Skip to main content
Nature Portfolio logoLink to Nature Portfolio
. 2021 Mar 15;5(9):1226–1239. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01069-w

The genetic architecture of structural left–right asymmetry of the human brain

Zhiqiang Sha 1, Dick Schijven 1, Amaia Carrion-Castillo 1, Marc Joliot 2, Bernard Mazoyer 2, Simon E Fisher 1,3, Fabrice Crivello 2, Clyde Francks 1,3,
PMCID: PMC8455338  PMID: 33723403

Abstract

Left–right hemispheric asymmetry is an important aspect of healthy brain organization for many functions including language, and it can be altered in cognitive and psychiatric disorders. No mechanism has yet been identified for establishing the human brain’s left–right axis. We performed multivariate genome-wide association scanning of cortical regional surface area and thickness asymmetries, and subcortical volume asymmetries, using data from 32,256 participants from the UK Biobank. There were 21 significant loci associated with different aspects of brain asymmetry, with functional enrichment involving microtubule-related genes and embryonic brain expression. These findings are consistent with a known role of the cytoskeleton in left–right axis determination in other organs of invertebrates and frogs. Genetic variants associated with brain asymmetry overlapped with those associated with autism, educational attainment and schizophrenia. Comparably large datasets will likely be required in future studies, to replicate and further clarify the associations of microtubule-related genes with variation in brain asymmetry, behavioural and psychiatric traits.

Subject terms: Genome-wide association studies, Magnetic resonance imaging, Behavioural genetics, Genetics of the nervous system, Psychiatric disorders


Genes associated with brain asymmetry are involved with the internal skeletons of cells, and are especially active in the embryo. Some of these genes are associated with handedness and risk for psychiatric disorders.

Main

The human brain is characterized by various population-level asymmetries on its left–right axis1, including an overall ‘torque’ whereby the left hemisphere extends posteriorly and ventrally relative to the right, a left–right difference in frontal–occipital gradients of cortical thickness2, and hemispheric differences of morphology around the Sylvian fissure3. Many brain functions are also lateralized, including hand motor control and language which show left-hemisphere dominance in roughly 85% of people413. Altered brain or behavioural asymmetries have been reported in various cognitive and psychiatric disorders7,1417, which suggests that population-typical asymmetries are linked to optimal human brain function.

Behavioural and anatomical brain asymmetries are already apparent in utero1,1820, which indicates an early genetic-developmental programme of brain left–right axis formation21,22. Studies of visceral organ development (the heart, stomach, liver, etc.) have revealed that the generation of population-level asymmetry requires at least three important steps in the early embryo23,24: (1) the breaking of bilateral symmetry to create a left–right axis in a consistent orientation relative to the anterior–posterior and dorsal–ventral axes, (2) the triggering of different patterns of gene expression on the left and right sides of early embryonic structures and (3) the translation of asymmetric gene expression into lateralized morphology and organ placement.

In principle, establishing an embryonic left–right axis requires chirality at some level, that is, key biomolecules or cellular structures that exist in only one of two possible mirror forms. Life on Earth is based on L-form amino acids rather than the mirror D-form, and this chirality carries through to the macrostructure and movement of primary cilia25,26, which help to create the left–right axis of the visceral organs in embryos25. However, hemispheric dominances for language and hand motor control do not typically reverse in people with situs inversus of the viscera, that is, reversal of the visceral organs on the left–right axis, when caused by mutations in genes that encode primary ciliary components2730. This observation indicates that there are distinct and possibly organ-intrinsic mechanisms at play in brain development, but such mechanisms remain unidentified. Therefore the genetic origins of human brain asymmetry remain unknown.

Post mortem studies that contrasted gene expression between the left and right sides of the embryonic central nervous system have identified molecular pathways that may be involved, including that the two sides may transition through developmental stages slightly out of synchrony with each other3133. However, these studies were necessarily based on data from just handfuls of samples, because of limited availability arising from appropriate ethical, legal and practical concerns. An alternative approach to identify genetic influences on brain asymmetry is to relate genomic variation in large population datasets to variation in adult brain asymmetry. Only three loci have previously been reported at a genome-wide significant level to affect variation in adult human brain asymmetries, in studies targeted at single features of temporal lobe anatomy, and they did not yield broader insights into biological pathways34,35. Here we made use of the unprecedented sample size available through the 2020 release of the UK Biobank magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data, in combination with genome-wide genotype data, to perform the first multivariate, brain-wide genetic analysis of human brain anatomical asymmetry. The results from this analysis also allowed us to test whether genetic polymorphisms that are associated with variation in brain asymmetry are also associated with neurodevelopmental disorders or other behavioural and psychological traits, using publicly available genome-wide association scan (GWAS) summary statistics for these traits.

Results

Heritabilities and genetic correlations of brain regional asymmetry measures

For each of 32,256 participants with post-quality-control MRI and genetic data (Methods), and each of 73 bilaterally paired regional measures of brain structure, we calculated hemispheric asymmetry indexes (AI) as (left − right)/((left + right)/2) (for 33 cortical surface area AIs, 33 cortical thickness AIs and 7 subcortical volume AIs; Supplementary Table 1). The measures were derived from cortical parcellation and subcortical segmentation of structural brain images (Methods). All but one of the regional mean AIs were significantly different from zero, indicating population-level asymmetries (Bonferroni-corrected P < 0.05, Supplementary Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table 2), consistent with previous reports5,6. For example, some language-related regions showed greater average left than right surface areas, including superior temporal and supramarginal cortex, and pars opercularis.

GCTA36 software was used to estimate the single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP)-based heritability (h2) for each AI, that is, the extent to which variance in each AI was linked to common genetic variation over the entire genome (Methods). Forty-two AIs showed significant SNP-based heritabilities (false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected P < 0.05), that is, 28 of the surface area AIs, 8 cortical thickness AIs and 6 subcortical volume AIs (Fig. 1a and Supplementary Table 3), ranging from 2.2% for the AI of entorhinal cortical thickness to 9.4% for the AI of superior temporal surface area. The overall pattern was consistent with previous, twin-based heritability analyses5,6.

Fig. 1. SNP-based heritability and correlation analysis of regional brain asymmetry measures.

Fig. 1

a, SNP-based heritability estimates for brain asymmetry measures. Only regions for which AIs were significantly heritable are indicated in colour. b, Genetic and phenotypic correlations between AIs. Phenotypic (upper right triangle) and genetic (lower left triangle) correlations between each pair of AIs. Only significantly heritable AIs that also have at least one significant phenotypic or genetic correlation after FDR correction are shown. The colours of the squares indicate the correlation coefficients according to the colour key, and their areas are proportional to the correlation coefficients.

SNP-based genetic correlation analysis (again using GCTA software) indicated overlapping genetic contributions to some of the AIs (Fig. 1b, Supplementary Fig. 2 and Supplementary Tables 410). Within a small number of cortical regions (Supplementary Table 10), surface area and thickness AIs had negative genetic correlations, which indicates that variants can have antagonistic effects on surface and thickness asymmetries of these regions.

Multivariate genome-wide association analysis

We performed a multivariate (mv)GWAS for 9,803,522 SNPs, using meta-canonical correlation analysis as implemented in MetaPhat37, with the 42 AIs that had significant SNP-based heritability. This analysis tested each SNP separately for its simultaneous associations with all 42 AIs (Methods). A multivariate approach had the dual advantages of achieving data reduction and increasing statistical power compared with running 42 separate univariate GWAS. FUMA38 software was used to clump mvGWAS results on the basis of linkage disequilibrium (LD), and identify lead SNPs (maximally associated SNPs) at each associated locus. There were 21 distinct genomic loci at the 5 × 10−8 significance level associated with different aspects of brain asymmetry (Fig. 2, Table 1 and Supplementary Fig. 3), represented by 27 independent lead SNPs (with pairwise LD r2 < 0.1, Table 1).

Fig. 2. Multivariate GWAS analysis of regional brain asymmetries in 32,256 participants.

Fig. 2

Manhattan plot for multivariate GWAS across asymmetries of surface area, cortical thickness and subcortical volumes. The red dashed line indicates the significance threshold P < 5 × 10−8 (Methods). The Q–Q plot is also shown.

Table 1.

Genomic loci associated with brain asymmetries on multivariate analysis. All lead SNPs are shown

Genomic locus Lead SNP Position Functional category Effect allele Effect allele frequency mvGWAS r value mvGWAS P value Nearest gene Central asymmetry indexesa
1 rs6658111 1p33 Intergenic G 0.37 0.0631 9.75 × 10−11 AL356458.1 Parahippocampal (SA), superior frontal (SA), parahippocampal (CT)
2 rs62130503 2p23.3 NcRNA_intronicb T 0.05 0.0630 1.22 × 10−10 MAPRE3 Thalamus (SUB), parahippocampal (SA)
2 rs12617392 2p23.3 Intronic A 0.44 0.0638 4.02 × 10−11 CGREF1 Inferior temporal (SA), caudal anterior cingulate (SA), isthmus of cingulate (SA)
3 rs368536282c 2q34 Intergenic T 0.03 0.0631 1.07 × 10−10 MAP2 Superior frontal (SA), accumbens (SUB), posterior cingulate (CT)
4 rs2279829 3q24 3′ UTRd T 0.22 0.0613 1.26 × 10−9 ZIC4 Isthmus of cingulate (CT), precuneus (SA), posterior cingulate (CT), fusiform (SA)
5 rs9307052c 4q22.1 Intronic T 0.11 0.0591 2.27 × 10−8 FAM13A Rostral anterior cingulate (CT), posterior cingulate (CT), medial orbitofrontal (SA)
6 rs869219775 5q15 Intergenic T 0.14 0.0606 3.06 × 10−9 NR2F1 Inferior parietal (SA), transverse temporal (SA)
7 rs7781 6p21.33 Downstream G 0.24 0.0628 1.62 × 10−10 TUBB Isthmus of cingulate (CT), rostral anterior cingulate (CT), pars triangularis (SA)
8 rs9385385 6q22.31 ncRNA_intronicb T 0.45 0.0595 1.37 × 10−8 NCOA7 Posterior cingulate (CT), pericalcarine (SA)
9 rs6947352 7p14.3 Intronic A 0.31 0.0585 4.38 × 10−8 BBS9 Banks of the superior temporal sulcus (SA)
10 rs911934 9q22.33 Intergenic G 0.70 0.0699 2.39 × 10−15 GALNT12 Inferior parietal (SA), isthmus of cingulate (SA), precuneus (SA), paracentral (SA), supramarginal (SA), entorhinal (CT)
11 rs41298373 10p14 Exonic A 0.10 0.0940 4.75 × 10−38 ITIH5 Superior temporal (SA), parahippocampal (SA), fusiform (SA), inferior temporal (CT), transverse temporal (SA)
12 rs10783306c 12q13.12 Intergenic C 0.33 0.0647 9.99 × 10−12 TUBA1B Superior frontal (SA), entorhinal (SA), medial orbitofrontal (SA), pars triangularis (SA)
13 rs160459 14q23.1 Intergenic C 0.46 0.0652 4.98 × 10−12 DACT1 Banks of the superior temporal sulcus (SA), transverse temporal (SA), pericalcarine (SA)
14 rs201816193 14q23.1 Intergenic G 0.12 0.0621 4.38 × 10−10 DAAM1 Isthmus of cingulate (SA), cuneus (SA)
15 rs72813426 16q24.3 Intronic G 0.24 0.0685 2.45 × 10−14 SPIRE2 Paracentral (SA), isthmus of cingulate (SA), middle temporal (SA)
15 rs111398992c 16q24.3 Intronic T 0.13 0.0694 5.99 × 10−15 TUBB3 Isthmus of cingulate (CT), fusiform (SA), rostral anterior cingulate (CT), pericalcarine (SA)
16 rs55938136 17q21.31 NcRNA_intronicb G 0.22 0.0695 4.91 × 10−15 CRHR1 Parahippocampal (SA), middle temporal (SA), pallidum (SUB), hippocampus (SUB), pars triangularis (SA)
16 rs35908989 17q21.31 Intronic C 0.23 0.0595 1.34 × 10−8 MAPT Supramarginal (SA), caudate (SUB)
16 rs35853889 17q21.31 3′ UTRd TG 0.19 0.0765 1.43 × 10−20 MAPT Rostral anterior cingulate (CT), cuneus (SA), isthmus of cingulate (SA), parahippocampal (SA), rostral anterior cingulate (SA), parahippocampal (CT)
16 rs80103986c 17q21.31 Intronic T 0.20 0.0708 5.16 × 10−16 KANSL1 Parahippocampal (SA), middle temporal (SA), pallidum (SUB), hippocampus (SUB), pars triangularis (SA)
16 rs568039055 17q21.31 3′ UTRd C 0.20 0.0692 7.87 × 10−15

LRRC37A2

ARL17A

Parahippocampal (SA), isthmus of cingulate (SA), rostral anterior cingulate (CT), cuneus (SA)
17 rs11672092c 19p13.3 Intronic T 0.22 0.0619 5.69 × 10−10 TUBB4A Isthmus of cingulate (CT), lateral orbitofrontal (SA), middle temporal (SA)
18 rs6135555 20p12.1 Intronic A 0.39 0.0600 7.00 × 10−9 MACROD2 Pericalcarine (SA), caudate (SUB)
19 rs7283026 21q22.3 Intronic C 0.27 0.0616 8.42 × 10−10 COL18A1 Supramarginal (SA), transverse temporal (SA)
20 rs9615351 22q13.31 Exonic G 0.25 0.0588 3.02 × 10−8 CELSR1 Isthmus of cingulate (CT), transverse temporal (SA)
21 rs12400461 Xp22.33 Intergenic C 0.58 0.0595 1.24 × 10−8 ASS1P4 Inferior temporal (SA), pars opercularis (SA)

aCentral traits for each SNP are those asymmetry indexes that contribute to its multivariate association (Methods). SA, surface area; CT, cortical thickness; SUB, subcortical volume.

bIntronic to a gene for a non-coding RNA.

cLead variants are in high LD with handedness-associated variants.

dUntranslated region.

For each lead SNP, phenotype decomposition37 identified the ‘central’ AIs that contributed to its multivariate association (Supplementary Table 11). Most central AIs associated with the 27 lead SNPs were distributed in core regions of the language system (for example, lateral temporal, pars opercularis and supramarginal) or limbic system (for example, cingulate, orbitofrontal and mesial temporal cortex; Fig. 3). For example, the most significant SNP, with multivariate r = 0.094, P = 4.75 × 10−38 (rs41298373 on 10p14) had five central AIs: the minor allele was associated with a leftward shift of surface area asymmetry for two lateral temporal regions, a rightward shift of surface area asymmetry for two medial temporal regions and a leftward shift of cortical thickness asymmetry in the inferior temporal gyrus (Supplementary Table 11). All lead SNPs were associated with at least one cortical regional surface area AI as one of their central traits. Thirteen lead SNPs were associated with cortical thickness AIs, and five lead SNPs were associated with subcortical volume AIs. Two lead SNPs, rs35853889 and rs6658111, were associated with the AIs of both surface area and cortical thickness within the same region, that is, parahippcampal area and thickness AIs were associated with both of these SNPs, and rostral anterior cingulate area and thickness AIs were associated with rs35853889. In addition, five lead SNPs affecting cortical surface area AIs were also associated with AIs of subcortical volumes (Table 1 and Fig. 3), and the locus on 17q21 was associated with asymmetries of cortical surface area, thickness and subcortical volume (Table 1). The univariate associations of lead variants separately with left and right hemispheric measures are presented in Supplementary Table 11.

Fig. 3. Overview of 27 independent lead variants associated with different regional brain asymmetries.

Fig. 3

Circle plot illustrating the 27 lead variants from mvGWAS (left) in relation to the central asymmetry indexes (right) underlying their specific multivariate associations. Different colours indicate different lead variants or regional asymmetries. Lines linking lead variants to regional asymmetries are coloured according to the regions. The closest genes to the lead variants are shown. Most central asymmetry indexes are of regional surface areas, and some variants affected multiple asymmetries of different types. SA, surface area; CT, cortical thickness; SUB, subcortical volume.

Functional annotations of genomic loci associated with brain asymmetry

FUMA38 software applied three strategies to annotate candidate SNPs to genes at significantly associated loci (Methods): physical position, expression quantitative trait locus (eQTL) information and chromatin interactions (Supplementary Table 12 and Supplementary Figs. 4 and 5). Here we summarize and cite notable information on each of the lead SNPs:

Ten of the loci had annotations involving cytoskeleton-related genes: On 1p33, rs6658111 is close to AL356458.1, a pseudogene of MTMR14 (myotubularin related protein 14). On 2p23.3, rs62130503 is intronic to MAPRE3 (microtubule associated protein RP/EB family member 3a), and rs12617392 is a brain eQTL39 of MAPRE3. Located between these two SNPs on 2p23.3 is also AGBL5, which is a post-translational modifier of tubulin40,41. On 2q34, rs368536282 is close to MAP2 (microtubule associated protein 2), a well-known dendrite-specific marker of neurons42, previously implicated in left-handedness by GWAS analysis4345. On 6p21.33, rs7781 is in the 3′ untranslated region (UTR) of TUBB (tubulin beta class I). On 12q13.12, rs10783306 is close to the alpha tubulin gene TUBA1B. This variant is also in high LD with a handedness-associated variant, rs11168884 (r2 = 0.89)44. On 14q23.1, two lead variants for two independent genomic loci, rs160459 and rs201816193, showed evidence for cross-locus chromatin interaction via the promoters of nearby genes in foetal cortex46 (Supplementary Fig. 4). The former is near to DACT1, a locus which has been reported to associate with superior temporal sulcus depth34, while the latter is close to DAAM1, which modulates the reorganization of the actin cytoskeleton and the stabilization of microtubules47,48. Two lead variants on 16q24.3, rs72813426 and rs111398992, are in introns of SPIRE2 and the tubulin gene TUBB3, respectively, both of which are key proteins in cytoskeleton organization49,50. rs111398992 is also in high LD with a handedness-associated variant, rs4550447 (r2 = 0.94)44. On 17q21.31 there were five independent lead SNPs: rs35908989 is intronic to MAPT which encodes microtubule-associated protein tau, and rs55938136, rs35853889 and rs568039055 are brain eQTLs39,51,52 of MAPT, while rs80103986 is in high LD (r2 = 0.91) with handedness-associated variant rs5597401444. On 19p13.3, rs11672092 is intronic to the tubulin gene TUBB4A, and in high LD with rs66479618 (r2 = 0.88), another handedness-associated SNP44.

The 11 other loci did not have obvious microtubule-related annotations, but most had annotations related to brain phenotypes or development: On 3q24, rs2279829 is a cortical eQTL39 of ZIC4, which is involved in visual and auditory pathway development53. rs9307052 on 4q22.1 is in high LD (r2 = 0.99) with the handedness-associated variant rs2865828244. On 5q15, rs869219775 is close to NR2F1, which is involved in neural activity during cortical patterning54. On 6q22.31-q22.32, rs9385385 is close to NCOA7, a nuclear receptor co-activator with its most abundant expression in the brain55. On 7p14.3, rs6947352 is intronic to BBS9, which causes Bardet–Biedl syndrome when mutated, involving retinopathy and intellectual disability56,57. On 9q22.33, rs911934 is located in a region having a chromatin interaction with TRIM14 in adult cortex46 (Supplementary Fig. 4), a gene which may activate Wnt/β-catenin signalling and affects mesodermal versus ectodermal differentiation of embryonic stem cells58. On 10p14, rs41298373 is a predicted deleterious missense coding variant in ITIH5, which was previously reported to affect planum temporale volumetric asymmetry35. Inter-alpha-trypsin inhibitor proteins are involved in extracellular matrix stabilization59. On 20p12.1, rs6135555 is in a region having a chromatin interaction with the FLRT3 promoter in neural progenitor cells60 (Supplementary Fig. 4), a gene which regulates axon guidance and excitatory synapse development61. On 21q22.3, rs7283026 is intronic to COL18A1, involved in neural tube closure and mutated in Knobloch syndrome62, which can include skull abnormalities. On 22q13.31, rs9615351 is an exonic variant of a gene involved in planar cell polarity, CELSR163. On Xp22.33, rs12400461 is close to pseudogene ASS1P4 and upstream of MXRA5; the latter encodes a matrix remodelling-associated protein and is implicated in autism64.

To further link asymmetry-associated SNPs to genes, the MAGMA65 software was used to perform genome-wide gene-based association analysis65 based on the results from mvGWAS. There were 57 significant genes at Bonferroni-corrected P < 0.05 (Supplementary Fig. 6 and Supplementary Table 13). Five of these genes were previously associated with handedness44: MAP2, FAM13A, TUBA1B, TUBB3 and CRHR1. Forty-three of the 57 genes have been reported to associate with educational attainment66 and 15 with intelligence67 (Supplementary Table 14). For the proteins encoded by the 57 genes, there were 80 known or putative pairwise interactions in the STRING database68, compared with 8 interactions expected for a random set of this size from the whole proteome (P < 1 × 10−16). This observation supports the validity of our mvGWAS association findings, as random noise would not lead to such functional clustering. Microtubule-related genes (for example, MAP2, MAPT, SPIRE2 and TUBA1A) linked different clusters together in the largest protein interaction network (Fig. 4a).

Fig. 4. Functional annotations of variants associated with brain asymmetry.

Fig. 4

a, Brain asymmetry-associated genes integrated into a protein–protein interaction network. Proteins are represented by nodes. Edges between nodes represent different types of protein–protein interactions according to the STRING database (Methods), including known interactions (turquoise and dark purple represent interactions identified by curated databases and biological experiments, respectively), predicted interactions (green, red and blue represent interactions predicted by gene neighbourhood, gene fusions and gene co-occurrence, respectively) and others (yellow, black and light purple represent interactions determined by text mining, co-expression and protein homology, respectively). Coloured nodes represent the queried proteins. Only medium-confidence (>0.4) links were retained, and disconnected proteins are not shown. b, Relation between gene-based association with brain asymmetries and relatively higher mRNA expression in the human brain at particular ages, using BrainSpan data from 29 age groups. Asterisks indicate significant age groups meeting P < 0.05 with FDR correction. pcw, post-conceptional weeks. c, Relation between gene-based association with brain asymmetries and relatively higher mRNA expression in the human brain at particular ages, using BrainSpan data from 11 defined age groups. Asterisks indicate significant groups meeting P < 0.05 with FDR correction.

We also used the genome-wide, gene-based P values for functional enrichment analysis using MAGMA65, in relation to 7,343 Gene Ontology ‘biological process’ sets defined within the MSigDB69 database. The gene set ‘regulation_of_microtubule_binding’ (P = 3.73 × 10−6) showed significant enrichment (adjusted P < 0.05, Bonferroni correction, Supplementary Table 15). Significant enrichment within various microtubule-related sets, such as ‘microtubule_cytoskeleton_organization’ (P = 2.19 × 10−7) and ‘microtubule_based_process’ (P = 2.36 × 10−6), was also found when using the list of single closest genes (Table 1) to the 27 lead SNPs (Supplementary Table 16). Enrichment in microtubule-related sets was not reported in a recent GWAS of bilaterally averaged cortical surface area and thickness measures in 51,665 individuals70, suggesting a particular involvement in hemispheric asymmetry rather than bilateral measures. We observed no statistically significant relation of our gene-based association P values with differential expression across cell types (Methods).

Testing our genome-wide, gene-based P values with respect to human gene expression data from the BrainSpan71 database, from either 29 age groups or 11 defined developmental stages, we found relatively higher mRNA expression of brain-asymmetry-associated genes during early-prenatal (P = 4.27 × 10−3) and early–mid-prenatal (P = 9.37 × 10−4) stages, from 9 (P = 1.84 × 10−3) to 24 (P = 7.36 × 10−3) post-conceptional weeks (FDR-corrected P values <0.05) (Fig. 4b,c and Supplementary Table 17). This is consistent with the fact that various anatomical asymmetries of the brain are already visible in utero18,19, and supports the existence of an early developmental mechanism for establishing the brain’s left–right axis31,32,72.

Genetic overlap of brain asymmetry with other traits

We next used iSECA software73 to perform genetic overlap analysis with our mvGWAS results in relation to GWAS summary statistics from neurodevelopmental disorders, behavioural and psychological traits which have been reported to associate phenotypically with aspects of structural and/or functional brain asymmetry: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder16,7477, autism spectrum disorder15,7882, educational attainment66,83,84, handedness2,4,45, intelligence67,8588 and schizophrenia17,8993. There was evidence for genetic overlap between brain asymmetries and autism (P = 0.005), educational attainment (P = 0.001) and schizophrenia (P = 0.002) which remained significant at Bonferroni-corrected P < 0.05 (Fig. 5, Supplementary Figs. 7 and 8 and Supplementary Table 18). In other words, SNPs that showed lower (more significant) association P values in our mvGWAS for brain asymmetry showed a statistically significant tendency to also show lower P values in previous, large-scale GWAS of autism, educational attainment and schizophrenia. Although we did not observe genetic overlap of brain asymmetry with handedness at a genome-wide level, we did note individual loci in common between these traits (above). In addition, we found no overlap between our mvGWAS results and those from a previous GWAS of intracranial volume in 32,438 participants94 (Supplementary Table 18, Supplementary Figs. 7 and 8), which again indicates that the genetic architecture of brain asymmetry is largely distinct from brain size.

Fig. 5. Genetic overlaps between brain asymmetries and other traits.

Fig. 5

ac, Heatmap plots illustrating pleiotropic effects between brain asymmetries and autism (a), educational attainment (b) and schizophrenia (c), based on per-SNP GWAS P values for these traits from previous studies (Methods), in relation to the mvGWAS P values from the present study of brain asymmetries.

Validation of lead SNPs associated with brain asymmetry

To achieve a reasonable level of genetic homogeneity in our mvGWAS, we had excluded any individuals not annotated as having ‘white British ancestry’ (through a combination of self-report and clustering based on principal components that capture major axes of ancestral diversity from the genotype data95 (Methods)). The UK Biobank includes additional participants who self-identify as being ‘white’, but who did not self-identify as British, or did not cluster genetically with the bulk of the ‘white British’ ancestry participants (Methods). After applying the same quality control criteria to these additional participants as in our discovery mvGWAS (except with respect to ancestry), and imposing the extra criterion that relatedness kinship coefficients should not be greater than 0.0442 with any participants from the discovery mvGWAS, data were available from 3,600 participants for an independent replication set. We tested each of the 27 lead SNPs from the mvGWAS in the replication set, using the same approach as the mvGWAS analysis, except that 40 genetic principal components were used as covariates to control for the greater degree of ancestral diversity in the replication set.

Ten of the 27 independent lead SNPs from the discovery mvGWAS showed association P values < 0.05 on multivariate testing in the replication set (Supplementary Table 19). The combined P value of the remaining 17 lead SNPs was P = 3.3 × 10−4 in the replication set (calculated by Stouffer’s method), which we confirmed by permutation with respect to 10,000 repeat random samplings of 17 SNPs from the whole genome in the replication set (permutation-based P = 4 × 10−4). This indicates that the limited sample size of the replication set, compared with the discovery set, did not provide adequate power to replicate at the level of some individual SNPs, but that in combination there was evidence for replication. Moreover, among the 17 SNPs that showed P > 0.05 on multivariate testing in the replication set, some showed association P < 0.05 on univariate testing of the specific central traits identified for those SNPs in the discovery mvGWAS (Supplementary Table 19). It is also worth noting that 4 of these 17 SNPs (or SNPs in high linkage disequilibrium with them) have been reported to associate with left-handedness at a genome-wide significant level44 (see above for details), which is an additional form of validation with respect to a phenotype related to brain asymmetry. As also mentioned above, the high degree of functional clustering of genes identified through gene-based association testing is another form of support for the association results in the mvGWAS.

Discussion

Despite the importance of asymmetry as an organizing feature of the human brain, the early developmental processes which establish its left–right axis are unknown. In this study, we carried out multivariate GWAS analysis which identified 21 genetic loci associated with different aspects of adult brain anatomical asymmetry. Functional annotation implicated genes particularly involved in microtubule organization and prenatal brain development. Our study therefore sheds new light on the molecular genetic foundations of human brain asymmetry. In addition, at the genome-wide level, SNPs associated with brain asymmetry overlapped with those associated with educational attainment, autism and schizophrenia, while five specific loci that are associated with both brain asymmetry and handedness were identified.

Previous studies in invertebrates and frog embryos have shown that the cytoskeleton plays a role in determining cellular chirality and asymmetrical patterning of other organs96102. Cellular chirality refers to directional biases in cellular morphology, position, rotation or migration, which arise because of the inherent chirality of intracellular macromolecules such as those composing the cytoskeleton96, and also manifest in terms of the intracellular distributions of organelles103. For example, during early cell divisions in Xenopus (frog) embryos, the cytoskeleton has been reported to mediate asymmetric intracellular protein localization, as protein transport molecules move along cytoskeletal tracks within cells104. Thus the cytoskeleton can provide a directionally consistent, organ-intrinsic bias during embryonic development that acts as a determinant of morphological asymmetry97,98,105, arising from fundamental aspects of molecular and cellular biology.

As our study associated cytoskeletal and embryonically expressed genes with variation in adult human brain asymmetry, it is possible that these genes are involved in the establishment of left–right asymmetry during early brain development, through a mechanism involving cellular chirality. As mentioned in Main, at least some aspects of human brain asymmetry appear to be uncoupled from the developmental pathway that leads to left–right organization of the visceral organs, which involves cilia and the nodal pathway. A cytoskeletal-mediated mechanism of brain asymmetry may therefore be organ intrinsic97,98,105, that is, distinct from other pathways that establish broader aspects of body asymmetry.

In this study, we identified genetic loci that are associated with 42 heritable aspects of brain asymmetry through a multivariate, brain-wide approach. A multivariate approach can boost statistical power while achieving data reduction, compared with separate univariate analyses of individual brain traits37. A single set of genome-wide association results, pertaining simultaneously to multiple aspects of brain asymmetry, was then taken forward into functional annotation and downstream analyses, such as testing for genetic overlaps with other traits. The multivariate approach therefore helped to detect and interpret key aspects of the genetic architecture of brain asymmetry, without the noise inherent in repeat univariate testing. An important challenge remained to identify the particular brain traits that drove the multivariate associations at each locus, which was achieved in MetaPhat37 by decomposing associations into sets of ‘central’ traits on the basis of the Bayesian information criterion and canonical correlation P values.

A consequence of the multivariate approach is that it does not yield univariate association effect sizes, and therefore mvGWAS results cannot be used for standard genetic correlation analyses, such as is performed with LD score regression106. Therefore, we used iSECA73 to explore the genetic overlap of brain asymmetry with neurodevelopmental disorders, behavioural and psychological traits. This analysis was based on SNP-wise P values of association over the whole genome, that is, a genetic overlap was found when the SNPs tending to show low P values in our mvGWAS for brain asymmetry also tended to show low P values in publicly available GWAS summary statistics for a given disorder or trait. We found significant genetic overlaps of brain asymmetry with autism, schizophrenia and educational attainment, which suggests that genes affecting brain asymmetry also influence these traits. This is in line with literature that has shown phenotypic associations between altered brain asymmetry and these traits (see Main), and indicates that such phenotypic associations are contributed to an extent by genetic factors. As we found that brain asymmetry-related genes tend to be especially highly expressed in the embryonic brain, it seems likely that the genetic overlap of brain asymmetry and disorders reflects a genetic susceptibility to alterations of early neurodevelopment away from the typical trajectory. However, brain asymmetry continues to develop throughout the lifespan107,108, and the UK Biobank consists of middle- to older-age adults, so that our mvGWAS may have also identified genetic factors that affect brain asymmetrical changes later in life.

Further research, for example, using Mendelian randomization109, will be needed to understand whether brain asymmetries mediate gene–disorder associations in a causal sense, or whether altered brain asymmetry and disorder susceptibility are two distinct consequences arising from a partly overlapping genetic basis. It will also be important to map, on a brain-regional basis, which aspects of asymmetry show the strongest genetic overlaps with disorder susceptibility. Larger imaging–genetic datasets may be needed to support causal mediation and mapping studies with respect to disorders, as the present genetic association analysis was based on brain-wide asymmetry (albeit in a multivariate context).

Many of the brain asymmetries were strong and directional at the population level, but their heritabilities were generally low, ranging up to 10%. This suggests that developmental mechanisms for brain asymmetry are tightly constrained and largely genetically invariant in the population, and that environmental factors and/or developmental randomness are responsible for most variability45,110112. A cytoskeleton-based origin of brain asymmetry would fit this scenario, as the cytoskeleton is essential for various fundamental functions in cellular biology, beyond axis formation113,114. Previous, twin- and family-based analyses5,6 have reported heritabilities up to roughly 25% for some of the same asymmetry measures that we analysed in the present study, with an overall similar regional pattern. Higher heritabilities were found particularly for regions that are important in the language system (for example, superior temporal cortex) and limbic system (for example, medial temporal and cingulate cortex). Twin-based heritability is often measured to be higher than SNP-based heritability, which may be expected because SNPs are just one class of genetic variation, and also because twin studies can overestimate heritability when certain assumptions are not fully met115. As twin studies have not indicated effects of shared environment on brain aymmetries5,6, early developmental randomness is likely to cause most variation116.

We did not correct for handedness as a covariate in our genetic analyses, as it is generally not advisable to correct for covariates which are themselves partly heritable. This is because biased genetic effects can be measured with respect to the target trait117 (in this case, brain asymmetry). Handedness cannot therefore be treated safely as a confound variable when analysing brain asymmetry. We were interested in any genetic effects associated with brain asymmetry, regardless of whether they might also be shared with other traits such as handedness. Having identified genetic variants associated with brain asymmetry, we then queried post hoc whether they have been reported as significant in previous GWAS of handedness in over 1 million people44. We did not observe a significant genetic overlap between structural brain asymmetry and handedness at the genome-wide level, which again may be due to the relatively low SNP-based heritabilities of these traits, in combination with limited statistical power in the present sample size for this kind of analysis. However, five individual SNPs associated with both brain asymmetry and handedness were identified, which suggests that a significant genome-wide overlap might be detected when using a larger dataset in the future.

The UK Biobank currently includes by far the largest single imaging–genetic dataset available to the scientific community. A limitation of the present study is the lack of a large, age-matched replication sample with comparable homogeneity of ancestry to the discovery mvGWAS analysis. We included an independent replication sample of 3,600 individuals from the UK Biobank, with greater diversity of ancestry than the 32,256 individuals of the primary mvGWAS. Association was replicated for 10 of the 27 independent lead SNPs from the mvGWAS, and for the remaining 17, their combined P value in the replication set was 3.3 × 10−4, indicating that a larger dataset would have supported replication at the level of more individual SNPs. Functional clustering of the closest genes to the 27 independent lead SNPs, according to microtubule-related biology and protein–protein interactions, also supported the validity of the mvGWAS findings, as this was unlikely to occur by chance. In addition, four of the SNPs that did not replicate individually at P < 0.05 (or SNPs in high linkage disequilibrium with them; see Results) were previously associated at a genome-wide significant level with left-handedness, which is an additional form of validation with respect to a behavioural asymmetry trait.

Another limitation of the present study is that functional enrichment analysis with respect to post-mortem, developmental gene expression was only possible from roughly 8 weeks post conception and onwards. The Brainspan database does not contain sufficient numbers of samples from earlier embryonic stages than this. It may be that the left–right brain axis is established extremely early in development, for example during formation of the neural tube, which begins in the third week post conception118. As noted in Main, possibilities for research using human embryos from this developmental stage are highly restricted. Gene expression studies using animal models may therefore be necessary to understand the establishment of the mammalian brain’s left–right axis.

In conclusion, our findings motivate genetic-developmental studies of left–right differentiation of the embryonic mammalian brain, focused on a possible cytoskeletal-mediated mechanism of axis formation. Our study also suggests that disruption of this mechanism may contribute to susceptibility to cognitive and psychiatric disorders, in line with asymmetry being an important aspect of healthy brain organization for many functions.

Methods

Participants

This study was conducted under UK Biobank application 16066, with C.F. as principal investigator. The UK Biobank is a general adult population cohort. The UK Biobank received ethical approval from the National Research Ethics Service Committee North West-Haydock (reference 11/NW/0382), and all of their procedures were performed in accordance with the World Medical Association guidelines. Informed consent was obtained for all participants. We used the brain imaging data released in February 2020, and data availability and processing (described below) resulted in a final sample of 32,256 participants of white British ancestry for the primary GWAS analysis, together with the structural MRI data and genotype data from the same participants. The age range of these participants was 45 to 81 years (mean 63.77 years), 15,288 were male and 16,968 were female. An independent replication dataset of 3,600 individuals was also drawn from the UK Biobank, who self-identified as white, but not British, or did not cluster genetically with the bulk of the ‘white British’ ancestry participants (see below). The age range of these participants was 45 to 80 years (mean 62.89 years), 1,574 were male and 2,026 were female.

Genetic quality control

We downloaded imputed SNP genotype data from the UK Biobank data portal (bgen files; imputed data v3-release March 2018). We first excluded subjects with a mismatch of their self-reported and genetically inferred sex, with putative sex chromosome aneuploidies, or who were outliers on the basis of heterozygosity (principle-component-corrected heterozygosity >0.19) and genotype missingness (missing rate >0.05) as calculated by Bycroft et al.95. The primary analyses were restricted to participants with ‘white British ancestry’, which was defined by Bycroft et al. (‘in.white.British.ancestry.subset’)95, using a combination of self-report and cluster analysis on the basis of the first six principal components which capture genetic ancestry from the genome-wide genotype data. We randomly excluded one from each pair of remaining individuals who had a kinship coefficient >0.0442, as defined by Bycroft et al.95. Next, QCTOOL (v.2.0.6) and PLINK119 were used to perform genotype quality control: excluding SNPs with minor allele frequency <1%, Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium test P value <1 × 10−7 and imputation INFO score <0.7 (a measure of genotype imputation confidence). We also excluded multi-allelic SNPs because most of the downstream software (below) could not handle them. This resulted in 9,803,522 bi-allelic variants.

The same process was applied to derive the independent replication dataset of 3,600 individuals, except that these participants did not self-identify as British (although they did identify as ‘white’), or they did not fall within the bounds of the ‘white British ancestry’ cluster as defined by Bycroft et al.95. We also imposed the extra requirement that participants in the replication set should not show a kinship coefficient >0.0442 with any individual in the primary discovery dataset.

Neuroimaging phenotypes and covariates

Brain anatomical measures of regional cortical surface area, cortical thickness and subcortical volumes were derived from the structural scans (Siemens Skyra 3-T MRI with 32-channel radiofrequency receive head coil) released by the UK Biobank Imaging Study (for the full protocol, see http://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/showcase/refer.cgi?id=2367). Briefly, in vivo whole-brain T1-weighted MRI scans were used to perform cortical parcellation into 34 regions per hemisphere with the Desikan–Killiany atlas120, and 7 subcortical structural segmentations. Surface area was measured at the grey–white matter boundary, and thickness was measured as the average distance in a region between the white matter and pial surfaces. Details of the image quality control and processing are described elsewhere121. Given that the data for the temporal pole were reported as unreliable121, we only used 33 surface area, 33 cortical thickness and 7 subcortical volume measures in each hemisphere (Supplementary Table 1). Per measure, we removed data points greater than six standard deviations from the mean. Then, we calculated the AI for each matching pair of left and right measures, in each participant, as (left − right)/((left + right)/2). Given this definition, a positive AI reflects leftward asymmetry (greater left than right). The AI is a widely used measure in brain asymmetry studies5,122,123. The denominator ensures that the index does not simply scale with brain size, that is, the left–right difference is adjusted for the bilateral measure. For each AI, one-sample t testing was used to examine whether the population mean AI was significantly different from zero, with Bonferroni correction at 0.05 for multiple testing. Subsequently, the distributions of AIs were normalized by rank-based inverse normalization to minimize statistical artifacts. The normalized AIs were used as input for subsequent analysis.

The Desikan–Killiany atlas120 was derived from manual segmentations of sets of reference brain images. The labelling system incorporates hemisphere-specific information on sulcal and gyral geometry with spatial information regarding the locations of brain structures, and shows a high accuracy when compared with manual labelling results120. Accordingly, the mean regional asymmetries in the UK Biobank might partly reflect left–right differences present in the reference dataset used to construct the atlas. However, our study was focused primarily on comparing relative asymmetry between genotypes, at the regional level. The use of an asymmetrical atlas based on healthy individuals had the advantage that regional identification was likely to be accurate for structures that are asymmetrical in the general population, while taking hemisphere-specific information into account.

We also made use of continuous variables as covariates in heritability estimation and genome-wide association analysis (below), which were: age when attended assessment centre (UK Biobank fields 21003-2.0), nonlinear age, that is (age − mean_age)2, the first ten genetic principal components capturing population genetic diversity (fields 22009-0.1 to 22009-0.10) (or the first 40 principal components in the replication dataset with higher diversity of ancestry), scanner position parameters (X, Y and Z position: fields 25756-2.0, 25757-2.0 and 25758-2.0), T1 signal-to-noise ratio (field 25734-2.0) and T1 contrast-to-noise ratio (field 25735-2.0), plus categorical covariates which were: assessment centre (field 54-2.0), genotype measurement batch (field 22000-0.0) and sex (field 31-0.0).

SNP-based heritability and genetic correlation analysis within the UK Biobank data

From the primary dataset, 9,516,074 autosomal variants with minor allele frequencies >1%, INFO score >0.7 and Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium P > 1 × 10−7 were used to build a genetic relationship matrix using GCTA36 (version 1.93.0beta). Specifically for analyses using GCTA, we further excluded one random participant from each pair having a kinship coefficient higher than 0.025 based on the calculated genetic relationship matrix (as this analysis is especially sensitive to higher levels of relatedness), resulting in 30,315 participants. Genome-based restricted maximum likelihood (GREML)36 analyses were performed to estimate the SNP-based heritability for each AI, controlling for the above-mentioned covariates, and applying FDR 0.05 across the 73 AIs to define significantly heritable AIs. Bivariate GREML124 analysis was used to estimate genetic correlations between pairs of AIs, separately for cortical surface area, cortical thickness and subcortical volume AIs, with FDR correction at 0.05 for multiple testing.

Multivariate genome-wide association analysis

In mvGWAS, a single association test is performed for each SNP in relation to multiple traits simultaneously. We used MetaPhat37 software to perform mvGWAS analysis across asymmetries for cortical surface area, cortical thickness and subcortical volume, including only the 42 AIs that had shown significant SNP-based heritability. MetaPhat performs meta-canonical correlation analysis, and uses univariate GWAS summary statistics as input from each separate AI, which were derived under an additive genetic model while controlling for the above-mentioned covariates, using BGENIE software (v1.2)95. Thus our mvGWAS tested effectively for association with 42 traits. This approach estimates the linear combination of traits that is maximally associated with genotype, which can differ for each SNP, while maintaining a correct false-positive rate. A total of 9,803,522 SNPs (see further above) were used for mvGWAS, spanning all autosomes and chromosome X. Statistically significant SNPs were considered as those with P < 5 × 10−8 in mvGWAS, which is a widely used threshold to account for multiple testing over the whole genome, in the context of LD in European-descent populations125,126.

MetaPhat also uses systematic criteria to define central traits which make the greatest contributions to significant multivariate associations, on the basis of an iterative process to optimize multivariate model properties with reference to canonical correlation analysis P values and the Bayesian information criterion37. For the lead SNPs at genome-wide significant loci (see below for how these were defined), we also performed post hoc analysis in which we examined their separate left and right hemispheric associations, using traits corresponding to the central AIs that were involved in the multivariate associations (Supplementary Table 11), again using BGENIE, an additive genetic model and covariates as described above.

As a sensitivity analysis, we re-ran the mvGWAS after excluding from the primary dataset 886 participants who had lifetime diagnoses of neurological conditions that could potentially disrupt brain structure (Supplementary Table 20). The significant mvGWAS loci were minimally affected by this exclusion (Supplementary Fig. 9).

Identification of genomic risk loci and functional annotations

FUMA (version v1.3.6)38, an online platform for functional annotation of GWAS results, was applied to the results from mvGWAS. A multi-step process, using default parameters, was used to identify distinct, significantly associated genomic loci, and independent lead SNPs within those loci. Briefly, on the basis of the pre-calculated LD structure from the 1000 Genomes European reference panel127, SNPs with genome-wide significant mvGWAS P values <5 × 10−8 that had LD r2 < 0.6 with any others were identified. For each of these SNPs, other SNPs that had r2 ≥ 0.6 with them were included for further annotation (see below), and independent lead SNPs were also defined among them as having low LD (r2 < 0.1) with any others. If LD blocks of significant SNPs are located within 250 kb of each other (default parameter), they are merged into one genomic locus. Therefore, some genomic loci could include one or more independent lead SNPs (Table 1). The major histocompatibility complex region on chromosome 6 was excluded from this process by default, because of its especially complex and long-range LD structure.

Functional annotations were applied by matching chromosome location, base-pair position, reference and alternate alleles to databases containing known functional annotations, which were ANNOVAR128 categories, Combined Annotation-Dependent Depletion129 scores, RegulomeDB130 scores and chromatin state131,132:

  1. ANNOVAR categories identify SNPs on the basis of their locations with respect to genes, such as exonic, intronic and intergenic, using Ensembl gene definitions.

  2. Combined Annotation-Dependent Depletion scores predict deleteriousness, with scores higher than 12.37 suggesting potential pathogenicity133.

  3. RegulomeDB scores integrate regulatory information from eQTL and chromatin marks, and range from 1a to 7, with lower scores representing more importance for regulatory function.

  4. Chromatin states show the accessibility of genomic regions, and were labelled by 15 categorical states on the basis of five chromatin marks for 127 epigenomes in the Roadmap Epigenomics Project132, which were H3K4me3, H3K4me1, H3K36me3, H3K27me3 and H3K9me3. For each SNP, FUMA calculated the minimum chromatin state across 127 tissue/cell-type in the Roadmap Epigenomics Project132. Categories 1–7 are considered as open chromatin states.

We also used FUMA to annotate independent significant SNPs and their candidate SNPs according to previously reported phenotype associations (P < 5 × 10−5) in the National Human Genome Research Institute–European Bioinformatics Institute catalogue134.

For a significant mvGWAS association in the major histocompatibility complex region (Table 1), we took the most significant individual SNP, rs7781 (P = 1.62 × 10−10), as the single lead SNP to represent this locus, and annotated it manually.

SNP-to-gene mapping

SNP-to-gene mapping at significant mvGWAS loci was performed using the default FUMA processes for these three strategies:

  1. Positional mapping was used to map SNPs to protein-coding genes on the basis of physical distance (within 10 kb) in the human reference assembly (GRCh37/hg19).

  2. eQTL mapping was used to annotate SNPs to genes (that is, when SNP genotypes are associated with variation in gene mRNA expression levels). eQTL mapping was carried out in relation to genes up to 1 Mb away on the basis of four brain-expression data repositories: PsychENCORE52, CommonMind Consortium39, BRAINEAC51 and GTEx v8 Brain135. FUMA applied a FDR of 0.05 within each analysis to identify significant eQTL associations.

  3. Chromatin interaction mapping was performed to map SNPs to genes on the basis of seven brain-related Hi-C chromatin conformation capture datasets: PsychENCORE EP link (one way)52, PsychENCORE promoter anchored loops39, HiC adult cortex46, HiC foetal cortex46, HiC (GSE87112) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex60, HiC (GSE87112) hippocampus60 and HiC (GSE87112) neural progenitor cell60. We further selected only those genes for which one or both regions involved in the chromatin interaction overlapped with a predicted enhancer or promoter region (250 bp up- and 500 bp downstream of the transcription start site) in any of the brain-related repositories from the Roadmap Epigenomics Project132, that is, E053 (neurospheres) cortex, E054 (neurospheres) ganglion eminence, E067 (brain) angular gyrus, E068 (brain) anterior caudate, E069 (brain) cingulate gyrus, E070 (brain) germinal matrix, E071 (brain) hippocampus middle, E072 (brain) inferior temporal lobe, E073 (brain) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, E074 (brain) substantia nigra, E081 (brain) foetal brain male, E082 (brain) foetal brain female, E003 embryonic stem (ES) H1 cells, E008 ES H9 cells, E007 (ES-derived) H1 derived neuronal progenitor cultured cells, E009 (ES-derived) H9 derived neuronal progenitor cultured cells and E010 (ES-derived) H9 derived neuron cultured cells. A FDR of 1 × 10−6 was applied to identify significant interactions (default parameter), separately for each analysis.

Gene-based association analysis

Genome-wide gene-based association analysis was performed using mvGWAS summary statistics as input into MAGMA (v1.08)65, using default parameters implemented in FUMA (SNP-wide mean model). This process examines the joint association signals of all SNPs within a given gene (including 50 kb upstream to 50 kb downstream of the gene), while considering the LD between the SNPs. SNPs were mapped to 20,146 protein-coding genes on the basis of National Center for Biotechnology Information build 37.3 gene definitions, and each gene was represented by at least one SNP. We applied a Bonferroni correction for the number of tested genes (P < 0.05/20,146).

Gene-set enrichment analysis

We used MAGMA65, again with default settings as implemented in FUMA, to test for enrichment of association within predefined gene sets. This process tests whether gene-based P values among all 20,146 genes are lower for those genes within pre-defined functional sets than the rest of the genes in the genome, while correcting for other gene properties such as the number of SNPs. A total of 7,343 gene sets, defined according to Gene Ontology biological processes, were tested from MSigDB version 7.069. In the main text we report the gene sets with P values that met Bonferroni correction for multiple testing (P < 0.05/7,343).

In addition, we used the list of single closest genes to the 27 lead SNPs arising from mvGWAS (Table 1) as input for gene set enrichment analysis, using the same 7,343 Gene Ontology biological process gene sets, but now on the basis of the hypergeometric test as implemented in GENE2FUNC of FUMA38, which is appropriate for gene lists.

Finally, we used the CELL TYPE function (as implemented within FUMA) to test whether lower gene-based association P values for brain asymmetry were associated with differential expression levels across cell types, using Bonferroni correction within each separate analysis with respect to each cell-type expression dataset included in FUMA.

Protein–protein interaction network

We used the Search Tool for the Retrieval of Interacting Genes/Proteins (STRING; http://string-db.org)68 for protein network analysis, using as input the names of 57 genes identified through gene-based association analysis, as described above. The STRING dataset includes protein–protein interaction information from numerous sources, including experimental data, publications and computational prediction methods. Only links with medium confidence or higher (confidence score >0.4; default parameter) were retained.

Developmental stage analysis

Using the gene-based association P values for all 20,146 genes genome wide, we used MAGMA (default settings as implemented in FUMA) to examine whether lower gene-based P values tended to be found for genes showing relatively higher expression in BrainSpan71 gene expression data from any particular ages compared with all other ages, separately for 29 different age groups ranging from 8 postconceptional weeks to 40 years old, and 11 defined developmental stages from early prenatal to middle adulthood. We corrected for multiple testing through a FDR of 0.05 (separately for the two analyses).

Genetic overlap of brain asymmetry with brain disorders, behavioural and cognitive traits

We applied the iSECA73 toolbox that can test for genetic overlap on the basis of per-SNP association P values only (mvGWAS does not produce univariate beta coefficient effect size estimates that can be used in standard genetic correlation analysis). We tested for genetic overlap in relation to traits previously reported to associate phenotypically with different aspects of brain structural asymmetry (Main), using GWAS P values from previously published, large-scale studies: educational attainment (n = 1,131,881)66, handedness (n = 331,037)45, intelligence (n = 269,867)67, autism spectrum disorder (n = 46,350)78, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (n = 55,374)74 and schizophrenia (n = 82,315)89. We also tested for genetic overlap in relation to brain intracranial volume (n = 32,438)94. After LD-based filtering and clumping using default parameters, iSECA tests for pleiotropy between two sets of GWAS results using an exact binomial statistical test at each of 12 P value levels: P ≤ (0.01, 0.05, 0.1, 0.2, 0.3, 0.4, 0.5, 0.6, 0.7, 0.8, 0.9, 1). The analysis compares the expected and observed overlap in the subsets of SNPs at these levels from two GWAS (144 combinations in total). In other words, iSECA iterates through each of the 12 P value levels and counts the number of overlapping variants between two GWAS at each P value threshold, and compares that number with the number expected under the null hypothesis of no genetic overlap, using the exact binomial test. iSECA then counts up the number of comparisons with evidence of overlap at a nominally significant level of P ≤ 0.05. To assess the significance level of overlap, we generated 1000 datasets through permutations (default parameter), which contained all the possible combinations for a pair of traits, and determined whether the number of levels with nominally significant genetic overlap was significantly more than expected by chance. Finally, Bonferroni correction <0.05 was applied for multiple testing of seven traits. Additionally, iSECA generated Q–Q plots for asymmetry mvGWAS P values conditioned on the other trait P values (for example, P ≤ 0.1, 0.2, 0.3, 0.4, 0.5, 0.75, 1.0) to visualize whether there is an excess of pleiotropic SNPs, which should be visible as a leftward shift of the curve as the P value threshold becomes tighter (Supplementary Fig. 8).

Reporting summary

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information (3.2MB, pdf)

List of Supplementary Tables and Supplementary Figures 1–9.

Reporting Summary (73KB, pdf)
Peer Review Information (192.3KB, pdf)
Supplementary Tables (135.6KB, xlsx)

Supplementary Tables 1–20 in one Excel file with multiple tabs.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the Max Planck Society (Germany) and grants from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) (054-15-101) and French National Research Agency (ANR, grant No. 15-HBPR-0001-03) as part of the FLAG-ERA consortium project ‘MULTI-LATERAL’, a Partner Project to the European Union’s Flagship Human Brain Project. The authors thank N. Tzourio-Mazoyer and A. Pepe for contributions to the MULTI-LATERAL project. This research was conducted using the UK Biobank resource under application no. 16066, with C.F. as the principal applicant. Our study made use of imaging-derived phenotypes generated by an image-processing pipeline developed and run on behalf of UK Biobank. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author contributions

Z.S.: conceptualization, methodology, analysis, visualization, original draft writing, review and editing. D.S.: methodology, analysis, bioinformatics, review and editing. A.C.-C.: conceptualization, methodology, analysis, visualization, review and editing. M.J.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, review and editing. B.M.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, review and editing. S.E.F.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, review and editing. F.C.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, review and editing. C.F.: conceptualization, direction, funding acquisition, supervision, original draft writing, review and editing.

Data Availability

The primary data used in this study are available via the UK Biobank, https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk. Other publicly available data sources and applications are cited in the Methods section. The GWAS summary statistics are made available online within the GWAS catalogue https://www.ebi.ac.uk/gwas/.

Code availability

This study used openly available software and codes, specifically GCTA (https://cnsgenomics.com/software/gcta/#GREML), BGENIE (https://jmarchini.org/bgenie/), MetaPhat (https://sourceforge.net/projects/meta-pheno-association-tracer/), FUMA (https://fuma.ctglab.nl/), MAGMA (https://ctg.cncr.nl/software/magma, also implemented in FUMA) and iSECA (https://web.archive.org/web/20190326171900/https://neurogenetics.qimrberghofer.edu.au/iSECA/).

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Peer review informationNature Human Behaviour thanks Philip Jansen and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editor(s): Jamie Horder; Charlotte Payne.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41562-021-01069-w.

References

  • 1.Duboc V, Dufourcq P, Blader P, Roussigne M. Asymmetry of the brain: development and implications. Annu. Rev. Genet. 2015;49:647–672. doi: 10.1146/annurev-genet-112414-055322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kong, X.-Z. et al. Handedness and other variables associated with human brain asymmetrical skew. Preprint at bioRxiv10.1101/756395 (2019).
  • 3.Galaburda AM, Corsiglia J, Rosen GD, Sherman GF. Planum temporale asymmetry, reappraisal since Geschwind and Levitsky. Neuropsychologia. 1987;25:853–868. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(87)90091-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Herve PY, Crivello F, Perchey G, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Handedness and cerebral anatomical asymmetries in young adult males. Neuroimage. 2006;29:1066–1079. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kong XZ, et al. Mapping cortical brain asymmetry in 17,141 healthy individuals worldwide via the ENIGMA Consortium. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2018;115:E5154–E5163. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1718418115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Guadalupe T, et al. Human subcortical brain asymmetries in 15,847 people worldwide reveal effects of age and sex. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11:1497–1514. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9629-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Toga AW, Thompson PM. Mapping brain asymmetry. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2003;4:37–48. doi: 10.1038/nrn1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mazoyer B, et al. Gaussian mixture modeling of hemispheric lateralization for language in a large sample of healthy individuals balanced for handedness. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e101165. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Corballis MC. The evolution of lateralized brain circuits. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1021. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Güntürkün O, Ströckens F, Ocklenburg S. Brain lateralization: a comparative perspective. Physiol. Rev. 2020;100:1019–1063. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00006.2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ocklenburg S, Hirnstein M, Beste C, Güntürkün O. Lateralization and cognitive systems. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:1143. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rentería ME. Cerebral asymmetry: a quantitative, multifactorial, and plastic brain Phenotype. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 2012;15:401–413. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Vingerhoets G. Phenotypes in hemispheric functional segregation? Perspectives and challenges. Phys. Life Rev. 2019;30:1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2019.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Karolis VR, Corbetta M, Thiebaut de Schotten M. The architecture of functional lateralisation and its relationship to callosal connectivity in the human brain. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:1417. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09344-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Postema MC, et al. Altered structural brain asymmetry in autism spectrum disorder in a study of 54 datasets. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:4958. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13005-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Postema, M. C. et al. Analysis of structural brain asymmetries in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in 39 datasets. Preprint at bioRxiv10.1101/2020.03.03.974758 (2020).
  • 17.Okada N, et al. Abnormal asymmetries in subcortical brain volume in schizophrenia. Mol. Psychiatry. 2016;21:1460–1466. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kasprian G, et al. The prenatal origin of hemispheric asymmetry: an in utero neuroimaging study. Cereb. Cortex. 2011;21:1076–1083. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Abu-Rustum RS, Ziade MF, Abu-Rustum SE. Reference values for the right and left fetal choroid plexus at 11 to 13 weeks: an early sign of “developmental” laterality? J. Ultrasound Med. 2013;32:1623–1629. doi: 10.7863/ultra.32.9.1623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.McCartney G, Hepper P. Development of lateralized behaviour in the human fetus from 12 to 27 weeks’ gestation. Dev. Med Child Neurol. 1999;41:83–86. doi: 10.1017/S0012162299000183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Francks C. In search of the biological roots of typical and atypical human brain asymmetry: Comment on “Phenotypes in hemispheric functional segregation? Perspectives and challenges” by Guy Vingerhoets. Phys. Life Rev. 2019;30:22–24. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2019.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Francks C. Exploring human brain lateralization with molecular genetics and genomics. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2015;1359:1–13. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Vandenberg LN, Lemire JM, Levin M. It’s never too early to get it right: a conserved role for the cytoskeleton in left–right asymmetry. Commun. Integr. Biol. 2013;6:e27155. doi: 10.4161/cib.27155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Vandenberg LN, Levin M. Perspectives and open problems in the early phases of left–right patterning. Semin. Cell Dev. Biol. 2009;20:456–463. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2008.11.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Norris DP. Cilia, calcium and the basis of left–right asymmetry. BMC Biol. 2012;10:102. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-10-102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fliegauf M, Benzing T, Omran H. When cilia go bad: cilia defects and ciliopathies. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 2007;8:880–893. doi: 10.1038/nrm2278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Postema MC, Carrion-Castillo A, Fisher SE, Vingerhoets G, Francks C. The genetics of situs inversus without primary ciliary dyskinesia. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:3677. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-60589-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.McManus IC, Martin N, Stubbings GF, Chung EM, Mitchison HM. Handedness and situs inversus in primary ciliary dyskinesia. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2004;271:2579–2582. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Vingerhoets G, et al. Brain structural and functional asymmetry in human situs inversus totalis. Brain Struct. Funct. 2018;223:1937–1952. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1598-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tanaka S, Kanzaki R, Yoshibayashi M, Kamiya T, Sugishita M. Dichotic listening in patients with situs inversus: brain asymmetry and situs asymmetry. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:869–874. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00144-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.de Kovel CGF, et al. Left–right asymmetry of maturation rates in human embryonic neural development. Biol. Psychiatry. 2017;82:204–212. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ocklenburg S, et al. Epigenetic regulation of lateralized fetal spinal gene expression underlies hemispheric asymmetries. eLife. 2017;6:e22784. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Sun T, et al. Early asymmetry of gene transcription in embryonic human left and right cerebral cortex. Science. 2005;308:1794–1798. doi: 10.1126/science.1110324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Le Guen Y, et al. Enhancer locus in ch14q23.1 modulates brain asymmetric temporal regions involved in language processing. Cereb. Cortex. 2020;30:5322–5332. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Carrion-Castillo A, et al. Genetic effects on planum temporale asymmetry and their limited relevance to neurodevelopmental disorders, intelligence or educational attainment. Cortex. 2020;124:137–153. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Yang J, et al. Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height. Nat. Genet. 2010;42:565–569. doi: 10.1038/ng.608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lin J, Tabassum R, Ripatti S, Pirinen M. MetaPhat: detecting and decomposing multivariate associations from univariate genome-wide association statistics. Front. Genet. 2020;11:431. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2020.00431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Watanabe K, Taskesen E, van Bochoven A, Posthuma D. Functional mapping and annotation of genetic associations with FUMA. Nat. Commun. 2017;8:1826. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01261-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Fromer M, et al. Gene expression elucidates functional impact of polygenic risk for schizophrenia. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1442–1453. doi: 10.1038/nn.4399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Berezniuk I, et al. Cytosolic carboxypeptidase 5 removes α- and γ-linked glutamates from tubulin. J. Biol. Chem. 2013;288:30445–30453. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M113.497917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Janke C, Magiera MM. The tubulin code and its role in controlling microtubule properties and functions. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 2020;21:307–326. doi: 10.1038/s41580-020-0214-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bernhardt R, Matus A. Light and electron microscopic studies of the distribution of microtubule-associated protein 2 in rat brain: a difference between dendritic and axonal cytoskeletons. J. Comp. Neurol. 1984;226:203–221. doi: 10.1002/cne.902260205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Wiberg A, et al. Handedness, language areas and neuropsychiatric diseases: insights from brain imaging and genetics. Brain. 2019;142:2938–2947. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Partida GC, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 48 common genetic variants associated with handedness. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;5:59–70. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00956-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.de Kovel CGF, Francks C. The molecular genetics of hand preference revisited. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:5986. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-42515-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Giusti-Rodriguez, P. M. & Sullivan, P. F. Using three-dimensional regulatory chromatin interactions from adult and fetal cortex to interpret genetic results for psychiatric disorders and cognitive traits. Preprint at bioRxiv10.1101/406330 (2019).
  • 47.Lu J, et al. Structure of the FH2 domain of Daam1: implications for formin regulation of actin assembly. J. Mol. Biol. 2007;369:1258–1269. doi: 10.1016/j.jmb.2007.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ang S-F, Zhao Z-S, Lim L, Manser E. DAAM1 is a formin required for centrosome re-orientation during cell migration. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e13064. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lancaster OM, Baum B. Shaping up to divide: coordinating actin and microtubule cytoskeletal remodelling during mitosis. Semin. Cell Dev. Biol. 2014;34:109–115. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2014.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Tischfield MA, et al. Human TUBB3 mutations perturb microtubule dynamics, kinesin interactions, and axon guidance. Cell. 2010;140:74–87. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Ramasamy A, et al. Genetic variability in the regulation of gene expression in ten regions of the human brain. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:1418–1428. doi: 10.1038/nn.3801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wang D, et al. Comprehensive functional genomic resource and integrative model for the human brain. Science. 2018;362:eaat8464. doi: 10.1126/science.aat8464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Horng S, et al. Differential gene expression in the developing lateral geniculate nucleus and medial geniculate nucleus reveals novel roles for Zic4 and Foxp2 in visual and auditory pathway development. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:13672–13683. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2127-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Del Pino I, et al. COUP-TFI/Nr2f1 orchestrates intrinsic neuronal activity during development of the somatosensory cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2020;30:5667–5685. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Shao W, Halachmi S, Brown M. ERAP140, a conserved tissue-specific nuclear receptor coactivator. Mol. Cell. Biol. 2002;22:3358–3372. doi: 10.1128/MCB.22.10.3358-3372.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Beales PL, Elcioglu N, Woolf AS, Parker D, Flinter FA. New criteria for improved diagnosis of Bardet–Biedl syndrome: results of a population survey. J. Med Genet. 1999;36:437–446. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Zaghloul NA, Katsanis N. Mechanistic insights into Bardet–Biedl syndrome, a model ciliopathy. J. Clin. Investig. 2009;119:428–437. doi: 10.1172/JCI37041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Nenasheva VV, Tarantul VZ. Many faces of TRIM proteins on the road from pluripotency to neurogenesis. Stem Cells Dev. 2020;29:1–14. doi: 10.1089/scd.2019.0152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Himmelfarb M, et al. ITIH5, a novel member of the inter-alpha-trypsin inhibitor heavy chain family is downregulated in breast cancer. Cancer Lett. 2004;204:69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.canlet.2003.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Schmitt AD, et al. A compendium of chromatin contact maps reveals spatially active regions in the human genome. Cell Rep. 2016;17:2042–2059. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.10.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.O’Sullivan ML, et al. FLRT proteins are endogenous latrophilin ligands and regulate excitatory synapse development. Neuron. 2012;73:903–910. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Sertie AL, et al. Collagen XVIII, containing an endogenous inhibitor of angiogenesis and tumor growth, plays a critical role in the maintenance of retinal structure and in neural tube closure (Knobloch syndrome) Hum. Mol. Genet. 2000;9:2051–2058. doi: 10.1093/hmg/9.13.2051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Feng J, Han Q, Zhou L. Planar cell polarity genes, Celsr1-3, in neural development. Neurosci. Bull. 2012;28:309–315. doi: 10.1007/s12264-012-1232-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Al-Mubarak B, et al. Whole exome sequencing reveals inherited and de novo variants in autism spectrum disorder: a trio study from Saudi families. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:5679. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-06033-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.de Leeuw CA, Mooij JM, Heskes T, Posthuma D. MAGMA: generalized gene-set analysis of GWAS data. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2015;11:e1004219. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Lee JJ, et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat. Genet. 2018;50:1112–1121. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Savage JE, et al. Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nat. Genet. 2018;50:912–919. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0152-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Szklarczyk D, et al. The STRING database in 2017: quality-controlled protein–protein association networks, made broadly accessible. Nucleic Acids Res. 2017;45:D362–D368. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Liberzon A, et al. Molecular signatures database (MSigDB) 3.0. Bioinformatics. 2011;27:1739–1740. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Grasby KL, et al. The genetic architecture of the human cerebral cortex. Science. 2020;367:eaay6690. doi: 10.1126/science.aay6690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Miller JA, et al. Transcriptional landscape of the prenatal human brain. Nature. 2014;508:199–206. doi: 10.1038/nature13185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.de Kovel CGF, Lisgo SN, Fisher SE, Francks C. Subtle left–right asymmetry of gene expression profiles in embryonic and foetal human brains. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:12606. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29496-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Nyholt DR. SECA: SNP effect concordance analysis using genome-wide association summary results. Bioinformatics. 2014;30:2086–2088. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Demontis D, et al. Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat. Genet. 2019;51:63–75. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Dang LC, et al. Caudate asymmetry is related to attentional impulsivity and an objective measure of ADHD-like attentional problems in healthy adults. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016;221:277–286. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0906-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Wu ZM, et al. Altered brain white matter microstructural asymmetry in children with ADHD. Psychiatry Res. 2020;285:112817. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Zou H, Yang J. Temporal variability-based functional brain lateralization study in ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 2019;25:839–847. doi: 10.1177/1087054719859074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Grove J, et al. Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Genet. 2019;51:431–444. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Carper RA, Treiber JM, DeJesus SY, Muller RA. Reduced hemispheric asymmetry of white matter microstructure in autism spectrum disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2016;55:1073–1080. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.09.491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.De Fosse L, et al. Language-association cortex asymmetry in autism and specific language impairment. Ann. Neurol. 2004;56:757–766. doi: 10.1002/ana.20275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Floris, D. L. et al. Atypical brain asymmetry in autism–a candidate for clinically meaningful stratification. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.08.008 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 82.Herbert MR, et al. Brain asymmetries in autism and developmental language disorder: a nested whole-brain analysis. Brain. 2005;128:213–226. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Noroozian M, Lotfi J, Gassemzadeh H, Emami H, Mehrabi Y. Academic achievement and learning abilities in left-handers: guilt or gift? Cortex. 2002;38:779–785. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70044-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Cheyne CP, Roberts N, Crow TJ, Leask SJ, Garcia-Finana M. The effect of handedness on academic ability: a multivariate linear mixed model approach. Laterality. 2010;15:451–464. doi: 10.1080/13576500902976956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Mellet E, et al. Weak language lateralization affects both verbal and spatial skills: an fMRI study in 297 subjects. Neuropsychologia. 2014;65:56–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Papadatou-Pastou M, Tomprou DM. Intelligence and handedness: meta-analyses of studies on intellectually disabled, typically developing, and gifted individuals. Neurosci. Biobehav Rev. 2015;56:151–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Prichard E, Propper RE, Christman SD. Degree of handedness, but not direction, is a systematic predictor of cognitive performance. Front Psychol. 2013;4:9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Reio TG, Jr., Czarnolewski M, Eliot J. Handedness and spatial ability: differential patterns of relationships. Laterality. 2004;9:339–358. doi: 10.1080/13576500342000220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature. 2014;511:421–427. doi: 10.1038/nature13595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.DeLisi LE, et al. Anomalous cerebral asymmetry and language processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1997;23:255–271. doi: 10.1093/schbul/23.2.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Shenton ME, Dickey CC, Frumin M, McCarley RW. A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2001;49:1–52. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00163-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Kawasaki Y, et al. Anomalous cerebral asymmetry in patients with schizophrenia demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. Biol. Psychiatry. 2008;63:793–800. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Sun Y, Chen Y, Collinson SL, Bezerianos A, Sim K. Reduced hemispheric asymmetry of brain anatomical networks is linked to schizophrenia: a connectome study. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27:602–615. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Adams HH, et al. Novel genetic loci underlying human intracranial volume identified through genome-wide association. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1569–1582. doi: 10.1038/nn.4398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Bycroft C, et al. The UK biobank resource with deep phenotyping and genomic data. Nature. 2018;562:203–209. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0579-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Tee YH, et al. Cellular chirality arising from the self-organization of the actin cytoskeleton. Nat. Cell Biol. 2015;17:445–457. doi: 10.1038/ncb3137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Inaki M, Liu J, Matsuno K. Cell chirality: its origin and roles in left–right asymmetric development. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2016;371:20150403. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Okumura T, et al. The development and evolution of left–right asymmetry in invertebrates: lessons from Drosophila and snails. Dev. Dyn. 2008;237:3497–3515. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.21788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Davison A, et al. Formin is associated with left–right asymmetry in the pond snail and the frog. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:654–660. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Steinhauer J, Kalderon D. Microtubule polarity and axis formation in the Drosophila oocyte. Dev. Dyn. 2006;235:1455–1468. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.20770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.McNiven MA, Porter KR. Organization of microtubules in centrosome-free cytoplasm. J. Cell Biol. 1988;106:1593–1605. doi: 10.1083/jcb.106.5.1593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Lobikin M, et al. Early, nonciliary role for microtubule proteins in left–right patterning is conserved across kingdoms. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2012;109:12586–12591. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202659109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Fan J, Zhang H, Rahman T, Stanton DN, Wan LQ. Cell organelle-based analysis of cell chirality. Commun. Integr. Biol. 2019;12:78–81. doi: 10.1080/19420889.2019.1605277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Levin M. Is the early left–right axis like a plant, a kidney, or a neuron? The integration of physiological signals in embryonic asymmetry. Birth Defects Res. Part C: Embryo Today.: Rev. 2006;78:191–223. doi: 10.1002/bdrc.20078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.McDowell G, Rajadurai S, Levin M. From cytoskeletal dynamics to organ asymmetry: a nonlinear, regulative pathway underlies left–right patterning. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2016;371:20150409. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Bulik-Sullivan BK, et al. LD Score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies. Nat. Genet. 2015;47:291–295. doi: 10.1038/ng.3211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Zhou D, Lebel C, Evans A, Beaulieu C. Cortical thickness asymmetry from childhood to older adulthood. Neuroimage. 2013;83:66–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Roe JM, et al. Asymmetric thinning of the cerebral cortex across the adult lifespan is accelerated in Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Commun. 2021;12:721. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-21057-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Davey Smith G, Ebrahim S. ‘Mendelian randomization’: can genetic epidemiology contribute to understanding environmental determinants of disease? Int. J. Epidemiol. 2003;32:1–22. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyg070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.de Kovel CGF, Carrion-Castillo A, Francks C. A large-scale population study of early life factors influencing left-handedness. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:584. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-37423-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.McManus IC. Handedness, language dominance and aphasia: a genetic model. Psychol. Med Monogr. Suppl. 1985;8:1–40. doi: 10.1017/S0264180100001879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Bishop DVM, Bates TC. Heritability of language laterality assessed by functional transcranial doppler ultrasound: a twin study. Wellcome Open Res. 2019;4:161. doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15524.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Janke C, Bulinski JC. Post-translational regulation of the microtubule cytoskeleton: mechanisms and functions. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 2011;12:773–786. doi: 10.1038/nrm3227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Geiger B, Bershadsky A, Pankov R, Yamada KM. Transmembrane crosstalk between the extracellular matrix–cytoskeleton crosstalk. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 2001;2:793–805. doi: 10.1038/35099066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Young AI. Solving the missing heritability problem. PLoS Genet. 2019;15:e1008222. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1008222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Mitchell, K. J. Innate: How the Wiring of Our Brains Shapes Who We Are (Princeton Univ. Press, 2018).
  • 117.Aschard H, Vilhjalmsson BJ, Joshi AD, Price AL, Kraft P. Adjusting for heritable covariates can bias effect estimates in genome-wide association studies. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2015;96:329–339. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.12.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Sadler TW. Embryology of neural tube development. Am. J. Med. Genet. C. Semin. Med. Genet. 2005;135C:2–8. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.c.30049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Purcell S, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2007;81:559–575. doi: 10.1086/519795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Desikan RS, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Alfaro-Almagro F, et al. Image processing and quality control for the first 10,000 brain imaging datasets from UK biobank. Neuroimage. 2018;166:400–424. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Kurth F, Gaser C, Luders E. A 12-step user guide for analyzing voxel-wise gray matter asymmetries in statistical parametric mapping (SPM) Nat. Protoc. 2015;10:293–304. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2015.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Leroy F, et al. New human-specific brain landmark: the depth asymmetry of superior temporal sulcus. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2015;112:1208–1213. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1412389112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Lee SH, Yang J, Goddard ME, Visscher PM, Wray NR. Estimation of pleiotropy between complex diseases using single-nucleotide polymorphism-derived genomic relationships and restricted maximum likelihood. Bioinformatics. 2012;28:2540–2542. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bts474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Hoggart CJ, Clark TG, De Iorio M, Whittaker JC, Balding DJ. Genome-wide significance for dense SNP and resequencing data. Genet. Epidemiol. 2008;32:179–185. doi: 10.1002/gepi.20292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Panagiotou OA, Ioannidis JP, Project G-WS. What should the genome-wide significance threshold be? Empirical replication of borderline genetic associations. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2012;41:273–286. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.The 1000 Genomes Project Consortium. A global reference for human genetic variation. Nature. 2015;526:68–74. doi: 10.1038/nature15393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Wang K, Li M, Hakonarson H. ANNOVAR: functional annotation of genetic variants from high-throughput sequencing data. Nucleic Acids Res. 2010;38:e164. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkq603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Rentzsch P, Witten D, Cooper GM, Shendure J, Kircher M. CADD: predicting the deleteriousness of variants throughout the human genome. Nucleic Acids Res. 2019;47:D886–D894. doi: 10.1093/nar/gky1016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Boyle AP, et al. Annotation of functional variation in personal genomes using RegulomeDB. Genome Res. 2012;22:1790–1797. doi: 10.1101/gr.137323.112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Ernst J, Kellis M. ChromHMM: automating chromatin-state discovery and characterization. Nat. Methods. 2012;9:215–216. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Roadmap Epigenomics Consortium. et al. Integrative analysis of 111 reference human epigenomes. Nature. 2015;518:317–330. doi: 10.1038/nature14248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Kircher M, et al. A general framework for estimating the relative pathogenicity of human genetic variants. Nat. Genet. 2014;46:310–315. doi: 10.1038/ng.2892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.MacArthur J, et al. The new NHGRI-EBI catalog of published genome-wide association studies (GWAS catalog) Nucleic Acids Res. 2017;45:D896–D901. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw1133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.GTEx Consortium. Human genomics. The Genotype-Tissue Expression (GTEx) pilot analysis: multitissue gene regulation in humans. Science. 2015;348:648–660. doi: 10.1126/science.1262110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information (3.2MB, pdf)

List of Supplementary Tables and Supplementary Figures 1–9.

Reporting Summary (73KB, pdf)
Peer Review Information (192.3KB, pdf)
Supplementary Tables (135.6KB, xlsx)

Supplementary Tables 1–20 in one Excel file with multiple tabs.

Data Availability Statement

The primary data used in this study are available via the UK Biobank, https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk. Other publicly available data sources and applications are cited in the Methods section. The GWAS summary statistics are made available online within the GWAS catalogue https://www.ebi.ac.uk/gwas/.

This study used openly available software and codes, specifically GCTA (https://cnsgenomics.com/software/gcta/#GREML), BGENIE (https://jmarchini.org/bgenie/), MetaPhat (https://sourceforge.net/projects/meta-pheno-association-tracer/), FUMA (https://fuma.ctglab.nl/), MAGMA (https://ctg.cncr.nl/software/magma, also implemented in FUMA) and iSECA (https://web.archive.org/web/20190326171900/https://neurogenetics.qimrberghofer.edu.au/iSECA/).


Articles from Nature Human Behaviour are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES