Abstract
We present experimental evidence on how pro-sociality, trust and attitudes towards risk and ambiguity evolved over the six weeks following the imposition of stringent Covid-19 related lockdown measures in the Hubei province of China. We compare incentivized economic decision-making in a baseline sample, collected pre-epidemic, with a series of repeated cross-sectional samples drawn from the same population between January and March, 2020. We find high rates of altruism, cooperation and aversion to risk taking under ambiguity in the immediate aftermath of the lockdown, while trust is significantly below its baseline level. Risk attitudes also differ in the post-lockdown sample, with decreased risk tolerance in the loss domain and lesser risk aversion in the gain domain. We further uncover significant transitory effects for trust and risk aversion around the date of a high-profile whistleblower’s death from Covid-19. Our findings suggest that the onset of a public health crisis may have unintended consequences for economic preferences that determine population compliance with interventions designed to reduce the spread of a novel coronavirus.
Keywords: Covid-19, Social preferences, Cooperation, Trust, Risk preferences
1. Introduction and background
In December 2019, a novel coronavirus and associated disease (Covid-19) was first reported in the city of Wuhan, the capital of China’s Hubei province. Person-to-person transmission of Covid-19 was publicly confirmed on January 20, 2020, and three days later the central government of China imposed a strict lockdown in Wuhan, quickly followed by lockdown measures in the other 15 cities of Hubei province. The Hubei lockdown lasted in its most stringent form until mid-March, by which time local virus cases had eased and restrictions gradually relaxed.
Since then, public health campaigns worldwide have sought to align private and collective interests, promoting behavioural change as the first line of defence in mitigating the virus’s impact on individuals’ well-being and livelihoods. Local and national lockdown measures have become essential and familiar components in the policy-maker’s toolkit. The success of such measures in containing the spread of the virus relies first and foremost on the pro-social and cooperative behaviours of individuals, their trust in others and the interplay between their decision-making and perceptions of risk and uncertainty (Van Bavel, Baicker, Boggio, Capraro, Cichocka, Cikara, Crockett, Crum, Douglas, Druckman, et al., 2020, Nikolov, Pape, Tonguc, Williams). Recent studies conducted during the Covid-19 pandemic find that pro-sociality (Campos-Mercade et al., 2021) and risk tolerance (Müller and Rau, 2021) are important predictors of compliant behaviours.
In this paper, we use financially incentivized decision-making experiments to assess the impact of the onset of Covid-19 and associated events in China - during the stringent lockdown period from late January to early March 2020 - on pro-sociality, cooperation, trust and risk-related attitudes. Our experiment sample is drawn at random from a population of 9000 pre-registered Wuhan University students. We administered a standard set of behavioural economics tasks to a cross-section of 396 subjects from this population at various times between late January and early March of 2020. Subjects were reached via an online platform based on the WeChat social media interface. We compare data from this sample to a pre-crisis baseline sample of 206 subjects drawn at random from the same population, recruited using the same process, and who completed the same set of decision-making tasks in May 2019. An advantage of this design is that we can track changes in incentivized behaviour using a consistent response elicitation method.
Our first empirical approach is a simple difference in means between the baseline and post-lockdown samples. To mitigate potential confounding effects of differential selection in the pre- and post-event comparison, we conduct regression analyses that control for a range of observed demographics and location-based variables that capture variable exposure to the virus. We find some evidence of greater pro-sociality and higher rates of cooperation in our post-lockdown sample; interestingly, this does not translate into higher expectations about others’ cooperation in a coordination game setting. Those subjects based in Wuhan during the initial outbreak were less inclined to trust than their counterparts in other parts of China, although this observation comes with the caveat of a small sample size.
The impact of the Covid-19 onset on risk-related attitudes is more nuanced. Since preventative measures often involve trading off a fixed cost versus a probabilistic outcome (e.g. the cost of wearing a mask versus the risk of catching the virus), we elicit risky choices in the loss as well as gain domain. Since it is difficult to quantify the probabilities involved in an emerging crisis, we also elicit attitudes towards risk taking under ambiguity. In the baseline sample, we observe risk aversion in the gain domain and risk-seeking in the loss domain, which is consistent with the diminishing sensitivity of prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979). Post-lockdown, subjects display less risk aversion in the gain domain and decreased risk tolerance in the loss domain. Thus, lottery choices after the shock are consistent with a linearization of the value function and a movement towards risk neutrality. These findings complement Ikeda et al. (2020), who uncover that individuals’ prospect-theory risk preferences in Japan were systematically and similarly altered after the onset of Covid-19.
Unlike many previous studies, we can go beyond a pre- and post-event comparison and identify acute preference perturbations within the formative period under study. This is because our post-lockdown sample is composed of five independent cross-sectional waves, collected over a period of six weeks. We observe no systematic differences in demographic characteristics among these five waves and, given the short time-frame, differential selection is likely to be mitigated. Our first post-lockdown wave was collected on January 24/26, immediately after the announcement of the Wuhan city lockdown. By this time, the daily search index on the word pneumonia using China’s main search engine Baidu had risen abruptly to over 760,000 and China Central Television was devoting more than 80 percent of its news coverage to pneumonia-related stories (top panel of Fig. 1 ).1
The second and third post-lockdown waves were collected on the days immediately either side of February 7. We conjecture that a formative event during the period under study was the death on this date of Dr. Li Wenliang, a high-profile Chinese whistleblower. To gain insight into the magnitude with which this event resonated in the Chinese public’s consciousness, compare the two Baidu search indices in the bottom panel of Fig. 1.2 The red series tracks the index on Li Wenliang and the blue series tracks the index on Zhong Nanshan, one of China’s leading medical scientists who warned about the transmissibility of Covid-19 on a visit to Wuhan in January.3 While the Zhong Nanshan index spiked to over one million around the date of his Wuhan visit, the Li Wenliang index increased nearly five times more than this to over five million, following the announcement of his death.
The fourth and final post-lockdown waves were implemented at bi-weekly intervals after this event. By the end of the sampling period, the level of public health emergency response had been lowered by provincial governments and Wuhan had closed its mobile cabin hospitals.
Our experimental data supports the presence of transient behavioural effects during the post-lockdown period. In the first post-lockdown wave, generalized trust is low. This is perhaps unsurprising: late January was the moment of greatest uncertainty about the virus in Wuhan. By the second post-lockdown wave, trust recovers while the remaining behavioural measures remain largely stable. Immediately after the death of Dr. Li Wenliang, trust moves significantly lower and we also observe a short-lived increase in risk aversion and a dip in cooperation. Both these measures recover towards their late January levels by the time of the final sampling wave.
The paper continues in Section 2 with a discussion of our contribution in the context of related literature and summarizes why we believe the uniqueness of our research setting to be a strength. In Section 3, we outline the experimental design, including details of sampling, the decision-making tasks and protocol. In Section 4, we present the experimental results. In Section 5, we conclude with a comparison of our results to the literature, a discussion of their implications for lockdown policies during an emerging public health crisis, and outline several important limitations of our study.
2. Contribution in context
This paper contributes to an established economics literature on the stability of economic preferences.4 Canonical models of economic decision-making assume that individuals’ preferences are stable over time. This assumption is at odds with psychological channels and the importance of personal experiences for economic behaviours (Hertwig, Barron, Weber, Erev, 2004, Filipski, Jin, Zhang, Chen, 2019, Banks, Bassoli, Mammi, 2020). Preference stability has been challenged by a series of empirical works in economics and finance (e.g. Choi, Laibson, Madrian, Metrick, 2009, Malmendier, Nagel, 2011, Guiso, Sapienza, Zingales, 2018, Andersen, Hanspal, Nielsen, 2019).
Prior experimental research suggests that traumatic life events can have a significant effect on risk-related behaviours. Several studies report that individuals become more risk averse for various time horizons after experiencing a natural disaster (Kim, Lee, 2014, Cameron, Shah, 2015, Beine, Charness, Dupuy, Joxhe, 2020). Still others uncover an increased preference for risk-seeking (Eckel, El-Gamal, Wilson, 2009, Page, Savage, Torgler, 2014, Hanaoka, Shigeoka, Watanabe, 2018, Kuroishi, Sawada, 2020) or suggest that the direction of the effect may be mediated by personal losses during a crisis (Said et al., 2015). Further research has been undertaken to the long-term impact of natural disasters (Andrabi, Das, 2017, Fleming, Chong, Bejarano, 2014, Cassar, Healy, von Kessler, 2017), economic recessions (Fisman et al., 2015), and wars (Bauer et al., 2016) on trust, reciprocity and cooperative behaviours. If there is a consensus, it is that the impact of formative events on economic preferences is context-specific.
We also add to a rapidly growing literature assessing the impact of the Covid-19 pandemic on economic preferences and beliefs (Angrisani, Cipriani, Guarino, Kendall, Ortiz de Zarate, 2020, Binder, 2020, Brück, Ferguson, Justino, Stojetz, 2020, Coibion, Gorodnichenko, Weber, 2020, Fetzer, Hensel, Hermle, Roth, 2020, Li, Qin, Wu, Yan, 2020, Lohmann, Gsottbauer, Kontoleon, You, 2021). Given the importance of context and cultural factors in determining the evolution of preferences in response to an exogenous shock, we restrict attention to related evidence from China.5
Li (2020) conducted an online experiment in China in early March 2020, eliciting incentivized measures of attitudes towards risk and ambiguity. He finds that ambiguity averse subjects are more pessimistic about the impact of the pandemic on economic growth and are more likely to reduce consumption and increase savings as a result. Unlike in the present study, there is no pre-epidemic sample available to examine how these attitudes may have been affected by the onset of Covid-19.
Closely related is Bu et al. (2020), who administered a survey to a panel of graduate students at the Wuhan University of Science and Technology before and after initial reports of the virus emerged.6 They observe a significant decrease in levels of financial risk taking and planned risk taking between waves and exploit geolocation data to show that those subjects quarantined in Wuhan allocate 45 percent less to a hypothetical gamble relative to those based in other provinces of China. The authors conduct a repeat survey in April 2020, in which they observe that financial risk taking rebounds after the quarantine in Wuhan ends, suggesting that Covid-related effects on economic preferences are likely to be short-lived. A drawback of their study is that they rely on non-incentivized and so possibly less reliable preference measures. We will also provide evidence to suggest that the timing of their initial post-event survey may have captured acute fluctuations in risk aversion during the post-lockdown period.
Lohmann et al. (2021) employ a comprehensive battery of behavioural economics tasks to examine the impact of exposure to the Covid-19 crisis on risk, time and trust preferences, cooperative, pro-social and norm enforcement behaviours, in a representative sample of 183 cities across China. Their longitudinal design affords strong internal validity using a difference-in-difference estimator to exploit within-subject variation between October and December 2019 versus mid-March 2020. They find a significant rise in anti-social behaviour among individuals most exposed to the virus, which is also associated with an increase in virus-related concern (as measured using the Baidu search index) and negative sentiment (based on text analysis from a popular Chinese micro-blogging site). They provide evidence that these changes are driven by psychological channels. They find no robust effect of exposure to the virus on risk aversion or trust.
Guo et al. (2020) deploy the same set of decision-making tasks as implemented in this study but using a quite different design, intended to measure behavioural and emotional responses of Wuhan University subjects to the transmission of viral social media videos connected to the Covid-19 crisis. Unlike the current paper, theirs is not an event study: they only elicit responses at a single point in time, during late January 2020.
3. Experimental design
3.1. Sampling information
A timeline of our experimental sessions and events related to the Covid-19 pandemic is presented in Fig. 2 . All subjects in our experiments were Wuhan University students, randomly selected from a population of 9000 subjects who had previously registered to participate in decision-making experiments. Subjects were not informed about the tasks that they would be asked to complete before registering for an experimental session.
Our baseline sample consists of 206 subjects recruited from this population, in May 2019, for an unrelated research project investigating the impact of the experimental interface on economic decision-making. Following the lockdown of Wuhan city on January 23, 2020, we collected comparable data from a further 396 (different) subjects from the same population, spread evenly across five sampling waves.7 The first wave of 80 subjects was collected on January 24/26, in the immediate aftermath of the Hubei lockdown. The second and third waves of 78 and 80 subjects, respectively, were collected on February 4/6 and 7/8, immediately before and after the death of Dr. Li Wenliang (see earlier discussion). The fourth wave of 78 subjects was collected on February 21/22, by which point there were over 63,000 confirmed cases and 2250 deaths in Hubei province. The final wave of 80 subjects was collected on March 6/7. By this stage, the number of local confirmed cases in Wuhan had dropped to less than one hundred per day, with almost no local confirmed cases in China outside of Wuhan.8 Wuhan closed its final mobile cabin hospital on March 10, just one day before the WHO declared a global pandemic.
In total, we recruited 602 subjects across the baseline and post-lockdown samples. An important factor in determining the generalizability of our results is the extent to which these two samples are comparable. It is possible that those subjects from the population who elected to participate in the experiments during the post-lockdown period differed in unobserved ways to those in the baseline sample. We acknowledge this concern and make two responses.
First, we control in our analysis for a range of observed demographic, location-based and interface variables (Table 1 ). The only systematic differences we identify between samples are in the proportions of female subjects and economics majors, which are higher in the baseline sample. All results derived below are robust to controlling for these variables. We also use geo-location data to identify those subjects based in Wuhan or elsewhere in Hubei province during the crisis. Since most students from areas outside of Wuhan had already left the city in early January to celebrate the Lunar New Year holiday, we can exploit quasi-random variation in exposure to the virus.
Table 1.
Full sample |
Baseline |
Wave 1 |
Wave 2 |
Wave 3 |
Wave 4 |
Wave 5 |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Number of subjects | 602 | 206 | 80 | 78 | 80 | 78 | 80 |
Age | 20.41 | 20.17 | 20.11 | 20.58 | 20.70 | 21.05 | 20.23 |
[1.84] | [1.67] | [1.51] | [1.90] | [1.78] | [2.68] | [1.27] | |
Female | 0.59 | 0.69 | 0.56 | 0.45 | 0.56 | 0.56 | 0.55 |
[0.49] | [0.46] | [0.50] | [0.50] | [0.50] | [0.50] | [0.50] | |
Econ major | 0.22 | 0.32 | 0.20 | 0.19 | 0.20 | 0.12 | 0.11 |
[0.41] | [0.47] | [0.40] | [0.40] | [0.40] | [0.32] | [0.32] | |
Monthly Expenditure | 2.58 | 2.57 | 2.54 | 2.51 | 2.62 | 2.59 | 2.65 |
[0.68] | [0.71] | [0.62] | [0.68] | [0.68] | [0.71] | [0.66] | |
Annual Income | 2.60 | 2.62 | 2.50 | 2.68 | 2.66 | 2.55 | 2.59 |
[0.94] | [0.93] | [0.87] | [1.05] | [1.01] | [0.91] | [0.90] | |
Wuhan | 0.10 | 0.11 | 0.10 | 0.12 | 0.06 | 0.08 | |
[0.29] | [0.32] | [0.31] | [0.33] | [0.25] | [0.27] | ||
Other Hubei | 0.21 | 0.18 | 0.19 | 0.25 | 0.14 | 0.31 | |
[0.41] | [0.38] | [0.40] | [0.44] | [0.35] | [0.47] | ||
Phone Size (in) | 5.89 | 5.74 | 6.04 | 5.93 | 5.93 | 5.99 | 5.93 |
[0.54] | [0.58] | [0.47] | [0.48] | [0.59] | [0.49] | [0.52] | |
iOS | 0.21 | 0.25 | 0.19 | 0.13 | 0.25 | 0.10 | 0.26 |
[0.41] | [0.44] | [0.39] | [0.34] | [0.44] | [0.31] | [0.44] |
Note: Mean values with standard deviation in square brackets. Monthly Expenditure (Annual Income): 1 = less than 800 (30,000) RMB; 2 = RMB; 3 = RMB; 4 = RMB; 5 = greater than 4000 (400,000) RMB.
