Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2021 Apr 8;34(4):1260–1271. doi: 10.1017/S0954579421000055

Impact of childhood maltreatment and resilience on behavioral and neural patterns of inhibitory control during emotional distraction

Lauren A Demers 1, Ruskin H Hunt 1, Dante Cicchetti 1,2, Julia E Cohen-Gilbert 3, Fred A Rogosch 2, Sheree L Toth 2, Kathleen M Thomas 1
PMCID: PMC8497637  NIHMSID: NIHMS1664873  PMID: 33827733

Abstract

Exposure to childhood maltreatment (CM) may disrupt typical development of neural systems underlying impulse control and emotion regulation. Yet, resilient outcomes are observed in some individuals exposed to CM. Individual differences in adult functioning may result from variation in inhibitory control in the context of emotional distractions, underpinned by cognitive-affective brain circuits. Thirty-eight healthy adults with a history of substantiated CM and 34 non-maltreated adults from the same longitudinal sample performed a Go/No-Go task in which task-relevant stimuli (letters) were presented at the center of task-irrelevant, negative or neutral images, while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging. The comparison group, but not the maltreated group, made increased inhibitory control errors in the context of negative, but not neutral, distractor images. Additionally, the comparison group had greater right inferior frontal gyrus and bilateral frontal pole activation during inhibitory control blocks with negative compared to neutral background images relative to the CM group. Across the full sample, greater adaptive functioning in everyday contexts was associated with superior inhibitory control and greater right frontal pole activation. Results suggest that resilience following early adversity is associated with enhanced attention and behavioral regulation in the context of task-irrelevant negative emotional stimuli in a laboratory setting.

Keywords: childhood maltreatment, inhibitory control, impulsivity, emotion, prefrontal cortex


Child maltreatment (CM) represents one of the most adverse and stressful challenges that a child may experience (Cicchetti & Lynch, 1995) and has complex and multifaceted long-term sequelae. There is a clear link between CM and heightened risk for long-lasting problems in domains associated with poor emotion regulation and impulse control (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010) including poor physical and mental health, drug and alcohol misuse, and criminal and other risky behaviors (Gilbert, Widom, Browne, Fergusson, Webb & Janson, 2009; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, 2011). However, there has been limited research examining the intersection of inhibitory control and emotion in the context of CM history. It is plausible that inhibitory control, the ability to prevent a response or stop an ongoing response (Nigg, 2017), is particularly disturbed during emotionally negative contexts for individuals who experienced maltreatment. For instance, when individuals with a history of CM encounter fearful stimuli in their environment, they might have trouble regulating their emotions while simultaneously avoiding distractions and engaging in goal-directed behavior. In fact, previous work has linked childhood abuse with deficits in emotional conflict regulation (Marusak, Martin, & Etkin & Thomason, 2015; Powers, Etkin, Gyurak, Bradley, & Jovanovic, 2015). Nonetheless, multifinality, or variance in outcomes has been observed in high-risk samples, including maltreated individuals (Cicchetti, Rogosch, Lynch, & Holt, 1993). Recent research provides preliminary evidence that resilient adults show an improved ability to regulate emotions, dampen threat processing, and habituate stress responses (for review, see Moreno-López, Ioannidis, Dahl Askelund, Smith, Shueler, & van Harmelen, 2019), whereas adults with long-lasting negative effects of trauma exposure (e.g., experiencing post-traumatic stress disorder; PTSD) demonstrate impaired attentional control in the context of emotional information (Fani, King, Clendinen, Hardy, Surapaneni, Blair, …, & Ressler, 2019). Individual differences in adaptive functioning during adulthood may result from variation in the ability to inhibit impulses in the context of emotional distractions, underpinned by coordination of cognitive-affective brain circuits.

Executive function differences following CM

A growing body of research that compares individuals with and without CM indicates that executive functions (EF) generally, and inhibitory control in particular, are impaired following CM (van der Bij, Op den Kelder, Montagne, & Hagenaars, 2020). Disruptions in EF, including attention shifting, cognitive and behavioral inhibition, working memory maintenance, and self-regulation and self-monitoring, have been identified consistently in both children and adults exposed to trauma. In children, exposure to familial trauma (relative to non-familial or no trauma exposure) has been associated with poorer performance on an EF composite including working memory, inhibitory control, auditory attention, and processing speed tasks (DePrince, Weinzierl, & Combs, 2009) and on specific measures of inhibitory control (Cowell, Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2015). Children with substantiated abuse histories show more impaired inhibitory control relative to non-abused children, including non-abused children with psychiatric issues (Mezzacappa, Kindlon, & Earls, 2001). Caregivers also report more EF challenges in children exposed to maltreatment relative to non-maltreated children (Fay-Stammbach & Hawes, 2019). In adults exposed to childhood trauma, diminished inhibitory control been also observed (Daly, Hildenbrand, Turner, Berkowitz, & Tarazi, 2017; Navalta, Polcari, Webster, Boghossian, & Teicher, 2006). Poor executive functioning, and elevated impulsivity in particular, often persists into adulthood in maltreated populations (Pechtel & Pizzagalli, 2011).

Several mutually compatible theories have been proposed to explain the link between CM and impaired EF, and elevated impulsivity in particular. It has been posited that impulsive dispositions and associated behavioral difficulties tax parental resources, resulting in an elevated likelihood of parental abuse, particularly when parents are prone to impulsivity themselves (Liu, 2019). A transactional relationship may exist as parental corporal punishment has been associated with an increased likelihood of children engaging in misbehavior (Gershoff, 2002). In addition, it has been theorized that the maltreating rearing environment, often marked by unpredictability, results in prioritization of short-term goals in the face of long-term uncertainties (Belsky, Schlomer, & Ellis, 2011). Furthermore, maltreated children often are deprived of many of the experiences believed to promote adaptive functioning across the lifespan, rendering them vulnerable to physical and psychosocial maladjustment (Cicchetti & Lynch, 1993; Cicchetti & Toth, 2005; Gilbert, Widom, Browne, Fergusson, Webb, & Janson, 2009). For instance, children in maltreating families are less likely to observe healthy emotion management in parents or to be taught how to cope with their own emotions (Muehlenkamp, Kerr, Bradley, & Larsen, 2010). Finally, elevated impulsivity following childhood maltreatment has been linked to the impacts of early adversity on neural development. CM has the potential to alter brain structure and function via disruption of neurodevelopmental processes that occur during childhood and adolescence including, synaptic remodeling, glial cell proliferation, myelination, dendritic and axonal branching, and programmed cell death (de Graaf-Peters and Hadders-Algra, 2006; Sowell, Peterson, Thompson, Welcome, Henkenius, & Toga, 2003). These alterations may be adaptive for children in maltreating contexts where it is important to be vigilant to threat and ready to flee, which sometimes translates to impulsive behaviors.

Psychobiology and neuroimaging research provide further insights into the link between CM and elevated impulsivity. Emerging evidence suggests that CM is associated with deficits in brain regions that support EF including lateral frontostriatal and parieto-temporal regions (Andersen, Tomada, Vincow, Valente, Polcari, & Teicher, 2008; Bremner, Vermetten, Vythilingam, Afzal, Schmahl, Elzinga, & Charney, 2004; Hanson et al., 2010; Hart & Rubia, 2012; Herzog, Niedtfeld, Rausch, Thome, Mueller-Engelmann, Steil, … & Schmahl, 2017; Mackiewicz Seghete, DePrince, Banich, 2018; Mackiewicz Seghete, Kaiser, DePrince, Banich, 2017). Within the frontal lobe, alteration of right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) was recently identified as a potential neurodevelopmental consequence of early adversity (Luby, Barch, Whalen, Tillman, & Belden, 2017; Sun, Haswell, Morey, & De Bellis, 2019). This region in particular has been implicated in inhibitory control with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies using paradigms such as the Go/No-Go and Stop Signal tasks (Aron et al., 2004; Bari & Robbins, 2013; Chikazoe et al., 2007). Thus, altered functioning in the right IFG may help explain CM-related impairments in inhibitory control. While these alterations may have supported functioning that was adaptive in a maltreating context, they are not necessarily adaptive as maltreated children grow into adulthood and are in non-maltreating contexts (Rieder & Cicchetti, 1989). At the same time, it is possible that individuals who are more resistant to the negative consequences associated with CM show a unique neural pattern associated with their resilience. Mental health after CM has been shown to be aided by increased or more flexible connectivity between central executive brain regions and emotion-processing limbic regions (for review, see Moreno-López et al., 2020). Little is known about how resilient functioning may relate to differences in the neural systems supporting EF.

Emotion regulation and processing differences following CM

Relative to the small body of work on EF in the context of CM, the findings of altered emotion regulation following CM are quite numerous. It has been shown that the high levels of stress hormones observed in maltreated children can alter neural systems, especially in prefrontal cortical regions involved in emotion regulation (Hart & Rubia, 2012; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, 2011; Teicher, Samson, Anderson, & Oshashi, 2016). The prefrontal brain regions most consistently identified as structurally altered following CM include ventromedial and orbitofrontal cortex (Hanson, Chung, Avants, Shirtcliff, Gee, Davidson, & Pollak, 2010; Hart & Rubia, 2012). These prefrontal regions have been consistently implicated in the regulation of affective signals from subcortical structures including the amygdala (Phillips, Drevets, Rauch, & Lane, 2003). Elevated threat-related activation of the amygdala itself, particularly on the right side, has also been linked to childhood trauma (Grant, Cannistraci, Hollon, Gore, & Shelton, 2011; Nooner, Mennes, Brown, Castellanos, Leventhal, Milham, & Colcombe, 2013; for review, see Hein & Monk, 2016). Furthermore, frontolimbic functional connectivity—the degree to which activity in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) relates to activity in the limbic system —has also been shown to be altered in maltreated individuals during emotion-processing tasks (Fonzo et al., 2013; Jedd et al., 2015). Thus, brain systems crucial to emotion processing and regulation, including prefrontal and limbic regions, as well as the connectivity between these regions, have been shown to be impacted by CM.

It is important to consider, however, that the impact of CM is not uniform; many individuals with maltreatment histories exhibit resilience in various domains of functioning. Thus, when only group differences in childhood trauma are evaluated, the impact of individual differences in adaptation may be masked. More recent research examining individual differences following CM provides preliminary evidence that altered emotion processing and improved emotion regulation may foster resilience. For instance, individual differences in threat-processing have recently been identified as a transdiagnostic mechanism contributing to the emergence of psychopathology (Weissman, Jenness, Colich, Bryant Miller, Sambrook, Sheridan, & McLaughlin, 2019). Further, a recent study indicates that greater recruitment of prefrontal control regions and stronger modulation of amygdala reactivity during emotional viewing and reappraisal are protective against psychopathology (Rodman, Jenness, Weissman, Pine, & McLaughlin, 2019). Previously maltreated adults without clinically significant psychopathology symptoms have been shown to have lower information transmission from the right amygdala to other brain network nodes when compared to maltreated adults with significant symptomatology (Ohashi, Anderson, Bolger, Khan, McGreenery, & Teicher, 2019). Other research has demonstrated stronger amygdala connectivity in frontal and parietal regions during emotional viewing in maltreated adults with greater current adaptive functioning (Demers, Jedd McKenzie, Hunt, Cicchetti, Cowell, …, & Thomas, 2018). Together, these findings suggest that effective emotion regulation, subserved by strong frontolimbic modulation, may support resilience following CM.

Maltreatment-related differences in inhibitory control in the context of emotion

It remains unknown whether individual differences in inhibitory control in the context of negative emotional distractions and associated neural activation are also protective against negative impacts of early adversity. One behavioral study addressed this interface by using two versions of the laboratory Stroop task in a maltreated sample: 1) a non-emotional task, consisting of emotionally neutral male or female faces presented with a congruent or incongruent word (male or female), and 2) an emotional task, consisting of a neutral or fearful face paired with a congruent or incongruent emotion word (neutral or fearful). Participants were instructed to ignore the word and respond based on the face. Results revealed impaired performance on the emotional, but not the non-emotional task in women with more self-reported childhood abuse. These individuals had the most difficulty with incongruent fearful stimuli (Caldwell, Krug, Carter, & Minzenberg, 2014). Emotional Stroop tasks have been used in two recent, comparably smaller fMRI studies investigating the effects of emotional interference on neural activation in brain regions associated with cognitive control (Herzog et al., 2017; Mackiewicz et al., 2017). While both studies found effects of CM history on neural activation to emotional content in brain regions associated with cognitive control, the direction of findings was mixed. Herzog and colleagues used a region of interest approach and showed greater activation in the dorsolateral PFC, ventromedial PFC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) in the context of trauma-related words in female patients with complex PTSD compared to healthy females with and without trauma exposure (Herzog et al., 2017). Their findings suggest current psychological functioning relates to emotional conflict regulation. In Mackiewicz and colleagues’ study, adult females exposed to childhood abuse showed greater activation of right IFG and the left cerebellum than the control group and less activation in the left dorsolateral PFC and right dACC compared to the control group during the cognitive control condition across all trial types, and greater activation in the left IFG than the control group during emotional trials in particular (Mackiewicz et al., 2017).

The extant literature on inhibitory control in emotional contexts following CM relies primarily on tasks like the emotional Stroop, where in some cases, the emotional stimuli are task-relevant, and in other cases, effects of word valence may be relatively weak. That is, in the study by Caldwell and colleagues, attention to and processing of the affective face stimuli is necessary for task performance (Caldwell et al., 2014). Therefore, this task likely indexes a variety of cognitive and emotional processes simultaneously, making it difficult to determine whether inhibitory control, separate from recognition or interpretation of emotional stimuli, is disrupted specifically in the context of negative distraction. In the studies by Herzog’s and Mackiewicz’s groups, which both use emotion color word Stroop tasks, the emotion word is considered task-irrelevant since reading the word is not actually required during the task and the word is a distractor that captures attention (Herzog et al., 2017; Mackiewicz et al., 2017). Yet, previous work has demonstrated that valence effects on PFC activation are more apparent when stimuli are emotional pictures than when they are words (Kensinger & Schacter, 2006). Therefore, a task with background emotional pictorial stimuli that are unrelated to the task goal may more robustly and accurately index the effect of emotional distraction on inhibitory control. As such, while preliminary evidence suggests CM may put one at risk for elevated emotional interference, the effect of negative emotional distraction on inhibitory control in adults with CM history requires further study.

The present study

In the present study, we evaluated the impact of task-irrelevant negative emotional content on inhibitory control and neural activity in a longitudinal sample of adults with documented histories of child maltreatment. Inclusion of a longitudinal non-maltreated comparison group recruited from the same schools and neighborhoods as the CM group allowed us more to clearly isolate effects of maltreatment from high socioeconomic adversity. Additionally, we assessed whether individual differences in current adaptive functioning related to behavioral or brain measures of inhibitory control in emotional contexts.

We predicted that previously maltreated adults would show poorer inhibitory control than non-maltreated comparison adults under negative distraction, as prior work has shown that maltreated children are more vigilant to threat. Therefore, at the level of the brain, during conditions requiring inhibitory control during emotional distraction, we expected a hyperactive right amygdala response in the CM group, which would indicate greater emotional reactivity to negative valence image distractors during the inhibitory control task. We predicted hypoactive prefrontal regulation, as evidenced by less prefrontal activation, due to its protracted development (de Graaf-Peters & Hadders-Algra, 2006; Sowell, Peterson, Thompson, Welcome, Henkenius, & Toga, 2003), its susceptibility to early life stress (Diorio, Vaiu, & Meaney, 1993), and its role in inhibitory control (Arnsten & Rubia, 2012). In particular, we expected the CM group would show greater activation in the right IFG, a region known to play an important role in impulse control and emotion regulation, based on recent findings implicating functional connectivity for this region in risk for externalizing problems following early adversity (Luby et al., 2017). Finally, we predicted that differences in frontal activity would relate to current adaptive functioning of the participants, such that greater adaptive functioning would be associated with greater frontal cortex engagement.

