichaud 1999; Zimmerman 1988), due to the fact that not all research participants in these studies had confirmed CHD.
Where necessary we contacted chief investigators to obtain additional information on study details and data.
Included studies
We include 26 studies with a total of 1369 participants. These studies examined the effects of music on psychological, physiological, and physical outcomes in people undergoing cardiac surgery and procedures (14 studies, 955 participants) (Barnason 1995; Blankfield 1995; Broscious 1999; Cadigan 2001; Chan 2007; Cutshall 2011; Hermele 2005; Jafari 2012; Nilsson 2009a; Ryu 2011; Schou 2008; Sendelbach 2006; Stein 2010; Voss 2004), myocardial infarction (MI) (seven studies, 267 participants) (Bolwerk 1990; Cohen 1999; Davis‐Rollans 1987; Elliott 1994; White 1992; White 1999; Winters 2005), and cardiac rehabilitation patients (five studies, 147 participants) (Barnes 1987; Emery 2003; Leist 2011; Mandel 2007a; Murrock 2002). The large majority of the participants included in these studies were men (74%). The average age of the participants was 62.86 years. For 14 trials, the ethnicity of the participants was not reported (Barnes 1987; Bolwerk 1990; Cadigan 2001; Chan 2007; Cutshall 2011; Davis‐Rollans 1987; Elliott 1994; Jafari 2012; Mandel 2007a; Murrock 2002; Nilsson 2009a; Ryu 2011; Schou 2008; Sendelbach 2006). For those studies that did report on ethnicity, the majority of the participants were white (average of 89.9%). Several studies combined non‐white ethnic groups together under 'other', making it hard to estimate the percentage of other specific ethnic groups represented in these trials (Blankfield 1995; Broscious 1999; Cohen 1999; Voss 2004). Trial sample size ranged from 9 to 179 participants with an average sample size of 64 (Median = 58).
Not all studies measured all outcomes identified for this review. For studies with more than one intervention group, we used only data of the participants in the music group and the standard care group.
Twenty‐three studies (Barnason 1995; Blankfield 1995; Bolwerk 1990; Broscious 1999; Cadigan 2001; Chan 2007; Cohen 1999; Cutshall 2011; Elliott 1994; Hermele 2005; Jafari 2012; Leist 2011; Mandel 2007a; Murrock 2002; Nilsson 2009a; Ryu 2011; Schou 2008; Sendelbach 2006; Stein 2010; Voss 2004; White 1992; White 1999; Winters 2005) used parallel‐group designs, whereas three studies (Barnes 1987; Davis‐Rollans 1987; Emery 2003) used cross‐over designs. For these cross‐over trials, we used only data from the first phase (i.e. before the cross‐over) in the meta‐analysis.
Details of the studies included in the review are shown in the table Characteristics of included studies.
Twenty‐three studies were categorized as music medicine studies (as defined in the Background section above) (Barnason 1995; Barnes 1987; Blankfield 1995; Bolwerk 1990; Broscious 1999; Cadigan 2001; Chan 2007; Cohen 1999; Cutshall 2011; Davis‐Rollans 1987; Elliott 1994; Emery 2003; Hermele 2005; Jafari 2012; Murrock 2002; Nilsson 2009a; Ryu 2011; Sendelbach 2006; Stein 2010; Voss 2004; White 1992; White 1999; Winters 2005). Three studies were categorized as music therapy (Leist 2011; Mandel 2007a; Schou 2008). All but two studies (Leist 2011; Mandel 2007a) used music listening as the main intervention. Twelve trials included one music session offered before or during a cardiac procedure or both (e.g. cardiac catheterization) or within 48 hours of hospitalization for MI (Broscious 1999; Cadigan 2001; Chan 2007; Cohen 1999; Davis‐Rollans 1987; Emery 2003; Jafari 2012; Nilsson 2009a; Ryu 2011; Voss 2004; White 1992; White 1999). Two trials included two sessions offered over two postoperative days (Barnason 1995; Stein 2010). Twelve trials offered three or more sessions on consecutive days (Barnes 1987; Blankfield 1995; Bolwerk 1990; Cutshall 2011; Elliott 1994; Hermele 2005; Leist 2011; Mandel 2007a; Murrock 2002; Schou 2008; Sendelbach 2006; Winters 2005). The duration of the music sessions varied across trials. Some trials offered music immediately prior to, during, and immediately following a procedure, whereas other trials only offered music during the procedure. For trials in those with MI (Bolwerk 1990; Cohen 1999; Davis‐Rollans 1987; Elliott 1994; White 1992; White 1999; Winters 2005), the average length of the music sessions was 30 minutes.
For all studies in this review, the participants in the control group received standard medical care. Within each study, data were obtained from the control group participants at the same time intervals as for the participants in the music intervention group.
All but one study (Hermele 2005) measured the outcome variables immediately following the music intervention.
Eight studies provided detailed information about the music that was used (composition title and composer) (Barnason 1995; Barnes 1987; Blankfield 1995; Bolwerk 1990; Davis‐Rollans 1987; Elliott 1994; Emery 2003; Ryu 2011). Twelve studies stated only the different styles of music that were offered to the participants (e.g. jazz, easy listening, country and western, classical music) without any composition‐ or performance‐specific information (Broscious 1999; Cadigan 2001; Chan 2007; Cohen 1999; Cutshall 2011; Nilsson 2009a; Schou 2008; Sendelbach 2006; Stein 2010; Voss 2004; White 1992; White 1999). Only one study provided composition title, composer, and tempo information (Murrock 2002).
Eleven studies used patient‐selected music (Barnason 1995; Barnes 1987; Broscious 1999; Chan 2007; Cohen 1999; Cutshall 2011; Jafari 2012; Mandel 2007a; Sendelbach 2006; Voss 2004; Winters 2005), whereas 15 studies used researcher‐selected music (Blankfield 1995; Bolwerk 1990; Cadigan 2001; Davis‐Rollans 1987; Elliott 1994; Emery 2003; Hermele 2005; Leist 2011; Murrock 2002; Nilsson 2009a; Ryu 2011; Schou 2008; Stein 2010; White 1992; White 1999).
The studies were conducted in seven different countries: USA (20 studies: Barnason 1995; Barnes 1987; Blankfield 1995; Bolwerk 1990; Broscious 1999; Cadigan 2001; Cohen 1999; Cutshall 2011; Davis‐Rollans 1987; Emery 2003; Hermele 2005; Leist 2011; Mandel 2007a; Murrock 2002; Sendelbach 2006; Stein 2010; Voss 2004; White 1992; White 1999; Winters 2005), Australia (one study: Elliott 1994), Denmark (one study: Schou 2008), Hong Kong (one study: Chan 2007), South Korea (one study: Ryu 2011), Sweden (one study: Nilsson 2009a), and Iran (one study: Jafari 2012).
Fifteen studies were funded or partly funded by a grant from a foundation (Cutshall 2011; Sendelbach 2006; Stein 2010; Mandel 2007a), University (Elliott 1994; Jafari 2012), professional association (Barnason 1995; Blankfield 1995), nursing honor society (Broscious 1999; Cadigan 2001; Voss 2004), local government (Nilsson 2009a), or federal government (Emery 2003; White 1999; Winters 2005). The remainder of the studies were either unfunded or no report of funding was included in the manuscript.
Excluded studies
The main reasons for exclusion of studies that appeared eligible for this review were (a) not a randomized or quasi‐randomized controlled trial, (b) lack of a standard treatment control group, (c) use of cardiac procedures for diagnostic purpose, and (d) participants with unconfirmed CHD diagnosis (e.g. suspected MI). Reasons for exclusion are listed in the table Characteristics of excluded studies.
Risk of bias in included studies
Allocation
We included 24 (92%) trials that used appropriate methods of randomization (e.g. computer‐generated table of random numbers, drawing of lots, flip of coins), one trial that used alternate group assignment as the allocation method, and one trial that reported using randomization but failed to state the randomization method.
Eighteen trials (69%) used allocation concealment whereas three trials did not. For the remainder of the trials, allocation concealment was not mentioned.
Blinding
In music intervention studies, participants cannot be blinded (unless in studies that compare different types of music interventions). Two studies that used music listening reported blinding personnel. This was achieved by having both music group and control group participants wear headsets and listen to a compact disk (CD). The control group listened to a blank CD. In music therapy trials, the music therapist cannot be blinded, given the interactive nature of the music‐making in the session.
Ten trials reported blinding of the outcome assessors for objective measures. For seven trials, the use of blinding was unclear. The other trials did not use blinding. However, it is important to point out that in case of assessment of subjective outcomes through self‐report measures, blinding is often not possible since participants know whether they received the music intervention or a no‐music control condition. Only in comparative studies (e.g. studies that compare active music‐making with passive listening to music) can participants be blinded to the intervention and can their self report be considered blinded as well (Bradt 2012). We would like to point out that the 'Risk of bias' figure (Figure 4) lists one study as having used blinding for subjective outcomes. However, this study did not include subjective outcomes. A rating of low risk was assigned if studies did not include subjective outcomes.
The lack of blinding of outcome assessors for objective outcomes as well as the inability to blind participants to their self report of subjective outcomes inevitably introduces the potential for biased assessment. As blinding of intervention allocation is not possible in music interventions, this added another layer of possible bias.
Incomplete outcome data
The drop‐out rate was less than 20% for 19 (73%) of the trials. For four trials, the drop‐out rate was unclear. Three trials had a drop‐out rate higher than 20% or their reasons for excluding participant data in the data analysis suggested potential for bias. Most studies reported reasons for drop‐out.
Selective reporting
Publication bias for anxiety (Figure 1), heart rate (Figure 2), and systolic blood pressure (SBP) (Figure 3) as outcomes was examined visually in the form of funnel plots. The funnel plots for anxiety and heart rate indicate that there may be publication bias. However, it is also possible that the two smaller studies that appear in the lower part of the funnel plots were of lesser quality and, consequently, resulted in exaggerated effect sizes. The funnel plot on SBP did not show evidence of publication bias.
Other potential sources of bias
The study reports did not suggest other potential sources of bias.
As a result of the risk of bias assessment, we concluded that two trials were at moderate risk of bias (Jafari 2012; Leist 2011). All other trials were at high risk of bias. The main reason for assigning a high risk of bias rating was the lack of blinding. As mentioned before, blinding is often impossible in music therapy and music medicine studies that use subjective outcomes, unless in studies where the music intervention is compared to another treatment intervention (e.g. progressive muscle relaxation or different type of music intervention). It therefore appears impossible for these types of studies to be rated at a low or even moderate risk of bias, even if all other risk factors (e.g. randomization, allocation concealment, etc.) have been adequately addressed.
Risk of bias is detailed for each trial in the risk of bias tables included with the Characteristics of included studies table and in the 'Risk of bias' Summary (Figure 4). In addition, an overall assessment of risk of bias can be viewed in Figure 5.
As all but two trials were rated at the same level (high risk), we did not carry out sensitivity analysis on the basis of overall quality rating. Instead, we conducted a sensitivity analysis to examine the impact of the method of random sequence generation. Excluding those studies that used alternate assignment or for which the randomization method was unclear did not alter the findings of this review. Specific sensitivity analysis findings are reported in the Effects of interventions section.
Overall the quality of the evidence was low. (See the 'Summary of findings' table)
Effects of interventions
See: Table 1
Primary outcomes
Psychological distress
Five studies examined the effects of music listening on psychological distress by use of the Profile of Mood States (POMS) (Cadigan 2001; Hermele 2005, Leist 2011; Schou 2008; Stein 2010) and one study by means of the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) (Mandel 2007a). The pooled estimate of those studies that used the POMS (N = 228) indicated a small beneficial effect of music interventions on distress, and this result was consistent across studies (mean difference (MD) = ‐1.26, 95% confidence interval (CI) ‐2.30 to ‐0.22, P = 0.02, I² = 0%) (Analysis 1.1). Mandel 2007a (N = 68) did not find statistically significant differences for psychological distress between the music therapy group and the standard care control group at posttest but did find an effect size of 0.54 at four‐month follow‐up.
Sixteen studies examined the effects of music on anxiety, seven of which reported mean state anxiety as measured by the Spielberger State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Nine studies reported mean anxiety measured by other scales such as numeric rating scale and visual analogue scale.
We first conducted an overall analysis of studies that used the STAI to measure anxiety as well as those that used other scales. In order to pool the effect sizes of studies that use different scales, we only included studies that reported post‐intervention scores in the analysis.The standardized mean difference (SMD) of those studies (10 studies, N = 353) that reported post‐intervention anxiety scores, regardless of the scale used, revealed a moderate (Cohen 1988) effect favoring music interventions (SMD = ‐0.70, 95% CI ‐1.17 to ‐0.22, P = 0.004), but results were inconsistent across studies (I² = 77%). Grouping the studies by participant type (myocardial infarction (MI) patients, surgical/procedural patients, rehabilitation patients) (Analysis 1.2) did not resolve heterogeneity. However, grouping the studies by music preference led to homogeneous results for the four studies (N = 144) that used participant‐selected music (SMD = ‐0.89, 95% CI ‐1.42 to ‐0.36, P = 0.001, I² = 48%) but not for the five that used researcher‐selected music (N = 179) (SMD = ‐0.74, 95% CI ‐1.55 to 0.08, P = 0.08, I² = 85%) (Analysis 1.3).
When pooling studies that only used STAI State Anxiety form (STAI‐S) to measure state anxiety (seven studies, N = 310), significantly lower state anxiety was found in participants who received standard care combined with music interventions than those who received standard care alone (MD = ‐4.58, 95% CI ‐7.78 to ‐1.39; P = 0.005). However, considerable statistical heterogeneity remained (I² = 88%). Six out of these seven studies included participants with MI and one study included surgical patients. Pooling the results of only the MI studies (N = 243) resulted in a larger effect size that was more homogeneous (MD = ‐5.87, 95% CI ‐7.99 to ‐.3.75, P < 0.00001, I² = 53%) (Analysis 1.4.1).
We then explored whether music preference affected the outcome of state anxiety as measured by STAI‐S. Grouping the studies by whether participant‐preferred music (three studies, N = 167) or researcher‐selected music (four studies, N = 143) was used did not reduce heterogeneity (Analysis 1.5). However, as noted above, all studies in this subgroup analysis were MI studies except for one (Barnason 1995). A subsequent analysis from which we excluded this surgical study suggested a greater anxiety‐reducing effect and homogeneity for studies that used participant‐preferred music. Studies of people with MI (two studies, N = 100) that used participant‐preferred music resulted in an average anxiety reduction of 7.36 units on the STAI (95% CI ‐9.45 to ‐5.27, P < 0.00001, I² = 0%) compared to a reduction of 4.68 units for those studies that used researcher‐selected music (four studies, N = 143) (95% CI ‐8.27 to ‐1.10, P = 0.01, I² = 66%) (Analysis 1.6).
The pooled estimate for studies that measured anxiety by scales other than the STAI‐S (seven studies, N = 248) suggested no strong evidence of an effect (SMD = ‐0.43, 95% CI ‐0.93 to 0.06, P = 0.09). Here too, results were statistically heterogeneous (I² = 70%). Two studies (Cutshall 2011; Stein 2010) could not be included in the meta‐analysis because change scores were used. Cutshall 2011 reported a small effect size of ‐0.12 (95% CI ‐0.51 to 0.27) in 100 participants, whereas Stein 2010 (N = 36) resulted in an effect size of ‐0.35 (95% CI ‐1.01 to 0.31). Neither effect size was statistically significant. Grouping the studies by type of participant resulted in a larger pooled estimate for surgical/procedural patients (four studies, N = 171) but heterogeneity remained (SMD = ‐0.63, 95% CI ‐1.25 to ‐.0.01, P = 0.05, I² = 73%). Three studies with MI and rehabilitation patients (N = 77) did obtain a homogeneous effect but this effect was very small and did not reach statistical significance (SMD = ‐0.03, 95% CI ‐0.61 to 0.56, P = 0.93; I² = 31%) (Analysis 1.7). A subgroup analysis on the impact of music preference suggested that the use of participant‐preferred music (four studies, N = 144) resulted in a large anxiety reduction that was statistically significant and consistent across studies (MD = ‐0.89, 95% CI ‐1.42 to ‐0.36, P = 0.001, I² = 48%). In contrast,researcher‐selected music (in this case, classical music was used) appeared to slightly increase anxiety although this was not statistically significant (three studies, N = 104) (SMD = 0.11, 95% CI ‐0.28 to 0.49, P = 0.58, I² = 0%) (Analysis 1.8).
Six studies (N = 217) included depression as an outcome. Their pooled estimate indicated that participants who listened to music did not significantly differ in their reported levels of depression from those participants who received standard care (SMD = ‐0.11, 95% CI ‐0.38 to 0.16, P = 0.42, I² = 0%) (Analysis 1.9).
Two studies (N = 97) used a numeric rating scale to measure the effects of music on mood. Their pooled estimate indicated that participants who listened to music reported greater mood enhancement than those receiving standard care (SMD = 1.08, 95% CI ‐0.02 to 2.17, P = 0.05); however, there was disagreement between the two studies about the size of the effect (I² = 80%) (Analysis 1.10). The results are therefore inconclusive.
Quality of life
Only one study (Mandel 2007a) considered quality of life as an outcome. However, significant data loss greatly reduces the usefulness of the data from this study.
Secondary outcomes
Heart rate
The pooled estimate of 13 studies (N = 828) showed a significant effect on heart rate, favoring music interventions over standard care (MD = ‐3.40, 95% CI ‐6.12 to ‐0.69, P = 0.01). However, the results were inconsistent among studies (I² = 78%). Grouping the studies by type of participant (MI, surgical/procedural, rehabilitation) reduced heterogeneity for surgical/procedural patient studies (seven studies, N = 604) somewhat (I² = 54%) but the effect was no longer statistically significant (MD = ‐2.61, 95% CI ‐5.62 to 0.34, P = 0.09). For studies with MI patients (5 studies, N = 194), the effect remained statistically significant but the results were highly inconsistent across studies (MD =‐4.75, 95% CI ‐9.26 to ‐0.25, P = 0.04) (Analysis 1.11).
A subgroup analysis by participant‐selected versus researcher‐selected music produced interesting results. The pooling of studies that used researcher‐selected music (six studies, N = 398) resulted in a smaller but homogeneous effect size (MD = ‐2.67, 95% CI ‐4.27 to ‐1.07, P = 0.001; I² = 0%). The use of participant‐selected music (seven studies, N = 430) resulted in a larger effect size (MD = ‐4.69, 95% CI ‐9.40 to 0.02, P = 0.05); however, the results were highly inconsistent between studies (I² = 84%) (Analysis 1.12).
Two studies (N = 90) included heart rate variability as an outcome in people with MI. Their pooled estimate suggests that music has no effect on heart rate variability (MD = 0.07, 95% CI ‐0.34 to 0.48, P = 0.74, I² = 0% ) (Analysis 1.13).
Respiratory rate
Seven studies (N = 442) examined the effects of music on respiratory rate in people with coronary heart disease (CHD). A heterogeneous pooled estimate of ‐2.50 (95% CI ‐3.61 to ‐1.39, P < 0.00001, I² = 79%) was found (Analysis 1.14).
Pooling the studies that used researcher‐selected music (four studies, N = 256) had a similar effect on heterogeneity as for the heart rate outcome: the use of researcher‐selected music led to a smaller but homogeneous effect size (MD = ‐1.66, 95% CI ‐2.20 to ‐1.12, P < 0.00001, I² = 0%). The use of participant‐selected music (three studies, N = 186) resulted in a larger but heterogeneous pooled estimate (three studies, MD = ‐4.42, 95% CI ‐7.37 to ‐1.46, P = 0.003, I² = 89%) (Analysis 1.14). Statistical heterogeneity was due to one trial (Chan 2007) reporting much larger beneficial effects than the other two trials.
Systolic blood pressure
Listening to music significantly reduced the systolic blood pressure of people with CHD, as indicated by a pooled estimate of ‐5.52 mmHg (95% CI ‐ 7.43 to ‐3.60 P < 0.00001) (11 studies, N = 775). The results were consistent across studies (I² = 0%) (Analysis 1.15).
Diastolic blood pressure
A pooled estimate of ‐1.12 mmHg (95% CI ‐2.57 to 0.34) (nine studies, N = 685) was found for diastolic blood pressure, favoring music listening, but this difference of effect was not statistically significant (P = 0.13). The results were consistent across studies (I² = 14%) (Analysis 1.16).
Mean arterial pressure
Three studies (N = 158) examined the impact of music on mean arterial pressure. Their pooled estimate was not statistically significant (MD = ‐0.91, 95% CI ‐4.08 to 2.26, P = 0.57, I² = 0%) Analysis 1.17.
Myocardial oxygen demand
Only one study (Winters 2005) included myocardial oxygen demand, or the amount of oxygen required by the heart to function properly, as an outcome. We could not establish the method of measurement, despite attempts to contact the authors. The average myocardial oxygen demand reduction for the music group (N = 30) was 1607.3 (SD = 640.5). In contrast, the average myocardial oxygen demand of the standard care group (N = 30) increased by 447.5 (SD = 1011.1).
Three studies (184 participants) included oxygen saturation levels as an outcome. Their pooled estimate suggested no effect of music (MD = ‐0.02, 95% CI ‐1.65 to 1.61. P = 0.98, I² = 92%) (Analysis 1.18).
Hormone levels
One study (Nilsson 2009a) examined the effects of listening to music on cortisol levels in 58 participants one day following cardiac surgery; no significant difference was found between the music group and the standard care control group (MD = 1.20, 95% CI ‐122.83 to 125.23, P = 0.98).
Pain
Music interventions (eight studies) resulted in a statistically significant reduction of pain compared to standard care in 630 participants (SMD = ‐0.43, 95% CI ‐0.80 to ‐0.05, P = 0.03); however, the results were not consistent between studies ( I² = 81%) (Analysis 1.19).
Only one study used researcher‐selected music, and all but one study included surgical or procedural patients. We therefore could not conduct meaningful subgroup analyses for patient type or music preference.
Pooling the effects of those studies that provided two or more music sessions to the participants resulted in a homogeneous effect size that would be considered clinically small in magnitude (three studies, N = 210, SMD = ‐0.27, 95% CI ‐0.55 to ‐0.00, P = 0.05, I² = 0%) (Barnason 1995; Mandel 2007a; Sendelbach 2006). In one study (Cutshall 2011), participants listened to pre‐recorded music twice a day for three consecutive postoperative days. Because the authors only reported change scores, these results could not be pooled with the other studies. Cutshall 2011 consistently reported lower pain levels in the music listening group compared to the standard care control group for each of the six measurement points. However, this difference was only statistically significant for the second music listening session on postoperative day two with a reduction of 1.4 (SD = 1.4) for the music group versus a reduction of 0.4 (SD = 1.4) for the control group (P = 0.001).
Length of hospital stay
The pooled estimate of two studies (N = 82) indicated no evidence for an effect of music interventions on length of hospital stay (MD = ‐0.06, 95% CI ‐1.03 to 0.92, P = 0.91, I² = 0%) (Analysis 1.20)..
Opioid intake
Two studies (N = 90) examined the effect of music interventions on opioid intake by patients after coronary artery bypass graft (CABG). Both studies found slightly lower opioid use in those who participated in the music group, but this pooled effect was not statistically significant (SMD = ‐0.25, 95% CI ‐0.67 to 0.16, P = 0.23, I² = 0%) (Analysis 1.21).
Quality of sleep
The pooled estimate of two studies (N = 122) suggests that listening to music may improve the quality of sleep (SMD = 0.91, 95% CI 0.03 to 1.79, P = 0.04) after a cardiac procedure or surgery. There was considerable heterogeneity between the studies (I² = 81%). However, both studies agreed on the direction of the effect and the heterogeneity was due to one study reporting much greater benefits (Analysis 1.22).
Peripheral skin temperature
Only one study (Cadigan 2001) (N = 140) included peripheral skin temperature as an outcome. This study examined the effects of listening to music on patients during bedrest due to procedural sheaths or an intra‐aortic balloon pump. No significant difference was found between the music listening treatment group (M = 88, SD = 5.8) and the control group (M = 88, SD = 6.2) for peripheral skin temperature.
Duration of cardiac procedure
No studies examined the effects of music interventions on duration of cardiac procedure.
Discussion
Summary of main results
Psychological outcomes
The results of 10 studies (N = 353) suggest that listening to music has a moderate anxiety‐reducing effect in people with coronary heart disease (CHD). However, the results were inconsistent between studies, which were generally small and at high risk of bias, and therefore, need to be treated with caution.
Studies using the same scale (STAI) to measure anxiety obtained consistent anxiety‐reducing effects of music (5.87 units on a 20‐ to 80‐point score range) in people with myocardial infarction (MI). A reduction of 5.87 units may be considered small; however, mean baseline STAI scores were relatively low to begin with (ranging from 35.3 to 48.2). In all MI studies, anxiety was reduced after the music intervention to STAI levels that are considered to represent low anxiety. Greater anxiety‐reducing effects were found for studies that used participant‐preferred music than for those that used researcher‐selected music.
The pooled estimate of five studies (N = 228) suggests that music has a small beneficial effect on psychological distress and this difference was consistent across studies.
We found no evidence of an effect of music on depression (six studies; N = 217). This finding is consistent with those of other Cochrane systematic reviews on the use of music interventions with cancer patients (Bradt 2011) and with patients at end‐of‐life (Bradt 2010a). This result suggests that short‐term interventions (one or two music sessions) focused on acute symptom reduction (e.g. state anxiety, pain, distress) are inadequate to relieve depression. The treatment of depression may need a more long‐term regimen of music therapy sessions that actively uses the relational aspects of music‐making to address the psychotherapeutic needs of the participants rather than focusing on symptom relief.
The pooled estimate of two studies (N = 97) suggests that music listening may enhance mood. However, this finding was not statistically significant. More studies are needed to further evaluate the effect of music on mood as evidenced by the inconsistencies of results across these two studies.
One music therapy study (N = 88) included quality of life as an outcome in a population of cardiac rehabilitation patients. Because of significant rates of attrition in this study, we could draw no conclusions.
Physiological outcomes
Results of this review suggest that listening to music reduces heart rate. However, there was variation among the 13 studies (N = 828) on the size of this effect. In examining the source of the heterogeneity, we discovered that listening to participant‐selected music resulted in a heart rate reduction of 4.69 beats per minute (bpm) compared to 2.67 bpm when listening to researcher‐selected music. However, in contrast to participant‐selected music, the results were consistent across studies when researcher‐selected music was used, i.e. no statistical heterogeneity.
In two small studies (N = 90) there was no evidence for an effect of music on heart rate variability.
For respiratory rate, the use of researcher‐selected music also led to smaller (reduction of 1.66 breaths per minute) but consistent results (four studies, N = 256), whereas the use of patient‐selected music led to larger (4.42 breaths per minute) but inconsistent results (three studies, N = 186).
Pooled estimates indicate that music reduces systolic blood pressure by 5.5 mm Hg consistently across studies (11 studies, N = 775). No evidence of an effect was found for diastolic blood pressure (nine studies, N = 685).
Only one study (N = 60) examined the effects of music listening on myocardial oxygen demand and found a reduction in myocardial oxygen demand in contrast to the standard care group. No evidence of benefit was apparent for music on blood oxygen saturation (three studies, N = 184).
One study (N = 58) reported on the effect of music on cortisol levels but found no evidence for an effect. It is surprising that no other studies examined the effects of music on hormone levels that are of particular relevance to people with CHD, including adrenaline and noradrenaline concentration and other stress hormones that can be deleterious to cardiac functioning.
A small effect was found for music listening on self‐reported pain; however the results were inconsistent across studies (eight studies, N = 630). Excluding those studies that only used one music session led to a small effect that was consistent across studies (three studies, N = 210).
The pooled estimate of two studies (N = 90) points to a small but non‐significant effect of music on reduced need for opioid pain relief.
Finally, music listening appears to improve patients' quality of sleep following a cardiac surgery or procedure (two studies, N = 122).
Duration‐specific outcomes
Two studies (N = 82) examined whether music therapy or music medicine interventions impact the length of hospital stay, but their results suggest no evidence of an effect.
Overall completeness and applicability of evidence
This review includes 26 randomized and quasi‐randomized controlled trials.
All but two trials used music listening as the clinical intervention. Twenty‐three trials were categorized as music medicine trials, meaning that the music was administered by non‐music therapist medical personnel. This clinical uniformity adds to the strength of this review but also limits the applicability of the evidence. The evidence, as presented in this review, speaks only to the effect of listening to music provided by the researcher or selected by the participant from music choices presented by the researcher. This review does not include enough music therapy studies to present evidence on the effects of music therapy interventions, wherein the person is actively involved in a therapeutic process in which a variety of musical experiences (e.g. music improvisation, singing) are used. The data from the three music therapy studies were not analyzed separately because of this small number of studies and because of clinical diversity.
No data can currently be provided regarding costs or cost effectiveness of music medicine applications in cardiac care, as these data were not included in the studies reviewed. Furthermore, no data were provided regarding costs for music therapy interventions, so that no comparisons between these two types of treatments can be conducted. It is recommended that future research include cost effectiveness measures of these two interventions, as well as cost comparisons between them.
The trials in general included very limited information about the music selections used, except for mentioning general music styles (e.g. classical, easy listening, jazz, country). Needless to say, music within each of these styles can vary widely, and more detailed information would help clinicians make well‐informed music selections. In several trials, only classical music choices were offered without a good rationale for the music selection. In several trials, participants were allowed to select the music from that which was offered. This decision was based on the assumption that music preference plays an important part in the effectiveness of music relaxation. Certainly, allowing participants to select music that they like may enhance their sense of control; the power of this should not be underestimated as hospitalized people often feel helpless and disempowered. The results of this review indeed suggest that the use of participant‐selected music resulted in greater anxiety reduction and more consistent results across studies than the use of researcher‐selected music. However, it needs to be noted that participants could only select from a limited number of music styles presented by the researcher. It is likely that the preferred music of some of the participants was not included in the music selection offered and, even if it was, that they may not have liked the specific composition or song being played. One study explicitly stated that three participants withdrew from the study because they disliked the music (Chan 2007). Another researcher reported that some participants indicated that they would have preferred different music or that they did not care for the music (Bolwerk 1990). We suspect that using music that is truly preferred by the participant may lead to even greater anxiety reduction.
In addition, musical parameters of the music choices offered to participants varied more widely than those of researcher‐selected music. This could explain why trials that used participant‐selected music had more heterogeneous results for physiological outcomes than trials using researcher‐selected music. The results suggest that different music styles (e.g. jazz, country and western, classical, new age, etc.) affect the heart rate and respiratory rate differently. This is not surprising, given the knowledge that music is able to entrain or exert influence on the rate of physiological responses in patients (Bradt 2009b). More research is needed to evaluate the effect of music that is truly patient‐preferred as well as the effect of music with different characteristics (tempo, timbre, harmony, emotional intensity, etc.).
The majority of the studies only provided one music session to the participants. Because not all studies in this review addressed all main outcome variables, it was not possible to conduct a subgroup analysis to examine frequency and duration of sessions as moderator variables. Winters 2005 compared the effects of multiple music sessions during the course of a day and found that offering two or three music sessions had greater effects than one or no music sessions on various physiological and psychological responses in individuals after a MI. Offering multiple music listening sessions allows for the participant to give feedback about the music, select different music if needed, and become more skilled in using music for relaxation purposes. In the case of music therapy interventions, multiple sessions allow for the development of a therapeutic relationship and deepening of the therapeutic process through the music. This may lead to greater health benefits. At this time, however, the relationship between the frequency/duration of treatment and treatment effect remains unclear. Further investigation is needed into the optimal frequency and duration of music interventions for people with CHD.
Since the vast majority of participants in these trials were white (90%), generalizability to other ethnic groups is limited. Cultural sensitivity in music selection should always be considered.
Because only a small number of trials investigated the effect of music listening on mood, quality of life, myocardial oxygen demand, stress hormone levels, opioid intake, length of hospital stay, and quality of sleep, this evidence is not clinically applicable at this time. More research is needed.
Quality of the evidence
The quality of reporting in general was poor with only a few authors detailing the method of randomization, allocation concealment, and level of blinding. We needed to contact the chief investigators of most studies to provide additional methodological and statistical information. For many outcomes in this review, there were inconsistencies in effects among studies. In addition, the trials included were generally small (Mean trial size = 64; median = 58) resulting in a lack of precision of treatment effects as evidenced by the rather large confidence intervals. This, combined with the high risk of bias, requires that the results of this review be interpreted with caution. In summary, the quality of evidence was low (Table 1).
Potential biases in the review process
The strength of our review is that we searched all available databases and a large number of music therapy journals (English, German, and French language), checked reference lists of all relevant trials, contacted relevant experts for identification of unpublished trials, and included publications without language restrictions. In spite of such a comprehensive search, it is still possible that we have missed some published and unpublished trials. We requested additional data where necessary for all trials we considered for inclusion. This allowed us to get accurate information on the trial quality and data for most trials and helped us make well‐informed trial selection decisions.
We were able to identify several unpublished studies through communication with experts in the field. It is possible that we did not identify some grey literature, but it is doubtful that this would have a significant impact on our results. Grey literature tends to include trials with relatively small numbers of participants and inconclusive results (McAuley 2000).
Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews
We found no other systematic reviews on music interventions with cardiac patients.
The aim of this review was to update the previous version (Bradt 2009a) with the results of trials completed during the four years since its publication. Overall, the results of this review are similar to those of the previous version. The review was expanded with some additional outcomes that are of importance to healthcare costs associated with the treatment of CHD, namely length of hospital stay and opioid intake. However, at this time, there were not enough studies with these outcomes to provide strong evidence for an effect of music interventions on these outcomes.
The anxiety‐reducing effects of music interventions found in this review are consistent with the findings of three other Cochrane systematic reviews on the use of music with mechanically‐ventilated patients (Bradt 2010b), cancer patients (Bradt 2011), and pre‐surgical patients (Bradt 2013).
Authors' conclusions
Implications for practice.
This systematic review indicates that listening to music may have a beneficial effect for people with coronary heart disease (CHD). The findings of this meta‐analysis indicate that listening to music may reduce systolic blood pressure and heart rate and also appears to be effective in reducing anxiety in people with myocardial infarction (MI) upon hospitalization. These results are consistent with the anxiety‐reducing benefits of music interventions reported in three other Cochrane systematic reviews on the use of music with mechanically‐ventilated patients (Bradt 2010b), cancer patients (Bradt 2011), and pre‐surgical patients (Bradt 2013).
All music medicine studies in this review used sedative music or music that is calming. However, there are many styles of sedative music (e.g. new age, classical, country and western, easy listening, etc.) and, at this time, it is unclear which type of music is most effective. The results of this review furthermore suggest a differential impact of researcher‐selected versus participant‐selected music on anxiety as well as physiological responses. The results suggest that more consistent yet smaller effects sizes are found in physiological responses when researcher‐selected music is used. However, for psychological outcomes such as anxiety, the findings suggest that patient‐preferred music leads to greater benefits.
Listening to music may also reduce pain and respiratory rate; however the magnitude of these effects is small. It may also improve patients' quality of sleep after a cardiac procedure or surgery. We found no evidence of effect for depression, heart rate variability, and length of hospitalization. Some studies suggested beneficial effects of music listening on opioid intake but this effect was not statistically significant. Inconsistent results were found for mood. However, only a small number of trials investigated the effects of music on these outcomes.
It is important to note that only three studies in this review used a trained music therapist. Music therapists in medical settings do not limit their interventions to offering music listening for relaxation purposes. Music therapists are specially trained clinically and academically to carefully select music interventions to offer emotional and spiritual support, enhance a sense of control, and improve physical well‐being in medical patients. Because of the lack of randomized controlled trials examining the effect of music interventions offered by a trained music therapist for people with coronary heart disease, it is impossible to establish at this time whether these interventions are more effective than listening to pre‐recorded music.
Implications for research.
This systematic review provides evidence that listening to pre‐recorded music may have health benefits for individuals with CHD. The use of other music therapy interventions, such as music improvisation, singing, listening to live music, songwriting, amongst others, with this population needs more research.
The effects of researcher‐selected versus participant‐selected music need to be further examined. In particular, studies are needed that use music that truly reflects participant preference. In addition, future trials should investigate the differential effects of researcher‐selected versus participant‐selected music. Future research needs to discuss in greater detail specific characteristics of the music selections. We recommend that researchers consult the reporting guidelines for music‐based interventions developed by Robb, Burns, and Carpenter (Robb 2011).
The music therapy literature recommends that when music is used for sedative purposes, patients select music that is characterized by a slow tempo and lack of abrupt changes and sharp timbres. In addition, music that evokes strong emotional reactions, which may be caused by intense memories associated to the music, should be avoided when used for stress and anxiety reduction purposes (Dileo 2007). These recommendations stem from the clinical experience and knowledge of music therapists as well as experimental research in the field of music psychology. More controlled trials are needed with medical patients to further examine which musical characteristics enhance the psychological and physiological benefits from music listening.
Besides music characteristics, more information is needed about dosages as well as timing of music interventions. Future studies need to examine the relationship between the frequency/duration of music interventions and treatment effects.
Are there optimal lengths of music interventions?
Do multiple sessions lead to better results?
For people with MI, are there preferred times during the day?
For procedural patients, what is the most effective time to start the music intervention?
Should the music intervention continue after completion of the procedure, and if so, for how long?
Does listening to music several days or weeks prior to it being used during the procedure impact outcomes differently than listening to the music for the first time just prior to or during the procedure?
Comparative studies are needed to answer many of these questions. Only one study in this review compared the effects of different dosages (once, twice, and three times per day) and timing (morning, afternoon, and evening) of music listening interventions in people with MI. In addition, future studies should examine the impact of patients' preferred coping strategies on the effectiveness of anxiety‐reduction interventions such as music listening. Some people may prefer distraction through music listening during a procedure, whereas others may prefer to closely monitor the procedure. None of the included studies considered preferred coping style as a possible confounding variable. The use of culturally relevant music needs to be considered when designing protocols for further research.
In addition, as recommended by Elliott 1994, patient personality traits (especially trait anxiety) and pre‐procedural state anxiety need to be considered as impact factors in future studies. People with high levels of trait and state anxiety may report differential benefits from music interventions than those with low levels of anxiety.
Furthermore, several authors recommend that future studies exert better control over the confounding effects of beta‐blockers, and other cardiac medication, as well as opioids (Cadigan 2001; Sendelbach 2006) on physiological and physical responses.
In summary, more studies are needed to examine the effects of music interventions on quality of life, mood, and depression in people with CHD, as these are factors relevant to the disease itself. In addition, future studies need to examine the effects of music on physiological responses beyond heart rate and blood pressure. Heart rate variability, myocardial oxygen demand, blood oxygen saturation levels, and stress hormone levels may provide more sensitive measures of effect and may, moreover, provide insight into the underlying physiology of anxiety and stress reduction. We also recommend that future studies consider duration of cardiac procedures as well as long‐term outcomes such as length of hospitalization and survival and death.
Few studies in this review included a power calculation. Future studies need to include power calculations so that sufficiently large samples are used.
Formal evaluation of the costs and benefits of music medicine and music therapy is needed.
What's new
Date | Event | Description |
---|---|---|
21 September 2021 | Amended | New studies have been identified with a recent search (24 October 2019) but the new information does not change the review's findings. The conclusions of this Cochrane Review are therefore still considered up to date. |
History
Protocol first published: Issue 3, 2007 Review first published: Issue 2, 2009
Date | Event | Description |
---|---|---|
23 July 2013 | New citation required but conclusions have not changed | 4 new included studies. Conclusion unchanged. New author added. |
25 February 2013 | New search has been performed | Searches updated November 2012. |
5 November 2012 | New search has been performed | Searches re‐run in November 2012. |
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank the Cochrane Heart Group editorial base for their excellent advice and support. We would also like to acknowledge Charla Thomas, graduate assistant, for her help in the handsearching of journals for the original review. Finally, we would like to express our gratitude to Ana Filipa Macedo, Chen Jing, and Farhad Shokraneh for their help with the translation of manuscripts.
Appendices
Appendix 1. Search strategies 2008
CENTRAL on The Cochrane Library
#1 MeSH descriptor Music this term only #2 MeSH descriptor Music Therapy this term only #3 music* in All Text #4 (#1 or #2 or #3) #5 MeSH descriptor Myocardial Ischemia explode all trees #6 MeSH descriptor Heart Diseases this term only #7 MeSH descriptor Myocardial Revascularization explode all trees #8 coronary in All Text #9 (heart in All Text near/6 disease in All Text) #10 angina in All Text #11 (heart in All Text near/6 infarct* in All Text) #12 (myocardial in All Text near/6 infarct* in All Text) #13 (coronary in All Text near/6 bypass* in All Text) #14 MeSH descriptor Cardiovascular Diseases this term only #15 cardiac in All Text #16 MeSH descriptor Cardiac Surgical Procedures explode all trees #17 MeSH descriptor Heart Function Tests explode all trees #18 cardiovascular next disease* in All Text #19 cabg in All Text #20 revasculari?ation in All Text #21 (coronary in All Text near/6 angiograph* in All Text) #22 (#5 or #6 or #7 or #8 or #9 or #10 or #11 or #12 or #13 or #14 or #15) #23 (#16 or #17 or #18 or #19 or #20 or #21) #24 (#22 or #23) #25 (#4 and #24)
MEDLINE
1 Music/ 2 Music Therapy/ 3 music$.tw. 4 or/1‐3 5 exp Myocardial Ischemia/ 6 Heart Diseases/ 7 exp Myocardial Revascularization/ 8 Cardiovascular Diseases/ 9 (coronary adj3 disease$).tw. 10 angina.tw. 11 (heart adj3 infarct$).tw. 12 (myocardial adj3 infarct$).tw. 13 (heart adj3 disease$).tw. 14 (coronary adj3 bypass$).tw. 15 exp Cardiac Surgical Procedures/ 16 exp Heart Function Tests/ 17 cardiac.tw. 18 or/5‐17 19 18 and 4 20 randomized controlled trial.pt. 21 controlled clinical trial.pt. 22 Randomized controlled trials/ 23 random allocation/ 24 double blind method/ 25 single‐blind method/ 26 or/20‐25 27 exp animal/ not human/ 28 26 not 27 29 clinical trial.pt. 30 exp Clinical trials/ 31 (clin$ adj25 trial$).ti,ab. 32 ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj (blind$ or mask$)).ti,ab. 33 placebos/ 34 placebo$.ti,ab. 35 random$.ti,ab. 36 research design/ 37 or/29‐36 38 37 not 27 39 38 not 28 40 comparative study/ 41 exp evaluation studies/ 42 follow up studies/ 43 prospective studies/ 44 (control$ or prospectiv$ or volunteer$).ti,ab. 45 or/40‐44 46 45 not 27 47 46 not (28 or 39) 48 28 or 39 or 47 49 19 and 48
EMBASE
1 music therapy/ 2 exp music/ 3 music$.tw. 4 or/1‐3 5 Cardiovascular Disease/ 6 exp heart surgery/ 7 exp Ischemic Heart Disease/ 8 Heart Disease/ 9 exp heart function test/ 10 (coronary adj3 disease$).tw. 11 angina.tw. 12 (heart adj3 infarct$).tw. 13 (myocardial adj3 infarct$).tw. 14 (heart adj3 disease$).tw. 15 (coronary adj3 bypass$).tw. 16 cardiac.tw. 17 or/5‐16 18 17 and 4 19 clinical trial/ 20 random$.tw. 21 randomized controlled trial/ 22 trial$.tw. 23 follow‐up.tw. 24 double blind procedure/ 25 placebo$.tw. 26 placebo/ 27 factorial$.ti,ab. 28 (crossover$ or cross‐over$).ti,ab. 29 (double$ adj blind$).ti,ab. 30 (singl$ adj blind$).ti,ab. 31 assign$.ti,ab. 32 allocat$.ti,ab. 33 volunteer$.ti,ab. 34 Crossover Procedure/ 35 Single Blind Procedure/ 36 or/19‐35 37 (exp animal/ or exp animal experiment/ or nonhuman/) not exp human/ 38 36 not 37 39 18 and 38
CINAHL
1music/ 2music therapy/ 3music$.tw. 4or/1‐3 5exp myocardial ischemia/ 6exp heart diseases/ 7exp myocardial revascularization/ 8cardiovascular diseases/ 9(coronary adj3 disease$).tw. 10angina.tw. 11(heart adj3 infarct$).tw. 12(myocardial adj3 infarct$).tw. 13(heart adj3 disease$).tw. 14(coronary adj3 bypass$).tw. 15exp Heart surgery/ 16exp Heart Function Tests/ 17cardiac.tw. 18or/5‐17 19(clin$ adj25 trial$).ti,ab. 20((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj (blind$ or mask$)).ti,ab. 21placebos/ 22placebo$.ti,ab. 23random$.ti,ab. 24(control$ or prospectiv$ or volunteer$).ti,ab. 25study design/ 26clinical trial.pt. 27exp clinical trial/ 28prospective studies/ 29comparative study/ 30exp evaluation studies/ 31Randomized controlled trials/ 32or/19‐31 33exp animal/ not human/ 3432 not 33 354 and 18 and 34
PsycINFO
1 Music/ 2 Music Therapy/ 3 music$.tw. 4 or/1‐3 5 exp myocardial infarction/ 6 exp heart diseases/ 7 angina pectoris/ 8 exp heart surgery/ 9 (coronary adj3 disease$).tw. 10 angina.tw. 11 (heart adj3 infarct$).tw. 12 (myocardial adj3 infarct$).tw. 13 (heart adj3 disease$).tw. 14 (coronary adj3 bypass$).tw. 15 cardiac.tw. 16 or/5‐15 17. empirical study.md 18 followup study.md 19 longitudinal study.md 20 prospective study.md 21 quantitative study.md 22 “2000”.md (is code for treatment outcome/randomized clinical trial) 23 treatment effectiveness evaluation/ 24 exp hypothesis testing/ 25 repeated measures/ 26 exp experimental design/ 27 placebo$.ti,ab 28 random$.ti,ab 29 (clin$ adj25 trial$).ti,ab. 30 ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj (blind$ or mask$)).ti,ab 31. or/19‐32 32 4 and 18 and 33 33 limit 32 to human
LILACS
1. Music$ [words] And 2. heart or cardiac or coronary or cabg or angina or cardiovascular or myocardial [words]
ISI Science Citation Index
#32 #31 AND #17 AND #4 #31 #30 OR #29 OR #28 OR #27 OR #26 OR #25 OR #24 OR #23 OR #22 OR #21 OR #20 OR #19 OR #18 #30 TS=(control$ or prospectiv$ or volunteer$) #29 TS=(prospective studies) #28 TS=(follow up studies) #27 TS=(evaluation studies) #26 TS=(comparative study) #25 TS=random$ #24 TS=placebo$ #23 TS=(Clinical trial$) #22 TS=(single‐blind method$) #21 TS=(double blind method$) #20 TS=(randomized controlled trial$) #19 TS=(controlled clinical trial$) #18 TS=(random allocation) #17 #16 OR #15 OR #14 OR #13 OR #12 OR #11 OR #10 OR #9 OR #8 OR #7 OR #6 OR #5 #16 TS=cardiac #15 TS=(Heart Function Test$) #14 TS=(Cardiac Surgical Procedures) #13 TS=(coronary bypass) #12 TS=(Myocardial infarct$) #11 TS=(heart infarct$) #10 TS=angina #9 TS=(coronary diseas*) #8 TS=(Cardiovascular Disease*) #7 TS=(Myocardial Revascularization) #6 TS=(Heart Disease*) #5 TS=(Myocardial Ischemia) #4 #1 OR #2 OR #3 #3 TS=(singing OR song) #2 TS=music* #1 TS=(music therapy) DocType=All document types; Language=All languages; Databases=SCI‐EXPANDED, SSCI, A&HCI; Timespan=1974‐2008
Specialist Music Therapy Research Database
The site's research register, dissertation archive, and bibliography were searched in 2007 for the following terms: "cardiac OR cardiovascular OR myocardial OR angina OR coronary OR heart OR CABG". This database is no longer functional.
CAIRSS
1. Cardiac OR (myocardial Ischemia) OR (heart diseas?) 2. Coronary OR Angina OR (heart infarct) 3. (cardiovascular diseas?) OR coronary bypass OR(cardiovascular surgical procedures) 4. cardiovascular OR CABG or revascularization
Proquest Digital Dissertations
Music AND Myocardial Ischemia Music AND Heart Disease* Music AND Myocardial Music AND coronary Music AND heart W/6 disease Music AND angina Music AND heart W/6 infarct* Music AND myocardial W/6 infarct* Music AND Cardiovascular Disease* Music AND cardiac Music AND Heart Function Tests Music AND cardiovascular W/3 disease* Music AND cabg Music AND revascularization
National Research Register
1. Music 2. (music near therapy) 3. 1 OR 2 4. (cardiac OR cardiovascular OR myocardial OR angina OR coronary) 5. (CABG or heart) 6. 4 OR 5 7. 3 AND 6
Current Controlled Trials and ClinicalTrials.gov
1. Music or "music therapy"
Appendix 2. Search strategies 2012
CENTRAL on The Cochrane Library (issue 10, 2012)
#1 MeSH descriptor Music this term only #2 MeSH descriptor Music Therapy this term only #3 music* in All Text #4 (#1 or #2 or #3) #5 MeSH descriptor Myocardial Ischemia explode all trees #6 MeSH descriptor Heart Diseases this term only #7 MeSH descriptor Myocardial Revascularization explode all trees #8 coronary in All Text #9 (heart in All Text near/6 disease in All Text) #10 angina in All Text #11 (heart in All Text near/6 infarct* in All Text) #12 (myocardial in All Text near/6 infarct* in All Text) #13 (coronary in All Text near/6 bypass* in All Text) #14 MeSH descriptor Cardiovascular Diseases this term only #15 cardiac in All Text #16 MeSH descriptor Cardiac Surgical Procedures explode all trees #17 MeSH descriptor Heart Function Tests explode all trees #18 cardiovascular next disease* in All Text #19 cabg in All Text #20 revasculari?ation in All Text #21 (coronary in All Text near/6 angiograph* in All Text) #22 (#5 or #6 or #7 or #8 or #9 or #10 or #11 or #12 or #13 or #14 or #15) #23 (#16 or #17 or #18 or #19 or #20 or #21) #24 (#22 or #23) #25 (#4 and #24)
MEDLINE (OvidSP)
1 Music/ 2 Music Therapy/ 3 music$.tw. 4 or/1‐3 5 exp Myocardial Ischemia/ 6 Heart Diseases/ 7 exp Myocardial Revascularization/ 8 Cardiovascular Diseases/ 9 (coronary adj3 disease$).tw. 10 angina.tw. 11 (heart adj3 infarct$).tw. 12 (myocardial adj3 infarct$).tw. 13 (heart adj3 disease$).tw. 14 (coronary adj3 bypass$).tw. 15 exp Cardiac Surgical Procedures/ 16 exp Heart Function Tests/ 17 cardiac.tw. 18 or/5‐17 19 18 and 4 20 randomized controlled trial.pt. 21 controlled clinical trial.pt. 22 Randomized controlled trials/ 23 random allocation/ 24 double blind method/ 25 single‐blind method/ 26 or/20‐25 27 exp animal/ not human/ 28 26 not 27 29 clinical trial.pt. 30 exp Clinical trials/ 31 (clin$ adj25 trial$).ti,ab. 32 ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj (blind$ or mask$)).ti,ab. 33 placebos/ 34 placebo$.ti,ab. 35 random$.ti,ab. 36 research design/ 37 or/29‐36 38 37 not 27 39 38 not 28 40 comparative study/ 41 exp evaluation studies/ 42 follow up studies/ 43 prospective studies/ 44 (control$ or prospectiv$ or volunteer$).ti,ab. 45 or/40‐44 46 45 not 27 47 46 not (28 or 39) 48 28 or 39 or 47 49 19 and 48
50.limit 49 to ed=20080612‐20121105
EMBASE (OvidSP)
1 music therapy/ 2 exp music/ 3 music$.tw. 4 or/1‐3 5 Cardiovascular Disease/ 6 exp heart surgery/ 7 exp Ischemic Heart Disease/ 8 Heart Disease/ 9 exp heart function test/ 10 (coronary adj3 disease$).tw. 11 angina.tw. 12 (heart adj3 infarct$).tw. 13 (myocardial adj3 infarct$).tw. 14 (heart adj3 disease$).tw. 15 (coronary adj3 bypass$).tw. 16 cardiac.tw. 17 or/5‐16 18 17 and 4 19 clinical trial/ 20 random$.tw. 21 randomized controlled trial/ 22 trial$.tw. 23 follow‐up.tw. 24 double blind procedure/ 25 placebo$.tw. 26 placebo/ 27 factorial$.ti,ab. 28 (crossover$ or cross‐over$).ti,ab. 29 (double$ adj blind$).ti,ab. 30 (singl$ adj blind$).ti,ab. 31 assign$.ti,ab. 32 allocat$.ti,ab. 33 volunteer$.ti,ab. 34 Crossover Procedure/ 35 Single Blind Procedure/ 36 or/19‐35 37 (exp animal/ or exp animal experiment/ or nonhuman/) not exp human/ 38 36 not 37 39 18 and 38
40. limit 39 to ew=200805$ to 20121109
CINAHL (OvidSp)
1 music/ 2 music therapy/ 3 music$.tw. 4 or/1‐3 5 exp myocardial ischemia/ 6 exp heart diseases/ 7 exp myocardial revascularization/ 8 cardiovascular diseases/ 9 (coronary adj3 disease$).tw. 10 angina.tw. 11 (heart adj3 infarct$).tw. 12 (myocardial adj3 infarct$).tw. 13 (heart adj3 disease$).tw. 14 (coronary adj3 bypass$).tw. 15 exp Heart surgery/ 16 exp Heart Function Tests/ 17 cardiac.tw. 18 or/5‐17 19 (clin$ adj25 trial$).ti,ab. 20 ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj (blind$ or mask$)).ti,ab. 21 placebos/ 22 placebo$.ti,ab. 23 random$.ti,ab. 24 (control$ or prospectiv$ or volunteer$).ti,ab. 25 study design/ 26 clinical trial.pt. 27 exp clinical trial/ 28 prospective studies/ 29 comparative study/ 30 exp evaluation studies/ 31 Randomized controlled trials/ 32 or/19‐31 33 exp animal/ not human/ 34 32 not 33 35 4 and 18 and 34
36 limit 36 to ew=200805$ to 20121109
PsycINFO (OvidSP)
1 Music/ 2 Music Therapy/ 3 music$.tw. 4 or/1‐3 5 exp myocardial infarction/ 6 exp heart diseases/ 7 angina pectoris/ 8 exp heart surgery/ 9 (coronary adj3 disease$).tw. 10 angina.tw. 11 (heart adj3 infarct$).tw. 12 (myocardial adj3 infarct$).tw. 13 (heart adj3 disease$).tw. 14 (coronary adj3 bypass$).tw. 15 cardiac.tw. 16 or/5‐15 17. empirical study.md 18 followup study.md 19 longitudinal study.md 20 prospective study.md 21 quantitative study.md 22 “2000”.md (is code for treatment outcome/randomized clinical trial) 23 treatment effectiveness evaluation/ 24 exp hypothesis testing/ 25 repeated measures/ 26 exp experimental design/ 27 placebo$.ti,ab 28 random$.ti,ab 29 (clin$ adj25 trial$).ti,ab. 30 ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj (blind$ or mask$)).ti,ab 31. or/19‐32 32 4 and 18 and 31 33 limit 32 to human
34. limit 33 to yr="2008 ‐ 2012"
LILACS (Virtual Health Library)
1. Music$ [words] And 2. heart or cardiac or coronary or cabg or angina or cardiovascular or myocardial [words]
(this database does not have the capacity to apply date limits. Results outputs were reviewed from 2008 onward)
Social Science Citation Index (ISI)
#33 Timespan=2008‐2012
#32 #31 AND #17 AND #4 #31 #30 OR #29 OR #28 OR #27 OR #26 OR #25 OR #24 OR #23 OR #22 OR #21 OR #20 OR #19 OR #18 #30 TS=(control$ or prospectiv$ or volunteer$) #29 TS=(prospective studies) #28 TS=(follow up studies) #27 TS=(evaluation studies) #26 TS=(comparative study) #25 TS=random$ #24 TS=placebo$ #23 TS=(Clinical trial$) #22 TS=(single‐blind method$) #21 TS=(double blind method$) #20 TS=(randomized controlled trial$) #19 TS=(controlled clinical trial$) #18 TS=(random allocation) #17 #16 OR #15 OR #14 OR #13 OR #12 OR #11 OR #10 OR #9 OR #8 OR #7 OR #6 OR #5 #16 TS=cardiac #15 TS=(Heart Function Test$) #14 TS=(Cardiac Surgical Procedures) #13 TS=(coronary bypass) #12 TS=(Myocardial infarct$) #11 TS=(heart infarct$) #10 TS=angina #9 TS=(coronary diseas*) #8 TS=(Cardiovascular Disease*) #7 TS=(Myocardial Revascularization) #6 TS=(Heart Disease*) #5 TS=(Myocardial Ischemia) #4 #1 OR #2 OR #3 #3 TS=(singing OR song) #2 TS=music* #1 TS=(music therapy)
Specialist Music Therapy Research Database
This database is no longer functional therefore this search was not included in the update.
CAIRSS (Webvoyage)
1. Cardiac OR (myocardial Ischemia) OR (heart diseas?) 2. Coronary OR Angina OR (heart infarct) 3. (cardiovascular diseas?) OR coronary bypass OR(cardiovascular surgical procedures) 4. cardiovascular OR CABG or revascularization
(this database does not have the capacity to apply date limits. Results outputs were reviewed from 2008 onward)
Proquest Digital Dissertations
Music AND Myocardial Ischemia Music AND Heart Disease* Music AND Myocardial Music AND coronary Music AND heart W/6 disease Music AND angina Music AND heart W/6 infarct* Music AND myocardial W/6 infarct* Music AND Cardiovascular Disease* Music AND cardiac Music AND Heart Function Tests Music AND cardiovascular W/3 disease* Music AND cabg Music AND revascularization
Search period limited to 2008 ‐ 2012
Current Controlled Trials and ClinicalTrials.gov
1. Music or "music therapy"
Appendix 3. Journals Handsearched
Australian Journal of Music Therapy (1990 ‐ 2012) Canadian Journal of Music Therapy (1976 ‐ 2012) International Journal of the Arts in Medicine (1993 ‐ 1999, no longer published after 1999) Journal of Music Therapy (1964 ‐ 2012) Musik‐,Tanz‐, und Kunsttherapie (1999 ‐ 2012) Musiktherapeutische Umschau (1980 ‐ 2012) Music Therapy (1981 ‐ 1996, no longer published after 1996) Music Therapy Perspectives (1982 ‐ 2012) Nordic Journal of Music Therapy (1992 ‐ 2012) Music Therapy Today (online journal of music therapy) (2001 ‐ 2007, no longer maintained) Voices (online international journal of music therapy) (2001 ‐ 2012) Arts in Psychotherapy (1983 ‐ 2012) International Latin‐American Journal of Music Therapy (1995 ‐ 2000, no longer published after 2000)
Data and analyses
Comparison 1. Music versus standard care.
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
1.1 Psychological distress | 5 | 228 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐1.26 [‐2.30, ‐0.22] |
1.2 Anxiety (all measures) ‐ patient type | 10 | 353 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.70 [‐1.17, ‐0.22] |
1.2.1 anxiety (all measures) (MI) | 4 | 143 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.94 [‐1.95, 0.06] |
1.2.2 anxiety (all measures)(surgical/procedural) | 4 | 171 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.63 [‐1.25, ‐0.01] |
1.2.3 anxiety (all measures)(rehabilitation) | 2 | 39 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.38 [‐1.60, 0.83] |
1.3 Anxiety (all measures) ‐ music preference | 9 | 323 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.79 [‐1.29, ‐0.29] |
1.3.1 Anxiety (all measures) ‐ partcipant‐selected | 4 | 144 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.89 [‐1.42, ‐0.36] |
1.3.2 Anxiety (all measures) ‐ researcher‐selected | 5 | 179 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.74 [‐1.55, 0.08] |
1.4 State anxiety (STAI) ‐ patient type | 7 | 310 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐4.58 [‐7.78, ‐1.39] |
1.4.1 State anxiety (STAI) ‐ MI | 6 | 243 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐5.87 [‐7.99, ‐3.75] |
1.4.2 State anxiety (STAI) ‐ surgical/procedural | 1 | 67 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.40 [‐1.33, 2.13] |
1.5 State Anxiety (STAI) ‐ music preference | 7 | 310 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐4.58 [‐7.78, ‐1.39] |
1.5.1 State Anxiety (STAI) ‐ participant‐preferred | 3 | 167 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐4.71 [‐10.76, 1.33] |
1.5.2 State Anxiety (STAI) ‐ researcher‐selected | 4 | 143 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐4.68 [‐8.27, ‐1.10] |
1.6 State Anxiety (STAI) ‐ music preference MI only | 6 | 243 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐5.87 [‐7.99, ‐3.75] |
1.6.1 State Anxiety (STAI) ‐ participant‐preferred | 2 | 100 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐7.36 [‐9.45, ‐5.27] |
1.6.2 State Anxiety (STAI) ‐ researcher‐selected | 4 | 143 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐4.68 [‐8.27, ‐1.10] |
1.7 Anxiety (non‐STAI)‐patient type | 7 | 248 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.43 [‐0.93, 0.06] |
1.7.1 Anxiety (surgical/procedural) | 4 | 171 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.63 [‐1.25, ‐0.01] |
1.7.2 Anxiety (MI and rehabilitation) | 3 | 77 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.03 [‐0.61, 0.56] |
1.8 Anxiety (non‐STAI) ‐ music preference | 7 | 248 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.43 [‐0.93, 0.06] |
1.8.1 Anxiety (non‐STAI) ‐ participant‐preferred | 4 | 144 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.89 [‐1.42, ‐0.36] |
1.8.2 Anxiety (non‐STAI) ‐ researcher‐selected | 3 | 104 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.11 [‐0.28, 0.49] |
1.9 Depression | 6 | 217 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐0.11 [‐0.38, 0.16] |
1.10 Mood | 2 | 97 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 1.08 [‐0.02, 2.17] |
1.11 Heart rate‐patient type | 13 | 828 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐3.40 [‐6.12, ‐0.69] |
1.11.1 heart rate (surg ical/procedural) | 7 | 604 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐2.61 [‐5.62, 0.39] |
1.11.2 Heart rate (MI) | 5 | 194 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐4.75 [‐9.26, ‐0.25] |
1.11.3 Heart rate (rehab) | 1 | 30 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 4.50 [‐9.68, 18.68] |
1.12 Heart rate ‐ music preference | 13 | 828 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐3.62 [‐6.28, ‐0.95] |
1.12.1 Heart rate ‐ participant‐selected music | 7 | 430 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐4.69 [‐9.40, 0.02] |
1.12.2 Heart rate ‐ researcher‐selected music | 6 | 398 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐2.67 [‐4.27, ‐1.07] |
1.13 Heart rate variability | 2 | 90 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.07 [‐0.34, 0.48] |
1.14 Respiratory rate ‐ music preference | 7 | 442 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐2.50 [‐3.61, ‐1.39] |
1.14.1 Respiratory Rate ‐ participant‐selected | 3 | 186 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐4.42 [‐7.37, ‐1.46] |
1.14.2 Respiratory Rate ‐ researcher‐selected | 4 | 256 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐1.66 [‐2.20, ‐1.12] |
1.15 Systolic blood pressure | 11 | 775 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐5.52 [‐7.43, ‐3.60] |
1.16 Diastolic blood pressure | 9 | 685 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐1.12 [‐2.57, 0.34] |
1.17 Mean A rterial Pressure | 3 | 158 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐0.91 [‐4.08, 2.26] |
1.18 Oxygen Saturation | 3 | 184 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.02 [‐1.65, 1.61] |
1.19 Pain | 8 | 630 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.43 [‐0.80, ‐0.05] |
1.19.1 One music session | 5 | 420 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.55 [‐1.16, 0.07] |
1.19.2 Two or more music sessions | 3 | 210 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.27 [‐0.55, ‐0.00] |
1.20 Length of hospital stay | 2 | 82 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐0.06 [‐1.03, 0.92] |
1.21 Opioid intake | 2 | 90 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐0.25 [‐0.67, 0.16] |
1.22 Quality of sleep | 2 | 122 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.91 [0.03, 1.79] |
Characteristics of studies
Characteristics of included studies [ordered by study ID]
Barnason 1995.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 3‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adult patients after CABG
Total N randomized: not reported N randomized to music group: not reported N randomized to control group: not reported N randomized to music video (visual imaging): not reported (not included in this review) N analyzed in music group: 33 N analyzed in control group: 34 N analyzed in music video group: 29 (not included in this review) Sex: 31 (32%) women, 65 (68%) men Age: 67 (SD 9.9) Ethnicity: 100% white Setting: inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Three study groups: 1. Music group: listening to music through earphones 2. Control group: scheduled rest Music provided: (a) Country Western instrumental, (b) Fresh Aire by Steamroller, (c) Winter into Spring by Winston, (d) Prelude and Comfort Zone by Halpern Number of sessions: 2 Length of session: 30 minutes Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (STAI): posttest scores postop day 2, posttest scores postop day 3
Anxiety (NRS): posttest scores postop day 2, posttest scores postop day 3
Mood (NRS): posttest scores postop day 2, posttest scores postop day 3
Pain (VRS): posttest scores postop day 2, posttestscores postop day 3
Pain (MPQ): posttest scores postop day 2, posttest scores postop day 3
Quality of sleep (Richards‐Campbell Sleep Questionnaire, average of 5 subscales): morning of postop day 3 Unable to use: HR, SBP, DBP: insufficient data |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: " The researcher randomly assigned subjects to one of the three intervention groups by drawing lots." (p.126) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Unclear risk | It is unclear whether outcome assessors were blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Partially supported by a grant from the American Heart Association‐Nebraska Affiliate |
Barnes 1987.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Quasi‐RCT Cross‐over trial |
|
Participants | 10 adults in cardiac rehabilitation program Total N randomized: 10 N randomized to music first sequence: 5 N randomized to control first sequence: 5 N analyzed music first sequence: 5 N analyzed control first sequence: 5 Sex: 4 (40%) women, 6 (60%) men Age: 56.4 Ethnicity: not reported Setting: outpatient rehab Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two conditions: 1. Music condition: exercise on bicycle ergometer while listening to prerecorded music 2. Control condition: exercise on bicycle ergometer without music Music provided: participants selected from Jazz: selections from Fun and Games (C. Mangione); Classical: Brandengurg concertos Nos 2 and 6 (JS Bach); Country Western: selections from Greatest Hits (Kenny Rogers); Popular: selections from Unforgettable (Nat King Cole) or The best of the Supremes (The Supremes). Number of sessions: 3 in each condition Length of session: 10 minutes Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Perceived exertion (Borg Scale of Rating of Perceived Exertion) Unable to use: HR, SBP: insufficient data reporting |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Quote: "The participant was alternately assigned to either group A or B" (p.35). |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Allocation concealment was not possible because of alternate assignment |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measure was used for subjective outcome. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Low risk | Not included in this review |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No subject loss |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Unfunded research |
Blankfield 1995.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Quasi‐randomized trial 3‐arm parallel group design |
|
Participants | Adults undergoing CABG Total N randomized: 66 N randomized to music group: 33 N randomized to control group: 33 N analyzed in music group: 32 N analyzed in control group: 29 Mean age: 61.93 (SD 6.61) years Sex: 18 (28%) women, 48 (72%) men Ethnicity: 57 (94%) white Setting: 2 inpatient settings in hospitals Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: participants listened to audiocassette tapes intraoperatively and postoperatively via headphones 2. Control group: listened to blank cassette tape during surgery to keep surgeon blinded with no postoperative Music provided: Dreamflight II by Herb Ernst (no further info about the music was provided in the study report) Number of sessions: Once during surgery and then twice daily for duration of hospitalization Length of sessions: Duration of surgery and then 30 minutes Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Postoperative stay (days): mean (SD) Surgical intensive care unit stay (days): mean (SD) Morphine and morphine equivalents: mean (SD) Meperidine: mean (SD) Depression: not included in this review since no standardized measurement tool was used |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomization method is not reported |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Use of blank tapes in the control group blinded the surgeon and medical staff |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Subjective outcomes are not included in this review since no standardized measures were used |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Unclear risk | It is unclear whether outcome assessors were blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Attrition rate is 7.6% (n = 5). 3 participants were excluded because their hospitalization stay extended beyond the 2‐week study duration and the authors considered them outliers. 2 participants died in the hospital and their data were excluded. The reasons for exclusion are questionable and therefore the study was considered at high risk for attrition bias. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Supported by a grant from the American Academy of Family Physicians along with financial assistance from Fairview General Hospital. |
Bolwerk 1990.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adults with medical diagnosis of myocardial infarction (MI) confirmed by enzyme and ECG changes Total N randomized: 40 N randomized to music group: 20 N randomized to control group: 20 N analyzed in music group: 17 N analyzed in control group:18 Sex: 8 (32%) women, 17 (68%) men Ethnicity: not reported Mean age: 58.65 years Setting: Inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to relaxing researcher‐selected music 2. Control group standard care Music provided: compilation tape of (a) Largo by Bach, (b) Largo by Beethoven, (c) Prelude to the afternoon of a Faun by Debussy Number of sessions: 3 sessions on 3 consecutive days Length of session: 22 minutes Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (STAI): posttest scores after the final session | |
Notes | Some participants stated that they didn't care for the music; 2 would have liked different music | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "Control was enhanced in the study by random assignment of subjects to two groups‐experimental and control‐using a table of random numbers" (p.67) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of the participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Low risk | No objective outcomes were used. |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition rate: n = 5 (12.5%) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | No report of funding |
Broscious 1999.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 3‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adult patients during chest tube removal (CTR)
Total N randomized: 156 N randomized to music group: 70 N randomized to control group: 50 N randomized white noise group: 36 (not included in this review) N analyzed in music group: 62 N analyzed in control group: 44 N analyzed in white noise group: 36 (not included in this review) Sex: 35 (29%) women, 85 (71%) men Mean age: 66.35 (SD 9.7) years Ethnicity: 97% white Setting: Inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Three study groups: 1. Music group: listening to self‐selected music through earphones 2. Control group: standard care 3. White noise group; not used in this review Music provided: (a) Big Band, (b) Blues, (c) Classical, (d) Country & Western, (e) Easy Listening, (f) Gospel, (g) Movie musicals, (h) New Age, (i) Patriotic, (j) Rock Number of sessions: 1 Length of session: 10 minutes before procedure and throughout duration of procedure Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Pain (NRS): posttest scores immediately following CTR HR, SBP, DBP: posttest scores immediately following CTR | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: " Subjects were randomly assigned to groups by the primary investigator or research assistant who blindly drew a chip from a box containing 3 chips. The chips were labelled "C" for control group, "N" for noise group, and "M" for music group." (p.411) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: " Subjects were randomly assigned to groups by the primary investigator or research assistant who blindly drew a chip from a box containing 3 chips." |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were blinded:"the physician assistant or cardiovascular technician removing the chest tubes did not know which tape the patient was listening to." |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Unclear risk | It is unclear whether outcome assessors were blinded. |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition rate: n = 14 (11.6%). Reason for withdrawal is not reported. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Supported by a grant from the Epsilon Chi Chapter of Sigma Theta Tau International. |
Cadigan 2001.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adult cardiac patients with either intravascular sheets or an intra‐aortic balloon pump (IABP) in place.
Total N randomized: 140 N randomized to music group: 75 N randomized to control group: 65 N analyzed in music group: 75 N analyzed in control group: 65 Sex: 40 (29%) women, 100 (71%) men Mean age: 62.25 (SD 12.7) years Ethnicity: not reported Setting: Inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to researcher‐selected music through headphones 2. Control group: standard care Music provided: a mixture of symphonic music and nature sounds Number of sessions: 1 Length of session: 30 min Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Psychological distress (POMS‐Short Form): posttest scores HR, SBP, DBP: posttest scores Pain (VAS): posttest scores Peripheral skin temperature:posttest scores | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "Patients were randomized to either the treatment or control group by means of a table of random numbers."(p.8) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants were not possible. Personnel were not blinded. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Unclear risk | It is unclear whether outcome assessors were blinded. |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition rate: n = 10 (7%). Data on all randomized participants were obtained for physiological data but there was a loss of 10 subjects for the POMS data. No reason was reported. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Supported by Eta Tau Chapter at Salem State College and Alpha Chi Chapter of Sigma Theta Tau International |
Chan 2007.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adults with diagnosis of MI, acute coronary syndrome (ACS), or coronary artery disease (CAD), undergoing C‐clamp procedure after percutaneous coronary intervention
Total N randomized: 70 N randomized to music group: 35 N randomized to control group: 35 N analyzed in music group: 31 N analyzed in control group: 35 Mean age: no means given Sex: 18 (27%) women, 48 (73%) men Ethnicity: not reported Setting: inpatient Country: Hong Kong |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to self‐selected music during procedure through earphones 2. Control group: standard care Music provided: Western and Chinese slow, soft music without lyrics Number of sessions: 1 Length of session: approx. 45 mins Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | HR, RR, SBP, DBP, oxygen saturation (O₂‐sat): measured every 15 minutes; measurement at 45 minutes used for this review Pain (NRS): posttest | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: " Participants were included in the study and, using a random digit generated by research randomizer, they were randomized into the music group or control group."(p.673) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Quote: "No method for concealment of allocation was used" (personal communication with author) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding was not possible for the participants. Personnel were not blinded. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | High risk | Quote:"The intervention and data collection were carried out by the same researcher" |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition rate: n = 4 (5.7%). 4 participants refused to continue. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | No reported funding |
Cohen 1999.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adults with MI
Total N randomized: 40 N randomized to music group: 20 N randomized to control group: 20 N analyzed in music group: 20 N analyzed in control group: 20 Mean age: 67.8 (SD 13.9) years Sex: 17 (43%) women, 23 (57%) men Ethnicity: 38 (95%) white, 2 (5%) other Setting: inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to self‐selected music 2. Control group: standard care Music provided: (a) New Age, (b) music from decades past, (c) contemporary solo instrumentalists, (d) religious, (e) classical Number of sessions: 1 Length of session: 30 min Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (STAI): change scores HR, mean arterial pressure (MAP), SBP, DBP: change scores | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "Subjects were then randomly assigned to the experimental or control groups by the researcher who drew from a box containing 20 slips of paper with "music" and 20 slips of paper with "rest" written on them" (p.66) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Allocation concealment was ensured by draw of lots method |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participant was not possible. Personnel were not blinded. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | High risk | Outcome assessors were not blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No loss of subjects |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Unfunded research study |
Cutshall 2011.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group |
|
Participants | Adults undergoing first‐time CABG or cardiac valve surgery Total N randomized: 173 N randomized to music group: 86 N randomized to control group: 87 N analyzed in music group: 49 N analyzed in control group: 51 Mean age: 62.9 (SD 12.65) years Sex: 23 (23%) women, 77 (77%) men Ethnicity: not reported Setting: inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to prerecorded music combined with nature sounds 2. Control group: 20 minutes of bed rest Music provided: participants were given the choice of four selections of music and nature sounds Number of sessions: 6 Duration of each session: 20 minutes Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Pain (VAS): change scores Anxiety (VAS): change scores Satisfaction (VAS): change scores Relaxation (VAS): change scores HR, SBP, DBP: change scores |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "Stratification for randomization was based on a pain level of 4 or less (the institutional pain level goal) or greater than 4. The randomization was blocked to ensure balanced allocation throughout the course of the study. There were 25 randomized blocks of 4 patients and 25 randomized blocks of 2 patients. Each set of 50 blocks was changed into a random order as well." (p.17) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "The use of cards in sealed envelopes prevented the study coordinator who was enrolling patients from knowing to which group each patient was randomly assigned." (p.17) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Quote: "The study coordinator read to the patient a printed script and obtained measurements of pain, anxiety, satisfaction, and relaxation orally with a visual analog scale (VAS)." (p.18) |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | High risk | Study coordinator obtained the measurements |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Attrition rate = 73 (42%). Reasons for withdrawal were not reported. The report states that recruitment continued until 100 participants had completed all six sessions |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | The prerecorded music used in this study was donated by Ambience Medical and the study was funded in part by Richer J and Sharon M Mrocek. |
Davis‐Rollans 1987.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT Cross‐over trial | |
Participants | Adults in coronary care unit (CCU) with diagnosis of MI or other cardiac condition
Total N randomized: not reported Total N analyzed: 24 Mean age: 62 years Sex: 5 (21%) women, 19 (79%) men Ethnicity: not reported Setting: Inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to researcher‐selected music through headphones 2. Congrol group: background CCU noise as heard through silent headphones Music provided: compilation tape of Symphony No. 6, first movement (Beethoven), Eine Kleine Nachtmusik, first and fourth movements (Mozart), and The Moldau (Smetana) Number of sessions: 1 Length of session: 37 min Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | HR: during session
Number of arrhythmias Mood change: not used in this review due to insufficient data RR: not used in this review due to insufficient data |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "With the use of Latin square design, the three musical selections (A, B, C) were randomly assigned to be presented to the patients in one of three different orders: A, B, C; C, A, B: and B, C, A." (p.372) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | Low risk | Data for one subjective outcome were obtained but not used in this review because insufficient |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Low risk | Outcome assessor was blinded. Control group participants wore a headset |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | No report of funding |
Elliott 1994.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adults with unstable angina pectoris or acute MI
Total N randomized: unclear N randomized to music group: unclear N randomized to control group: unclear N randomized to muscle relaxation group: unclear (not included in this review) N analyzed in music group:19 N analyzed in control group:19 N analyzed in muscle relaxation group: 18 (not included in this review) Mean age: 60.6 years Sex: 16 (29%) women, 40 (71%) men for total sample (including muscle relaxation group) Ethnicity: all participants were Australian but no further information is provided Setting: Inpatient Country: Australia |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to researcher‐selected music via earphones 2. Control group: standard care. Music provided: light classical music relaxation tape designed by Bonny. Number of sessions: 2 or 3 Length of session: 30 min. Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (STAI): posttest Anxiety (LAAS): posttest Depression (HADS D‐subscale): posttestHR, SBP, DBP: not used in this review because for many participants measurements were only taken 2 ‐ 3 hrs after the intervention. | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Table of random numbers (personal communication with author) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Serially numbered opaque envelopes (personal communication with author) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | High risk | Outcome assessors were not blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Supported in part by a Cumberland College Research Grant |
Emery 2003.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT Cross‐over trial | |
Participants | Adults with CAD enrolled in standard university‐based 12‐week Phase II CR program.
Total N randomized: 33 Total N analyzed: 30 Mean age: 62.6 (SD 10.5) years Sex: 14 (42%) women, 19 (58%) men Ethnicity: 27 (93%) white, 2 (7%) African‐American Setting: outpatient university‐based CR program Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to researcher‐selected music through earphones 2. Congrol group: listening to a blank tape through earphones Music used: Four Seasons (Vivaldi) Number of sessions: 1 music listening and 1 blank tape Length of session: as long as possible for the participant Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (POMS‐SF, tension subscale): posttest Depression (POMS‐SF, depression subscale): posttest HR, SBP, DBP: peak exercise Cognitive function (verbal fluency test): posttest Exercise time (mins) | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | List of random numbers (personal communication with author) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Recruiters were concealed to random sequence (personal communication with author) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Low risk | Outcome assessors were blinded (personal communication with author) |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition rate: n = 3 (9%) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Supported in part by a grant from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (HL45290). |
Hermele 2005.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 3‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adult patients during CABG
Total N randomized: 117 consented but only 63 were randomized (47 did not complete baseline and 7 did not have CABG) N randomized to music group: 21 assumed (not reported) N randomized to control group: 21 assumed (not reported) N randomized to guided imagery group: 21 assumed (not reported) N analyzed in music group: 17 N analyzed in control group: 19 N analyzed in guided imagery: 20 (not included in this review) Mean age: none reported Sex: 17 (30%) women, 39 (70%) men for total sample (including guided imagery group) Ethnicity: 51 (91.1%) white, 1 (1.8%) African‐American, 3 (5.4%) Hispanic, 1 (1.8%) Asian Setting: inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to researcher‐selected music 2. Control group: standard care Music provided: no specifications Number of sessions: daily for one week prior to CABG, during the procedure Length of session: determined by the participant Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (HADS, anxiety scale): 1 week postop Depression (HADS, depression scale): 1 week postoperatively Mood disturbance (POMS): 1 week postoperatively | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Drawing of lots (personal communication with author) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | None used (personal communication with author) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Low risk | No objective outcomes were included in this study |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition rate: N = 6 (9.5%). 6 participants did not complete posttest. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Unfunded research study |
Jafari 2012.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design |
|
Participants | Adults who had undergone first‐time CABG and valvular surgery Total N randomized: 60 N randomized to music group: 30 N randomized to control group: 30 N analyzed in music group: 30 N analyzed in control group: 30 Mean age: 57.83 (SD 10.62) years Sex: 30 (50%) women, 30 (50%) men Ethnicity: not reported Setting: Inpatient Country: Iran |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: participants listened to one pre‐recorded selection of music 2. Control group: participants were provided headphones with no music Music provided: participants selected their music after listening to one‐minute previews of each music option. Relaxation music pieces were selected with consideration for the cultural conditions of the society and the type of recommended music in the literature, i.e. with a tempo of 60 ‐ 80 beats (or even less) per minute Number of sessions: 1 Length of sessions: 30 minutes Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Pain (0 ‐ 10 NRS): immediately postintervention (used in this review), 30 min after intervention, 1 hr after intervention | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Drawing of lots (personal communication with author) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Drawing of lots concealed allocation |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self reports were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Low risk | This study did not address objective outcomes |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition rate: 0 (0%) Quote: "All randomized patients were included in the analysis and there were no drop outs" (p.3) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Funding for this study was provided by the Research Deputy and Student Research Committee of Mazandaran University of Medical Sciences |
Leist 2011.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design |
|
Participants | Adults who had a heart attack or a heart condition or both, including coronary heart disease, angina, valve disease, or arrhythmia, and had a heart procedure or surgery, including CABG, surgery or percutaneous transluminal coronary angioplasty (PTCA) with stenting, a valve replacement, or placement of a pacemaker or implantable cardioverter defibrillator (ICD) Total N randomized: 10 N randomized in music therapy group: 5 N randomized on control group: 5 N analyzed in music therapy group: 4 N analyzed in control group: 5 Mean age: 68 years Sex: 5 (56%) women, 4 (44%) men Ethnicity: 39 (100%) white (Italian) Setting: a group meeting room in a neutral non‐medical setting Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music therapy group: each session had an opening, music‐assisted relaxation (MAR), active music therapy, and a closing. 2. Control group: standard care (wait‐list control) Music provided: the active music‐making component included song lyric analysis, expressive singing, songwriting, and instrumental improvisation. Instrumental music selections were drawn from the classical and new age genres. The selections had tempi of 60 to 70 beats per minute, were 3 to 6 minutes in length, and had consistent tempo, dynamics, and instrumentation. The selections gradually increased in length and complexity as the sessions progressed and then ended with a shorter, less complex selection for the last session. The relaxation scripts included autogenic and image‐based inductions Number of sessions: 6 weekly sessions over 6 weeks Length of sessions: not reported Categorized as music therapy |
|
Outcomes | Stress (Hassles Scale): posttest scores Anxiety (POMS): posttest scores Depression (POMS): posttest scores Mood disturbance (POMS): change scores Anger‐Hostility (POMS): posttest scores Vigor‐Activity (POMS): posttest scores |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "Each person was given a number between 1 and 10. Using a random number generator (Haahr, n.d.), the investigator assigned each person to one of the groups. A coin toss determined that the first five numbers would comprise the experimental group and the last five numbers would comprise the comparison group." (p.51) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Allocation was concealed through both the drawing of lots and flip of a coin |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Participants and personnel were unable to be blinded due to the interactive nature of the music therapy session |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Low risk | This study did not address objective outcomes |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition rate: n = 1 (10%) One participant attended only one treatment session. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Unfunded research study |
Mandel 2007a.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adults in phase II cardiac rehabilitation program
Total N randomized: 103 Randomized to music group: 55 Randomized to control group: 48 N analyzed in music therapy group: 35 N analyzed in control group: 33 Age: music therapy group: median age 65; control group: median age 64 Sex: 34 (50%) women, 34 (50%) men Ethnicity: not reported Setting: Rehabilitation setting Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music therapy group: standard care + one music therapy session every other week with a min of 4 music therapy session (max. 6 sessions) 2. Control group: standard care alone Music provided: live vocal music to stimulate discussion and offer verbal support, live music making with assorted instruments, song lyric writing, song lyric interpretation, sharing musical recordings, music‐assisted relaxation and imagery. Number of sessions: min of 4 music therapy sessions, max. 6 Duration of session: 90 mins. Categorized as music therapy |
|
Outcomes | Trait anxiety (STAI‐T): posttest scores Depression (CES‐D): posttest scores Distress (BSI): posttest scores General health (MOS SF‐36): posttest scores Bodily pain (MOS SF‐36): posttest scores SBP, DBP: posttest scores | |
Notes | Follow‐up measures were taken at 1 month, 4 months, and 10 months. These were not included in this review. | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "Patients' research study numbers (last 4 digits of their social security number) were recorded by the research assistant. A random‐number table was utilized by the hospital's research department staff to assign participants to condition one or two, based on their study number" (p.180). |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Central randomization was used |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | In music therapy trials, participants and the music therapist cannot be blinding because of the active participation in music making |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Attrition: n = 35 (34%). Reasons for participant loss: illness, non‐compliance, music therapist's leave of absence |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Supported by a grant from the Kulas Foundation, Cleveland OH |
Murrock 2002.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design |
|
Participants | Adults enrolled in cardiac rehab Phase II program after having undergone their 1st CABG
Total N randomized: 33 N randomized to music group: unclear N randomized to control group: unclear N analyzed in music group: 15 N analyzed in control group:15 Mean age: 70.43 years Sex: 13 (43%) women, 17 (57%) men Ethnicity: not reported Setting: rehab setting Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to researcher‐selected music during exercise session 2. Control group: standard care Music provided: Hooked on Classics by Louis Clark and the Royal Philharmonic Orchestra (upbeat tempo of 128 to 160 bpm) Number of sessions: 10 sessions Length of session: 40 min Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Mood (Rejeski's Feeling scale; +5 to ‐5): posttest (during 10th session) Rate of perceived exertion (Borg scale; 12‐point scale): posttest (during 10th session) | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Drawing of lots (personal communication with author) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Drawing of lots prevented knowledge of randomization sequence (personal communication with author) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report was used for subjective measures |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Low risk | No objective outcomes included in this study |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition: n = 3 (9%). No reason for participant loss is given |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Unfunded research study |
Nilsson 2009a.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design |
|
Participants | Adults who underwent CABG or aortic valve replacement Total N randomized: 60 N randomized to music group: 30 N randomized to control group: 30 N analyzed in music group: 28 N analyzed in control group: 30 Mean age: 62 (SD 9.5) years Sex: 13 (22%) women, 47 (78%) men Ethnicity: not reported Setting: inpatient Country: Sweden |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: participants listened to pre‐recorded music through a music pillow on their first postoperative day 2. Control group: provided a space for rest with reduced environmental stimuli Music provided: Quote from study report (p. 203): "The music was soft and relaxing, 60 to 80 beats per minute, included different melodies in new‐age style for 30 minutes, and played with a volume of 50 to 60 dB". Number of sessions: 1 Length of sessions: 30 minutes Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | HR: change scores RR, MAP, O₂‐sat: posttest scores S‐Cortisol: change scores Not used: Anxiety (NRS): not included in this review since range of scores but no SDs were reported. Pain (NRS): not included in this review since range of scores but no SDs were reported. |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "The patients were randomly allocated to 2 groups, 1 music group and 1 control group, using a computer‐generated randomization list created by the statistician" (p. 202). |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "Three special research nurses allocated the next available number on entry into the trial and conducted all interventions and outcome assessments. The code was revealed to the re‐ searcher once recruitment, data collection, and laboratory analyses were complete" (p.202) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | High risk | Outcome assessors were not blinded. However, low risk of bias for blood serum cortisol levels as lab technicians were blinded. |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition rate: n = 2 (3.3%). Quote: "Two of those patients, who gave informed consent to participate in the music group, were excluded because of chest pain and the drainage procedure" (p. 203). |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Funding for this study was provided by grants received from the Research Committee of Orebro County Council. |
Ryu 2011.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design |
|
Participants | Adults with confirmed CAD diagnosis undergoing percutaneous transluminal coronary angiography procedures Total N randomized: 60 N randomized to music group: 30 N randomized to control group: 30 N analyzed in music group: 29 N analyzed in control group: 29 Mean age: 61.2 years Sex: 20 (34%) women, 38 (66%) men Ethnicity: no information provided Setting: inpatient Country: South Korea |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: participants listened to sleep‐inducing music from 10:00 pm to 5:00 am the next morning 2. Control group: ear plugs were provided from 10:00 pm to 5:00 am the next morning Music provided: the sleep‐inducing album entitled Korean’s Brain, Delta wave Clinic Vol 1. by KK Park which included sequencing of nature sounds, delta wave control music, and Goldberg Variations BWV. 988 Number of sessions: 1 Length of sessions: 30 minutes Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Quantity of sleep (min) Quality of sleep (Verran and Synder‐Halpern Sleeping Scale) |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "The 60 participants were randomly assigned to experimental group or control group using a card number. The participants having an even number were assigned to the experimental group, and those with an odd number were assigned to the control group". (p.730) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Allocation was concealed by having participants draw the card number |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Low risk | Quote: "The research assistants were nurses having more than two years of experience in the CCU and who were blinded to which subject was assigned to the experimental group or the control group" (p.731) |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition rate: n = 2 (3.3%) One participant in the experimental group was excluded for having taken a sleep‐inducing drug taken, and one participant in the control group was transferred to another unit |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | No report of funding |
Schou 2008.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design |
|
Participants | Adults, valve replacement or valve replacement and CABG Total N randomized: 68 N randomized to music therapy group:25 N randomized to placebo group: 23 (not used in this review) N randomized to control group: 20 N analyzed in music therapy group: 22 N analyzed in control group: 19 N randomized in placebo group: 22 Mean age: 65 (SD 9.5) years Sex: 14 (21%) women, 54 (79%) men Ethnicity: not reported Setting: inpatient Country: Denmark |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music therapy group: music‐guided relaxation 2. Control group: standard care Music provided: (a) Easy listening, (b) classical, (c) specially composed (musicure), (d) jazz Number of sessions: 1 pre‐operative session and up to 4 postoperative sessions (most participants received 2 postop sessions) Duration of session: 35 mins Categorized as music therapy |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (VAS): posttest 2nd postop session Mood (POMS): posttest 2nd postop session Pain (VAS): posttest 2nd postop session Use of strong opioids (mg): on day of 2nd session Use of mild opioids (mg): on day of 2nd session Use of paracetamol (gram): on day of 2nd session Length of hospital stay |
|
Notes | Most participants only received 2 sessions postoperatively. Therefore, data of the 2nd postop sessions was used for this analysis | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Random block |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Use of codes as group labels, recruiters did not know what group the codes identified (personal communication with author) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Music therapist and participants could not be blinded given the interactive nature of the music therapy session |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | Unclear risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition: n = 4 (8.8%). Withdrawals due to early discharge. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Unfunded research study |
Sendelbach 2006.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adults following non‐emergent CAB and/or valve replacement surgery
Total N randomized: not reported N randomized to music group: unclear N randomized to control group: unclear N analyzed in music group: 50 N analyzed in control group: 36 Mean age: 63.5 years Sex: 26 (30%) women, 60 (70%) men Ethnicity: not reported Setting: inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to self‐selected sedative music through earphones 2. Control group: standard care Music selections provided: (a) easy listening, (b) classical, (c) jazz Number of sessions: 2 sessions/day for POD 1 through 3 Length of session: 20 mins Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (STAI): 6 measurement points. Due to high number of missing values, only posttests POD1 am, POD1 pm and POD2 am were used in research report HR and SBP: 6 measurement points. Due to high number of missing values, only posttest POD1 am, POD1 pm and POD2 am were used in research report Pain (NRS): 6 measurement points. Due to high number of missing values, only posttests POD1 am, POD1 pm and POD2 am were used in research report | |
Notes | N is highly variable due to high number of missing data | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Flip of coin |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Flip of coin prevented prior knowledge of randomization sequence |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded. |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | High risk | Outcome assessors were not blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Supported by a grant from the Allina Foundation Nursing Research Trust Fund. |
Stein 2010.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 3‐arm parallel group design |
|
Participants | Adults awaiting CABG surgery or CABG plus aortic valve repair or replacement Total N randomized: 70 N randomized to music group: unclear N randomized to guided imagery: unclear (not used in this review) N randomized to control group: unclear N analyzed in music group: 17 N analyzed in control group: 19 N analyzed in guided imagery: 20 (not included in this review) Mean age: no information provided Sex: 8 (22%) women, 28 (78%) men Ethnicity: 36 (92%) white, 1 (3%) African‐American, 2 (5%) Hispanic Setting: inpatient and outpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: participants listened to audiotapes at least once a day, every day, for 1 week before surgery. Participants were also asked to listen to their tapes intraoperatively 2. Control group: standard care which included access to CAM therapies, including audiotapes, upon request Music provided: Successful Surgery by Belleruth Naparstek without the pre‐recorded voice‐over providing imagery and affirmations Number of sessions: Varied Length of sessions: Varied Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (HADS ‐ anxiety subscale): change scores Depression (HADS ‐ depression subscale): change scores Mood disturbance (POMS): posttest scores |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "patients were randomly assigned via a coin toss" (p.215) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Coin toss |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Music was self‐administered at home after participants filled out baseline measurements |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Low risk | No objective outcomes were included in this study |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Unclear how many participants were lost in the music and the control groups. In total (for 3 groups), 14 (20%) participants were lost. Reasons: 7 participants did not undergo CABG procedures or were transferred to another hospital; 7 did not complete the posttest. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Funding for this study was provided by the Foundation for the Advancement of Cardiac Therapies. |
Voss 2004.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adults in ICU after CABG
Total N randomized: 62 N randomized to music group: 20 N randomized to scheduled rest group: 21 (not included in this review) N randomized to control group: 21 N analyzed in music group: 19 N analyzed in scheduled rest: 21 (not included in this review) N analyzed in control group: 21 Mean age: 63 (SD 13) years Sex: 22 (36%) women, 39 (64%) men Ethnicity: 53 (87%) white, 8 (13%) American‐Indian Setting: inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to self‐selected sedative music through earphones 2. Control group: standard care during chair rest. Music provided: (a) synthesizer music, (b) harp, (c) piano, (d) orchestra, (e) slow jazz, (f) flute. All music was without lyrics with sustained melodic quality, with a rate of 60 ‐ 80 bpm and a general absence of strong rhythms or percussion Number of sessions: 1 Length of session: 30 mins Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (VAS): posttest Pain sensation (VAS): posttest Pain distress (VAS): posttest | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: "Written informed consent was obtained, and participants were randomly assigned to the sedative music, scheduled rest, or control group using sealed envelopes with a varied block size prepared by the statistician. The investigator was blind to the block size and could not anticipate group assignment" (p.198) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Serially numbered opaque sealed envelopes |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | High risk | Investigator measured objective outcomes |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Attrition: n = 1 (2.4%). One participant was deleted from analysis because of extreme scores (outlier) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Supported by Sigma Theta Tau Phi Chapter Research Grant, 2001–2002 |
White 1992.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 2‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adults with confirmed diagnosis of MI, with STAI scores > 40 Total N randomized: 40 N randomized to music group: 20 N randomized to control group: 20 N analyzed in music group: 20 N analyzed in control group: 20 Mean age: 57.7 (SD 7.57) years Sex: 11 (28%) women, 29 (72%) men Ethnicity: 36 (90%) white, 4 (10%) African‐American Setting: inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to researcher‐selected music through earphones 2. Control group: quiet, uninterrupted rest Music provided: 4 classical adagios, tempo of approx. 60 bpm Number of sessions: 1 Length of session: 25 min Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (STAI): posttest scores HR, RR: posttest scores | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer‐generated random number list (personal communication with author) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Study recruiters were blind to allocation (personal communication with author) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | High risk | Outcome assessors were not blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No participant loss |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Unfunded research study |
White 1999.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 3‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adults with confirmed diagnosis of MI Total N randomized:45 N randomized to music group: 15 N randomized to control group: 15 N randomized to quiet rest group: 15 (not included in this review) N analyzed in music group: 15 N analyzed in control group:15 Mean age: 63 years Sex: 7 (23%) women, 23 (67%) men Ethnicity: 23 (76.6%) white, 6 (20%) African‐American, 1 (3.4%) Hispanic Setting: inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Two study groups: 1. Music group: listening to researcher‐selected music through earphones 2. Control group: standard care Music used: classical music (no further specifications) Number of sessions: 1 Length of session: 20 mins Categorized as music medicine |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (STAI): posttest HR, RR, SBP: posttest High frequency heart rate variability (HF HRV) (variability power) | |
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer‐generated random number list (personal communication with author) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Study recruiters were blind to allocation (personal communication with author) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | High risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | High risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | High risk | Outcome assessors were not blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No participant loss |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Supported in part by NSRA F 31; Marquette Medical Systems, Inc,; and Eta Nu Chapter of the Sigma Theta Tau International. |
Winters 2005.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | RCT 6‐arm parallel group design | |
Participants | Adults less than 72 hrs post‐MI Total N randomized: 184 N randomized to music group who received 1 session (am): unclear N randomized to music group who received 2 sessions (am and pm):unclear N randomized to music group who received 2 sessions (am and noc):unclear N randomized to music group who received 3 sessions (am, pm, noc): unclear N randomized to standard care control group: unclear N randomized to quiet rest group: unclear (not included in this review) N analyzed in standard care group (N = 30) N analyzed in quiet rest group (N = 29) N analyzed in music listening group, 1 session in am (N = 30) N analyzed in music listening group, 2 sessions, am and pm (N = 30) N analyzed in music listening group, 2 sessions, am and noc (N = 30) N analyzed in music listening group, 3 sessions, am, pm, and noc (N = 30). Sex: 38 (64%) women, 22 (36%) men Age: no age data reported Etnicity: 117 (63.7%) white, 60 (32.4%) African‐American, 4 (2.2%) Asian, 1 (0.6%) Native American, 2 (1.1%) unknown (ethnicity per arm was not reported) Setting: inpatient Country: USA |
|
Interventions | Six study groups: (1) standard care group (N = 30), (2) quiet rest group (N = 29), (3) music listening group, 1 session in am (N = 30), (4) music listening group, 2 sessions, am and pm (N = 30), (5) music listening group, 2 sessions, am and noc (N = 30), and (6) music listening group, 3 sessions, am, pm, and noc (N = 30). Music used: patient‐selected relaxing music Number of sessions: 3 (only data of group 6 compared to group 1 was used for this analysis) Duration of session: 20 minutes Categorized as music medicine study. |
|
Outcomes | Anxiety (STAI): change scores HR, RR, SBP, HF HRV (variability power), myocardial oxygen (MVO₂) demand: change scores |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer‐generated random number list (personal communication with author) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Study recruiters were blind to allocation (personal communication with author) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding of participants was not possible. Personnel were not blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Subjective outcomes | Unclear risk | Self report measures were used for subjective outcomes |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) Objective outcomes | Unclear risk | Outcome assessors were not blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | For all study arms combined, there was an attrition of n = 5 (2.7%). Reasons: 2 because of delirium tremors, 3 because of extensive periods of atrial fibrillation. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not sufficient information available to make judgment |
Other bias | Low risk | Supported by NINR 5R01NR005004‐06 |
ACS: acute coronary syndrome; BSI: Brief Symptom Inventory; CABG: coronary artery bypass graft; CAD: coronary artery disease; CAM: complementary and alternative medicine ;CCU: coronary care unit; CES‐D: Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale; CR: cardiac rehabilitation; DBP: diastolic blood pressure; ECG: electrocardiogram; HADS: Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; HR: heart rate; ICU: intensive care unit; LAAS: Linear Analogue Anxiety Scale; MI: myocardial infarction; mg: milligram; MPQ: McGill Pain Questionnaire; NRS: Numeric Rating Scale, POD: post‐operative days; POMS: Profile of Mood States; POMS‐SF: Profile of Mood States Short Form; POD: post‐operative day; RCT: randomized controlled trial; RR: respiratory rate; SBP: systolic blood pressure; SD: standard deviation; STAI: Spielberger State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory; STAI‐S: Spielberger State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory State Anxiety form; STAI‐T:Spielberger State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory Trait Anxiety form; VAS; Visual Analogue Scale; VRS: Verbal Rating Scale.
Characteristics of excluded studies [ordered by study ID]
Study | Reason for exclusion |
---|---|
Aragon 2002 | Not a randomized controlled trial |
Argstatter 2006 | This study was included in the original review but is now excluded because not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Bally 2003 | Not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Bonny 1983 | Not a randomized controlled trial. Pretest‐posttest single group design |
Byers 1997 | Not a randomized controlled trial |
Chang 2011 | Not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Claire 1986 | Not a randomized controlled trial |
Diamandi 2008 | No standard care control group. Study compared music therapy with music listening |
Dritsas 2006 | Insufficient data available |
Escher 1996 | Insufficient data available |
Garcia 2003 | Not a randomized controlled trial |
Ghetti 2011 | Not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Goertz 2011 | Not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Guzzetta 1989 | This study was included in the original review but is now excluded because not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Hamel 2001 | Not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Harris 1971 | Not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Hatem 2006 | Interquartile ranges are reported instead of standard deviations. This suggests that the outcome distribution was severely skewed. |
Ibhler 2011 | Insufficient data available |
Jiang 2008 | The study intervention was a combination of relaxation training and music listening |
MacNay 1995 | Not a randomized controlled trial |
Mandel 2007b | Not a randomized controlled trial |
Micci 1984 | Participants received diagnostic angiography procedure |
Moradipanah 2009 | Participants received diagnostic angiography procedure |
Nilsson 2009b | Participants received diagnostic angiography procedure |
Nilsson 2012 | Participants received diagnostic angiography procedure |
Okada 2009 | Not a randomized controlled trial |
Reisinger 1995 | Not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Richardson 2004 | No standard care control group. Study compared music listening with music/imagery |
Robichaud 1999 | This study was included in the original review but is now excluded because not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Schwartz 2002 | No randomization used |
Schwartz 2009 | Group assignment was based on availability of space |
Short 2011 | Experimental group was not randomized and there was no control group |
Slyfield 1992 | Insufficient data |
Taylor‐Piliae 2002 | Not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Thorgaard 2004 | Unclear randomization methods. Poor data reporting |
Twiss 2003 | Lack of proper randomization method. In the thesis author explicitly states that only 4 CD players were available. If all CD players were in use, the next group of participants were placed in the control group |
Vanderboom 2012 | Participants received diagnostic cerebral angiography procedure |
Watanabe 2011 | Participants received diagnostic angiography procedure |
Weeks 2011 | Participants received diagnostic angiography procedure |
Zimmerman 1988 | This study was included in the original review but is now excluded because not all participants had confirmed CHD |
Differences between protocol and review
The following subgroup analysis was not included in the protocol:
A comparison of (a) MI patients, (b) surgical or procedural patients, and (c) rehabilitation patients. Although this subanalysis was not determined a priori, the reviewers decided it was important to conduct a subanalysis comparing the effect of these three groups of studies for those outcome variables for which significant heterogeneity was found.
The original review only included randomized controlled trials. For the update of this review, we decided to also include quasi‐randomized controlled trials and conduct a sensitivity analysis to examine the impact of such trials on the effect size.
Finally, an explicit statement was added to the method section regarding the exclusion of studies with participants who did not all have a confirmed CHD.
Contributions of authors
Joke Bradt: conceived and designed the review, developed the search strategies and wrote the protocol. She is the guarantor for the review and identified potentially relevant trials, extracted eligible articles, extracted data from included studies, performed the statistical analysis and wrote the review text.
Cheryl Dileo: conceived and designed the review, and contributed to writing the protocol. For the original review, she identified potentially relevant trials, tracked eligible articles, extracted data from them, and contributed to writing the text. For the update of the review, she completed the quality assessment of the trials.
Noah Potvin: contributed to the update of this review by screening the database search outputs for eligible trials, updating the handsearches, retrieving full‐text articles, completing quality assessment of the trials, extracting data from included studies, and reviewing the text of this review.
Sources of support
Internal sources
Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
External sources
State of Pennsylvania Formula Fund, USA
Declarations of interest
All three authors are trained music therapists.
Edited (no change to conclusions)
References
References to studies included in this review
Barnason 1995 {published data only}
- Barnason S, Zimmerman L, Nieveen J. The effects of music interventions on anxiety in the patient after coronary artery bypass grafting. Heart & Lung 1995;24(2):124-32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman L, Nieveen J, Barnason S, Schmaderer M. The effects of music interventions on postoperative pain and sleep in coronary artery bypass graft (CABG) patients. Scholarly Inquiry for Nursing Practice 1996;10(2):153-70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Barnes 1987 {published data only}
- Barnes JD. Relationship of Music to Cardiac Rehabilitation Patients' Perceived Exertion during Exercise. Gainesville, Florida: University of Florida College of Nursing, 1987. [Google Scholar]
Blankfield 1995 {published data only (unpublished sought but not used)}
- Blankfield RP, Zyzanski SJ. Taped therapeutic suggestions and taped music as adjuncts in the care of coronary-artery-bypass patients. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 1995;37(3):32-42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bolwerk 1990 {published data only}
- Bolwerk CA. Effects of relaxing music on state anxiety in myocardial infarction patients. Critical Care Nursing Quarterly 1990;13(2):63-72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Broscious 1999 {published data only}
- Broscious SK. Music: an intervention for pain during chest tube removal after open heart surgery. American Journal of Critical Care 1999;8(6):410-15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Cadigan 2001 {published data only}
- Cadigan ME, Caruso NA, Haldeman SM, McNamara ME, Noyes DA, Spadafora MA, et al. The effects of music on cardiac patients on bed rest. Progress in Cardiovascular Nursing 2001;16(1):5-13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Chan 2007 {published and unpublished data}
- Chan MF, Wong OC, Chan HL, Fong MC, Lai SY, Lo CW, et al. Effects of music on patients undergoing a C-clamp procedure after percutaneous coronary interventions. Journal of Advanced Nursing 2006;53(6):669-79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan MF. Effects of music on patients undergoing a C-clamp procedure after percutaneous coronary interventions: A randomized controlled trial. Heart & Lung 2007;36(6):431-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Cohen 1999 {unpublished data only}
- Cohen RL. The Effect of Music Therapy on the Reduction of Anxiety for Myocardial Infarction Patients. Omaha, Nebraska: Clarkson College, 1999. [Google Scholar]
Cutshall 2011 {published data only}
- Cutshall SA, Anderson PG, Prinsen SK, Wentworth LJ, Olney TJ, Messner PK, et al. Effect of the combination of music and nature sounds on pain and anxiety in cardiac surgical patients: a randomized study. Alternative Therapies in Health & Medicine 2011;17(4):16-23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Davis‐Rollans 1987 {published data only}
- Davis-Rollans C, Cunningham SG. Physiologic responses of coronary care patients to selected music. Heart & Lung 1987;16(4):370-8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Elliott 1994 {published and unpublished data}
- Elliott D. The effects of music and muscle-relaxation on patient anxiety in a coronary-care unit. Heart & Lung 1994;23(1):27-35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Emery 2003 {published and unpublished data}
- Emery CF, Hsiao ET, Hill SM, Frid DJ. Short-term effects of exercise and music on cognitive performance among participants in a cardiac rehabilitation program. Heart & Lung 2003;32(6):368-73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hermele 2005 {unpublished data only}
- Hermele SL. The Effectiveness of a Guided Imagery Intervention for Patients undergoing Coronary Artery Bypass Graft Surgery. New York, New York: Fordham University, 2005. [Google Scholar]
Jafari 2012 {published data only}
- Jafari H, Zeydi AE, Khani S, Esmaeili R, Soleimani A. The effect of preferred music on pain intensity after open heart surgery. Iranian Journal of Nursing and Midwifery Research 2012;17(1):1-8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeydi AM, Jafari H, Kahn S, Esmaieli R, Bardari AG. The effect of music on the vital signs and sp02 of patients. Journal of Mazandaran University of Medical 2011;21(82):73-82. [Google Scholar]
Leist 2011 {published data only}
- Leist CP. A Music Therapy Support Group to Ameliorate Psychological Distress in Adults with Coronary Heart Disease in a Rural Community (Dissertation). East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University, 2011. [Google Scholar]
Mandel 2007a {published data only}
- Mandel SE, Hanser SB, Secic M, Davis BA. Effects of music therapy on health-related outcomes in cardiac rehabilitation: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Music Therapy 2007;44(3):176-97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Murrock 2002 {published and unpublished data}
- Murrock CJ. The effects of music on the rate of perceived exertion and general mood among coronary artery bypass graft patients enrolled in cardiac rehabilitation phase II. Rehabilitation Nursing 2002;27(6):227-31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Nilsson 2009a {published data only}
- Nilsson, U. Soothing music can increase oxytocin levels during bed rest after open-heart surgery: a randomised control trial. Journal of Clinical Nursing 2009;18(15):2153-61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nilsson U. The effect of music intervention in stress response to cardiac surgery in a randomized clinical trial. Heart & Lung 2009;38(3):201-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Ryu 2011 {published data only}
- Ryu MJ, Park JS, Park H. Effect of sleep-inducing music on sleep in persons with percutaneous transluminal coronary angiography in the cardiac care unit. Journal of Clinical Nursing 2011;21(5-6):728-35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Schou 2008 {unpublished data only}
- Schou K. Music Guided Relaxation (Dissertation). Alborg, Denmark: Aalborg University, 2008. [Google Scholar]
Sendelbach 2006 {published and unpublished data}
- Sendelbach SE, Halm M A, Doran KA, Miller EH, Gaillard P. Effects of music therapy on physiological and psychological outcomes for patients undergoing cardiac surgery. Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing 2006;21(3):194-200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Stein 2010 {published data only}
- Stein TR, Olivo EL, Grand SH, Namerow PB, Costa J, Oz MC. A pilot study to assess the effects of a guided imagery audiotape intervention on psychological outcomes in patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Holistic Nursing Practice 2010;24(4):213-22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Voss 2004 {published data only}
- Voss JA, Good M, Yates B, Baun MM, Thompson A, Hertzog M. Sedative music reduces anxiety and pain during chair rest after open-heart surgery. Pain 2004;112(1-2):197-203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voss JA. Effect of Sedative Music and Scheduled Rest on Anxiety, Pain, and Myocardial Oxygen Demand during Chair Rest in Adult Postoperative Open-Heart Patients. Nebraska: University of Nebraska Medical Center, 2003. [Google Scholar]
White 1992 {published and unpublished data}
- White JM. Music therapy: an intervention to reduce anxiety in the myocardial infarction patient. Clinical Nurse Specialist 1992;6(2):58-63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
White 1999 {published and unpublished data}
- White JM. Effects of relaxing music on cardiac autonomic balance and anxiety after acute myocardial infarction. American Journal of Critical Care 1999;8(4):220-30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White JM. Effects of Relaxing Music on Cardiac Autonomic Balance and Anxiety following Acute Myocardial Infarction. Milwaukee, US: University of Wisconsin, 1997. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Winters 2005 {unpublished data only}
- Winters J. Effects of relaxing music after myocardial infarction. In: Midwest Nursing Research Society 29th Annual Research Conference, Cincinnati OH, 1 - 5th April. Wheat Ridge, Colorado: Midwest Nursing Research Society, 2005. [Grant number: NINR 5R01NR005004-06]
References to studies excluded from this review
Aragon 2002 {published data only}
- Aragon A, Farris C, Byers J. The effects of harp music in vascular and thoracic surgical patients. Alternative Therapies 2002;8(5):52-60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Argstatter 2006 {published and unpublished data}
- Argstatter H, Haberbosch W, Bolay HV. Study of the effectiveness of musical stimulation during intracardiac catheterization. Clinical Research in Cardiology 2006;95(10):514-22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Argstatter H, Haberbosch W, Bolay HV. Study of the effectiveness of musical stimulation during intracardiac catheterization [Untersuchung der Wirksamkeit von musikalischer Stimulation bei Herzkatheteruntersuchungen]. Musik-, Tanz- und Kunsttherapie 2006;17(1):11-20. [Google Scholar]
Bally 2003 {published data only}
- Bally K, Campbell D, Chesnick K, Tranmer JE. Effects of patient-controlled music therapy during coronary angiography on procedural pain and anxiety distress syndrome. Critical Care Nurse 2003;23(2):50-8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bonny 1983 {published data only}
- Bonny HL. Music listening for intensive coronary care units: a pilot project. Music Therapy 1983;3(1):4-16. [Google Scholar]
Byers 1997 {published data only}
- Byers JF. Efficacy of a Music Intervention on Noise Annoyance and Selected Physiologic Variables in Cardiac Surgery Patients with High and Low Noise Sensitivity. Florida: University of Florida, 1997. [Google Scholar]
Chang 2011 {published data only}
- Chang HK, Peng TC, Wang JH, Lai HL. Psychophysiological responses to sedative music in patients awaiting cardiac catheterization examination: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing 2011;25(5):E11-E18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Claire 1986 {published data only}
- Claire JB, Erickson S. Reducing distress in pediatric patients undergoing cardiac catheterization. Children's Health Care 1986;14(3):146-52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Diamandi 2008 {unpublished data only}
- Diamandi V. Music Therapy and Intracardiac Catheterization - Implementation and Comparison of Two Treatments (MSc Thesis). Heidelberg, Germany: Zentrum für Musiktherapieforschung, 2008. [Google Scholar]
Dritsas 2006 {published data only}
- Dritsas A, Pothoulaki M, MacDonald RAR, Flowers P, Cokkinos DV. Effects of music listening on anxiety and mood profile in cardiac patients undergoing exercise testing. European Journal of Cardiovascular Prevention & Rehabilitation 2006;13(Suppl 1):S76. [Google Scholar]
Escher 1996 {published data only}
- Escher J, Dayer E, Anthenien L. Music therapy in early rehabilitation of patients with acute myocardial infarction. In: Pratt RR, Spintge R, editors(s). Music Medicine. Vol. 2. Gilsum, New Hampshire: Barcelona Publishers, 1996:129-141. [Google Scholar]
Garcia 2003 {published data only}
- Garcia H, Rosenbaum MB. The bellows effect of intermittent bundle branch block [El efecto fuelle en los bloqueos intermitentes de rama]. Argentina Journal of Cardiology 2003;71(3):230-5. [Google Scholar]
Ghetti 2011 {published data only}
- Ghetti C. Effect of Music Therapy with Emotional-Approach Coping on Pre-procedural Anxiety in Cardiac Catherization (Dissertation). Lawrence, Kansas: University of Kansas, 2011. [Google Scholar]
Goertz 2011 {published data only}
- Goertz W, Dominick K, Heussen N, Vom Dahl J. Music in the cath lab: who should select it? Clinical Research in Cardiology 2011;100(5):395-402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Guzzetta 1989 {published and unpublished data}
- Guzzetta CE. Effects of relaxation and music therapy on patients in a coronary care unit with presumptive acute myocardial infarction. Heart & Lung 1989;18(6):609-16. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hamel 2001 {published data only}
- Hamel WJ. The effects of music intervention on anxiety in the patient waiting for cardiac catheterization. Intensive & Critical Care Nursing 2001;17(5):279-85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Harris 1971 {unpublished data only}
- Harris SA. An Investigation of the Effects of Sedative Music on Affectively Determined Anxiety Levels of Adult Patients undergoing Cardiac Catheterization. Seattle, Washington: University of Washington, 1971. [Google Scholar]
Hatem 2006 {published data only}
- Hatem, TP, Lira PIC, Mattos SS. The therapeutic effects of music in children following cardiac surgery [Efeito terapeutico da musica em criancas em pos-operatorio de cirurgia cardiaca]. Jornal de Pediatria 2006;82(3):186-92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Ibhler 2011 {published data only}
- Iblher P, Mahler H, Heinze H, Huppe M, Klotz K-F, Eichler W. Does music harm patients after cardiac surgery? A randomized, controlled study. Applied Cardiopulmonary Pathophysiology 2011;15(1):14-23. [Google Scholar]
Jiang 2008 {published data only}
- Jiang L-Y. Psychological intervention to anxiety and depression in geriatric patients with chronic heart failure. Chinese Mental Health Journal 2008;22(11):829-32. [Google Scholar]
MacNay 1995 {published data only}
- MacNay SK. The influence of preferred music on the perceived exertion, mood, and time estimation scores of patients participating in a cardiac rehabilitation exercise program. Music Therapy Perspectives 1995;13(2):91-6. [Google Scholar]
Mandel 2007b {unpublished data only}
- Mandel SE, Hanser SB, Ryan, LJ. Effects of a music-assisted relaxation and imagery compact disc recording on health-related outcomes in cardiac rehabilitation. Music Therapy Perspectives 2010;28(1):11-21. [Google Scholar]
- Mandel SE. Effects of Music-assisted Relaxation and Imagery (MARI) on Health-related Outcomes in Cardiac Rehabilitation: Follow-up Study. Cincinnati, Ohio: Union Institute and University, 2007. [1400957851] [Google Scholar]
Micci 1984 {published data only}
- Micci, NO. The use of music therapy with pediatric patients undergoing cardiac catheterization. The Arts In Psychotherapy 1984;11(4):261-6. [Google Scholar]
Moradipanah 2009 {published data only}
- Moradipanah F, Mohammadi E, Mohammadil AZ. Effect of music on anxiety, stress, and depression levels in patients undergoing coronary angiography. Eastern Mediterranean Health Journal 2009;15(3):639-47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Nilsson 2009b {published data only}
- Nilsson U, Lindell L, Eriksson A, Kellerth T. The effect of music intervention in relation to gender during coronary angiographic procedures: a randomized clinical trial. European Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing 2009;8(3):200-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Nilsson 2012 {published data only}
- Nilsson, U. Effectiveness of music interventions for women with high anxiety during coronary angiographic procedures: a randomized controlled. European Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing 2012;11(2):150-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Okada 2009 {published data only}
- Okada K, Kurita A, Takase B, Otsuka T, Kodani A, Kusama Y, et al. Effects of music therapy on autonomic nervous system activity, incidence of heart failure events, and plasma cytokine and catecholamine levels in elderly patients with cerebrovascular disease and dementia. International Heart Journal 2009;50(1):P95-110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Reisinger 1995 {unpublished data only}
- Reisinger MA. Effects of Music Therapy on Anxiolytic Drug Reduction in Clients Undergoing Cardiac Catheterization. Connecticut: Southern Connecticut State University, 1995. [Google Scholar]
Richardson 2004 {published data only}
- Richardson RS. The Psychological Effects of Anxiolytic Music/Imagery on Anxiety and Depression following Cardiac Surgery. Minneapolis, Minnesota: Walden University, 2004. [Google Scholar]
Robichaud 1999 {published and unpublished data}
- Robichaud-Ekstrand S. The influence of music on coronary diseases (CHC) patients waiting for cardiac catherization. Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation 1999;19(5):304. [Google Scholar]
- Robichaud-Ekstrand S. The influence of music on coronary heart disease patients' relaxation levels [L’influence de la musique sur le niveau de relaxation des patients cardiaques]. Revue Francophone de Clinique Comportementale et Cognitive 2004;9(2):20-8. [Google Scholar]
Schwartz 2002 {unpublished data only}
- Schwartz FJ, Ramey, GA, Pawli S. Benefits of headphone music on the ICU postoperative recovery of CABG patients. In: Paper presented at the Conference of the International Society of Music in Medicine, Hamburg, Germany. International Society of Music in Medicine, 2002.
Schwartz 2009 {published data only}
- Schwartz FJ. A pilot study of participants in postoperative cardiac surgery. Music and Medicine 2009;1(1):70-4. [Google Scholar]
Short 2011 {published data only}
- Short A, Gibb, H, Holmes C. Integrating words, images, and text in BMGIM: finding connections through semiotic intertextuality. Nordic Journal of Music Therapy 2011;20(1):3-21. [Google Scholar]
Slyfield 1992 {published data only}
- Slyfield CM. The Effect of Music Therapy on Patient's Pain, Blood Pressure, and Heart Rate after Coronary Artery Bypass Graft Surgery. Gainesville, Florida: University of Florida College of Nursing, 1992. [Google Scholar]
Taylor‐Piliae 2002 {published data only}
- Taylor-Piliae RE, Chair SY. The effect of nursing interventions utilizing music thearpy or sensory information on Chinese patients' anxiety prior to cardiac catheterization: A pilot study. European Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing 2002;1(3):203-11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Thorgaard 2004 {published data only}
- Thorgaard B, Henriksen BB, Pedersbaek G, Thomsen I. Specially selected music in the cardiac laboratory-an important tool for improvement of the wellbeing of patients. European Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing 2004;3(1):21-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Twiss 2003 {published and unpublished data}
- Twiss E, Seaver J, McCaffrey R. The effect of music listening on older adults undergoing cardiovascular surgery. Nursing in Critical Care 2006;11(5):224-31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Twiss EJ. The Effect of Music as a Nursing Intervention to Reduce Anxiety in Coronary Bypass and Valve Replacement Surgery Patients. Boca Raton, Florida: Florida Atlantic University, 2003. [Google Scholar]
Vanderboom 2012 {published data only}
- Vanderboom TL, Arcari PM, Duffy ME, Somarouthu B, Rabinov JD, Yoo AJ, et al. Effects of a music intervention on patients undergoing cerebralangiography: a pilot study.. Journal of Neurointerventional Surgery 2012;4(3):229-33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Watanabe 2011 {published data only}
- Watanabe DM. The Effect of Music on Anxiety of Patients Undergoing Coronary Angiography (Thesis). São Paulo, Brazil: University of São Paulo, 2011. [Google Scholar]
Weeks 2011 {published data only}
- Weeks BP, Nilsson U. Music interventions in patients during coronary angiographic procedures: a randomized controlled study of the effect on patients' anxiety and well-being. European Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing 2011;10(2):88-93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Zimmerman 1988 {published and unpublished data}
- Zimmerman LM. Effects of music on patient anxiety in coronary care units. Heart & Lung 1988;17(5):560-6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Additional references
Arslan 2008
- Arslan S, Özer N, Özyurt F. Effect of music on preoperative anxiety in men undergoing urogenital surgery. Australian Journal of Advanced Nursing 2008;26(2):46-54. [DN: 198783205186632] [Google Scholar]
Bradt 2009b
- Bradt J. Music for breathing regulation. In: Loewy J, editors(s). Music, the Breath & Health: Advances in Integrative Music Therapy. New York: Louis Armstrong Center for Music & Medicine, 2009. [Google Scholar]
Bradt 2010a
- Bradt J, Dileo C. Music therapy for end-of-life care. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2010, Issue 1. Art. No: CD007169. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD007169.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bradt 2010b
- Bradt J, Dileo C, Grocke D. Music interventions for mechanically ventilated patients. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2010, Issue 12. Art. No: CD006902. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006902.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bradt 2011
- Bradt J, Dileo C, Grocke D, Magill L. Music interventions for improving psychological and physical outcomes in cancer patients. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2011, Issue 8. Art. No: CD006911. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006911.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bradt 2012
- Bradt J. Randomized controlled trials in music therapy: Guidelines for design and implementation . Journal of Music Therapy 2012;49(2):120-49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bradt 2013
- Bradt J, Dileo C, Shim M. Music interventions for preoperative anxiety. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2013, Issue 6. Art. No: CD006908. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006908.pub2] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Chlan 1998
- Chlan L. Effectiveness of a music therapy intervention on relaxation and anxiety for patients receiving ventilatory assistance. Heart & Lung 1998;27(3):169-76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Chockalingam 1999
- Chockalingam A, Balaguer-Vintro I (editors). Impending Global Pandemic of Cardiovascular Diseases: Challenges and Opportunities for the Prevention and Control of Cardiovascular Diseases in Developing Countries and Economies in Transition. Barcelona: Prous Science, 1999. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Cohen 1988
- Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd edition. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, 1988. [Google Scholar]
Dileo 1999
- Dileo C. Music Therapy & Medicine. Silver Spring, Maryland: American Music Therapy Assocation, 1999. [Google Scholar]
Dileo 2005
- Dileo C, Bradt J. Medical Music Therapy: a meta-analysis & agenda for future research. Cherry Hill, New Jersey: Jeffrey Books, 2005. [Google Scholar]
Dileo 2007
- Dileo C, Bradt J. Music Therapy: applications to stress management. In: Lehrer P, Woolfolk R, editors(s). Principles and Practice of Stress Management. 3rd edition. New York, New York: Guilford Press, 2007. [Google Scholar]
Gillen 2008
- Gillen E, Biley F, Allen D. Effects of music listening on adult patients’ pre-procedural state anxiety in hospital. International Journal of Evidence-Based Healthcare 2008;6(1):24-49. [2008-02471-003] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Higgins 2003
- Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 2003;327(7414):557-60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Higgins 2011
- Higgins JPT, Green S (editors). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from www.cochrane-handbook.org.
Lee 2005
- Lee O, Chung Y, Chan M, Chan W. Music and its effect on the physiological responses and anxiety levels of patients receiving mechanical ventilation: a pilot study. Journal of Clinical Nursing 2005;14(5):609-20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Malan 1992
- Malan SS. Psychological adjustment following MI: current views and nursing implications. Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing 1992;6(4):57-70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
McAuley 2000
- McAuley L, Pham B, Tugwell P, Moher D. Does the inclusion of grey literature influence estimates of intervention effectiveness reported in meta-analyses? Lancet 2000;356(9237):1228-31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Mitchell 2003
- Mitchell M. Patient anxiety and modern elective surgery: A literature review. Journal of Clinical Nursing 2003;12(6):806-15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Nilsson 2008
- Nilsson U. The anxiety and pain-reducing effects of music interventions: a systematic review. AORN Journal 2008;87(4):780–807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Revman 2012 [Computer program]
- The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration Review Manager (RevMan). Version 5.2. Copenhagen: The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration, 2012.
Robb 2011
- Robb SL, Burns DS, Carpenter JS. Reporting guidelines for music-based interventions. Music and Medicine 2011;3(4):271-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Standley 1986
- Standley JM. Music research in medical/dental treatment: a meta-analysis and clinical implications. Journal of Music Therapy 1986;23(2):56-122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Standley 2000
- Standley JM. Music research in medical treatment. In: Effectiveness of music therapy procedures: Documentation of research and clinical practice. 3rd edition. Silver Spring: American Music Therapy Association, 2000:1-64. [Google Scholar]
Vollert 2002
- Vollert JO, Stork T, Rose M, Rocker L, Klapp BF, Heller G, et al. Reception of music in patients with systemic arterial hypertension and coronary artery disease: Endocrine changes, hemodynamics and actual mood. Perfusion 2002;15:142-52. [Google Scholar]
WHO 2012
- World Health Organization. Cardiovascular diseases. Fact sheet N°317. www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs317/en/index.html (accessed 30th November 2012).
Zimmerman 1996
- Zimmerman L, Nieveen J, Barnason S, Schmaderer M. The effects of music interventions on postoperative pain and sleep in coronary artery bypass graft (CABG) patients. Scholarly Inquiry for Nursing Practice 1996;10(2):153-70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
References to other published versions of this review
Bradt 2009a
- Bradt J, Dileo C. Music for stress and anxiety reduction in coronary heart disease patients. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2009, Issue 2. Art. No: CD006577. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006577.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]