Second, we followed up with 92 subjects from our baseline sample over February 15, and 16, 2020 and elicited repeat measures for the full set of decision-making tasks. We will discuss the comparability of behaviours between this repeat sample and the temporally closest post-lockdown waves after presenting the main results.
3.2. Economic games
The following two sub-sections describe the decision-making tasks that were implemented in our experiment.9 We employed a standard set of incentivized two-person games used in the experimental economics literature to measure strategic behaviours. In each game, subjects were randomly matched into pairs within the session and assigned to the role of either player 1 or player 2. Details of the games and what they measure are summarized below:
-
•
Dictator game. Player 1 is allotted 5 RMB and decides how to allocate this sum of money between the two players in the pair. Player1s allocation is final. Player 2 has no decision to make. Amounts allocated by player 1 provide a measure of subjects’ altruism.
-
•
Ultimatum game. Player 1 is allotted 8 RMB and proposes an allocation of this sum between the two players in the pair. Player 2 can choose to accept or reject the allocation. In case of rejection, both players receive zero payoff for the task. Acceptance rates capture subjects’ perceptions of fairness; offers capture expectations about reciprocity.
-
•
Trust game. Player 1 is allotted 8 RMB and decides how much of this sum of money to send to player 2. Any money sent is multiplied by a factor of three before reaching player 2. Any money not sent is kept by player 1. Player 2 observes the multiplied transfer and decides how much of it to return to player 1. Any money not returned is kept by player 2. Amounts sent capture subjects’ trust; amounts returned capture subjects’ trustworthiness.
-
•
Prisoner’s Dilemma game. Each player makes a simultaneous decision to Cooperate or Defect. The choices are framed neutrally as options C or D. If both players choose Cooperate, both players earn 6 RMB. If both players choose Defect, both players earn 3 RMB. If one player chooses Cooperate and the other player chooses Defect, the cooperating player earns 0 RMB and the defecting player earns 9 RMB. Choices in this game provide a measure of subjects’ cooperative tendencies.
-
•
Stag Hunt game. Each player makes a simultaneous decision to choose a Safe or Risky option. The choices are framed neutrally as options A or B. If both players choose Safe, both players earn 3 RMB. If both players choose Risky, both players earn 8 RMB. If one player chooses Safe and the other player chooses Risky, the safe player earns 3 RMB and the risky player earns 0 RMB. As suggested by Skyrms (2004), we interpret a Risky choice as the propensity to coordinate. We measure the level of coordination in the Stag Hunt game as the percentage of players choosing the Risky action.
3.3. Individual decision-making tasks
In addition to the economic games described in the previous sub-section, we elicited information about subjects’ attitudes towards risk and ambiguity by implementing the following incentivized decision-making tasks:10
-
•
Risk attitude elicitation (gain domain). Subjects are presented with a series of nine pairwise choices between a lottery (option A) and a sure amount of money (option B). The lottery remains fixed across all choices: a 50 percent chance of receiving 9 RMB, and a 50 percent chance of receiving 3 RMB. The sure amount increases uniformly with each choice from 3 RMB to 9 RMB in increments of 0.75 RMB.11 After all choices have been made, the system randomly selects one of the nine pairs of options and, depending on the option chosen for this pair, determines the payoff for the task. A later switching point (higher certainty equivalent) indicates greater willingness to take risks.
-
•
Risk attitude elicitation (loss domain). Identical to the risk attitude elicitation in the gain domain except that now the lotteries and sure amounts are framed as losses and the sure amount decreases uniformly with each choice from 9 RMB to 3 RMB. A later switching point (higher certainty equivalent) indicates greater willingness to take risks. The payoff-relevant amount was subtracted from the subject’s earnings at the end of the experiment.
-
•
Ambiguity attitude elicitation. Identical to the risk attitude elicitation except that now, if subjects choose the lottery, a ball is randomly drawn from an opaque urn. The urn contains both red and blue balls, but the number of each colour is unknown. If the draw is red, they earn 9 RMB. If the draw is blue, they earn 3 RMB.12 A later switching point (higher certainty equivalent) indicates greater willingness to seek out unknown situations.
3.4. Procedural details
Invitations to participate in an experimental session were sent using the cloud-based Ancademy platform (https://www.ancademy.org/). Ancademy is based on the open interface of WeChat. Upon joining a session, subjects were redirected to a welcome screen describing the general experiment guidelines. The economic games and individual decision-making tasks were then completed sequentially, with instructions provided on arrival at each task.13 Feedback was provided only after completion of all tasks. We excluded from a session either the Ultimatum or Trust game task.14 At the end of a session, subjects answered a short questionnaire eliciting standard demographic information, before viewing a screen containing his or her decision outcomes and payment information.
Subjects were paid based upon the outcomes of all tasks. We made payments via the WeChat pay facility on the same day. The experimental tasks were computerized using oTree (Chen et al., 2016). Subjects were instructed to complete the tasks using their mobile phone and we were able to check compliance with this instruction in the data. Subjects were able to contact the experimenter via WeChat during the course of a session in case of any questions. No subject could participate in more than one session. Sessions lasted approximately forty-five minutes and payments averaged 65.68 RMB (about 9.5 US dollars), including a participation fee of 10 RMB.
4. Results
4.1. Pre- versus post-lockdown analysis
We first conduct statistical comparison tests of the null hypotheses that our preference measures are equal between baseline and post-lockdown samples. Table 2 summarizes the non-parametric test results. We observe some evidence of an increase in pro-sociality between samples as measured by first-mover amounts sent in the Dictator and Ultimatum games, although the evidence for this is weak (respectively, p-value = 0.083 and p-value = 0.076). The propensity to cooperate in a Prisoner’s Dilemma game increases by nearly one-third in the post-lockdown sample (p-value = 0.017). This does not, however, coincide with an improvement in coordination: the percentage of players choosing the Risky action in the Stag Hunt game is 11 percentage points lower in the post-lockdown sample versus the baseline (p-value 0.01). There is no significant difference in amounts sent by first-movers in the Trust game (p-value = 0.50); later we will provide evidence to suggest that this null trust result masks systematic variation within the post-lockdown waves.15
Table 2.
Sample | 2019 Baseline |
2020 Post-lockdown |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
mean | sd. | mean | sd. | ||||
Dictator game [0,5] | 1.45 | 1.08 | 1.65 | 1.08 | * | ||
Stag Hunt game {0,1} | 0.88 | 0.33 | 0.77 | 0.42 | *** | ||
Prisoner’s Dilemma game {0,1} | 0.31 | 0.46 | 0.41 | 0.49 | ** | ||
Trust game sent [0,8] | 3.39 | 2.59 | 3.68 | 2.62 | |||
Ultimatum game offer [0,8] | 3.08 | 1.04 | 3.30 | 1.21 | * | ||
Risk attitude, gain {1,2,...,10} | 4.45 | 1.13 | 4.71 | 1.34 | ** | ||
Risk attitude, loss {1,2,...,10} | 6.42 | 1.14 | 6.27 | 1.18 | ** | ||
Ambiguity attitude {1,2,...,10} | 4.49 | 0.33 | 4.23 | 1.55 | *** | ||
Observations | 206 | 396 |
, , , based on two-tailed Wilcoxon rank-sum tests except for Stag Hunt and Prisoner’s Dilemma games, which are based on two-tailed Fisher’s Exact tests.
We also uncover significant differences in risk-related attitudes between the two samples. Recall that risk neutrality would require a switching point between 5 and 6 in our risk elicitation tasks. The aggregate measures in both samples imply risk aversion in the gain domain and risk-seeking in the loss domain, which would be consistent with the diminishing sensitivity predictions of prospect theory.16 In our post-lockdown sample, the average switching point moves significantly higher in the gain domain elicitation task (p-value = 0.011) and significantly lower in the loss domain elicitation task (p-value = 0.039). In other words, subjects’ preferences move towards risk neutrality after the Covid-19 shock. There is also heightened ambiguity aversion among subjects in the post-lockdown sample, who reveal a significantly lower valuation of uncertain lotteries (p-value 0.01).
These diverging patterns in risk-related attitudes can be seen in Appendix Fig. A1, in which we present cumulative probability distributions of the underlying switching points across the risk and ambiguity attitude elicitation tasks.17 Approximately 80% of subjects in both the baseline and post-lockdown samples display risk aversion in gains and risk-seeking in losses. The post-lockdown distribution of switching points in the gain domain lies weakly to the right of the baseline distribution; the reverse is observed in the loss domain among all but the most risk-seeking subjects. There is also a shift left in the distribution of switching points in the ambiguity elicitation task post-lockdown.
Since differential selection between baseline and post-lockdown samples is a potential confound, we cannot rely on perfect randomization from the population to establish a causal effect. Thus, we conduct regression analyses to check whether the aggregate differences reported above remain after controlling for the set of observable sample characteristics documented in Table 1. In particular, this enables us to account for the imbalances observed in gender and academic major. The regression results are reported in Table 3 . In the main text, we report specifications based on OLS and Logistic regressions only. The impact of the exogenous Covid-19 shock is captured by a dummy variable for the post-lockdown sample. The results are robust to the use of alternative estimation methods that mitigate statistical issues involved in the use of censored and count data for the dependent variable (see Appendix Tables A2 and A3).
Table 3.
Dependent variable |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Trust | Pro-sociality |
Cooperation |
Risk |
Ambiguity | ||||
DG | UG | SH | PD | Gain | Loss | |||
OLS | OLS | OLS | Logistic | Logistic | OLS | OLS | OLS | |
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | |
Post-lockdown | 0.85 | 0.27 | 0.15 | 0.70 | 0.63 | 0.26 | 0.24 | 0.32 |
(0.45) | (0.14) | (0.21) | (0.27) | (0.21) | (0.12) | (0.11) | (0.13) | |
Wuhan | 2.35 | 0.26 | 0.03 | 0.08 | 0.76 | 0.16 | 0.01 | 0.20 |
(0.68) | (0.24) | (0.50) | (0.42) | (0.41) | (0.21) | (0.17) | (0.25) | |
Other Hubei | 1.28 | 0.20 | 0.65 | 0.06 | 0.33 | 0.23 | 0.10 | 0.01 |
(0.68) | (0.19) | (0.24) | (0.31) | (0.26) | (0.16) | (0.16) | (0.20) | |
Female | 1.11 | 0.35 | 0.19 | 0.19 | 0.47 | 0.19 | 0.13 | 0.10 |
(0.40) | (0.12) | (0.19) | (0.22) | (0.18) | (0.11) | (0.09) | (0.12) | |
Econ. major | 0.91 | 0.05 | 0.39 | 0.15 | 0.17 | 0.06 | 0.06 | 0.08 |
(0.54) | (0.15) | (0.28) | (0.28) | (0.23) | (0.12) | (0.12) | (0.14) | |
Intercept | 3.15 | 4.09 | 5.58 | 1.79 | 2.74 | 3.36 | 3.28 | 0.21 |
(6.73) | (1.55) | (2.41) | (2.93) | (2.30) | (1.77) | (1.30) | (1.82) | |
Control Variables | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Observations | 153 | 304 | 151 | 593 | 594 | 565 | 581 | 573 |
0.16 | 0.09 | 0.13 | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.03 | |||
Log Likelihood | 280.50 | 382.35 |
Note:0.1; 0.05; 0.01. Coefficient estimates with robust standard errors in parentheses.
Table A2.
Dependent variable: |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Trust | DG | UG | Risk+ | Risk | Ambiguity | |
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | |
Post-lockdown | 1.47 | 0.31 | 0.15 | 0.27 | 0.23 | 0.32 |
(0.67) | (0.18) | (0.21) | (0.13) | (0.11) | (0.14) | |
Wuhan | 3.79 | 0.33 | 0.10 | 0.16 | 0.01 | 0.20 |
(1.22) | (0.33) | (0.55) | (0.21) | (0.17) | (0.26) | |
Other Hubei | 1.81 | 0.26 | 0.65 | 0.25 | 0.10 | 0.03 |
(1.01) | (0.23) | (0.24) | (0.17) | (0.17) | (0.22) | |
Female | 1.60 | 0.45 | 0.18 | 0.18 | 0.13 | 0.11 |
(0.60) | (0.16) | (0.20) | (0.11) | (0.10) | (0.13) | |
Econ | 1.58 | 0.04 | 0.41 | 0.07 | 0.06 | 0.08 |
(0.83) | (0.19) | (0.29) | (0.12) | (0.12) | (0.15) | |
(Intercept) | 1.28 | 4.29 | 5.46 | 3.31 | 3.19 | 0.28 |
(10.08) | (1.94) | (2.45) | (1.86) | (1.35) | (1.97) | |
Control Variables | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Observations | 153 | 304 | 151 | 565 | 581 | 573 |
Log Likelihood | 322.52 | 466.37 | 228.00 | 954.51 | 909.21 | - 1,053.97 |
Wald Test | 38.44 | 173.02 | 23.75 | 16.57 | 33.11 | 26.22 |
Note:; ; . Coefficient estimates with robust standard errors in parentheses.
Table A3.
Dependent variable: |
|||
---|---|---|---|
Risk+ | Risk | Ambiguity | |
(1) | (2) | (3) | |
Post-lockdown | 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.07 |
(0.03) | (0.02) | (0.03) | |
Wuhan | 0.03 | 0.002 | 0.05 |
(0.04) | (0.03) | (0.06) | |
Other Hubei | 0.05 | 0.02 | 0.002 |
(0.03) | (0.03) | (0.05) | |
Female | 0.04 | 0.02 | 0.02 |
(0.02) | (0.01) | (0.03) | |
Econ | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.02 |
(0.03) | (0.02) | (0.03) | |
(Intercept) | 1.25 | 1.36 | 0.51 |
(0.39) | (0.20) | (0.42) | |
Control Variables | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Observations | 565 | 581 | 573 |
Log Likelihood | 1,049.87 | 1,134.67 | 1,087.14 |
Note:; ; . Coefficient estimates with robust standard errors in parentheses.
The regressions support all but one of the aggregate results reported in Table 2. There is support at the 5% level of statistical significance for the observed reductions of risk aversion in gains and risk tolerance in losses, and for the increase in ambiguity aversion, post-lockdown. We observe a strong positive effect of the post-lockdown dummy on cooperation in the Prisoner’s Dilemma and the negative effect on coordination in the Stag Hunt game (both p-values 0.01). There is also some evidence of higher altruism and trust in the post-lockdown sample (p-value = 0.054 and p-value 0.060, respectively).18 Offers in the Ultimatum Game, however, are no longer significantly above those in the baseline sample (p-values = 0.46). This effect appears to be subsumed by a strong positive coefficient estimate on the indicator for being in Hubei (outside Wuhan) during the lockdown period. Other notable heterogeneous effects include a significant fall in trust among those subjects located in Wuhan during the lockdown (p-value 0.01), although we emphasize that these subjects make up a relatively small fraction of the sample. Female subjects are more risk averse, trusting, altruistic and cooperative than their male counterparts.19
In Table A1, we present an additional regression analysis of second-mover behaviour in the Trust and Ultimatum game tasks. For the Trust game, the dependent variable is player 2’s return amount and for the Ultimatum game, the dependent variable is player 2’s acceptance decision. Since the distribution of first-mover actions varies between the baseline and post-lockdown samples, we interact the amount sent/offered with the post-lockdown sample dummy to check for differential levels of reciprocity. As expected, there is a positive and significant correlation between amounts sent (offered) and return amounts (acceptances). There is no significant difference in either trustworthiness or conditional acceptance decisions between the baseline and post-lockdown samples. There is strong evidence that those subjects based in Wuhan during 2020 are more agreeable during Ultimatum bargaining, although again this observation is based on a small sample of player 2 s.
Table A1.
Dependent variable: |
||
---|---|---|
TG return | UG accept | |
OLS | logistic | |
(1) | (2) | |
Send/offer | 1.28 | 2.76 |
(0.15) | (1.48) | |
Post-lockdown | 0.12 | 2.45 |
(0.54) | (4.12) | |
Wuhan | 0.74 | 15.53 |
(0.54) | (1.48) | |
Other Hubei | 0.75 | 0.27 |
(0.78) | (1.37) | |
Female | 0.65 | 0.58 |
(0.37) | (0.77) | |
Econ | 0.26 | 1.04 |
(0.46) | (0.74) | |
Send/offer*Post-lockdown | 0.23 | 0.72 |
(0.19) | (1.41) | |
(Intercept) | 7.21 | 14.54 |
(5.63) | (11.76) | |
Control Variables | Yes | Yes |
Observations | 144 | 146 |
0.74 | ||
Log Likelihood | 27.18 |
Note:; ; . Coefficient estimates with robust standard errors in parentheses.
4.2. Post-lockdown transitory effects
An advantage of our repeated cross-sectional wave design is that we can complement the event analysis of the previous sub-section with an investigation of how preferences evolved within the six weeks after announcement of the first Covid-19 related lockdown. Differential selection between the five 2020 sampling waves is unlikely to be an issue given the short time horizon involved and since all these waves were elicited in a “post-Covid” world.
In Fig. 3 , we plot the evolution of mean responses for the decision-making tasks in each experimental sampling wave, with associated 95 percent confidence intervals. There are interesting transitory effects between sampling waves. To formally examine these, we conduct a regression analysis on the post-lockdown data only (see Table 4 ) and include dummy variables for each independent 2020 sampling wave, along with the full set of control variables in Table 1.20 We specify the reference wave to be Wave 3; this is the midpoint during post-lockdown sampling and has the benefit of clearly revealing any short-term preference changes in the immediate aftermath of the death of Dr. Li Wenliang.
Table 4.
Dependent variable: |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Trust | DG | UG | SH | PD | Risk+ | Risk | Ambiguity | |
OLS | OLS | OLS | Logistic | Logistic | OLS | OLS | OLS | |
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | |
Wave 1 | 1.85 | 0.16 | 0.11 | 0.71 | 0.50 | 0.43 | 0.22 | 0.04 |
(0.64) | (0.22) | (0.36) | (0.40) | (0.35) | (0.17) | (0.18) | (0.25) | |
Wave 2 | 1.47 | 0.29 | 0.18 | 0.67 | 0.34 | 0.67 | 0.18 | 0.25 |
(0.66) | (0.22) | (0.38) | (0.40) | (0.34) | (0.23) | (0.20) | (0.26) | |
Wave 4 | 0.28 | 0.17 | 0.39 | 0.02 | 0.34 | 0.44 | 0.38 | 0.25 |
(0.81) | (0.23) | (0.33) | (0.36) | (0.34) | (0.21) | (0.18) | (0.27) | |
Wave 5 | 0.18 | 0.51 | 0.03 | 0.64 | 0.11 | 0.70 | 0.20 | 0.31 |
(0.71) | (0.22) | (0.29) | (0.40) | (0.33) | (0.23) | (0.20) | (0.27) | |
Intercept | 12.77 | 2.84 | 3.32 | 1.50 | 4.59 | 0.62 | 2.18 | 2.23 |
(7.72) | (1.96) | (3.47) | (3.52) | (2.95) | (2.52) | (1.75) | (2.56) | |
Control Variables | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Observations | 99 | 196 | 97 | 388 | 389 | 366 | 378 | 370 |
0.38 | 0.15 | 0.26 | 0.06 | 0.07 | 0.05 | |||
Log Likelihood | 199.71 | 252.30 |
Note:0.1; 0.05; 0.01. Coefficient estimates with robust standard errors in parentheses.
This analysis suggests several acute preference perturbations. Wave 3 is strongly associated with lower trust and increased risk aversion in gains, versus Wave 2 (both p-values 0.01). Moreover, these effects are short-lived: by Waves 5, both measures return to their levels recorded in Wave 2. While we cannot definitively identify these effects with the death of Dr. Li Wenliang, the data provides compelling evidence that temporary changes in public sentiment may have marked behavioural implications. The other notable finding is that generalized trust is at a very low level in the first few days after the Wuhan lockdown, but recovered quickly in the second Wave.
With this in mind, recall that our earlier finding as to the impact of the Covid-19 outbreak and associated measures on risk attitudes contrasts with Bu et al. (2020), who report a generalized increase in risk aversion in a separate population of Wuhan-based subjects. Aside from differences in dates of the baseline sample for comparison (May versus October 2019) and risk elicitation measures (incentivized lottery choices in our study versus hypothetical allocation and self-reported attitudes in theirs), one explanation for the discrepancy may be that their follow-up survey was conducted in late February 2020, after the death of Dr. Li Wenliang.
4.3. Baseline re-sampling and selective participation
In our final empirical analysis, we present results from a wave of 92 subject who participated in our original baseline study and then again in a wave conducted February 15–16 2020. We conducted this re-sampling to evaluate whether any changes in their paired responses matched the treatment effects observed in our pre- and post-Covid announcements. We provide further evaluation by comparing the re-sampling responses to the immediately preceding and succeeding waves. We view these evaluations as a second check, in addition to previous multivariate regression analyses, for potential selective subject participation in our experiment.
In Table 5 , we report the averages of the sub-sample’s Baseline and Re-sample responses to the two risk preference and the ambiguity tasks.21 Comparing their baseline and re-sample choices via Wilcoxon signed-rank tests we don’t find treatment effects for the Risk attitude task both in the gain and loss domain. On the other hand we do find a significant increase (-value 0.01) in average ambiguity aversion; consistent with our previous findings.
Table A4.
Baseline |
Wave 1 |
Wave 2 |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
mean | sd. | mean | sd. | mean | sd. | |||
Dictator game [0,5] | 1.45 | 1.08 | 1.58 | 1.07 | 1.67 | 1.05 | ||
Stag Hunt game {0,1} | 0.88 | 0.33 | 0.84 | 0.37 | 0.82 | 0.39 | ||
Prisoner’s Dilemma game {0,1} | 0.31 | 0.46 | 0.50 | 0.50 | ||||
Trust game sent [0,8] | 3.39 | 2.59 | 1.80 | 2.48 | ||||
Ultimatum game offer [0,8] | 3.08 | 1.04 | 3.36 | 1.21 | 3.16 | 1.31 | ||
Risk attitude, gain {1,2,...,10} | 4.45 | 1.13 | 0.86 | 1.47 | ||||
Risk attitude, loss {1,2,...,10} | 6.42 | 1.14 | 6.29 | 0.98 | 1.18 | |||
Ambiguity attitude {1,2,...,10} | 4.49 | 1.33 | 1.4 | 1.46 | ||||
Number of subjects | 206 | 80 | 78 | |||||
Wave 3 |
Wave 4 |
Wave 5 |
||||||
mean | sd. | mean | sd. | mean | sd. | |||
Dictator game [0,5] | 1.44 | 1.01 | 1.65 | 1.12 | 1.04 | |||
Stag Hunt game {0,1} | 0.46 | 0.46 | 0.80 | 0.40 | ||||
Prisoner’s Dilemma game {0,1} | 0.39 | 0.49 | 0.47 | 0.49 | 0.50 | |||
Trust game sent [0,8] | 2.44 | 3.66 | 2.73 | 2.59 | ||||
Ultimatum game offer [0,8] | 3.38 | 1.04 | 3.05 | 1.28 | 1.23 | |||
Risk attitude, gain {1,2,...,10} | 1.18 | 4.71 | 1.42 | 1.58 | ||||
Risk attitude, loss {1,2,...,10} | 1.31 | 1.00 | 6.28 | 1.34 | ||||
Ambiguity attitude {1,2,...,10} | 1.75 | 4.41 | 1.55 | 1.55 | ||||
Number of subjects | 80 | 78 | 80 |
The following tests are two-tailed Wilcoxon rank-sum tests. Note:, , for comparison of means Wave versus Baseline. , , for comparison of means Wave 5 versus Wave 1. , , for comparison of means Wave 3/4/5 versus Wave 2.
Table 5.
Baseline Re-sample |
2020 new subjects |
|||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2019 May |
2020 Feb 15–16 |
Wave 3 |
Wave 4 |
Wave 3 & 4 |
||||||||||
mean | sd. | mean | sd. | mean | sd. | mean | sd. | mean | sd. | |||||
Risk attitude, gain | 4.49 | 0.92 | 4.51 | 1.06 | 4.26 | 1.18 | 4.71 | 1.42 | 4.48 | 1.32 | ||||
Risk attitude, loss | 6.31 | 0.97 | 6.49 | 1.02 | 1.31 | 6.50 | 1.00 | 6.30 | 1.19 | |||||
Ambiguity attitude | 4.50 | 1.13 | 1.15 | 4.18 | 1.75 | 4.41 | 1.55 | 4.29 | 1.66 | |||||
Number of subjects | 92 | 92 | 80 | 78 | 158 |
0.1, 0.05, 0.01 for comparison of means unpaired sample Wave versus Re-sample; 0.1, 0.05, 0.01 for comparison of means paired Re-sample versus Baseline. For unpaired sample comparisons, two-tailed Wilcoxon rank-sum tests. For paired sample comparisons, two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank tests.
We next compare the responses of the Re-sample to those of Waves 3 and 4. If there isn’t a substantive issue of selective participation we should find no differences. When comparing average responses from individual waves to the Re-sample the only significant difference is for risk attitude for losses in Wave 3. These null results could arise from low power from the small samples. In response, we pool the responses from Waves 3 and 4 before comparing to the Re-sample responses. Hypothesis tests fail to reject a difference in the mean response between the Re-sample and the pooled data. In conclusion, we believe this resampling exercise combined with the previous multivariate regression analysis provides evidence against selective participation bias for our five waves.
5. Discussion and conclusion
In this study, we compare incentivized choice data on pro-social and economic preferences from a baseline sample of Wuhan University students collected pre-Covid, with five independent samples elicited from the same subject population over the six weeks following stringent Covid-related lockdown measures were introduced in China at the beginning of 2020. Our experimental design affords a consistent response medium between samples and instruments. We also use geo-location data to exploit the quasi-random variation in subjects’ exposure to the virus and test for differences in behaviour among those subjects who remained in Wuhan and the wider Hubei province during the post-lockdown period.
We propose that the time and place of our sample gives us a unique opportunity to shed light on this question. Hubei province, during those early days of 2020, is an ideal domain to test the impact of lockdown measures not only because it was the first (and so most unexpected) lockdown, but also because it was the “harshest of lockdowns”.22 To this extent, the uniqueness of our setting is a strength; it cleanly isolates the preference shock under study (List, 2020).
The experimental results suggest that social and risk-related preferences are liable to be influenced by formative events associated with a public health crisis. We find high levels of altruism, cooperation and aversion to risk taking under ambiguity in the immediate aftermath of the lockdown, while trust is significantly lower. Subjects also exhibit greater sensitivity to risk in both gain and loss domains. We go further and uncover significant transitory effects on trust and risk aversion within the post-lockdown sample. In particular, we observe temporary falls in these measures in our third sampling wave, which was conducted during the days that followed the death of a high-profile Chinese whistleblower.
At this point, it is important to compare our findings to those of Lohmann et al. (2021), who also experimentally investigate the short-term impact of the Covid-19 pandemic on pro-social and economic preferences in China. Unlike Lohmann et al. (2021), we find significantly lower risk aversion in gains during the post-lockdown period. One explanation for this discrepancy is differences in the respective sampling periods. Whereas we elicit a series of independent preference measurements between late January and early March, Lohmann et al. (2021) collected their post-Covid data on March 14–17, by which time the most stringent lockdown measures in China were easing.23 Thus, at the time of our sampling, the risks associated with the novel coronavirus were less well-defined.
An interpretation consistent with our data is that individuals were more cautious when the probabilities associated with their actions were unknown (e.g., in risking exposure to the new and unfamiliar virus), but more willing to seek out risk in situations where those probabilities were well-defined (e.g., in seeking out established medical treatments). Since Lohmann et al. (2021) implemented a longitudinal design, it is further possible that attrition in their sample is masking some differences; they found that attrited individuals were significantly less risk averse than non-attrited individuals (see their Table B3).
Lohmann et al. (2021) also find no significant changes in trust or pro-social preferences between their pre- and post-Covid samples. Aside from differences in the saliency of outcomes (the trust game and public good game implemented in Lohmann et al. (2021) were not incentivized in any wave), we again believe differences in sampling dates to be important. Whereas we observe no significant aggregate effect on trust in our post-lockdown sample, we do see a sharp fall in trust within the first sampling wave conducted in late January, a time when the Chinese government’s response to the virus lagged behind public awareness.24
Moreover, in China mortality was largely concentrated in Hubei province. Whereas 42% of subjects in our post-lockdown sample are located in Hubei, this is true of less than 2% of subjects in Lohmann et al. (2021); indeed, half of the cities inhabited by subjects in their dataset had yet to report a death by the time of their survey in mid-March. Thus, exposure to the worst effects of the virus was likely higher in our sample. A related driver of differences could stem from the subject population itself. Subjects in our study were recruited from a database of Wuhan University students; Lohmann et al. (2021) conducted experiments on a sample drawn from a population of students in Beijing. Students of Wuhan University may reasonably be expected (on average) to have closer academic and social ties to Wuhan city, which may have further increased the saliency of events early on.
Next, we comment on possible policy implications of our findings. Many non-pharmaceutical interventions designed to reduce the spread of respiratory diseases rely on initial, short-term behavioural responses among community members. Survey data collected during the SARS 1 outbreak of the early 2000s suggested the importance of behavioural and psycho-social factors in the adoption of protective measures (Leung et al., 2003). Recent evidence on measures implemented to mitigate the transmission of Covid-19 have uncovered a positive correlation between an individual’s propensity to engage in precautionary behaviour and his or her level of pro-sociality and risk aversion (Campos-Mercade, Meier, Schneider, Wengström, 2021, Müller, Rau, 2021), and between generalized trust levels and perceptions of the appropriateness of media coverage (Müller and Rau, 2021).
Our results imply an additional confound in this relationship. That is, the onset of a public health crisis itself may have unintended consequences for economic preferences that determine population compliance with interventions designed to reduce the spread of a novel coronavirus. For example, if greater risk aversion is associated with an increased propensity to avoid public gatherings, then a shift towards greater risk tolerance in the population will make deterring such behaviours more difficult. Conversely, if individuals become less willing to take risk under ambiguity, then governments can harness this knowledge by openly communicating to the public uncertainties surrounding scientific estimates and rapidly emerging evidence (see OECD, 2020, for policy recommendations in this vein).
A related implication concerns pro-social preferences. Many Western governments have appealed to the public’s notions of pro-sociality in their pandemic response (the slogan ‘Stay Home, Protect the NHS, Save Lives’ in the UK is but one example). If crises induce greater cooperative tendencies, then such an approach is more likely to pay social dividends. Similarly, if generalized trust correlates positively with perceptions of media coverage on the virus, then a negative shock to trust will reduce individuals’ willingness to engage with a government’s messaging about the virus and in turn their likelihood of complying with recommended personal protective measures. This underscores the requirement that governments take measures to actively promote the public’s trust in their actions. The UK government, for example, has recently recognised the role of data transparency underpinning decisions.25
Finally, there are some important limitations of our study that we want to emphasize. As discussed in detail in the previous section, differential selection into the baseline and post-lockdown samples is a potential confound. While we took steps to mitigate sample imbalances in our analysis, we cannot rule out that unobserved factors influenced subjects’ selection into the post-lockdown sample and that these may also correlate with the deep economic parameters of interest. Second, we use a student sample. While student samples are not representative of the full population, their behaviours display reduced measurement error and correlate positively with non-student samples (Snowberg and Yariv, 2018). They are also a demographic that has received attention in the West regarding compliance with lockdown policies (Moore et al., 2020). Third, we cannot observe the underlying channels driving the observed preference effects. For example, Lohmann et al. (2021) provide evidence to suggest that psychological and emotional factors were important behavioural mediators in China during the pandemic. Finally, our study is context-specific and we cannot generalize to other populations. We thus view our study as adding to a collective body of evidence on the behavioural impact of the Covid-19 pandemic.
Declaration of competing interest
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
All experiments in this study were approved by the Academy of Humanities and Social Sciences, Wuhan University. The authors gratefully acknowledge the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant no. 72173093), the National Key R&D Program of China (Grant no. 2019YFE0126800), and the ESRC/FNR under grant number ES/P011829/1.
In China, Covid-19 was referred to as pneumonia early on.
This event was also widely covered by media outlets in the Western world. See, for example: https://www.theguardian.com/global-development/2020/feb/07/coronavirus-chinese-rage-death-whistleblower-doctor-li-wenliang.
Zhong Nanshan is often referred to as the public face of China’s fight against the virus. See, for example: https://www.npr.org/2020/04/15/835308147/meet-dr-zhong-nanshan-the-public-face-of-the-Covid-19-fight-in-china?t=1596625232619.
For a survey of some important results in this literature, see Chuang and Schechter (2015).
Several perturbations in pro-social and risk-related preferences have been documented for Europe since the onset of the Covid-19 pandemic (Brañas-Garza, Jorrat, Alfonso, Espín, García, Kovářík, 2020, David, Sade, 2020, Huber, Huber, Kirchler, 2020, Graeber, Schmidt, Schroeder, Seebauer, 2020).
Wuhan University of Science and Technology is a separate research institution to Wuhan University.
In separate work in progress, we find that subjects’ interface affects certain risk-related behaviours. Accordingly we maintain a consistent experimental interface between the pre-crisis baseline sample and the subsequent experimental waves. We also exclude from our analysis data a further four subjects in our 2020 waves and eighteen subjects in our baseline sample who completed the tasks using a computer after we asked them to complete tasks using a mobile phone.
Virus case data is obtained from WHO situation reports, which can be found at: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/situation-reports/.
We collected data from a further four tasks designed to inform on subjects’ levels of cognitive reasoning, lying propensity and time preferences, which are not reported on here. Details of these tasks are included in the experimental instructions.
We exclude data from 29 (13) [21] subjects who switch more than once in the risk attitude gain domain (loss domain) [ambiguity] elicitation task.
An English translation of the original experiment instructions for each task is available in the Supplementary Materials.
We did this to preclude the possibility that second movers condition their actions on the first mover’s prior decision (due to the sequential nature of the tasks).
It is difficult to draw valid inferences from unconditional statistics on second-mover decisions and so we postpone discussion of these measures for the Trust and Ultimatum games until the regression analysis.
We are unable to say more than this because our design does not permit identification of the underlying value or probability weighting functions.
Similar inferences can be made from the kernel density estimates; the cumulative probabilities are preferred here because they do not rely on any distributional assumptions.
The impact on trust is significant at the 5% level if we account for censoring in the dependent variable (see Table A2).
It has been suggested that the social preferences of women are more malleable (Croson and Gneezy, 2009). We find no significant interaction between gender and pre-/post-lockdown periods in our sample.
Non-parametric test results for the comparisons discussed in this sub-section are contained in Table A4 and confirm the main results.
Due to the Re-sample subjects having the opportunity to learn in the game tasks after from their previous play and feedback we don’t report those results.
For example, on March 13, Huangshi became the first Hubei city to lift local travel restrictions: https://news.sina.com.cn/c/2020-03-13/doc-iimxyqwa0259279.shtml.
See parliamentary committee report available at:https://publications.parliament.uk/pa/cm5801/cmselect/cmpubadm/803/80302.htm.
This task is an adaptation of the well-established Holt and Laury (2002) multiple price list format.
This task is based on the original thought experiment of Ellsberg (1961).
Appendix A.
References
- Andersen S., Hanspal T., Nielsen K.M. Once bitten, twice shy: the power of personal experiences in risk taking. J. Financ. Econ. 2019;132(3):97–117. [Google Scholar]
- Andrabi T., Das J. In aid we trust: hearts and minds and the Pakistan earthquake of 2005. Rev. Econ. Stat. 2017;99(3):371–386. doi: 10.1162/REST_a_00638. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Angrisani M., Cipriani M., Guarino A., Kendall R., Ortiz de Zarate J. Working Paper. FRB of New York Staff Report No. 927; 2020. Risk Preferences at the Time of Covid-19: An Experiment with Professional Traders and Students. [Google Scholar]
- Banks J., Bassoli E., Mammi I. Changing attitudes to risk at older ages: the role of health and other life events. J. Econ. Psychol. 2020;79:102208. [Google Scholar]; The Impact of Life Experiences on Risk Taking
- Bauer M., Blattman C., Chytilová J., Henrich J., Miguel E., Mitts T. Can war foster cooperation? J. Econ. Perspect. 2016;30(3):249–274. doi: 10.1257/jep.30.3.249. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Beine M., Charness G., Dupuy A., Joxhe M. Working Paper. 2020. Shaking Things Up: On the Stability of Risk and Time Preferences. [Google Scholar]
- Binder C. Coronavirus fears and macroeconomic expectations. Rev. Econ. Stat. 2020;102(4):721–730. [Google Scholar]
- Brañas-Garza P., Jorrat D., Alfonso A., Espín A.M., García T., Kovářík J. Working Paper. 2020. Exposure to the Covid-19 Pandemic and Generosity. [Google Scholar]
- Brück T., Ferguson N.T.N., Justino P., Stojetz W. Working Paper. 2020. Trust in the Time of Corona. [Google Scholar]
- Bu D., Hanspal T., Liao Y., Liu Y. Risk taking during a global crisis: evidence from Wuhan. Covid Econ. 2020;5:106–146. [Google Scholar]
- Cameron L., Shah M. Risk-taking behavior in the wake of natural disasters. J. Hum. Resour. 2015;50(2):484–515. [Google Scholar]
- Campos-Mercade P., Meier A.N., Schneider F.H., Wengström E. Prosociality predicts health behaviors during the Covid-19 pandemic. J. Public Econ. 2021;195:104367. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2021.104367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassar A., Healy A., von Kessler C. Trust, risk, and time preferences after a natural disaster: experimental evidence from Thailand. World Dev. 2017;94:90–105. doi: 10.1016/j.worlddev.2016.12.042. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chen D.L., Schonger M., Wickens C. oTree—An open-source platform for laboratory, online, and field experiments. J. Behav. Exp. Finance. 2016;9:88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jbef.2015.12.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Choi J.J., Laibson D., Madrian B.C., Metrick A. Reinforcement learning and savings behavior. J. Finance. 2009;64(6):2515–2534. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6261.2009.01509.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chuang Y., Schechter L. Stability of experimental and survey measures of risk, time, and social preferences: a review and some new results. J. Dev. Econ. 2015;117:151–170. doi: 10.1016/j.jdeveco.2015.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coibion O., Gorodnichenko Y., Weber M. Working Paper. National Bureau of Economic Research; 2020. The Cost of the Covid-19 Crisis: Lockdowns, Macroeconomic Expectations, and Consumer Spending. [Google Scholar]
- Croson R., Gneezy U. Gender differences in preferences. J. Econ. Lit. 2009;47(2):448–474. doi: 10.1257/jel.47.2.448. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- David D.B., Sade O. Working Paper. 2020. Robo-Advisor Adoption, Willingness to Pay, and Trust—Before and at the Outbreak of the Covid-19 Pandemic. [Google Scholar]
- Eckel C.C., El-Gamal M.A., Wilson R.K. Risk loving after the storm: a Bayesian-network study of Hurricane Katrina evacuees. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2009;69(2):110–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2007.08.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ellsberg D. Risk, ambiguity, and the savage axioms. Q. J. Econ. 1961;75(4):643–669. [Google Scholar]
- Fetzer T., Hensel L., Hermle J., Roth C. Coronavirus perceptions and economic anxiety. Rev. Econ. Stat. 2020:1–36. doi: 10.1162/rest_a_00946. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Filipski M., Jin L., Zhang X., Chen K.Z. Living like there’s no tomorrow: the psychological effects of an earthquake on savings and spending behavior. Eur. Econ. Rev. 2019;116:107–128. doi: 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2019.04.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fisman R., Jakiela P., Kariv S. How did distributional preferences change during the great recession? J. Public Econ. 2015;128:84–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2015.06.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming D.A., Chong A., Bejarano H.D. Trust and reciprocity in the aftermath of natural disasters. J. Dev. Stud. 2014;50(11):1482–1493. [Google Scholar]
- Graeber D., Schmidt U., Schroeder C., Seebauer J. Working Paper. 2020. The Effect of a Major Pandemic on Risk Preferences-Evidence from Exposure to COVID-19. [Google Scholar]
- Guiso L., Sapienza P., Zingales L. Time varying risk aversion. J. Financ. Econ. 2018;128(3):403–421. [Google Scholar]
- Guo Y., Shachat J., Walker M.J., Wei L. ESI Working Paper. 2020. Viral Social Media Videos Can Raise Pro-Social Behaviours When an Epidemic Arises. [Google Scholar]; https://digitalcommons.chapman.edu/esi_working_papers/310/.
- Hanaoka C., Shigeoka H., Watanabe Y. Do risk preferences change? Evidence from the great east Japan earthquake. Am. Econ. J. 2018;10(2):298–330. [Google Scholar]
- Hertwig R., Barron G., Weber E.U., Erev I. Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choice. Psychol. Sci. 2004;15(8):534–539. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00715.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holt C.A., Laury S.K. Risk aversion and incentive effects. Am. Econ. Rev. 2002;92(5):1644–1655. [Google Scholar]
- Huber C., Huber J., Kirchler M. Working Paper. 2020. Market Shocks and Professionals’ Investment Behavior Evidence from the Covid-19 Crash. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikeda S., Yamamura E., Tsutsui Y. Working Paper. 2020. Covid-19 Enhanced Diminishing Sensitivity in Prospect-Theory Risk Preferences: A Panel Analysis. [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D., Tversky A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 1979;47(2):263–292. [Google Scholar]
- Kim Y.-I., Lee J. The long-run impact of a traumatic experience on risk aversion. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2014;108:174–186. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2014.09.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kuroishi Y., Sawada Y. Working Paper. 2020. On the Stability of Preferences: Experimental Evidence from Two Disasters. [Google Scholar]
- Leung G.M., Lam T.-H., Ho L.-M., Ho S.-Y., Chan B.H.Y., Wong I.O.L., Hedley A.J. The impact of community psychological responses on outbreak control for severe acute respiratory syndrome in hong kong. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2003;57(11):857–863. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.11.857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li K., Qin Y., Wu J., Yan J. Working Paper. 2020. Containing the Virus or Reviving the Economy? Evidence from Individual Expectations during the Covid-19 Epidemic. [Google Scholar]
- Li K.K. Working Paper. 2020. How Does the Covid-19 Outbreak Affect People’s Expectation about the Macroeconomy? [Google Scholar]
- List J.A. Working Paper. 2020. Non Est Disputandum de Generalizability? A Glimpse into the External Validity Trial. [Google Scholar]
- Lohmann P., Gsottbauer E., Kontoleon A., You J. Working Paper. 2021. Social Preferences and Economic Decision-Making in the Wake of COVID-19: Experimental Evidence from China. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malmendier U., Nagel S. Depression babies: do macroeconomic experiences affect risk taking? Q. J. Econ. 2011;126(1):373–416. [Google Scholar]
- Moore, R. C., Lee, A., Hancock, J. T., Halley, M., Linos, E., 2020. Experience with social distancing early in the Covid-19 pandemic in the United States: implications for public health messaging. medRxiv. 10.1101/2020.04.08.20057067
- Müller S., Rau H.A. Economic preferences and compliance in the social stress test of the Covid-19 crisis. J. Public Econ. 2021;194:104322. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikolov, P., Pape, A., Tonguc, O., Williams, C., 2020. Predictors of social distancing and mask-wearing behavior: Panel survey in seven U.S. states. arXiv:2009.13103
- OECD . OECD Publishing, Paris; 2020. Scientific Advice During Crises: Facilitating Transnational Co-operation and Exchange of Information. [Google Scholar]
- Page L., Savage D.A., Torgler B. Variation in risk seeking behaviour following large losses: a natural experiment. Eur. Econ. Rev. 2014;71:121–131. doi: 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2014.04.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Said F., Afzal U., Turner G. Risk taking and risk learning after a rare event: evidence from a field experiment in Pakistan. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2015;118:167–183. [Google Scholar]; Economic Experiments in Developing Countries.
- Skyrms B. Cambridge University Press; 2004. The Stag Hunt and the Evolution of Social Structure. [Google Scholar]
- Snowberg E., Yariv L. Technical Report. National Bureau of Economic Research; 2018. Testing the Waters: Behavior Across Participant Pools. [Google Scholar]
- Van Bavel J.J., Baicker K., Boggio P.S., Capraro V., Cichocka A., Cikara M., Crockett M.J., Crum A.J., Douglas K.M., Druckman J.N., et al. Using social and behavioural science to support Covid-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;4:460–471. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]