Methods

Participants

Participants included 72 adults (M = 30.18; 37 males and 35 females) from a longitudinal sample first recruited when they were 6–12 years old through a research summer camp for low-income, high-risk children. At the initial recruitment, 93% of parents reported a history of receiving public assistance. Thirty-eight participants had a history of CM as documented by Department of Human Services (DHS) records, and 34 participants were classified as non-CM based on a lack of DHS records of maltreatment through age 17. The Maternal Maltreatment Classification Interview (MMCI; Cicchetti, Toth, & Manly, 2003) was used to further verify CM history or the lack thereof. Comprehensive DHS records were coded using the maltreatment classification system to classify the type (i.e., neglect, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse) of each report of substantiated maltreatment (Barnett, Manly, & Cicchetti, 1993). The majority (60%) of the CM group experienced more than one type of maltreatment. In adulthood, internalizing and externalizing symptoms were in the broad average range for 90% of the current sample, and not different between groups, suggesting that the present sample is relatively healthy and may represent a resilient subset of maltreated individuals. Relative to participants from the larger longitudinal sample who did not participate in this phase of the study, the current sample had lower rates of adolescent conduct disorder and attention problems and better self-reported attachment to fathers during adolescence, although there were no differences in demographics, maltreatment history, cognitive ability during adolescence, or other psychopathology measures during adolescence. All participants provided informed consent in compliance with the University of Rochester’s Institutional Review Board and were compensated for their time. Demographic information for the current subsample is provided in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographics and sample characteristics for the maltreated and comparison groups

Sample Characteristics Maltreated Group (N=38) Comparison Group (N=34) p-value
Age (years), M (SD) 30.89 (3.25) 29.38 (3.70) .07
Gender, n 17 Males, 21 Females 20 Males, 14 Females .24
Race, n [%] .32
 Black 25 [65.8%] 26 [76.5%]
 White 10 [26.3%] 3 [8.8%]
 Other/multiracial 3 [7.9%] 5 [14.7%]
Current annual family income, M (SD) $27.39k ($19.39k) $33.86k ($24.02k) .21
 Range $2.30k - $99.90k $5.20k - $99.90k
Marital status, n [%] .39
 Not married 34 [89.5%] 28 [82.4%]
 Married 4 [10.5%] 6 [17.8%]
Current work status, n [%] .52
 Working full time 16 [42.1%] 17 [50.0%]
 Working part time 6 [15.8%] 8 [23.5%]
 Not working 16 [42.1%] 9 [26.5%]
Education, n [%] .70
 Some high school 9 [23.7%] 4 [11.8%]
 High school diploma or GED 13 [34.2%] 15 [44.1%]
 Tech degree, associate’s degree, or some college 12 [31.6%] 14 [41.2%]
 Bachelor’s or master’s degree 4 [10.5%] 1 [2.9%]
Number of CM subtypes (1–4), M (SD) 1.97 (0.91) 0
Adult adaptive functioning, M (SD) 6.53 (3.06) 7.58 (2.80) .13
ASR Internalizing Symptoms, M (SD) 51.66 (10.73) 51.68 (12.64) .99
ASR Externalizing Symptoms, M (SD) 52.023 (10.87) 51.29 (10.94) .78

Note: Statistical effects were tested by ANOVA or two-tailed t-test as appropriate for the number of levels. ASR = Achenbach Adult Self-Report

Data from an additional 29 individuals were collected but excluded from the final analysis due to: serious mental illness identified by history of hospitalization (2 CM), structural brain anomalies (4 CM, 5 comparison), excessive head motion (7 CM, 5 comparison), or low response rate on the task (4 CM, 2 comparison). Imaging data were not collected on an additional 13 individuals who returned to the lab for the study but either had metal in the body (1 comparison), claustrophobia (3 CM), or were too large to scan (6 CM, 3 comparison). An additional 57 longitudinal participants were contacted and screened but either declined to participate at the adult time point, or were unable to participate due to incarceration, death, MRI contraindications, or job or personal scheduling conflicts.

Measure of Adult Adaptive Functioning

Adult adaptive functioning, or competence, was assessed by examining each participant’s competence or success on several stage-salient developmental tasks. A developmental task has been defined as a task typical to a certain period of life for which successful achievement leads to approval by society as well as competence or future successes (Havighurst, 1956; Schulenberg, Bryant, & O’Malley, 2004). We used a composite (range 0–14) of rank scores based on participants’ progress in seven domains of development: education, work, financial autonomy, romantic involvement, peer involvement, family involvement, and substance abuse. All participants completed the Adult Self-Report (ASR; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003) and a demographics questionnaire. Information from each domain was drawn from these measures. Adaptive functioning was defined in relation to others from similar economic and social backgrounds, given that participants were ranked on each developmental task relative to other study participants. For each domain, rankings were based on cutoffs that approximately divided the participants into thirds (lowest, middle, and highest; see details in the Appendix Material). This approach was based on work by Schulenberg, Bryant, and O’Malley (2004) and has been previously published by our group (Demers et al., 2018; Demers et al., 2019). Further details are included in the Appendix. While ranking based on comparison to others within the sample limits external validity, it increases our ability to observe individual variation within our study population.

Behavioral fMRI Imaging Paradigm

Participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during performance of an IAPS Go/No-Go task (Cohen-Gilbert & Thomas, 2013). The IAPS Go/No-Go measures the ability to inhibit a dominant response in the context of visual distractors. In this task, letters were presented sequentially in a small box at the center of the screen while negative, positive, neutral or scrambled images were displayed in the background. Participants were instructed to ignore the background images and respond as quickly as possible with a button press to the presentation of every letter (Go stimuli), except the letter X (No-Go stimulus). The letters included H, P, R, S, T, and X. Go stimuli made up 73% of all trials such that participants acquired a prepotent tendency to press and needed to actively inhibit responses during No-Go trials.

Images were selected from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang et al., 2008), a collection of photographs selected to span a wide range of content and emotional valence. One hundred and eighty images were selected for use in this task. One third of the images had negative valence ratings (valence M = 3.17, SD = 0.58; arousal M = 5.53, SD = 0.83), one third had positive valence ratings (valence M = 7.36, SD = 0.39; arousal M = 5.05, SD = 0.82), one third ratings as close to neutral (5) as possible (valence M = 5.25, SD = 0.50; arousal M = 3.34, SD = 0.83). To create an additional emotionally neutral control condition that did not include object information, 120 of the selected IAPS images were also scrambled using a 32 × 32 grid.

The task was presented using E-Prime software (Psychological Software Tools Inc., Sharpsburg, PA) while participants were in the MRI scanner. The task included two runs, with trials blocked by stimulus valence within each run. Each run began and ended with a block of 15 scrambled image Go trials followed by a block of rest (fixation cross). Each run also included eight Go/No-Go blocks (two of each background type: negative, positive, neutral, or scrambled), presented in pseudorandom order. Each Go/No-Go block contained 15 trials, each with a unique IAPS image. Background IAPS images covered the entire screen and appeared for 200ms before a small white box containing a black letter appeared in the center of the image for 500ms. This design, with images presented alone prior to presentation of the letter stimulus, was used to make it more difficult for participants to ignore picture content. An inter-stimulus interval of 540ms followed each trial. Participants’ responses (press or no press) and reaction times were recorded using a hand-held button box. Behavioral performance was measured by accuracy on No-Go trials across the emotional background types, and reaction time on accurate Go trials by background type.

fMRI Data Acquisition

Individuals were scanned on a Siemens 3T TIM Trio whole-body scanner using a 32-channel head coil. High-resolution, T1-weighted images were acquired for each participant using an MPRAGE sequence (echo time [TE] = 3.4 ms, repetition time [TR] = 2530 ms, field of view = 256 mm, matrix = 256×256, slice thickness = 1 mm, flip angle = 7°, 192 sagittal slices) for co-registration of functional images. Functional data were acquired using an echo-planar imaging sequence (TE = 30 ms, TR = 2500 ms, field of view = 224 mm, matrix = 64×64, slice thickness = 3.5 mm with a 29% gap, flip angle = 90°, 36 interleaved oblique axial slices). To correct geometric distortion in the functional data, a fieldmap volume was collected immediately prior to the functional data acquisition using the same slice prescription (TE1 = 5.19 ms, TE2 = 7.65 ms, TR = 400 ms, field of view = 224 mm, matrix = 64×64, slice thickness = 3.5 mm with a 29% gap, flip angle = 60°, 36 interleaved oblique axial slices).

fMRI Data Analysis

Preprocessing

Functional imaging data were analyzed using FSL6.0.1 software. Head motion in the scanner was assessed and data points were censored based on the following parameters: 1) absolute motion exceeding one voxel of overall displacement from the first volume in the series and 2) relative motion exceeding one half voxel from one volume to the next. Volumes immediately preceding and following those that meet the relative motion criterion were also excluded. Motion displacement was quantified using the root mean square across the six head motion parameters. Participants with above threshold motion in more than 25% of data points (TRs) were excluded from further analyses, such that the final sample of 72 participants each had at least 171 TRs, or 7 minutes 8 seconds, of usable task data. Additionally, usable structural scans were required for inclusion in the analysis. In addition to fMRI data quality, participants with less than 60% accuracy on Go trials with scrambled background images were deemed to demonstrate poor understanding of the task demands and were excluded from further analyses. fMRI preprocessing steps included motion correction with MCFLIRT, skull stripping using the Brain Extraction Tool, slice time correction, geometric unwarping based on a fieldmap volume, spatial smoothing using a 6-mm full width at half maximum Gaussian kernel, and high-pass temporal filtering with a filter cutoff of 100 sec based on the block design task. Each participant’s functional images were registered to the corresponding high-resolution anatomical image (using 6 df), which were in turn registered to the Montreal Neurological Institute standard space (152 individual T1 2-mm template) using 12 df.

Task Analysis

Single-subject data were entered into a general linear model using gamma-convolved predictors for the four image-background conditions (negative valence, positive valence, neutral valence, scrambled images), with rest blocks serving as the unmarked baseline. Additional predictors of noninterest included a predictor for an unused buffer period (short fixation period at the beginning of the task), a predictor for the all-Go trial blocks, six predictors for head motion (three rotation and three linear translation), and a censoring (motion displacement) predictor for each motion-affected TR. The activation contrast of interest was response to negative > response to neutral.

Group Analysis

Whole-brain mixed effects regression analyses were conducted to assess activation differences between the maltreated and comparison groups, with age and gender as centered nuisance variables. We used FSL/FEAT (FLAME 1) to correct for multiple testing across voxels with a voxel-level threshold of p < .005 and a cluster threshold (calculated using Gaussian random field theory maximum height thresholding) of p < .05.

Statistical Analyses

We first conducted independent t-tests or χ2 tests on demographic variables to confirm group equivalence, and on psychopathology variables to explore group differences in symptoms. To parallel the imaging contrast of negative vs. neutral valence background conditions, we used 2×2 mixed-model analyses of variance (ANOVA) to test effects of group (CM vs. comparison) and emotional background type (negative vs. neutral) on task performance, with age and gender as covariates. Analyses were run on the main inhibitory measures, No-Go accuracy and Go trial reaction time, and also on Go trial accuracy. Follow-up paired-sample t-tests were used to examine significant effects. To examine the relationship between adult adaptive functioning and task performance, another set of ANOVAs was run with this index as a covariate of interest. We used SPSS/ PASW 25.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL) to conduct behavioral analyses.

Results

Group demographics and psychopathology

There were no group differences in age, gender race, ethnicity, other demographic variables, or psychopathology (see Table 1).

IAPS Go/No-Go task performance

A mixed-model ANOVA was conducted to compare the effects of background image valence (neutral or negative), a within-subjects factor, and group (comparison or CM), a between-subjects factor, on No-Go accuracy. There was a statistically significant interaction between image valence and group for No-Go accuracy, F(1, 68) = 5.8787, p = .02, η2 = .081. Simple main effects analyses showed that the comparison group was significantly less accurate on trials with negative compared to neutral background images (p = .01, d = .45), but there were no accuracy differences by background image valence for the CM group (p = .30, d = .17). The CM group was more accurate than the comparison group on No-Go trials with negative backgrounds [t(70) = −1.98, p = .050, d = .47; see Figure 1, Table 2], but there was no group difference for No-Go trials with neutral background [t(70) = 0.12, p = .90].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Accuracy on No-Go trials was impaired by negative background images relative to neutral backgrounds in the comparison group, but not the maltreated group. Accuracy on No-Go trials with negative background images was lower in the comparison group than the maltreated group. Error bars show 95% confidence intervals.

Table 2.

IAPS Go/No-Go task performance

CM (n = 38) Comp (n = 34)
Negative Background Neutral Background Negative Background Neutral Background
Go Acc. .95 (.07) .96 (.07) .91 (.12) .91 (.12)
No-Go Acc. .80 (.15) .77 (.15) .72 (.20) .78 (.19)
Go RT 442 (38) 426 (42) 424 (43) 408 (40)

CM = Childhood maltreatment; Comp = comparison group; Acc. = accuracy; RT = reaction time (in milliseconds). Values are given as mean (standard deviation).

A second mixed-model ANOVA was conducted to compare the effects of background image valence and group (comparison or CM) on Go trial reaction time. There were no significant interactions or main effects. When Go trial accuracy was inspected, there were no significant effects of image valence, group, and their interaction [F(68, 1) = 0.07, p = .79], F(68, 1) = 3.37, p = .07, F(68, 1) = 0.46, p = .50, respectively].

Finally, analyses were conducted to evaluate the association between adult adaptive functioning and task performance above and beyond the influence of group. There was a significant main effect of adult adaptive functioning on No-Go trial accuracy, F(1,67) = 3.9595, p = .050, d = .06, but not Go trial reaction time (p = .19). Greater adaptive functioning was associated with higher No-Go accuracy. There were no significant interactions between CM group and adult adaptive functioning for either accuracy or reaction time (p’s > .68).).

As a check of our assumptions, we confirmed that when background images were scrambled, reaction time and accuracy did not differ between groups (No-Go trial accuracy, t(70) = −.33, p = .74; Go trial reaction time, t(70) = −1.51, p = .14).

Task-Related Brain Activity

An analysis of task-related brain activation in the whole sample (all subjects, negative > neutral contrast) revealed significant activation in multiple brain regions, including visual cortex, PFC, cingulate gyrus, amygdala, and hippocampus. These regions are consistent with those reported in previous studies using Go/No-Go paradigms (e.g., Chester, Lynam, Milich, Powell, Andersen, & DeWall, 2016; Cohen-Gilbert, Nickerson, Sneider, Oot, Seraikas, Rohan, & Silveri, 2017) and emotional tasks (Frank, Dewitt, Hudgens-Haney, Schaeffer, Ball, Schwarz, …, & Sabatinelli, 2014).

Group Differences in Task-Related Brain Activity

Comparison of task-related activity by group revealed that the comparison group recruited prefrontal regions (left frontal pole, right inferior frontal gyrus, right frontal pole; Table 3, Figure 2) more than the CM group when performing the inhibitory control task in the context of negative background images. Follow-up simple effects analyses using percent signal change values extracted from the three significant ROIs showed that the comparison group showed a 0.29%, 0.38%, and 0.15% mean difference signal change for the three ROIs respectively, whereas the CM group showed a 0.05%, 0.09% and .05% mean difference signal change for the three ROIs respectively.

Table 3.

Group differences in task-related brain activity for negative > neutral background image contrast

Region Brodmann Volume, mm3 MNI Coordinates Z value
Area x y z Max Mean SD
Left frontal pole 10 510 −24 38 16 3.81 2.92 0.25
Right inferior frontal gyrus 45 329 50 24 4 3.69 2.90 0.25
Right frontal pole 9 508 8 60 36 4.01 2.98 0.31

Figure 2.

Figure 2

The comparison group showed greater activity than the maltreated group for Negative > Neutral images in three PFC regions including (A) left frontal pole, (B) right inferior frontal gyrus, (C) right frontal pole (p < .05 cluster corrected).

Relationship between Current Adaptive Functioning and Task-Related Activation

We evaluated whether differences in task activation were related to individual differences in current adaptive functioning across the full sample. Correlations were run between percent signal change values extracted from the three significant ROIs and adult adaptive functioning scores, with age and sex as covariates. Adult adaptive functioning was significantly related to task-related activity of the right frontal pole r(68) = .41, p < .001, but not related to task-related activity of the left frontal pole, r(68) = .22, p = .07, nor task-related activity of the right inferior frontal gyrus, r(68) = .01, p = .98. Individuals with greater adult adaptive functioning showed more activity in frontal pole regions when the background images were negative compared to neutral. The relationship between adaptive functioning and right frontal pole activation survived Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Discussion

This study examined the interface between inhibitory control and emotion and associated neural underpinnings in the context of CM history. Contrary to our predictions, negative emotional images adversely affected inhibitory control in the comparison group, but not the maltreated group. Additionally, relative to the maltreated group, the comparison group showed greater prefrontal activation during conditions when inhibitory control was required and when background images were negative compared to neutral. Better adaptive functioning in everyday contexts was related to this greater prefrontal activation and superior inhibitory control in the context of negative distractors.

In the present study, we did not observe the expected performance deficit in inhibitory control in our CM group. This could be due to the fact that the comparison group was well matched on other dimensions of early risk apart from CM which likely acted as confounds in prior studies. Therefore, our results suggest that CM history in healthy adults does not predict general impulsivity on a laboratory task when compared to peers from similar contexts. This finding is consistent with Liu’s (2019) recent meta-analysis which also did not show support for an association with behavioral task performance. Liu (2019) did find an association between CM history and self- and parent-reported impulsivity, although only a small number of behavioral studies were included.

The effects of emotional distraction on behavioral performance differed by group. Accuracy was reduced in the context of negative images only for the comparison group and not the maltreated group. It is possible that adults with a history of maltreatment have learned based on their early experiences to maintain inhibitory control even in negative contexts. A group comparison of accuracy and reaction time for scrambled image backgrounds, which inherently do not require as much attention regulation, allowed us to rule out differences in inhibitory control in non-emotional contexts. Given the lack of more general group differences, it seems that the CM group is showing enhanced attention regulation in the face of negative images, rather than simply allocating more attentional resources to inhibitory control overall. The decreased emotion-related impulsivity seen in the maltreated group relative to the comparison group aligns with prior work reporting that young adults with child abuse histories demonstrated less impulsivity on laboratory-based measures than those without abuse histories (Sujan, Humphreys, Ray, & Lee, 2014). This adaptive strategy may reflect a form of long-term resilience despite early adverse experience.

It is also possible that the negative images used in this study were differentially arousing to the two groups, such that they were not arousing enough to elicit a strong interference response from adults in the maltreated group. It is possible that individuals with a history of CM have been relatively desensitized to negative images through consistent exposure to negative emotional contexts early in life. Previous research has suggested that arousal improves behavioral response inhibition in nonclinical populations (Shields, Sazma, & Yonelinas, 2016). This effect, however, may be moderated by emotion-related impulsivity. In a recent study by Pearlstein, Johnson, Modavi, Peckham, and Carver (2019), individuals who rated themselves as having lower emotion-related impulsivity showed improving response inhibition on a trial-by-trial basis following increased arousal (indexed by pupil dilation), whereas for those rating themselves higher in emotion-related impulsivity, response inhibition declined following higher arousal. However, we did not measure either physiological or self-reported arousal in this study and therefore cannot address this possibility.

The accuracy detriment associated with negative backgrounds in the comparison group was unexpected although in a logical direction. Previous research in normative samples has shown a negligible effect of emotional distraction on task accuracy, whereas effects on reaction time are more typical (e.g., Chester et al., 2016; Cohen-Gilbert & Thomas, 2013; Cohen-Gilbert et al., 2017). However, in prior work with this task, younger adolescents (ages 13–14 years) showed poorer No-Go accuracy when the background image was negative (Cohen-Gilbert & Thomas, 2013). Given that the adults in the current sample have all experienced high levels of stress vis-à-vis poverty, results may suggest that early adversity may lead to immature inhibitory control in the face of negative emotional content. However, without a low-risk control group in the present sample, this possibility cannot be tested. The comparison group’s impaired behavioral performance on the task in the context of task-irrelevant negative images was accompanied by increased recruitment of frontal brain regions, including regions associated with inhibitory control (e.g., right IFG; Aron et al., 2004; Chester et al., 2016; Chikazoe et al., 2007), relative to the CM group. Although this particular executive function domain (i.e., inhibitory control) has not been assessed in previous imaging studies of maltreatment, Schweizer and colleagues found similar evidence of enhanced cognition in the context of strong emotion following early adversity (Schweizer, Walsh, Stretton, Dunn, Goodyet, & Dalgleish, 2016). Their work suggests that the neural underpinnings of emotion regulation in healthy adolescents and young adults exposed to moderate childhood adversities may be operating more efficiently (Schweizer et al., 2016) than in those with low adversity. Specifically, the group with higher childhood adversities showed enhanced emotion regulation over positive and negative affect during a film-based task, which was associated with reduced recruitment of frontal regulatory brain regions and amygdala activation. These findings align with our results of enhanced behavioral performance and reduced recruitment of brain regions associated with inhibitory control in the maltreated relative to comparison group. The sample and task paradigm used in the present study and in Schweizer’s differed substantially (i.e., Schweizer’s sample had moderate childhood adversity but not maltreatment, and the paradigm involved explicit instructions to regulate one’s emotional reaction to film clips). Still, the similarity in results may indicate that generally healthy adults who experienced extreme stress in childhood may develop an enhanced ability to regulate their attention and emotional reactions in the context of task-relevant or - irrelevant emotional stimuli, at least within the controlled laboratory environment.

Others have also found CM history to be associated with differential activation in multiple prefrontal regions during emotionally-charged executive function tasks. For instance, one recent study found CM was associated with reduced activation in the left dorsolateral PFC and right dACC, and increased activation in the IFG (Mackiewicz et al., 2017), while another study found increased activation in dorsolateral PFC, dACC, and ventromedial and ventromedial PFC, and no differences in the right IFG (Herzog et al., 2017). However, in both of these studies, the maltreated groups had significantly higher levels of depression and/or PTSD symptomatology than the comparison groups. Unlike these two samples, the present sample may represent a resilient subset of maltreated individuals, as internalizing and externalizing symptoms were in the broad average range for 90% of the current sample. In this way, our sample is more similar to Schweizer’s sample of healthy individuals exposed to moderate childhood adversities. Together, these studies may indicate that, in healthy adults with histories of early adversity and/or abuse, regulatory control is not disrupted by negative contexts, despite less recruitment of neural systems typically implicated in inhibitory control.

Better performance and more efficient neural recruitment of inhibitory control systems when engaged in a laboratory task does not necessarily extend to real-life application of these skills. Our results showed that across the entire sample, greater activation of the frontal pole related to higher levels of current adaptive functioning. Previous research indicates that activation in this brain region is involved in monitoring action outcomes (Koechlin, 2011). The ability to monitor one’s behavior and inhibit impulses within aversive contexts is likely critical to success in everyday situations reflected in the adaptive functioning measure. For instance, resisting the urge to use alcohol or other substances when depressed and refraining from hostile behavior when angered, can serve an individual in social relationships as well as educational and occupational progress. Our prior work in this sample has demonstrated that adult adaptive functioning also relates to frontolimbic functional connectivity, an index of efficient emotion regulation (Demers et al., 2018). It is possible that the maltreated group is able to regulate their responding with less recruitment of typical neural systems despite negative distractions in a laboratory-based task, but that adaptive functioning within the real world requires greater activation of neural regions involved in inhibitory control for all participants.

This study makes several important contributions to the existing maltreatment and resilience literatures. First, unlike the previous studies on inhibitory control during emotional contexts in maltreated individuals, this sample includes both a larger total sample and a comparison group that is well-matched longitudinally on socioeconomic characteristics. The two groups also had similar levels of current internalizing and externalizing symptoms and current adaptive functioning. Therefore, we were better able to isolate effects specific to maltreatment without confounding group differences in poverty, general risk, or adult level of functioning. Unlike most studies of maltreatment that only evaluate risk factors, we also investigated promotive factors by examining whether group differences in neural activity related to individual differences in adaptive functioning. Similarly, the sample was assessed prospectively from childhood into adulthood, reducing typical measurement errors inherent in retrospective report. Finally, this study is unique in its focus on brain systems involved in inhibitory control during emotional distraction in the context of maltreatment history. The majority of prior work has used facial viewing tasks that do not tap into executive functioning skills in emotional contexts or have used emotional Stroop tasks that don’t include highly arousing stimuli. In contrast, we examined inhibitory control during emotional contexts using a paradigm with task-irrelevant emotional stimuli, thereby allowing us to isolate the impact of negative emotional distraction on executive function. Further, by using this paradigm in the scanner, we were able to evaluate the impact of negative emotional distraction on brain activation during inhibitory control.

Despite important contributions to the extant literature, this study had several limitations. First, the sample size per group limits our ability to confidently draw conclusions due to low statistical power and necessitates replication. Unfortunately, few prospective neuroimaging studies of maltreatment have large enough sample sizes to adequately power complex statistical analyses capable of teasing apart the influences of multiple risk factors for poor adaptive functioning. Although participants were followed longitudinally since childhood, MRI assessment was conducted only at the most recent time point in adulthood. Therefore, we cannot determine the directionality of the relationships between maltreatment, emotion-related inhibitory control, adaptive functioning, and brain circuitry. Also, without a low-risk control sample, we could not determine whether the observed disruptions in inhibitory control by negative stimuli are present only in individuals who have experienced high-risk early environments, including poverty, or would also be evident in low-risk adult populations. Additional work with a low-risk comparison group is needed to determine if relationships between current functioning and neural measures of inhibitory control are specific to adaptation after early adversity or if they can be generalized to adaptive functioning and competence, regardless of early environment. It is also possible that the composition of participants included in our comparison and maltreated groups is not representative of individuals from high-risk, low-income backgrounds. It is probable that current life stressors and factors related to adult adaptive functioning influenced which participants were able to continue in the study. For instance, some individuals had employment conflicts prohibiting research participation, while other individuals from the original sample (maltreated or not) were excluded based on incarceration, bullets or other metal fragments in the body, obesity significant enough to preclude MRI scanning, severe mental illness (e.g., schizophrenia) or cognitive impairment, or an inability to locate the individual for recruitment (e.g., homelessness or high mobility) – conditions that are often exacerbated by a history of chronic poverty, racial discrimination, and various forms of trauma. Consequently, our sample represents a restricted range of developmental outcomes seen in high-risk, low socioeconomic status populations. Replication with a larger sample is warranted. In addition, while adult adaptive functioning may indicate the capacity for resilience, further study of resilient processes throughout development is needed.

Finally, given the inherent constraints of fMRI, we chose to use a block design rather than an event-related design for the IAPS Go/No-Go paradigm. Therefore, we were unable to compare neural activity on Go and No-Go trials or accurate and inaccurate trials. Also due to the block design nature of the task, comparison of neural signal between negative and neutral blocks likely represents contextual monitoring more broadly than just inhibitory control. We also did not have any autonomic measures of arousal, so we were unable to determine if participants had different physiological reactions to the images in the task. As noted above, it is possible that performance by the maltreated group was less disrupted by task-irrelevant negative images because they found the images less arousing, perhaps owing to a systematic desensitization based on past life experiences. Future research should consider level of arousal, as well as level of emotion-related impulsivity (Pearlstein et al., 2019) when evaluating the effect of emotional distraction on inhibitory control.

In conclusion, results suggest that psychiatrically healthy adults who endured childhood maltreatment may have an enhanced ability to regulate their attention and limit impulsive reactions in the context of task-irrelevant emotional stimuli within a laboratory task. This enhancement may reflect learning from earlier experiences such that the negative content in a laboratory setting may be less arousing and therefore less distracting for adults who have experienced CM. Additionally, while early maltreatment experience may train enhanced inhibitory control in the face of negative or threatening stimuli, it is not necessarily synonymous with positive real-world outcomes. Adult adaptive functioning within real world contexts was associated with greater activation of neural regions involved in inhibitory control, suggesting that multiple risk and protective mechanisms may be at work in this sample.

Acknowledgements:

This work was supported by a McKnight Presidential Chair, William Harris Endowed Chair, and a Klaus J. Jacobs Research Prize (to DC), imaging support from the Rochester Center for Brain Imaging, and a pilot grant from the College of Arts, Science and Engineering, University of Rochester. Trainee support was provided by the University of Minnesota’s Institute of Child Development via a National Institute of Mental Health National Research Service Award Grant no. 2T32MH015755-39 (to LAD). We thank the staff at the Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester, and the Institute of Child Development at the University of Minnesota. We thank the Minnesota Supercomputing Institute at the University of Minnesota for providing resources that contributed to the research results reported within this paper. We thank Emily Hunt and Pat Weber for work in data collection, and the families and longitudinal participants of the Mt. Hope Family Center for their participation.

Appendix

Description of Adult Adaptive Functioning

The adult adaptive functioning scores ranged from 0 through 14 and were generated through a composite of rank scores (0 through 2) on seven domains of functioning. Participants were ranked in one of three categories for each domain based on their success on the developmental task relative to other participants in the study. This approach was taken to emulate work by Schulenberg et al (2004). Information from each domain was drawn from the Adult Self Report measure (ASR; Achenbach, 2003) and a demographics questionnaire. Thereby, this score includes both objectively verifiable components (e.g., education attainment, annual income) and subjective components (e.g., friendship quality, family involvement).

For the education domain, 25 individuals did not finish high school (however 12 of those received their GED) and were categorized as lower, 16 graduated from high school and were categorized as middle, and 31 pursued further education (18 earned a vocational technical diploma or completed part of a collegiate program, 8 earned an associate’s degree, 3 earned a bachelor’s degree, and 2 earned a master’s degree) and were categorized as upper.

Success in work was based on occupational standing according to the Hauser and Warren Socioeconomic Index (SEI) score that considers earnings, education, and prestige associated with occupations (1997), and the job satisfaction and confidence scores on the ASR. Scores for the participants’ current work and usual work were averaged to create one score. Eighteen were categorized as lower in this domain, including individuals who were currently unemployed or disabled. Individuals who reported that they were keeping house or in school, or held a job of mediocre occupational standing (e.g., maid, janitor, construction laborer, kitchen worker), or an adaptive functioning job score of < 1.5 (low job satisfaction and confidence) were considered middle rank in this domain. This group contained 36 individuals. Finally, 18 participants who had a relatively high SEI score (e.g., health aide, teacher or teacher’s aide, general office clerk, sales worker) and an average ASR job score greater than 1.5 (medium-high job satisfaction and confidence) were considered in the upper rank.

Financial autonomy was based on total family income rank within this sample. The range of family income levels were divided into approximate thirds. Twenty-two individuals were in the lower rank category, which included those earning less than $20k/year. Thirty-one individuals’ family income was between $20–40k and were in the middle rank category. Lastly, 19 individuals were in the upper rank category with family earnings of $40–120k. Based on the 2013 Federal Poverty Guidelines, the poverty line is defined as household income of less than $23.5k/year for a family of four (US Department of Health and Human Services, 2013).

Ranking of success in the romantic involvement domain differed from rankings by Schulenberg and colleagues (2004) to reflect the average age of marriage in New York State (28 years of age, as opposed to 26 years, which was used in Schulenberg’s ranking). Unmarried and non-cohabiting individuals who were 28 years old or younger were classified as in the middle category. Otherwise, rankings were based on marital status, divorce history, and relationship ratings given on the ASR. To be classified as lower, individuals had to have been divorced more than twice, single and not cohabiting, or in a low-quality marriage (ASR adaptive functioning Spouse/Partner score < 1). This group contained 21 individuals. The middle rank group, which contained 33 individuals, included divorced but remarried participants, unmarried but cohabiting participants, and married but unsatisfied participants (ASR adaptive functioning spouse/partner score = 1–1.5). Eighteen individuals were classified as high rank in the romantic involvement domain, which included individuals who had never been divorced and were currently in a high-quality marriage (ASR average Spouse/Partner satisfaction-related score > 1.5).

For the peer involvement domain, ranking was based on the ASR adaptive functioning friends scale. This scale encompasses quantity of friendships, contact with friends and quality of friendships. Twenty-four participants were low (ASR score < 1.75), 20 participants were middle (score = 1.75 – 2.25), and 28 participants were high (score > 2.25) in this domain.

Family involvement rankings were also based on the ASR report, using the adaptive functioning family scale, which indexes how well one gets along with family members. These scores were averaged across family members that participants reported having contact with (including parents, siblings, and children), as it may actually be adaptive to not have contact with some family members, particularly if maltreatment was perpetrated by a family member. Twenty-seven participants were categorized as low (score < 1.25), while 18 were middle (score = 1.25 – 1.75) and 27 were high (score > 1.75).

The last developmental task domain indexed in this sample was related to substance abuse. Rankings were based on ASR Substance Use Scales for tobacco, alcohol, and drugs. Scores on these three subscales (ranging from 50 to 100) were averaged. The sample was nearly evenly divided into thirds, with 29 individuals ranked as low (score > 66.67), 31 ranked as middle (score = 50 – 66.67), and 28 ranked as high (score = 50).

Behavioral Effects of Sensitivity

Behavioral performance was also measured by the sensitivity index, d’ (d prime) by background type. d’ is the standardized difference between the means of the signal present (i.e., Go trials) and signal absent (i.e., NoGo trials) distributions. Larger absolute values of d’ indicate that a person is more sensitive to the difference between the Go and NoGo trials, whereas d’ values near zero indicate chance performance. d’ is calculated using an individual’s hit rate (H) and false alarm rate (FA) with the following formula: d’ = z (FA) – z (H). To account for situations when participants detect every signal (H = 1.00) and/or make no false alarms (FA = 0.00), we used the loglinear approach (Hautus, 1995) in which we added 0.5 to both the number of hits and the number of false alarms and added 1 to both the number of signal trials and the number of noise trials.

A mixed-model ANOVA was conducted to compare the effect of background image valence (neutral, negative, or positive) and group (comparison or maltreated) on sensitivity to the difference between the go and nogo trials (i.e., d’). The relationships between condition and group were nonsignificant: effect of condition, F(2, 69) = 2.77, p = .07, effect of group, F(1, 70) = 2.31, p = .13, interaction between condition and group p = .24). Simple main effects analyses showed that the participants were significantly less sensitive, or less able to correctly respond on Go trials and inhibit responding on No-Go trials, on trials with negative compared to positive background images (p = .02).

In addition, a mixed-model ANOVA was conducted to evaluate the effects of adult adaptive functioning, modeled as a continuous variable, on behavioral task sensitivity, with maltreatment history as a between-group variable. There was an insignificant effect of adult adaptive functioning on d’, F(1,69) = 1.89, p = .17.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003): Manual for ASEBA adult forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families.

Hauser, R. M., & Warren, J. R. (1997). Socioeconomic indexes for occupations: A review, update, and critique. Sociological methodology, 27(1), 177–298.

Schulenberg, J. E,, Bryant, A. L., & O’Malley, P. M. (2004): Taking hold of some kind of life: How developmental tasks relate to trajectories of well-being during the transition to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology. 16:1119–1140.

US Department of Health and Human Services (2013): Poverty guidelines. Federal Register 78: 5182–5183.

Footnotes

Disclosures:

The authors report no financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Achenbach TM, & Rescorla LA (2003). Manual for the ASEBA adult forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont. Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersen SL, Tomada A, Vincow ES, Valente E, Polcari A, & Teicher MH (2008). Preliminary evidence for sensitive periods in the effect of childhood sexual abuse on regional brain development. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 20(3), 292–301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arnsten AF, & Rubia K (2012). Neurobiological circuits regulating attention, cognitive control, motivation, and emotion: disruptions in neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(4), 356–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aron AR, Robbins TW, & Poldrack RA (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci 8 (4), 170–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bari A, & Robbins TW (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in neurobiology, 108, 44–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barnett D, Manly JT, Cicchetti D, & Toth SL (1993). Defining child maltreatment: The interface between policy and research. In Cicchetti D & Toth SL (Eds.), Child abuse, child development, and social policy (pp. 7–74). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. [Google Scholar]
  7. Beck AT, Steer RA, & Brown GK (1996). Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, 78(2), 490–498. [Google Scholar]
  8. Belsky J, Schlomer GL, & Ellis BJ (2012). Beyond cumulative risk: distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy. Developmental psychology, 48(3), 662–673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bremner JD, Vermetten E, Vythilingam M, Afzal N, Schmahl C, Elzinga B, & Charney DS (2004). Neural correlates of the classic color and emotional stroop in women with abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological psychiatry, 55(6), 612–620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Caldwell JG, Krug MK, Carter CS, & Minzenberg MJ (2014). Cognitive control in the face of fear: Reduced cognitive-emotional flexibility in women with a history of child abuse. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 23(5), 454–472. [Google Scholar]
  11. Chester DS, Lynam DR, Milich R, Powell DK, Andersen AH, & DeWall CN (2016). How do negative emotions impair self-control? A neural model of negative urgency. NeuroImage, 132, 43–50. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chikazoe J, Konishi S, Asari T, Jimura K, & Miyashita Y (2007). Activation of right inferior frontal gyrus during response inhibition across response modalities. J. Cogn. Neurosci 19 (1), 69–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cicchetti D, & Lynch M (1993). Toward an ecological/transactional model of community violence and child maltreatment: Consequences for children’s development. Psychiatry, 56(1), 96–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cicchetti D, & Lynch M (1995). Failures in the expectable environment and their impact on individual development: The case of child maltreatment. In Cicchetti D & Cohen DJ (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk, disorder, and adaptation (Vol. 2, pp. 32–71). New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Lynch M, & Holt KD (1993). Resilience in maltreated children: Processes leading to adaptive outcome. Development and psychopathology, 5(4), 629–647. [Google Scholar]
  16. Cicchetti D, & Toth SL (2005). Child maltreatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 409–438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cicchetti D, Toth SL, & Manly JT (2003). Maternal maltreatment classification interview. Unpublished manuscript, Mt. Hope Family Center, Rochester, NY [Google Scholar]
  18. Cohen-Gilbert JE, Nickerson LD, Sneider JT, Oot EN, Seraikas AM, Rohan ML, & Silveri MM (2017). College binge drinking associated with decreased frontal activation to negative emotional distractors during inhibitory control. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 1650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cohen-Gilbert JE, & Thomas KM (2013). Inhibitory control during emotional distraction across adolescence and early adulthood. Child development, 84(6), 1954–1966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cowell RA, Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA & Toth SL (2015). Childhood maltreatment and its effect on neurocognitive functioning: Chronicity and timing matter. Development and Psychopathology. 27, 521–33. doi: 10.1017/s0954579415000139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Daly BP, Hildenbrand AK, Turner E, Berkowitz S, & Tarazi RA (2017). Executive Functioning Among College Students With and Without History of Childhood Maltreatment. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 26(7), 717–735. [Google Scholar]
  22. Dannlowski U, Kugel H, Huber F, Stuhrmann A, Redlich R, Grotegerd D, … & Arolt V (2013). Childhood maltreatment is associated with an automatic negative emotion processing bias in the amygdala. Human brain mapping, 34(11), 2899–2909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. de Graaf-Peters VB, & Hadders-Algra M (2006). Ontogeny of the human central nervous system: what is happening when?. Early human development, 82(4), 257–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Demers LA, Handley ED, Hunt RH, Rogosch FA, Toth SL, Thomas KM, & Cicchetti D (2019). Childhood maltreatment disrupts brain-mediated pathways between adolescent maternal relationship quality and positive adult outcomes. Child maltreatment, 1077559519847770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Demers LA, McKenzie KJ, Hunt RH, Cicchetti D, Cowell RA, Rogosch FA, … & Thomas KM (2018). Separable effects of childhood maltreatment and adult adaptive functioning on amygdala connectivity during emotion processing. Biological psychiatry: cognitive neuroscience and neuroimaging, 3(2), 116–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. DePrince AP, Weinzierl KM, & Combs MD (2009). Executive function performance and trauma exposure in a community sample of children. Child abuse & neglect, 33(6), 353–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Diorio D, Viau V, & Meaney MJ (1993). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex (cingulate gyrus) in the regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Journal of Neuroscience, 13(9), 3839–3847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fani N, King TZ, Clendinen C, Hardy RA, Surapaneni S, Blair JR, … & Ressler KJ (2019). Attentional control abnormalities in posttraumatic stress disorder: Functional, behavioral, and structural correlates. Journal of affective disorders, 253, 343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fay-Stammbach T, & Hawes DJ (2019). Caregiver ratings and performance-based indices of executive function among preschoolers with and without maltreatment experience. Child neuropsychology, 25(6), 721–741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Frank DW, Dewitt M, Hudgens-Haney M, Schaeffer DJ, Ball BH, Schwarz NF, … & Sabatinelli D (2014). Emotion regulation: quantitative meta-analysis of functional activation and deactivation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 45, 202–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fonzo GA, Flagan TM, Sullivan S, Allard CB, Grimes EM, Simmons AN, … & Stein MB (2013). Neural functional and structural correlates of childhood maltreatment in women with intimate-partner violence-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 211(2), 93–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gilbert R, Widom CS, Browne K, Fergusson D, Webb E, & Janson S (2009). Burden and consequences of child maltreatment in high-income countries. The Lancet, 373(9657), 68–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Grant MM, Cannistraci C, Hollon SD, Gore J, & Shelton R (2011). Childhood trauma history differentiates amygdala response to sad faces within MDD. Journal of psychiatric research, 45(7), 886–895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hanson JL, Chung MK, Avants BB, Shirtcliff EA, Gee JC, Davidson RJ, & Pollak SD (2010). Early stress is associated with alterations in the orbitofrontal cortex: a tensor-based morphometry investigation of brain structure and behavioral risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(22), 7466–7472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hart H, & Rubia K (2012). Neuroimaging of child abuse: a critical review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(March), 1–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hautus MJ (1995). Corrections for extreme proportions and their biasing effects on estimated values ofd’. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 27(1), 46–51. [Google Scholar]
  37. Havighurst RJ (1956): Research on the developmental-task concept. School Review 64:215–223. [Google Scholar]
  38. Hein TC, & Monk CS (2017). Research Review: Neural response to threat in children, adolescents, and adults after child maltreatment–a quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 58(3), 222–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Herzog JI, Niedtfeld I, Rausch S, Thome J, Mueller-Engelmann M, Steil R, … & Schmahl C (2017). Increased recruitment of cognitive control in the presence of traumatic stimuli in complex PTSD. European archives of psychiatry and clinical neuroscience, 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jedd K, Hunt RH, Cicchetti D, Hunt E, Cowell RA, Rogosch FA, … & Thomas KM (2015). Long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment: Altered amygdala functional connectivity. Development and psychopathology, 27(4pt2), 1577–1589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kensinger EA, & Schacter DL (2006). Processing emotional pictures and words: Effects of valence and arousal. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(2), 110–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Koechlin E (2011). Frontal pole function: what is specifically human?. Trends in cognitive sciences, 15(6), 241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, & Cuthbert BN (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. University of Florida, Gainesville. Tech Rep A-8. [Google Scholar]
  44. Luby JL, Barch D, Whalen D, Tillman R, & Belden A (2017). Association between early life adversity and risk for poor emotional and physical health in adolescence: a putative mechanistic neurodevelopmental pathway. JAMA pediatrics, 171(12), 1168–1175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Liu RT (2019). Childhood maltreatment and impulsivity: a meta-analysis and recommendations for future study. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 47(2), 221–243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Mackiewicz Seghete KL, DePrince AP, & Banich MT (2018). Association between initial age of exposure to childhood abuse and cognitive control: preliminary evidence. Journal of traumatic stress. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Marusak HA, Martin KR, Etkin A, & Thomason ME (2015). Childhood trauma exposure disrupts the automatic regulation of emotional processing. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(5), 1250–1258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mackiewicz Seghete KL, Kaiser RH, DePrince AP, & Banich MT (2017). General and emotion-specific alterations to cognitive control in women with a history of childhood abuse. NeuroImage: Clinical, 16(June), 151–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Moreno-López L, Ioannidis K, Askelund AD, Alicia JS, Schueler K, & van Harmelen AL (2019). The resilient emotional brain: a scoping review of mPFC and limbic structure and function in resilient adults with a history of childhood maltreatment. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Muehlenkamp JJ, Kerr PL, Bradley AR, & Larsen MA (2010). Abuse subtypes and nonsuicidal self-injury: Preliminary evidence of complex emotion regulation patterns. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 198(4), 258–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Navalta CP, Polcari A, Webster DM, Boghossian A, & Teicher MH (2006). Effects of childhood sexual abuse on neuropsychological and cognitive function in college women. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 18(1), 45–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Nigg JT (2017). Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 58(4), 361–383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nooner KB, Mennes M, Brown S, Castellanos FX, Leventhal B, Milham MP, & Colcombe SJ (2013). Relationship of trauma symptoms to amygdala-based functional brain changes in adolescents. Journal of traumatic stress, 26(6), 784–787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ohashi K, Anderson CM, Bolger EA, Khan A, McGreenery CE, & Teicher MH (2019). Susceptibility or resilience to maltreatment can be explained by specific differences in brain network architecture. Biological psychiatry, 85(8), 690–702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pearlstein JG, Johnson SL, Modavi K, Peckham AD, & Carver CS (2019). Neurocognitive mechanisms of emotion-related impulsivity: The role of arousal. Psychophysiology, 56(2), e13293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Pechtel P, & Pizzagalli DA (2011). Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology, 214(1), 55–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, & Lane R (2003). Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biological psychiatry, 54(5), 504–514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Powers A, Etkin A, Gyurak A, Bradley B, & Jovanovic T (2015). Associations between childhood abuse, posttraumatic stress disorder, and implicit emotion regulation deficits: Evidence from a low-income, inner-city population. Psychiatry, 78(3), 251–264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rieder C, & Cicchetti D (1989). Organizational perspective on cognitive control functioning and cognitive-affective balance in maltreated children. Developmental Psychology, 25, 382–393. [Google Scholar]
  60. Rodman AM, Jenness JL, Weissman DG, Pine DS, & McLaughlin KA (2019). Neurobiological markers of resilience to depression following childhood maltreatment: The role of neural circuits supporting the cognitive control of emotion. Biological psychiatry, 86(6), 464–473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Schulenberg JE, Bryant AL, O’Malley PM (2004). Taking hold of some kind of life: How developmental tasks relate to trajectories of well-being during the transition to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 1119–1140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Schweizer S, Walsh ND, Stretton J, Dunn VJ, Goodyer IM, & Dalgleish T (2015). Enhanced emotion regulation capacity and its neural substrates in those exposed to moderate childhood adversity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(2), 272–281. 10.1093/scan/nsv109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Shields GS, Sazma MA, & Yonelinas AP (2016). The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 651–668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, & Toga AW (2003). Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nature neuroscience, 6(3), 309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sujan AC, Humphreys KL, Ray LA, & Lee SS (2014). Differential association of child abuse with self-reported versus laboratory-based impulsivity and risk-taking in young adulthood. Child maltreatment, 19(3–4), 145–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sun D, Haswell CC, Morey RA, & De Bellis MD (2019). Brain structural covariance network centrality in maltreated youth with PTSD and in maltreated youth resilient to PTSD. Development and psychopathology, 31(2), 557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Teicher MH, Samson JA, Anderson CM, & Ohashi K (2016). The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 17(10), 652–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. van der Bij J, den Kelder RO, Montagne B, & Hagenaars MA (2020). Inhibitory Control in Trauma-Exposed Youth: A Systematic Review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Weissman DG, Jenness JL, Colich NL, Miller AB, Sambrook KA, Sheridan MA, & McLaughlin KA (2019). Altered neural processing of threat-related information in children and adolescents exposed to violence: a transdiagnostic mechanism contributing to the emergence of psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.08.471 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES