Skip to main content
Heliyon logoLink to Heliyon
. 2021 Sep 22;7(9):e07894. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e07894

The relation between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms, adult attachment and Instagram sociability: An exploratory analysis

Alessandro Carollo a, Andrea Bonassi a,b, Ilaria Cataldo a, Giulio Gabrieli c, Moses Tandiono d,e, Jia Nee Foo d,e, Bruno Lepri b, Gianluca Esposito a,c,d,
PMCID: PMC8477146  PMID: 34611556

Abstract

Oxytocin is a primary neuropeptide which coordinates affiliative behavior. Previous researchers pointed to the association between genetic vulnerability on Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) and environmental factors (e.g., social relationships) to comprehend social behavior. Although an extensive knowledge of in-person social interactions has been obtained, little is known about online sociability. A gene-environment perspective is adopted to examine how OXTR and adult attachment moderate Instagram behavior. The genetic factors within the regions OXTR/rs53576 (A/A homozygotes vs G-carriers) and OXTR/rs2254298 (G/G homozygotes vs A-carriers) were assessed. The Experience in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) questionnaire was used to collect participants' (N = 57, 16 males) attachment with a partner. The number of posts, followed people (“followings”) and followers were obtained from Instagram, and the Social Desirability Index (SDI) was calculated as the ratio of followers to followings. Interaction effects between OXTR groups and ECR-R scores on the number of posts and SDI were hypothesized. Results showed an effect of rs53576 on the number of Instagram followings. Specifically, people with A/A OXTR/rs53576 genotype had more followings than G-carriers independent of the anxiety or avoidance felt towards their partner. These preliminary results offer insights into future investigations on social media behavior.

Keywords: Gene*Environment, Adult attachment, Close relationship, Oxytocin Receptor Gene, rs53576, rs2254298, Online behavior, Social media, Social network, Instagram


Gene*Environment; Adult attachment; Close relationship; Oxytocin Receptor Gene; rs53576; rs2254298; Online behavior; Social media; Social network; Instagram

1. Introduction

People forge social bonds throughout their life, from the first sight of their primary caregiver to the affection felt for their partner. Social attitudes not only have a re-creative value, but they are also crucial for the development of social and life skills. In the human life course, socialization's origins have been traced back to the interplay between genes and environment.

1.1. The role of the oxytocin receptor gene on socialization

Social behavior finds its origins in the biological roots of the individual starting from the genetic predispositions. In the field of behavioral genetics on sociability, some authors have focused on the genes that rule the levels of oxytocin regulated by the Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR). Generally, oxytocin is a neuropeptide that plays a crucial role for social skills in mammals [1], [2], [3], [4], [5], [6], [7]. Oxytocin has a primary effect on the parental motivation towards the offspring: oxytocin plasma levels in mothers and fathers are correlated with positive engagement in parent-infant interactions consequently influencing the infant's socio-emotional development [8], [9], [10]. As candidate substrates of early and long term epigenetic changes and mediators of caregiving affect social behavior throughout human development [11], [12], oxytocin receptors are mapped in brain areas that are typically involved in reproductive, social and adaptive behaviors, such as the limbic system and the brainstem [13], [14]. Higher oxytocin levels could reinforce social experiences by causing a reduction in the levels of anxiety [15], [16]. Numerous OXTR Single-Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs), such as OXTR/rs53576 and OXTR/rs2254298, have been shown to predict parenting behavior and attachment [17], [18], [19], [20], [21]. For instance, the G allele of OXTR/rs53576 is associated with increased parental responsiveness [22], whereas the same allele on OXTR/rs2254298 is linked to lower plasma oxytocin and to less parental touch [23].

Different theoretical frameworks have been proposed to provide an explication of sociability under a gene*environment perspective. In the intersection of hereditary factors and environmental triggers which shape human development, the Social Salience Hypothesis of oxytocin [24], [25] attributes distinctive social behavioral patterns associated with different OXTR alleles to the perceived cues of a given social environment [26], [27]. An intriguing view is also offered by the Differential Susceptibility model [28], [29], which stipulates that the susceptibility to the environment depends on the so-called “plasticity genes”. These genes (e.g., OXTR) lead to broader individual plasticity. In fact, given a specific allele, individuals would not only show amplified adverse effects when exposed to a negative environment [30], [31], but also they would experience greater benefit from a positive and healthy environments [32], [33].

The two distinct OXTR/rs53576 and OXTR/rs2254298 SNPs appear to be favorable candidates when looking for the genetic modulation of social behavior and cognition [34], [35]. Specifically, a given allele within each of the two genetic regions could confer a peculiar level of vulnerability or susceptibility, either in an adaptive or in a maladaptive direction, to the environment in the individual [36].

1.2. Environmental influences on socialization and their interplay with the oxytocin receptor gene

Among multiple factors that shape sociability, environmental influences such as familiar education, parental bonding and adult attachment play a role in regulating in-person and online sociability [37], [38], [39], [40], [41]. According to the prototypical hypothesis of attachment, the relational patterns with peers and the partner are influenced by the early relationships with parents during infancy and childhood, which appear to be stable throughout life [42], [43], [44], [45]. Nevertheless, the revisionist hypothesis points to the evidence that the adult relationships may alter the early attachment representations and form new attachment models based on the novel social experiences [46]. Similarly, the quality of adult relationships with peers and one's partner affects different domains of life. In fact, people from high-quality marriages (i.e., people who show better marital adjustment and higher marital satisfaction) manifest better physical and mental health than the ones in low-quality marriages [47], [48], [49], [50], [51], [52]. Within the context of gene-environment interactions, multiple studies focused on social behavior as a product of a differential genetic sensitivity (i.e., OXTR/rs53576; OXTR/rs2254298) to positive and negative environments, such as successful or dysfunctional relationships with a partner. Previous works reported that people with the G allele of the OXTR/rs53576 are more sensitive to hostile environments seeking higher social support when compared to the A allele [53], [54]. Individuals carrying G/G genotype or having a partner with G/G genotype also reported higher marital satisfaction than A-carriers [55]. With regards to OXTR/rs2254298, the debate on the role of vulnerability genes is still open. In fact, if on one side G/G genotype has been observed to be highly associated with separation anxiety and depression [56], on the other, females G/G homozygotes showed less attachment anxiety than A-carriers, while male G/G homozygotes exhibited less autism-associated traits than A-carriers [57]. The majority of studies consider A/A genotype as the one conferring the vulnerability to the environment when adopting the Differential Susceptibility Model [58], [59].

However, there is a paucity in research investigating online sociability, particularly those considering the continuous and reciprocal interaction between the environment to which a person is exposed and their genetic profile [60], [61], [62].

1.3. Socialization in the context of social networks sites

Nowadays, socialization takes place both offline and online, persuading researchers to examine not only the in-person social interactions but also virtual social interactions. The last decade has observed an increasing number of studies inspecting numerous social networks sites (SNSs), such as Instagram, Facebook, Twitter, whose centrality in human life has grown impressively [63], [64]. These digital environments have become the theaters of novel and interactive social behaviors, instantly shared with large communities from all over the world.

Among the SNSs, Instagram proved to be a useful platform to explore online sociability [65], [66]. Here, users connect by posting pictures and daily stories, following other users' profiles and chatting with them actively. The studies investigating online sociability on Instagram have detected both positive (e.g. decreased feelings of solitude or increased motivation and inspiration) [67], [68] and negative effects, usually linked to poorer mental health outcomes on users [69], [70], [71].

Although there is a growing number of studies on SNSs such as Instagram, it is not yet clear whether in-person and online social interactions share the same mechanisms. To fill this existing gap in the literature, the current paper aims to adopt a gene*environment perspective to investigate Instagram sociability. Specifically, the current exploratory research focuses on Instagram sociability as a result of potential interaction between genetic factors (i.e. Oxytocin Receptor Gene) and the environmental effects related to the quality of adult relationships with their partners (i.e. adult attachment with an intimate partner). Specifically, two OXTR SNPs (OXTR/rs53576 and OXTR/rs2254298) were selected as genetic factors. The Experience in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) questionnaire was used to assess the individual's self-perception of adult attachment with an intimate partner [72]. Three Instagram variables were collected: 1) the number of posts, 2) the number of followed people (here called “followings”) and the number of followers. An index, called “Social Desirability Index” (SDI), was computed as the ratio of followers to followings. Higher values of the SDI would indicate greater online sociability.

In line with the Differential Susceptibility model, an interaction effect between the genetic factors and the close relationship scores was hypothesized for each Instagram variable. Specifically, regardless of gender, we formulated one hypothesis: OXTR/rs2254298 A-carriers and OXTR/rs53576 G-carriers would show a higher number of posts and values of SDI when involved in a positive and favorable relationship with their partner (lower scores in the ECR-R dimensions Avoidance and Anxiety) compared to OXTR/rs2254298 G/G homozygotes and OXTR/rs53576 A/A homozygotes. Conversely, OXTR/rs2254298 A-carriers and OXTR/rs53576 G-carriers would show a lower number of posts and SDI when involved in a negative and unfavorable relationship with their partner (higher scores in the ECR-R dimensions Avoidance and Anxiety) compared to OXTR/rs2254298 G/G homozygotes and OXTR/rs53576 A/A homozygotes.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Sixty-one (N = 61) non-parent adults were recruited among students enrolled at the Nanyang Technological University (Singapore). Participants of the study were all Singaporean. Exclusion criteria were: (i) present or lifetime history of psychiatric, neurological or genetic disorders, (ii) not having an Instagram account, and (iii) being older than 30 years. This third criterion was adopted in order to get a pool of comparable data from participants in the same age range. It is to note that this criterion did not affect the distribution of genetic and attachment data, and reflected anyway the majority of the examined university population. We could not take into account data from four (N = 4) participants because of failures in either the online compilation of questionnaires or in Instagram data extraction. In the end, our final sample consisted of 57 adults (16 males) between 18 and 25 years-old (M = 20.89, SD = 1.59; Males: M = 22.56, SD = 1.15; Females: M = 20.24, SD = 1.24).

2.2. Data collection

The methods and analytic plan for this study were preregistered on the Open Science Framework at the following address: https://osf.io/t78fu. Genetic, behavioral and Instagram data were collected. The research was authorized by the Ethical Committee of Nanyang Technological University (IRB-2015-08-020-01). Informed consent was obtained from all participants. The study followed the Declaration of Helsinki. The genetic assessment was conducted on anonymized samples at the Nanyang Technological University (Singapore). Instagram data and questionnaires' data were anonymized at the beginning of the data collection. With regards to genetic data, a sterile cotton swab for genotyping was used to collect a buccal mucosa sample of DNA from each participant. Concerning the behavioral assessment, every participant completed online the self-report questionnaire Experience in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R). To collect Instagram indexes, an in-house built Python-based web-scraper developed on BeautifulSoup, a python package designed for web-scraping [73], was employed. Firstly, the algorithm extracted the content of participants' Instagram profile from the DOM of the page. Subsequently, the algorithm detected the specific objects — namely “posts”, “followers”, and “following” — in order to limit the amount of personal information accessed from each profile. A manual inspection of the data was then conducted to check for the presence of missing values due to technical errors, which were then manually updated (e.g. connection timeout, server errors). The same script was employed in previous works [60].

The final dataset generated for this work is available online on the Data Repository of the Nanyang Technological University (DR-NTU Data) [74].

2.3. Genetic assessment

The genetic assessment of the present study employed the method reported by Bonassi et al. [75]. DNA extraction and genotyping were performed by ACGT, Inc. (Wheeling, IL). DNA was extracted by using Oragene DNA purification reagent and its concentrations were evaluated through spectroscopy (NanoDrop Technologies, USA). Each DNA sample was increased by polymerase chain reaction (PCR) for the OXTR gene rs53576 region target with the primers 5-GCC CAC CAT GCT CTC CAC ATC-3 and 5-GCT GGA CTC AGG AGG AAT AGG GAC-3. A PCR reaction of 20 ll, comprising 1.5 ll of genomic DNA from the test sample, PCR buffer, 1 mM each of the forward and reverse primers, 10 mM deoxyribonucleotides, KapaTaq polymerase, and 50 mM MgCl2 was executed. PCR operation comprised of 15 minute denaturation at 95 C, and 35 cycles at 94 C (30 s), 60 C (60 s), 72 C (60 s) and a final 10 minute step at 72 C. PCR reactions were genotyped with an ABI 3730xl Genetic Analyzer (Applied Biosystems Inc.) and normalized with GeneScan 600 LIZ (Applied Biosystems, Inc.) size standards on each sample. Genotypic data were inspected using GeneMapper ID (Applied Biosystems, Inc.).

The same procedure was used to assess the OXTR gene rs2254298 region. However, the forward and reverse primers were instead 5-TGA AAG CAG AGG TTG TGT GGA CAG G-3 and 5-AAC GCC CAC CCC AGT TTC TTC-3 respectively.

2.4. Close relationships

In order to assess the participants' relationship with their partner, the Experience in Close Relationship-Revised questionnaire was used. Developed by Fraley et al. [76], the ECR-R (average Cronbach's α=0.89) is a 36-item self-report questionnaire on a 7-point response scale, that examines the close relationship across two dimensions: Anxiety (Cronbach's α=0.93) and Avoidance (Cronbach's α=0.85). While the former is usually linked to worries about the relationship, and fear of rejection from one's partner (i.e., “I find that my partner(s) don't want to get as close as I would like”), the latter mostly refers to discomfort with intimacy and closeness to one's partner (i.e., “I find it difficult to allow myself to depend on romantic partners”). The current instrument has obtained extensive approval, high reliability and adequate consistency in appraising the constructs related to the attachment styles with the romantic partner [77], [78], [79]. Several studies applied the ECR-R to understand how the adult attachment security can be modulated by infant attachment, childhood maltreatment and can moderate social cognition, emotional regulation, personality and mindfulness [80], [81], [82], [83]. The ECR-R has also been used by researchers in the field of behavioral genetics. In this context, the ECR-R dimensions were conceived as potential environmental variables able to affect physiological and psychological variables [84], or the product of gene*environment interactions [85], [86]. We calculated the scores for the two constructs of anxiety and avoidance by following the scoring indications reported by Picardi et al. [87].

2.5. Instagram variables

Four variables were considered as highly representative of online sociability on Instagram. Number of posts indicates the number of content published by a participant's profile. On Instagram, the number of posts is equivalent to the number of published pictures, since this social network allows accounts to publish pictures and video with an auxiliary text. The literature regarding the determinants of one's number of posts shows contrasting results. For instance, depressed mood can be an antecedent of an increased posting activity as well as narcissism, or even extraversion [88], [89], [90]. Therefore, an increased activity on SNSs is not a mere positive or negative indicator of the user's well-being, but may depend on diverse and even contrasting drives (e.g., the need to escape from reality, the need to get in touch with others, and so on). The number of followed people (“followings”) is an index that reflects the number of profiles that a participant follows. On Instagram, following allows the user (the follower) to view — or ask to view, in the case of private profiles — other users' posts (the “followings”) in the general feed. Conversely, the number of followers is defined by the number of profiles that follows the considered participant. Broadly speaking, publishing a post can be considered as an active form of social communication, for the user decides what others can see from their profile. Following, instead, is a type of passive communication, for the user is exposed to the contents selected by others. Additionally, the SDI was computed as the ratio of followers to followings. This parameter, previously adopted as a measure of general sociability [60], [61], estimates the balance between the number of followers and followings for each account. By simultaneously taking into account the amount of profiles that one follows or is followed by, the SDI can furnish an estimate of the centrality of a specific user within the network of contacts. SDI can theoretically range from 0 to infinite, where 0 indicates a strong interest on following other people's contents and infinite indicates a scarce interest on following other accounts together with a strong appeal for other people. Values close to 1 for SDI would indicate a balanced activity in the network, ideally with the same values of followers and followings.

2.6. Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis and data visualization were performed using R (R-core base version 4.0.0.). Instagram data were standardized using z-score transformation and visually inspected for normality. To reduce the variability of the data, and to increase the statistical power of performed tests [91], [92], outliers — defined as values that differ by two or more standard deviations from the mean of the distribution — were inspected in the database. Overall, 8 data point were identified as outliers (3 for followings number, 2 each for posts number and SDI, 1 for follower number).

Before and after the outliers removal, normality of ECR-R and Instagram scores were tested by computing the values of skewness and kurtosis, and their distributions were manually inspected by visualization via boxplots, density plots, and quantile-quantile plots. Of the investigated variables, when all the data points were analyzed, the participants' number of posts, SDI and number of followers were not distributed in a Gaussian fashion. When outliers were removed from the dataset, only the participant's number of posts did not follow the Gaussian distribution. Thus, the related samplings were adjusted and log-transformed. Subsequently, the assumption of the homogeneity of variance was verified.

To detect potential gene*environment interactions on Instagram variables considered for the two hypotheses (number of posts, SDI values), two different mixed ANCOVAs were employed for each OXTR gene SNP: one ANCOVA with Instagram number of posts and one with Instagram SDI as DV for OXTR/rs53576, whereas one ANCOVA with Instagram number of posts and one with Instagram SDI as DV for OXTR/rs2254298 [59], [93], [94]. To take into account the increased possibility of committing a type I error, a false discovery rate correction was employed by means of a Bonferroni Correction (corrected α=0.025). Bonferroni's method for multiple testing correction was chosen rather than the available alternative approaches to apply a more conservative correction.

A prior sensitivity power analysis conducted on G*Power [95] (power = 0.80) estimated that the analysis could detect a medium effect size (f = 0.42) via analysis of covariance given the current sample size (N = 57) and the corrected α=0.025 (ANCOVA: fixed effects, main effects and interactions).

In the analysis, OXTR/rs53576 (A/A vs. G-carriers) and OXTR/rs2254298 (G/G vs. A-carriers) were considered as separate predictors in distinct analyses. Two out of four mixed ANCOVAs included the OXTR gene genotype rs53576 (A/A and G-carriers) as a between-subject factor and the ECR-R dimensions (Anxiety and Avoidance) as continuous covariates, as in Equation (1).

Instagramvariable=OXTR SNP(ECRAnxiety+ECRAvoidance) (1)

Likewise, the two remaining ANCOVAs included the OXTR gene genotype rs2254298 (G/G and A-carriers) as the between-subject factor, and the ECR-R dimensions (Anxiety and Avoidance) as continuous covariates, as in Equation (1). If any significant difference emerged on the values of the Instagram variables in relation to the participants' gender, gender was included in the ANCOVA equation as a between-subject factor.

3. Results

3.1. Results observed on unaltered data

3.1.1. Preliminary analysis on the genetic variables

As common practice in the genetic studies, in order to have statistically comparable groups of participants, the heterozygotes and less numerous homozygotes groups were merged together in a group of “allele-carriers” [94]. Therefore, within the OXTR/rs53576 DNA region, participants with at least one G allele (G/G homozygotes or A/G) were categorized into a single G-carriers group. The distribution in our sample was 39% for A/A homozygous and 61% for G-carriers. The frequencies of the genotype were: A/A = 22 (38.60%), A/G = 30 (52.63%), G/G = 5 (8.77%), and the distribution was consistent with the Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium (X2 (1) = 1.38, p = 0.241, ns). For the OXTR gene rs2254298 region, participants with at least one A allele (A/A homozygotes or G/A) were classified into a single A-carriers group. The distribution in our sample was 56% for G/G homozygotes and 44% for A-carriers. The frequencies of the genotype were: A/A = 4 (7.02%), G/A = 21 (36.84%), G/G = 32 (56.14%), and the distribution was consistent with the Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium (X2 (1) = 0.05, p = 0.828, ns). Table 1 reports the observed frequency of OXTR/rs53576 and OXTR/rs2254298 genotypes by the total sample size. In the context of OXTR/rs53576, participants gender (X2 (1) = 0.17, p = 0.683, ns) did not significantly differ between the two groups A/A vs G. As regards OXTR/rs2254298, participants' gender (X2 (1) = 0.81, p = 0.367, ns) did not significantly differ between the two groups G/G vs A.

Table 1.

Frequency values of the total sample in Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs53576 and Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs2254298 genotypes. The frequency percentage of each genotype is reported between parenthesis. The presented allelic frequencies are consistent with the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (for OXTR/rs53576, X2 (1) = 1.38, p = 0.241, ns; for OXTR/rs2254298, X2 (1) = 0.05, p = 0.828, ns). The present data refer to the 57 participants which completed the entire assessment across three levels: genetic (OXTR/rs53576; OXTR/rs2254298), behavioral (Experience in Close Relationships-Revised questionnaire) and Instagram (number of posts, following, followers, and the Social Desirability Index) data collections.

OXTR/rs53576
Alleles Total
AA 22 (38.60%)
AG 30 (52.63%)
GG 5 (8.77%)
G-carriers 35 (61.40%)
Total 57

OXTR/rs2254298

Alleles Total
AA 4 (7.02%)
GA 21 (36.84%)
GG 32 (56.14%)
A-carriers 25 (43.86%)
Total 57

3.1.2. Preliminary analysis on ECR-R and Instagram variables

Table 2 describes the values concerning the data distribution of each continuous variable (two ECR-R dimensions and four Instagram variables) in terms of skewness, kurtosis and descriptive statistics (see “Statistical Analysis” for further details).

Table 2.

Summary of the descriptive statistics for each continuous variable. The distribution of each ECR-R (Experience in Close Relationships-Revised) dimension and Instagram variable is described in terms of Skewness, Kurtosis, Minimum (Min), first Quartile (1st Q), Median, Mean, third Quartile (3rd Q), Maximum (Max) and Standard Deviation (SD). Instagram variables were standardized using z-score transformation and inspected for normality. Skewness and Kurtosis values were computed for each continuous subscale of the ECR-R questionnaire and each Instagram variable. Participants' number of posts, Social Desirability Index, and number of followers did not follow the Gaussian distribution, and were thus adjusted by log-transformation.

Variable Skewness Kurtosis Min 1st Q Median Mean 3rd Q Max SD
Anxiety -0.38 -0.02 1.39 3.33 3.94 3.94 4.56 6.22 1.08
Avoidance -0.15 -0.39 1.06 2.11 2.94 2.95 3.67 5.39 0.93
Posts number 3.43 12.61 -0.60 -0.51 -0.36 0.00 0.04 4.61 1.00
Log-transformed posts number 2.08 4.78 0.33 0.40 0.49 0.62 0.71 1.89 0.34
Social Desirability Index 5.66 35.39 -1.16 -0.27 -0.12 0.00 0.01 6.76 1.00
Log-transformed Social Desirability Index 2.28 12.73 -0.17 0.55 0.63 0.64 0.70 2.17 0.29
Followings 0.53 -0.46 -1.53 -0.77 -0.12 0.00 0.67 2.47 1.00
Followers 4.74 27.70 -0.80 -0.50 -0.17 0.00 0.24 6.40 1.00
Log-transformed followers number 1.71 5.84 0.18 0.40 0.61 0.63 0.81 2.13 0.33

To exclude any significant effect of participants' gender on Instagram variables, four preliminary two-tailed Student's t-tests were performed (corrected α=0.0125). As expected, no significant differences in the standardized Instagram number of posts (t = -0.16, df = 55, p = 0.874), 95% CI [-0.22, 0.18], followings (t = 2.20 df = 55, p = 0.032), 95% CI [0.06, 1.20], and on the standardized SDI (t = 1.60, df = 55, p = 0.116), 95% CI [-0.03, 0.31] were found between male and female participants. Conversely, a significant difference between genders was detected for Instagram number of followers (t = 2.60, df = 55, p = 0.012), 95% CI [0.05, 0.42]. This evidence allows to analyze data of female and male participants together in the subsequent analysis on Instagram number of posts, followings, and SDI. As for Instagram number of followers, participants' gender was included in the subsequent analysis and examined as a potential moderator factor.

3.1.3. Hypothesis-driven analysis: Instagram number of posts and SDI

Means and standard error means of Instagram number of posts and SDI for OXTR/rs53576 and OXTR/rs2254298 are reported in Table 3.

Table 3.

Mean values in Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs53576 A/A homozygotes and G-carriers and Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs2254298 G/G homozygotes and A-carriers divided by Experience in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) dimensions (high or low) on Instagram number of posts and Social Desirability Index. Standard error means are reported between parentheses. Instagram behavior is reported by users' OXTR/rs53576 phenotype and ECR-R dimensions, consistently with the literature on the topic and a previous publication [60].

OXTR/rs53576

Number of posts

ECR-R dimension Low/AA Low/G High/AA High/G
Anxiety 0.76 (0.12) 0.54 (0.05) 0.67 (0.15) 0.58 (0.07)
Avoidance 0.91 (0.15) 0.62 (0.06) 0.49 (0.04) 0.50 (0.06)

Social Desirability Index

ECR-R dimension Low/AA Low/G High/AA High/G
Anxiety 0.59 (0.03) 0.74 (0.11) 0.63 (0.02) 0.58 (0.07)
Avoidance 0.62 (0.02) 0.66 (0.07) 0.59 (0.02) 0.66 (0.11)

OXTR/rs2254298

Number of posts

ECR-R dimension Low/GG Low/A High/GG High/A
Anxiety 0.60 (0.06) 0.67 (0.13) 0.67 (0.11) 0.54 (0.07)
Avoidance 0.74 (0.09) 0.74 (0.12) 0.52 (0.05) 0.48 (0.06)

Social Desirability Index

ECR-R dimension Low/GG Low/A High/GG High/A
Anxiety 0.69 (0.11) 0.65 (0.05) 0.62 (0.08) 0.58 (0.04)
Avoidance 0.66 (0.06) 0.62 (0.06) 0.65 (0.13) 0.60 (0.02)

Results revealed no significant main effects of OXTR/rs53576 (p > 0.025) on Instagram number of posts and SDI. Nevertheless, a main effect emerged for ECR-R avoidance on the number of posts (F(1, 56) = 9.96, p < 0.003, pη2 = 0.16; see Fig. 1). Moreover, a significant interaction between ECR-R avoidance and OXTR/rs53576 on Instagram number of posts was found (F(1, 56) = 7.12, p < 0.010, pη2 = 0.12; see Fig. 2). No other main effect or interaction effect was detected on the number of posts and SDI (Table 4).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Main Effect of Avoidance in Close Relationships on Instagram Number of Posts. (A) Negative linear association between avoidance and Instagram number of posts. (B) Comparison between the number of posts in participants with low (blue) and high (red) avoidance in close relationships (** p < 0.01).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Interaction between Avoidance in Close Relationships and OXTR/rs53576 genotype on determining the Instagram number of posts. (A) Linear relationships between avoidance in close relationships and Instagram number of posts divided by genotype (in orange A/A homozygotes, in light blue C-carriers). (B) Comparison between the number of Instagram posts in participants with high and low levels of avoidance. Barplots are reported for both the genetic groups: A/A homozygotes in orange and C-carriers in light blue.

Table 4.

Hypothesis-driven analysis: statistics' summary of ANCOVAs on Instagram number of posts and Social Desirability Index. For each Instagram variable considered in the hypothesis (Instagram number of posts and SDI) as dependent variable, one mixed ANCOVA was performed with OXTR gene genotype rs53576 (A/A and G-carriers) as a between-subject factor and the ECR-R dimensions (Anxiety and Avoidance) as continuous covariates (see the top section of the Table). For each Instagram variable, one mixed ANCOVA was also performed with the OXTR gene genotype rs2254298 (G/G and A-carriers) as a between-subject factor and the other parameters unchanged (see the below section of the Table). A significant main effects of the avoidance in close relationships was found on the number of posts. Also, avoidance in close relationships by interacting with the OXTR/rs53576 resulted to modulate the number of posts. (* p < 0.025; ** p < 0.001).

OXTR/rs53576

Number of posts

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs53576 1 0.338 0.3381 3.925 0.05299
Anxiety 1 0.068 0.0676 0.785 0.37972
Avoidance 1 0.858 0.8579 9.958 0.00269 **
OXTR/rs53576: Anxiety 1 0.025 0.0248 0.287 0.59417
OXTR/rs53576: Avoidance 1 0.613 0.6130 7.116 0.01022 *
Residuals 51 4.394 0.0861

Social desirability index

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs53576 1 0.035 0.03469 0.373 0.544
Anxiety 1 0.034 0.03417 0.367 0.547
Avoidance 1 0.000 0.00023 0.002 0.961
OXTR/rs53576: Anxiety 1 0.023 0.02330 0.250 0.619
OXTR/rs53576: Avoidance 1 0.004 0.00424 0.046 0.832
Residuals 51 4.748 0.09309

OXTR/rs2254298

Number of posts

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs2254298 1 0.014 0.0144 0.146 0.70366
Anxiety 1 0.034 0.0341 0.348 0.55801
Avoidance 1 1.017 1.0172 10.358 0.00224 **
OXTR/rs2254298: Anxiety 1 0.108 0.1075 1.095 0.30034
OXTR/rs2254298: Avoidance 1 0.113 0.1133 1.153 0.28792
Residuals 51 5.008 0.0982

Social desirability index

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs2254298 1 0.027 0.02727 0.293 0.590
Anxiety 1 0.029 0.02887 0.311 0.580
Avoidance 1 0.001 0.00113 0.012 0.912
OXTR/rs2254298: Anxiety 1 0.026 0.02594 0.279 0.600
OXTR/rs2254298: Avoidance 1 0.022 0.02177 0.234 0.630
Residuals 51 4.739 0.09293

Similarly, when considering OXTR/rs2254298, the same main effect of avoidance in close relationships on the number of posts on Instagram was detected (F(1, 56) = 10.36, p < 0.002, pη2 = 0.17) (see Table 4; see Fig. 1).

To test the directionality of the effects found in the hypothesis-driven analyses, consequent post-hoc analyses were implemented. In particular, avoidance from the partner emerged to negatively predict one's number of posts on Instagram (t(55) = -2.54, r = -0.32, p = 0.014, 95% CI [-0.54, -0.07]; see Fig. 1A). In other words, the higher the avoidance, the lower the number of Instagram posts. Moreover, after applying the median split to the values of avoidance in close relationships, the result was confirmed by a two-tailed Student's t test. Specifically, a significant difference emerged when comparing participants with low and high values of avoidance in regards to their number of Instagram posts (t(55) = 2.85, p = 0.006, 95% CI [0.07, 0.41]) (see Fig. 1B).

Also the effect found for the interplay between the OXTR/rs53576 genotype and ECR-R avoidance on the number of posts was examined with post-hoc tests. In particular, after applying the median split procedure on the ECR-R avoidance, it emerged that the distribution of genotypes (G/G vs. A-carriers groups) was not significantly different between participants showing high rather than low avoidance (X2 (1) = 0.03, p = 0.867, ns). Avoidance in close relationships resulted to be significantly correlated with the number of posts for the individual in the group of A/A carriers (t = -2.80, df = 20, r = -0.53, p = 0.011, 95% CI [-0.78, -0.14]). Conversely, the correlation between avoidance and number of posts on Instagram was not significant for the G-carriers (t = -0.22, df = 33, r = -0.04, p = 0.831, 95% CI [-0.37, 0.30]). Homogeneity of variance of the Instagram number of posts by avoidance in close relationships was checked (K2 = 9.55, df = 1, p = 0.002) (see Fig. 2).

3.1.4. Exploratory analysis: Instagram number of followings and followers

Means and standard error means of Instagram number of followings and followers for OXTR/rs53576 and OXTR/rs2254298 are reported in Table 5.

Table 5.

Mean values in Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs53576 A/A homozygotes and G-carriers and Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs2254298 G/G homozygotes and A-carriers divided by Experience in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) dimensions (high or low) on Instagram number of followings and followers. Standard error means are indicated between parentheses.

OXTR/rs53576

Number of followings

ECR-R Dimension Low/AA Low/G High/AA High/G
Anxiety 0.78 (0.30) -0.23 (0.19) -0.08 (0.34) -0.26 (0.21)
Avoidance 0.54 (0.31) -0.04 (0.25) 0.21 (0.38) -0.44 (0.14)

Number of followers

ECR-R Dimension Low/AA Low/G High/AA High/G
Anxiety 0.76 (0.06) 0.67 (0.11) 0.59 (0.09) 0.52 (0.06)
Avoidance 0.72 (0.08) 0.62 (0.07) 0.62 (0.08) 0.57 (0.10)

OXTR/rs2254298

Number of followings

ECR-R Dimension Low/GG Low/A High/GG High/A
Anxiety 0.16 (0.27) 0.23 (0.27) -0.21 (0.28) -0.18 (0.23)
Avoidance 0.08 (0.26) 0.37 (0.31) -0.12 (0.29) -0.31 (0.16)

Number of followers

ECR-R Dimension Low/GG Low/A High/GG High/A
Anxiety 0.72 (0.11) 0.68 (0.07) 0.54 (0.08) 0.55 (0.06)
Avoidance 0.63 (0.07) 0.70 (0.08) 0.64 (0.13) 0.53 (0.04)

The hypothesis-driven approach adopted for Instagram number of posts and SDI was extended as exploratory analysis to the number of followings and followers. Therefore, a Bonferroni's correction was separately applied for the two exploratory tests (corrected α=0.025). To identify exploratory gene*environment interactions on two further Instagram variables that were collected (number of followings and followers), two mixed ANCOVAs were employed for each OXTR gene SNP: one ANCOVA with Instagram's number of followings and one with Instagram number of followers as DV for OXTR/rs53576, whereas one ANCOVA with Instagram's number of followings and one with Instagram number of followers as DV for OXTR/rs2254298. For each ANCOVA the between-subject factor and the covariates were unchanged, as in Equation (1). Moreover, since Instagram number of followers emerged to be significantly different between genders, participants' gender itself was included as a moderator factor in the ANCOVA equations regarding the number of followers.

The ANCOVA performed on Instagram number of followings revealed a significant main effect of OXTR/rs53576 genotype (F(1, 56) = 6.19, p < 0.016, pη2 = 0.11), as indicated in Table 6. No further significant interactions between the ECR-R subscales and OXTR/rs53576 on the Instagram number of followings were observed (see Table 6).

Table 6.

Exploratory analysis: statistics' summary of ANCOVAs on Instagram number of followings and number of followers. For each Instagram variable considered in the exploratory analysis (Instagram number of followings and followers) as dependent variable (DV), one mixed ANCOVA was performed with OXTR gene genotype rs53576 (A/A and G-carriers) as a between-subject factor and the ECR-R dimensions (Anxiety and Avoidance) as continuous covariates, as in Equation (1) (see the top section of the Table). Gender was added as a further between-subjects factor in the analysis including Instagram number of followers. For each Instagram variable, one mixed ANCOVA was also performed with the OXTR gene genotype rs2254298 (G/G and A-carriers) as a between-subject factor and the other parameters unchanged (see the below section of the Table). A gender effect on the number of followings was confirmed, and a significant main effects of the genotype was found on the number of followings. (* p < 0.025).

OXTR/rs53576

Number of followings

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs53576 1 5.50 5.505 6.193 0.0161 *
Anxiety 1 0.27 0.266 0.299 0.5870
Avoidance 1 1.72 1.717 1.932 0.1706
OXTR/rs53576: Anxiety 1 2.37 2.369 2.665 0.1088
OXTR/rs53576: Avoidance 1 0.81 0.809 0.910 0.3446
Residuals 51 45.33 0.889

Number of followers

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs53576 1 0.095 0.0948 0.886 0.3515
Gender 1 0.712 0.7118 6.652 0.0132 *
Anxiety 1 0.132 0.1317 1.231 0.2731
Avoidance 1 0.020 0.0197 0.184 0.6699
OXTR/rs53576: Gender 1 0.049 0.0485 0.453 0.5042
OXTR/rs53576: Anxiety 1 0.166 0.1661 1.552 0.2192
OXTR/rs53576: Avoidance 1 0.002 0.0020 0.019 0.8921
Gender: Anxiety 1 0.008 0.0077 0.072 0.7892
Gender: Avoidance 1 0.008 0.0085 0.079 0.7795
OXTR/rs53576: Gender: Anxiety 1 0.003 0.0032 0.029 0.8644
OXTR/rs53576: Gender: Avoidance 1 0.018 0.0182 0.170 0.6821
Residuals 45 4.816 0.1070

OXTR/rs2254298

Number of followings

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs2254298 1 0.01 0.0138 0.014 0.908
Anxiety 1 0.61 0.6106 0.603 0.441
Avoidance 1 2.58 2.5842 2.553 0.116
OXTR/rs2254298: Anxiety 1 0.61 0.6118 0.604 0.440
OXTR/rs2254298: Avoidance 1 0.56 0.5602 0.553 0.460
Residuals 51 51.62 1.0121

Number of followers

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs2254298 1 0.009 0.0088 0.082 0.7761
Gender 1 0.650 0.6500 6.035 0.0179 *
Anxiety 1 0.164 0.1637 1.520 0.2241
Avoidance 1 0.034 0.0344 0.319 0.5749
OXTR/rs2254298: Gender 1 0.140 0.1400 1.300 0.2603
OXTR/rs2254298: Anxiety 1 0.004 0.0036 0.033 0.8566
OXTR/rs2254298: Avoidance 1 0.017 0.0167 0.155 0.6953
Gender: Anxiety 1 0.025 0.0253 0.235 0.6305
Gender: Avoidance 1 0.003 0.0033 0.031 0.8608
OXTR/rs2254298: Gender: Anxiety 1 0.094 0.0941 0.874 0.3549
OXTR/rs2254298: Gender: Avoidance 1 0.041 0.0410 0.381 0.5403
Residuals 45 4.847 0.1077

To explore the direction of the ANCOVA effect, a further post-hoc two-tailed Student's t test was computed. The Student's t test confirmed that the number of Instagram followings was significantly different between the A/A vs G allele groups (t = 2.45, df = 55, p = 0.018, 95% CI [0.12, 1.16]; see Fig. 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Main Effect of OXTR/rs53576 on Instagram Number of Followings. (A) Effect of Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs53576 on the standardized Instagram number of followings. Green circles = A/A homozygotes; blue circles = G-carriers. Lines constitute the linear models for A/A homozygotes (green) and G-carriers (blue). (B) Comparison between the number of Instagram followings in A/A homozygotes (green) and G-carriers (blue) (* p < 0.025).

Besides the effect of gender on the number of followers, no other significant main effect of OXTR/rs53576 or interaction effect between the ECR-R dimensions, gender and OXTR/rs53576 were found on Instagram number of followers (p > 0.025), as reported in Table 6.

With regards to OXTR/rs2254298, main effect of gender on the number of followers apart, no other significant main effect of genotype or interaction effect between the ECR-R subscales, gender and genotype was identified on Instagram number of followings or followers, as reported in Table 6.

3.2. Results obtained excluding outliers

3.2.1. Preliminary analysis on the genetic variables

When excluding the outlier values, the genetic distributions varied in relation to the Instagram variable that was considered for the analysis. For OXTR/rs53576, the frequencies of genotype were: A/A = 20 (36.36%), A/G = 30 (54.55%), G/G = 5 (9.09%) for the number of posts (Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium: X2 (1) = 1.754, p = 0.185, ns).; A/A = 22 (40.00%), A/G = 28 (50.91%), G/G = 5 (9.09%) for SDI (Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium: X2 (1) = 0.869, p = 0.351, ns); A/A = 22 (39.28%), A/G = 29 (51.79%), G/G = 5 (8.93%) for the number of followers (Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium: X2 (1) = 1.111, p = 0.292, ns); A/A = 20 (37.04%), A/G = 29 (53.70%), G/G = 5 (9.26%) for the number of followings (Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium: X2 (1) = 1.450, 0.228, ns). For OXTR/rs53576, the frequencies of genotype were: A/A = 3 (5.46%), G/A = 21 (38.18%), G/G = 31 (56.36%) for the number of posts (Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium: X2 (1) = 0.052, p = 0.819, ns); A/A = 4 (7.27%), G/A = 21 (38.18%), G/G = 30 (54.55%) for SDI (Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium: X2 (1) = 0.015, p = 0.902, ns); A/A = 4 (7.14%), G/A = 21 (37.50%), G/G = 31 (55.36%) for the number of followers (Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium: X2 (1) = 0.029, p = 0.864, ns); A/A = 4 (7.41%), G/A = 21 (38.89%), G/G = 29 (53.70%) for the number of followings (Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium: X2 (1) = 0.005, p = 0.941, ns).

3.2.2. Preliminary analysis on ECR-R and Instagram variables

Considering the large amount of outliers that emerged from the previous analysis, such values were therefore removed in relation to the dependent variable of interest. In particular, when considering the number of posts in subsequent analysis, 2 participants were removed for they represented extreme values. The same number of participants was removed when considering the SDI as the dependent variable. Conversely, 3 and 1 participants were removed when considering the number of followings and followers respectively as dependent variables. Table 7 reports skewness, kurtosis, and descriptive statistics for the continuous variables of the study.

Table 7.

Summary of the descriptive statistics for each continuous variable after removing the outliers (N = 2 for Instagram posts; N = 2 for Social Desirability Index; N = 3 for Instagram followings; N = 1 for Instagram followers) from the data pool. The distribution of each ECR-R (Experience in Close Relationships-Revised) dimension and Instagram variable is described in terms of Skewness, Kurtosis, Minimum (Min), first Quartile (1st Q), Median, Mean, third Quartile (3rd Q), Maximum (Max) and Standard Deviation (SD). Instagram variables were standardized using z-score transformation and inspected for normality. Skewness and kurtosis values were computed for each continuous subscale of the ECR-R questionnaire and each Instagram variable. Of all variables, only participants' number of posts did not follow the Gaussian distribution, and was thus adjusted by log-transformation. Log-transformed number of posts shows increased values compared to the original sampling of the same variable.

Variable Skewness Kurtosis Min 1st Q Median Mean 3rd Q Max SD
Anxiety -0.38 -0.02 1.39 3.33 3.94 3.94 4.56 6.22 1.08
Avoidance -0.15 -0.39 1.06 2.11 2.94 2.95 3.67 5.39 0.93
Posts number 1.53 1.64 -0.60 -0.52 -0.39 -0.16 0.01 1.36 0.50
Log-transformed posts number 1.14 0.33 0.33 0.39 0.48 0.58 0.70 1.21 0.24
Social Desirability Index -0.69 2.45 -1.16 -0.28 -0.13 -0.16 -0.01 0.61 0.30
Followings 0.34 -0.74 -1.53 -0.79 -0.14 -0.12 0.57 1.78 0.89
Followers 0.77 0.11 -0.80 -0.51 -0.17 -0.11 0.22 1.45 0.51

Again, four preliminary two-tailed Student's t-tests (corrected α=0.0125) were computed to examine the existence of a gender effect on the Instagram variables. No significant difference between males and females participants' scores was detected for any of the Instagram variables (number of posts: t = 0.68, df = 53, p = 0.498, 95% CI [-0.09, 0.19]; SDI: t = 0.90, df = 55, p = 0.374, 95% CI [-0.10, 0.26]; number of followings: t = 2.53, df = 52, p = 0.015, 95% CI [0.13, 1.15]; number of followers: t = 1.84, df = 54, p = 0.071, 95% CI [-0.02, 0.58]). Thus, participants' gender was not included in the subsequent analysis as a moderator variable.

3.2.3. Hypothesis-driven analysis: Instagram number of posts and SDI

Means and standard error means of Instagram number of posts and SDI for OXTR/rs53576 and OXTR/rs2254298 are reported in Table 8.

Table 8.

Mean values in Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs53576 A/A homozygotes and G-carriers and Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs2254298 G/G homozygotes and A-carriers divided by Experience in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) dimensions (high or low) on Instagram number of posts and Social Desirability Index, after removing the outlier values. Standard error means are reported between parentheses. Instagram behavior is reported by users' OXTR/rs53576 phenotype and ECR-R dimensions, consistently with the literature on the topic and a previous publication [60].

OXTR/rs53576

Number of posts

ECR-R dimension Low/AA Low/G High/AA High/G
Anxiety 0.66 (0.07) 0.54 (0.05) 0.54 (0.07) 0.58 (0.07)
Avoidance 0.71 (0.08) 0.61 (0.06) 0.49 (0.04) 0.51 (0.06)

Social desirability index

ECR-R dimension Low/AA Low/G High/AA High/G
Anxiety -0.20 (0.05) -0.04 (0.10) -0.13 (0.04) -0.26 (0.08)
Avoidance -0.13 (0.04) -0.12 (0.10) -0.20 (0.04) -0.20 (0.09)

OXTR/rs2254298

Number of posts

ECR-R dimension Low/GG Low/A High/GG High/A
Anxiety 0.60 (0.06) 0.56 (0.07) 0.59 (0.07) 0.54 (0.07)
Avoidance 0.67 (0.06) 0.61 (0.06) 0.52 (0.05) 0.49 (0.07)

Social desirability index

ECR-R dimension Low/GG Low/A High/GG High/A
Anxiety -0.14 (0.08) -0.06 (0.10) -0.22 (0.08) -0.21 (0.06)
Avoidance -0.14 (0.06) -0.11 (0.11) -0.23 (0.10) -0.16 (0.04)

As in the statistical plan of the study, Equation (1) was employed to test the existence of potential gene*environment interactions on Instagram number of posts and SDI. These hypothesis-driven analyses did not result in any main effect on the Instagram number of posts or SDI. Moreover, no interaction effect emerged between the SNP of interest (either OXTR/rs53576 or OXTR/rs2254298) and the two attachment dimensions (see Table 9).

Table 9.

Hypothesis-driven analysis: statistics' summary of ANCOVAs on Instagram number of posts and Social Desirability Index. For each Instagram variable considered in the hypothesis (Instagram number of posts and SDI) as dependent variable, one mixed ANCOVA was performed with OXTR gene genotype rs53576 (A/A and G-carriers) as a between-subject factor and the ECR-R dimensions (Anxiety and Avoidance) as continuous covariates (see the top section of the Table). For each Instagram variable, one mixed ANCOVA was also performed with the OXTR gene genotype rs2254298 (G/G and A-carriers) as a between-subject factor and the other parameters unchanged (see the below section of the Table).

OXTR/rs53576

Number of posts

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs53576 1 0.0229 0.02292 0.429 0.5156
Anxiety 1 0.0131 0.01314 0.246 0.6223
Avoidance 1 0.1682 0.16820 3.147 0.0823
OXTR/rs53576: Anxiety 1 0.2006 0.20064 3.754 0.0584
OXTR/rs53576: Avoidance 1 0.0075 0.00749 0.140 0.7097
Residuals 49 2.6187 0.05344

Social desirability index

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs53576 1 0.000 0.00027 0.003 0.957
Anxiety 1 0.163 0.16284 1.772 0.189
Avoidance 1 0.012 0.01170 0.127 0.723
OXTR/rs53576: Anxiety 1 0.108 0.10788 1.174 0.284
OXTR/rs53576: Avoidance 1 0.000 0.00001 0.000 0.993
Residuals 49 4.504 0.09192

OXTR/rs2254298

Number of posts

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs2254298 1 0.0284 0.02835 0.500 0.4828
Anxiety 1 0.0080 0.00804 0.142 0.7082
Avoidance 1 0.1834 0.18339 3.235 0.0782
OXTR/rs2254298: Anxiety 1 0.0116 0.01157 0.204 0.6534
OXTR/rs2254298: Avoidance 1 0.0221 0.02208 0.390 0.5354
Residuals 49 2.7777 0.05669

Social desirability index

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs2254298 1 0.024 0.02375 0.261 0.612
Anxiety 1 0.155 0.15462 1.702 0.198
Avoidance 1 0.009 0.00856 0.094 0.760
OXTR/rs2254298: Anxiety 1 0.033 0.03327 0.366 0.548
OXTR/rs2254298: Avoidance 1 0.114 0.11382 1.253 0.269
Residuals 49 4.453 0.09087

3.2.4. Exploratory analysis: Instagram number of followings and followers

Means and standard error means of Instagram number of followings and followers for OXTR/rs53576 and OXTR/rs2254298 are reported in Table 10.

Table 10.

Mean values in Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs53576 A/A homozygotes and G-carriers and Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs2254298 G/G homozygotes and A-carriers divided by Experience in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) dimensions (high or low) on Instagram number of followings and followers, after removing the outlier values. Standard error means are indicated between parentheses. Instagram behavior is reported by users' OXTR/rs2254298 phenotype and ECR-R dimensions, consistently with the literature on the topic and a previous publication [60].

OXTR/rs53576

Number of followings

ECR-R dimension Low/AA Low/G High/AA High/G
Anxiety 0.53 (0.30) -0.23 (0.19) -0.08 (0.34) -0.42 (0.15)
Avoidance 0.54 (0.31) -0.20 (0.20) -0.25 (0.28) -0.44 (0.14)

Number of followers

ECR-R dimension Low/AA Low/G High/AA High/G
Anxiety 0.18 (0.14) -0.16 (0.12) -0.14 (0.16) -0.26 (0.12)
Avoidance 0.13 (0.15) -0.07 (0.14) -0.07 (0.16) -0.35 (0.07)

OXTR/rs2254298

Number of followings

ECR-R dimension Low/GG Low/A High/GG High/A
Anxiety -0.10 (0.23) 0.23 (0.27) -0.40 (0.22) -0.18 (0.23)
Avoidance -0.07 (0.23) 0.37 (0.31) -0.46 (0.21) -0.31 (0.16

Number of followers

ECR-R dimension Low/GG Low/A High/GG High/A
Anxiety -0.05 (0.13) 0.04 (0.14) -0.20 (0.16) -0.24 (0.10)
Avoidance -0.03 (0.15) 0.08 (0.15) -0.23 (0.14) -0.28 (0.07)

From the performed ANCOVAs, a main effect of the OXTR/rs53576 on the number of followings on Instagram was observed (F(1, 53) = 5.78, p = 0.020, pη2 = 0.11). No other main effect of the two SNPs or ECR-R dimensions, and neither for their interplay, was observed on the Instagram variables (see Table 11).

Table 11.

Exploratory analysis: statistics' summary of ANCOVAs on Instagram number of followings and number of followers. For each Instagram variable considered in the exploratory analysis (Instagram number of followings and followers) as dependent variable (DV), one mixed ANCOVA was performed with OXTR gene genotype rs53576 (A/A and G-carriers) as a between-subject factor and the ECR-R dimensions (Anxiety and Avoidance) as continuous covariates, as in Equation (1) (see the top section of the Table). For each Instagram variable, one mixed ANCOVA was also performed with the OXTR gene genotype rs2254298 (G/G and A-carriers) as a between-subject factor and the other parameters unchanged (see the below section of the Table). A significant main effects of the genotype was found on the number of followings for OXTR/rs53576. (* p < 0.025).

OXTR/rs53576

Number of followings

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs53576 1 3.83 3.831 5.776 0.0202 *
Anxiety 1 1.42 1.416 2.135 0.1505
Avoidance 1 2.40 2.398 3.617 0.0632
OXTR/rs53576: Anxiety 1 1.03 1.034 1.560 0.2178
OXTR/rs53576: Avoidance 1 0.13 0.130 0.197 0.6595
Residuals 48 31.83 0.663

Number of followers

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs53576 1 0.835 0.8346 3.378 0.072
Anxiety 1 0.398 0.3981 1.611 0.210
Avoidance 1 0.398 0.3979 1.610 0.210
OXTR/rs53576: Anxiety 1 0.168 0.1680 0.680 0.414
OXTR/rs53576: Avoidance 1 0.139 0.1395 0.565 0.456
Residuals 50 12.354 0.2471

OXTR/rs2254298

Number of followings

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs2254298 1 0.91 0.906 1.273 0.2649
Anxiety 1 1.78 1.784 2.506 0.1200
Avoidance 1 3.51 3.507 4.928 0.0312
OXTR/rs2254298: Anxiety 1 0.05 0.050 0.070 0.7920
OXTR/rs2254298: Avoidance 1 0.23 0.234 0.329 0.5692
Residuals 48 34.16 0.712

Number of followers

Variable DF Sum square Mean square F value p-value

OXTR/rs2254298 1 0.003 0.0031 0.012 0.914
Anxiety 1 0.537 0.5369 2.040 0.159
Avoidance 1 0.534 0.5336 2.028 0.161
OXTR/rs2254298: Anxiety 1 0.036 0.0362 0.138 0.712
OXTR/rs2254298: Avoidance 1 0.026 0.0258 0.098 0.755
Residuals 50 13.156 0.2631

As before, the direction of the observed effect was studied with a post-hoc two-tailed Student's t test. In particular, a significant difference between allelic groups (A/A vs. G alleles) on the number of followings was confirmed (t = 2.33, df = 52, p = 0.024, 95% CI [0.08, 1.03]; see Fig. 4).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Main Effect of OXTR/rs53576 on Instagram Number of Followings. (A) Effect of Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) rs53576 on the standardized Instagram number of followings. Green circles = A/A homozygotes; blue circles = G-carriers. Lines constitute the linear models for A/A homozygotes (green) and G-carriers (blue). (B) Comparison between the number of Instagram followings in A/A homozygotes (green) and G-carriers (blue) (* p < 0.025).

4. Discussion

For the more rigorous analyses applied, the discussion will focus on the results obtained excluding outliers. Two gene*environment interactions on the number of posts and SDI (OXTR/rs53576 SNP * close relationship in adulthood; OXTR/rs2254298 SNP * close relationship in adulthood) were initially hypothesized. Contrary to these predictions (see the section “Limitations” for an extensive overview on the expected patterns and the obtained results), only a main effect of gene OXTR/rs53576 on Instagram number of followings was found. Post-hoc tests revealed differential Instagram behavior between the two genetic groups within the OXTR/rs53576. Independent of the reported scores on anxiety and avoidance in adult relationships, A/A homozygotes showed a higher number of followings compared to G-carriers. Previous researches highlighted that OXTR/rs53576 G allele is linked to higher marital satisfaction, support seeking in hostile circumstances, and higher empathy than A-carriers [55], [96]. Conversely, in a few cases, A/A individuals were found to display increased social attitudes and higher social sensitivity than G-carriers [97]. On the other hand, the G allele of the OXTR/rs2254298 has been associated with separation anxiety, depression, decreased online sociability and lower reciprocity in a support-giving interaction [56], [61], [98]. Overall, the role played by the A and G alleles on the OXTR SNPs towards general social behavior is debated [53]. This debate will be clearer if the genetic correlates of social media behavior were investigated.

In connection to the findings of the study, distinct biological roots could have differently shaped online behavior among genetic groups (i.e. OXTR/rs53576 A/A versus G-carriers), specifically the level of online sociability on SNSs, like Instagram. A high number of followings could represent the desire of Instagram's user to be involved in social interactions [61], [99]. Specifically, A/A homozygotes could exhibit a genetically-founded inclination to online sociability witnessed by a higher frequency of online behaviors (i.e. Instagram number of followings) than G-carriers. As a protective factor [100], [101], A allele could predispose the user to be less anxious in online prosocial activities and proactively seek contact with other Instagram users. In contrast, G allele could predispose the user to be less interested or less motivated to search for others on Instagram. Given the contradictory results from the literature on the alleles of OXTR/rs53576 [102], [103], [104], it is open to question which allele is the bearer of a detrimental effect. However, in light of the Differential Susceptibility model [105], we assumed that the genetic properties related to a given allele could not wholly determine online sociability only in terms of the “positive” (protective factor) versus “negative” (risk factor) predisposition [106]. Rather, every individual could exhibit a range of virtual social behaviors as a function of the interaction between genetic sensitivity to experiences, and environmental influences [107], [108]. Within this framework, the current results suggest that no interaction between the quality of close adult relationships and the genetic predispositions for OXTR/rs53576 or OXTR/rs2254298 could explain Instagram sociability under a gene*environment perspective. This outcome finds agreement with previous research which did not identify an association between the OXTR/rs53576 and close relationship main dimensions [109], [110]. In contrast to the present results, a previous work [60] revealed that the interaction between OXTR/rs2254298 and the early parental bonding could modulate the Instagram number of posts and SDI. Nevertheless, the authors did not detect any gene*environment effect for OXTR/rs53576 on Instagram sociability. In conclusion, multiple environmental components (i.e. quality of relationships with people) and genetic factors (i.e., OXTR/rs53576 or OXTR/rs2254298) could differently modulate users' online prosociality and frequency of virtual interactions (i.e. high number of followings). Since no interaction effect between genotype and close relationships was found, here, OXTR/rs53576 could shape the level of Instagram sociability independent of the potential influence of the close adult relationship with one's partner. The current findings could inspire future research in exploring online sociability under a gene-environment perspective.

5. Limitations

5.1. A genetic overview

One specific hypothesis was formulated a-priori: OXTR/rs224529 A-carriers and OXTR/rs53576 G-carriers Instagram users would show a differential number of posts and values of SDI in relation to the quality of relationship with the partner (a higher number of posts and values of SDI when low scores in anxiety and avoidance are reported; a lower number of posts and values of SDI when high scores in anxiety and avoidance are reported) compared to OXTR/rs53576 A/A homozygotes and OXTR/rs224529 G/G homozygotes users. To verify our hypothesis, multiple mixed ANCOVAs have been employed. The existence of an interaction effect with the predicted directionality would have confirmed our hypotheses. Contrary to our expectations, no significant interaction effects were found. In addition to the ANCOVAs employed to verify our hypothesis, exploratory analyses were performed. Contrary to the results of our hypothesis-driven tests, the exploratory analysis revealed a main effect of the gene OXTR/rs53576 on Instagram number of followings. Within the OXTR/rs53576, post-hoc tests confirmed a significant difference of Instagram behavior between A/A and G-carriers. Independent of the anxiety and avoidance felt towards the partner, A/A homozygotes showed a higher number of followings compared to G-carriers. At a genetic level, the discrepancy between the expected patterns and the obtained results requires some considerations. For instance, an absolute consensus on the predisposition given by OXTR/rs53576 A or G allele does not exist [111] and the culture-dependent variation of allelic distributions should not be ignored [112]. Nonetheless, no interaction effect was detected between genes and close relationship scores. This finding is theoretically valid and confirmed by previous researches which highlighted no associations between OXTR and adult attachment [113], [114], [115]. However, a potential issue in this study could be traced back to the limited sample size, a condition that is likely to determine that power values were not sufficient to obtain the hypothesized effect. Moreover, no results were found for the region OXTR/rs2254298, which was previously found to be highly involved in general sociability [116]. Overall, the present results cannot unequivocally define the role played by OXTR/rs53576 and OXTR/rs224529 on social media behavior.

5.2. Methodological limitations

Alongside the aforementioned genetic considerations, other methodological limitations need to be discussed. Firstly, the sample size was small (N = 57) with a substantial prevalence of female participants. Secondly, Instagram data were not adjusted by the participants' time of membership. Although Instagram data were extracted at the same time-point, the current study does not take into account the year of the first registration for each Instagram user or the evolution of the online social activity across time (e.g., in terms of short-term daily usage and long-term increased number of posts, followings and followers). While this would have been an interesting variable to include in our analysis, this information is not publicly available, and therefore not acquirable with our method. Obtaining access to this information could allow to correct the number of posts, followers, and followings by the amount of time passed between the date of creation of an account and the instant in which data were extracted. Future works should consider obtaining this information in order to obtain a more standardized set of variables. Thirdly, the obtained results cannot be generalized to online sociability which exist outside Instagram. The activity of alternative social network sites like Facebook and Twitter should be monitored to discover similar or dissimilar patterns of online socialization. Moreover, the current investigation did not collect Instagram data from the participants' partners. Being aware of Instagram attitudes on both participant and the partner would allow for comparing the quality in the intimate relationship and the frequency of social media behavior in terms of posts, followings, followers and SDI. Participants were not asked to report elements on the quality of their relationship with Instagram followings. Furthermore, participants were not questioned to indicate how many followings and followers they know and how often they meet them in daily life. Finally, important information may be provided by dispositional characteristics such as personality traits and “real-life” social environments that cannot be accounted by the partnership. It is worth noting that the same housing conditions of the participants and their availability of technological devices could alter their access to Instagram.

6. Conclusions

Overall, the present results should be interpreted with a great degree of caution in the panorama of genetic association studies [117], [118], [119]. Data interpretation is even more challenging in the presence of a genetic influence independent of the environmental exposure [120], [121] on an underexplored social variable such as Instagram number of followings. Although there is no strong evidence of gene-environment interactions on online sociability, the existing literature on general or in-person sociability or emotional support reveals that the allelic distribution varies across ethnicities and multiple factors could determine the role that A or G alleles play in the individuals' lives [122]. For instance, several studies tested the socio-emotional behavior and distress explained by variations in OXTR/rs53576 [123], [124], [125] or by its potential interaction with attachment [110] on a collectivist culture sample. The same differential social responsibility towards other Instagram users between A/A homozygotes and G-carriers could also depend on baseline differences, such as personality traits, ethnicity, vulnerability to psychiatric disorders [126]. Altogether, these factors could similarly have implicitly modulated the online sociability of the participants. A provocative question will be if OXTR/rs53576 A and G allele maintain the same influence on physical (i.e. in-person social interaction) and virtual environments (i.e. online sociability on Instagram).

Future studies should measure such factors and control their impact on Instagram behavior to control potential biases. Moreover, future works should increase the sample size and explore close relationships through alternative questionnaires, such as self-reported measures or observational techniques (e.g. using the Adult Attachment Interview [127]). Finally, consider the different genetic regions involved in the modulation of other neurotransmitters (e.g. 5-HTTLPR and DRD4) as potential factors of predisposition.

These preliminary and exploratory results are shared with the scientific community to refine future research in the field.

Declarations

Author contribution statement

Alessandro Carollo, Andrea Bonassi: Analyzed and interpreted the data; Contributed analysis tools or data; Wrote the paper.

Ilaria Cataldo: Conceived and designed the experiments; Performed the experiments; Wrote the paper.

Giulio Gabrieli: Analyzed and interpreted the data; Wrote the paper.

Moses Tandiono, Jia Nee Foo: Contributed reagents, materials, analysis tools or data.

Bruno Lepri, Gianluca Esposito: Conceived and designed the experiments; Wrote the paper.

Funding statement

This research was supported by grants from the NAP SUG (M4081597, 2015-2021) and the Ministry of Education, Singapore, under its Academic Research Fund Tier 1 (RG55/18) to G.E.

Data availability statement

Data associated with this study has been deposited at NTU's Data repository (DR NTU Data) under the url: https://doi.org/10.21979/N9/GUSTLQ.

Declaration of interests statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Additional information

No additional information is available for this paper.

Acknowledgements

All participants in this study are gratefully acknowledged. We would also like to acknowledge An An Lieu, Lim Mengyu and Jan Paolo Macapinlac Balagtas (Nanyang Technological University, Singapore) for editing the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Bielsky I.F., Young L.J. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and social recognition in mammals. Peptides. 2004;25:1565–1574. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2004.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Domes G., Heinrichs M., Michel A., Berger C., Herpertz S.C. Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;61:731–733. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Heinrichs M., Domes G. Neuropeptides and social behaviour: effects of oxytocin and vasopressin in humans. Prog. Brain Res. 2008;170:337–350. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00428-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.MacDonald K., MacDonald T.M. The peptide that binds: a systematic review of oxytocin and its prosocial effects in humans. Harv. Rev. Psychiatr. 2010;18:1–21. doi: 10.3109/10673220903523615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Wermter A.-K., Kamp-Becker I., Hesse P., Schulte-Körne G., Strauch K., Remschmidt H. Evidence for the involvement of genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in the etiology of autistic disorders on high-functioning level. Am. J. Med. Genet., Part B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2010;153:629–639. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jurek B., Neumann I.D. The oxytocin receptor: from intracellular signaling to behavior. Physiol. Rev. 2018;98:1805–1908. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00031.2017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Grinevich V., Neumann I.D. Brain oxytocin: how puzzle stones from animal studies translate into psychiatry. Mol. Psychiatry. 2020:1–15. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0802-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2007;48:329–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Feldman R. Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Horm. Behav. 2012;61:380–391. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.L.J. Young, Oxytocin, social cognition and psychiatry, 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 11.Young L.J. Oxytocin and vasopressin receptors and species-typical social behaviors. Horm. Behav. 1999 doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Theodosis D.T., Poulain D.A. Maternity leads to morphological synaptic plasticity in the oxytocin system. Prog. Brain Res. 2001;133:49–58. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(01)33004-2. Elsevier. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Carter C.S. Developmental consequences of oxytocin. Physiol. Behav. 2003;79:383–397. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00151-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Veening J., De Jong T., Waldinger M., Korte S., Olivier B. The role of oxytocin in male and female reproductive behavior. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2015;753:209–228. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.07.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Heinrichs M., Baumgartner T., Kirschbaum C., Ehlert U. Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biol. Psychiatry. 2003;54:1389–1398. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00465-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ellison P.T., Gray P.B. Harvard University Press; 2009. Endocrinology of Social Relationships. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Chen F.S., Barth M., Johnson S.L., Gotlib I.H., Johnson S.C. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) polymorphisms and attachment in human infants. Front. Psychol. 2011;2:200. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Feldman R., Zagoory-Sharon O., Weisman O., Schneiderman I., Gordon I., Maoz R., Shalev I., Ebstein R.P. Sensitive parenting is associated with plasma oxytocin and polymorphisms in the OXTR and CD38 genes. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;72:175–181. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Walum H., Lichtenstein P., Neiderhiser J.M., Reiss D., Ganiban J.M., Spotts E.L., Pedersen N.L., Anckarsäter H., Larsson H., Westberg L. Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with pair-bonding and social behavior. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;71:419–426. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Pearce E., Wlodarski R., Machin A., Dunbar R.I. The influence of genetic variation on social disposition, romantic relationships and social networks: a replication study. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 2018;4:400–422. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0101-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Julian M.M., King A.P., Bocknek E.L., Mantha B., Beeghly M., Rosenblum K.L., Muzik M. Associations between oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphisms, childhood trauma, and parenting behavior. Dev. Psychol. 2019 doi: 10.1037/dev0000783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bakermans-Kranenburg M.J., van IJzendoorn M.H. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) and serotonin transporter (5-HTT) genes associated with observed parenting. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2008;3:128–134. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Rilling J.K., Young L.J. The biology of mammalian parenting and its effect on offspring social development. Science. 2014;345:771–776. doi: 10.1126/science.1252723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kemp A.H., Guastella A.J. The role of oxytocin in human affect: a novel hypothesis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2011;20:222–231. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tabak B.A. Oxytocin and social salience: a call for gene-environment interaction research. Front. Neurosci. 2013;7:199. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Striepens N., Scheele D., Kendrick K.M., Becker B., Schäfer L., Schwalba K., Reul J., Maier W., Hurlemann R. Oxytocin facilitates protective responses to aversive social stimuli in males. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2012;109:18144–18149. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208852109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Wittfoth-Schardt D., Gründing J., Wittfoth M., Lanfermann H., Heinrichs M., Domes G., Buchheim A., Gündel H., Waller C. Oxytocin modulates neural reactivity to children's faces as a function of social salience. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1799–1807. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Belsky J. Variation in susceptibility to environmental influence: an evolutionary argument. Psychol. Inq. 1997;8:182–186. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Belsky J. Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development. 2005. Differential susceptibility to rearing influence; pp. 139–163. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Monroe S.M., Simons A.D. Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychol. Bull. 1991;110:406. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.M. Zuckerman, J.H. Riskind, Vulnerability to psychopathology: a biosocial model, 2000.
  • 32.Esposito G., Azhari A., Borelli J.L. Gene × environment interaction in developmental disorders: where do we stand and what's next? Front. Psychol. 2018;9:2036. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Truzzi A., Bornstein M.H., Senese V.P., Shinohara K., Setoh P., Esposito G. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms and early parent-infant interactions are related to adult male heart rate response to female crying. Front. Physiol. 2017;8:111. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2017.00111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kumsta R., Heinrichs M. Oxytocin, stress and social behavior: neurogenetics of the human oxytocin system. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013;23:11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cataldo I., Azhari A., Lepri B., Esposito G. Oxytocin receptors (OXTR) and early parental care: an interaction that modulates psychiatric disorders. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018;82:27–38. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Krueger F., Parasuraman R., Iyengar V., Thornburg M., Weel J., Lin M., Clarke E., McCabe K., Lipsky R. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation promotes human trust behavior. Front. Human Neurosci. 2012;6:4. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lee S.-J., Chae Y.-G. Children's internet use in a family context: influence on family relationships and parental mediation. CyberPsychol. Behav. 2007;10:640–644. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.9975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Yaakobi E., Goldenberg J. Social relationships and information dissemination in virtual social network systems: an attachment theory perspective. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2014;38:127–135. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Blackwell D., Leaman C., Tramposch R., Osborne C., Liss M. Extraversion, neuroticism, attachment style and fear of missing out as predictors of social media use and addiction. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2017;116:69–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Flynn S., Noone C., Sarma K.M. An exploration of the link between adult attachment and problematic Facebook use. BMC Psychol. 2018;6:34. doi: 10.1186/s40359-018-0245-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sampasa-Kanyinga H., Goldfield G.S., Kingsbury M., Clayborne Z., Colman I. Social media use and parent–child relationship: a cross-sectional study of adolescents. J. Community Psychol. 2020;48:793–803. doi: 10.1002/jcop.22293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Griffin D.W., Bartholomew K. Models of the self and other: fundamental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1994;67:430. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Feeney J.A. Attachment, caregiving, and marital satisfaction. Pers. Relatsh. 1996;3:401–416. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hamilton C.E. Continuity and discontinuity of attachment from infancy through adolescence. Child Dev. 2000;71:690–694. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.I. Cataldo, A. Bonassi, B. Lepri, J.N. Foo, P. Setoh, G. Esposito, Recalled parental bonding interacts with oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism in modulating anxiety and avoidance in adult relationships, bioRxiv, 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 46.Jorgensen-Wells M.A., James S.L., Holmes E.K. Attachment development in adolescent romantic relationships: a conceptual model. J. Fam. Theory Rev. 2021;13:128–142. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Coombs R.H. Marital status and personal well-being: a literature review. Fam. Relat. 1991:97–102. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Cotten S.R. Marital status and mental health revisited: examining the importance of risk factors and resources. Fam. Relat. 1999:225–233. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Simon R.W. Revisiting the relationships among gender, marital status, and mental health. Am. J. Sociol. 2002;107:1065–1096. doi: 10.1086/339225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Holt-Lunstad J., Birmingham W., Jones B.Q. Is there something unique about marriage? The relative impact of marital status, relationship quality, and network social support on ambulatory blood pressure and mental health. Ann. Behav. Med. 2008;35:239–244. doi: 10.1007/s12160-008-9018-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Braithwaite S.R., Delevi R., Fincham F.D. Romantic relationships and the physical and mental health of college students. Pers. Relatsh. 2010;17:1–12. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Santini Z.I., Koyanagi A., Tyrovolas S., Haro J.M. The association of relationship quality and social networks with depression, anxiety, and suicidal ideation among older married adults: findings from a cross-sectional analysis of the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA) J. Affect. Disord. 2015;179:134–141. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kim H.S., Sherman D.K., Sasaki J.Y., Xu J., Chu T.Q., Ryu C., Suh E.M., Graham K., Taylor S.E. Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2010;107:15717–15721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010830107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.McQuaid R.J., McInnis O.A., Matheson K., Anisman H. Distress of ostracism: oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism confers sensitivity to social exclusion. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2015;10:1153–1159. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Monin J.K., Goktas S.O., Kershaw T., DeWan A. Associations between spouses' oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism, attachment security, and marital satisfaction. PLoS ONE. 2019;14 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Costa B., Pini S., Gabelloni P., Abelli M., Lari L., Cardini A., Muti M., Gesi C., Landi S., Galderisi S. Oxytocin receptor polymorphisms and adult attachment style in patients with depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:1506–1514. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Chen F.S., Johnson S.C. An oxytocin receptor gene variant predicts attachment anxiety in females and autism-spectrum traits in males. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2012;3:93–99. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Thompson R.J., Parker K.J., Hallmayer J.F., Waugh C.E., Gotlib I.H. Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (rs2254298) interacts with familial risk for psychopathology to predict symptoms of depression and anxiety in adolescent girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:144–147. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Esposito G., Truzzi A., Setoh P., Putnick D.L., Shinohara K., Bornstein M.H. Genetic predispositions and parental bonding interact to shape adults' physiological responses to social distress. Behav. Brain Res. 2017;325:156–162. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.06.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Bonassi A., Cataldo I., Gabrieli G., Foo J.N., Lepri B., Esposito G. Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms and early parental bonding interact in shaping Instagram social behavior. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:7232. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17197232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.A. Bonassi, I. Cataldo, G. Gabrieli, M. Tandiono, J.N. Foo, B. Lepri, G. Esposito, The interaction between serotonin transporter allelic variation and maternal care modulates sociability on Instagram, PsyArXiv, 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 62.A. Bonassi, A. Carollo, I. Cataldo, G. Gabrieli, M. Tandiono, J.N. Foo, B. Lepri, G. Esposito, Modulation of Instagram number of followings by avoidance in close relationships in young adults under a gene x environment perspective, 2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 63.Seabrook E.M., Kern M.L., Rickard N.S. Social networking sites, depression, and anxiety: a systematic review. JMIR Ment. Health. 2016;3:e50. doi: 10.2196/mental.5842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Kuss D.J., Griffiths M.D. Social networking sites and addiction: ten lessons learned. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14:311. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Ferrara E., Interdonato R., Tagarelli A. Proceedings of the 25th ACM Conference on Hypertext and Social Media. 2014. Online popularity and topical interests through the lens of Instagram; pp. 24–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Wong D., Amon K.L., Keep M. Desire to belong affects Instagram behavior and perceived social support. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2019;22:465–471. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2018.0533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Pittman M., Reich B. Social media and loneliness: why an Instagram picture may be worth more than a thousand twitter words. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016;62:155–167. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Meier A., Schäfer S. The positive side of social comparison on social network sites: how envy can drive inspiration on Instagram. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2018;21:411–417. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Rosenquist J.N., Fowler J.H., Christakis N.A. Social network determinants of depression. Mol. Psychiatry. 2011;16:273–281. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Lowe-Calverley E., Grieve R. Do the metrics matter? An experimental investigation of Instagram influencer effects on mood and body dissatisfaction. Body Image. 2020;36:1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Cataldo I., Lepri B., Neoh M.J.Y., Esposito G. Social media usage and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence: a review. Front. Psychiatry. 2020;11 doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.508595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Esbjørn B.H., Breinholst S., Niclasen J., Skovgaard L.F., Lange K., Reinholdt-Dunne M.L. Identifying the best-fitting factor structure of the experience of close relations-revised in a Scandinavian example. PLoS ONE. 2015;10 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Richardson L. Beautiful soup documentation, Dosegljivo. 2007. https://www.crummy.com/software/BeautifulSoup/bs4/doc/
  • 74.G. Gabrieli, A. Bonassi, A. Carollo, G. Esposito, Related data for: association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms and number of followed people on Instagram: an exploratory analysis, 2020.
  • 75.Bonassi A., Ghilardi T., Truzzi A., Cataldo I., Azhari A., Setoh P., Shinohara K., Esposito G. Dataset on genetic and physiological adults' responses to social distress. Data Brief. 2017;13:742–748. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2017.06.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Fraley R.C., Waller N.G., Brennan K.A. An item response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000;78:350. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.2.350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Sibley C.G., Fischer R., Liu J.H. Reliability and validity of the revised experiences in close relationships (ECR-R) self-report measure of adult romantic attachment. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2005;31:1524–1536. doi: 10.1177/0146167205276865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Cameron J.J., Finnegan H., Morry M.M. Orthogonal dreams in an oblique world: a meta-analysis of the association between attachment anxiety and avoidance. J. Res. Pers. 2012;46:472–476. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Busonera A., Martini P.S., Zavattini G.C., Santona A. Psychometric properties of an Italian version of the experiences in close relationships-revised (ECR-R) scale. Psychol. Rep. 2014;114:785–801. doi: 10.2466/03.21.PR0.114k23w9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Mikulincer M., Shaver P.R. Guilford Press; 2007. Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and Change. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Hocking E.C., Simons R.M., Surette R.J. Attachment style as a mediator between childhood maltreatment and the experience of betrayal trauma as an adult. Child Abuse Negl. 2016;52:94–101. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Nielsen S.K.K., Lønfeldt N., Wolitzky-Taylor K.B., Hageman I., Vangkilde S., Daniel S.I.F. Adult attachment style and anxiety—the mediating role of emotion regulation. J. Affect. Disord. 2017;218:253–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Baryshnikov I., Joffe G., Koivisto M., Melartin T., Aaltonen K., Suominen K., Rosenström T., Näätänen P., Karpov B., Heikkinen M. Relationships between self-reported childhood traumatic experiences, attachment style, neuroticism and features of borderline personality disorders in patients with mood disorders. J. Affect. Disord. 2017;210:82–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Donnellan M.B., Burt S.A., Levendosky A.A., Klump K.L. Genes, personality, and attachment in adults: a multivariate behavioral genetic analysis. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2008;34:3–16. doi: 10.1177/0146167207309199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Török I., Vincze G. The effect of genetic based temperament and environment factors on the development of attachment avoidance from evolutionary psychiatric aspect. Eur. Psychiatry. 2015;30:1. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Cataldo I., Neoh M.J.-Y., Chew W.F., Foo J.N., Lepri B., Esposito G. Oxytocin receptor gene and parental bonding modulate prefrontal responses to cries: a NIRS study. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-65582-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Picardi A., Vermigli P., Toni A., D'Amico R., Bitetti D., Pasquini P. Further evidence of the validity of the Italian version of the questionnaire “experiences in close relationships” (ECR), a self-report instrument to assess adult attachment. Ital. J. Psychopathol. 2002;8:282–294. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Moon J.H., Lee E., Lee J.-A., Choi T.R., Sung Y. The role of narcissism in self-promotion on Instagram. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2016;101:22–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Frison E., Eggermont S. Browsing, posting, and liking on Instagram: the reciprocal relationships between different types of Instagram use and adolescents' depressed mood. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2017;20:603–609. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Bowden-Green T., Hinds J., Joinson A. How is extraversion related to social media use? A literature review. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2020;164 [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Erceg-Hurn D.M., Mirosevich V.M. Modern robust statistical methods: an easy way to maximize the accuracy and power of your research. Am. Psychol. 2008;63:591. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Leys C., Delacre M., Mora Y.L., Lakens D., Ley C. How to classify, detect, and manage univariate and multivariate outliers, with emphasis on pre-registration. Int. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2019;32 [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Alexander N., Kuepper Y., Schmitz A., Osinsky R., Kozyra E., Hennig J. Gene–environment interactions predict cortisol responses after acute stress: implications for the etiology of depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:1294–1303. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Truzzi A., Poquérusse J., Setoh P., Shinohara K., Bornstein M.H., Esposito G. Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms (rs53576) and early paternal care sensitize males to distressing female vocalizations. Dev. Psychobiology. 2018;60:333–339. doi: 10.1002/dev.21606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Faul F., Erdfelder E., Buchner A., Lang A.-G. Statistical power analyses using g* power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods. 2009;41:1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Gong P., Fan H., Liu J., Yang X., Zhang K., Zhou X. Revisiting the impact of OXTR rs53576 on empathy: a population-based study and a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;80:131–136. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Zheng S., Masuda T., Matsunaga M., Noguchi Y., Ohtsubo Y., Yamasue H., Ishii K. Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and childhood adversity influence trust. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2020;121 doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Schneiderman I., Kanat-Maymon Y., Ebstein R.P., Feldman R. Cumulative risk on the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) underpins empathic communication difficulties at the first stages of romantic love. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014;9:1524–1529. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Sherlock M., Wagstaff D.L. Exploring the relationship between frequency of Instagram use, exposure to idealized images, and psychological well-being in women. Psychol. Pop. Media Cult. 2019;8:482. [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Laursen H.R., Siebner H.R., Haren T., Madsen K., Grønlund R., Hulme O., Henningsson S. Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with behavioral and neural correlates of empathic accuracy. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014;8:423. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Smearman E.L., Winiarski D.A., Brennan P.A., Najman J., Johnson K.C. Social stress and the oxytocin receptor gene interact to predict antisocial behavior in an at-risk cohort. Dev. Psychopathol. 2015;27:309. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Bethlehem R.A., Baron-Cohen S., van Honk J., Auyeung B., Bos P.A. The oxytocin paradox. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014;8:48. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Andreou D., Comasco E., Åslund C., Nilsson K.W., Hodgins S. Maltreatment, the oxytocin receptor gene, and conduct problems among male and female teenagers. Front. Human Neurosci. 2018;12:112. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Cataldo I., Azhari A., Esposito G. A review of oxytocin and arginine-vasopressin receptors and their modulation of autism spectrum disorder. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2018;11:27. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2018.00027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Belsky J., Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol. Bull. 2009;135:885. doi: 10.1037/a0017376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Roisman G.I., Newman D.A., Fraley R.C., Haltigan J.D., Groh A.M., Haydon K.C. Distinguishing differential susceptibility from diathesis–stress: recommendations for evaluating interaction effects. Dev. Psychopathol. 2012;24:389. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Belsky J., Bakermans-Kranenburg M.J., Van IJzendoorn M.H. For better and for worse: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007;16:300–304. [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Ellis B.J., Boyce W.T. Biological sensitivity to context. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2008;17:183–187. [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Li J., Zhao Y., Li R., Broster L.S., Zhou C., Yang S. Association of oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) rs53576 polymorphism with sociality: a meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2015;10 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Gillath O., Shaver P.R., Baek J.-M., Chun D.S. Genetic correlates of adult attachment style. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2008;34:1396–1405. doi: 10.1177/0146167208321484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.McQuaid R.J., McInnis O.A., Stead J.D., Matheson K., Anisman H. A paradoxical association of an oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism: early-life adversity and vulnerability to depression. Front. Neurosci. 2013;7:128. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Sasaki J., Kim H., Xu J. Religion and well-being: an analysis of an oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) and culture. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2011;42:1394–1405. [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Lee Raby K., Cicchetti D., Carlson E.A., Egeland B., Collins W. Andrew. Genetic contributions to continuity and change in attachment security: a prospective, longitudinal investigation from infancy to young adulthood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2013;54:1223–1230. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Pearce E., Wlodarski R., Machin A., Dunbar R.I. Variation in the β-endorphin, oxytocin, and dopamine receptor genes is associated with different dimensions of human sociality. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2017;114:5300–5305. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1700712114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Gong P., Wang Q., Liu J., Xi S., Yang X., Fang P., Wang B., He L., Guo W., Zhang M. The OXTR polymorphisms are not associated with attachment dimensions: a three-approach study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2020;120 doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Ebstein R.P., Knafo A., Mankuta D., Chew S.H., San Lai P. The contributions of oxytocin and vasopressin pathway genes to human behavior. Horm. Behav. 2012;61:359–379. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Munafò M.R. Reliability and replicability of genetic association studies. Addiction. 2009 doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02662.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Liu C.-y., Maity A., Lin X., Wright R.O., Christiani D.C. Design and analysis issues in gene and environment studies. Environ. Health. 2012;11:1–15. doi: 10.1186/1476-069X-11-93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Mitchell K.J. Neurogenomics-towards a more rigorous science. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2018;47:109–114. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Lee J.-H., Kim H.-T., Hyun D.-S. Possible association between serotonin transporter promoter region polymorphism and impulsivity in Koreans. Psychiatry Res. 2003;118:19–24. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(03)00065-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Bugental D.B., Grusec J.E. Handbook of Child Psychology, vol. 3. 2007. Socialization processes. [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Luo S., Han S. The association between an oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism and cultural orientations. Cult. Brain. 2014;2:89–107. [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Rodrigues S.M., Saslow L.R., Garcia N., John O.P., Keltner D. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106:21437–21441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909579106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Kawamura Y., Liu X., Akiyama T., Shimada T., Otowa T., Sakai Y., Kakiuchi C., Umekage T., Sasaki T., Akiskal H.S. The association between oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphisms and affective temperaments, as measured by TEMPS-A. J. Affect. Disord. 2010;127:31–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Weisman O., Pelphrey K.A., Leckman J.F., Feldman R., Lu Y., Chong A., Chen Y., Monakhov M., Chew S.H., Ebstein R.P. The association between 2D:4D ratio and cognitive empathy is contingent on a common polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR rs53576) Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;58:23–32. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Shamay-Tsoory S.G., Abu-Akel A. The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol. Psychiatry. 2016;79:194–202. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.C. George, N. Kaplan, M. Main, et al., Adult attachment interview, The Authors, 1996.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Data associated with this study has been deposited at NTU's Data repository (DR NTU Data) under the url: https://doi.org/10.21979/N9/GUSTLQ.


Articles from Heliyon are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES