Abstract
Background
Suboptimal diets are a leading risk factor for death and disability. Nutrition labelling is a potential method to encourage consumers to improve dietary behaviour. This systematic review and network meta-analysis (NMA) summarises evidence on the impact of colour-coded interpretive labels and warning labels on changing consumers’ purchasing behaviour.
Methods and findings
We conducted a literature review of peer-reviewed articles published between 1 January 1990 and 24 May 2021 in PubMed, Embase via Ovid, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, and SCOPUS. Randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and quasi-experimental studies were included for the primary outcomes (measures of changes in consumers’ purchasing and consuming behaviour). A frequentist NMA method was applied to pool the results. A total of 156 studies (including 101 RCTs and 55 non-RCTs) nested in 138 articles were incorporated into the systematic review, of which 134 studies in 120 articles were eligible for meta-analysis. We found that the traffic light labelling system (TLS), nutrient warning (NW), and health warning (HW) were associated with an increased probability of selecting more healthful products (odds ratios [ORs] and 95% confidence intervals [CIs]: TLS, 1.5 [1.2, 1.87]; NW, 3.61 [2.82, 4.63]; HW, 1.65 [1.32, 2.06]). Nutri-Score (NS) and warning labels appeared effective in reducing consumers’ probability of selecting less healthful products (NS, 0.66 [0.53, 0.82]; NW,0.65 [0.54, 0.77]; HW,0.64 [0.53, 0.76]). NS and NW were associated with an increased overall healthfulness (healthfulness ratings of products purchased using models such as FSAm-NPS/HCSP) by 7.9% and 26%, respectively. TLS, NS, and NW were associated with a reduced energy (total energy: TLS, −6.5%; NS, −6%; NW, −12.9%; energy per 100 g/ml: TLS, −3%; NS, −3.5%; NW, −3.8%), sodium (total sodium/salt: TLS, −6.4%; sodium/salt per 100 g/ml: NS: −7.8%), fat (total fat: NS, −15.7%; fat per 100 g/ml: TLS: −2.6%; NS: −3.2%), and total saturated fat (TLS, −12.9%; NS: −17.1%; NW: −16.3%) content of purchases. The impact of TLS, NS, and NW on purchasing behaviour could be explained by improved understanding of the nutrition information, which further elicits negative perception towards unhealthful products or positive attitudes towards healthful foods. Comparisons across label types suggested that colour-coded labels performed better in nudging consumers towards the purchase of more healthful products (NS versus NW: 1.51 [1.08, 2.11]), while warning labels have the advantage in discouraging unhealthful purchasing behaviour (NW versus TLS: 0.81 [0.67, 0.98]; HW versus TLS: 0.8 [0.63, 1]). Study limitations included high heterogeneity and inconsistency in the comparisons across different label types, limited number of real-world studies (95% were laboratory studies), and lack of long-term impact assessments.
Conclusions
Our systematic review provided comprehensive evidence for the impact of colour-coded labels and warnings in nudging consumers’ purchasing behaviour towards more healthful products and the underlying psychological mechanism of behavioural change. Each type of label had different attributes, which should be taken into consideration when making front-of-package nutrition labelling (FOPL) policies according to local contexts. Our study supported mandatory front-of-pack labelling policies in directing consumers’ choice and encouraging the food industry to reformulate their products.
Protocol registry
PROSPERO (CRD42020161877).
Jing Song and co-workers report a systematic review and network meta-analysis assessing evidence on food labeling and purchasing decisions.
Author summary
Why was this study done?
Interpretive front-of-package labelling is considered a cost-effective strategy to promote a more healthful diet and mitigate the burden of non communicable diseases (NCDs), and colour-coded labels and warning labels are the most adopted interpretive front-of-package labelling schemes worldwide.
Prior to this study, evidence on the impact of each type of colour-coded labels and warning labels on modifying consumers purchasing behaviour was mixed.
The feasibility and likely effectiveness of each label type applied in different contexts was unclear.
What did the researchers do and find?
This network meta-analysis summarised the currently available 118 peer-reviewed studies to update knowledge of the most mainstream interpretive front-of-package nutrition labelling (FOPL) schemes.
We found that the traffic light labelling system (TLS), Nutri-Score (NS), nutrient warning (NW), and health warning (HW) were all able to direct consumers towards more healthful purchasing behaviour.
Colour-coded labels (TLS and NS) performed better in promoting the purchase of more healthful products, while warning labels (NW and HW) had the advantage in discouraging unhealthful purchasing behaviour.
The difference in consumers’ behaviour could be explained by different underlying psychological mechanisms for each label.
What do these findings mean?
We provide more comprehensive evidence to guide policy-makers in choosing the optimal front-of-package labelling policies. This evidence synthesis may inform further generalisation of mandatory front-of-package labelling schemes and help to mitigate the burden of NCDs.
Future studies should focus on the impact of FOPLs on dietary consumption in individuals, and industrial reformulation at the population level, especially in real-world settings and over a longer time frame. This will provide crucial, robust, and comprehensive evidence to guide policy making.
Introduction
Suboptimal diets, linked to food environments that promote food and drink high in salt, sugar, and saturated fat, are a leading risk factor for death and disability worldwide, due to their relationship with non communicable diseases (NCDs) [1–6]. Nearly 8 million deaths in 2019 were attributable to dietary risk factors such as high salt intake and low wholegrain intake. To mitigate the healthcare burden resulting from NCDs, providing clear information about the nutritional profile of products is a recognised method to nudge consumers to more healthful food and drink options and exert pressure on manufacturers to carry out reformulation to improve the nutritional profile of their products [7,8]. As a minimum, many countries have mandatory nutrition tables on the back of food packaging [9], but the World Health Organization (WHO) additionally recommends front-of-pack nutrition labelling (FOPL) to promote healthful diets and help reduce NCD prevalence [10–13]. FOPL provides key nutritional information, typically including calorie, saturated fat, salt, and sugar content, in a visible format [14], and many countries have a voluntary FOPL system in place [15]. FOPL generally falls into 2 main categories—interpretive and noninterpretive. Interpretive labels present symbols, figures, or cautionary text to indicate the overall healthfulness or nutrient content of a product, such as the Nutri-Score (NS) label [16], Chilean style warning labels [17], Health Star Ratings (Australia and New Zealand), and the “traffic light” labelling system (TLS). For example, the United Kingdom’s TLS has red (high), amber (medium), or green (low) labels to indicate levels of total fat, saturated fat, sugars, and salt [18]. Noninterpretive FOPL systems, such as the Guideline Daily Amount, convey nutritional content as numbers rather than graphics, symbols, or colours, allowing consumers to create their own judgements on healthfulness.
At the time of completing this manuscript, a total of 31 countries had implemented interpretive FOPL systems, including 6 countries that had adopted mandatory warning labels on packaged foods and 3 countries that had utilised mandatory colour-coded FOPL systems [19]. Interpretive warning labels and colour-coded labels were the most adopted labels endorsed by governments. So far, real-world evidence is limited and mainly focused on the Chilean style warning label (a type of interpretative nutrient warning (NW) label). Observational studies found that Chile’s Law of Food Labelling and Advertising, which included the mandatory implementation of warning labels nationwide, resulted in lower sales of beverages high in sugars, salt, saturated fat, or energy [20] and was likely to improve understanding and utilisation, especially in children [21]. However, no data are available on changes in consumption.
Based on the health communication theory and previous similar studies [22–26] (Fig 1), visual attention to labels is a prerequisite for the perception and understanding of FOPLs, but the mechanisms linking label interpretation and behavioural changes differ across types of FOPLs. Warning labels may elicit negative perception of unhealthful foods (e.g., perception of severe risk, lower grade of healthfulness, lower level or frequency of recommended consumption for unhealthful products) in the process of changing food choice. However, interpretative colour-coded labels (e.g., TLS) tend to modify purchasing behaviour by increasing the perception of healthfulness for healthier food options. The effects of FOPLs are also modified by study population demographics, knowledge of nutrition labels, frequency of grocery shopping, familiarity with the brand, level of weight consciousness and health status, product categories, and characteristics of labels [23–28]. For example, women and people with special diet needs or higher perceived nutrition knowledge are more likely to look at FOPLs; the provision of serving size information or percentage of recommended intake may add to consumers’ difficulty in understanding FOPLs. Due to the abovementioned reasons, experimental evidence regarding the effectiveness of FOPL in modifying consumers’ purchasing or consuming behaviour was mixed [14,23–40]. Meta-analyses showed that interpretative colour-coded FOPLs (e.g., TLS) significantly increased consumers’ selection of more healthful options from a range of products, as well as decreased calorie and salt content of food purchased [31,41], but no synthesised results for warning labels were available. Experimental studies also provided some insights into the underlying mechanisms of how colour-coded and warning labels change consumers’ behaviour, suggesting that warning labels effectively elicited negative emotions and raised health awareness, which led to the ultimate modifications in food choice; on the other hand, colour-coded labels (e.g., TLS) were indicated to have a favourable effect on increasing consumers’ preference for healthier foods [25,27,28,31].
To summarise the existing findings and provide evidence for policy-makers in the proposed implementation or modification of food labelling policies, we aim to assess the impact of colour-coded and warning labels, the most studied and promising labels, on changes in both intended and actual purchasing and consumption behaviour. We also aim to gain insight into the underlying psychological mechanism based on the health communication theory to explore the heterogeneity across label types [22–26].
Methods
The protocol of this systematic review was registered on PROSPERO (CRD42020161877).
Search strategy
The systematic review was conducted and reported in compliance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) Network Meta-analysis guideline (S1 PRISMA Checklist). We searched 4 databases (PubMed, Embase via Ovid, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, and SCOPUS) initially on 26 November 2019 using the following search strategy: (“food and beverages” OR “food” OR “drink” OR “beverage” OR “meal” OR “nutrition” OR “menu” OR “restaurant”) AND (“warning” OR “traffic light” OR “Wheel of health” OR “colored GDA” OR “coloured GDA” OR “5 CNL” OR “Color Nutrition” OR “Nutriscore” OR “Nutri-score” OR “simplified nutrition labelling system” OR “SENS” OR “colour-coded” OR “color-coded” OR “colour coding” OR “color coding” OR “red label” OR “green label” OR “Evolved Nutrition Label”). There was no restriction on language. The search was updated on 24 May 2021 to capture the latest publications up to the submission of manuscript. Additional articles were identified by reviewing the bibliographic reference of identified articles. Articles of all types published from 1 January 1990 were included at this stage.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria, data extraction, and bias assessment
Original studies were selected based on the population, intervention, comparator, outcome, and study design (PICOS) framework (Table 1). All randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and quasi-experimental studies assessing 4 types of interpretative FOPLs (TLS, NS, NW, and HW) were included. In addition, cross-sectional and cohort studies were included in the assessment of secondary outcomes (see “Measurement of outcomes”). We followed the criteria of a previous meta-analysis, which set a control group by merging both Nutrition Facts table (NFt, also known as Nutrition Information Panel in some countries) and no-label condition into the control group in the main analyses, so as to increase the statistical power and precision of network meta-analysis (NMA) [25,42]. Studies that featured other interpretive or noninterpretive FOPLs (e.g., Guideline Daily Amount, Health Star Rating) as the reference group were not eligible as they differed too much from the control group specified (i.e., back-of-package labels and no-label control). Reviews, study protocols, and conference abstracts were also excluded.
Table 1. PICOS criteria for inclusion and exclusion of studies.
Criteria | Description |
---|---|
Population | General population |
Intervention | Inclusion: |
1. All colour-coded labels and warning front-of-package labels: TLS, NS, NW, and HW; | |
2. For each of TLS, NW, and HW, different formats were also separately included into analysis: summary TLS versus nutrient-specific TLS, negative message framing versus positive message framing, and textual warning versus textual + nontextual warning (e.g., picture of tooth decay, stop sign). | |
Exclusion: | |
1. Other colour-coded labels (e.g., Wheel of health, ENL, and SENS) were not included because they had been scrapped or no longer actively developed due to political reasons or lack of public popularity. | |
2. Studies that combined effects of nutrition labels of included types and excluded types (e.g., TLS + HSR). | |
3. Studies investigated the combined effect of nutrition labelling and other intervention (e.g., TLS + food placement). | |
4. Studies that assessed only the effect of nutritional education using traffic light label as educational tools were not included. | |
5. Studies that applied colour-coding schemes (traffic light label score, NS) only to categorise the healthfulness of products were excluded. | |
Comparator | 1. Absence of nutritional information (no-label) |
2. Only NFt present | |
Outcome | All measured outcomes were included based on the health communication model. |
Study design | 1. RCTs, including randomised controlled crossover trials and clustered randomised trials |
2. Quasi-experimental studies: | |
• Nonequivalent control group design | |
• Interrupted time series design | |
• One-group prepost design | |
• Choice-based conjoint studies | |
3. Cross-sectional studies evaluating at least 2 intervention labels, or 1 intervention labels and control condition (no-label exposure or NFt): only eligible for the secondary outcomes. |
ENL, evolved nutrition label; FOPL, front-of-package nutrition labelling; HSR, Health Star Rating; HW, health warning; NFt, Nutrition Facts table; NS, Nutri-Score; NW, nutrient warning; PICOS, population, intervention, comparator, outcome, and study design; RCT, randomised controlled trial; TLS, traffic light labelling system.
To select eligible studies, titles, abstracts, and main texts were reviewed based on the eligibility criteria. The bibliographic references of the eligible articles were also reviewed to identify additional articles missed out by the database search strategy. Data extraction was carried out using an extraction spreadsheet, consisting of the following variables: publication year, language, country of study, study design, sample size, setting (real-world setting that utilises the sales data before and after the implementation of the actual labelling intervention in retail outlets, or controlled laboratory settings based on a virtual food environment), race, mean age or age range, percentage of female, education, income, occupation, socioeconomics, investigated food types, number of food types (single or multiple), intervention condition, control condition, access to NFt for both intervention and control groups, time pressure, outcome, measure of outcome, and effect estimate.
Bias was assessed using the revised Cochrane risk-of-bias tool for randomised trials (ROB 2) for RCTs [43], Risk Of Bias In Non-randomised Studies of Interventions (ROBINS-I) for quasi-experimental studies [44], and National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies [45] for cross-sectional studies. The overall risk-of-bias judgement for each study was summarised as low risk of bias if the study received the assessment of “low risk” for all domains, and high risk of bias if the study was judged to be at high risk in at least one domain. Studies with insufficient information to assess the risk of bias for some domains was categorised into “some concerns” (S1 Text and S10–S12 Tables).
Inclusion and exclusion, data extraction, and risk of bias were first assessed by 2 independent reviewers (JS and MB). A total of 16 discrepancies (13.6%) was found and were referred to a third reviewer (MT).
Measurement of outcomes
Based on the theory conceptualised in Fig 1, we grouped the outcome measures into 3 categories: (1) changes in consumers’ purchasing and consumption behaviour; (2) consumers’ perception and attitudes towards products; and (3) consumers’ attention, understanding, and perception of colour-coded and warning labels (S1 Table). The primary outcomes of our systematic review were measures regarding the changes in consumers’ purchasing and consuming behaviour, which contain the probability of choosing less healthful or more healthful products, self-reported ratings of purchase intention, overall healthfulness of products purchased, and energy and nutrient (salt/sodium, sugar, fat, and saturated fat) content of products purchased/consumed. The healthful and unhealthful products were grouped based on the levels of salt/sodium, sugar, saturated fat, and calories indicated by the front-of-package labelling systems. Products with warning texts, symbols, or colours (e.g., red in multiple traffic light label) indicating high levels of sugar, salt, saturated fat, or calorie content per 100 g or per 100 ml (per 100 g/ml) relative to reference intake, or overall low healthfulness (e.g. red in single traffic light, or orange and red [D and E] in NS), were defined as unhealthful. A list of measures was included in the other 2 secondary outcome domains (S1 Table). Data on the comparisons between intervention labels and control group or pairwise comparisons between any 2 types of intervention labels (e.g., TLS versus NW) were collected. Depending on the results reported in the original studies, we calculated odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) as the summary estimates for categorical outcomes, and relative mean differences (RMDs, the percentage of change comparing intervention and control group) plus standard errors (SEs) for continuous outcomes [46] (S1 Text and S1 Table).
Data synthesis and network meta-analysis
As multiple nutrition labels were evaluated, and several multiarmed trials were included in our analysis, we used the frequentist NMA method to synthesise studies and make both direct (observed) and indirect (unobserved) comparisons of multiple interventions [47,48]. Random-effect models were fitted in the NMA as we assumed the heterogeneity in our network model was high. Cochran’s Q-statistic and Higgin’s and Thompson’s I2 were applied to assess the degree of total heterogeneity in the network model, which was further divided into within- (conventional between-study heterogeneity) and between-design (overall inconsistency) variations, respectively. To test the transitivity and consistency assumptions underlying NMA, we further calculated the Q-statistic in a full design-by-treatment intervention random-effect inconsistency model [49]. When there was inconsistency between the effect estimate of direct and indirect comparisons, we took the direct effect estimate into consideration instead. We also applied the method of separate indirect from direct evidence (SIDE), generating the proportion of direct evidence for each comparison (local inconsistency) [50]. A detailed explanation of the NMA can be found in the S1 Text.
In addition to the analysis of 4 main label types (TLS, NS, NW, and HW) for each outcome measure, we also synthesised the evidence grouped by labelling formats and framing. TLS was further divided into summary TLS and detailed TLS (summary TLS was defined as the single TLS indicating the overall nutritional quality as good, medium, and poor using predefined algorithms; detailed TLS included information on individual nutrients). NW and HW were further categorised into textual or nontextual NW/HW (nontextual NW/HW included graphs and symbols that alert consumers to the high levels of nutrient content or health risks associated with high nutrient content). In addition, HW was also divided into negative or positive, depending on the framing of the warning (positive framing highlighted the health benefits resulting from lowered consumption of a nutrient; negative emphasised the harm associated with excessive consumption of a nutrient). To explore the presence of effect modifiers leading to overall inconsistency between direct and indirect comparisons, subgroup analyses by sex composition (primarily female: female >60%; primary male: female <40%; otherwise equally distributed), age group (primary adults: >70% aged 18 or older; primary children and adolescent: >70% aged less than 18; otherwise general population), setting (real-world or laboratory setting), types of products investigated (single/multiple), and display NFt both in intervention were conducted if at least 2 studies were available for quantitative synthesis in every stratum for each primary outcome measure. Sensitivity analyses of the primary outcomes were conducted by (1) evaluating only RCTs and (2) removing studies using NFt control. We did not perform meta-regression adjusting the effect modifier variables, given that there were a low number of studies available for most outcomes.
A comparison-adjusted funnel was plotted to assess the risk of publication bias under the priori hypothesis that studies identifying the superiority of newly developed labels to an existing labelling system tend to be published in higher frequency [50]. The funnel plot was only created when there were at least 10 studies available for each outcome, as recommended by Cochrane [51]. The results were considered statistically significant when p < 0.05.
All the statistical analyses were implemented in R v3.5.1. The NMA was conducted using the netmeta v1.2–1 R package [48].
Results
Study characteristics
Using the search strategy mentioned above, we obtained 15,058 records from the 4 databases. After removing duplicate articles and excluding ineligible articles based on the title, abstract, and main text, and identifying studies in the bibliographic references of the eligible articles, 156 studies nested in 138 articles remained (S2 Table), of which 22 studies were excluded because the results reported in original papers were incomplete (e.g., missing standard deviations/SEs) and the authors were unable to provide the complete results after contacting via email. In total, 134 studies were eligible for quantitative synthesis (Fig 2). The details of included studies are presented in S1 Data.
Of the 156 studies eligible for systematic review, 154 were published in English, and 2 in Spanish. The majority of articles were published 2018 to present (64%), and the most common study populations were in the regions of Europe (31%), Latin America (28%), and North America (24%). Most studies were carried out in a laboratory setting (95%). Of the studies based in real-world settings (5%), half were conducted in the out-of-home sector and half in retail outlets. Most studies were RCTs (65%). None of the studies were industry funded.
The majority of studies assessed TLS (62%), while 40% studies investigated NW, 22% studied NS, and 17% evaluated HW (Fig 3). For comparators, 80% studies used a no-label condition. Most studies (78%) did not provide NFt along with the labels in the intervention group. Most studies used multiple types of foods and drinks to assess the effect of labels (56%), and 40% focused on a single type. Nearly all studies leveraged individual-level data (97%), and only 4 studies were based on sales data. Most studies had a population of primarily adults (88%) (Table 2).
Table 2. Characteristics of studies included in the systematic review (n = 135).
Study characteristics | Number of studies | Proportion (%) |
---|---|---|
Publication year | ||
2021 | 17 | 10.9 |
2020 | 26 | 16.67 |
2019 | 32 | 20.51 |
2018 | 25 | 16.03 |
2017 | 8 | 5.13 |
2016 | 9 | 5.77 |
2015 | 9 | 5.77 |
2014 | 8 | 5.13 |
2013 | 5 | 3.21 |
2012 | 4 | 2.56 |
2011 | 5 | 3.21 |
2010 and before | 8 | 5.13 |
Language | ||
English | 154 | 98.72 |
2 | 1.28 | |
Country | ||
US | 27 | 17.31 |
Uruguay | 19 | 12.18 |
France | 16 | 10.26 |
Canada | 11 | 7.05 |
Australia | 10 | 6.41 |
UK | 9 | 5.77 |
New Zealand | 7 | 4.49 |
Chile | 7 | 4.49 |
Brazil | 7 | 4.49 |
Germany | 6 | 3.85 |
Ecuador | 5 | 3.21 |
Switzerland | 3 | 1.92 |
Mexico | 3 | 1.92 |
Other | 19 | 16.67 |
Region | ||
Europe | 49 | 31.41 |
Latin America | 43 | 27.56 |
North America | 38 | 24.36 |
Oceania | 17 | 10.9 |
Asia | 4 | 2.56 |
Africa | 1 | 0.64 |
Multiple | 4 | 2.56 |
Setting | ||
Laboratory | 148 | 94.87 |
Real-world | 8 | 5.13 |
Out-of-home sectors* | 4 | 2.56 |
Retailer outlets | 4 | 2.56 |
Study design | ||
RCT | 101 | 64.74 |
Quasi-experimental studies and cross-sectional survey | 55 | 35.26 |
Intervention label | ||
TLS | 97 | 62.18 |
NW | 62 | 39.74 |
NS | 35 | 22.44 |
HW | 27 | 17.31 |
Control condition | ||
No label | 125 | 80.13 |
NFt | 12 | 7.69 |
Compared against each other colour-coded or warning labels | 19 | 12.18 |
Types of foods and drinks tested | ||
Multiple | 87 | 55.77 |
Single | 62 | 39.74 |
Not stated | 7 | 17.31 |
Research data | ||
Individual data | 152 | 97.44 |
Sales data | 4 | 2.56 |
Age group | ||
Primarily adults | 134 | 88.16 |
Primarily children or adolescents | 5 | 3.29 |
Mixed population made up of adults, children, and adolescents | 5 | 3.29 |
Not stated | 8 | 5.26 |
Sex | ||
Mixed | 85 | 55.92 |
Mostly female | 60 | 39.47 |
Mostly male | 1 | 0.66 |
Not stated | 6 | 3.95 |
Education # | ||
High | 47 | 30.92 |
Low | 45 | 29.61 |
Not stated | 60 | 39.47 |
Individual/Familial income | ||
High | 1 | 0.66 |
Low | 6 | 3.95 |
Mixed | 48 | 31.58 |
Not stated | 97 | 63.82 |
Occupation | ||
Undergraduate students | 6 | 3.95 |
Primary school students | 1 | 0.66 |
Mixed | 11 | 7.24 |
Not stated | 134 | 88.16 |
Presence of NFt along with intervention label | ||
Yes | 34 | 21.79 |
No | 122 | 78.21 |
Risk of bias | ||
High | 107 | 68.59 |
Moderate/Some concerns | 35 | 22.44 |
Low | 14 | 8.97 |
*Out-of-home sector includes any outlet where food or drink is prepared for immediate consumption by consumers, such as restaurants, cafes, and takeaways.
#Education was classified as “high” if >50% of study population completed university or college education, otherwise classified into “low.”
HW, health warning; NFt, Nutrition Facts table; NS, nutri-score; NW, nutrient warning; RCT, randomised controlled trial; TLS, traffic light labelling system.
The comparison-adjusted funnel showed that there was no publication bias detected for most outcomes (S4–S6 Figs). For a few outcomes (e.g., probability of selecting more healthful options, energy and sugar of purchased products, objective understanding and perceived effectiveness of FOPLs) presenting funnel asymmetry, we cannot simply predict them as publication biases either as controversy remains in the test accuracy as the appearance of the plot may be affected by the coding of outcomes and choice of measures [52].
Consumers’ attention, perception, and understanding of colour-coded and warning labels
Objective understanding
The objective understanding of labels was measured by 4 indexes that represent different aspects of label interpretation: (1) comparison or ranking; (2) recall; (3) classification; and (4) mathematical manipulation (estimation) of overall healthfulness and nutrient content. TLS was the only label observed to improve understanding of nutrition information in all 4 types of tasks (Tables 3 and S5 and S2 Fig). NS was also observed to boost the participants’ ability to compare/rank products and estimate overall healthfulness, and NW was associated with improved ability to compare/rank and classify overall healthfulness. When colour-coded labels and warning labels were compared against each other, TLS was found to outperform NW in classifying sugar and saturated fat than NW, and NS was linked to better capacity in the comparison/ranking of overall healthfulness than NW. Comparisons between the 2 colour-coded labels suggested that NS might perform better than TLS in comparing/ranking overall healthfulness task.
Table 3. The network effects of objective understanding of different label types.
Outcome | Comparison | Number of comparisons | Direct estimate (OR and 95% CI) | Indirect estimate (OR and 95% CI) | Network estimate (OR and 95% CI) | Direct evidence proportion |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Comparison/Ranking of overall healthfulness and nutrient content | ||||||
Overall healthfulness | NS vs. control | 14 | 4.84 (3.36, 6.98) | 3.52 (1.72, 7.19) | 4.53 (3.28, 6.28) | 0.79 |
NS vs. NW | 7 | 1.62 (0.97, 2.72) | 1.93 (1.08, 3.44) | 1.75 (1.19, 2.57) | 0.56 | |
NS vs. TLS | 8 | 1.38 (0.85, 2.24) | 1.87 (1.07, 3.26) | 1.57 (1.09, 2.27) | 0.57 | |
NW vs. control | 14 | 2.22 (1.54, 3.2) | 3.92 (2.15, 7.17) | 2.59 (1.89, 3.54) | 0.73 | |
NW vs. TLS | 10 | 0.91 (0.59, 1.41) | 0.88 (0.51, 1.53) | 0.9 (0.64, 1.26) | 0.61 | |
TLS vs. control | 21 | 2.91 (2.15, 3.94) | 2.75 (1.44, 5.24) | 2.88 (2.19, 3.79) | 0.82 | |
Energy | TLS vs. control | 2 | 3.1 (1.36, 7.08) | . | 3.1 (1.36, 7.08) | 1.00 |
Sodium/Salt | TLS vs. control | 2 | 2.92 (1.06, 8.06) | . | 2.92 (1.06, 8.06) | 1.00 |
Mathematical manipulation (estimation) of overall healthfulness and nutrient content | ||||||
Overall healthfulness | NS vs. control | 0 | . | 2.57 (1.42, 4.65) | 2.57 (1.42, 4.65) | 0 |
NS vs. TLS | 1 | 0.88 (0.69, 1.14) | . | 0.88 (0.69, 1.14) | 1.00 | |
TLS vs. control | 1 | 2.91 (1.7, 4.98) | . | 2.91 (1.7, 4.98) | 1.00 | |
Energy | TLS vs. control | 2 | 2.4 (0.39, 14.69) | . | 2.4 (0.39, 14.69) | 1.00 |
Fat | TLS vs. control | 1 | 1.32 (0.87, 2) | . | 1.32 (0.87, 2) | 1.00 |
Recall of overall healthfulness and nutrient content | ||||||
Overall healthfulness | TLS vs. control | 1 | 1.34 (0.8, 2.22) | . | 1.34 (0.8, 2.22) | 1.00 |
Energy | TLS vs. control | 1 | 1.55 (1.14, 2.11) | . | 1.55 (1.14, 2.11) | 1.00 |
Classification of overall healthfulness and nutrient content | ||||||
Overall healthfulness | NW vs. control | 4 | 2.63 (1.42, 4.88) | 6.46 (1.72, 24.18) | 3.09 (1.77, 5.41) | 0.82 |
NW vs. TLS | 2 | 1.81 (0.75, 4.39) | 1.02 (0.4, 2.63) | 1.39 (0.73, 2.65) | 0.53 | |
TLS vs. control | 5 | 2.45 (1.4, 4.27) | 1.25 (0.31, 4.98) | 2.23 (1.33, 3.73) | 0.86 | |
Energy | TLS vs. control | 3 | 4.53 (0.63, 32.37) | . | 4.53 (0.63, 32.37) | 1.00 |
Sodium/Salt | TLS vs. control | 4 | 3.97 (1.89, 8.32) | . | 3.97 (1.89, 8.32) | 1.00 |
Sugar | NW vs. control | 1 | 1.31 (0.56, 3.06) | . | 1.31 (0.56, 3.06) | 1.00 |
NW vs. TLS | 0 | . | 0.31 (0.11, 0.89) | 0.31 (0.11, 0.89) | 0 | |
TLS vs. control | 2 | 4.28 (2.23, 8.18) | . | 4.28 (2.23, 8.18) | 1.00 | |
Fat | TLS vs. control | 3 | 3.09 (1.26, 7.6) | . | 3.09 (1.26, 7.6) | 1.00 |
Saturated fat | NW vs. control | 1 | 1.8 (0.96, 3.38) | . | 1.8 (0.96, 3.38) | 1.00 |
NW vs. TLS | 0 | . | 0.4 (0.18, 0.88) | 0.4 (0.18, 0.88) | 0.00 | |
TLS vs. control | 2 | 4.54 (2.78, 7.43) | . | 4.54 (2.78, 7.43) | 1.00 |
CI, confidence interval; HW, health warning; NS, Nutri-Score; NW, nutrient warning; OR, odds ratio; TLS, traffic light labelling system.
Subjective understanding
We assessed different dimensions of subjective understanding of the labels in terms of ease of understanding (16 studies), elimination of confusion (5 studies), perceived provision of information (13 studies), and perceived workload (5 studies) in label processing (S5 Table and S3 Fig). TLS caused more confusion for consumers (OR and 95% CI for elimination of confusion: 0.66 [0.45, 0.97]) but was seen as providing sufficient information (3.08 [1.6, 5.93]). NS was considered easy to understand (1.84 [1.19, 2.85]) and providing sufficient information for consumers (2.44 [1.03, 5.78]). NW was evaluated as the easiest to understand (NW versus control: 2.82 [1.82, 4.36]; NW versus TLS: 2.03 [1.32, 3.12]).
Perceived effectiveness and credibility
Thirteen studies were included for the evaluation of perceived effectiveness, while 8 studies for the credibility assessment (S5 Table and S3 Fig). TLS, NS, and HW were all perceived effective by consumer surveys (OR and 95% CI: TLS, 3.2 [1.43, 7.19]; NS, 3.92 [1.42, 10.8]; HW: 4.75 [1.03, 21.88]). Comparing colour-coded labels against the warning labels suggested that NS was believed to be more effective than NW. None of the FOPLs were thought more credible than the control group.
Salience and visual attention
Several self-reported measures were applied to evaluate the salience of labels, including how conspicuous labels were (10 studies included using self-reported ratings of the question “to what degree to you think the label stands out”) and how frequently they were noticed (5 studies assessed the frequency of participants claiming that they noticed the labels) and recalled (3 studies evaluated if participants correctly recalled the labels). TLS, NW, NW, and HW were all noticed more frequently than the control group (OR and 95% CI: TLS, 2.58 [1.65, 4.02]; NS, 5.65 [2.84, 11.22]; NW, 3.04 [1.87, 4.95]; HW, 6.09 [3, 12.36]). Only NS was perceived to be conspicuous (7.53 [1.78, 31.89]) and was more likely to be recalled (3.46 [2.74, 4.37]) than the control. Comparing colour-coded labels and warning labels showed that NS and HW was noticed more often than other options. NS was correctly recalled more frequently than TLS (S5 Table and S3 Fig).
To measure visual attention more accurately and precisely, some studies utilised eye-tracking devices to record the eye movement, and a fixation was defined as low velocity of eye movement (S5 Table and S3 Fig). Only TLS and NS were evaluated in 5 eye-tracking studies. Our analysis indicated that TLS attracted fixations more frequently and quickly with longer duration. The use of TLS also delayed the fixation on NFt and reduced the visual attention on NFt. According to our findings, TLS was perceived to provide more nutrition information and thus required more time to interpret. In addition, the dependence on conventional NFt was lessened due to the salience and perceived provision of information of TLS. NS also captured visual attentions faster than the control condition, but total duration and number of fixations on the label were also reduced compared with NFt control, probably due to the nature of summarised label simplifying the interpretation of nutrition information.
Consumers’ perception and attitudes towards foods and drinks
To explore the mechanisms underpinning behavioural changes (formation of attitudes), we further assessed the influence of different colour-coded and warning labels on consumers’ perception and attitudes towards products, based on perceived healthfulness and risks, perceived recommended amount and frequency of consumption, self-reported product appeal, and willingness-to-pay (WTP).
The results indicated that TLS, NW, and HW all reduced the perception of healthfulness for less healthful products (RMD and 95% CI: TLS, −0.077 [−0.116, −0.038]; NW, −0.224 [−0.263, −0.186]; HW, −0.126 [−0.16, −0.092]) and reduced the perceived recommended amount to consume unhealthful foods (perceived recommended amount to consume: TLS, −0.05 [−0.054, −0.046]; NS, −0.08 [−0.082, −0.078]; NW, −0.47 [−0.509, −0.431]), but NW performed significantly better than TLS and HW (perceived healthfulness for unhealthful products: NW versus TLS, −0.147 [−0.195, −0.1]; NW versus NS, −0.188 [−0.301, −0.075]; perceived recommended amount of unhealthful products to consume: NW versus TLS, −0.42 [−0.459, −0.381]; NW versus NS, −0.39 [−0.429, −0.351]; perceived recommended frequency of unhealthful products to consume: NW versus TLS, −0.41 [−0.449, −0.371]; NW versus NS, −0.455 [−0.529, −0.381]) (Tables 4 and S4 and S1 Fig). NW and HW also increased the perceived disease risk of unhealthful products, as well as reduced the appeal of unhealthful products (Tables 5 and S4). On the other hand, however, TLS and NS promoted the perceived healthfulness for more healthful products better than NW (NW versus TLS, −0.145 [−0.282, −0.008]; NW versus NS, 0.201 [−0.399, −0.003]) (Tables 4 and 5 and S4 and S1 Fig).
Table 4. The network effects of different label types on the perceived healthfulness of products.
Outcome | Comparison | Number of comparisons | Direct estimate (RMD and 95% CI) | Indirect estimate (RMD and 95% CI) | Network estimate (RMD and 95% CI) | Proportion of direct evidence |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Less healthful products | HW vs. control | 10 | −0.125 (−0.159, −0.091) | −0.289 (−0.712, 0.133) | −0.126 (−0.16, −0.092) | 1.00 |
HW vs. NS | 0 | . | −0.089 (−0.204, 0.025) | −0.089 (−0.204, 0.025) | 0 | |
HW vs. NW | 1 | 0.099 (−0.061, 0.259) | 0.098 (0.045, 0.152) | 0.098 (0.048, 0.149) | 0 | |
HW vs. TLS | 0 | . | −0.049 (−0.1, 0.003) | −0.049 (−0.1, 0.003) | 0 | |
NS vs. control | 1 | −0.078 (−0.383, 0.227) | −0.031 (−0.148, 0.087) | −0.037 (−0.146, 0.073) | 0.05 | |
NS vs. NW | 0 | . | 0.188 (0.075, 0.301) | 0.188 (0.075, 0.301) | 0 | |
NS vs. TLS | 1 | 0.046 (−0.064, 0.156) | −0.001 (−0.309, 0.306) | 0.041 (−0.063, 0.144) | 0.95 | |
NW vs. control | 6 | −0.215 (−0.255, −0.175) | −0.327 (−0.46, −0.194) | −0.224 (−0.263, −0.186) | 0.99 | |
NW vs. TLS | 3 | −0.181 (−0.244, −0.118) | −0.102 (−0.175, −0.03) | −0.147 (−0.195, −0.1) | 0.96 | |
TLS vs. control | 6 | −0.086 (−0.127, −0.046) | 0.014 (−0.114, 0.143) | −0.077 (−0.116, −0.038) | 0.98 | |
Products of mixed healthfulness | NS vs. control | 2 | 0.066 (−0.026, 0.157) | . | 0.066 (−0.026, 0.157) | 1.00 |
NS vs. TLS | 0 | . | 0.108 (−0.01, 0.226) | 0.108 (−0.01, 0.226) | 0 | |
TLS vs. control | 2 | −0.043 (−0.118, 0.032) | . | −0.043 (−0.118, 0.032) | 1.00 | |
More healthful products | HW vs. control | 2 | −0.016 (−0.128, 0.097) | . | −0.016 (−0.128, 0.097) | 1.00 |
HW vs. NS | 0 | . | −0.137 (−0.319, 0.045) | −0.137 (−0.319, 0.045) | 0 | |
HW vs. NW | 0 | . | 0.064 (−0.113, 0.241) | 0.064 (−0.113, 0.241) | 0 | |
HW vs. TLS | 0 | . | −0.081 (−0.215, 0.053) | −0.081 (−0.215, 0.053) | 0 | |
NS vs. control | 2 | 0.121 (−0.022, 0.264) | . | 0.121 (−0.022, 0.264) | 1.00 | |
NS vs. NW | 0 | . | 0.201 (0.003, 0.399) | 0.201 (0.003, 0.399) | 0 | |
NS vs. TLS | 0 | . | 0.056 (−0.104, 0.216) | 0.056 (−0.104, 0.216) | 0 | |
NW vs. control | 1 | −0.069 (−0.222, 0.084) | −0.125 (−0.437, 0.186) | −0.08 (−0.217, 0.057) | 1.00 | |
NW vs. TLS | 1 | −0.156 (−0.309, −0.003) | −0.1 (−0.411, 0.212) | −0.145 (−0.282, −0.008) | 1.00 | |
TLS vs. control | 6 | 0.066 (−0.006, 0.139) | . | 0.065 (−0.007, 0.138) | 1.00 |
RMD refers to the percentage of change comparing intervention with control group (RMD = (x2 − x1) / x1; x1: mean of continuous outcome in the intervention group or after implementation of intervention, x2: mean of continuous outcome in the control group or before implementation of intervention).
CI, confidence interval; HW, health warning; NS, Nutri-Score; NW, nutrient warning; RMD, relative mean difference; TLS, traffic light labelling system.
Table 5. The network effects of different label types on the perceived disease risk of consuming products.
Outcome | Comparison | Number of comparisons | Direct estimate (RMD and 95% CI) | Indirect estimate (RMD and 95% CI) | Network estimate (RMD and 95% CI) | Proportion of direct evidence |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Less healthful products | HW vs. control* | 10 | 0.124 (0.053, 0.196) | 0.6 (0.153, 1.048) | 0.136 (0.065, 0.207) | 1.00 |
HW vs. NW* | 2 | 0.063 (−0.09, 0.215) | −0.562 (−0.766, −0.357) | −0.16 (−0.283, −0.038) | 0.67 | |
HW vs. NW + HW* | 1 | 0.03 (−0.181, 0.241) | −0.653 (−1.006, −0.3) | −0.15 (−0.331, 0.031) | 0.93 | |
NW vs. control* | 3 | 0.43 (0.299, 0.561) | −0.304 (−0.582, −0.027) | 0.296 (0.178, 0.415) | 0.66 | |
NW vs. NW + HW* | 1 | −0.11 (−0.324, 0.104) | 0.463 (0.048, 0.878) | 0.01 (−0.18, 0.2) | 0.61 | |
NW + HW vs. control | 1 | 0.36 (0.146, 0.574) | 0.102 (−0.236, 0.439) | 0.286 (0.105, 0.467) | 0.23 | |
More healthful products | HW vs. control | 2 | 0.024 (0.008, 0.04) | . | 0.024 (0.008, 0.04) | 1.00 |
RMD refers to the percentage of change comparing intervention with control group (RMD = (x2 − x1) / x1; x1: mean of continuous outcome in the intervention group or after implementation of intervention, x2: mean of continuous outcome in the control group or before implementation of intervention).
*The direct and indirect effects were observed significantly inconsistent (p < 0.05), and the network estimate may violate the assumption of consistency and transitivity of NMA, thus only direct evidence was used for interpretation.
CI, confidence interval; HW, health warning; NMA, network meta-analysis; NS, Nutri-Score; NW, nutrient warning; RMD, relative mean difference; TLS, traffic light labelling system.
WTP is an economic concept used to evaluate consumers’ demand for a product [53]. In our systematic review, 3 studies considered WTP of less healthful products, suggesting that NS significantly reduced the WTP for less healthful products by 16%, but no significant effect was observed for textual NW or HW. Another 3 studies investigated the WTP for more healthful products but did not find evidence for change in WTP when labelled with TLS, textual NW, or HW (S1 Data).
Changes in consumers’ purchasing and consuming behaviour or intentions
The NMA revealed that all colour-coded and warning labels were significantly associated with changes in purchasing behaviour. Warning labels also have a significant effect on purchasing intention (Fig 4 and S3 Table). NS and NW were both associated with an increasing overall healthfulness of products purchased. TLS, NS, and NW all were associated with purchasing lower energy, sodium/salt, total fat, or saturated fat (Figs 5 and 6 and S3 Table). Only one study looked at the outcome of consumption (S3 Table), thus we did not include it in our meta-analysis. This study suggested that TLS was not significantly associated with change in energy consumption.
Eight studies conducted in real-world settings examined the association between FOPLs and sales of unhealthful or healthful products (S7 Table). NW was linked to reduced probability of purchasing/selecting of unhealthful items (OR and 95% CI: 0.50 [0.34, 0.73]), while TLS was associated with increased likelihood of purchasing/selecting more healthful products (1.32 [1.02, 1.72]).
When colour-coded labels and warning labels were compared against each other, we found that NW appeared to outperform TLS in discouraging purchasing unhealthful products (OR and 95% CI: 0.81 [0.67, 0.98]), reducing intended purchase of unhealthful products (RMD and 95% CI: −0.197 [−0.352, −0.042]), and lowering total amount of energy purchased (RMD and 95% CI: −0.064 [−0.125, −0.004]). NW was also observed to perform better than NS in improving overall healthfulness (RMD and 95% CI: 0.127 [0.029, 0.225]) and reduce total energy (RMD and 95% CI: −0.07 [−0.008, −0.131]) and saturated fat (RMD and 95% CI: −0.156 [−0.264, −0.049]) in shopping basket, but NS appeared to encourage the purchase of healthful products better than NW (OR and 95% CI: 1.51 [1.08, 2.11]) (S3 Table).
Comparisons within colour-coded labels or warning labels suggested moderate difference as well. NW reduced the purchase intention for unhealthful products more than HW (RMD and 95% CI: 0.178 [0.002, 0.355]), but HW performed better in boosting the intended purchase of healthful products (0.429 [0.058, 0.8]). TLS did not differ from NS much in terms of the behavioural changes. When comparing the results of different subtypes of TLS (summary versus nutrient-specific TLS), NW (textual NW versus nontextual NW), and HW (textual HW versus nontextual HW, negative message versus positive message), we did not find any difference on the behavioural changes.
Considering the evident heterogeneity and inconsistency for some outcome measures that might violate the consistency and transitivity assumptions required for an NMA (S3 Table), our findings should be interpreted with caution for these outcomes.
Discussion
This systematic review evaluated the impact of colour-coded and warning labels on consumers’ purchasing behaviour, the psychological mechanism underpinning purchasing modification, including consumers’ perception and liking for foods and drinks, as well as the understanding and evaluation of label attributes. We found that all colour-coded and warning labels appeared to have beneficial effects by encouraging the purchase of more healthful products, reducing the purchase of less healthful options, improving overall nutritional quality, and reducing the energy, sodium/salt, fat, and saturated fat content of processed foods and drinks purchased/chosen. Based on the health communication theory, the results suggested that colour-coded and warning labels successfully drew more of consumers’ attention than the control condition and improved consumers’ understanding of nutrition information. The labels also modified perceived healthfulness, recommended consumption amount, and frequency of consumption for products. These mechanisms can establish more healthful purchasing behaviour by improving both the nutritional quality and nutrient content purchased/chosen by consumers.
Despite the heterogeneity in label types, labelling formats, position, study population, study design, and experimental settings across studies [14,23–40], FOPLs were generally considered to have positive effects on guiding consumers in making more healthful food choices, especially in populations with low socioeconomic status and limited knowledge of nutrition labels [24,54]. The health communication theory suggested that attention to the FOPL is a prerequisite for establishing specific perception of the label itself and an understanding of the nutrition information. Our study, together with previous evidence [23,24,30], suggested that colour-coded labels and warning labels were able to ease the difficulty in processing nutrition information and improve the objective understanding of nutrition information with the use of eye-catching design, but the perception of labels might differ, especially between the 2 colour-coded labels (TLS and NS). Although TLS and NS both used colour scales to indicate healthfulness of foods, TLS scores the level of each target nutrient, while NS summarises the overall nutritional quality taking all preferable and detrimental nutrients into consideration. Therefore, according to our study, TLS was perceived to provide sufficient information, which was also reflected in the finding that TLS was associated with a better performance in complex understanding tasks (e.g., classification of sugar and saturated fat). However, too much information could also elicit confusion in consumers when reading TLS. NS, on the other hand, was perceived to be more salient and thus thought easier to understand for consumers.
According to the health communication theory and previous studies [22–26], mechanisms of motivating consumers’ behavioural change vary across FOPL types [26]. NW and HW, compared with the colour-coded counterparts, are more dependent on eliciting the perception of severe risk and negative emotions, which mediate the reduced selection of unhealthful products [26]. TLS and NS, on the other hand, rely more on enhancing the perception of healthfulness for more healthful options and thereby perform better at promoting purchase of healthful foods [27,28,31], which were supported by our findings (Tables 4 and 5). Our study, together with previous evidence, explained the role of perception of healthfulness in mediating the effect of colour-coded labels and warning labels on consumers’ purchasing behaviour. Warning labels are more often associated with “danger” due to the use of symbols (e.g., octagon stopping sign), colour (black and white), and cautionary texts, thus elicit negative perception and emotions towards unhealthful food products marked with warnings on the front of packages. For healthful products, warnings are not displayed, but TLS is presented with green lights on, which is indicative of the concept of “health,” “nature,” and “sustainability,” thus associated with perception of better healthfulness for food products.
While our review found that FOPL can effectively change purchasing behaviour, mandatory FOPL policies are likely to be much more effective at changing consumption than voluntary policies. Voluntary FOPL systems have been adopted slowly in the marketplace, and consumers also perceived the products without FOPL as more healthful, albeit the nutritional quality might be worse [15]. Even though most FOPL policies are currently implemented on a voluntary basis, over the past 3 years mandatory FOPL systems have increasingly been favoured, of which warning labels and TLS were the most popular interpretative FOPLs worldwide. More countries have also proposed to make ongoing voluntary FOPL policies mandatory, which would better guide consumers’ food choice and stimulate reformulation in the food industry [7,8]. To ensure the effectiveness of mandatory FOPL policies, consumer education and monitoring systems for the market should be launched, and further studies should also be carried out in real-world settings to add to the evidence of mandatory FOPL policies in different populations and regional cultures.
Compared to previous meta-analyses that assessed the effectiveness of colour-coded labels and warning labels against a control group (mostly no labels) [25,31,41,46], we utilised the NMA method to explore the difference in the effectiveness and indicators of psychological mechanisms of one type of FOPL compared to another. This provided a deeper insight into the question of which is the optimal FOPL that can be applied to each country. Generally, our findings suggested that different FOPL types might dominate different outcome measures. NS performed better in nudging the purchase of more healthful products than NW, while NW had the advantage in discouraging unhealthful purchasing behaviour. The underlying psychological mechanisms also vary across labels. Warning labels reduced the perceived healthfulness of unhealthful products and remind consumers to eat less of unhealthful foods, while colour-coded labels enhanced the perception of healthfulness for more healthful products (Table 4 and S4 Table). Consumers’ perception towards labels differed across types as well. NW was recognised as easier to understand and improved the classification of nutrient content (e.g., sugar and saturated fat) compared to TLS. According to our NMA, TLS was considered to provide more sufficient information than NS, but the latter was recalled more frequently (see Results in the “Salience and visual attention” section). The performance of colour-coded labels and warning labels on multiple dimensions make it necessary for policy-makers to weigh pros and cons according to local context. For example, in countries with high levels of NCDs, which place a large burden on individuals and healthcare systems, and a food system dominated by ultraprocessed food and drinks, NW or HW could help lower supply-driven consumption as they are known to be easier to interpret by consumers, who can recognise that they are applied to unhealthful products high in sugar, salt, and saturated fat [55,56].
Our review included empirical evidence from multiple databases, providing the latest and most comprehensive evidence for different aspects of colour-coded and warning labels, including different subtypes: summary and nutrient-specific type, nontextual and textual types, and positive- and negative-framed types. We applied the NMA method to make full use of both direct and indirect evidence of the comparisons between intervention labels and the control condition. These results can support policy-makers to make decisions based on the performance of labelling in different dimensions. In addition to the effect on purchasing and consuming behaviour, we also investigated the underlying psychological outcomes quantitatively based on the health communication framework, which systematically suggested the mechanism underpinning the effect of consumers’ behaviour.
Our study had some limitations. First, compared to the relatively large amount of evidence on the purchasing behaviour elicited by FOPLs, the data on food consumption were quite limited. Our findings suggested that both warning labels and colour-coded labels would reduce the perceived recommended consumption amount or frequency of unhealthful products, and warning labels might outperform colour-coded, which could be seen as an indirect evidence that FOPLs are able to change dietary consumption. However, the research gap between purchase and actual intake of different nutrients remains to be validated by future studies, which is crucial to inform decision-making on labelling policies. Second, most of the studies were laboratory experiments, and, thus, findings mainly indicate the immediate or short-term effect of colour-coded and warning label interventions. There were very few real-world studies assessing the effect of mandatory labelling policies on genuine “purchase.” However, considering the highly controlled condition in RCTs and quasi-experimental studies, our findings will need further demonstration by real-world evidence to build the evidence for the generalisation of labelling to other parts of the world. In our meta-analysis, we found that most of the existing real-world studies evaluated TLS and found it effectively increased the purchase/selection of more healthful products. For NS, NW, and other HW types, more research in real-world settings is needed to confirm their effectiveness. Third, more than half of the studies included had a high risk of bias. Considering the nature of nutrition labelling intervention studies, it was inevitable that participants would be aware of their assigned interventions, and, in turn, such awareness could influence the assessment of outcomes. Therefore, in the sensitivity analyses, we included only RCTs in the analyses of consumers’ behaviour, and the results were consistent with those of the primary analyses. Fourth, we only searched for peer-reviewed articles in 4 of the most commonly used databases and the bibliographic references of eligible articles to ensure that our search strategy could be easily replicated and repeated for the update. We believed that most relevant studies should have been covered in this way, though some relevant studies reported in other databases or in the grey literature may have been missed. Fifth, we did not conduct meta-regression to explore the heterogeneity and inconsistency between direct and indirect comparisons, due to the limited number of studies for most outcomes. Instead, we used a random-effect inconsistency model to accommodate the inconsistency and heterogeneity within and across comparisons. We also carried out a series of subgroup analyses by a range of effect modifiers based on previous studies, including age, sex, study setting, accessibility of NFt, and product types. Another sensitivity analysis was also performed excluding studies using NFt as control setting. The results of the sensitivity analyses did not differ much from our findings in the primary analysis, which suggested the biases generated from combining 2 control settings (NFt control and no-label control) might be relatively small. Sixth, we only considered energy and unfavourable nutrients (e.g., sugar, salt, fat, and saturated fat) that are common in various FOPLs and are considered the major risk factors of NCDs burden [55], but favourable nutrients (e.g., fibre, protein) are also components of interest in many FOPLs (e.g., NS and Health Star Rating) for their beneficial health effect [57,58]. So far, few studies have evaluated the effect of FOPLs on favourable nutrients [59], and there has been a disagreement in the inclusion of favourable nutrients in FOPLs as they might have a health halo effect to products that are high in salt, sugar, or fat [60,61]. For these reasons, we did not summarise the results on unfavourable nutrients in this systematic review, and further studies are needed to clarify the pros and cons of favourable nutrients on colour-coded and warning labels. Finally, the numbers of studies were limited for some comparisons (e.g., only one study provided direct evidence on the comparison between NS and HW for the probability of purchasing more healthful products), especially in the analysis of outcomes concerning the perception and understanding of FOPLs, and perception and attitudes towards food options. Our findings for these secondary outcomes need to be validated by more studies in the future.
In summary, our findings suggest that both colour-coded labels and warnings appeared effective in nudging consumers’ behaviour towards more healthful products by changing the healthfulness perception and eliciting negative emotions. Each type of label may have some different attributes, but the difference between different forms of labels remains to be demonstrated by further studies. Our study can support policy-makers to push forward mandatory FOPL policies to make use of the full potential of FOPL in directing consumers’ food choice and encouraging reformulation in the food industry.
Supporting information
Abbreviations
- CI
confidence interval
- FOPL
front-of-package nutrition labelling
- HW
health warning
- NCD
non communicable disease
- NFt
Nutrition Facts table
- NMA
network meta-analysis
- NS
Nutri-Score
- NW
nutrient warning
- OR
odds ratio
- PICOS
population, intervention, comparator, outcome, and study design
- RCT
randomised controlled trial
- RMD
relative mean difference
- SE
standard error
- SIDE
separate indirect from direct evidence
- TLS
traffic light labelling system
- WHO
World Health Organization
- WTP
willingness-to-pay
Data Availability
All relevant data are contained within the Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
The authors received no specific funding for this work.
References
- 1.Roth GA, Abate D, Abate KH, Abay SM, Abbafati C, Abbasi N, et al. Global, regional, and national age-sex-specific mortality for 282 causes of death in 195 countries and territories, 1980–2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet. 2018;392(10159):1736–88. Epub 2018/11/08. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32203-7 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.He FJ, Li J, Macgregor GA. Effect of longer term modest salt reduction on blood pressure: Cochrane systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised trials. BMJ. 2013;346:f1325. Epub 2013/04/03. doi: 10.1136/bmj.f1325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Thout SR, Santos JA, McKenzie B, Trieu K, Johnson C, McLean R, et al. The Science of Salt: Updating the evidence on global estimates of salt intake. J Clin Hypertens (Greenwich). 2019;21(6):710–21. Epub 2019/04/29. doi: 10.1111/jch.13546 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Guideline: Sugars intake for adults and children. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2015. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Factsheet: Overweight and Obesity. World Health Organization; 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Marmot M, Bell R. Social determinants and non-communicable diseases: time for integrated action. BMJ. 2019;364:l251. Epub 2019/01/28. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Pomeranz JL, Wilde P, Mozaffarian D, Micha R. Mandating Front-of-Package Food Labels in the U.S.—What are the First Amendment Obstacles? Food Policy. 2019;86. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2019.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Building on the success of front-of-pack nutrition labelling in the UK: a public consultation. Department of Health & Social Care; 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 9.World Cancer Research Fund International. NOURISHING policy database. 2020 [cited 2020 Nov]. Available from: www.wcrf.org/NOURISHING.
- 10.Global action plan for the prevention and control of noncommunicable diseases 2013–2020. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Final report of the Commission on ending childhood obesity. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 12.’Best buys’ and other recommended interventions for the prevention and control of noncommunicable diseases—updated (2017) appendix III of the global action plan for the prevention and control of non-communicable diseases 2013–2020. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Guiding principles and framework manual for front-of-pack labelling for promoting healthy diet. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Crockett RA, King SE, Marteau TM, Prevost AT, Bignardi G, Roberts NW, et al. Nutritional labelling for healthier food or non-alcoholic drink purchasing and consumption. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2018;2:CD009315. Epub 2018/02/27. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD009315.pub2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kanter R, Vanderlee L, Vandevijvere S. Front-of-package nutrition labelling policy: global progress and future directions. Public Health Nutr. 2018;21(8):1399–408. Epub 2018/03/21. doi: 10.1017/S1368980018000010 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.France SP. Nutri-Score. 2020. [cited 2020 Nov]. Available from: https://www.santepubliquefrance.fr/determinants-de-sante/nutrition-et-activite-physique/articles/nutri-score. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Corvalán C, Reyes M, Garmendia ML, Uauy R. Structural responses to the obesity and non-communicable diseases epidemic: Update on the Chilean law of food labelling and advertising. Obes Rev. 2019;20(3):367–74. Epub 2018/12/13. doi: 10.1111/obr.12802 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Guide to creating a front of pack (FoP) nutrition label for pre-packed products sold through retail outlets. Department of Health; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Global Food Research Program. Front-of-Package Labeling around the World. 2020 [cited 2020 Nov]. Available from: http://globalfoodresearchprogram.web.unc.edu/resources.
- 20.Taillie LS, Reyes M, Colchero MA, Popkin B, Corvalán C. An evaluation of Chile’s Law of Food Labeling and Advertising on sugar-sweetened beverage purchases from 2015 to 2017: A before-and-after study. PLoS Med. 2020;17(2):e1003015. Epub 2020/02/12. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Correa T, Fierro C, Reyes M, Dillman Carpentier FR, Taillie LS, Corvalan C. Responses to the Chilean law of food labeling and advertising: exploring knowledge, perception and behaviors of mothers of young children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2019;16(1):21. Epub 2019/02/13. doi: 10.1186/s12966-019-0781-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.McGuire WJ. Behavioral medicine, public health and communication theories. Health Educ. 1981;12(3):8–13. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cowburn G, Stockley L. Consumer understanding and use of nutrition labelling: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr. 2005;8(1):21–8. Epub 2005/02/12. doi: 10.1079/phn2005666 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Campos S, Doxey J, Hammond D. Nutrition labels on pre-packaged foods: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr. 2011;14(8):1496–506. doi: 10.1017/S1368980010003290 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Grummon AH, Hall MG. Sugary drink warnings: A meta-analysis of experimental studies. PLoS Med. 2020;17(5):e1003120. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Taillie LS, Hall MG, Popkin BM, Ng SW, Murukutla N. Experimental Studies of Front-of-Package Nutrient Warning Labels on Sugar-Sweetened Beverages and Ultra-Processed Foods: A Scoping Review. Nutrients. 2020;12(2). Epub 2020/02/27. doi: 10.3390/nu12020569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hawley KL, Roberto CA, Bragg MA, Liu PJ, Schwartz MB, Brownell KD. The science on front-of-package food labels. Public Health Nutr. 2013;16(3):430–9. doi: 10.1017/S1368980012000754 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hersey JC, Wohlgenant KC, Arsenault JE, Kosa KM, Muth MK. Effects of front-of-package and shelf nutrition labeling systems on consumers. Nutr Rev. 2013;71(1):1–14. Epub 2013/01/04. doi: 10.1111/nure.12000 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sebastian-Ponce MI, Sanz-Valero J, Wanden-Berghe C. Food labeling and the prevention of overweight and obesity: a systematic review. Cad Saude Publica. 2011;27(11):2083–94. Epub 2011/11/30. doi: 10.1590/s0102-311x2011001100002 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mhurchu CN, Gorton D. Nutrition labels and claims in New Zealand and Australia: a review of use and understanding. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2007;31(2):105–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-6405.2007.00026.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Croker H, Packer J, Russell SJ, Stansfield C, Viner RM. Front of pack nutritional labelling schemes: a systematic review and meta-analysis of recent evidence relating to objectively measured consumption and purchasing. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2020. Epub 2020/05/05. doi: 10.1111/jhn.12758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Torris C, Mobekk H. Improving Cardiovascular Health through Nudging Healthier Food Choices: A Systematic Review. Nutrients. 2019;11(10). Epub 2019/10/23. doi: 10.3390/nu11102520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Swartz JJ, Braxton D, Viera AJ. Calorie menu labeling on quick-service restaurant menus: an updated systematic review of the literature. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2011;8:135. Epub 2011/12/14. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-8-135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Christoph MJ, An R. Effect of nutrition labels on dietary quality among college students: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr Rev. 2018;76(3):187–203. Epub 2018/01/27. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nux069 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Brown HM, Rollo ME, de Vlieger NM, Collins CE, Bucher T. Influence of the nutrition and health information presented on food labels on portion size consumed: a systematic review. Nutr Rev. 2018;76(9):655–77. Epub 2018/05/17. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuy019 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Sacco J, Lillico HG, Chen E, Hobin E. The influence of menu labelling on food choices among children and adolescents: a systematic review of the literature. Perspect Public Health. 2017;137(3):173–81. Epub 2016/07/21. doi: 10.1177/1757913916658498 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Cantu-Jungles TM, McCormack LA, Slaven JE, Slebodnik M, Eicher-Miller HA. A Meta-Analysis to Determine the Impact of Restaurant Menu Labeling on Calories and Nutrients (Ordered or Consumed) in U.S. Adults. Nutrients. 2017;9(10). Epub 2017/10/05. doi: 10.3390/nu9101088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bleich SN, Economos CD, Spiker ML, Vercammen KA, VanEpps EM, Block JP, et al. A Systematic Review of Calorie Labeling and Modified Calorie Labeling Interventions: Impact on Consumer and Restaurant Behavior. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2017;25(12):2018–44. Epub 2017/10/19. doi: 10.1002/oby.21940 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sarink D, Peeters A, Freak-Poli R, Beauchamp A, Woods J, Ball K, et al. The impact of menu energy labelling across socioeconomic groups: A systematic review. Appetite. 2016;99:59–75. Epub 2016/01/03. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.12.022 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sinclair SE, Cooper M, Mansfield ED. The influence of menu labeling on calories selected or consumed: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2014;114(9):1375–88.e15. Epub 2014/07/20. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2014.05.014 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Cecchini M, Warin L. Impact of food labelling systems on food choices and eating behaviour: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized studies. Obes Rev. 2016;17(3):201–10. Epub 2015/12/24. doi: 10.1111/obr.12364 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Faltinsen EG, Storebo OJ, Jakobsen JC, Boesen K, Lange T, Gluud C. Network meta-analysis: the highest level of medical evidence? BMJ Evid Based Med. 2018;23(2):56–9. doi: 10.1136/bmjebm-2017-110887 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Higgins JP, Altman DG, Gotzsche PC, Juni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, et al. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2011;343:d5928. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Sterne JA, Hernan MA, Reeves BC, Savovic J, Berkman ND, Viswanathan M, et al. ROBINS-I: a tool for assessing risk of bias in non-randomised studies of interventions. BMJ. 2016;355:i4919. doi: 10.1136/bmj.i4919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Heart National, Lung Blood Institute. Quality assessment tool for observational cohort and cross-sectional studies. Bethesda: National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Shangguan S, Afshin A, Shulkin M, Ma W, Marsden D, Smith J, et al. A Meta-Analysis of Food Labeling Effects on Consumer Diet Behaviors and Industry Practices. Am J Prev Med. 2019;56(2):300–14. Epub 2018/12/24. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2018.09.024 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bucher HC, Guyatt GH, Griffith LE, Walter SD. The results of direct and indirect treatment comparisons in meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Clin Epidemiol. 1997;50(6):683–91. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(97)00049-8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Rücker G SG, Krahn U, König J. Package ‘Netmeta’.” Network Meta-Analysis Using Frequentist Methods. R package version 08–0. 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Higgins JP, Jackson D, Barrett JK, Lu G, Ades AE, White IR. Consistency and inconsistency in network meta-analysis: concepts and models for multi-arm studies. Res Synth Methods. 2012;3(2):98–110. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1044 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Konig J, Krahn U, Binder H. Visualizing the flow of evidence in network meta-analysis and characterizing mixed treatment comparisons. Stat Med. 2013;32(30):5414–29. doi: 10.1002/sim.6001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Higgins JP, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, et al. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions: John Wiley & Sons; 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lau J, Ioannidis JP, Terrin N, Schmid CH, Olkin I. The case of the misleading funnel plot. BMJ. 2006;333(7568):597–600. doi: 10.1136/bmj.333.7568.597 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Breidert C, Hahsler M, Reutterer T. A review of methods for measuring willingness-to-pay. Innov Mark. 2006;2 (4):8–32. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Bhana N, Utter J, Eyles H. Knowledge, Attitudes and Behaviour Related to Dietary Salt Intake in High-Income Countries: a Systematic Review. Curr Nutr Rep. 2018;7(4):183–97. Epub 2018/09/05. doi: 10.1007/s13668-018-0239-9 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Swinburn BA, Kraak VI, Allender S, Atkins VJ, Baker PI, Bogard JR, et al. The Global Syndemic of Obesity, Undernutrition, and Climate Change: The Lancet Commission report. Lancet. 2019;393(10173):791–846. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32822-8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Kanter R, Reyes M, Swinburn B, Vandevijvere S, Corvalan C. The Food Supply Prior to the Implementation of the Chilean Law of Food Labeling and Advertising. Nutrients. 2018;11(1). doi: 10.3390/nu11010052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Threapleton DE, Greenwood DC, Evans CE, Cleghorn CL, Nykjaer C, Woodhead C, et al. Dietary fibre intake and risk of cardiovascular disease: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2013;347:f6879. doi: 10.1136/bmj.f6879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Naghshi S, Sadeghi O, Willett WC, Esmaillzadeh A. Dietary intake of total, animal, and plant proteins and risk of all cause, cardiovascular, and cancer mortality: systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMJ. 2020;370:m2412. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m2412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Acton RB, Hammond D. Impact of sugar taxes and front-of-package nutrition labels on purchases of protein, calcium and fibre. Prev Med. 2020;136:106091. Epub 2020/04/19. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2020.106091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Talati Z, Pettigrew S, Dixon H, Neal B, Ball K, Hughes C. Do Health Claims and Front-of-Pack Labels Lead to a Positivity Bias in Unhealthy Foods? Nutrients. 2016;8(12). doi: 10.3390/nu8120787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bullock K, Lahne J, Pope L. Investigating the role of health halos and reactance in ice cream choice. Food Qual Prefer. 2020;80:103826. [Google Scholar]