Skip to main content
Dementia & Neuropsychologia logoLink to Dementia & Neuropsychologia
. 2021 Jul-Sep;15(3):350–356. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642021dn15-030006

Depressive symptoms may be associated with semantic memory decline in elderly adults

Sintomas depressivos podem estar associados a declínio na memória semântica em idosos

Mariana Faoro 1, Amer Cavalheiro Hamdan 1
PMCID: PMC8485648  PMID: 34630922

ABSTRACT

Depressive symptoms are associated with a decline of episodic memory, but the relationship with semantic memory remains unclear.

Objective:

The aim of the study was to analyze the relationship of depressive symptoms with the semantic memory in a community-based sample of elderly adults. The sample comprised two groups, namely, clinical (with depressive symptoms) and control.

Methods:

The following instruments were used General Evaluation Questionnaire, Montreal Cognitive Assessment-Basic, Wechsler Abbreviate Scale of Intelligence, Geriatric Depression Scale-30 (GDS-30), Beck Depression Inventory-II, Boston Nomination Test (BNT), vocabulary, verbal fluency test (fruits and animals), and Brief Cognitive Screening Battery.

Results:

The results showed a significant difference between groups only in BNT. A significant correlation was observed between the BNT and GDS-30. Participants with symptoms of severe depression performed poorly on BNT when compared with individuals with mild symptoms.

Conclusion:

These results support the hypothesis that depressive symptoms in elderly adults can affect semantic memory and may worsen with the severity of symptoms.

Keywords: depression, elderly, memory, semantic memory disorders, neuropsychological tests, cognition

INTRODUCTION

Depressive symptoms and cognitive impairment are the most common complaints in elderly people. It has a high incidence in the elderly people and can be considered a risk factor for the development of dementia.1 , 2 , 3 , 4 , 5 A meta-analysis showed that depression in patients with mild cognitive impairment is 32%, and cognitive deficits were found more often in elderly depressed people than in younger depressed adults.6 , 7 In contrast, semantic memory deficits of 12 years before diagnosing Alzheimer’s disease were established.8

Depressive symptoms can affect cognition in elderly adults. According to Naismith et al.,9 the performance of participants with depression were significantly lower than the control group in executive functions, psychomotor speed, verbal learning, and memory. Some studies have shown that semantic memory is affected by the presence of depressive symptoms.10 , 11 , 12 Brunet et al.10 conducted a study to verify the relationship between depressive symptoms and semantic memory in elderly people with mild cognitive impairment-amnestic (MCIa) and depression. The results showed that depressive symptoms modulate the presentation of semantic memory deficits in people with MCIa.

In another study, Callahan et al.11 evaluated semantic memory impairment for knowledge of natural and artificial objects in patients with MCIa and depression. The results showed that patients with MCIa and depression present inferior performance compared with the other groups, including the group with depression, in which there were no changes in semantic memory. Vogel et al.12 evidenced that people with some type of affective disorder may perform poorly in semantic memory tests. However, Elderkin-Thompson et al.13 developed a systematic review to trace the neurocognitive profiles of patients with depression. The findings showed that people with depression tend to underperform in all cognitive domains except in semantic memory and executive skills.

The relationship between depression symptoms and semantic memory remains unclear.14 , 15 , 16 , 17 , 18 This study has two objectives, namely, first, to analyze the relationship of depressive symptoms with semantic memory in a community-based sample of elderly adults, and second, to verify the predictive capacity of variables on performance in instruments that assess the semantic memory construct. We considered a hypothesis that the severity of depressive symptoms worsens semantic memory impairment in elderly adults.

METHODS

Participants

The recruitment was carried out in a community-based sample of 88 adults. The inclusion criteria were as follows: elderly adults above 60 years of age, both sexes, and literate. The exclusion criteria were as follows: patients possibly diagnosed with dementia and having intelligence quotient (IQ) below 70. A total of 13 participants were excluded: 11 with a probable diagnosis of dementia and 2 with IQ below 70. Then, the volunteers were divided into two groups according to the cutoff point established in the performance on two depression scales: Geriatric Depression Scale-30 (GDS-30)19 and Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II).20 The cutoff points for identifying depressive symptoms are presented in the description of the instruments discussed in the next section. The final samples comprised 42 volunteers in the control group (without depressive symptoms) and 33 in the clinical group (with depressive symptoms). The clinical group was subdivided according to the severity of depressive symptoms (i.e., mild depressive symptoms and severe depressive symptoms) by means of performance assessed by the BDI-II. Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics of the sample divided by groups.

Table 1. Characteristics of the clinical group and the control group.

Clinical group (M±SD) Control group (M±SD) t-test/χ2 p-value
Age 67.5±5.87 68.8±6.38 -0.904 0.369
Sex (F/M) (29/04) (38/03) 0.577* 0.693
Schooling 10.7±4.67 12.4±5.1 − 1.515 0.134
IQ 95.5±14.5 99.8±14 − 1.303 0.197
GDS 15.9±4.7 5.17±3.56 11.26 <0.001
BDI 21.9±8.78 6.64±3.97 10.07 <0.001

M: mean; SD: standard deviation; M: male; F: female; GDS: Geriatric Depression Scale; BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; IQ: intelligence quotient. *Fisher’s exact test.

Instruments

The following instruments were used to evaluate participants, divided according to the function evaluated.

General Evaluation Questionnaire: This instrument was designed specifically for this research to obtain general information about the participant, including demographic data, previous diagnoses, family history, awareness of general health, and medication use.

Montreal Cognitive Assessment-Basic (MoCA-B): Originally developed by Nasreddine,21 this cognitive screening instrument was developed for low-educated population. It includes assessing the same cognitive domains (i.e., executive functions, language, orientation, calculation, abstraction, memory, visual perception, attention, and concentration) as the original MoCA, divided into 10 items that add up to 30 points.

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI): It was developed by Wechsler22 as a brief measure of intelligence and adapted for the Brazilian population by Trentini et al.23 The battery is composed of four subtests, which evaluate verbal and cognitive execution skills. This research used two subtests, namely, vocabulary and matrix reasoning, to obtain the total IQ. In addition to general intelligence, vocabulary was used in the evaluation of semantic memory.22 , 23 It consists of 42 items that the subject needs to define orally; the score ranges from 0 to 2 in each word according to the accuracy of the answers.

GDS-30: Originally developed by Yesavage et al.,19 this test is used for detecting depressive symptoms in the elderly population (above 60 years). It consists of 30 items, for which the participants must answer only yes or no, thinking about how they felt the previous week. According to the Brazilian scale validation research,24 10 was adopted as the cutoff point, that is, the participants who obtained the highest score were present in the clinical group. The GDS-30 is more reliable than its reduced form GDS-15, so the original form was used.25

BDI-II: It is an inventory created by Beck et al.20to measure the severity of depressive symptoms and adapted to the Brazilian population by Gorenstein et al.26 In the present research, the revised version BDI-II27 was used. It is a self-report scale with 21 items and a score ranging from 0 to 3; the total score is obtained by summing each item.28 According to the application manual, BDI-II score levels are minimum (0–11), mild (12–19), moderate (20–35), and severe (36–63).

Boston Naming Test (BNT): Originally developed by Kaplan et al.,29 it is widely used to detect language deficits. The volunteer is presented with 60 pictures, which must be correctly named, with 20 s for each item.30 There are two studies of adaptation of the test to Brazilian Portuguese,30 , 31 and both agree on the applicability of the test if the educational level of the participant is considered. According to Leite et al.,32 the adapted version of BNT has a higher number of hits at all educational levels due to cultural differences and socioeconomic status. Therefore, in the present research, we used the original translated version of the BNT, which showed good internal consistency in adaptation research (α=0.862).32 According to adaptation research, the cutoff scores were 34 for the elderly people with at least 3 years of schooling and age of 60–79 years, and for people aged above 80 years, it was 33 points.

Verbal fluency test — animals: it is used to evaluate executive functions and semantic memory. It works by questioning the subject to articulate as many animals as he/she can remember within a minute.33 , 34 , 35 The score is given according to the number of words correctly remembered.

Verbal fluency test — fruit: this is one of the MoCA-B subtests used to evaluate executive functions and semantic memory,36 and it requires the subject to say as many fruits as he/she can remember in 60 s. Within MoCA-B, the average for a maximum score on this test is 13 words or more.

Brief Cognitive Screening Battery (BCSB): this instrument developed by Nitrini et al.34 can evaluate episodic memory, attention, concentration, language, visual perception, construction, and planning. It is a list of 10 figures that the examiner must first name and then memorize. After the three initial presentations of the figure (i.e., 30 s), the participant is instructed to remember as many images as he/she can. Later on (after 5 min), the participant is instructed to recall the figures presented. This score is the long-term memory score (M5 index). Finally, participants are shown a sheet that contains, besides the target figures, others that are intrusive. The task of the participant is to recognize the correct images. The cutoff score, according to Nitrini et al.,37 is 7 points on the M5 index.

Procedure and ethical consideration

All instruments were applied in a single session, lasting around 90 min. The order of application of the instruments were General Evaluation Questionnaire, MoCA-B, WASI, GDS-30, BDI-II, BNT, verbal fluency test — animals, verbal fluency test — fruits, and BCSB.

The research was submitted to the Ethics Committee of the Health Sciences Sector of the Universidade Federal do Paraná (UFPR) and approved by number CAAE: 69654817.6.0000.0102. Through the wide dissemination of research in the media, the volunteers were recruited via the UFPR Press Office and evaluated at the Center for Applied Psychology of the university. All participants signed an informed consent form, which had all questions about the research and guaranteed anonymity.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using the Jamovi software version 0.8.2.3.38 For the description of the results, measures of central tendency, mean, and standard deviations were used. The Shapiro-Wilk test was used to assess the normal distribution of variables. The data showed a normal distribution. Subsequently, to compare the means between the control and the clinical groups, the Student’s t-test was used. According to the severity of depressive symptoms, the clinical group was subdivided into mild depressive symptoms and severe depressive symptoms. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the symptoms and the severity of the control group. The significance level for rejecting the null hypothesis was p<0.05. To verify the degree of association between the variables, the Pearson correlation coefficient was used. Multiple linear regression (Enter method) was performed to verify the predictive capacity of variables on performance in instruments that assess the semantic memory construct. Finally, to understand the clinical applications of the results, the effect size, Cohen’s d, and η² were calculated. According to Cohen,39 for Student’s t-test, we considered 0.20 (small), 0.50 (medium), and 0.80 (large). For ANOVA, we considered 0.10 (small), 0.25 (medium), and 0.40 (large).

RESULTS

Table 1 presents the sociodemographic variables of the sample. There are no statistically significant differences regarding the age, education, and IQ of the variables. However, the difference between groups in the depression scales appears significant. The sample was mostly composed of women in both groups.

Significant differences were observed in the performances of the clinical and the control groups only in BNT (Table 2). However, Cohen’s d variable shows the average effect size in MoCA-B and BNT and small size in vocabulary and verbal fluency test — fruits.

Table 2. Sample performance.

Clinical group (M±SD) Control group (M±SD) t-test p-value Cohen’s d
MoCA-B 24.7±3.7 26.07±2.81 −1.821 0.073 −0.426
Vocabulary 45.12±10.3 49.1±9.93 −1.692 0.095 −0.394
BNT 44.48±7.75 48.02±7.45 −1.995 0.05* −0.467
Animals 15.7±5.07 16.5±4.19 −0.756 0.452 −0.176
Fruits 12.12±2.99 13.14±3.38 −1.368 0.176 −0.318
BCSB 8.21±1.81 8.55±1.92 −0.726 0.471 −0.179

M: mean; SD: standard deviation; MoCA-B: Montreal Cognitive Assessment-Basic; BNT: Boston Naming Test; BCSB: Brief Cognitive Screening Battery. *p<0.05.

In Table 3, the clinical group was subdivided into mild depression (n=24) and severe depression (n=9) and was compared with the control group. The ANOVA showed significant differences in BNT between the three groups. Post-hoc analysis (Bonferroni test) showed that the difference was significant between the control group and the severe depression group (p=0.04) but not between the mild depression and severe depression groups (p=0.427). The control group and the mild depression group showed no significant difference (p=0.538). The analysis also showed the average effect size in BNT.

Table 3. Comparison between the clinical groups and the control group about applied tests.

Mild depression (M±SD) Severe depression (M±SD) Control (M±SD) F-test p-value Ƞ2
MoCA-B 24.6±3.63 25.1±3.86 26.1±2,81 1.76 0.179 0.047
Vocabulary 45.5±10.8 42.3±10 49.1±9.93 2.09 0.131 0.054
BNT 45.4±8.04 41.1±6.03 48±7.45 3.39 0.039* 0.086
Animals 15.9±5.17 14.4±4.88 16.5±4.19 0.762 0.470 0.020
Fruits 12.2±3.23 11.3±2.4 13.1±3.38 1.43 0.245 0.038
BCSB 8.33±1.62 7.88±2.23 8.55±1.92 0.456 0.636 0.014

M: mean; SD: standard deviation; MoCA-B: Montreal Cognitive Assessment-Basic; BNT: Boston Naming Test; BCSB: Brief Cognitive Screening Battery. *p<0.05.

The correlations between the tests that evaluate semantic memory and the depression scales are shown in Table 4. In this regard, there is a significant correlation (small) between GDS-30 and BNT (r=-0.30; p<0.01).

Table 4. Correlations.

Vocabulary BNT Animals Fruits BCSB
BNT 0.64***
Animals 0.51*** 0.58***
Fruits 0.35** 0.48*** 0.54***
BCSB 0.34** 0.51*** 0.36** 0.34**
GDS-30 −0.27* −0.30** −0.13 −0.14 −0.04
BDI-II −0.196 −0.26* −0.14 −0.05 −0.12

GDS-30: Geriatric Depression Scale-30; BDI-II: Beck Depression Inventory-II; BNT: Boston Naming Test; BCSB: Brief Cognitive Screening Battery. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, and ***p<0.001.

Table 5 presents the multiple linear regression model performed. The age, education, gender, and GDS-30 score of the variables were selected as predictors for the BNT. The model showed R-squared=0.61 and adjusted R-squared=0.37.

Table 5. Multiple linear regression model.

Predictor Slope SE 95%CI
Lower Upper t-test p-value
Interception 60.089 11.191 37.763 82.4153 5.37 <0.001*
GDS −0.278 0.114 −0.507 −0.0502 −2.43 0.018
Age −0.245 0.138 −0.520 0.0298 −1.78 0.080
Schooling 0.629 0.172 0.285 0.9729 3.65 <0.001*
Sex MAL–FEM 3.566 2.606 −1.634 8.7650 1.37 0.176

95%CI: 95% confidence interval; MAL: male; FEM: female; GDS: Geriatric Depression Scale. *p<0.05.

DISCUSSION

This objective of the study was to analyze the relationship between depressive symptoms and semantic memory in a community-based sample of elderly adults. The results showed a significant difference between the clinical (with depression symptoms) and control groups only in BNT. A significant negative correlation was found between GDS-30 and BNT, indicating that the higher the score on the scale, the lower the BNT score. These results support the initial hypothesis that the severity of depressive symptoms affects the impairment of semantic memory.

The results in the present study corroborate some studies in the literature.10 , 11 , 12 On the other hand, these findings contradict some studies.13 , 40 The systematic review by Elderkin-Thompson et al.13 did not find a decline in semantic memory in patients with depression. Koening et al.40 showed a similar result in the verbal fluency test compared with that of people who were never depressed. In the present study, difficulties with verbal fluency were not observed.

An explanation for this discrepancy is related to the characteristics, particularly demographic characteristics, of the clinical samples. Sheline et al.41 suggested that age, education, and sex are associated with the GDS-30 score. Another possible explanation is related to the duration of depressive symptoms. Herrmann et al.42 showed that participants who have lived with the disorder for a longer time tend to have more significant semantic memory changes than controls, but when compared with patients with depression they perform similarly.

However, how can depressive symptoms affect semantic memory? Semantic memory is a neural network of connections distributed across the sensory, motor, and linguistic areas.43 There are three views of how this network would organize: (1) distributed-only suggests that it would organize according to the neuroanatomy of related systems; (2) distributed plus-hub view defines that there would be a central activation axis formed by a group of amodal neurons that would coordinate the connections and cross information;44 and (3) hub-and-spoke, which proposed the existence of a radial rather than a distributed model, that is, the amodal center (hub) would be located in the anterior temporal lobe and interact with the rays (spokes) that would be specific regions for the modality.45

In other words, the naming task requires the subject to observe the image, activate the sensory, motor, and linguistic areas related to this object, cross the information and correctly identify it, and demand the semantic neural network. Depression implies inhibition of the nervous system; it theorized that semantic connections are also affected by this state, making it more difficult for these patients to access the correct name of the object or, in some cases, increasing the response latency. Neuroimaging studies have found functional abnormalities that affect regulation neurocircuits in patients with depression, involving changes in connectivity between the cortical regions such as the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and subcortical regions amygdala and hippocampus.46 When triggered, semantic memory demands the activation of the PFC to evoke information, so, in theory, depression could directly influence this process, and the longer the symptoms last without a proper treatment, the more efficiently the clinical symptoms would be observed.

Cognitive factors, such as recall, semantic representation, and executive functions, in the structure and processing of semantic memory during the aging process are still a topic of debate.47 Some studies have shown that executive functions have a “top-down” effect on basic cognitive processes in the presence of depressive symptoms.48 , 49 However, there is evidence that depression alone is due to the impairment of semantic memory.

This study had some limitations. It is important to note that there are many variables to be considered in a patient with depression, for example, when depression was diagnosed, medication, and the effect of nonpharmacological treatments. In this study, these variables were not controlled. A cohort study would allow a more detailed understanding of the effects of depressive symptoms on semantic memory.

In summary, this study analyzed the relationship between depressive symptoms and semantic memory. The results showed that deficits in semantic memory increase in proportion to the severity of depressive symptoms. The implications of this study suggest that in cognitive assessments and the presence of depressive symptoms in the elderly people, possible deficits in semantic memory should be considered.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to thank the University Press Office for their assistance in disseminating the research and the Applied Psychology Center for providing the essential space, materials, and service. We thank the National Council for the Improvement of Higher Education (CAPES) for financial support.

Footnotes

1

This study was conducted by the Department of Psychology, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Curitiba, PR, Brazil.

Funding: National Council for the Improvement of Higher Education (CAPES), process number 08856021960.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Ma L. Depression, anxiety, and apathy in mild cognitive impairment: current perspectives. Front Aging Neurosci. 2020;12:9. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lima CM, Alves HV, Mograbi DC, Pereira FF, Fernandez JL, Charchat-Fichman H. Performance on cognitive tests, instrumental activities of daily living and depressive symptoms of a community-based sample of elderly adults in Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. Dement Neuropsychol. 2017;11(1):54–81. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642016dn11-010009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lee CH, Kim DH, Moon YS. Differential associations between depression and cognitive function in MCI and AD: a cross-sectional study. Int Psychogeriatr. 2019;31(8):1151–58. doi: 10.1017/s1041610218001527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Araujo NB, Moraes HS, Silveira H, Arcoverde C, Vasques PE, Barca ML, et al. Impaired cognition in depression and Alzheimer (AD): a gradiente from depression to depresssion in AD. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2014;72(9):671–9. doi: 10.1590/0004-282x20140108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Moon B, Kim S, Park YH, Lim JS, Youn YC, Kim S, et al. Depressive symptoms are associated with progression to dementia in patients with amyloid-positive mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2017;58(4):1255–64. doi: 10.3233/jad-170225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ismail Z, Elbayoumi H, Fischer CE, Hogan DB, Millikin CP, Schweizer T, et al. Prevalence of depression in patients with mild cognitive impairment. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(1):58–67. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Thomas AJ, Gallagher P, Robinson LJ, Porter RJ, Young AH, Ferrier IN, et al. A comparison of neurocognitive impairment in younger and older adults with major depression. Psychol Med. 2009;39(5):725–33. doi: 10.1017/s0033291708004042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Amieva H, Le Goff M, Millet X, Orgogozo JM, Peres K, Barberger-Gateau P, et al. Prodromal Alzheimer’s disease: successive emergence of the clinical symptoms. Ann Neurol. 2008;64(5):492–8. doi: 10.1002/ana.21509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Naismith SL, Hickie IB, Turner K, Little CL, Winter V, Ward PB, et al. Neuropsychological performance in patients with depression is associated with clinical, etiological, and genetic risk factors. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2003;25(6):866–77. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.6.866.16472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Brunet J, Hudon C, Macoir J, Belleville S, Rousseau F, Bouchard RW, et al. The relation between depressive symptoms and semantic memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and late-life depression. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17(5):865–74. doi: 10.1017/s1355617711000877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Callahan BL, Joubert S, Tremblay M, Macoir J, Belleville S, Bouchard R, et al. Semantic memory impairment for biological and man-made objects in individuals with Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment or Late-life Depression. J Geriatr Psychiatr Neurol. 2015;28(2):108–16. doi: 10.1177/0891988714554708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Vogel A, Johannsen P, Stokholm J, Jorgensen K. Frequency and severity of semantic deficits in a consecutive memory clinic cohort. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2014;38(3-4):214–23. doi: 10.1159/000357794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Elderkin-Thompson V, Boone KB, Hwang S, Kumar A. Neurocognitive profiles in elderly patients with frontotemporal degeneration or major depressive disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(5):753–71. doi: 10.1017/s1355617704105067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Donovan NJ, Amariglio RE, Zoller AS, Rudel RK, Gomez-Isla T, Blacker D, et al. Subjective cognitive concerns and neuropsychiatric predictors of progression to the early clinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014;22(12):1642–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2014.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kassel MT, Rao JA, Walker SJ, Briceño EM, Gabriel LB, Weldon AL, et al. Decreased Fronto-Limbic activation and disrupted semantic-cued list learning in major depressive disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2016;22(4):412–25. doi: 10.1017/s1355617716000023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Morimoto SS, Gunning FM, Kanellopoulos D, Murphy CF, Klimstra SA, Kelly RE, et al. Semantic organizational strategy predicts verbal memory and remission rate of geriatric depression. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;27(5):506–12. doi: 10.1002/gps.2743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Segertrom SC, Geiger PJ, Boggero IA, Schmitt FA, Sephton SE. Endogenous cortisol exposure and declarative verbal memory: a longitudinal study of healthy older adults. Psychosom Med. 2016;78(2):182–91. doi: 10.1097/psy.0000000000000249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Singh TD, Duffy JR, Strand EA, Machulda MM, Whitwell JL, Josephs KA. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in primary progressive aphasia and apraxia of speech. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2015;39(3-4):228–38. doi: 10.1159/000369062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Yesavage YA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17(1):37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatr. 1961;4(6):561–71. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Julayanont P, Tangwongchai S, Hemrungrojn S, Tunvirachaisakul C, Phanthumchinda K, Hongsawat J, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment - Basic: a screening tool for mild cognitive impairment in illiterate and low-educated elderly adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63:2550–4. doi: 10.1111/jgs.13820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wechsler D. WASI: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Trentini CM, Yates DB, Heck VS. Manual de aplicação: WASI – Escala Wechsler Abreviada de Inteligência. 1a ed. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Stoppe A, Júnior, Jacaob W, Filho, Louza MR., Neto Avaliação de depressão em idosos através da “Escala de Depressão em Geriatria”: resultados preliminares. Rev ABP-APAL. 1994;4(16):149–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sousa RL, Medeiros JG, Moura AC, Souza CL, Moreira IF. Validade e fidedignidade da Escala de Depressão Geriátrica na identificação de idosos deprimidos em um hospital geral. J Bras Psiquiatr. 2007;56(2):102–07. doi: 10.1590/s0047-20852007000200005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gorenstein C, Pang WY, Argimon IL, Werlang BSG. BDI-II - Inventário de depressão de Beck - Manual. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Personal Assess. 1996;67(3):588–97. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mansur LL, Radanovic M, Araújo GC, Taquemori LY, Greco LL. Teste de nomeação de Boston: desempenho de uma população de São Paulo. Pró-Fono. 2006;18(1):13–20. doi: 10.1590/s0104-56872006000100003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Miotto EC, Sato J, Lucia MC, Camargo CH, Scaff M. Development of an adapted version of the Boston Naming Test for Portuguese speakers. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2010;32(3):279–82. doi: 10.1590/s1516-44462010005000006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Leite KS, Miotto EC, Nitrini R, Yassuda MS. Boston Naming Test (BNT) original, Brazilian adapted version and short forms: normative data for illiterate and low-educated older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2016;29(5):825–33. doi: 10.1017/s1041610216001952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Brucki SM, Malheiros SM, Okamoto IH, Bertolucci PH. Dados normativos para o teste de fluência verbal categoria animais em nosso meio. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 1997;3(1):56–61. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x1997000100009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Nitrini R, Lefèvre BH, Mathias SC, Caramelli P, Carrilho PE, Sauaia N, et al. Testes neuropsicológicos de aplicação simples para o diagnóstico de demência. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 1994;52(4):457–65. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x1994000400001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Aprahamian I, Martinelli JE, Cecato JF, Yassuda MS. Screening for Alzheimer’s Disease among illiterate elderly: accuracy analysis for multiple instruments. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011;25:1–9. doi: 10.3233/jad-2011-110125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Caramelli P, Nitrini R, Carthery-Goulart MT, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Bahia VS. Teste de fluência verbal no diagnóstico da doença de Alzheimer leve: notas de corte em função da escolaridade. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61(2):32. doi: 10.1097/wad.0b013e31802f244f. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Herrera E, Júnior, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Carthery MT, et al. Performance of illiterate and literate nondemented elderly subjects in two tests of long-term memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(4):634–38. doi: 10.1017/s1355617704104062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Jamovi Project . Jamovi (version 0.8.2.3) 2018. [software] Available at: https://www.jamovi.org. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112(1):155–9. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Koening AM, Bhalla RK, Butters MA. Cognitive functioning and late-life depression. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(5):461–7. doi: 10.1017/s1355617714000198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sheline YI, Barch DM, Garcia K, Gersing CP, Welsh-Bohmer K, Steffens DC, et al. Cognitive function in late life depression: relationships to depression severity, cerebrovascular risk factors, and processing speed. Biol Psychiatr. 2006;60(1):58–65. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Herrmann LL, Goodwin GM, Ebmeier KP. The cognitive neuropsychology of depression in the elderly. Psychol Med. 2007;37(12):1693–702. doi: 10.1017/s0033291707001134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Patterson K, Nestor PJ, Rogers T. Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(12):976–87. doi: 10.1038/nrn2277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Nastase SA, Haxby JV. In: Learning and memory: a comprehensive reference. 2nd. Byrne JN, editor. Oxford; Elsevier: 2017. Structural basis of semantic memory. editor. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Pobric G, Jefferies E, Lambon Ralph MA. Category-specific versus category-general semantic impairment induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Curr Biol. 2010;20(10):964–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Dean J, Keshavan M. The neurobiology of depression: an integrated view. Asian J Psychiatr. 2017;27:101–11. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2017.01.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wulff DU, De Deyne S, Jones MN, Mata R, Aging Lexicon Consortium New Perspectives on the Aging Lexicon. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23(8):686–98. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Morimoto SS, Gunning FM, Murphy CF, Kanellopoulos D, Kelly RE, Alexopoulos GS. Executive function and short-term remission of geriatric depression: the role of semantic strategy. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;19(2):115–22. doi: 10.1097/jgp.0b013e3181e751c4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Cristancho P, Lenze EJ, Dixon D, Miller DJ, Mulsant BH, Reynolds CF, 3rd, et al. Executive Function Predicts Antidepressant Treatment Noncompletion in Late-Life Depression. J Clin Psychiatry. 2018;79(3):16m11371. doi: 10.4088/jcp.16m11371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Dementia & Neuropsychologia are provided here courtesy of Academia Brasileira de Neurologia

RESOURCES