Abstract
Living organisms experience a worldwide continuous increase in artificial light at night (ALAN), negatively affecting their behaviour. The field cricket, an established model in physiology and behaviour, can provide insights into the effect of ALAN on insect behaviour. The stridulation and locomotion patterns of adult male crickets reared under different lifelong ALAN intensities were monitored simultaneously for five consecutive days in custom-made anechoic chambers. Daily activity periods and acrophases were compared between the experimental groups. Control crickets exhibited a robust rhythm, stridulating at night and demonstrating locomotor activity during the day. By contrast, ALAN affected both the relative level and timing of the crickets' nocturnal and diurnal activity. ALAN induced free-running patterns, manifested in significant changes in the median and variance of the activity periods, and even arrhythmic behaviour. The magnitude of disruption was light intensity dependent, revealing an increase in the difference between the activity periods calculated for stridulation and locomotion in the same individual. This finding may indicate the existence of two peripheral clocks. Our results demonstrate that ecologically relevant ALAN intensities affect crickets’ behavioural patterns, and may lead to decoupling of locomotion and stridulation behaviours at the individual level, and to loss of synchronization at the population level.
Keywords: light pollution, insects, bioacoustics, circadian rhythm, biological clock
1. Introduction
Artificial light at night (ALAN) is increasing worldwide by about 3–6% annually [1], with more than 80% of the world population living under light-polluted skies [2]. Awareness of the harmful effects of ALAN on living organisms is also increasing [3,4], including reports of changes in the length and quality of sleep [5,6], and in temporal activity shifts in mammals [5,7,8], birds [6,9,10], anurans [11] and marine species [12,13]. In insects, ALAN-induced changes in foraging activity could lead to higher predation risk [14,15], possible loss of camouflage [16–19] and increased mortality, partially owing to the attraction of many flying insects to light [20–22]. Insects also experience temporal disorientation under ALAN [17], leading to changes in community structure [1,14,16], and insect-induced environmental changes such as reduced pollination in meadows subjected to ALAN [23]. However, our knowledge of the effects of ALAN, specifically of its various intensities, on insect behaviour, temporal activity partitioning and fitness is far from complete.
Light is the most reliable Zeitgeber, synchronizing behavioural and physiological events through entrainment of the endogenous clock mechanism [24,25]. Diverse species, as well as different behaviours in the same organism, may react differently to the same light triggers or regimes, depending on their diurnal, nocturnal or crepuscular way of life [26,27]. Circadian rhythms and daily activity patterns can be expressed in locomotion, sleeping, foraging, singing, gene expression and more [25,26,28]. Violation of the natural light–dark cycle may affect these rhythmical patterns. Changes in a light cycle or its disruption may induce either masking (an immediate response to the light stimulus that does not synchronize the pacemaker) [7,27] or entrainment (synchronization of the rhythms to environmental cues). Constant light conditions may evoke free-running (an endogenous rhythm with a period differing from 24 h that is not synchronized to any external cue), or arrhythmic behaviour [7,24,26,27].
Crickets (Gryllidae) have been widely used as models for the study of insect physiology, neurobiology and behaviour, including circadian activity [29,30]. They are known to demonstrate clear diel cycles in two fundamental behaviours, stridulation and locomotion [31–35]. In several cricket species, including Gryllus bimaculatus, temporal shifts in the insect's locomotor or stridulatory circadian activity have been reported following exposure to changes in illumination patterns [31–33]. These temporal shifts in activity patterns were in accord with changes in gene expression patterns connected to the circadian pathway [25,36,37]. Locomotion is important for the individual's fitness, especially in the context of foraging and the risk of predation. Stridulation, explicitly calling songs used for communication and female attraction, is crucial for the reproduction of the species. Any asynchrony in these behaviours among the population is critical. Nevertheless, the possible effects of ecologically relevant ALAN intensities on crickets, specifically the insects' susceptibility to different light intensities, have not been investigated to date.
Here, we studied the effects of exposing male G. bimaculatus crickets to lifelong ALAN on their stridulation and locomotion patterns. This is one of very few examples in which the two behaviours have been monitored simultaneously [32,34,35], conducing to a more comprehensive understanding of the ALAN-induced behavioural effects.
2. Material and methods
(a) . Rearing conditions
Gryllus bimaculatus crickets were reared under a constant temperature of 26 ± 2°C. Different experimental groups were reared from the egg stadium to hatching and through all life stages (i.e. lifelong) under one of four different light regimes. The rearing chambers were illuminated with white fluorescent light (CFL, NeptOn, 6500 K, 380–780 nm, peak: 547 and 612 nm; different light intensities were achieved by shading the light bulbs). All groups were exposed to 12 h daylight of 40 lux. Conditions between groups varied during the 12 h night period, as follows: (i) 12 h daylight : 12 h dark (LD, control), (ii) 12 h daylight : 12 h 2 lux ALAN (LL2), (iii) 12 h daylight : 12 h 5 lux ALAN (LL5), and (iv) 24 h constant daylight (LL). Crickets were fed three times a week with dog-food pellets and vegetables. The rearing boxes contained water flasks with absorbent cotton wool. Humidity was between 60 and 70%.
(b) . Experimental set-up
Male crickets, 3–7 days post-adult emergence, were individually assigned to one of four similar custom-made experimental anechoic chambers, enabling continuous and concomitant monitoring of the insect's locomotor and stridulation behaviours, and eliminating intraspecific communication (figure 1). The chambers were composed of Formica-laminated plywood plates (17 mm thick) covered with black acoustic foam (5 cm thick, density 33). Light was provided via a 5 W white CFL bulb (NeptOn, 6500 K, 380–780 nm, peak: 547 and 612 nm, see the electronic supplementary material, figure S1). Lighting conditions and the light regimes in the experimental chamber were similar to those that the insect had experienced in the rearing chambers (one of four, as described above). Particular care was taken to randomize the group-experimental chamber linkage. Light intensities were measured at four locations at the bottom of the chamber. Measurements were conducted at the cricket's eye level, at a distance of approximately 65 cm from the light bulb, using a digital light meter (TES-1337, TES, Taiwan). The light spectra were recorded using a Sekonic Spectromaster C-700 (North White Plans, NY, USA). The mean ambient temperature was 25 ± 2°C. The anechoic chamber contained a plastic box with food, water and a paper cupcake mould, providing shelter and an elevated stridulation platform.
Figure 1.
The experimental set-up: a cricket (1) was placed in a plastic box (2) in an anechoic chamber covered with acoustic insulation (3), equipped with a light source (4), a microphone (5) and an infrared camera (6). Sound and movement were recorded simultaneously and continuously (7, 8, respectively).
Acoustic recordings were conducted using a NT6 condenser microphone (Rhode Microphones, Sydney, Australia), placed 70–85 cm above the animal, connected to a M-track Quad amplifier (M-AUDIO, Cumberland, RI, USA), at 44 100 Hz and 16 bit depth, using a computer and RavenLite2.0.0 [38]. Locomotion activity was captured from above at 2 frames s−1, by an infrared (IR) surveillance camera (constantly emitting IR light, peak: 799 nm), connected to a computer using the Active WebCam program (PY Software) for motion detection [39]. A threshold was defined as a change of more than 2% of pixels in the picture. IR enabled tracking the animals' locomotion under all light regimes, including scotophases.
(c) . Data processing and statistical analysis
Only recordings containing at least five consecutive days and nights of behavioural data were used. In cases in which the crickets did not perform either locomotion or stridulation behaviour in the first few days, or when system failure led to missing days of data, these days were removed from the analysis of that behaviour. Stridulation data extraction was conducted using ‘R’, version 3.4.1 [40], the ‘Rraven’ open source package [41] and RavenPro1.5 [42]. Data processing and statistical analyses were conducted in Python v. 3.7 (PyCharm, JetBrains), MATLAB (The MathWorks, MA, USA), SPSS v. 21 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) and Prism 8 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA, USA). The number of stridulation syllables and locomotion events were assessed per animal in 10 min bouts. For diurnal and nocturnal quantitative activity comparisons, values were normalized for each individual by dividing that individual's values by its own mean value. For behaviour, rhythmicity, and period analyses and comparisons, values were normalized for each individual by dividing that individual's values by its own maximum value, resulting in an activity index ranging from 0 (no activity) to 1 (maximal activity). Periodogram analyses of the activity rhythm periods were determined using the ImageJ plugin ActogramJ [43].
Comparisons of nocturnal and diurnal activity, as well as the absolute difference within groups, were conducted with the Kruskal–Wallis test, followed using a Dunn's test. Control nocturnal and diurnal activities of the same individuals were compared by a paired t-test. The analysis of the nocturnal and diurnal activity in the constant daylight (LL) crickets was based on the objective day and night periods. The analyses of the medians and variance of the cycle periods were conducted using MATLAB. Values over three times the absolute deviations from the scaled median were considered as outliers and removed. The median and variance of the periods were compared using the Kruskal–Wallis test and the Brown–Forsythe test for equality of variance, respectively. Both tests were followed by a Bonferroni multiple comparison post hoc test. Spearman's rank-order correlation was used to evaluate the relationship between stridulation and locomotion behaviour. Differences between both behaviours were assessed using the Kruskal–Wallis test. A χ2 test was used to evaluate a possible connection between ALAN intensity and rhythm types. It should be noted that most of our data are characterized by inequality of variance and/or by non-normal distribution. Hence, medians rather than means were often presented, and non-parametric statistical tests were used.
The mean acrophase (the time at which the peak of a rhythm occurs) of 5 days was calculated for each animal, period and behaviour using the CosinorPy package [44]. Circular statistical analyses were conducted using the Oriana software, v. 4 (Kovach Computing Wales, UK) [45]. Phases were averaged per treatment and behaviour. The Mardia–Watson–Wheeler test was used for distribution comparisons among treatments and the Hotellings paired test assessed phase differences within the same individual. Both tests were followed by a Bonferroni correction.
3. Results
(a) . Crickets’ morphological characteristics
The lifelong lighting conditions had a somewhat inconsistent effect on the crickets' morphological characteristics (size, weight and morphometric relationships; electronic supplementary material, figure S2). While we observed a significant effect on the body condition index (Kruskal–Wallis, p < 0.038), no significant differences were found among the different treatments (Dunn's post hoc multiple comparison, p > 0.9 for all comparisons; electronic supplementary material, figure S2).
(b) . Temporal patterns of stridulation and locomotion behaviours
The control crickets exhibited an activity rhythm of 24 h, with stridulation behaviour displayed predominantly at night (figure 2a(i)(ii); 5.6% diurnally and 94.4% nocturnally, paired t-test, n = 15, p < 0.0001), and locomotor activity mainly during the day (figure 2b(i)(ii); 68.3% diurnally and 31.7% nocturnally; paired t-test, n = 11, p < 0.05, electronic supplementary material, tables S1 and S2). Figure 2a(iii),b(iii) presents examples of a double-plotted actogram of two individuals representing typical LD behaviours.
Figure 2.
Light-dependent behaviour of control animals. Normalized activity cycle of stridulation (a)(i) and locomotion (b)(i) (mean ± s.e.). Normalized day and night activity levels (a(ii),b(ii); mean ± s.e.). Stridulation; n = 15; locomotion; n = 11; *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001. Values were normalized by dividing each individual's values by its own maximum value, resulting in an activity index ranging from 0 (no activity) to 1 (maximal activity). Double-plotted actograms representing typical LD behaviours of two individuals (a(iii),b(iii)). Yellow and black bars indicate diurnal and nocturnal phases, respectively. (Online version in colour.)
A quantitative comparison of diurnal and nocturnal activity in the different experimental groups revealed that the median of the normalized diurnal, as well as nocturnal LD stridulation activity level significantly differed from the LL5 and LL treatments (p < 0.007 for all, Kruskal–Wallis test with Dunn's multiple comparisons; figure 3a(i); electronic supplementary material, table S1). Significant differences in diurnal locomotor activity level were found between LL and all other treatments, while for the nocturnal locomotor activity levels, differences were significant only between LL and both LL2 and LL5 (p < 0.01 for diurnal locomotion, p < 0.02 for nocturnal locomotion, Kruskal–Wallis test with Dunn's multiple comparisons, figure 3b(i); electronic supplementary material, table S1). In both behaviours, the effects of the light regime and ALAN were also apparent when comparing the difference between diurnal and nocturnal activity levels (Kruskal–Wallis test, p < 0.0001, figure 3a(ii),b(ii); electronic supplementary material, table S1).
Figure 3.
ALAN intensities induce variation in the temporal activity and rhythmicity of crickets. Quantitative comparisons of diurnal (yellow, small letters) and nocturnal (black and grey, capital letters) stridulation (a)(i) and locomotion (b)(i) activity under LD (n = 15, n = 11, respectively), LL2 (n = 20, n = 25, respectively), LL5 (n = 20, n = 17, respectively) and LL (n = 21, n = 40, respectively) treatments (mean ± s.e.). Different letters represent statistical differences. The differences between nocturnal and diurnal activity of stridulation (a)(ii) and locomotion (b)(ii) under LD (blue), LL2 (purple), LL5 (green) and LL (red) treatments (mean ± s.e.). Normalized activity cycles per hour of (a)(iii) stridulation and (b)(iii) locomotion under LD (blue), LL2 (purple), LL5 (green) and LL (red) treatments (mean ± s.e.). Five consecutive days (yellow) and nights (black, grey) are presented (LD show the same data as in figure 2a(i) and b(i)). Values for a(i)(ii) and b(i)(ii) were normalized by dividing each individual's values by its own mean value, while values for a(iii) and b(iii) were normalized by dividing each individual's values by its own maximum value, resulting in an activity index ranging from 0 (no activity) to 1 (maximal activity). (Online version in colour.)
The different experimental groups also differed in the temporal patterns of the two monitored behaviours, with a clear ALAN-dependent decrease in rhythmicity (figure 3a(iii),b(iii)). This was observed in both stridulation (figure 3a(iii)) and locomotion (figure 3b(iii)) behaviours. For both behaviours, averaging the free-running data of the different LL individuals resulted in a practically absent rhythm.
(c) . Period and group variance significantly differed among treatments
The data in figure 4 present the analysis of the periods of the recorded behavioural patterns. Only individuals that exhibited a significant period (electronic supplementary material, table S1) were included in this analysis. The median period of stridulation activity cycles differed significantly between the LD crickets and both the LL5 and LL groups (24.0 h, 25.17 h and 25.67 h, respectively; n = 15, 19 and 17, respectively; Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons). The LL group also differed from LL2 (24.67 h; n = 19). Furthermore, the variance in the period of stridulation of all three ALAN treatments significantly differed from that in the LD treatment (figure 4a; Brown–Forsythe test with the Bonferroni correction).
Figure 4.
ALAN affects the individual periods, the medians and variance of stridulation (a) and locomotor (b) activity of the groups. Red lines represent medians, and individual males are plotted in coloured dots. Coloured areas are kernel histogram, rotated by 90°. Black vertical lines represent the inter-quartile range 25% below and 25% above the median. Upper asterisks: differences between medians; bottom asterisks: differences between variances. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. ALAN affects the acrophase of individuals' behaviour, and the group variance in stridulation (c) and locomotion (d) behaviour. Each point in the circular plots represents the mean phase of 5 days of activity of an individual male cricket. Grid lines = 0.25. The black arrow represents the mean vector of phase, calculated for the entire experimental group. (Online version in colour.)
The median period of locomotor activity differed significantly between LL and all three other treatments (figure 4b; 25.5 h in LL; 24.0 h in LD, LL2, and LL5; n = 24, 11, 25 and 13, respectively; Bonferroni correction). In addition, the variance in the period of locomotor behaviour differed significantly between LL and both LD and LL2 treatments (figure 4b; Brown–Forsythe test with the Bonferroni correction).
No correlation was found between stridulation and locomotion activity cycle periods (Spearman's rank-order correlation, rs47 = 0.204, p = 0.160). A similar lack of correlation persisted when examining the different treatments separately. Moreover, the calculated individual absolute difference between the rhythm periods of stridulation and locomotion was found to differ significantly among treatments (Kruskal–Wallis test, χ23,49 = 9.75, p = 0.021), suggesting the asynchrony of these two periods.
(d) . Artificial light at night elicited three types of activity patterns
Three types of activity pattern were observed in the different treatments, in both stridulation and locomotion activity: synchronized rhythms (periods of 24 h; figure 5a(i)), free-run rhythms (periods different from 24 h; figure 5a(ii)) and arrhythmic activity (lack of any period; figure 5a(iii)). The proportions of these types of rhythms in each of the experimental groups reflect a clear effect of the ALAN intensities on the behavioural activity (figure 5b(i)(ii); electronic supplementary material, table S2). Both the percentage of synchronized stridulation and locomotion rhythms dropped steeply from the LD to all ALAN treatments (figure 5b(i)(ii); electronic supplementary material, tables S1 and S2). An opposite trend was observed for the free-run rhythms, which were lowest in the control and high in all other treatments. In both behaviours, arrhythmic activity was not observed in LD, while common in LL, and also present in LL2 and LL5 (figure 5b(i)(ii); electronic supplementary material, table S2). The occurrence of an activity type was not associated with the type of behaviour (i.e. stridulation or locomotion; χ22,190 = 0.00, p = 1). However, the activity type was significantly dependent on the treatment (i.e. the light regime (χ26,190 = 105.57, p < 0.0001).
Figure 5.
ALAN induces different types or activity patterns. Double-plotted actograms demonstrating activity patterns of adult male crickets: (a)(i) a synchronized rhythm of 24 h; (a)(ii) a free-run rhythm of 26 h; and (a)(iii) arrhythmic behaviour. Yellow and black bars indicate diurnal and nocturnal phases, respectively. Percentage of synchronized (dark red), free-run (orange) and arrhythmic (yellow) stridulation (b)(i) and locomotion (b)(ii) behaviour in male adult crickets exposed to the different lifelong ALAN intensities. (Online version in colour.)
(e) . Phase distributions significantly differed among treatments
Only individuals that exhibited a significant period (electronic supplementary material, table S1) were included in the acrophase analysis of stridulation and locomotion behaviour (figure 4c,d; electronic supplementary material, table S3). In stridulation behaviour, vector length was gradually reduced with ALAN intensity (LD: 0.81, LL2: 0.40, LL5: 0.42 and LL: 0.30), while in locomotion behaviour, the intensity was reduced with LL5 and LL only (LD: 0.67, LL2: 0.75, LL5: 0.47, and LL: 0.26; electronic supplementary material, table S3). In addition, the variance in both activities became larger with higher ALAN intensity (figure 4c,d; electronic supplementary material, table S3), while the mean phase vector angles remained steady in the first three treatments (LD, LL2, LL5), indicating an overall loss of phase synchronization with higher ALAN intensity. The mean phase vector angles of stridulation activity differed significantly between the LL and all other treatments and that of the locomotion activity differed between LL, LD and LL2 (Watson–Williams F-test, p < 0.0167). However, an ALAN-intensity-dependent phase distribution was observed, with LD narrowly distributed and LL evenly distributed. Stridulation LD differed significantly from that of LL2 and LL (Mardia–Watson–Wheeler test, p = 0.003 and p = 0.001, respectively), while in locomotion behaviour, the LL treatment differed significantly from that of the LL2 and somewhat also from LD (Mardia–Watson–Wheeler test, p < 0.0002 and p = 0.033, respectively). A moderate circular–linear correlation was found between the treatments and both activity behaviours (r = 0.4, and r = 0.44, respectively).
Conducting the same analysis only on animals for which we had data for both stridulation and locomotion (for n, see the electronic supplementary material, table S1) enabled the comparison of the phase between the two behaviours. This revealed yet another effect of ALAN: in both, LL5 and LL, the significant difference between the stridulation and locomotion phases, observed under control conditions, was lost (Hotelling's paired test, LDstridulation − LDlocomotion: p < 0.0001, LL2stridulation − LL2locomotion: p < 0.0001, LL5stridulation − LL5locomotion: p = 0.136, LLstridulation − LLlocomotion: p = 0.765).
4. Discussion
(a) . Crickets as a model for artificial light at night research
Despite the use of crickets as model insects to study behaviour, physiology and neurobiology for over a century [30,46], simultaneous and parallel long-term monitoring of individual insect stridulation and locomotion has, to our knowledge, never been carried out in this insect. Our unique custom-made set-up enabled such monitoring under various light conditions, providing an excellent signal-to-noise ratio. Our findings add to the studies of Tomioka & Matsumoto [25] in supporting G. bimaculatus as a suitable model insect for ALAN research.
In the present study, we report on the temporal partitioning of field cricket behaviour, with stridulation being predominantly nocturnal and locomotion predominantly diurnal. This is in contrast with previous studies in the same species that demonstrated the diurnal locomotion of nymphs, but nocturnal locomotor activity of adults [47]. The growth chamber and experimental set-up temperatures were 24–28°C. Hence, the above discrepancy cannot be explained by rhythm reversal as a result of exposure to low temperatures [48]. The natural behaviour of the species might differ among different laboratory colonies, and reflect certain adaptive colony-specific behaviours. Additionally, there may be some indirect effects of the different illumination and motion detection methods used in the different laboratories. While, as noted, our method revealed mostly diurnal locomotion, we did observe in the LD group a peak in locomotion at sunset, preceding the beginning of stridulation behaviour, which is in accord with the actograms shown in [47, fig. 1 therein]. Although this has little impact on the main findings reported here, this issue deserves further investigation.
When studying the effects of light on behaviour, the visual system of the model insect should be taken into consideration. The following spectral types have been described in the cricket G. bimaculatus: UV (peak: 332 nm), blue (peak: 445 nm) and green (peak: 515 nm), resulting in a spectral sensitivity of up to 600 nm [49], with the performance of the photoreceptors leading to the conclusion that the crickets' eye has evolved for signal processing in dim light [50]. In our experiments, shading the light bulbs in order to create lower light intensities resulted in a relative decrease in the blue wavelength (400–500 nm) intensity and an increase in the red wavelength (greater than 700 nm) intensity. Interestingly, while this increase in red wavelength had no influence on the experiments, as crickets do not possess the ability to see red light, a clear effect of LL2 was observed despite the decrease in the blue wavelength.
As reported, the different lifelong lighting regimes used in our study did not reveal any clear effect on the general properties of the crickets’ morphology. This aspect should also be further explored; see available data showing larger femur length in ALAN-exposed Teleogryllus commodus, in [51]).
(b) . Ecological perspectives
Despite increasing concern regarding the harmful effects of ALAN on humans and other animals [18,52–54], there is still insufficient knowledge regarding its impact on insects, which constitute a major bio-indicator of environmental changes and pollution [55,56]. The ALAN intensities investigated here had been previously reported as prevalent and ecologically relevant in urban environments, affecting the behaviour and ecology of various species [5,9,17,54,57], as well as having a ‘sink’ effect on flying insects attracted towards street lights [3,18,20,58,59]. We explored the effect of ALAN on two key behaviours: locomotion, which is important in foraging; and stridulation, which is a major component of the cricket's courtship behaviour, sexual selection and intraspecific communication [60–66]. The timing of these behaviours, as well as their synchrony among individuals, has an important impact on both the fitness of the individual and the reproductive success of the population.
In our study, both behaviours presented a robust rhythm in LD, with clear diurnal and nocturnal partitioning, while exposure to lifelong ALAN resulted in free-run behaviour or even arrhythmicity, and consequent loss of quantitative and temporal partitioning and higher group variance. Initially, the acrophase of both behaviours appears to reveal some resilience, despite the changing periods. However, a deeper examination revealed a trend of ALAN-induced shortened mean vectors with growing phase variance, and even loss of phase synchrony among stridulation and locomotion activity in the same individual. Interestingly, our findings suggest differences between the two studied behavioural patterns, specifically in their susceptibility to ALAN. Stridulation behaviour was strongly affected even by low ALAN intensity, while the period of the locomotion activity patterns revealed some resilience to ALAN exposure, demonstrating a gradual ALAN-dependent change. It should be noted that a period of 24 h does not necessarily reflect a stable endogenous cycle but could also be the result of masking. Under natural conditions, however, other factors, such as the diurnal temperature, as well as conspecific activity, might also contribute to population synchronization [67]. We, therefore, conclude that G. bimaculatus populations are vulnerable to low levels of ALAN, raising concerns about the possible detrimental ecological effects of light pollution on this as well as other insects.
(c) . Perspectives for future research
Some of our findings indicate major routes for future research into circadian rhythms and their underlying mechanisms in the cricket. For example, while the period of stridulation and locomotion has been shown to correlate in other species [32,35], no correlation of either the period or the phase was found in our present study. The differential effects of lifelong exposure to increasing ALAN intensities on the activity cycles and phases of these two behavioural patterns in a single individual may indicate the decoupling of possibly two peripheral clocks.
Our experiments were conducted under laboratory conditions, with a relatively low daylight intensity in order to avoid both high chamber temperatures and visual discomfort to the insects (which live a cryptic life). The chosen light intensity may not faithfully represent natural light conditions. Furthermore, the experimental illuminations were turned on and off abruptly, lacking the natural gradual processes of sunrise and sunset. It is possible that the free-run pattern was triggered not only by the nocturnal light intensity per se, but also by the differences in intensity between the diurnal and nocturnal light. Hence, it is important to conduct such experiments also under natural conditions, in order to determine whether and how these ALAN intensities might affect the field crickets in their natural habitat.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Izhak Idelstein, Stan Moaraf, Yael Ballon, Daniel Knebel and the staff of the I. Meier Segals Garden for Zoological Research at Tel Aviv University for their assistance, and Eran Tauber for his valuable comments on the manuscript.
Contributor Information
Anat Barnea, Email: anatba@openu.ac.il.
Amir Ayali, Email: ayali@post.tau.ac.il.
Data accessibility
Electronic supplementary material and data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.wm37pvmmf [68].
Authors' contributions
K.L.: conceptualization, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, validation, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; Y.W.: software, validation, visualization; R.E.: software, validation, visualization; A.B.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, supervision, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; A.A.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, supervision, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing. All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Competing interests
The authors have no competing interests related to this study.
Funding
K.L. is grateful to the Cornell Bioacoustics Research Program for supporting attendance to the 2018 Sierra Nevada Sound Recording and Analysis Workshop. This research was funded by The Open University of Israel Research Fund.
References
- 1.Hölker F, et al. 2010The dark side of light: a transdisciplinary research agenda for light pollution policy. Ecol. Soc. 15, 215-224. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Falchi F, Cinzano P, Duriscoe D, Kyba CCM, Elvidge CD, Baugh K, Portnov BA, Rybnikova NA, Furgoni R. 2016The new world atlas of artificial night sky brightness. Sci. Adv. 2, 1-26. ( 10.1126/sciadv.1600377) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rich C, Longcore T. 2006Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting. Washington, DC: Island Press. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sanders D, Frago E, Kehoe R, Patterson C, Gaston KJ. 2021A meta-analysis of biological impacts of artificial light at night. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 5, 74-81. ( 10.1038/s41559-020-01322-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bedrosian TA, Vaughn CA, Weil ZM, Nelson RJ. 2013Behaviour of laboratory mice is altered by light pollution within the housing environment. Anim. Welf. 22, 483-487. ( 10.7120/09627286.22.4.483) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Dominoni D, Quetting M, Partecke J. 2013Artificial light at night advances avian reproductive physiology. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20123017. ( 10.1098/rspb.2012.3017) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kronfeld-Schor N, Visser ME, Salis L, van Gils JA. 2017Chronobiology of interspecific interactions in a changing world. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372, 20160248. ( 10.1098/rstb.2016.0248) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Spoelstra K, van Grunsven RHA, Ramakers JJC, Ferguson KB, Raap T, Donners M, Veenendaal EM, Visser ME. 2017Response of bats to light with different spectra: light-shy and agile bat presence is affected by white and green, but not red light. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 11-15. ( 10.1098/rspb.2017.0075) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.de Jong M, Jeninga L, Ouyang JQ, van Oers K, Spoelstra K, Visser ME. 2016Dose-dependent responses of avian daily rhythms to artificial light at night. Physiol. Behav. 155, 172-179. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.12.012) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Raap T, Pinxten R, Eens M. 2015Light pollution disrupts sleep in free-living animals. Sci. Rep. 5, 1-8. ( 10.1038/srep13557) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Buchanan BW. 2006Observed and potential effects of artificial night lighting on anuran amphibians. In Ecological consequences of artificial night lighting (eds Rich C, Longcore T), pp. 192-220. Washington, DC: Island Press. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rosenberg Y, Doniger T, Harii S, Sinniger F, Levy O. 2019Demystifying circalunar and diel rhythmicity in Acropora digitifera under constant dim light. iScience 22, 477-488. ( 10.1016/j.isci.2019.11.040) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Riley WD, Bendall B, Ives MJ, Edmonds NJ, Maxwell DL. 2012Street lighting disrupts the diel migratory pattern of wild Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar L., smolts leaving their natal stream. Aquaculture 330–333, 74-81. ( 10.1016/j.aquaculture.2011.12.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Davies TW, Bennie J, Gaston KJ. 2012Street lighting changes the composition of invertebrate communities. Biol. Lett. 8, 764-767. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0216) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kyba CCM, Hölker F. 2013Do artificially illuminated skies affect biodiversity in nocturnal landscapes? Landsc. Ecol. 28, 1637-1640. ( 10.1007/s10980-013-9936-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Manfrin A, Singer G, Larsen S, Weiß N, van Grunsven RHA, Weiß N-S, Wohlfahrt S, Monaghan MT, Hölker F. 2017Artificial light at night affects organism flux across ecosystem boundaries and drives community structure in the recipient ecosystem. Front. Environ. Sci. 5, 1-14. ( 10.3389/fenvs.2017.00061) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Owens ACS, Lewis SM. 2018The impact of artificial light at night on nocturnal insects: a review and synthesis. Ecol. Evol. 8, 11 337-11 358. ( 10.1002/ece3.4557) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Owens ACS, Cochard P, Durrant J, Farnworth B, Perkin EK, Seymoure B. 2020Light pollution is a driver of insect declines. Biol. Conserv. 241, 108259. ( 10.1016/j.biocon.2019.108259) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sanders D, Kehoe R, Cruse D, van Veen FJF, Gaston KJ. 2018Low levels of artificial light at night strengthen top-down control in insect food web. Curr. Biol. 28, 2474-2478.e3. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.078) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Eisenbeis G, Hänel A. 2009Light pollution and the impact of artificial night lighting on insects. In Ecology of cities and towns (eds McDonnell MJ, Hahns AH, Breuste JH), pp. 243-263. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Eisenbeis G, Hassel F. 2000Zur Anziehung nachtaktiver Insekten durch Straßenlaternen. Natur und Landschaft 75, 145-156. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Verheijen FJ. 1960The mechanisms of the trapping effect of artificial light sources upon animals. Arch. Néerlandaises Zool. 13, 1-107. ( 10.1163/036551660X00017) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Knop E, Zoller L, Ryser R, Gerpe C, Hörler M, Fontaine C. 2017Artificial light at night as a new threat to pollination. Nature 548, 206-209. ( 10.1038/nature23288) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Merrow M, Spoelstra K, Roenneberg T. 2005The circadian cycle: daily rhythms from behaviour to genes. EMBO Rep. 6, 0-5. ( 10.1038/sj.embor.7400541) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Tomioka K, Matsumoto A. 2019The circadian system in insects: cellular, molecular, and functional organization. Adv. Insect Physiol. 56, 73-115. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Helm B, Visser ME, Schwartz W, Kronfeld-Schor N, Gerkema M, Piersma T, Bloch G. 2017Two sides of a coin: ecological and chronobiological perspectives of timing in the wild. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372, 20160246. ( 10.1098/rstb.2016.0246) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mrosovsky N. 1999Masking: history, definitions, and measurement. Chronobiol. Int. 16, 415-429. ( 10.3109/07420529908998717) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Nielsen ET. 1984Relation of behavioural activity rhythms to the changes of day and night. A revision of views. Behaviour 89, 147-173. ( 10.1163/156853984X00083) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tomioka K, Chiba Y. 1989Photoperiodic entrainment of locomotor activity in crickets (Gryllus bimaculatus) lacking the optic lobe pacemaker. J. Insect Physiol. 35, 827-835. ( 10.1016/0022-1910(89)90098-X) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Horch HW, Mito T, Popadi A, Ohuchi H, Noji S. 2017The cricket as a model organism. Tokyo, Japan: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lutz FE. 1932Experiments with Orthoptera concerning diurnal rhythm. Am. Mus. Novit. 550, 1-24. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sokolove PG. 1975Locomotory and stridulatory circadian rhythms in the cricket, Teleogryllus commodus. J. Insect Physiol. 21, 537-558. ( 10.1016/0022-1910(75)90159-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Loher W. 1972Circadian control of stridulation in the cricket Teleogryllus commodus Walker. J. Comp. Physiol. 79, 173-190. ( 10.1007/BF00697770) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Loher W. 1989Temporal organization of reproductive behavior. In Cricket behavior and neurobiology (eds Huber F, Moore TE, Loher W), pp. 83-113. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Fergus DJ, Shaw KL. 2013Circadian rhythms and period expression in the Hawaiian cricket genus Laupala. Behav. Genet. 43, 241-253. ( 10.1007/s10519-012-9576-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Abe Y, Ushirogawa H, Tomioka K. 1997Circadian locomotor rhythms in the cricket, Gryllodes sigiilatus. I. Localization of the pacemaker and the photoreceptor. Zool. Sci. 14, 719-727. ( 10.2108/zsj.14.719) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Uryu O. 2014Molecular analysis of the circadian clock system in the cricket. Okayama, Japan: Okayama University. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bioacoustics Center for Conservation. 2016Raven lite: interactive sound analysis software (version 2.0.1). [computer software]. Ithaca, NY: The Cornell Laboratory Ornithology. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ballon Y. 2015The effects of different light regimes on activity rhythms of the eastern spadefoot (Pelobates syriacus). Tel-Aviv, Israel: Tel-Aviv University. [Google Scholar]
- 40.R Core Team. 2020R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
- 41.Araya-Salas M. 2017Rraven: connecting R and raven sound analysis software. See https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/Rraven/vignettes/Rraven.html.
- 42.Bioacoustics Research Program. 2013Raven Pro: interactive sound analysis software (version 1.5). Ithaca, NY: The Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology. See http://www.birds.cornell.edu/raven. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Schmid B, Helfrich-Förster C, Yoshii T. 2011A new ImageJ plug-in ‘actogramJ’ for chronobiological analyses. J. Biol. Rhythms 26, 464-467. ( 10.1177/0748730411414264) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Moškon M. 2020CosinorPy: a python package for cosinor-based rhythmometry. BMC Bioinf. 21, 1-12. ( 10.1186/s12859-020-03830-w) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kovach WL.2011Oriana—circular statistics for Windows. See https://www.kovcomp.co.uk/oriana/oriana4man.pdf.
- 46.Huber F, Moore TE, Loher W. 1989Cricket behavior and neurobiology. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Tomioka K, Chiba Y. 1982Post-embryonic development of circadian rhythm in the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus: a rhythm reversal. J. Comp. Physiol. A 147, 299-304. ( 10.1007/BF00609663) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ikeda M, Tomioka K. 1993Temperature dependency of the circadian locomotor rhythm in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Zool. Sci. 10, 597-604. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Zufall F, Schmitt M, Menzel R. 1989Spectral and polarized light sensitivity of photoreceptors in the compound eye of the cricket (Gryllus bimaculatus). J. Comp. Physiol. A 164, 597-608. ( 10.1007/BF00614502) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Frolov RV, Immonen EV, Weckström M. 2014Performance of blue- and green-sensitive photoreceptors of the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 200, 209-219. ( 10.1007/s00359-013-0879-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Durrant J, Botha LM, Green MP, Jones TM. 2018Artificial light at night prolongs juvenile development time in the black field cricket, Teleogryllus commodus. J. Exp. Zool. Part B Mol. Dev. Evol. 330, 225-233. ( 10.1002/jez.b.22810) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Svechkina A, Portnov BA, Trop T. 2020The impact of artificial light at night on human and ecosystem health: a systematic literature review. Landsc. Ecol. 35, 1725-1742. ( 10.1007/s10980-020-01053-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Dominoni DM, Borniger JC, Nelson RJ. 2016Light at night, clocks and health: from humans to wild organisms. Biol. Lett. 12, 20160015. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0015) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hölker F, Wolter C, Perkin EK, Tockner K. 2010Light pollution as a biodiversity threat. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25, 681-682. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2010.09.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Renato J, da Rocha M, Almeida JR, Lins GA, Durval A. 2010Insects as indicators of environmental changing and pollution: a review of appropriate species and their monitoring. Holos Environ. 10, 250. ( 10.14295/holos.v10i2.2996) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Wagner DL. 2020Insect declines in the anthropocene. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 65, 457-480. ( 10.1146/annurev-ento-011019-025151) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Moaraf S, Vistoropsky Y, Pozner T, Heiblum R, Okuliarová M, Zeman M, Barnea A. 2020Artificial light at night affects brain plasticity and melatonin in birds. Neurosci. Lett. 716, 134639. ( 10.1016/j.neulet.2019.134639) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Van Grunsven RHA, Lham D, Van Geffen KG, Veenendaa EM. 2014Range of attraction of a 6-W moth light trap. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 152, 87-90. ( 10.1111/eea.12196) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Perkin EK, Hölker F, Tockner K. 2014The effects of artificial lighting on adult aquatic and terrestrial insects. Freshw. Biol. 59, 368-377. ( 10.1111/fwb.12270) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Simmons LW, Richie MG. 1996Symmetry in the song of crickets. Proc. R. Soc. B 263, 305-311. ( 10.1098/rspb.1996.0191) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Schöneich S. 2020Neuroethology of acoustic communication in field crickets—from signal generation to song recognition in an insect brain. Prog. Neurobiol. 194, 101882. ( 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2020.101882) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Greenfield MD. 2002Signalers and receivers: mechanisms and evolution of arthropod communication. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Simmons LW. 1988The calling song of the field cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus (De Geer): constraints on transmission and its role in intermale competition and female choice. Anim. Behav. 36, 380-394. ( 10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80009-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Doherty JA. 1985Temperature coupling and ‘trade-off’ phenomena in the acoustic communication system of the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus De Geer (Gryllidae). J. Exp. Biol. 114, 17-35. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Loher W, Weber T, Huber F. 1993The effect of mating on phonotactic behaviour in Gryllus bimaculatus (De Geer). Physiol. Entomol. 18, 57-66. ( 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1993.tb00449.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Schöneich S, Hedwig B. 2012Cellular basis for singing motor pattern generation in the field cricket (Gryllus bimaculatus De Geer). Brain Behav. 2, 707-725. ( 10.1002/brb3.89) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kannan NN, Tomiyama Y, Nose M, Tokuoka A, Tomioka K. 2019Temperature entrainment of circadian locomotor and transcriptional rhythms in the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus. Zool. Sci. 36, 95. ( 10.2108/zs180148) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Levy K, Wegrzyn Y, Efronny R, Barnea A, Ayali A. 2021Data from: Lifelong exposure to artificial light at night impacts stridulation and locomotion activity patterns in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.wm37pvmmf) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Citations
- Levy K, Wegrzyn Y, Efronny R, Barnea A, Ayali A. 2021Data from: Lifelong exposure to artificial light at night impacts stridulation and locomotion activity patterns in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.wm37pvmmf) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Electronic supplementary material and data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.wm37pvmmf [68].