Abstract
Objective
To assess cortical thickness (CT) and surface area (SA) of frontal, temporal, and parietal brain regions in a large clinical high risk for psychosis (CHR) sample, and to identify cortical brain abnormalities in CHR who convert to psychosis and in the whole CHR sample, compared with the healthy controls (HC).
Methods
Magnetic resonance imaging, clinical, and cognitive data were acquired at baseline in 92 HC, 130 non-converters, and 22 converters (conversion assessed at 1-year follow-up). CT and SA at baseline were calculated for frontal, temporal, and parietal subregions. Correlations between regions showing group differences and clinical scores and age were also obtained.
Results
CT but not SA was significantly reduced in CHR compared with HC. Two patterns of findings emerged: (1) In converters, CT was significantly reduced relative to non-converters and controls in the banks of superior temporal sulcus, Heschl’s gyrus, and pars triangularis and (2) CT in the inferior parietal and supramarginal gyrus, and at trend level in the pars opercularis, fusiform, and middle temporal gyri was significantly reduced in all high-risk individuals compared with HC. Additionally, reduced CT correlated significantly with older age in HC and in non-converters but not in converters.
Conclusions
These results show for the first time that fronto-temporo-parietal abnormalities characterized all CHR, that is, both converters and non-converters, relative to HC, while CT abnormalities in converters relative to CHR-NC and HC were found in core auditory and language processing regions.
Keywords: clinical high risk for psychosis, SA, CT, language network, prediction of conversion
Introduction
Structural abnormalities in frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes are common in both early and chronic schizophrenia.1–6 Cognitive functions sub-served by these cortices, including language and reasoning, are also affected,7,8 as is the progression of these structural abnormalities over the course of illness.4,5,9–11 Across diagnostic categories, including affective and psychotic illnesses, disruption of a common set of brain regions, together forming the fronto-parietal connectome network has recently been described.12,13 Individuals at clinical high risk for developing psychosis (CHR) represent a heterogeneous population, whose clinical profiles are shaped by prodromal symptoms and whose rate of conversion to psychosis is typically between 15% and 30%, depending in part on whether follow-up assessments were obtained 1, 2, or 3 years past baseline (eg,14–17). We hypothesize that similar to populations with established psychiatric diagnoses13 who show abnormalities in frontal, temporal, and parietal cortices, CHR will demonstrate abnormalities in these brain regions as well, with some abnormalities characterizing all CHR compared with controls, and other abnormalities characterizing subjects who transition to psychosis (converters: CHR-C) compared with both subjects who do not transition to psychosis (non-converters: CHR-NC) and healthy controls (HC).
Evidence of brain structural abnormalities is well documented in chronic and first episode schizophrenia, while evidence of such abnormalities is less established for individuals at clinical high risk for developing schizophrenia, although the genetic contribution to schizophrenia suggests a neurodevelopmental component18–23 to observed brain changes. Several brain structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies in CHR report volume reductions in converters, compared with non-converters: CHR-NC.9,24,25 Additionally, brain surface contractions in prefrontal cortex26 and reductions in gray matter volume of temporal, and parietal cortices, and cingulate gyrus27,28 have been observed, but significance of results is limited by small sample sizes and methodological approaches.26,28 On the other hand, a somewhat larger CHR study (7 converters during a 28 months follow-up) in an Asian population did not find gray matter volume reductions,29 nor did a large study involving a North American sample at baseline.30 Instead, steeper rates of gray matter reductions in converters compared with non-converters and controls were reported.30 Chung et al31 grouped CHR into CHR stable/remitted (mild symptoms or remission) and CHR decline (severe symptoms or conversion) with each group further subdivided into younger (12–17 years old) and older (18–35 years old) groups. This approach to CHR categorization yielded evidence of cortical deficits in the younger but not in the older CHR group. While approach allows for constructing larger and more balanced subject groupings, it does not inform about brain differences in relation to conversion, an approach adopted in the current study. Furthermore, Cannon30 and Chung31 articles are based on data acquired at eight different sites/scanners, in contrast to our study based on one scanning site.
Several recent studies investigating brain structural integrity in CHR have adopted a measure of cortical thickness (CT)30–34 (see Bartholomeusz et al35 for review), while surface area (SA) has only been investigated in just a handful of CHR studies.31,34,36,37 Since brain volume is the product of CT and SA, investigating both in the same study makes it possible to evaluate their relative contribution to volumetric brain abnormalities in CHR and tease apart different neurodevelopmental contributions to schizophrenia pathology. Indeed, CT and SA are genetically unrelated,38 with distinct maturational stages.39,40 In the current study, accordingly, we assessed both CT and SA.
By using a priori defined regions of interests (ROIs), several studies33,41,42 have identified CT abnormalities in CHR populations, although some of these studies included no converters41 or relied on multisite MRI acquisition and/or low resolution scanners33,42 CT reductions in both prefrontal and parietal cortices have also been identified by using whole brain analysis (eg, Jung et al43 [six converters included] and Kwak et al44 [eight converters included], but not all studies report abnormalities.29,30
The analytic approach in this study falls between a conservative whole brain analysis and an a priori defined ROI approach. Specifically, we focused on the frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes.12 Within these three lobes, we examined all lobe subregions. We were interested in testing the hypothesis that there would exist a set of fronto-temporo-parietal brain abnormalities characterizing the whole at-risk group, as reported in older populations with mixed psychiatric diagnoses, while another set of abnormalities would characterize converters relative to both non-converters and HC at 1-year clinical follow-up assessment; this hypothesis, to the best of our knowledge, has not been assessed before.
Compared with previous studies, we reduced several sources of heterogeneity and variability. First, this large Chinese sample (N = 244), similar to most Asian population studies, had low rates of drug abuse and low dosages of antipsychotic medications, major confounds in detecting subtle structural brain abnormalities.6,29,44,45 Second, subject recruitment was based on physician referrals only, thereby ensuring a more homogenous patient sample.44 Third, all brain scans were acquired on one MR scanner/one site, thereby reducing variability resulting from the use of different scanners46 (eg, Cannon study30 (see, eg, harmonization papers and discussions by Mirzaalian et al47 and Cetin-Karayumak et al48). The issue of multiple scanners is of crucial importance for the study of subtle neuroimaging abnormalities.
As brain abnormalities in language-related brain regions occur in prodromal stages20,49,50 and language abnormalities are core features of schizophrenia,20,51–53 we further hypothesized that primary language regions, as defined by current models of language and current research54–58 (see Supplementary figure S1) would be affected in converters. In support of this hypothesis, several recent studies of populations at CHR indicate that features of abnormal language distinguish converters from non-converters50,59,60 and schizophrenia from other disorders.61
Additionally, we hypothesized that a negative association between age and CT observed in HC, and indicative of developmentally driven maturation,40 would be absent in CHR, suggesting a possible neurodevelopmental component to predicted CT and SA abnormalities.
Method
Participants and Clinical Procedures
Data for this study were acquired at the Shanghai Mental Health Center (SMHC), Shanghai, China, as part of the ShangHai-At-Risk-for-Psychosis (SHARP) program. Participants included CHR individuals (n = 152), of whom 22 were CHR-C: 21 were diagnosed with schizophrenia and 1 with bipolar disorder with psychotic features at a 1-year clinical follow-up, and 130 were CHR-NC. These subjects were matched to HC (n = 92) for age, gender, and handedness (see table 1). A total of 5 of 22 CHR-C (22.7%) and 9/130 CHR-NC (9%) were medicated with low dose second generation antipsychotics.
Table 1.
Subjects # | HC | CHR-NC | CHR-C | Statistics | Group Differences |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
92 | 130 | 22 | P (F/χ2) | ||
Age | 18.8 ± 4.7 | 18.7 ± 4.9 | 19.5 ± 5.2 | .77 | |
Gender M/F | 46/46 | 61/69 | 15/7 | .183 | |
Race/ethnicity | Chinese | Chinese | Chinese | ||
GAF | 80.3 ± 2.2 | 54.3 ± 8.5 | 52.1 ± 7.7 | .001 | CHR-C=CHR-NC<HC |
Medicated/un-medicated | 0/92 | 21/109 | 6/16 | .23 | |
SOPS total | N/A | 37.2 ± 10.9 | 36.6 ± 11.1 | .84 | CHR-C=CHR-NC |
Positive symptoms | N/A | 10.1 ± 3.6 | 10.0 ± 3.2 | .86 | CHR-C=CHR-NC |
Disorganized symptoms | N/A | 6.53 ± 3.2 | 6.5 ± 2.9 | .97 | CHR-C=CHR-NC |
Negative symptoms | N/A | 11.3 ± 6.0 | 11.6 ± 6.5 | .84 | CHR-C=CHR-NC |
General symptoms | N/A | 9.2 ± 3.3 | 8.5 ± 3.0 | .41 | CHR-C=CHR-NC |
HVLT | 26.2 ± 3.7 | 22.8 ± 5.5 | 20.9 ± 4.5 | <.001 | CHR-C=CHR-NC<HC |
BVMT | 30.3 ± 3.6 | 27.2 ± 5.8 | 24.0 ± 6.8 | <.001 | CHR-C<CHR-NC<HC |
Recruitment and clinical assessments, including conversion criteria, are described in detail in Li et al17 and Zhang et al.62 Briefly, the Prodromal Symptoms/Scale of Prodromal Syndromes (SIPS/SOPS), which was validated for use in Chinese samples,63 was administered. Subjects who met SIPS diagnostic criteria were classified as CHR. This study was approved by the Human Subjects Review Committees at SMHC and at the Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center (BIDMC) in Boston. All participants signed an informed consent document prior to study participation.
MRI Acquisition and Parameters
MR scans were acquired on a Siemens 3T MR B17 (Verio) system, 32-channel head coil, located at SMHC. For the T1-weighted images the following parameters were used: MP-RAGE, repetition time (TR) = 2300 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.96 ms, flip angle = 9 degree, field of view (FOV) = 256 mm, and voxel size = 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm for 192 contiguous sagittal slices.
Image Processing
Images were visually inspected for possible movement or ghosting artifacts and were then axis realigned and centered. Automatic brain masking was conducted using Multi Atlas Brain Segmentation.64 Segmentation of the scans according to the Desikan–Killiany atlas was executed using FreeSurfer 5.365 to extract CT and SA. All segmentations were then validated by visual inspection.
Statistical Analyses
Demographic and clinical variables were analyzed using univariate ANOVAs or χ 2 tests (see table 1).
All subregions within each lobe were delineated according to the Desikan–Killiany Atlas. Prior to MRI analyses, data were tested for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Additionally, CT and SA of each region in the right and left frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes were Z-scored according to appropriate mean and standard deviation of HC. The focus of analyses was on group differences in each of the lobes with secondary focus on hemisphere effects. In the first step, separate analyses were performed for each lobe, that is, for frontal, temporal, and parietal lobe, and separately for CT and for SA. In the analyses, repeated measures MANOVAs were employed with group (HC, CHR-NC, and CHR-C) with gender as between-subject factors, and hemisphere (left and right) and lobe-specific subregions (region) as within-subject factors. Age was used as a covariate in all analyses. The intracranial volume (ICV) was used as covariate in all analyses involving SA variables. The MANOVA for the frontal lobe included 11 sub-regions in each hemisphere: superior (SFG), rostral and caudal middle frontal gyri, the pars opercularis, triangularis, and orbitalis; lateral and medial orbitofrontal cortex, precentral, paracentral gyri, and frontal pole. The MANOVA for the temporal lobe included nine sub-regions, nine in each hemisphere: superior (STG), middle (MTG), and inferior temporal gyri (ITG); banks of the superior temporal sulcus (bSTS); fusiform gyrus (FG); Heschl’s gyrus (HG); entorhinal cortex; temporal pole and para-hippocampal cortex. The MANOVA for the parietal lobe included five sub-regions, in each hemisphere: superior (SPG), inferior (IPG), supramarginal (sMG), postcentral gyri (pCG), and the precuneus. Bonferroni correction was entered into each MANOVA model.
In the second step of analyses, we followed up on all significant interactions involving group: between group and region, or between group, gender, and region. Accordingly, repeated measures MANOVAs with group or group and gender as independent variables and hemisphere as a within-subjects factor were carried out for each subregion of each lobe separately. Bonferroni correction was entered into each MANOVA model.
Where group differences were identified, pairwise comparisons were used to examine group differences among three groups in a given region. Bonferroni corrections were applied to correct for multiple comparisons.
Correlational Analyses
For clinical-structural MRI correlations, positive, negative, general, and disorganization scores from the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes (SIPS)63,66 were used. Age was controlled for, and Spearman’s r values are reported.
Correlations between age and CT were also assessed for CHR-C and CHR-NC separately given the different statistical results obtained for these two groups (SA was not included given a lack of significant group differences).
Results
Participants’ Group Characteristics
The three groups did not differ in age or gender, but CHR-C and CHR-NC Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) scores were significantly lower than HC, as expected (see table 1; see Supplementary materials for morphometric analyses as a function of GAF scores).
Imaging Results
Main imaging results are summarized in tables 2–5; and further in Supplementary table S1A–C.
Table 2.
Effect | F | Hypothesis df | Error df | Sig. | Partial η2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Superior frontal gyrus | |||||
Group | 0.83 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .44 | .007 |
Hemisphere | 7.3 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .007 | .03 |
Hemisphere × Group | 1.1 | 2.000 | 237 | 0.35 | .009 |
Rostral middle frontal gyrus | |||||
Group | 2.1 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .13 | .01 |
Hemisphere | 4.3 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .04 | .018 |
Hemisphere × Group | 1.8 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .165 | .015 |
Caudal middle frontal | |||||
Group | .71 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .49 | .006 |
Hemisphere | 1.7 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .192 | .007 |
Hemisphere × Group | 179 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .836 | .002 |
Pars opercularis | |||||
Group | 3.8 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .024 | .036 |
Hemisphere | .238 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .626 | .001 |
Hemisphere × Group | 2.179 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .115 | .018 |
Pars orbitalis | |||||
Group | 1.1 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .33 | .004 |
Hemisphere | 4.99 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .026 | .021 |
Hemisphere × Group | .347 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .707 | .003 |
Pars triangularis | |||||
Group | 5.0 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .007 | .035 |
Hemisphere | 5.782 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .017 | .024 |
Hemisphere × Group | .467 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .628 | .004 |
Latero-orbital frontal | |||||
Group | 1.1 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .33 | .005 |
Hemisphere | .110 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .741 | .000463 |
Hemisphere × Group | .625 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .536 | .005 |
Medial-orbital Frontal | |||||
Group | 1.24 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .29 | .012 |
Hemisphere | 18.83 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .00002 | .074 |
Hemisphere × Group | 1.892 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .153 | .016 |
Precentral | |||||
Group | .26 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .77 | .004 |
Hemisphere | 2.112 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .147 | .009 |
Hemisphere × Group | 3.397 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .035 | .028 |
Paracentral | |||||
Group | 0.485 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .62 | .007 |
Hemisphere | .59 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .443 | .002 |
Hemisphere × Group | .634 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .531 | .005 |
Frontal Pole | |||||
Group | 1.67 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .19 | .012 |
Hemisphere | 28.3 | 1.000 | 237.000 | .00001 | .1 |
Hemisphere × Group | .374 | 2.000 | 237.000 | .689 | .003 |
Table 3.
Effect | F | Hypothesis df | Error df | Sig. | Partial η2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Superior temporal gyrus | |||||
Group | 2.6 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .075 | .121 |
Hemisphere | 1.186 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .277 | .005 |
Hemisphere × Group | .547 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .005 | .019 |
Middle temporal gyrus | |||||
Group | 5.9 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .003 | .049 |
Hemisphere | 6.813 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .01 | .028 |
Hemisphere × Group | 2.283 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .104 | .019 |
Inferior temporal gyrus | |||||
Group | 2.8 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .06 | .024 |
Hemisphere | .294 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .588 | .001 |
Hemisphere × Group | 1.477 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .230 | .012 |
Banks of the superior temporal sulcus | |||||
Group | 4.4 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .013 | .037 |
Hemisphere | .371 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .543 | .002 |
Hemisphere × Group | .309 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .734 | .003 |
Fusiform | |||||
Group | 4.6 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .01 | .039 |
Hemisphere | .007 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .934 | .00003 |
Hemisphere × Group | .758 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .47 | .006 |
Heschl gyrus | |||||
Group | 6.1 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .003 | .047 |
Hemisphere | 1.224 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .27 | .005 |
Hemisphere × Group | .234 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .792 | .002 |
Entorhinal cortex | |||||
Group | .73 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .48 | .006 |
Hemisphere | 3.079 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .08 | .013 |
Hemisphere × Group | .57 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .566 | .005 |
Temporal pole | |||||
Group | 2.116 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .123 | .017 |
Hemisphere | .738 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .391 | .003 |
Hemisphere × Group | 1.172 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .311 | .01 |
Para-hippocampal cortex | |||||
Group | .14 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .87 | .001 |
Hemisphere | 6.207 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .013 | .025 |
Hemisphere × Group | 1.243 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .29 | .01 |
Table 4.
Effect | F | Hypothesis df | Error df | Sig. | Partial η2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Superior parietal gyrus | |||||
Group | 1.916 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .15 | .015 |
Hemisphere | 1.940 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .165 | .008 |
Hemisphere × Group | .138 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .871 | .001 |
Inferior parietal gyrus | |||||
Group | 7.64 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .001 | .063 |
Hemisphere | .553 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .458 | .002 |
Hemisphere × Group | .29 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .749 | .002 |
Supramarginal | |||||
Group | 6.33 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .002 | .053 |
Hemisphere | 4.485 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .035 | .018 |
Hemisphere × Group | .331 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .718 | .003 |
Postcentral | |||||
Group | 1.96 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .143 | .015 |
Hemisphere | .907 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .342 | .004 |
Hemisphere × Group | 1.526 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .219 | .013 |
Precuneus | |||||
Group | 2.69 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .02 | .022 |
Hemisphere | 1.945 | 1.000 | 240.000 | .164 | .008 |
Hemisphere × Group | .432 | 2.000 | 240.000 | .650 | .004 |
Table 5.
Group Differencesa | HC vs CHR-NC | Cohen’s db | HC vs CHR-C | Cohen’s dc | CHR-NC vs CHR-C | Cohen’s dd |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Converters-only CT abnormalities | ||||||
Pars Triangularis | P = .99 (0.33) | d = 0.03 | P = .036 (0.012) | d = 0.72 | P = .022 (0.0073) | d = 0.72 |
Banks of STS | P = .72 (0.24) | d = 0.17 | P = .024 (0.008) | d = 0.74 | P = .048 (0.016) | d = 0.6 |
Heschl Gyrus | P = .3 (0.1) | d = 0.14 | P = .006 (0.002) | d = 0.96 | P = .036 (0.012) | d = 0.6 |
CT abnormalities common to All CHR | ||||||
Pars Opercularis | P = .075 (0.025) | d = 0.3 | P = .036 (0.012) | d = 0.6 | P = .54 (.18) | d = 0.26 |
Supramarginal | P = .03 (0.01) | d = 0.7 | P = .003 (0.001) | d = 0.76 | P = .18 (0.06) | d = 0.38 |
Inferior Parietal | P = .006 (0.002) | d = 0.4 | P = .002 (0.0007) | d = 0.7 | P = .33 (0.1) | d = 0.3 |
MTG | P = .07 (0.023) | d = 0.3 | P = .003 (0.001) | d = 0.8 | P = .135 (0.045) | d = 0.3 |
Fusiform | P = .072 (0.024) | d = 0.3 | P = .018 (0.006) | d = 0.63 | P = .42 (0.14) | d = 0.3 |
aFor each post hoc comparison, uncorrected P values are listed in parenthesis.
bFor all, HC>CHR-NC.
cFor all, HC>CHR-C.
dFor all, CHR-NC>CHR-C.
CT Analyses
Frontal Lobe
The omnibus MANOVA with 11 subregions showed a significant interaction of Group by Region by Gender [F(20, 456) = 1.7; P = .016; η2 = 0.07]. The follow-up MANOVAs demonstrated that this significant interaction was driven by significant CT group differences in the pars triangularis and the pars opercularis (see table 2).
Pairwise comparisons showed that CT of pars triangularis in converters was significantly lower than in non-converters (P = .0073; corrected P = .022) and in HC (P = .012; corrected P = .036). No significant difference was found between non-converters and HC (see table 5).
Pars Opercularis
CT in converters was significantly lower than in HC (P = .012, corrected P = .036) but was not different than in non-converters (P = .18; corrected P = .54), with a trend level CT reduction in non-converters relative to HC (P = .023; corrected P = .076; table 5).
Frontal lobe CT hemisphere effects identified in follow-up MANOVAs:
Several regions showed larger left than right hemisphere, without significant group interactions (see table 2).
Temporal Lobe
The omnibus MANOVA with nine regions showed that there was a main effect of group [F(2,237) = 3.82; P = .023, η2 = 0.031] with lower temporal lobe CT in converters compared with HC (P = .02; corrected P = .06) and CT of non-converters not different from converters (P = .11; corrected P = .33) or from HC (P = .65; corrected P = 1.00). The interaction between Region and Group was significant [F(16,460) = 2.1; P = .007, η2 = 0.07].
The follow-up MANOVAs showed that the interaction was driven by four regions, including Heschl gyrus, the banks of the superior temporal sulcus, the middle temporal gyrus, and the fusiform gyrus (see table 3).
Pairwise comparisons showed that CT in converters was lower than in non-converters and HC for the Heschl gyrus and the banks of the superior temporal sulcus; while CT of non-converters and HC did not differ (see table 5).
CT of the middle temporal and the fusiform gyri was equivalent in converters and non-converters, and lower than in HC (see table 5).
Temporal lobe CT hemisphere effects identified in follow-up MANOVAs:
The middle temporal gyrus and the parahippocampal cortex had greater left than right CT in all groups (see table 3).
Parietal Lobe
The omnibus MANOVA with five regions showed a main effect of group [F(2,237) = 3.8, P = .024; η2 = 0.031] with smaller CT in converters relative to HC (P = .013; corrected P = .04), while CT of non-converters did not differ from converters (P = .047; corrected P = .14) or HC (P = .21; corrected P = .63). There was also a significant interaction between Region and Group [F(8,468) = 2.16, P = .03, η2 = 0.036].
Follow-up MANOVAs showed that the interaction was driven by significant CT differences of the inferior parietal and the supra-marginal gyri (see table 4).
Pairwise comparisons showed that CT of the inferior parietal did not differ between converters and non-converters but was lower than in HC (table 5). CT of the supra-marginal gyrus did not differ in converters and non-converters but was lower than in HC (table 5).
Parietal lobe CT hemisphere effects identified in follow-up MANOVAs.
The CT of the supra-marginal gyrus, inferior parietal gyrus, and precuneus was greater in the left hemisphere in all groups (see table 4).
SA Analysis
Lobes
SA did not differ statistically among the three groups for any of the three lobes (frontal lobe [F(2,241) = 1.0; P = .37], temporal lobe [F(2,241) = 0.45; P = .64], or parietal lobe [F(2,237) = 2.3, P = .1]).
Correlational Analyses
Relationship Between Age and CT
We explored the relationship between age and CT separately in CHR-C, CHR-NC, and HC in the eight brain regions that showed statistically significant group differences. CT for all regions, with the exception of HG, was strongly and inversely correlated with age in both HC and CHR-CN. That is, older age was associated with reduced CT. In contrast, no statistically significant correlations were found in the CHR-C group (see figure 2 and Supplementary table S2).
Clinical Variables
No significant correlations were found between CT and SIPS scores.
Discussion
This examination of CT and SA in frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes in a large group (N = 152) of individuals at CHR revealed that CT, but not SA, was reduced in CHR. As volume is the product of CT and SA, these results suggest that CT is the major contributor to reported volume reductions in CHR. Notably, this is the first report to identify both a set of CT abnormalities that characterizes converters and non-converters (ie, the whole CHR sample), and another set of abnormalities that characterizes converters relative to non-converters and HC (see figure 1).
Individuals at CHR represent a diverse population, where only up to a third of individuals will convert to psychosis.17 In populations with established, mixed psychiatric diagnoses, functional connectivity studies have identified abnormalities in a set of regions involving the frontal, temporal and parietal cortices, and the fronto-parietal network.12,13,67
In this study, the CHR group irrespective of outcome, as assessed at 1-year follow-up, showed CT abnormalities largely overlapping with the fronto-parietal network described in functional connectivity studies68 of mixed psychiatric populations.12,13 The converters to psychosis, relative to both non-converters and HC, showed CT abnormalities in the Heschl gyrus, the banks of the superior temporal sulcus and the pars triangularis. These regions are regarded as the core of the language network.54–58
Baseline CT reductions found in the current study contrast with two other large CHR studies of European ancestry29,30 but agree with the large study of Asian CHR,44 although this study lacks comparisons between converters and non-converters. Possible reasons for the lack of CT reductions29,30 may include a low number of converters (eg,29) and the use of multiple scanners30 which might make accurate measurements of smaller cortical regions more challenging. As discussed above, Chung et al31 study reported CHR-HC differences across a number of measures but did not examine converter-non-converter status relative to HC, which complicates direct comparisons with the current study.
Thus, the different findings may reflect heterogeneity introduced by recruitment methods,16,17,29,44,69 smaller subject cohorts and the utilization of multiple scanners (eg,30,36,46,70,71). In the current large Asian study, MR data were acquired on one scanner at one acquisition site using a large CHR cohort with minimal exposure to drug abuse and with limited use of antipsychotic medication. Furthermore, all subjects were accepted into the study based on physician referral, which reduces sampling variability. Thus, most common sources of variability were reduced in this study.
Two important findings emerge from this study. (1) Cortical thinning, at baseline, in all CHR, that is, irrespective of the conversion status, was observed in the fronto-temporo-parietal network including in: (a) the frontal lobe, where CT reductions were found in the pars opercularis, (b) the temporal lobe, where CT reductions were found in the middle temporal and the fusiform gyri, and (c) the parietal lobe, with CT reductions in the supramarginal and the inferior parietal gyri (see figure 1). (2) CT reductions in CHR-C relative to both HC and CHR-NC were observed in the frontal (pars triangularis) and temporal regions (Heschl gyrus and banks of superior temporal sulcus; see figure 1). We interpret these regions as primary auditory and core language regions following the current literature (eg,54,55,72–74 also see Supplementary figure S1).
The status of conversion was assessed at 1-year clinical follow-up. Thus, observed CT reductions in CHR-C at baseline may serve as markers of transition to psychosis, while CT abnormalities observed in all CHR (ie, both converters and non-converters) might serve as markers of high-risk status, irrespective of conversion. CT reductions in converters relative to both non-converters and HC affected regions including the pars triangularis, the Heschl gyrus and the banks of the superior temporal sulcus, which are involved in auditory processing75 speech (including prosody processing)75 and semantic and syntactic processing.76,77 Thus, conversion to psychosis was related to CT reductions in primary auditory and core portions of language regions. These findings implicating language-related brain regions in CHR are novel but dovetail nicely with recent functional MRI studies in CHR that identified abnormalities78–80 in several brain regions where CT reductions were observed in the current study.
The regions affected in the whole CHR group (ie, converters + non-converters) spanned the frontal, the temporal, and the parietal lobe. These fronto-temporo-parietal brain subregions are involved in an array of cognitive functions and are activated according to the cognitive task at hand.68,81–83 Functions that have been ascribed to these regions include semantic, phonological, and orthographic processes (IPG and FG),84 multimodal association functions involved in word reading, comprehension, and semantic analyses (angular gyrus; see Seghier85), interfacing between phonetic and articulatory representations (sMG; Gow86), and between phonetic and semantic representations (MTG). Furthermore, the fronto-temporo-parietal regions contribute to cognitive flexibility: self-agency processing,87 working memory processes,88 bottom-up attention, undirected thinking, episodic memory, and social cognition (see ref. 89 for a review), delayed reward discounting,90 theory of mind91 (important for facilitating the development of inferences about complex linguistic messages), and face processing.92,93 Notably, all of these functions make full and flexible use of human language possible.94
Furthermore, our data cautiously support the hypothesis of abnormal neurodevelopmental CT trajectories in CHR. More specifically, significant negative relationships were observed between CT and age in HC and in CHR-NC in several of the regions where CT reductions were observed (age range: HC:14–34; CHR-NC: 13–32). Importantly, this relationship (indicative of a neurotypical development) was absent in converters to psychosis (age range 13–36; see figure 2).
Cortical thinning is characteristic of brain maturation as a function of age40,95,96 and likely results from multiple neurodevelopmental processes such as pruning,97 myelination, and cortical morphology.98 Pruning is believed to reflect prolonged fine-tuning of neuronal connections that extends beyond adolescence in typically developing individuals,40,96,99,100 while myelination indicates growth of myelin sheaths around axons, a process that in traditionally analyzed MRI appears as thinning of the gray matter at the gray/white matter boundary. Increased gyrification/cortical folding might also explain the apparent perceived thinning of the cortex during development.101 The three possibilities are not mutually exclusive, and our data in relation to age might result from any of these processes.
Two other studies (de Wit et al37 and Chung et al31) also hint at the interactions between neurodevelopmental and disease processes. While the results of these studies are somewhat difficult to compare with our results given differences in methodology, together they underscore the importance of accounting for age when examining neuroanatomical changes in CHR samples. Our correlational analyses of relationships between age and CT explicitly show the impact of conversion on neurodevelopmental trajectories (but see below) and, together with the examination of GAF scores and CT reductions (see Supplementary Materials) further clarify both the de Wit and the Chung results: conversion to psychosis impacts normal developmental trajectories. The role of age is succinctly and eloquently discussed in Andreou and Borgwardt’s recent review.102
However, given the relatively low N of CHR-C, which may have contributed to the absence of significant negative correlations in the CHR-C group in the current results, this observation will need to be replicated in a larger cohort of CHR-C.
Several limitations to this study deserve mention. First, no effects of gender were observed, although gender-related brain differences in CHR have been reported in a study examining CT in 26 CHR and 29 HC using a 1.5 T magnet.103 It is possible that the sample low N and low image resolution contributed to the discrepant results. Second, we did not obtain significant correlations between abnormal CT and clinical symptoms, in contrast to the Kwak et al44 which assessed 74 CHR (6 converters) and 34 HC. The source of these differences remains unclear, though the current study, with 152 CHR (22 converters) and 92 HC, is the largest to date conducted in an Asian population both in terms of overall sample size and number of converters. Differences in sample characteristics29,30,43,44 might have contributed to the different outcomes. Third, the age analysis in relation to CT was cross-sectional rather than longitudinal. We will follow-up this cohort longitudinally at 2 and 3 years post-baseline to assess CT changes over time in individual subjects. Fourth, our CHR sample was assessed clinically at a 1-year follow-up, which means that some CHR-NC subjects will likely convert to psychosis after the 1-year and alter observed relationships between the CHR-NC, CHR-C, and HC groups. Fifth, while functional connectivity studies have reported specific abnormalities in psychotic populations13 that do not necessarily overlap with abnormalities reported here in converters, we note that those findings apply to older populations while the CHR group studied here was young and still maturing. In summary, these novel data indicate that CT reductions contribute to volumetric reductions assessed in a large MRI study sample of CHR drawn from an Asian population. Our data indicate for the first time that CT reductions span a set of brain regions that largely overlap with the fronto-parietal network whose abnormalities characterize older populations with established, mixed psychiatric diagnosis.12,13,68 These data support our hypothesis of a set of cortical abnormalities common to all CHRs, and of a second set of abnormalities that are characteristic of converters to psychosis relative to non-converters and to HC.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This work was conducted with support from Harvard Catalyst | The Harvard Clinical and Translational Science Center (National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health Award UL 1TR002541) and financial contributions from Harvard University and its affiliated academic healthcare centers. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of Harvard Catalyst, Harvard University and its affiliated academic healthcare centers, or the National Institutes of Health. The Authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH111448) and by R21MH109819 to E.C.D.R.
References
- 1.Shenton ME, Dickey CC, Frumin M, McCarley RW. A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2001;49(1–2):1–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Takayanagi Y, Takahashi T, Orikabe L, et al. . Classification of first-episode schizophrenia patients and healthy subjects by automated MRI measures of regional brain volume and cortical thickness. PLoS One. 2011;6(6):e21047. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ellison-Wright I, Glahn DC, Laird AR, Thelen SM, Bullmore E. The anatomy of first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(8):1015–1023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Vita A, De Peri L, Deste G, Sacchetti E. Progressive loss of cortical gray matter in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis and meta-regression of longitudinal MRI studies. Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ohtani T, Del Re E, Levitt JJ, et al. . Progressive symptom-associated prefrontal volume loss occurs in first-episode schizophrenia but not in affective psychosis. Brain Struct Funct. 2018;223(6):2879–2892. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1634-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lee TY, Kwon JS. Psychosis research in Asia: advantage from low prevalence of cannabis use. NPJ Schizophr. 2016;2:1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cavelti M, Kircher T, Nagels A, Strik W, Homan P. Is formal thought disorder in schizophrenia related to structural and functional aberrations in the language network? A systematic review of neuroimaging findings. Schizophr Res. 2018;199:2–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Sumner PJ, Bell IH, Rossell SL. A systematic review of task-based functional neuroimaging studies investigating language, semantic and executive processes in thought disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;94:59–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Dietsche B, Kircher T, Falkenberg I. Structural brain changes in schizophrenia at different stages of the illness: a selective review of longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2017;51(5):500–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Torres US, Duran FL, Schaufelberger MS, et al. . Patterns of regional gray matter loss at different stages of schizophrenia: a multisite, cross-sectional VBM study in first-episode and chronic illness. Neuroimage Clin. 2016;12:1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Cobia DJ, Smith MJ, Wang L, Csernansky JG. Longitudinal progression of frontal and temporal lobe changes in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;139(1–3):1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Baker JT, Holmes AJ, Masters GA, et al. . Disruption of cortical association networks in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(2):109–118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Baker JT, Dillon DG, Patrick LM, et al. . Functional connectomics of affective and psychotic pathology. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116(18):9050–9059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fusar-Poli P. Predicting the development of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2012;200(3):254–255; author reply 255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Fusar-Poli P, Deste G, Smieskova R, et al. . Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(6):562–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Fusar-Poli P, Bechdolf A, Taylor MJ, et al. . At risk for schizophrenic or affective psychoses? A meta-analysis of DSM/ICD diagnostic outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(4):923–932. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Li H, Zhang T, Xu L, et al. . A comparison of conversion rates, clinical profiles and predictors of outcomes in two independent samples of individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis in China. Schizophr Res. 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.11.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Blokland GAM, Mesholam-Gately RI, Toulopoulou T, et al. . Heritability of neuropsychological measures in schizophrenia and nonpsychiatric populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(4):788–800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Blokland GAM, Del Re EC, Mesholam-Gately RI, et al. . The Genetics of Endophenotypes of Neurofunction to Understand Schizophrenia (GENUS) consortium: a collaborative cognitive and neuroimaging genetics project. Schizophr Res. 2018;195:306–317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Li X, Wu K, Zhang Y, Kong L, Bertisch H, DeLisi LE. Altered topological characteristics of morphological brain network relate to language impairment in high genetic risk subjects and schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res. 2019;208:338–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Birnbaum R, Weinberger DR. Genetic insights into the neurodevelopmental origins of schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017;18(12):727–740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Li M, Wang Y, Zheng XB, et al. . Meta-analysis and brain imaging data support the involvement of VRK2 (rs2312147) in schizophrenia susceptibility. Schizophr Res. 2012;142(1–3):200–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Gscheidle T, et al. . Relative risk for cognitive impairments in siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(2):98–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Brent BK, Thermenos HW, Keshavan MS, Seidman LJ. Gray matter alterations in schizophrenia high-risk youth and early-onset schizophrenia: a review of structural MRI findings. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2013;22(4):689–714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, Crescini A, et al. . Neuroanatomy of vulnerability to psychosis: a voxel-based meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(5):1175–1185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sun D, Phillips L, Velakoulis D, et al. . Progressive brain structural changes mapped as psychosis develops in “at risk” individuals. Schizophr Res. 2009;108(1–3):85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Pantelis C, Yücel M, Wood SJ, McGorry PD, Velakoulis D. Early and late neurodevelopmental disturbances in schizophrenia and their functional consequences. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2003;37(4):399–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Borgwardt SJ, McGuire PK, Aston J, et al. . Reductions in frontal, temporal and parietal volume associated with the onset of psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2008;106(2–3):108–114. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Klauser P, Zhou J, Lim JKW, et al. . Lack of evidence for regional brain volume or cortical thickness abnormalities in youths at clinical high risk for psychosis: findings from the longitudinal youth at risk study. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(6):1285–1293. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cannon TD, Chung Y, He G, et al. . Progressive reduction in cortical thickness as psychosis develops: a multisite longitudinal neuroimaging study of youth at elevated clinical risk. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77(2):147–157. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.05.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Chung Y, Allswede D, Addington J, et al. . Cortical abnormalities in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis: findings from the NAPLS2 cohort. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;23:101862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Buchy L, Stowkowy J, MacMaster FP, Nyman K, Addington J. Meta-cognition is associated with cortical thickness in youth at clinical high risk of psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2015;233(3):418–423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fornito A, Yung AR, Wood SJ, et al. . Anatomic abnormalities of the anterior cingulate cortex before psychosis onset: an MRI study of ultra-high-risk individuals. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(9):758–765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sprooten E, Papmeyer M, Smyth AM, et al. . Cortical thickness in first-episode schizophrenia patients and individuals at high familial risk: a cross-sectional comparison. Schizophr Res. 2013;151(1–3):259–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bartholomeusz CF, Cropley VL, Wannan C, Di Biase M, McGorry PD, Pantelis C. Structural neuroimaging across early-stage psychosis: Aberrations in neurobiological trajectories and implications for the staging model. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2017;51(5):455–476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Takayanagi Y, Kulason S, Sasabayashi D, et al. . Reduced thickness of the anterior cingulate cortex in individuals with an at-risk mental state who later develop psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(4):907–913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.de Wit S, Wierenga LM, Oranje B, et al. . Brain development in adolescents at ultra-high risk for psychosis: Longitudinal changes related to resilience. Neuroimage Clin. 2016;12:542–549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Panizzon MS, Fennema-Notestine C, Eyler LT, et al. . Distinct genetic influences on cortical surface area and cortical thickness. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(11):2728–2735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Vijayakumar N, Allen NB, Youssef G, et al. . Brain development during adolescence: A mixed-longitudinal investigation of cortical thickness, surface area, and volume. Human Brain Mapping. 2016;37(6):2027–2038. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Tamnes CK, Herting MM, Goddings A-L, et al. . Development of the cerebral cortex across adolescence: a multisample study of inter-related longitudinal changes in cortical volume, surface area, and thickness. J Neurosci. 2017;37(12):3402–3412. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3302-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Benetti S, Pettersson-Yeo W, Hutton C, et al. . Elucidating neuroanatomical alterations in the at risk mental state and first episode psychosis: a combined voxel-based morphometry and voxel-based cortical thickness study. Schizophr Res. 2013;150(2–3):505–511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Tognin S, Riecher-Rössler A, Meisenzahl EM, et al. . Reduced parahippocampal cortical thickness in subjects at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Psychol Med. 2014;44(3):489–498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Jung WH, Kim JS, Jang JH, et al. . Cortical thickness reduction in individuals at ultra-high-risk for psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(4):839–849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kwak YB, Kim M, Cho KIK, Lee J, Lee TY, Kwon JS. Reduced cortical thickness in subjects at clinical high risk for psychosis and clinical attributes. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2019;53(3):219–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Jung WH, Jang JH, Shin NY, et al. . Regional brain atrophy and functional disconnection in Broca’s area in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and schizophrenia. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e51975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gifford G, Crossley N, Fusar-Poli P, et al. . Using neuroimaging to help predict the onset of psychosis. Neuroimage. 2017;145(Pt B):209–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Mirzaalian H, Ning L, Savadjiev P, et al. . Inter-site and inter-scanner diffusion MRI data harmonization. Neuroimage. 2016;135:311–323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Cetin Karayumak S, Bouix S, Ning L, et al. . Retrospective harmonization of multi-site diffusion MRI data acquired with different acquisition parameters. Neuroimage. 2019;184:180–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Rosenstein M, Foltz PW, DeLisi LE, Elvevåg B. Language as a biomarker in those at high-risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2015;165(2–3):249–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Corcoran CM, Carrillo F, Fernández-Slezak D, et al. . Prediction of psychosis across protocols and risk cohorts using automated language analysis. World Psychiatry. 2018;17(1):67–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kuperberg GR. Language in schizophrenia Part 1: an introduction. Lang Linguist Compass. 2010;4(8):576–589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mesholam-Gately RI, Giuliano AJ, Goff KP, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ. Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(3):315–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Rabagliati H, Delaney-Busch N, Snedeker J, Kuperberg G. Spared bottom-up but impaired top-down interactive effects during naturalistic language processing in schizophrenia: evidence from the visual-world paradigm. Psychol Med. 2019;49(8):1335–1345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Fedorenko E, Thompson-Schill SL. Reworking the language network. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18(3):120–126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Friederici AD. Evolution of the neural language network. Psychon Bull Rev. 2017;24(1):41–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hagoort P. The core and beyond in the language-ready brain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;81(Pt B):194–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hagoort P. The neurobiology of language beyond single-word processing. Science. 2019;366(6461):55–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Nasios G, Dardiotis E, Messinis L. From Broca and Wernicke to the neuromodulation era: insights of brain language networks for neurorehabilitation. Behav Neurol. 2019;2019:9894571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Rezaii N, Walker E, Wolff P. A machine learning approach to predicting psychosis using semantic density and latent content analysis. NPJ Schizophr. 2019;5(1):9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Ratana R, Sharifzadeh H, Krishnan J, Pang S. A Comprehensive review of computational methods for automatic prediction of schizophrenia with insight into indigenous populations. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Palaniyappan L, Mota NB, Oowise S, et al. . Speech structure links the neural and socio-behavioural correlates of psychotic disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2019;88:112–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Zhang T, Li H, Woodberry KA, et al. . Prodromal psychosis detection in a counseling center population in China: an epidemiological and clinical study. Schizophr Res. 2014;152(2–3):391–399. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.11.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Zheng L, Wang J, Zhang T, Li H, Li C, Jiang K. The Chinese version of the SIPS/SOPS: a pilot study of reliability and validity. Chinese Mental Health Journal. 2012;26(8):571–576. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Del Re EC, Gao Y, Eckbo R, et al. . A new MRI masking technique based on multi-atlas brain segmentation in controls and schizophrenia: a rapid and viable alternative to manual masking. J Neuroimaging. 2016;26(1):28–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, et al. . Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33(3):341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Rosen JL, et al. . Prodromal assessment with the structured interview for prodromal syndromes and the scale of prodromal symptoms: predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(4):703–715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Tu PC, Lee YC, Chen YS, Li CT, Su TP. Schizophrenia and the brain’s control network: aberrant within- and between-network connectivity of the frontoparietal network in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2013;147(2–3):339–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Marek S, Dosenbach NUF. The frontoparietal network: function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2018;20(2):133–140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Witthaus H, Kaufmann C, Bohner G, et al. . Gray matter abnormalities in subjects at ultra-high risk for schizophrenia and first-episode schizophrenic patients compared to healthy controls. Psychiatry Res. 2009;173(3):163–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Mechelli A, Riecher-Rössler A, Meisenzahl EM, et al. . Neuroanatomical abnormalities that predate the onset of psychosis: a multicenter study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(5):489–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Cropley VL, Lin A, Nelson B, et al. . Baseline grey matter volume of non-transitioned “ultra high risk” for psychosis individuals with and without attenuated psychotic symptoms at long-term follow-up. Schizophr Res. 2016;173(3):152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.05.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Friederici AD, Rüschemeyer SA, Hahne A, Fiebach CJ. The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13(2):170–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Friederici AD, Gierhan SM. The language network. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(2):250–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Chai LR, Mattar MG, Blank IA, Fedorenko E, Bassett DS. Functional network dynamics of the language system. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(11):4148–4159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Alho K, Rinne T, Herron TJ, Woods DL. Stimulus-dependent activations and attention-related modulations in the auditory cortex: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Hear Res. 2014;307:29–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Walenski M, Europa E, Caplan D, Thompson CK. Neural networks for sentence comprehension and production: an ALE-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40(8):2275–2304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Kroczek LOH, Gunter TC, Rysop AU, Friederici AD, Hartwigsen G. Contributions of left frontal and temporal cortex to sentence comprehension: evidence from simultaneous TMS-EEG. Cortex. 2019;115:86–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Natsubori T, Hashimoto R, Yahata N, et al. . An fMRI study of visual lexical decision in patients with schizophrenia and clinical high-risk individuals. Schizophr Res. 2014;157(1–3):218–224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Sabb FW, van Erp TG, Hardt ME, et al. . Language network dysfunction as a predictor of outcome in youth at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2010;116(2–3):173–183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Thermenos HW, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Seidman LJ, et al. . Altered language network activity in young people at familial high-risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2013;151(1–3):229–237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Smith K, Azami H, Escudero J, Parra MA, Starr JM. Comparison of network analysis approaches on EEG connectivity in beta during visual short-term memory binding tasks. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2015;2015:2207–2210. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2015.7318829 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Finn ES, Shen X, Scheinost D, et al. . Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(11):1664–1671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Kong J, Jensen K, Loiotile R, et al. . Functional connectivity of the frontoparietal network predicts cognitive modulation of pain. Pain. 2013;154(3):459–467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Wu CY, Ho MH, Chen SH. A meta-analysis of fMRI studies on Chinese orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing. Neuroimage. 2012;63(1):381–391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Seghier ML. The angular gyrus: multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. Neuroscientist. 2013;19(1):43–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Gow DW Jr. The cortical organization of lexical knowledge: a dual lexicon model of spoken language processing. Brain Lang. 2012;121(3):273–288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Koreki A, Maeda T, Okimura T, et al. . Dysconnectivity of the agency network in schizophrenia: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Andre J, Picchioni M, Zhang R, Toulopoulou T. Working memory circuit as a function of increasing age in healthy adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analyses. Neuroimage Clin. 2016;12:940–948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Igelström KM, Graziano MSA. The inferior parietal lobule and temporoparietal junction: a network perspective. Neuropsychologia. 2017;105:70–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Owens MM, Gray JC, Amlung MT, Oshri A, Sweet LH, MacKillop J. Neuroanatomical foundations of delayed reward discounting decision making. Neuroimage. 2017;161:261–270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Feng W, Wu Y, Jan C, Yu H, Jiang X, Zhou X. Effects of contextual relevance on pragmatic inference during conversation: an fMRI study. Brain Lang. 2017;171:52–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Grill-Spector K, Weiner KS, Kay K, Gomez J. The functional neuroanatomy of human face perception. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2017;3:167–196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Dricu M, Frühholz S. Perceiving emotional expressions in others: activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses of explicit evaluation, passive perception and incidental perception of emotions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:810–828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Kolodny O, Edelman S. The evolution of the capacity for language: the ecological context and adaptive value of a process of cognitive hijacking. Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2018;373(1743). doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, et al. . Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(21):8174–8179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Toga AW. Mapping changes in the human cortex throughout the span of life. Neuroscientist. 2004;10(4):372–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Keshavan MS, Anderson S, Pettegrew JW. Is schizophrenia due to excessive synaptic pruning in the prefrontal cortex? The Feinberg hypothesis revisited. J Psychiatr Res. 1994;28(3):239–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Natu VS, Gomez J, Barnett M, et al. . Apparent thinning of human visual cortex during childhood is associated with myelination. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116(41):20750–20759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Fjell AM, Westlye LT, Amlien I, et al. . High-expanding cortical regions in human development and evolution are related to higher intellectual abilities. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25(1):26–34. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Ducharme S, Albaugh MD, Nguyen TV, et al. . Trajectories of cortical thickness maturation in normal brain development–the importance of quality control procedures. Neuroimage. 2016;125:267–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Hogstrom LJ, Westlye LT, Walhovd KB, Fjell AM. The structure of the cerebral cortex across adult life: age-related patterns of surface area, thickness, and gyrification. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(11):2521–2530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Andreou C, Borgwardt S. Structural and functional imaging markers for susceptibility to psychosis. Mol Psychiatry. 2020:1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0679-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Guma E, Devenyi GA, Malla A, Shah J, Chakravarty MM, Pruessner M. Neuroanatomical and symptomatic sex differences in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Front Psychiatry. 2017;8:291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.