Abstract
Many organisms have evolved adaptations to increase the odds of survival of their offspring. Parental care has evolved several times in animals including ectotherms. In amphibians, ~ 10% of species exhibit parental care. Among these, poison frogs (Dendrobatidae) are well-known for their extensive care, which includes egg guarding, larval transport, and specialized tadpole provisioning with trophic eggs. At least one third of dendrobatids displaying aposematism by exhibiting warning coloration that informs potential predators about the presence of defensive skin toxins. Aposematism has a central role in poison frog diversification, including diet specialization, and visual and acoustic communication; and it is thought to have impacted their reproductive biology as well. We tested the latter association using multivariate phylogenetic methods at the family level. Our results show complex relationships between aposematism and certain aspects of the reproductive biology in dendrobatids. In particular, aposematic species tend to use more specialized tadpole-deposition sites, such as phytotelmata, and ferry fewer tadpoles than non-aposematic species. We propose that aposematism may have facilitated the diversification of microhabitat use in dendrobatids in the context of reproduction. Furthermore, the use of resource-limited tadpole-deposition environments may have evolved in tandem with an optimal reproductive strategy characterized by few offspring, biparental care, and female provisioning of food in the form of unfertilized eggs. We also found that in phytotelm-breeders, the rate of transition from cryptic to aposematic phenotype is 17 to 19 times higher than vice versa. Therefore, we infer that the aposematism in dendrobatids might serve as an umbrella trait for the evolution and maintenance of their complex offspring-caring activities.
Subject terms: Behavioural ecology, Evolutionary ecology, Phylogenetics
Introduction
Many organisms evolve morphological or behavioral adaptations that increase offspring survival1,2. In animals, these strategies commonly include elaborate nest building3,4, costly periodic food provisioning5,6 and aggressive defense of their progeny7. These adaptions have evolved independently several times across vertebrates, and have been shown to increase the likelihood of offspring reaching reproductive age. However, these parental care strategies often entail significant life history trade-offs for parents, such as the reduction of their reproductive output while caring for their offspring8–12.
Parental care investments fall within a continuum that ranges from limited or no protection to prolonged offspring attendance. In one extreme of this spectrum, species that present limited or no parental care usually have hundreds of offspring, few of which survive after birth and only a fraction of which reach maturity13. Many such low-parental care taxa have repeated bouts of reproduction, and their offspring are relatively inexpensive to produce (i.e., small seeds, tiny larvae or thousands of eggs14). On the opposite side of the parental care spectrum, species with elaborate and prolonged care of their young (e.g., brood attendance and provisioning) aim their care at a small number of high-quality offspring whose survival is significantly increased by a consistent input of food (energy) and protection during early development. For parents, such behaviors are costly in terms of self-maintenance and the loss of future reproductive events1,2,13,15.
Understanding the parental care spectrum and its associated trade-offs requires dissecting a syndrome of behaviors into their individual components (e.g., ethological and morphological correlates). These components must be integrated into models that reveal the underlying parental care architecture in specific species as emergent properties. Likewise, other aspects of the natural history of focal species (e.g., anti-predator strategies) might affect the trade-offs that parental care can impose. Finding such cost–benefit relationships is a significant endeavor in evolutionary ecology, especially when trying to determine how individuals balance offspring-rearing and self-maintenance16.
To best elucidate the evolutionary trajectories leading to parental care, we must characterize how diverse morphological traits (e.g., body size) or antipredator strategies (e.g., aposematism or crypsis) correlate with parental care components (e.g., number of offspring, food provisioning, and male-/female-biased attendance). To study such complexity, we must focus on a group of closely related species with diverse phenotypes regarding reproduction and parental care, and account for interspecific variation in life history traits including sexual size dimorphism (male-biased vs. female-biased), number and quality of offspring (i.e., few vs. many), and asymmetric parental care investment (i.e., absence vs. extensive biparental care). In addition, we should focus on clades with innate (i.e., not learned) forms of parental care, which would reveal how natural selection affects parental care evolution without the confounding effects of learning. Finally, an ideal study system should involve a lineage with recurrent and contrasting types of parental care to allow the effective use of phylogenetic comparative methods.
Among vertebrates, parental care is common in birds and mammals17–19, and present in some reptiles and fishes20–22. In amphibians, ~ 90% of species do not display parental care, but the remaining species exhibit a wide diversity of caring behaviors that are innate and seemingly shaped by natural selection23,24. Neotropical poison frogs (i.e., Dendrobatidae) form a clade that evolved from other hyloid frogs at ~ 40 MYA, and many species have diverse forms of parental care. Such behavioral variability has stimulated extensive research on parental care origins, maintenance, and trade-offs among anurans, which dates back to the 60’s24–26. Parental care diversity in dendrobatids ranges from a few taxa without any form of offspring care to species with male only or male-biased care (comprising the majority of species), and some with extensive biparental care including the provisioning of unfertilized eggs to developing tadpoles by their mothers27. In sum, dendrobatids are characterized by small clutches of terrestrial eggs (mean = 12.41 ± 9.26 eggs, range = 1–40, N = 107 species; see Table S2) which are cared for by one parent, mostly the male, until tadpoles hatch and are transported to a water body24,27.
In most dendrobatids, tadpoles are transported to small streams, ponds (hereafter referred to as non-phytotelma) or bodies of water in plant structures (hereafter referred to as phytotelmata) where development continues until metamorphosis. In some species with large clutches, the transport of all tadpoles requires several trips by the attending parent26,27. Pools in phytotelmata (i.e., tree-holes, palm bracts, bromeliad axils, etc.) offer a suitable rearing environment where large predators are rare, but nutritional resources are scarce28. Thus, only a few tadpoles (e.g., 1–2 individuals) are transported to, and deposited in, these environments at the same time by a given parent, and it is not uncommon for larval cannibalism to occur. Because of resource scarcity in phytotelmata, some species go one step further and periodically provision their developing tadpoles with unfertilized eggs29–31. Both tadpole transport and food-provisioning visits are done during the day and often involve trips over long distances, which may increase the exposure of the caring parent to predators, and presumably incurs significant energetic costs32–34.
Alongside their elaborate parental care, another feature common to many dendrobatids is aposematism35–37, an antipredator strategy defined as the coupling of a warning signal (e.g., warning coloration) and a secondary (e.g., chemical) defense38. In this clade, ~ 100 species exhibit aposematism, while the remaining ~ 210 species39,40 are assumed to use crypsis to blend with their environment and avoid predator detection or recognition. The origin of warning coloration in dendrobatids is likely associated with their diurnal habits, while the chemical defenses are likely derived from a specialized diet consisting mostly of ants and mites39, followed by the sequestration of toxic alkaloids from these key arthropod prey41,42. Furthermore, aposematism has been co-opted to provide an extensive audiovisual communication channel during mating and territorial signaling, and has played a central role in the diversification of poison frogs43.
To date, the implications of aposematism for reproductive strategies across the Dendrobatidae (i.e., its ~ 310 species) have not been explored extensively. Previous comparative analyses in this clade suggest that aposematism likely affects most, if not all, aspects of dendrobatid life history35,39,43–45. Such analyses have also suggested that male-only care may have preceded and favored the emergence of aposematism46, and that tadpole deposition in large bodies of water is likely the ancestral state27,47. However, both macroevolutionary (e.g.,27,47,48) and empirical49–57 studies on reproductive behavior in dendrobatids are limited to a few, predominantly aposematic species, while most non-aposematic species (i.e., ~ 210 species) have been largely overlooked (but see e.g.,57–61 for detailed studies on the reproductive biology of cryptic species). In spite of this bias, most observations suggest that aposematic species (e.g. D. pumilio and D. imitator mimicry system63) tend to have more complex reproductive behaviors than cryptic species, involving elaborate audiovisual and tactile interactions (e.g.,49,54,61,63,64) in which warning colors might be used as cues by females to select mates65. Likewise, many aposematic species have evolved female-biased or female-only care, which involves spatial field-mapping abilities to repeatedly locate their developing tadpoles in phytotelmata and provision them with unfertilized eggs26,29,47.
Here, we aim to determine whether aspects of parental care behavior in dendrobatid frogs, such as sex of the main caregiver, clutch size, and the transition from streams and ponds to ephemeral deposition sites (i.e., phytotelmata), may have evolved in association with the aposematic phenotype across the entire family. To do so, we characterized the morphological and behavioral aspects of parental care in dendrobatids using phylogenetic multivariate tools35. More specifically, we (1) estimated the ancestral state reconstructions and used multivariate models to determine how aposematism is related to parental care; (2) established whether aposematism and tadpole traits are related using phylogenetic multivariate logistic regressions; and (3) tested the transition rates between aposematism components (i.e., conspicuous coloration and chemical defenses) and phytotelm-breeding behavior. We found no evidence of a direct relationship between aposematism and offspring morphological traits, but found that aposematic species tend to use more specialized tadpole-rearing sites (e.g., phytotelmata), and ferry fewer tadpoles than non-aposematic species. In addition, we found that in phytotelm-breeders the rate of transition to aposematic phenotype is 17–19 times higher than its reversal to the ancestral cryptic, non-defended phenotypes. We thus propose that aposematism served as an “evolutionary platform” where a diversity of behaviors could evolve as predation pressure decreased. While we do not claim that aposematism causes complex parental behaviors to evolve in the first place, we suggest that it might serve as an umbrella trait that provides protection from predators while allowing, in turn, further diversification of parental care behaviors.
Results
Ancestral reconstructions
We obtained information on morphological and behavioral traits of 220 dendrobatid species (Fig. 1, Table S2). We found males to be the caregiver in most cases (92 species), and ancestral reconstructions support this as the ancestral condition in Dendrobatidae; female care (10 species) evolved independently at least two times, once within the genus Colostethus and once in the genus Dendrobates sensu lato (Tables 1, 2, Fig. 2B). Tadpole transport by males (132 species) is ratified as the ancestral condition in dendrobatids, whereas tadpole transport by females (17 species) has appeared at least five times in the genera Dendrobates (subgenus: Oophaga), Allobates, Hyloxalus, Colostethus, and Mannophryne (Tables 1, 2). Tadpole deposition in non-phytotelmata is more common (112 species) than in phytotelmata (52 species), and non-phytotelm breeding is supported as the ancestral condition. Changes in the tadpole deposition site from non-phytotelmata to phytotelmata have evolved independently two times, once in the aposematic Dendrobates (subgenera: Adelphobates, Andinobates, Dendrobates, Excidobates, Minyobates, Oophaga, Ranitomeya; 45 species in total), and once in the non-aposematic genus Anomaloglossus (only 3 species; Fig. 1).
Table 1.
Trait | Dendrobates sensu lato | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Adelphobates | Andinobates | Dendrobates | Excidobates | Minyobates | Oophaga | Ranitomeya | Phyllobates | Ameerega | Epipedobates | |
Ntaxa | 3 | 12 | 4 | 3 | 1 | 10 | 17 | 5 | 29 | 7 |
♂ body size (mm) | 23.727 ± 8.471 (3) | 15.617 ± 2.128 (11) | 32.525 ± 6.408 (4) | 20.695 ± 5.133 (3) | 14 (1) | 25.639 ± 6.05 (8) | 16.09 ± 1.436 (16) | 29.54 ± 8.89 (5) | 24.55 ± 5.13 (25) | 17.75 ± 1.71 (7) |
♀ body size (mm) | 26.16 ± 8.606 (3) | 16.117 ± 2.344 (11) | 35.975 ± 7.514 (4) | 22.103 ± 5.781 (3) | 16 (1) | 25.759 ± 5.507 (8) | 17.454 ± 1.506 (16) | 32.48 ± 8.50 (5) | 27.46 ± 6.03 (25) | 19.05 ± 2.19 (6) |
Habitat | 3 | 10 | 4 | 3 | 1 | 9 | 17 | 5 | 27 | 7 |
Near streams | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Far streams | 3 | 10 | 4 | 3 | 1 | 9 | 17 | 5 | 27 | 7 |
Clutch size | 5.633 ± 3.925 (3) | 2.425 ± 1.15 (4) | 5.4 ± 1.451 (4) | 8 ± (1) | 6 (1) | 6.43 ± 2.526 (8) | 3.009 ± 1.46 (14) | 15.8 ± 2.95 (5) | 18.43 ± 10.18 (16) | 17.75 ± 6.34 (4) |
Neggs | 2.4 ± (1) | 3.75 ± 2.475(2) | 7 ± 2.309 (4) | – | – | 5.333 ± 1.538 (6) | 3.464 ± 1.595 (11) | 18.87 ± 7.59 (4) | 18.53 ± 8.02 (7) | 19.37 ± 3.73 (4) |
Egg size (mm) | – | 2.49 ± 0.875 (3) | 3.495 ± 0.816 (4) | 1.35 ± (1) | – | 1.506 ± 0.403 (5) | 1.783 ± 0.409 (7) | 2.33 ± 0.29 (3) | 2.26 ± 0.77 (9) | 1.33 ± 0.09 (4) |
Tadpole size (mm) | 14.94 ± (1) | 12.586 ± 3.871 (6) | 14.096 ± 3.106 (4) | 14.987 ± 4.255 (3) | 11.796 (1) | 10.156 ± 0.791 (6) | 14.41 ± 3.032 (13) | 11.41 ± 1.16 (4) | 12.93 ± 1.95 (15) | 10.57 ± 1.49 (6) |
Ntadpoles carried | 1 ± 0 (3) | 1.45 ± 0.369 (10) | 1.75 ± 0.289 (4) | 1.5 ± 0 (2) | 2 (1) | 1.016 ± 0.044 (8) | 1.346 ± 0.718 (13) | 5.87 ± 1.88 (5) | 11.61 ± 6.75 (19) | 8.52 ± 4.41 (7) |
Ntaxa (Cannibalism) | 1 | 1 | 4 | 3 | 6 | 1 | 1 | |||
Present | 1 | 1 | 4 | – | 3 | 6 | 6 | – | – | |
Absent | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | 1 | 1 |
Caregiver (Ntaxa) | 6 | 4 | 2 | 10 | 13 | 5 | 17 | 7 | ||
♂ | – | 6 | 4 | 2 | – | – | 5 | 17 | 7 | |
♀ | – | – | – | – | 8 | 11 | – | – | – | |
Both | – | – | – | – | 2 | 2 | – | – | – | |
None | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
Transporter (N taxa) | 3 | 10 | 4 | 2 | 1 | 10 | 14 | 5 | 19 | 7 |
♂ | 3 | 10 | 4 | 2 | 1 | – | 14 | 5 | 17 | 7 |
♀ | – | – | – | – | – | 8 | – | – | 2 | – |
Both | – | – | – | – | – | 2 | – | – | – | – |
None | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Deposition site (N) | 1 | 10 | 4 | 3 | 1 | 10 | 16 | 5 | – | 7 |
Phytotelmata | 1 | 10 | 4 | 3 | 1 | 10 | 16 | 5 | – | – |
Non–phytotelmata | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | 17 | 7 |
Conspicuousnessa | 2.702 ± 0.219 (3) | 2.452 ± 0.293 (11) | 2.449 ± 0 (4) | 1.966 ± 0.837 (3) | 2.646 (1) | 2.449 ± 0 (8) | 2.525 ± 0.198 (17) | 2.53 ± 0.11 (5) | 2.51 ± 0.24 (26) | 2.51 ± 0.54 (7) |
Alkaloidsa | – | 6.217 ± 0.978 (2) | 7.179 ± 0.234 (3) | – | – | 7.152 ± 0.222 (5) | 6.909(1) | 5.52 ± 0 (3) | 6.91 ± 0 (4) | 6.91 ± 0 (2) |
Table 2.
Trait | Al | An | Ar | Co | Ec | Hy | Le | Ma | Par | Rh | Si |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ntaxa | 75 | 25 | 11 | 9 | 2 | 40 | 10 | 17 | 1 | 3 | 5 |
♂ body size (mm) | 18.94 ± 3.51 (43) | 18.45 ± 2.29 (17) | 25.54 ± 8.33 (10) | 23.81 ± 4.99 (5) | 20.33 (1) | 21.21 ± 3.68 (34) | 20.62 ± 1.60 (5) | 24.05 ± 4.77 (17) | 21 (1) | 31.4 ± 5.3 (2) | 16.71 ± 0.84 (4) |
♀ body size (mm) | 19.57 ± 3.32 (42) | 20.74 ± 3.43 (15) | 28.87 ± 10.19 (10) | 25.62 ± 3.68 (6) | 23.6 ± 1.41 (2) | 23.31 ± 3.99 (34) | 22.12 ± 1.89 (6) | 27.71 ± 5.22 (17) | 23.7 (1) | 35.1 (1) | 18.67 ± 1.72 (5) |
Habitat | 42 | 18 | 10 | 7 | – | 33 | 5 | 17 | 1 | 2 | 5 |
Near from Stream | – | 8 | – | 3 | – | 21 | 1 | 17 | 1 | 2 | – |
Far from Stream | 42 | 8(2b) | 10 | 4 | – | 12 | 4 | – | – | – | 5 |
Clutch size | 16.63 ± 9.57 (21) | 4.59 ± 1.79 (6) | – | 25 (1) | – | 16.28 ± 7.64 (12) | 16.5 ± 3.53 (2) | 18.42 ± 10.39 (4) | – | 23.5 (1) | – |
Neggs | 16.39 ± 9.82 (13) | 4 ± 1.41 (2) | – | – | – | 21.01 ± 11.37 (11) | 22 ± 4.24 (2) | 11.44 ± 1.78 (2) | – | 23.5 (1) | – |
Egg size (mm) | 1.69 ± 0.5 (15) | 2.6 ± 0.94 (4) | 1.5 (1) | 1.7 ± 0.14 (2) | 2.5 (1) | 2.04 ± 0.56 (12) | 1.29 (1) | 2.21 ± 0.96 (5) | – | 3.35 (1) | – |
Tadpole Size (mm) | 11.44 ± 4.32 (23) | 16.94 ± 3.49 (7) | – | 12.47 ± 0.49 (3) | – | 16.06 ± 6.89 (28) | 13.6 ± 0 (1) | 16.29 ± 3.69 (10) | 9.32 (1) | 21.2 (1) | 11.33 ± 1.04 (3) |
Ntadpoles carried | 11.75 ± 6.68 (24) | 3.64 ± 3.54 (7) | – | 15.5 ± 9.13 (3) | – | 9.45 ± 4.35 (25) | 12 ± 2.82 (2) | 9.43 ± 3.08 (7) | 3 (1) | 15.9 (1) | 6.67 ± 2.02 (3) |
N taxa (Cannibalism) | – | – | – | – | – | 1 | – | – | – | 1 | – |
Present | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Absent | – | – | – | – | – | 1 | – | – | – | 1 | – |
Caregiver (N taxa) | 15 | 4 | – | 2 | – | 18 | 1 | 7 | – | 1 | 1 |
♂ | 12 | 1 | – | – | – | 17 | 1 | 7 | – | 1 | 1 |
♀ | – | – | – | 2 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Both | 1 | 2 | – | – | – | 1 | – | – | – | – | – |
None | 2* | 1 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Transporter (N taxa) | 24 | 10 | – | 4 | – | 25 | 3 | 8 | 1 | 1 | 3 |
♂ | 14 | 9 | – | 1 | – | 20 | 3 | 7 | 1 | 1 | 3 |
♀ | 2 | – | – | 3 | – | 1 | – | 1 | – | – | – |
Both | 6 | – | – | – | – | 4 | – | – | – | – | – |
None | 2* | 1 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Tadpole Deposition | 27 | 11 | 4 | 3 | – | 29 | 4 | 10 | 1 | 1 | 3 |
Phytotelmata | 2* | 3(2*) | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Non-Phytotelmata | 25 | 6 | 4 | 3 | – | 29 | 44 | 10 | 1 | 1 | 3 |
Conspicuousnessa | 1.55 ± 0.45 (38) | 1.41 ± 0.57 (16) | 1.44 ± 0.69 (10) | 1.79 ± 0.31 (7) | 1.41 (1) | 1.59 ± 0.46 (33) | 2.09 ± 0.22 (5) | 1.87 ± 0.14 (17) | 1.41 (1) | 1.57 ± 0.22 (2) | 1.60 ± 0.17 (5) |
Alkaloidsa | 0 ± 0 (7) | – | 0 (1) | 0.15 ± 0.23 (2) | – | 0 ± 0 (3) | 0 (1) | – | – | – | 0 (1) |
Most dendrobatids (164 species) have home ranges that are not adjacent to streams, and this was reconstructed as the ancestral condition for the family; shifts towards habitats near streams (64 species) have occurred eight times in non-aposematic genera including Anomaloglossus, Aromobates, Colostethus, Ectopoglossus, Hyloxalus, Leucostethus, Mannophryne, Paruwrobates, and Rheobates (Table 2, Fig. 2A, D, for an example). Tadpole cannibalism has evolved only twice; in Anomaloglossus beebei and in the clade formed by Phyllobates and Dendrobates sensu lato (subgenera: Adelphobates, Andinobates, Dendrobates sensu stricto, Oophaga and Ranitomeya; 15 species in total; Fig. 2C). Non-conspicuous coloration (143 species) is the ancestral state in dendrobatids (Fig. 1), whereas conspicuous coloration (77 species) evolved independently at least seven times across the family in the genera Allobates, Ameerega, Anomaloglossus, Dendrobates sensu lato, Epipedobates, Hyloxalus, and Phyllobates (Tables 1, 2).
Pairwise correlations and logistic regressions
Our pairwise analyses (Table 3) show that the components of aposematism (i.e., conspicuousness and skin alkaloids) in dendrobatids are significantly and positively correlated (P = 0.030), but do not correlate with tadpole life history traits (P > 0.050 in all cases). These results suggest that aposematism does not covary directly with reproductive traits related to clutch size, number of offspring transported or tadpole body size after hatching. We did not find a significant correlation between tadpole size and egg size, but found a significant relationship between clutch size and the number of tadpoles carried by parents. Moreover, we found a positive relationship between alkaloid quantity and tadpole size. These results suggest that the aposematism and life history components form two clusters of traits that result in two separate emergent properties, namely anti-predator defenses (i.e., aposematism or crypsis) and reproductive strategies (i.e., offspring quality and parental care). However, these results were expanded and challenged when we incorporated them in our multivariate logistic regressions (see Table 4 and S3 for the complete model values).
Table 3.
Conspicuousness score | SSD | Clutch size | Egg size | Tadpole size | Tadpoles carried | Alkaloids | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
– | |||||||
SSD | 0.64NS | – | |||||
Clutch size | − 0.001NS | 0.003NS | – | ||||
Egg size | 0.028NS | − 0.015NS | − 2.53NS | – | |||
Tadpole Size | − 0.055NS | 0.627NS | 0.521NS | 0.06NS | – | ||
Number of Tadpoles carried | − 0.012NS | 0.005NS | 0.578*** | − 0.20NS | 0.01NS | – | |
Alkaloids | 0.231* | 0.003NS | 0.001NS | 0.215NS | − 0.05* | − 0.13NS | – |
Table 4.
Binary dependent | N | Intercept | Coloration score | SSD | Number of Tadpoles transported | Tadpole size* | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β0 | P | β1 | P | β2 | P | β3 | P | β4 | P | ||
Tadpole deposition | 112 | − 255.207 | 0.087 | − 224.170 | 0.001 | 348.390 | 0.532 | 125.920 | < 3e−04 | − 1.122 | 0.953 |
Cannibalism | 23 | − 7.620 | 0.769 | 573.730 | 0.614 | − 952.465 | 0.366 | − 224.700 | 0.038 | − 19.480 | 0.327 |
Alkaloids | 61 | − 693.700 | 0.003 | 497.460 | < 3e−04 | 856.480 | 0.324 | − 136.360 | 0.013 | − 15.210 | 0.383 |
Tadpole−ferrying sex | 110 | − 47.990 | 0.800 | 114.230 | 0.198 | 847.570 | 0.081 | − 0.350 | 0.988 | 11.302 | 0.179 |
Conspicuousness | 114 | − 227.266 | 0.084 | − | − | 697.208 | 0.242 | 21.313 | 0.650 | 6.207 | 0.403 |
Habitat | 114 | 226.180 | 0.045 | 255.790 | 0.014 | − 851.212 | 0.064 | − 90.205 | 0.0134 | − 11.880 | 0.100 |
Type care | 82 | 194.769 | 0.402 | − 6.936 | 0.949 | 700.860 | 0.286 | 1.352 | 0.985 | 9.757 | 0.451 |
Tadpole deposition in phytotelmata tends to occur more often in conspicuous species which, in turn, carry fewer tadpoles at once (Table 4, P < 0.003 in both cases). In contrast, species that transport tadpoles to non-phytotelmata water bodies tend to be cryptic and carry a much larger number of tadpoles. We found no differences in tadpole size (controlled by female body size) between phytotelm-breeders and non-phytotelm-breeders (P = 0.840). Because species that exhibit egg-feeding have very small eggs and tadpoles with a small amount of yolk reserves (hence the need of food supplementation by the mothers) (Summers & McKeon 2004), they can potentially bias these results. For this reason, we also ran the phylogenetic logistic regression excluding egg-feeding species. However, as shown in Table S5, the results did not change.
Species with territories away from streams tend to have more conspicuous coloration and transport fewer tadpoles than those that live in riparian sites (P ~ 0.013). We did not find differences in SSD or tadpole size when comparing between tadpole deposition sites (P > 0.050). Likewise, no associations were found between SSD and clutch size or tadpole length (P > 0.050 in all cases).There were no differences in the number of tadpoles transported, SSD, tadpole size, and coloration between species with male-biased and female-biased care (P > 0.050). Species that exhibit larval cannibalism tend to carry fewer tadpoles than species without cannibalism (P ~ 0.038), yet no differences in SSD, tadpole body size or coloration were detected in association with cannibalism (P > 0.050). Finally, when only the presence of alkaloids was accounted for in our analyses, species with skin alkaloids tend to carry fewer tadpoles at once than species without alkaloids (P ~ 0.003).
We also tested whether the components of aposematism (conspicuousness and skin alkaloids) affect the transition of tadpole deposition site from phytotelmata to non-phytotelmata and vice versa. For this purpose, we tested if a pair of binary traits for aposematism components and tadpole disposition site have evolved independently (Pagel 1994; Pagel and Meade 2006) using the BayesTraits framework (Barker et al. 2007). This approach allowed us to compare the fit between a model that considers the traits to have evolved independently and models which assume that the traits’ evolution depends on one another (i.e., present correlated evolution); these models can be modified to present different constraints on their transition rate parameters.
Our results provide strong evidence of dependent evolution between aposematism components and phytotelm-breeding (Bayes factor-BF = 39.314; see Fig. 3, Tables 4 and S6–S8 for details in all values and comparation between models). Briefly, when comparing the independent and dependent models, we found the latter to be the best fit (i.e., for both conspicuousness and skin alkaloids with phytotelm-breeding). For the test between conspicuousness and tadpole deposition site, we found that the highest transition rate estimate was from non-conspicuous to conspicuous when phytotelm-breeding is present (q24 = 17.18). When skin alkaloids are incorporated in the model instead of conspicuousness, the highest transition rate was from absence to presence of alkaloids when phytotelm-breeding is present (q24 = 19.13). The reversal rates in these models, i.e., when conspicuousness and alkaloid presence favor the transition to phytotelm-breeding, were much smaller (i.e., q31 = 1.56 for conspicuousness and q31 = 1.20 under alkaloid presence). Overall, phytotelm-breeding species have a rate of transition to aposematic phenotype (e.g., warning coloration and alkaloid defenses) that is 17–19 times the rate of the opposite transition, i.e., reversal to the ancestral cryptic and non-defended phenotypes. Table 4 and Fig. 3 provide details on transition rate estimates of all character state combinations and all models tested.
Discussion
One of the costs of parental care is the increased predation risk or the caregivers through prolonged exposure to predators while provisioning and protecting their young. These costs can be partially mitigated if the parents exhibit warning colors that signal predators about the presence of defenses such as toxic or noxious compounds (e.g., alkaloids, venoms, etc.). For example, adult burying beetles (Nicrophorus vespilloides), which are known for their elaborate parental care, exhibit a conspicuous warning coloration coupled with an anal exudate that deters potential predators87. Using a phylogenetic framework, we investigated whether aspects of poison frog ecology and life history covary with their parental care behavior. Our findings indicate that, indeed, aposematism may have facilitated the diversification of parental care strategies in dendrobatids. Therefore, we propose that aposematism might serve as an “evolutionary platform” where parental behavior can further diversify as predation pressure is reduced.
Results of the ancestral reconstruction indicate that aposematism (i.e., conspicuousness and alkaloid sequestration) evolved first than most complex reproductive behaviors in dendrobatids. For instance, our tests of correlation between aposematism and tadpole deposition site (BayesTraits analyses) suggest that, once this defensive strategy is established, phytotelm-breeding behavior requires aposematism for its maintenance. Furthermore, dendrobatids that use phytotelmata for tadpole depositionmay become more colorful and more toxic than non-phytotelm-breeding species. In fact, phytotelm-breeding species have transition rates from cryptic to aposematic coloration that are about 17 times the change rates in the opposite direction (Fig. 3, 2.69 times). Likewise, the transition rate in species with phytotelm-breeding suggests that alkaloid sequestration is favored almost 19 times more than in the opposite direction (Fig. 3, 0.6 times). This suggests that if phytotelm-breeding is lost, most species that already have conspicuous coloration or sequester alkaloids are less likely to lose both traits.
We suggest several evolutionary scenarios for the association between aposematism and parental care. First, our ancestral reconstructions suggest that aposematism appeared before phytotelm-breeding and other complex reproductive behaviors, but once these complex behaviors are established, losing aposematism is less likely. Second, aposematism also seems to further the diversification of parental care strategies (e.g., biparental care) due to reduced predation pressure. Third, the low rates of reversal from the aposematic to the non-aposematic phenotype in phytotelm-breeding species suggest a higher predation pressure in species that lack or lost the aposematic phenotype, which might result in increased extinction rates. Overall, aposematism is necessary to maintain complex reproductive behaviors, like phytotelm-breeding, but this trait can serve for further diversification of other parental care strategies (e.g., egg provisioning).
Field evidence supports our results on the correlated evolution of aposematism and phytotelm-breeding, which might have enabled the further diversification of microhabitat use in the context of reproduction in aposematic dendrobatids. In species that transport tadpoles to arboreal phytotelmata, parents could be under high predation risk as they climb trees without concealing structures (e.g., leaf litter) and with reduced understory coverage, limiting the availability of places to hide if detected by a potential predator25,88,89. Parents may also prefer to go far from their territories to deposit their tadpoles as an offspring dispersal strategy to reduce competition and inbreeding33,90. Given the efficacy of aposematism in poison frogs, long forays while caring for offspring are likely safer for aposematic than for cryptic species. Aposematism might not only warn predators about unprofitability, but it may also allow defended frogs caring for their offspring to widen their core territories. These areas tend to be larger in aposematic than in cryptic dendrobatids90, thus allowing aposematic species enhanced access to food resources and shelter91.
The evolutionary transition of tadpole rearing sites from non-phytotelmata waterbodies to phytotelmata is assumed to be a strategy to reduce or avoid large predators, which are abundant in ponds and streams47,92–94. However, phytotelmata are harsh environments with low-nutrient availability and a high desiccation risk. As a consequence, tadpoles deposited in such sites may require further investment by parents to be able to complete their development to metamorphosis29,47,55. Thus, it is not surprising that species whose tadpoles develop in phytotelmata had evolved food provisioning with unfertilized eggs. This harsh environment with limited resources and an ephemeral nature seems to facilitate the evolution of larval cannibalism as a facultative behavior and, in some instances as an optimal foraging strategy.
The shift from using large waterbodies to phytotelmata may have been accompanied by adaptations such as the observed reduction in clutch size and, consequently, in the number of tadpoles transported (Fig. 1 and Table S1). We propose two possible, non-mutually exclusive explanations for this change in aposematic species. First, because tadpoles occupy a significant area on the back of the parents, a high number of tadpoles on the parent’s back might interfere with the display of their warning signal, thus increasing predation risk. In other words, fewer tadpoles on the parent’s back may prevent the concealment of the warning signal during tadpole transport. Some species of aposematic frogs transport a large number of tadpoles on their back (e.g., Ameerega trivittata). In this case, we hypothesize that the conspicuous head coloration of the parent could be enough to warn a potential predator about its unprofitability. Alternatively, the potentially disruptive pattern created by tadpoles on the back of the parent could confuse would-be (and maybe inexperienced) predators. Both ideas could be tested experimentally, for example, either using clay models of aposematic frogs with markings that mimic tadpoles on their back and determining if these models elicit more attacks by predators than tadpole-free models, or testing predation risk upon prey (e.g., mealworms) placed on printed drawings of poison frogs, and using naïve chickens as predators95,96.
An alternative explanation for the clutch size reduction is that natural selection can promote a small brood size under Lack's principle97,98. According to this hypothesis, dendrobatid frogs with restrictive tadpole deposition sites may have evolved an optimal clutch size that corresponds with the largest number of tadpoles that the parents can care for47,119. Likewise, biparental care should have evolved along with egg provisioning as a direct food resource that only females can provide by sacrificing large gametes. These tactics would thus be part of an optimal life history strategy to deal with the limited resources and space in phytotelmata. Variants of this hypothesis have been proposed for some Dendrobates species (e.g., D. variabilis, D. imitator) that use arboreal phytotelmata for tadpole development55,99–101. We consider that our hypothesis could also explain the small clutch size for other non-aposematic genera such Anomaloglossus that also use phytotelmata as deposition sites30,102.
A prediction derived from Lack's principle is that parents must invest time to test the quality of phytotelmata, and they should have evolved high cognitive abilities to memorize the spatial location of the clutch to minimize travel time for food provisioning (see also103,104). Therefore, particularly in species without biparental care, unfertilized egg provisioning may require the evolution of a high cognitive capacity in the mothers to remember where their offspring are located in tridimensional space120–122. If the provisioning of females to their offspring is absent or inefficient, this might have enabled the facultative occurrence of tadpole cannibalism. In this case, if more than one individual is deposited in a given pool, cannibalism might allow tadpoles to prey on other individuals as an optimal strategy to avoid starvation103. This inference implies that most species using phytotelmata as rearing sites should exhibit tadpole cannibalism, which appears not to be the case. Larval cannibalism has been studied in only a few species37,95,105–109 and, in some instances, it appears to be facultative or opportunistic. The fact that few instances of cannibalistic behavior have been documented might be the result of limited observations, and we thus propose that this behavior might be more widespread than currently thought.
From the male’s perspective, tadpole transport can be especially costly in terms of territorial ownership110 and in time invested for calling to attract additional mates111. This is because dendrobatid males are often polygamous and can take care of multiple egg clutches, laid by different females60,90,112. In species that use phytotelmata, males might be selected to invest less than females in finding optimal phytotelmata with specific features for tadpole deposition, for example, without non-kin tadpoles93. Under this ecological scenario, natural selection could have favored higher cognitive capacities in females than males. Likewise, it should have favored tadpole transport to high quality phytotelmata based on multiple parameters such as food availability, number of potential depredators,and physicochemical characteristics of the water, and egg provisioning if nutrients are deficient120–122. However, it remains to be determined if this hypothesis applies to non-aposematic species such as the three species of Colostethus in which females transport their tadpoles to streams and larval cannibalism is absent57. One potential explanation for the latter case is that males tend to decrease their care of an older clutch in a set of clutches with multiple females, and mothers of this older clutch tend to transport their tadpoles in cases in which males do not do so113. All of these possible explanations warrant further investigation, ideally with an experimental approach.
Previous studies suggest that territories near streams might be the ancestral habitat of dendrobatids and their hyloid relatives47. However, our results suggest that the ancestral habitat of poison frogs was away from streams. This alternative hypothesis finds support in other phylogenetic hypotheses stating that the sister group of Dendrobatidae could be Bufonidae (Pyron and Wiens 2011114), Leptodactylidae115 or Craugastoridae/Eleutherodactylidae117. All of which have numerous species occurring in terrestrial habitats. Under this scenario, we infer that the ancestral condition for Dendrobatidae is the terrestrial habitat, whereas the riparian habitat seems to be derived. In support of this inference, similar results on habitat evolution across anurans were found by Moen et al. (118; see this paper’s supplementary materials). In their study, the ancestral microhabitat of Dendrobatidae was suggested to be terrestrial and away from streams. For amphibians, terrestrial habitats are hostile environments due to the increased risk of desiccation, unless a parent provides much needed moisture (e.g., watery urine) to developing offspring in terrestrial sites, but also across distant taxa such as insects (e.g., thrips116).
Studying the reproductive strategies and parental care in poison frogs is fundamental to understanding diversification patterns within the Dendrobatidae55,99–101. While a terrestrial environment appears to be ancestral habitat of poison frogs and is common to other hyloid clades (e.g., Leptodactylidae and Bufonidae), the evolution of more complex tadpole deposition sites such as phytotelmata likely imposed a strong selection pressure in early dendrobatids. With the evolution of aposematism and the concomitant reduction in predation risk, complex behaviors might have emerged and further diversified into the remarkable spatial/cognitive abilities, elaborate biparental care, and nutritious egg provisioning seen today in some dendrobatids. Therefore, while we do not claim that aposematism caused complex parental behaviors to evolve in the first place, we suggest that aposematism might serve as an umbrella trait against predation. Once phytotelm-breeding appears in aposematic species, this defensive strategy is less likely to revert to the non-aposematic phenotype. Likewise, aposematism and phytotelm-breeding seem to have allowed further diversification of parental care behavior. Phytotelm-breeding, which may have originated as a response to the high predation pressure in streams, requires the evolution of further strategies for tadpoles to withstand, and thrive in, sites with low nutrient availability. Consequently, a small clutch size, egg provisioning, and cannibalism may be further adaptations in species with tadpoles that develop in phytotelmata which, in turn, may require higher parental investment and may have favored the evolution of complex offspring-caring activities, specially by female parents, towards their offspring.
Methods
Dendrobatids include > 310 species with widespread distribution (e.g., from lowlands to paramos in the Andes) across Central and Northern South America43,66,67. We obtained morphological and behavioral traits associated with parental care in 289 species of dendrobatids (aposematic: 91 species and cryptic: 198 species). Information about those traits was obtained from the scientific literature, examination of vouchers in biological collections (for morphological information), and personal observations in the field (Tables 1 and 2). Detailed summary datasets and references are provided in Tables S1–S2. We provide a brief description of the data used in our analyses below.
Parental care and tadpole life history traits (discrete)
Our analyses include traits measured both as continuous and discrete binary variables. In the case of multistate discrete traits, we binarized such phenotypes using their most common or their broadest definition, as required for phylogenetic logistic regressions. These traits included the sex of the caregiver (usually characterized as none, male, female or both), sex of the transporting parent, and tadpole deposition sites (usually classified as streams, ponds, and phytotelmata). In the case of sex of the caregiver (113 species with reports, Tables 1, 2), we binarized all known cases as male-biased or female-biased, excluding from analyses those which could not be unambiguously binarized (see below). The male-biased state (92 species, Tables 1, 2) was assigned when most of the care (i.e., clutch attendance and tadpole transport) was done by the male, while the female only contributed sporadic care or gametes. The female-biased state (10 species, Tables 1, 2) was assigned when females contributed more to the parental care than males by providing periodic unfertilized eggs to developing tadpoles. We excluded species with no apparent parental care (i.e., 3 species or 2.65% of the diversity of species with reports) or in which we could not assign unambiguously a dominant sex involved in parental care (i.e., 8 species or 7.07% of species with reports). In both of these excluded groups, we assumed that the available reports reflected limited observations and thus required further investigation in the field.
We assigned of the sex of the tadpole-carrying parent based on the sex that is most frequently observed. It is not uncommon in some species that both males and females transport tadpoles (Tables S1–S2), yet the frequency of such events is usually much higher in one sex. For example, both parents of Hyloxalus elachyhistus may transport tadpoles, but the ratio is five to one male to female as the ferrying parent68. Thus, in this (and similar) cases, we assigned the sex of tadpole-carrying parent as male. We also designated the habitat of the parents on the basis of their typical habitat (near-riparian) or away from streams (terrestrial-forest floor). This qualitative trait represents the habitat where each species lives during most of its life cycle, as many taxa are limited to streams while others spend most of their time on the forest floor away from streams, although they may range more widely in the habitats and sites where they deposit tadpoles.
We binarized tadpole deposition sites as non-phytotelmata and phytotelmata, considering the latter as the most specialized tadpole deposition site; species which could use both ponds and streams for tadpole deposition were assigned to the ‘non-phytotelmata’ category. Most dendrobatids deposit their tadpoles in non-phytotelmata environments which are rich in nutrients (e.g., algae and detritus), and their larvae are herbivorous. In contrast, phytotelmata, water-holding structures formed in terrestrial or epiphyte plants (e.g., modified leaves, leaf axils, and tree holes), are characterized by low nutrients (e.g., lack algal debris). Therefore, tadpoles developing therein are usually entirely dependent on the food provisioned by their mother or have evolved facultative cannibalism to acquire basic nutrients if parental investment is limited only to transport. We thus scored larval cannibalistic behavior as present or absent.
In sum, the binary (qualitative) variables associated with parental care and tadpole life history were: (1) sex of the main caregiver (male or female), (2) sex of tadpole carrier (male or female), (3) habitat of the parents (near or far from streams), (4) tadpole deposition site (non-phytotelmata or phytotelmata), and (5) tadpole cannibalism (present or absent).
Parental care and tadpole life history traits (continuous)
For quantitative traits we included sexual size dimorphism (SSD), which was calculated as the ratio between female’s and male’s body length measured in mm. SSD is a common measurement of the intensity of sexual selection acting on male and female body size, and could have an effect on behavior and reproductive traits due to an allometric relationship69–71. For example, females with larger body size could have more eggs per clutch, males with smaller body size could be able to transport fewer or smaller tadpoles. We also included the following continuous variables, which are associated with parental investment: mean clutch size, defined as the number of eggs per clutch; mean egg size, measured as egg diameter in mm; mean number of tadpoles simultaneously transported by a parent; and body length of transported tadpoles (measured in mm on tadpoles at the 25 developmental stage sensu72, which is the larval stage at which tadpoles are transported). We controlled the egg size, clutch size and tadpole size by female size, and we used these residual values in posterior analysis. For all these variables, we used the median of available ranges when only these were reported in the literature. All variables (i.e., not residuals values) were log-transformed to improve their distribution when used in statistical analyses.
Aposematism-related traits
To account for warning signal, we assigned a relative index of conspicuousness to each species based on a quantification of color description made by multiple independent human observers (sensu43). While we are aware that human vision is different from that in other animals73,74, and that there are established methods to reliably measure animal color patterns and assess how receivers (e.g., predators and conspecifics) see them74–78, we opted for this method for the following two reasons. First, previous research indicates that humans are able to detect a considerable portion of color variation in the visible range79. Second, this approach allowed us to cover the entire 220 taxa (i.e., 68% of the known >310 dendrobatid species) that were included in our analyses where such information could be derived from color descriptions of live male specimens were derived from species descriptions, photographs, field notes by independent observers (multiple independent human observers; mean value of 2.8 ± 1.17; range 1–6 observations). Although measuring reflectance spectra or taking standardized photographs for further analyses would be ideal, for this number of species it would be logistically nearly impossible, as many of these species are distributed across the Neotropics, there are not recent reports of their conservation status, and some are extremely hard to find.
Coloration scores were taken from43. The frog's body was divided into 11 non-overlapping skin segments covering dorsal, ventral, and lateral views, and the presence-absence of dorsal stripe(s), lateral stripe(s), and flash marking in arms and legs. Based in human vision perception of chromatic contrast each skin segment of the body was scored with 1 (i.e. conspicuous; red, yellow, blue, orange, green) if it contrasts against a natural background [leaf litter (i.e., gray, brown, and black)] or 0 (non-conspicuous) if otherwise. For each species, an overall coloration score was derived from the sum of all the individual segment scores, which provided a continuous estimate of coloration, ranging from 0 (all Sects. 0 or 0 × 11 = 0; no contrast) to 11 (all Sects. 1 or 1 × 11 = 11; maximum contrast). Additionally, we binarized this coloration score, such that taxa with coloration scores of ≥ 6 were considered as conspicuous, whereas taxa with coloration scores of < 6 was considered as non-conspicuous. This 6-points threshold is based on the observation that all species reported to have skin alkaloids (i.e., chemical defenses) have scored at least 6-points and all these species have color usually considered aposematic (e.g., blue, yellow, red on their dorsum). For further details, and possible constraints and limitations of this methodology, see43. To help the readers get a better idea of how we calculated the color scores, see to illustrate the skin segments of each frog in 43. We have also included a table in the supplementary materials with the raw coloration scores (Table S5). To account for the presence of chemical defense, we also classified each species based on the presence (> 10 µg of alkaloids per 100 mg skin) or absence (< 10 µg of alkaloids per 100 mg skin) of skin alkaloids, as reported in the literature (Tables S1–S2). In sum, we included as continuous variables: (1) conspicuousness index (a value between 0 and 11), and (2) alkaloid measurement score as a proxy of μg of alkaloids per 100 mg of skin; and as qualitative variables: (1) coloration (conspicuous or non-conspicuous) based on a 6-points threshold as indicated above, and (2) chemical defense (present or absent) based on whether or not the species has been reported to have skin alkaloids.
Statistical analyses
For the phylogenetic comparative analyses, we used the time-calibrated phylogeny of Dendrobatidae as provided by43 and updated based on80 With our data matrix and this phylogeny, we estimated the likelihood of marginal ancestral state reconstruction81 of conspicuousness, sex of the caregiver, habitat, sex of tadpole-carrying parent, tadpole deposition site and occurrence of cannibalism in tadpoles. For this, we used an all-equal rates like (ER) parameter of transition among traits using the R-package ‘ape’ version 5.382. To test for the relationship between continuous traits and parental care, we performed a pairwise comparison between values resulting from the independent phylogenetic regressions; we adjusted P-values for False Discovery Rates (FDR83). Then, to explore whether continuous variables (log transformed) explained conspicuousness, sex of the caregiver, habitat, sex of tadpole-carrying parent, tadpole deposition site and tadpole cannibalism, we performed phylogenetic multivariate logistic regressions using the R-package ‘MCMCglmm’ version 2.2984. For these analyses, we used the dendrobatid chronogram to derive the inverse of the matrix of phylogenetic correlations and a list of priors, which includes the default prior for the fixed effects and a set of priors for the random effects (G), and residual variance (R) that correspond to an inverse-Gamma distribution with shape and scale parameters equal to 0.001. The number of iterations was set to 20 × 106 with a burn-in parameter of 1000 and a thining parameter of 500. We ran a total of four Monte Carlo Markov Chains with four independent starting points and the convergence was checked after all runs. We used the Gelman–Rubin convergence diagnostic85 to determine the convergence of runs. This diagnostic evaluates MCMC convergence by analyzing the difference between multiple Markov chains. The convergence is assessed by comparing the estimated between-chains and within-chain variances for each model parameter. Values close to 1 indicate convergence in the models. The values are reported in Table S3 in the Supplementary materials.
Finally, we analyzed whether coloration (or presence of alkaloids) and breeding site have evolved independently or not using BayesTraits v3.0.286 For this purpose, we applied the discrete function to our data and estimated the fit of independent and dependent models under a Bayesian framework. First, we tested whether the coloration (or absence or presence of alkaloids) and breeding site evolved independently (independent model). Second, we tested whether the rate of change in one trait exhibits an evolutionary dependence on the value of the other trait (dependent model). We estimated the marginal likelihood using a stepping-stone sampler with 100 stones and 10,000 iterations per stone. We employed the marginal likelihood of complex and simple models to obtain the Log Bayes Factor (logBF) to test whether the traits are evolutionary correlated between the independent and the dependent model. Bayes Factor values were considered as weak evidence of evolutionary correlation if LogBF < 2; positive evidence of evolutionary correlation if LogBF > 2; strong evidence if LogBF was 5–10; and very strong evidence if LogBF > 10. We further modified the dependent models to improve its fit by constraining the transition rates q34 = q43 = q12 = q21 (Tables 5 and S6–S8). Finally, we examined the potential evolutionary pathways of the traits using the posterior distributions of the eight transitions rates between four possible combination of character states (i.e. coloration and phytotelm-breeding, coloration and no phytotelm-breeding, no coloration and phytotelm-breeding, no coloration and no phytotelm-breeding). We repeated the same procedure as described above but including presence of alkaloids in our models (i.e. alkaloids and phytotelm-breeding, alkaloids and no phytotelm-breeding, no alkaloids and phytotelm-breeding, no alkaloids and no phytotelm-breeding).
Table 5.
Dependent on | Trait | Parameter | Transitions |
---|---|---|---|
Conspicuousness = 1 | Phytotelm-breeding | q34 | 0 → 1 |
or Skin-Alkaloids = 1 | q43 | 1 → 0 | |
Conspicuousness = 0 | Phytotelm-breeding | q12 | 0 → 1 |
or Skin-Alkaloids = 0 | q21 | 1 → 0 | |
Phytotelm-breeding = 1 | Conspicuousness | q24 | 0 → 1 |
or Skin-Alkaloids | q42 | 1 → 0 | |
Phytotelm-breeding = 0 | Conspicuousness | q13 | 0 → 1 |
or Skin-Alkaloids | q31 | 1 → 0 |
Ethics declarations
Information was obtained from literature, examination of vouchers in biological collections (for morphological information), and personal observations in the field. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. No experimental protocols were performed.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
JDCC and JCS thanks SJU starting funds and NSF-DEB # 2016372. JDCC thanks the Humboldt Institute for logistical support. BR is funded by the Academy of Finland (Academy Research Fellowship No. 21000042021). FVS and SCC thank the Universidad del Quindío and the Colección de Anfibios y Reptiles (ARUQ) for logistic support during the completition of this study. Special thanks to MJ Tovar, AM Ospina-L and DL Rivera for the drawings used in Fig. 1.
Author contributions
J.D.C.C., F.V.S, S.C.C., B.R., J.C.S. developed the research concept; J.D.C.C., S.C.C., B.R., J.C.S collected data; J.D.C.C., B.R., J.C.S analyzed and interpreted the data; J.D.C.C. did the former manuscript draft. All authors planned the research, read, edited, and approved the final manuscript.
Data availability
The datasets supporting this article will be uploaded to the online repository figshare (https://figshare.com/s/338fc287ef792d432bb8).
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-97206-6.
References
- 1.Clutton-Brock TH. The Evolution of Parental Care. Princeton University Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Royle NJ, Smiseth PT, Kölliker M. The Evolution of Parental Care. Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hansell M. Bird Nests and Construction Behaviour. Cambridge University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Doody JS, Freedberg S, Keogh JS. Communal egg-laying in reptiles and amphibians: evolutionary patterns and hypotheses. Q. Rev. Biol. 2009;84:229–252. doi: 10.1086/605078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Boness DJ, Don Bowen W. The evolution of maternal care in pinnipeds: new findings raise questions about the evolution of maternal feeding strategies. Bioscience. 1996;46:645–654. doi: 10.2307/1312894. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Salomon M, Mayntz D, Toft S, Lubin Y. Maternal nutrition affects offspring performance via maternal care in a subsocial spider. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2011;65:1191–1202. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1132-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Summers K. Mating and aggressive behaviour in dendrobatid frogs from Corcovado National Park, Costa Rica: a comparative study. Behaviour. 2000;137:7–24. doi: 10.1163/156853900501845. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Li D, Jackson RR. A predator’s preference for egg-carrying prey: a novel cost of parental care. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2003;55:129–136. doi: 10.1007/s00265-003-0689-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Stiver KA, Alonzo SH. Parental and mating effort: is there necessarily a trade-off? Ethology. 2009;115:1101–1126. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01707.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ercit K, Martinez-Novoa A, Gwynne DT. Egg load decreases mobility and increases predation risk in female black-horned tree crickets (Oecanthus nigricornis) PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e110298. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ghalambor CK, Martin TE. Fecundity-survival trade-offs and parental risk-taking in birds. Science. 2001;292:494–497. doi: 10.1126/science.1059379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Thorogood R, Ewen JG, Kilner RM. Sense and sensitivity: responsiveness to offspring signals varies with the parents' potential to breed again. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2011;278:2638–2645. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Stearns SC. The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford University Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Weir BJ, Rowlands I. Reproductive strategies of mammals. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 1973;4:139–163. doi: 10.1146/annurev.es.04.110173.001035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kvarnemo C. In: Evolutionary Behavioral Ecology. FoxWestneat CW, editor. Oxford University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Alonso-Alvarez, C. & Velando, A. Benefits and costs of parental care. The evolution of parental care, 40–61 (2012).
- 17.Farmer C. Parental care: the key to understanding endothermy and other convergent features in birds and mammals. Am. Nat. 2000;155:326–334. doi: 10.1086/303323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ar A, Yom-Tov Y. The evolution of parental care in birds. Evolution. 1978;32:655–669. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1978.tb04610.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gubernick, D. J. Parent and infant attachment in mammals. In Parental care in mammals 243–305 (Springer, 1981).
- 20.Case TJ. Endothermy and parental care in the terrestrial vertebrates. Am. Nat. 1978;112:861–874. doi: 10.1086/283328. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gross MR, Shine R. Parental care and mode of fertilization in ectothermic vertebrates. Evolution. 1981;35:775–793. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1981.tb04937.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Balshine S. Patterns of parental care in vertebrates. Evol. Parental Care. 2012;62:80. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Furness AI, Capellini I. The evolution of parental care diversity in amphibians. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07882-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Schulte LM, Ringler E, Rojas B, Stynoski JL. Developments in amphibian parental care research: history, present advances, and future perspectives. Herpetol. Monogr. 2020;34:71–97. doi: 10.1655/HERPMONOGRAPHS-D-19-00002.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wells KD. The Ecology and Behavior of Amphibians. University of Chicago Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Weygoldt P. Evolution of parental care in dart poison frogs (Amphibia: Anura: Dendrobatidae) J. Zoolog. Syst. Evol. 1987;25:51–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0469.1987.tb00913.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Summers, K. & Tumulty, J. in Sexual Selection 289–320 (Elsevier, 2014).
- 28.Lehtinen R, Lannoo MJ, Wassersug RJ. Phytotelm-breeding anurans: past, present and future research. Misc. Publ. Museum Zool. Univ. Michigan. 2004;193:1–9. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Brust DG. Maternal brood care by Dendrobates pumilio: a frog that feeds its young. J. Herpetol. 1993;27:96–98. doi: 10.2307/1564914. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Bourne GR, Collins AC, Holder AM, McCarthy CL. Vocal communication and reproductive behavior of the frog Colostethus beebei in Guyana. J. Herpetol. 2001;35:272–281. doi: 10.2307/1566118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Schulte LM. Feeding or avoiding? Facultative egg feeding in a Peruvian poison frog (Ranitomeya variabilis) Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 2014;26:58–68. doi: 10.1080/03949370.2013.850453. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Beck KB, Loretto M-C, Ringler M, Hödl W, Pašukonis A. Relying on known or exploring for new? Movement patterns and reproductive resource use in a tadpole-transporting frog. PeerJ. 2017;5:e3745. doi: 10.7717/peerj.3745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Pašukonis A, Loretto M-C, Rojas B. How far do tadpoles travel in the rainforest? Parent-assisted dispersal in poison frogs. Evol. Ecol. 2019;33:613–623. doi: 10.1007/s10682-019-09994-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Summers K. Metabolism and parental care in ectotherms: a comment on Beekman et al. Behav. Ecol. 2019;30:593–594. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arz038. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Santos JC, Cannatella DC. Phenotypic integration emerges from aposematism and scale in poison frogs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2011;108:6175–6180. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010952108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Stynoski JL, Schulte LM, Rojas B. Poison frogs. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:R1026–R1028. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rojas B, Valkonen J, Nokelainen O. Aposematism. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:R350–R351. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Poulton EB. The Colours of Animals: Their Meaning and Use, Especially Considered in the Case of Insects. D. Appleton; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Santos JC, Coloma LA, Cannatella DC. Multiple, recurring origins of aposematism and diet specialization in poison frogs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2003;100:12792–12797. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2133521100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Vences M, et al. Convergent evolution of aposematic coloration in Neotropical poison frogs: a molecular phylogenetic perspective. Org. Divers. Evol. 2003;3:215–226. doi: 10.1078/1439-6092-00076. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Daly JW, et al. An uptake system for dietary alkaloids in poison frogs (Dendrobatidae) Toxicon. 1994;32:657–663. doi: 10.1016/0041-0101(94)90335-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Saporito RA, Spande TF, Garraffo HM, Donnelly MA. Arthropod alkaloids in poison frogs: a review of the dietary hypothesis. Heterocycles. 2009;79:277–297. doi: 10.3987/REV-08-SR(D)11. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Santos JC, et al. Aposematism increases acoustic diversification and speciation in poison frogs. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2014;281:20141761. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Caldwell JP. The evolution of myrmecophagy and its correlates in poison frogs (Family Dendrobatidae) J. Zool. 1996;240:75–101. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1996.tb05487.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Summers K, Symula R, Clough M, Cronin T. Visual mate choice in poison frogs. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 1999;266:2141–2145. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Duellman WE, Trueb L. Biology of Amphibians. JHU Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Summers K, McKeon CS. The evolutionary ecology of phytotelmata use in Neotropical poison frogs. Misc. Publ. Mus. Zool. Univ. Mich. 2004;193:55–73. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Summers K, Sea McKeon C, Heying H. The evolution of parental care and egg size: a comparative analysis in frogs. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2006;273:687–692. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wells KD. Courtship and parental behavior in a Panamanian poison-arrow frog (Dendrobates auratus) Herpetologica. 1978;34:148–155. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Summers K. Sexual selection and intra-female competition in the green poison-dart frog, Dendrobates auratus. Anim. Behav. 1989;37:797–805. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90064-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Summers K. Paternal care and the cost of polygyny in the green dart-poison frog. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1990;27:307–313. doi: 10.1007/BF00164001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Summers K, Amos W. Behavioral, ecological, and molecular genetic analyses of reproductive strategies in the Amazonian dart-poison frog, Dendrobates ventrimaculatus. Behav. Ecol. 1997;8:260–267. doi: 10.1093/beheco/8.3.260. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Limerick S. Courtship behavior and oviposition of the poison-arrow frog Dendrobates pumilio. Herpetologica. 1980;36:69–71. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Pröhl H, Hödl W. Parental investment, potential reproductive rates, and mating system in the strawberry dart-poison frog, Dendrobates pumilio. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1999;46:215–220. doi: 10.1007/s002650050612. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Brown JL, Morales V, Summers K. A key ecological trait drove the evolution of biparental care and monogamy in an amphibian. Am. Nat. 2010;175:436–446. doi: 10.1086/650727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Yang Y, Blomenkamp S, Dugas MB, Richards-Zawacki CL, Pröhl H. Mate choice versus mate preference: inferences about color-assortative mating differ between field and lab assays of poison frog behavior. Am. Nat. 2019;193:598–607. doi: 10.1086/702249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Wells KD. Behavoral ecology and social organization of a dendrobatid frog (Colostethus inguinalis) Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1980;6:199–209. doi: 10.1007/BF00569201. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Luddecke H. Behavioral aspects of the reproductive biology of the Andean frog Colostethus palmatus (Amphibia: Dendrobatidae) Rev. Acad. Colomb. Cienc. Exact. Fis. Nat. 1999;23:S303–S303. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Montanarin A, Kaefer IL, Lima AP. Courtship and mating behaviour of the brilliant-thighed frog Allobates femoralis from Central Amazonia: Implications for the study of a species complex. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 2011;23:141–150. doi: 10.1080/03949370.2011.554884. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Ursprung E, Ringler M, Jehle R, Hoedl W. Strong male/male competition allows for nonchoosy females: High levels of polygynandry in a territorial frog with paternal care. Mol. Ecol. 2011;20:1759–1771. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05056.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Stückler S, et al. Spatio-temporal characteristics of the prolonged courtship in brilliant-thighed poison frogs, Allobates femoralis. Herpetologica. 2019;75:268–279. doi: 10.1655/Herpetologica-D-19-00010.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Symula R, Schulte R, Summers K. Molecular phylogenetic evidence for a mimetic radiation in Peruvian poison frogs supports a Müllerian mimicry hypothesis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2001;268:2415–2421. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Summers K. Mating strategies in two species of dart-poison frogs: a comparative study. Anim. Behav. 1992;43:907–919. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(06)80004-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Rojas B, Pašukonis A. From habitat use to social behavior: natural history of a voiceless poison frog, Dendrobates tinctorius. PeerJ. 2019;7:e7648. doi: 10.7717/peerj.7648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Maan ME, Cummings ME. Poison frog colors are honest signals of toxicity, particularly for bird predators. Am. Nat. 2012;179:E1–E14. doi: 10.1086/663197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Grant T, et al. Phylogenetic systematics of dart-poison frogs and their relatives (Amphibia: Athesphatanura: Dendrobatidae) Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. 2006;2006:1–262. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Grant T, et al. Phylogenetic systematics of dart-poison frogs and their relatives revisited (Anura: Dendrobatoidea) S. Am. J. Herpetol. 2017;12:S1–S90. doi: 10.2994/SAJH-D-17-00017.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Duellman, W. E. Frogs of the genus Colostethus (Anura; Dendrobatidae) in the Andes of northern Peru (2004).
- 69.Fairbairn DJ. Odd Couples: Extraordinary Differences Between the Sexes in the Animal Kingdom. Princeton University Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Fairbairn DJ, Blanckenhorn WU, Székely T. Sex, Size and Gender Roles: Evolutionary Studies of Sexual Size Dimorphism. Oxford University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Vági B, Végvári Z, Liker A, Freckleton RP, Székely T. Parental care and the evolution of terrestriality in frogs. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2019;286:20182737. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.2737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Gosner KL. A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on identification. Herpetologica. 1960;16:183–190. [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kelber A, Vorobyev M, Osorio D. Animal colour vision–behavioural tests and physiological concepts. Biol. Rev. 2003;78:81–118. doi: 10.1017/S1464793102005985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Renoult JP, Kelber A, Schaefer HM. Colour spaces in ecology and evolutionary biology. Biol. Rev. 2017;92:292–315. doi: 10.1111/brv.12230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Endler JA. On the measurement and classification of colour in studies of animal colour patterns. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 1990;41:315–352. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1990.tb00839.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Kemp DJ, et al. An integrative framework for the appraisal of coloration in nature. Am. Nat. 2015;185:705–724. doi: 10.1086/681021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Troscianko J, Stevens M. Image calibration and analysis toolbox—a free software suite for objectively measuring reflectance, colour and pattern. Methods. Ecol. Evol. 2015;6:1320–1331. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Maia R, White TE. Comparing colors using visual models. Behav. Ecol. 2018;29:649–659. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ary017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Bergeron ZT, Fuller RC. Using human vision to detect variation in avian coloration: how bad is it? Am. Nat. 2018;191:269–276. doi: 10.1086/695282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Jetz W, Pyron RA. The interplay of past diversification and evolutionary isolation with present imperilment across the amphibian tree of life. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2018;2:850–858. doi: 10.1038/s41559-018-0515-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Yang Z, Kumar S, Nei M. A new method of inference of ancestral nucleotide and amino acid sequences. Genetics. 1995;141:1641–1650. doi: 10.1093/genetics/141.4.1641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Paradis E, Claude J, Strimmer K. APE: analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R language. Bioinformatics. 2004;20:289–290. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btg412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Series. B. Stat. Methodo. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- 84.Hadfield JD. MCMC methods for multi-response generalized linear mixed models: the MCMCglmm R package. J. Stat. Softw. 2010;33:1–22. doi: 10.18637/jss.v033.i02. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Gelman A, Rubin DB. Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Stat. Sci. 1992;7:457–472. [Google Scholar]
- 86.Barker D, Meade A, Pagel M. Constrained models of evolution lead to improved prediction of functional linkage from correlated gain and loss of genes. Bioinformatics. 2007;23:14–20. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btl558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Lindstedt C, Boncoraglio G, Cotter SC, Gilbert JDJ, Kilner RM. Parental care shapes evolution of aposematism and provides lifelong protection against predators. bioRxiv. 2019;25:644864. [Google Scholar]
- 88.Donnelly MA. Demographic effects of reproductive resource supplementation in a territorial frog, Dendrobates pumilio. Ecol. Monogr. 1989;59:207–221. doi: 10.2307/1942599. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Rojas B, Endler JA. Sexual dimorphism and intra-populational colour pattern variation in the aposematic frog Dendrobates tinctorius. Evol. Ecol. 2013;27:739–753. doi: 10.1007/s10682-013-9640-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Pröhl H. Territorial behavior in dendrobatid frogs. J. Herpetol. 2005;39:354–365. doi: 10.1670/162-04A.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Speed MP, Brockhurst MA, Ruxton GD. The dual benefits of aposematism: predator avoidance and enhanced resource collection. Evolution. 2010;64:1622–1633. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00931.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Fincke OM. Organization of predator assemblages in Neotropical tree holes: effects of abiotic factors and priority. Ecol. Entomol. 1999;24:13–23. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2311.1999.00166.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Summers K. The effects of cannibalism on Amazonian poison frog egg and tadpole deposition and survivorship in Heliconia axil pools. Oecologia. 1999;119:557–564. doi: 10.1007/s004420050819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.McKeon CS, Summers K. Predator driven reproductive behavior in a tropical frog. Evol. Ecol. 2013;27:725–737. doi: 10.1007/s10682-013-9641-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Amézquita A, Castro L, Arias M, González M, Esquivel C. Field but not lab paradigms support generalisation by predators of aposematic polymorphic prey: the Oophaga histrionica complex. Evol. Ecol. 2013;27:769–782. doi: 10.1007/s10682-013-9635-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Lawrence JP, Rojas B, Fouquet A, Mappes J, Blanchette A, Saporito R, Bosque RJ, Courtois E, Noonan BP. Weak warning signals can persist in the absence of gene flow. PNAS. 2019;116:19037–19045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1901872116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Lack, D. The natural regulation of animal numbers. The Natural Regulation of Animal Numbers. (1954).
- 98.Williams GC. Natural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a refinement of Lack's principle. Am. Nat. 1966;100:687–690. doi: 10.1086/282461. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Brown J, Morales V, Summers K. Divergence in parental care, habitat selection and larval life history between two species of Peruvian poison frogs: an experimental analysis. J. Evol. Biol. 2008;21:1534–1543. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01609.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Brown JL, Morales V, Summers K. Tactical reproductive parasitism via larval cannibalism in Peruvian poison frogs. Biol. Lett. 2009;5:148–151. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Brown JL, Morales V, Summers K. Home range size and location in relation to reproductive resources in poison frogs (Dendrobatidae): a Monte Carlo approach using GIS data. Anim. Behav. 2009;77:547–554. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.10.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Kok PJ, Willaert B, Means DB. A new diagnosis and description of Anomaloglossus roraima (La Marca, 1998) (Anura: Aromobatidae: Anomaloglossinae), with description of its tadpole and call. S. Am. J. Herpetol. 2013;8:29–45. doi: 10.2994/SAJH-D-12-00021.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Pašukonis A, et al. The significance of spatial memory for water finding in a tadpole-transporting frog. Anim. Behav. 2016;116:89–98. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.02.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Pašukonis A, Warrington I, Ringler M, Hödl W. Poison frogs rely on experience to find the way home in the rainforest. Biol. Lett. 2014;10:20140642. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Poelman EH, Dicke M. Offering offspring as food to cannibals: oviposition strategies of Amazonian poison frogs (Dendrobates ventrimaculatus) Evol. Ecol. 2007;21:215–227. doi: 10.1007/s10682-006-9000-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Caldwell JP, de Araujo MC. Cannibalistic interactions resulting from indiscriminate predatory behavior in tadpoles of poison frogs (Anura: Dendrobatidae) Biotropica. 1998;30:92–103. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.1998.tb00372.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Gray, H. M., Summers, K. & Ibáñez, R. Kin discrimination in cannibalistic tadpoles of the Green Poison Frog, Dendrobates auratus (Anura, Dendrobatidae). Phyllomedusa (2009).
- 108.Rojas B. Strange parental decisions: fathers of the dyeing poison frog deposit their tadpoles in pools occupied by large cannibals. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2014;68:551–559. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1670-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Schulte, L. M. & Mayer, M. Poison frog tadpoles seek parental transportation to escape their cannibalistic siblings. J. Zool.303, 83–89, 12472 (2017).
- 110.Ringler E, Pašukonis A, Hödl W, Ringler M. Tadpole transport logistics in a Neotropical poison frog: indications for strategic planning and adaptive plasticity in anuran parental care. Front. Zool. 2013;10:67. doi: 10.1186/1742-9994-10-67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Pröhl H. Variation in male calling behaviour and relation to male mating success in the strawberry poison frog (Dendrobates pumilio) Ethology. 2003;109:273–290. doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2003.00863.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Summers K, Earn DJ. The cost of polygyny and the evolution of female care in poison frogs. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 1999;66:515–538. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1999.tb01924.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Ringler E, et al. Flexible compensation of uniparental care: female poison frogs take over when males disappear. Behav. Ecol. 2015;26:1219–1225. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arv069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Pyron RA, Wiens JJ. A large-scale phylogeny of Amphibia including over 2800 species, and a revised classification of extant frogs, salamanders, and caecilians. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 2011;61:543–583. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2011.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Streicher JW, et al. Evaluating methods for phylogenomic analyses, and a new phylogeny for a major frog clade (Hyloidea) based on 2214 loci. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 2018;119:128–143. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2017.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Gilbert JD. Thrips domiciles protect larvae from desiccation in an arid environment. Behav. Ecol. 2014;25:1338–1346. doi: 10.1093/beheco/aru128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Hime PM, et al. Phylogenomics reveals ancient gene tree discordance in the amphibian tree of life. Syst. Biol. 2021;70:49–66. doi: 10.1093/sysbio/syaa034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Moen DS, Morlon H, Wiens JJ. Testing convergence versus history: convergence dominates phenotypic evolution for over 150 million years in frogs. Syst. Biol. 2016;65:146–160. doi: 10.1093/sysbio/syv073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Gomez-Mestre I, Pyron RA, Wiens JJ. Phylogenetic analyses reveal unexpected patterns in the evolution of reproductive modes in frogs. Evolution. 2012;66:3687–3700. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01715.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Liu Y, Day LB, Summers K, Burmeister SS. Learning to learn: advanced behavioural flexibility in a poison frog. Anim. Behav. 2016;111:167–172. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.10.018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Liu Y, Day LB, Summers K, Burmeister SS. A cognitive map in a poison frog. J. Exp. Biol. 2019;222:jeb97467. doi: 10.1242/jeb.197467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Liu Y, Jones CD, Day LB, Summers K, Burmeister SS. Cognitive phenotype and differential gene expression in a hippocampal homologue in two species of frog. Integr. Comp Biol. 2020;60:1007–1023. doi: 10.1093/icb/icaa032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets supporting this article will be uploaded to the online repository figshare (https://figshare.com/s/338fc287ef792d432bb8).