Skip to main content
Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders logoLink to Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders
. 2021 Sep 21;13:41. doi: 10.1186/s11689-021-09382-1

The prevalence and profile of autism in individuals born preterm: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Catherine Laverty 1,, Andrew Surtees 1,2, Rory O’Sullivan 3, Daniel Sutherland 3, Christopher Jones 3, Caroline Richards 1
PMCID: PMC8454175  PMID: 34548007

Abstract

Introduction

Preterm birth (<37 weeks) adversely affects development in behavioural, cognitive and mental health domains. Heightened rates of autism are identified in preterm populations, indicating that prematurity may confer an increased likelihood of adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes. The present meta-analysis aims to synthesise existing literature and calculate pooled prevalence estimates for rates of autism characteristics in preterm populations.

Methods

Search terms were generated from inspection of relevant high-impact papers and a recent meta-analysis. Five databases were searched from database creation until December 2020 with PRISMA guidelines followed throughout.

Results

10,900 papers were retrieved, with 52 papers included in the final analyses, further classified by assessment method (screening tools N=30, diagnostic assessment N=29). Pooled prevalence estimates for autism in preterm samples was 20% when using screening tools and 6% when using diagnostic assessments. The odds of an autism diagnosis were 3.3 times higher in individuals born preterm than in the general population.

Conclusions

The pooled prevalence estimate of autism characteristics in individuals born preterm is considerably higher than in the general population. Findings highlight the clinical need to provide further monitoring and support for individuals born preterm.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s11689-021-09382-1.

Keywords: Autism, Prematurity, Meta-analysis, Preterm, Low birth weight

Introduction

Preterm birth is defined as birth occurring at a gestational age of less than 37 weeks [1] and accounts for 15 million births worldwide each year [2]. Many infants born preterm experience immediate and significant health complications, which often lead to extended periods of hospitalisation on neonatal units [3]. Longitudinal research demonstrates that even babies born preterm who do not present with immediate health complications show a significant increased likelihood for later adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes such as intellectual disability [4, 5]. This increased likelihood perseveres across the lifespan, with employment rates, educational qualifications and socioeconomic status negatively impacted into adulthood [6]. As a result of medical and technological advances, survival rates for infants born preterm are rising [7]. It is therefore of growing importance to quantify the neurodevelopmental trajectory of children born preterm.

Preterm birth is associated with heightened rates of autism when compared to birth that occurs at term [8, 9]. Autism is characterised by impairments in social communication and social interaction, and restricted patterns of repetitive behaviour [2, 10, 11]. Advances in understanding of subtle and nuanced manifestations of autism now mean that individuals can be identified earlier, with diagnoses as early as 2 years old shown to be stable over time [12]. Advances in research also highlight that individuals may display difficulties in isolated areas comparable to those with autism, yet do not meet the criteria for formal diagnosis [13]. Considering the presence of these broader autism characteristics or an atypical autism phenotype is therefore imperative when aiming to meet individual needs regarding support and service use.

A notable increased prevalence of autism has been identified for those born very preterm (4–6%) [14]. Lower birth weight, earlier gestational age at birth and male gender have been associated with heightened rates of autism in preterm samples [15]. Historically, clinical resources were focussed towards those very preterm births (<32+0/7 weeks), who represent around 10% of all preterm births [16], with infants born closer to the term often considered to be as biologically mature as term-born infants [17]. Recent literature has documented a shift in understanding the needs of those born moderately-late preterm (32–36 weeks of gestation), who represent the majority of all preterm births [18]. Although children born moderate-late preterm often present without immediate medical complications, they are still at greater risk for adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes and even at greater risk of infantile mortality than those born at term [19]. In an attempt to recognise the difficulties which those born closer to term experience, the definition ‘near term’ was changed to ‘late preterm’ to acknowledge that infants born closer to term are still at heightened risk, with clinicians now considering gestational age and subsequent increased likelihood as a continuum [20]. It is therefore important that the synthesis of the current literature regarding neurodevelopmental outcomes reflects this and considers outcomes across gestational age without being limited to those born at the earliest gestations.

A previous meta-analysis examined studies identifying the prevalence of children who met clinical cut-off for autism using comprehensive diagnostic assessments [9]. A pooled estimate of 7% was returned. This strategy was important in identifying the rates of preterm children who are likely to obtain diagnoses. It does not, however, address three vital concerns: (i) studies using diagnostic assessments have tended to focus on very preterm samples at the expense of moderate-late preterm samples. Current literature shows a clear bias within assessment methods and population samples, in which diagnostic assessments are used more in very preterm populations [21, 22], whereas screening measures are utilised more in moderate-late preterm groups [23]. (ii) Studies using clinical cut-off on diagnostic assessments may miss children born preterm who show atypical presentations. Research suggests prematurity may impact development through distinct pathways, leading to distinct behavioural phenotypes and potentially alternate profiles of autism behaviour [24]. (iii) Studies using diagnostic assessments provide no quantification of those children who show sub-threshold difficulties with social communication, interaction and restricted repetitive behaviours. Screening tools are used more commonly in preterm populations as a method of risk stratification that is both cost and time effective [25]. Addressing these concerns is vital to ensure that prevalence rates are better understood in (i) all children born preterm, (ii) children with atypical presentations and (iii) children whose traits may cause significant difficulties, but may fall short of full diagnostic criteria identified via screening tools.

In summary, heightened rates of autism have been identified in individuals born preterm. While methods of early identification in high-risk samples have become more reliable, the precise profile of autism characteristics in individuals born preterm is not well documented. Most recent prevalence rates confirm that preterm populations are high-risk groups, yet meta-analytic approaches have excluded screening measures, meaning they have focussed mainly on very preterm groups, and those with the most typical and severe presentations. Therefore, the present systematic review and meta-analysis aim the following:

  • i.

    Synthesise existing literature and calculate pooled prevalence estimates for autism based on diagnostic and screening measures of autism characteristics in preterm samples.

  • ii.

    Compare pooled prevalence estimates in the preterm population with estimates of autism in the general population

  • iii.

    Identify participant characteristics that may be associated with autism characteristics in preterm samples

Method

Search strategy

Before a search was undertaken, the study was preregistered on PROSPERO (Available at: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42019125412). Search terms were generated from inspection of relevant high-impact papers and recent meta-analyses (See Table 1). Additionally, we hand-searched a recent meta-analysis to identify further publications [9].

Table 1.

Search terms used to search Ovid MEDLINE, Ovid PsychINFO, Ovid Embase, Ovid Embase Classic and PubMed from the beginning of creation to late January 2020

Search terms
#1 Autism “autis*”, “autism*”, “autistic*”, “ASD”, “autism spectrum disorder*”, “PDD-NOS”, “PDDNOS”, “ unspecified PDD”, “pervasive developmental disorder*”, “pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified”, “Asperger*”, and “Asperger* syndrome”. The premature search terms included premature*”, “preterm”, “prematur*”, “low birth weight”.
#2 Preterm “premature*”, “preterm”, “prematur*”, “low birth weight”.

Study selection

Initial searches returned 10,900 responses that were systematically assessed for suitability and inclusion (Fig. 1). An initial automatic ‘de-duplication’ process was run using EndNote software, with the corresponding researcher then manually inspecting for any that were missed. Papers were then assessed in three stages. For the first stage of selection, we used predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria to assess the titles/abstracts for inclusion. In the second stage, we reviewed papers against stage two criteria in full text (see Supplementary Table 1 & Supplementary Table 2 in supplementary materials for additional information). Finally, for the third stage, we rated all included papers against a multi-level quality matrix described below.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

A flow chart detailing papers included and excluded at each stage of screening and review

To eliminate the risk of researcher bias, two additional researchers screened a subsample of papers at both stages 1 and 2 outlined above (19%). They then completed quality ratings against the quality weighting framework for all included papers. A good level of reliability between the two independent researchers was obtained (weighted Cohen’s Kappa 0.7). Where discrepancies were identified, an agreement was made between the two raters.

Quality criteria

The quality of all studies that progressed to stage three of the review process was assessed using standardised quality weighting criteria to control for threats to validity. The quality effects model extends the random-effects model, allowing for papers rated as higher quality to be given more weight in estimates of prevalence three key areas were assessed; autism assessment, sample identification and study design (Table 2).

Table 2.

Quality Criteria for autism Assessment, Sample Identification and Study Design

0 – Poor 1 – Adequate 2 – Good 3 - Excellent
Autism Assessment

Not specified / reported

Clinical judgement only

Informant report / self-report instrument

Screening instrument

Clinical judgement against specified diagnostic criteria (DSM-5 or ICD 10)

Diagnostic instrument / interviews - Consensus from multiple assessments, including at least one diagnostic instrument
Sample Identification Not specified / reported Single restricted or non-random sample (specialist clinic or previous research study) Multiple restricted or non-random samples (multi-region specialist clinics) Random or total population sample
Study Design Not specified / reported Case series Historically identified cohort (e.g., via patient records) Prospective cohort

A visual matrix of quality weighting is presented in Table 3, alongside study characteristics and outcome data. From each paper, data were then extracted and the number of participants meeting cutoff for an autism diagnosis was taken.

Table 3.

Sample characteristics and quality criteria for included papers

Author Location Gestational age of sample Diagnostic tools used Sample Size No. preterm individuals scored over the autism threshold Quality Criteria
Autism Sample Identification Design
Abolfotouh et al, 2018 [26] Saudi Arabia 22-23wks (10) Denver developmental screening test 63

4

(Hyperactive autistic)

1 1 3
24-25wks (43)
26-28wks (52)
29-30wks (12)
Al-Hathlol et al., 2020 [27] Saudi Arabia < 32 weeks or < 1500 g Unspecified questionnaire 158 3 1 1 2
Atladottir et al, 2016 [28] Denmark 24–43 weeks Diagnosis was retrieved from the Danish Psychiatric Central Register 82,911 1203 1 3 2
Bakian et al, 2018 [29] Utah < 37 weeks Registry of Autism and Developmental Disabilities 4855

112

(ASD)

1 2 2
Boone et al, 2018 [30] Columbus < 30 weeks PDDST-II-DCS 528 190 3 2 3
ADOS-2 555

24

(ASD)

Bröring et al, 2018 [31] Amsterdam Very preterm (30.2 mean) SRS, Children’s Communication Checklist, SCQ 57 0 1 1 3
Brumbaugh et al., 2020 [32] Minnesota < 37 weeks Medical and educational records 7876 266 1 3 3
Chen et al., 2020 [33] Taiwan < 32 weeks or < 1500 g ADOS and ADI-R 324 30 3 2 3
De Groote et al, 2006 [34] Belgium < 37 weeks Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic 25

2

(Autism)

2 1 3
De Oliveira Holanda et al., 2020 [35] Brazil < 37 weeks MCHAT 40 20 1 1 3
Dudova et al, 2014 [36] Prague NR M-CHAT 157 28 3 2 3
ADOS 33 15
Gray et al, 2015 [37] Brisbane very preterm MCHAT, The CBCL & DASS 97

13

(Autism)

1 1 3
Guy et al, 2015 [23] East Midlands 32–36 weeks MCHAT & follow up phone interview 634 92 1 2 3
Hack et al, 2009 [38] America 26 weeks Parent Child Symptom Inventory 219 8 1 1 2
Harel-Gadassi et al., 2018 [39] Jerusalem, Israel 31.16 (mean) M-CHAT 93 25 3 1 3
ADOS-T 101 8
Hubert et al., 2020 [40] Poland GA Mean 27.8 Childhood autism spectrum test 89 5 1 1 3
Hvidtjorn et al., 2011 [41] Denmark < 37 Public child mental health service 37,283 277 1 2 3
Hwang et al, 2013 [42] Taiwan Late preterm = 1078, Later preterm = 28,947, Full-term = 1,104,071 Coded by doctors based on ICD-9-CM Early preterm = 1078, Later preterm = 28,947 Early preterm = 24, Later preterm = 387 1 2 3
Ikejiri et al, 2016 [43] Juntendo > 33 Weeks DSM-4-TR 59

9

(ASD)

1 1 3
Indredavik et al., 2004 [44] Norway GA mean: 28.8 Interviewed and conclusions drawn according to DSM 56 1 1 2 3
Johnson et al, 2010 [45] UK & Ireland < 26 SCQ 189 29 (ASD) 1 3 3
Johnson et al, 2018 [24] England 32 wks = 38 MCHAT 638 92 1 2 3
Joo et al., 2015 [46] Korea 24–36 CARS 58 1 2 2 3
Kihara et al., 2015 [47] Japan GA mean: 27.4 Clinical assessments & DSM criteria 321 35 1 2 3
Klimek et al., 2018 [48] Poland 28 weeks (Mean) The Childhood Autism Spectrum Test 86 5 1 1 3
Kuban et al 2009 [49] US before 28 weeks gestation MCHAT 988 212 1 2 3
Kuzniewicz et al, 2014 [50] California <  24 weeks ASD evaluation centre 15,696 280 3 2 3
Laerum et al., 2019 [51] Norway 28.9 weeks (mean) Autism Spectrum Quotient 59 21 1 1 3
Lean et al., 2020 [52] USA < 30 weeks ADOS & Parent report 85 11 3 1 3
Leavey et al, 2013 [53] California Diagnostic codes 33,121 213 1 2 2
Lederman et al, 2018 [54] São Paulo, Brazil 29.5 (mean) M-CHAT & Autism Behaviour Checklist 60 4 3 1 3
Limperopoulos, et al, 2008 [15] Boston NR MCHAT 91 23 1 1 3
Matheis et al, 2018 [55] Louisiana < 37 Weeks BISCUIT Part 1 687 213 1 2 2
Mir et al., 2020 [56] Texas <  28 weeks MCHAT 218 31
ADOS & CARS 218 16
Mohammed et al., 2016 [57] Saudi Arabia GA 27–33 Clinical assessments & DSM 107 5 1 1 3
Moore et al, 2012 [25] England NR MCHAT 523

216

(Positive result on the MCHAT Autism)

1 3 3
Nagai et al., 2020 [58] Japan VLBW < 1500 DSM-5 and ADOS 38 10 3 1 3
Persson et al., 2020 [59] Norway < 37 weeks Medical records 165,845 3544 1 3 2
Pineda et al., 2014 [60] USA < 30 weeks MCHAT 77 19 1 2 2
Pinto-Martin et al., 2011 [21] New Jersey GA mean:31.2 ADI-R/ADOS 623 14 2 2 3
SCQ 623 117
Pritchard et al, 2016 [61] Australia < 29 Weeks ADOS-G 15 3 2 1 3
M-CHAT 169 22
Rand., et al. 2016 [62] New Zealand < 32 DAWBA 102 3 1 2 3
Rutkowska, et al, 2018 [63] Poland < 28 Weeks

Screening Tool for Autism in Toddlers &

Young Children

10 4 1 1 1
Sharp et al., 2018 [64] Australia 22–24 wks Multidisciplinary team assessment 159 9 1 1 2
Stephens et al, 2012 [65]

NICHD Neonatal

Research Network

< 27 weeks PDDST-II & adapted items from the ADOS 554 Positive screen - 113 2 2 3
Sumanasena et al., 2018 [66] Sri Lanka < 34 DSM Criteria 39 3 1 2 3
Treyvaud et al, 2013 [14] Melbourne Australia, < 30 weeks DAWBA 177`

8

(ASD)

1 1 3
Twilhaar et al., 2019 [67] Amsterdam, Netherlands 29.2 weeks (mean) Social Responsiveness Scale 60 18 1 1 3
Verhaeghe et al, 2016 [68] Belgium Before 27 weeks SRS 47 21 3 2 3
ADOS, and The ADI-R 43 14
Vermeirsch et al., 2020 [69] Belgium <  30 weeks SRS 55 22
ADOS, ADI-R & clinical information 55 7
Yaari, et al, 2016 [70] Israel 24–34 weeks The AOSI and ADOS-T 99 High ASD risk – 8, Low ASD risk - 91
Yang et al., 2015 [71] Taiwan Mean BW 1200 g Diagnostic tools, observations and parental reports 61 2 1 2 3
You et al., 2019 [72] China 35.5 weeks (mean) M-CHAT 102 9 1 2 3

Statistical analysis

All estimated prevalence rates for autism in preterm samples were extracted from papers remaining in the final stage of review. These estimates were collated based upon the type of assessment tool used; screening tools or diagnostic assessments. These data were analysed to generate two pooled prevalence estimates, with random- and fixed-effects models created for both. Fixed-effects models assume equal weighting of studies, with any error attributed to sampling error, whereas random-effects models allow the true effect to vary between studies, with weighting fluctuating between studies [73]. To calculate the random-effects model within the current study, the restricted maximum-likelihood estimator was used. This estimator is more robust than traditional DerSimonian-Laird estimates in non-normal distributions of effect, as the method restricts the likelihood estimates to control for underestimation and minimise bias [74]. This decision was supported from analysing the Quantile-Quantile plots (see Supplementary Figure 1 in supplementary materials), which suggested the fixed-effects model did not conform to normal distribution. A quality effects model (QEM) for each assessment method was also produced to assess the impact of methodological variation as defined and weighted by the quality framework outlined above, and this quality effects estimate was compared to the random-effects estimate (see Supplementary Figure 2 in supplementary materials). In order to explore the prevalence of autism amongst individuals born preterm in comparison to rates amongst the general population, odds ratios (OR) with 95% CI were generated. This analysis compared the random-effects pooled prevalence estimates from diagnostic assessment methods with the most recent total population surveillance prevalence estimate for autism diagnosis (one in 54) [75]. This particular population surveillance was chosen given its use of gold standard assessments and diagnostic and statistical manual definitions to confirm the diagnosis. Data from this paper were also referenced by the CDC and others widely support the conservative estimate of 1 in 54 as a representative and inclusive population estimate.

Results

Prevalence of autism characteristics in individuals born preterm

52 studies were included in the final meta-analysis; 23 screening tools only, 22 direct assessment only and 7 both. Pooled prevalence estimates of autism using random- and fixed-effects models were generated for both screening tools and diagnostic assessment (See Figure 2).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Fixed- and random-effects pooled prevalence estimates for autism characteristics in individuals born preterm using screening and diagnostic tools

Screening tools

The fixed-effects model generated a weighted prevalence estimate of 11% (z = 31.89, p = <.001; 95% CI 0.1018; 0.1151%) for autism characteristics in individuals born preterm. The random-effects model generated a prevalence estimate of 20% (z = 7.97, p = <.001; 95% CI 14.98; 24.75%). The random-effects model extended with a quality weighting estimated a prevalence of 21% (z = 8.39, p = <.001; 95% CI 16.38; 26.37%).

Diagnostic assessments

The fixed-effects model generated a weighted prevalence estimate of 1% (z = 73.54, p = 0; 95%, CI 1.36; 1.43%) for autism characteristics in individuals born preterm. The random-effects model generated a prevalence estimate of 6% (z = 5.8, p = <.001; 95% CI 3.74; 7.57%). The random-effects model extended with a quality weighting estimated a prevalence of 9% (z = 8.17, p = <.001; 95% CI 7.19; 11.74%).

In summary, the prevalence of autism characteristics was explored in individuals born preterm for both screening tools and diagnostic assessments in turn, with estimates ranging across the models created. Both fixed-effects models revealed high levels of heterogeneity (I2 = 97.3–97.8%) indicating that the fixed-effects model is not appropriate given it could not be concluded that studies were conducted under similar conditions. The random- and quality-effects models that account for variability between studies and quality respectively produced estimates between 6 and 21% (screening tools; random-effects model - 20%; quality effects model - 21%; diagnostic assessments; random-effects model - 6%; quality effects model - 9%).

Sources of heterogeneity

Analyses investigating influential studies did not identify any differences associated with increased prevalence rates (see Supplementary Figure 3 in supplementary materials).

Publication bias

Visual inspection of funnel plots created to detect publication bias showed a broad symmetrical distribution. Given the subjective nature of the visual inspection, linear regression analysis was conducted to statistically test for asymmetry [76]. A non-significant result (p = 1.494) suggested no evidence of publication bias, exaggerated estimates of smaller studies or the inclusion of poor quality studies.

Comparing pooled prevalence estimates in the preterm population with estimates of autism in the general population

To explore the prevalence of autism amongst individuals born preterm in comparison to rates amongst the general population, odds ratios (OR) with 95% CI were generated. In the case of diagnostic assessments, OR analysis suggested the odds of an autism diagnosis were 3.3 times higher in individuals born preterm than in the general population (95% CI 0.24–47.60).

To identify participant characteristics that may be associated with autism characteristics in preterm samples

To explore participant characteristics associated with autism characteristics, the influence of participants’ age at assessment and gestational age on prevalence rates were assessed using a meta-regression (Table 4). Both meta-regressions revealed no significant associations between age at assessment (screening tools, p =.118; diagnostic assessment, p =.579) or mean gestational age (screening tools, p =.192; diagnostic assessment, p =.579) and prevalence of autism characteristics. Due to insufficient data, it was not possible to assess the profile of autism characteristics in individuals born preterm and consider how this differed from individuals born at term.

Table 4.

Participant characteristics influencing the prevalence of autism diagnosis in individuals born preterm

Covariate Estimate S.E. Z p Lower 95%CI Upper 95%CI
Screening tools
 Age at assessment −0.00010 0.0005 −1.9172 .0552 −0.0021 0.0000
 Gestational age −0.0080 0.0144 −0.5546 .5791 −0.0362 0.0202
Diagnostic tools
 Age at assessment −0.00005 0.0004 −1.3022 .1928 −0.0012 0.0002
 Gestational age −0.0175 0.0112 −1.5602 .1187 −0.0394 0.0045

Discussion

The prevalence of autism characteristics in individuals born preterm was systematically reviewed and meta-analysed. This was the first study to meta-analyse data across both diagnostic and screening tools. Due to the inclusion bias present in studies using diagnostic assessments, this meta-analysis therefore provides novel estimates of prevalence across gestational age. The use of a robust and standardised search strategy improves the accuracy of estimates, with stringent inclusion and exclusion criteria considerably enhancing the internal validity of findings. Overall, autism characteristics were estimated to have a prevalence rate of between 6 and 20% for individuals born preterm, dependent upon assessment method used. The odds of an autism diagnosis were 3.3 times higher in individuals born preterm than in the general population. Both meta-regressions revealed no association between age at assessment or gestational age and autism characteristics in preterm samples. Results have significant clinical importance for both individuals born preterm and the provision of services.

Autism characteristics were present in 6–20% of preterm individuals in the current study. This is considerably higher than general population estimates (1.5%; [75]) and consistent with a recent meta-analysis of autism in preterm individuals that included only diagnostic tools (7%; [9]). The current study confirms and extends the findings that preterm individuals are more likely to have difficulties consistent with an autism diagnosis and also show heightened rates of autism characteristics compared to the general population.

The inclusion of data from papers that used screening tools is a key strength of the current study, as these prevalence estimates capture a broader range of autism characteristics. One consideration when comparing screening measures to diagnostic assessments is that the differing tools are perhaps asking different questions; one surrounding the presence and levels of autism characteristics while the other only identifies those that reach diagnostic cutoff. It has previously been suggested that screening measures are not as valid as diagnostic assessments [77], over-identifying the individual likelihood of autism [78]. Nevertheless, screening measures are increasingly used to help reduce clinical waiting lists and enable earlier access to support, which has been shown to improve outcomes [79]. The results from this meta-analysis suggest that 1 in 5 children born preterm warrants further assessment for autism, and as such, future research should quantify the extent to which screening tools within preterm samples generate prevalence estimates similar to diagnostic tools through the consideration of their psychometric properties.

High levels of heterogeneity were identified during analysis (I2 = 97.3–97.8%). A potential reason for the high heterogeneity could be the variability in both sample identification and populations studied. Studies with poorly identified samples (inclusive of all gestational ages) provided lower prevalence estimates of autism characteristics, while studies with well-defined samples consistently comprised individuals born very preterm, where rates of autism characteristics identified are notably higher than those born closer to term [80]. This has wide implications for future research; studies of neurodevelopmental outcomes should adhere to classifying groups using standard definitions of prematurity and consider these outcomes across all gestational ages. While samples continuously use differing definitions of prematurity, the ability to assess these groups robustly decreases.

To explore the influence of gestational age on prevalence estimates of autism characteristics, both meta-regressions were conducted on available data. No significant associations were found between the gestational age of participants and autism characteristics. Previous research has highlighted an increase in autism prevalence amongst those born very preterm when compared to those born closer to term [45, 61]. Although the results of the current analysis suggest no significant difference, caution must be taken with interpreting this result. A noteworthy limitation of this analysis was the lack of power and clarity in sample descriptions reporting effects across included studies. As mentioned previously, the most well-defined populations are often those born very preterm, with most of the current studies simply providing a mean gestational age. It is vital future research clearly defines participant groups so that outcomes can be rigorously stratified.

Further analysis was conducted to explore any mediating effect age of assessment had upon autism prevalence estimates, with no significant associations found. Research highlights the reliability of diagnosis as young as 24 months [81], and the beneficial impact this can have upon service use and support [82], the current study did not identify a significant difference in prevalence based upon the age of assessment, although more research would be needed to confidently rule out any effect of age. While this could suggest the stability of autism across the lifespan, there was again a large amount of missing data surrounding the age of participants at assessments. Research has highlighted the importance of understanding the early behavioural phenotype of preterm infants, as early behaviours and increased likelihood could all result in a later diagnosis of autism [70]. In line with World Health Organization (WHO) guidance on routine early medical and developmental follow-up after preterm birth [83], and with evidence that poorer outcomes can be identified throughout the lifespan, it is important future research focuses on providing assessments and intervening at a younger age.

Due to insufficient data, it was not possible to assess the profile of autism characteristics in individuals born preterm and consider how this differed from autism characteristics in individuals born at term. This highlights a substantial gap in the current literature, as subscale scores for social communication and restricted repetitive domains are consistently not reported, despite diagnostic and screening tools providing these outputs. While many studies utilised standardised assessment methods that are well validated, the omission of reported subscale scores significantly impacts any conclusions that can be drawn from them. Recent literature highlights that individual characteristics and traits (often measured separately through subscales of measures) are often deemed more important than an overall ‘autism severity’ score [84]. Similarly, current interventions target specific social cognitive differences and or divergent social behaviours evidenced in individuals with autism characteristics instead of a global level [8588]. Without precise documentation of the profile of autism evidenced by individuals born preterm, it is not yet clear if interventions created for use in term populations with autism can be successfully used for individuals born preterm.

Conclusion

The results of this meta-analysis are of significant clinical importance. Following the publication of the WHO recommendations for improving outcomes of those born preterm, neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm infants are considered to be of particular importance [69]. Services offered to preterm individuals are lacking, with further support deemed necessary in areas such as infant neurodevelopment as well as more specific domains such as feeding and sleeping. Current data show considerably elevated prevalence of autism characteristics in individuals born preterm; it is therefore vital that services providers reflect this increased likelihood in the support and professional follow-up they offer.

Supplementary Information

11689_2021_9382_MOESM1_ESM.docx (3.1MB, docx)

Additional file 1. Supplementary analysis and figures provided for additional clarity.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Authors’ contributions

CL, AS and CR conceptualised and designed the study. CL, RoS and DS did the literature screening, assessed study eligibility and quality and analysed the data. CL and CJ contributed to the statistical analysis. CL, AS and CR contributed to writing the manuscript. The authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.

Authors’ information

Not applicable.

Funding

Not applicable.

Availability of data and materials

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article.

Declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Catherine Laverty, Email: cml704@student.bham.ac.uk.

Andrew Surtees, Email: A.Surtees@bham.ac.uk.

Rory O’Sullivan, Email: R.OSullivan-17@student.lboro.ac.uk.

Daniel Sutherland, Email: DLS740@student.bham.ac.uk.

Christopher Jones, Email: C.A.Jones@bham.ac.uk.

Caroline Richards, Email: C.R.Richards@bham.ac.uk.

References

  • 1.Howson CP, Kinney MV, McDougall L, Lawn JE. Born toon soon: preterm birth matters. Reprod Health. 2013;10(1):1–9. doi: 10.1186/1742-4755-10-S1-S1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Eisfeld J. International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems. Transgender Stud Q. 2014;1(1-2):107–110. doi: 10.1215/23289252-2399740. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Escobar GJ, McCormick MC, Zupancic JAF, Coleman-Phox K, Armstrong MA, Greene JD, et al. Unstudied infants: outcomes of moderately premature infants in the neonatal intensive care unit. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2006;91:F238. doi: 10.1136/adc.2005.087031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Johnson S. Cognitive and behavioural outcomes following very preterm birth. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2007;12:363–373. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Soleimani F, Zaheri F, Abdi F. Long-term neurodevelopmental outcomes after preterm birth. Iran Red Crescent Med J. 2014;16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 6.Bilgin A, Mendonca M, Wolke D. Preterm birth/low birth eight and markers reflective of wealth in adulthood: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2018;142:e20173625. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-3625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Saigal S, Doyle LW. An overview of mortality and sequelae of preterm birth from infancy to adulthood. Lancet. 2008;371:261–269. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60136-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Singh GK, Kenney MK, Ghandour RM, Kogan MD, Lu MC. Mental health outcomes in US children and adolescents born prematurely or with low birthweight. Depress Res Treat. 2013;2013:1. doi: 10.1155/2013/570743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Agrawal S, Rao SC, Bulsara MK, Patole SK. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder in preterm infants: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2018;142:e20180134. doi: 10.1542/peds.2018-0134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.American Psychiatric Association . Cautionary statement for forensic use of DSM-5. 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wong HS, Huertas-Ceballos A, Cowan FM, Modi N. Evaluation of early childhood social-communication difficulties in children born preterm using the quantitative checklist for autism in toddlers. J Pediatr. 2014;164:26. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Moore V, Goodson S. How well does early diagnosis of autism stand the test of time? Follow-up study of children assessed for autism at age 2 and development of an early diagnostic service. Autism. 2003;7:47–63. doi: 10.1177/1362361303007001005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Happé F, Ronald A, Plomin R. Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:1218–1220. doi: 10.1038/nn1770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Treyvaud K, Ure A, Doyle LW, Lee KJ, Rogers CE, Kidokoro H, et al. Psychiatric outcomes at age seven for very preterm children: rates and predictors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2013;54:772–779. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Limperopoulos C, Bassan H, Sullivan NR, Soul JS, Robertson RL, Moore M, et al. Positive screening for autism in ex-preterm infants: prevalence and risk factors. Pediatrics. 2008;121:758–765. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Blencowe H, Cousens S, Chou D, Oestergaard M, Say L, Moller AB, et al. Born too soon: the global epidemiology of 15 million preterm births. Reprod Health. 2013;10(1):1. doi: 10.1186/1742-4755-10-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lantos JD, Lauderdale DS. Late preterm birth. Preterm Babies Fetal Patients Childbear Choices. 2018;3:10–19. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/10389.003.0006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Office for National Statistics . Statistical bulletin birth characteristics in England and. 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Frey HA, Klebanoff MA. The epidemiology, etiology, and costs of preterm birth. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2016;21:68–73. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2015.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Morgan JC, Boyle EM. The late preterm infant. Paediatr Child Health. 2018;28:13–17. doi: 10.1016/j.paed.2017.10.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Pinto-Martin JA, Levy SE, Feldman JF, Lorenz JM, Paneth N, Whitaker AH. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder in adolescents born weighing <2000 grams. Pediatrics. 2011;128:883–891. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-2846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Joseph RM, O’Shea TM, Allred EN, Heeren T, Hirtz D, Paneth N, et al. Prevalence and associated features of autism spectrum disorder in extremely low gestational age newborns at age 10 years. Autism Res. 2017;10:224–232. doi: 10.1002/aur.1644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Guy A, Seaton SE, Boyle EM, Draper ES, Field DJ, Manktelow BN, et al. Infants born late/moderately preterm are at increased risk for a positive autism screen at 2 years of age. J Pediatr. 2015;166:269–275.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2014.10.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Johnson S, Waheed G, Manktelow BN, Field DJ, Marlow N, Draper ES, et al. Differentiating the preterm phenotype: distinct profiles of cognitive and behavioral development following late and moderately preterm birth. J Pediatr. 2018;193:85–92.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Moore T, Johnson S, Hennessy E, Marlow N. Screening for autism in extremely preterm infants: problems in interpretation. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2012;54:514–520. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04265.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Abolfotouh MA, Al Saif S, Altwaijri WA, Al Rowaily MA. Prospective study of early and late outcomes of extremely low birthweight in Central Saudi Arabia. BMC Pediatr. 2018;18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 27.Al-Hathlol K, Al-Obaid OM, Al-Gholaiqa TS, Al-Hathlol B, Abdulaal AE, Al-Hajress RI, et al. School performance and long-term outcomes of very preterm children conceived via in vitro fertilization. J Bras Reprod Assist. 2020;24:61–65. doi: 10.5935/1518-0557.20190063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Atladóttir HO, Schendel DE, Henriksen TB, Hjort L, Parner ET. Gestational age and autism spectrum disorder: trends in risk over time. Autism Res. 2016;9:224–231. doi: 10.1002/aur.1525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bakian AV, Bilder DA, Korgenski EK, Bonkowsky JL. Autism spectrum disorder and neonatal serum magnesium levels in preterm infants. Child Neurol Open. 2018;5:2329048X1880056. doi: 10.1177/2329048X18800566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Boone KM, Brown AK, Keim SA. Screening accuracy of the brief infant toddler social-emotional assessment to identify autism spectrum disorder in toddlers born at less than 30 weeks’ gestation. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2018;49:493–504. doi: 10.1007/s10578-017-0768-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bröring T, Oostrom KJ, van Dijk-Lokkart EM, Lafeber HN, Brugman A, Oosterlaan J. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder symptoms in school-age children born very preterm. Res Dev Disabil. 2018;74:103–112. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Brumbaugh JE, Weaver AL, Myers SM, Voigt RG, Katusic SK. Gestational age, perinatal characteristics, and autism spectrum disorder: a birth cohort study. J Pediatr. 2020;220:175–183.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2020.01.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Chen LW, Wang ST, Wang LW, Kao YC, Chu CL, Wu CC, et al. Early neurodevelopmental trajectories for autism spectrum disorder in children born very preterm. Pediatrics. 2020;146. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 34.De Groote I, Roeyers H, Warreyn P. Social-communicative abilities in young high-risk preterm children. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2006;18:183–200. doi: 10.1007/s10882-006-9011-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.De Oliveira Holanda NS, Da Costa LDO, Santos Sampaio SS, Da Fonseca Filho GG, Bezerra RB, Azevedo IG, et al. Screening for autism spectrum disorder in premature subjects hospitalized in a neonatal intensive care unit. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17:1–8. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17207675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Dudova I, Markova D, Kasparova M, Zemankova J, Beranova S, Urbanek T, et al. Comparison of three screening tests for autism in preterm children with birth weights less than 1,500 grams. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2014;10:2201–2208. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S72921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Gray PH, Edwards DM, O’Callaghan MJ, Gibbons K. Screening for autism spectrum disorder in very preterm infants during early childhood. Early Hum Dev. 2015;91:271–276. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hack M, Taylor HG, Schluchter M, Andreias L, Drotar D, Klein N. Behavioral outcomes of extremely low birth weight children at age 8 years. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2009;30:122–130. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31819e6a16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Harel-Gadassi A, Friedlander E, Yaari M, Bar-Oz B, Eventov-Friedman S, Mankuta D, et al. Risk for ASD in preterm infants: a three-year follow-up study. Autism Res Treat. 2018;2018:1–9. doi: 10.1155/2018/8316212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hubert J, Gilarska M, Klimek M, Nitecka M, Dutkowska G, Kwinta P. Small for gestational age is an independent risk factor for neurodevelopmental impairment. Iran J Pediatr. 2020;30:1–8. doi: 10.5812/ijp.84628. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hvidtjørn D, Grove J, Schendel D, Schieve LA, Sværke C, Ernst E, et al. Risk of autism spectrum disorders in children born after assisted conception: a population-based follow-up study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2011;65:497–502. doi: 10.1136/jech.2009.093823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hwang YS, Weng SF, Cho CY, Tsai WH. Higher prevalence of autism in Taiwanese children born prematurely: a nationwide population-based study. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:2462–2468. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ikejiri K, Hosozawa M, Mitomo S, Tanaka K, Shimizu T. Reduced growth during early infancy in very low birth weight children with autism spectrum disorder. Early Hum Dev. 2016;98:23–27. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2016.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Indredavik MS, Vik T, Heyerdahl S, Kulseng S, Fayers P, Brubakk AM. Psychiatric symptoms and disorders in adolescents with low birth weight. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2004;89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 45.Johnson S, Hollis C, Kochhar P, Hennessy E, Wolke D, Marlow N. Autism spectrum disorders in extremely preterm children. J Pediatr. 2010;156. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 46.Joo JW, Choi JY, Rha DW, Kwak EH, Park ES. Neuropsychological outcomes of preterm birth in children with no major neurodevelopmental impairments in early life. Ann Rehabil Med. 2015;39:676–685. doi: 10.5535/arm.2015.39.5.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kihara H, Nakamura T. Early standard development assessment characteristics in very low birth weight infants later classified with autism spectrum disorder. Early Hum Dev. 2015;91:357–359. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Klimek M, Nitecka M, Dutkowska G, Gilarska M, Kwinta P. Temperament traits in 4-year-old children born prematurely – may they suggest a threat for mental functioning? Psychiatr Pol. 2018;52:371–386. doi: 10.12740/PP/OnlineFirst/66229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Kuban KCK, O’Shea TM, Allred EN, Tager-Flusberg H, Goldstein DJ, Leviton A. Positive screening on the modified checklist for autism in toddlers (M-CHAT) in extremely low gestational age newborns. J Pediatr. 2009;154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 50.Kuzniewicz MW, Wi S, Qian Y, Walsh EM, Armstrong MA, Croen LA. Prevalence and neonatal factors associated with autism spectrum disorders in preterm infants. J Pediatr. 2014;164:20–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.09.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lærum AMW, Reitan SK, Evensen KAI, Lydersen S, Brubakk AM, Skranes J, et al. Psychiatric symptoms and risk factors in adults born preterm with very low birthweight or born small for gestational age at term. BMC Psychiatry. 2019;19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 52.Lean RE, Lessov-Shlaggar CN, Gerstein ED, Smyser TA, Paul RA, Smyser CD, et al. Maternal and family factors differentiate profiles of psychiatric impairments in very preterm children at age 5-years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2020;61:157–166. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Leavey A, Zwaigenbaum L, Heavner K, Burstyn I. Gestational age at birth and risk of autism spectrum disorders in Alberta, Canada. J Pediatr. 2013;162:361–368. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.07.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Lederman VRG, Goulart AL, dos Santos AMN, Schwartzman JS. Rastreamento de sinais sugestivos de TEA em prematuros com muito baixo peso ao nascer. Psicol - Teor e Prática. 2018;20.
  • 55.Matheis M, Matson JL, Burns CO. Premature birth, low birth weight, and positive screening for autism spectrum disorder in an early intervention sample. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2018;30:689–705. doi: 10.1007/s10882-018-9613-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Mir IN, White SP, Steven Brown L, Heyne R, Rosenfeld CR, Chalak LF. Autism spectrum disorders in extremely preterm infants and placental pathology findings: a matched case–control study. Pediatr Res. 2020. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 57.Mohammed HS, Wahass SH, Mahmoud AA. Incidence of autism in high risk neonatal follow up. Neurosciences. 2016;21:43–46. doi: 10.17712/nsj.2016.1.20150471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Nagai Y, Nomura K, Uemura O. Primitive reflexes in very low birth weight infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Minerva Pediatr. 2020. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 59.Persson M, Opdahl S, Risnes K, Gross R, Kajantie E, Reichenberg A, et al. Gestational age and the risk of autism spectrum disorder in Sweden, Finland, and Norway: a cohort study. PLoS Med. 2020;17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 60.Pineda RG, Neil J, Dierker D, Smyser CD, Wallendorf M, Kidokoro H, et al. Alterations in brain structure and neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm infants hospitalized in different neonatal intensive care unit environments. J Pediatr. 2014;164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 61.Pritchard MA, De Dassel T, Beller E, Bogossian F, Johnston L, Paynter J, et al. Autism in toddlers born very preterm. Pediatrics. 2016;137. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 62.Rand KM, Austin NC, Inder TE, Bora S, Woodward LJ. Neonatal infection and later neurodevelopmental risk in the very preterm infant. J Pediatr. 2016;170:97–104. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Rutkowska M, Bekiesińska-Figatowska M, Kmita G, Terczyńska I, Polak K, Kalisiak M, et al. Neuroimaging results, short-term assessment of psychomotor development and the risk of autism spectrum disorder in extremely premature infants (≤28 GA) - a prospective cohort study (preliminary report) Dev Period Med. 2018;22:39–48. doi: 10.34763/devperiodmed.20182201.3948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Sharp M, French N, McMichael J, Campbell C. Survival and neurodevelopmental outcomes in extremely preterm infants 22–24 weeks of gestation born in Western Australia. J Paediatr Child Health. 2018;54:188–193. doi: 10.1111/jpc.13678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Stephens BE, Bann CM, Watson VE, Sheinkopf SJ, Peralta-Carcelen M, Bodnar A, et al. Screening for autism spectrum disorders in extremely preterm infants. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2012;33:535–541. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31825fd0af. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Sumanasena SP, Vipulaguna DV, Mendis MM, Gunawardena NS. Beyond survival: 5-year neurodevelopmental follow-up of a cohort of preterm infants in Colombo, Sri Lanka. Paediatr Int Child Health. 2018;38:128–136. doi: 10.1080/20469047.2017.1380944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Twilhaar ES, de Kieviet JF, Bergwerff CE, Finken MJJ, van Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J. Social adjustment in adolescents born very preterm: evidence for a cognitive basis of social problems. J Pediatr. 2019;213:66–73.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2019.06.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Verhaeghe L, Dereu M, Warreyn P, De Groote I, Vanhaesebrouck P, Roeyers H. Extremely preterm born children at very high risk for developing autism spectrum disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016;47:729–739. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0606-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Vermeirsch J, Verhaeghe L, Casaer A, Faes F, Oostra A, Roeyers H. Diagnosing autism spectrum disorder in toddlers born very preterm: estimated prevalence and usefulness of screeners and the autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS) J Autism Dev Disord. 2021;51:1508–1527. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04573-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Yaari M, Yitzhak N, Harel A, Friedlander E, Bar-Oz B, Eventov-Friedman S, et al. Stability of early risk assessment for autism spectrum disorder in preterm infants. Autism. 2016;20:856–867. doi: 10.1177/1362361315614758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Yang P, Chen YH, Yen CF, Chen HL. Psychiatric diagnoses, emotional–behavioral symptoms and functional outcomes in adolescents born preterm with very low birth weights. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2014;46:358–366. doi: 10.1007/s10578-014-0475-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.You J, Shamsi BH, Hao MC, Cao CH, Yang WY. A study on the neurodevelopment outcomes of late preterm infants. BMC Neurol. 2019;19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 73.Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR. A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res Synth Methods. 2010;1:97–111. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Cheung MWL. Implementing restricted maximum likelihood estimation in structural equation models. Struct Equ Model. 2013;20:157–167. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2013.742404. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Maenner MJ, Shaw KA, Baio J, Washington A, Patrick M, DiRienzo M, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years-autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2016. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2020;69:1–12. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6904a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C. Papers bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 77.Charman T, Gotham K. Measurement issues: screening and diagnostic instruments for autism spectrum disorders - lessons from research and practise. Child Adolesc Mental Health. 2013;18:52–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-3588.2012.00664.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Kim SH, Joseph RM, Frazier JA, O’Shea TM, Chawarska K, Allred EN, et al. Predictive validity of the modified checklist for autism in toddlers (M-CHAT) born very preterm. J Pediatr. 2016;178:101–107.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.07.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Landa RJ, Holman KC, Garrett-Mayer E. Social and communication development in toddlers with early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:853–864. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Fenoglio A, Georgieff MK, Elison JT. Social brain circuitry and social cognition in infants born preterm. J Neurodev Disord. 2017;9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 81.Johnson CP, Myers SM, Lipkin PH, Cartwright JD, Desch LW, Duby JC, et al. Identification and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics. 2007;120:1183–1215. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Daniels AM, Mandell DS. Explaining differences in age at autism spectrum disorder diagnosis: a critical review. Autism. 2014;18:583–597. doi: 10.1177/1362361313480277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.WHO. WHO recommendations on interventions to improve preterm birth outcomes. Geneva; 2015. Available from: www.who.int/reproductivehealth [PubMed]
  • 84.Robertson SM. Neurodiversity, quality of life, and autistic adults: shifting research and professional focuses onto real-life challenges. Disabil Stud Q. 2009;30.
  • 85.Wallace KS, Rogers SJ. Erratum: intervening in infancy: implications for autism spectrum disorders (journal of child psychology and psychiatry (2010) 51 (1300-1320)) J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2011;52:627. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02377.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Whalen C, Schreibman L, Ingersoll B. The collateral effects of joint attention training on social initiations, positive affect, imitation, and spontaneous speech for young children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:655–664. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0108-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Rogers SJ, Williams JHG. Imitation in autism findings and controversies. Imitation Soc Mind Autism Typ Dev. 2006:277–309.
  • 88.Young RL, Brewer N, Pattison C. Parental identification of early behavioural abnormalities in children with autistic disorder. Autism. 2003;7:125–143. doi: 10.1177/1362361303007002002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

11689_2021_9382_MOESM1_ESM.docx (3.1MB, docx)

Additional file 1. Supplementary analysis and figures provided for additional clarity.

Data Availability Statement

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article.


Articles from Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES