Abstract
Considering the immense societal and personal costs and suffering associated with multiple drug use or “polytoxicomania”, better understanding of environmental and genetic causes is crucial. While previous studies focused on single risk factors and selected drugs, effects of early-accumulated environmental risks on polytoxicomania were never addressed. Similarly, evidence of genetic susceptibility to particular drugs is abundant, while genetic predisposition to polytoxicomania is unexplored. We exploited the GRAS data collection, comprising information on N~2000 deep-phenotyped schizophrenia patients, to investigate effects of early-life environmental risk accumulation on polytoxicomania and additionally provide first genetic insight. Preadult accumulation of environmental risks (physical or sexual abuse, urbanicity, migration, cannabis, alcohol) was strongly associated with lifetime polytoxicomania (p = 1.5 × 10−45; OR = 31.4), preadult polytoxicomania with OR = 226.6 (p = 1.0 × 10−33) and adult polytoxicomania with OR = 17.5 (p = 3.4 × 10−24). Parallel accessibility of genetic data from GRAS patients and N~2100 controls for genome-wide association (GWAS) and phenotype-based genetic association studies (PGAS) permitted the creation of a novel multiple GWAS–PGAS approach. This approach yielded 41 intuitively interesting SNPs, potentially conferring liability to preadult polytoxicomania, which await replication upon availability of suitable deep-phenotyped cohorts anywhere world-wide. Concisely, juvenile environmental risk accumulation, including cannabis and alcohol as starter/gateway drugs, strongly predicts polytoxicomania during adolescence and adulthood. This pivotal message should launch more effective sociopolitical measures to prevent this deleterious psychiatric condition.
Subject terms: Addiction, Schizophrenia, Genetics
Introduction
Substance use disorders and multiple drug consumption are frequent in the general population and recurrent comorbidities of schizophrenia and other neuropsychiatric disorders [1–3]. Multiple drug use, also called polytoxicomania (the preferred term throughout this manuscript), is defined as the use of ≥3 drugs in a given period and associated with a range of negative behavioral, physical and mental health outcomes. These comprise higher psychological distress, impaired neuropsychological function, increased anxiety and depression, unsafe sexual behavior, greater risk for infectious diseases, and enhanced mortality, including suicides [4, 5]. Considering the large societal and personal costs and suffering associated with polytoxicomania, its high prevalence in teenagers (8–18%) [6–8] is alarming. Yet, its underlying causes are largely unknown. Previous studies have proposed that different individual (e.g., gender, school grades, ethnicity, depression, personality traits), familial (e.g., parental monitoring, inner-family conflicts), and other social factors (e.g., connectedness, drug use by peers) can increase the risk for polytoxicomania [9–12], but systematic investigations of a broader spectrum of risk factors, especially in large samples, are rare.
The preadult accumulation of environmental risk factors is a potent predictor of various health outcomes and negative behaviors, including earlier age at schizophrenia onset [13], reduced global functioning and higher severity of psychopathology [14, 15], boosted violent-aggressive behavior [16], unfavorable child development [17], enhanced use of tobacco, alcohol and other drugs as well as delinquency and suicidal behavior in students [18]. The predisposing risks for polytoxicomania have mostly been studied in isolation, and reports on how cumulative risk affects polytoxicomania are scarce (e.g., [19, 20]). Therefore, the current study investigates for the first time effects of preadult environmental risk accumulation on polytoxicomania based on the unique high-quality deep-phenotyping data of GRAS (Göttingen Research Association for Schizophrenia) [21].
Moreover, we aimed at shedding first light on possible genetic risk factors predisposing to polytoxicomania by exploiting the extensive genotyping information of GRAS. Past studies focused on genetic associations with single drugs (e.g., [22–24]). Few reports addressed any genetic underpinnings of polytoxicomania, concentrating on single genes, or performing genome-wide association studies (GWAS) on lifetime drug experimentation [25–27]. Even though twin studies suggest genetic liability to substance use disorders in adolescence [28], no study has explored preadult polytoxicomania on a genome-wide level likely due to world-wide missing databases on this condition which impedes any kind of replication study. Therefore, our novel multiple GWAS–PGAS (phenotype-based genetic association studies) approach utilizes extensive phenotyping information to improve the confidence with which genetic signals from single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in relatively small (n < 3500), but deep-phenotyped samples, can be identified.
We show here that environmental risk accumulation, including physical abuse, sexual abuse, migration, urbanicity, and the use of gateway drugs, cannabis, and alcohol [29, 30], strongly promotes polytoxicomania. Further, we support existing evidence that preadult polytoxicomania induces aggressive and criminal behavior later in life. Finally, we deliver first hints for a genetic susceptibility to preadult polytoxicomania, which ultimately require independent replication upon availability of suitable cohorts anywhere worldwide.
Subjects and methods
Subjects
The extended GRAS data collection consists of N~2000 schizophrenic and schizoaffective subjects (according to DSM-IV-TR). Ethics Committees of Georg-August-University, Göttingen, and participating centers across Germany approved GRAS, complying with Helsinki Declaration. All patients (and/or legal representatives) gave written informed consent. For genetic association analyses, also healthy blood donors (N = 2111; age 33.68 ± 12.21 years, 58.46% males) were included. Complete phenotypical information was available for N = 1904 schizophrenia/schizoaffective patients (40.28 ± 13.11 years, 66.02% males), complete genotype information for N = 1718 individuals (40.44 ± 13.07 years, 66.94% males) after exclusion of individuals due to relatedness, ancestral outlier status, and missing phenotype data. We note that individuals were only included in phenotypic or genetic analyses if unambiguous information on environmental risk, drug use/autistic traits/suicidal behavior was obtainable, explaining some N number variation.
Phenotyping
Environmental risk
Environmental risk factors, including physical abuse, sexual abuse, migration, urbanicity, cannabis use and problematic alcohol use before age of 18 years and onset of schizophrenia, were operationalized as described in detail earlier [13, 16]. Additional environmental risk factors (for evaluation of autistic traits only) included perinatal complications, season of birth, early neurotrauma, paternal age, number of infections (positive serology) [13, 16].
Polytoxicomania
Medical reports and SCID-I allowed assessment of current and previous drug use. Drug classes included cocaine, opioids, hallucinogens, amphetamines, ecstasy, barbiturates, benzodiazepines, and inhalants. In the environmental risk study, use of ≥3 drugs was coded as polytoxicomania, use of ≤2 drugs as non-polytoxicomanic behavior. For the multiple GWAS–PGAS approach, exact definitions of polytoxicomania for each GWAS are provided in display items. Note that caffeine was not considered as “drug”.
Other outcomes
Premorbid intelligence was measured by Mehrfachwahl–Wortschatz–Intelligenztest-B (MWT-B [31]), corrected for age and medication (CPZ; chlorpromazine equivalents) using standardized residuals from linear regression. Suicidality: Suicide attempts served as readout. Autistic traits: PANSS Autism Severity Score (PAUSS) [32] was used to assess severity.
Genotyping
Genotyping was performed using a semi-custom Axiom® myDesignTM genotyping array (Affymetrix, Santa Clara, CA, USA), based on a CEU (Caucasian residents of European ancestry from Utah, USA) marker backbone including 518,722 SNPs, and a custom marker set including 102,537 SNPs (described in detail [33]). A total of 530,316 autosomal variants passed quality control, had minor allele frequency (MAF) > 0.05, were in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE; p > 0.001) and therefore included in genetic analyses.
Genetic association analysis
From each pair of related individuals (second to third-degree relatives, PIHAT > 0.185) one individual was randomly excluded. In healthy blood donor-patient pairs, priority was given to patient samples. Ancestral outliers were excluded based on principal component analysis (PCA; principal component1 (PC1 ± 3 SD) and PC2 ± 5 SD). PCs were calculated on LD-pruned marker set (long-range LD-regions removed) of 96,836 autosomal SNPs. In all association tests (SNP test: logistic regression; gene-based test: PC regression), sex and first 10 PCs were entered as covariates to control for population stratification.
Human expression data
Human tissue expression data (standardized) was downloaded from Harmonizome [34] (https://maayanlab.cloud/Harmonizome/dataset/GTEx+Tissue+Gene+Expression+Profiles, accessed 23/11/2020).
Statistical analysis
Calculations of relatedness, principal components, genetic association analyses, and LD-based clumping (index variant p value threshold=0.01) of GWAS results were performed in PLINK v1.90 [35], gene-based association tests in MAGMA v1.07b [36]. Chi-square tests and Fisher’s exact tests were applied to test for categorical associations. Cochran–Armitage or Jonckheere–Terpstra tests (permutations n = 20,000) were employed for trends. Levene’s test was used to assess homogeneity of variances. In case of homoscedasticity, Welch’s, Games–Howell test, or Welch’s ANOVA (R-package “onewaytests”) were applied for group comparisons and post-hoc tests with continuous outcomes (Bonferroni-corrected). In all instances where mathematical assumptions could have been violated, robust statistical tests were carried out to exclude a negative impact of such violations on statistical outcomes. Calculation of variance inflation factors (VIF) assured absence of multicollinearity between predictor variables (all VIF < 1.2), except when smoking was included (thus rejecting it as predictor). Odds ratio (OR) was calculated using R-package “DescTools” or exact logistic regression with permutation (PROC LOGISTIC) when cell count was equal to 0 (SAS 9.4). Except for OR calculations, all statistical tests and workflows were performed in R v3.5.2 [37] or IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.
Results
Preadult polytoxicomania and aggressive behavior
We first explored the predictive potential of individual illicit drugs for aggressive behavior using a violent aggression proxy (Fig. 1A) [16]. The proportion of violently aggressive individuals increased step-wise with the number of illicit drugs consumed before age 18 years (Fig. 1B): Nearly 50% of individuals consuming ≥4 drugs in early life, i.e., who were clearly polytoxicomanic at a young age, showed violent aggression as compared to just 15% in the group of individuals, who did not use any illicit drug before age 18 (extreme group comparison OR = 5.3, 95% CI [3.4–8.3]).
Contrasting drug use in non-polytoxicomanic and polytoxicomanic individuals
A comparison of lifetime patterns of illicit drug use amongst non-polytoxicomanic (<3 drugs) and polytoxicomanic (≥3 drugs) individuals showed a highly diverse drug use in the latter group (Supplementary Table 1). While the vast majority of non-polytoxicomanic individuals never had contact with illicit/hard drugs in their life (>95% for any drug), the majority of polytoxicomanic individuals had used most of the respective drug classes at least once, e.g., only 13% and 24% did not have any contact with cocaine or ecstasy, respectively. Furthermore, frequency of use in polytoxicomanic individuals varied substantially between drug classes, e.g., although only 18% never tried hallucinogens, a minority of individuals (~3%) report using it (almost) daily. This combination makes it difficult to entirely exclude certain drug preferences at the individual level. Overall, however, polytoxicomanic individuals in our sample displayed a highly diverse and frequent use of drugs, without systematic preference. This clearly discriminates them from individuals classified here as non-polytoxicomanic.
Accumulated preadult environmental risk predicts polytoxicomania
Next, we evaluated the prognostic value of individual preadult environmental risks for polytoxicomania. Whereas all factors by themselves showed some tendency, our “secondary” environmental risks, alcohol, and cannabis, revealed already here their expected role as gateway/starter drugs (Fig. 1C). The following accumulation analysis showed that the higher the number of preadult risk factors, the higher the likelihood of polytoxicomania over lifetime (extreme group comparison OR = 31.4, 95% CI [15.9–61.9]) (Fig. 1D). We next stratified by preadult polytoxicomania (≥3 drugs consumed before age 18 years/onset of schizophrenia, Fig. 1E) and adult polytoxicomania (≥3 drugs consumed throughout life, but after age 18 years/onset of schizophrenia, Fig. 1F). This resulted in extreme probabilities for developing polytoxicomania preadult with OR = 226.6 (extreme group comparison; 95% CI [50.8-∞]) and adult with OR = 17.5 (extreme group comparison; 95% CI [8.4–36.4]), strongly pointing to a causal relationship between preadult environmental risk and polytoxicomania.
Preadult environmental risk accumulation predicts polytoxicomania also without inclusion of starter drugs in the model
To alleviate concerns of potential circularity when including the starter drugs cannabis and alcohol as secondary environmental risks of polytoxicomania, we omitted them on a trial basis, using only 4 risk factors as predictors (urbanicity, migration, physical abuse, sexual abuse) (Supplementary Fig 1A–C). Reassuringly, the overall pattern of risk accumulation predicting polytoxicomania remained identical even though the effect was expectedly less pronounced.
Lack of evidence of an appreciable role of tobacco as gateway drug
Smoking prevalence in our sample was very high, with 85% of individuals reporting to have smoked at least once in their life. Of those, 80% started before age 18 and onset of schizophrenia. Thus, although nicotine may be often consumed before other drugs, due to its high prevalence (and thus “non-specificity”) its predictive value for use of illicit or hard drugs is rather limited. For example, whereas 37% and 34% of our individuals, who consumed cannabis (mostly also smokers) or had problematic alcohol use (either in isolation or in combination with other risk factors) in preadulthood, respectively, developed preadult polytoxicomania, “only” 19% of young smokers (including those with a combination of tobacco with cannabis and possibly other risk factors) progressed to preadult polytoxicomania.
Of note, multicollinearity excluded smoking as valid predictor. In fact, there was a very strong relationship between tobacco and cannabis use (cannabis is mostly smoked with tobacco). In our sample only 2.5% of individuals, who used cannabis at young age, never smoked before age 18 and onset of schizophrenia. They consumed cannabis in cookies or similar. Vice versa, 53% of all preadult smokers in our sample also report usage of cannabis. Nonetheless, when including smoking in a purely exploratory fashion as additional risk factor, the accumulation model expectedly does not improve (Supplementary Fig 1D–F). Interestingly, when smoking was the only risk factor an individual was exposed to, it did not increase the probability of being polytoxicomanic, questioning a substantial role of tobacco by itself as starter drug.
Risk load comparison in individuals with or without polytoxicomania and role of cannabis and alcohol as starter drugs re-visited
Risk load in preadult polytoxicomanic subjects was immense (Fig. 2A). Nearly 50% of all individuals showing preadult polytoxicomania had experienced ≥3 risk factors before the age of 18 years/onset of schizophrenia. In the group of adult polytoxicomania, 34% had ≥3 risk factors in contrast to only 12% of individuals without polytoxicomania. All preadult polytoxicomanic individuals (100%) experienced at least 1 risk factor in early life. Likewise, in the adult polytoxicomanic group, only 5% of individuals had no risk factor before adulthood. In contrast, 32% of individuals in the non-polytoxicomanic group were “risk-free” (Fig. 2A).
Subsequently, we investigated in detail the distribution of individual risk factors in the consumption groups. There was a strong group difference regarding preadult cannabis exposure (p = 4.59e–93, Table 1), with 100% of preadult polytoxicomanic individuals (irrespective of risk numbers) having consumed cannabis at early age. Among adult polytoxicomanic individuals, this number dropped to 91.7%, 87.5%, and 69.6% in the risk groups with ≥3, 2, or 1 risk factors, respectively. In comparison, within the non-polytoxicomanic group, the percentage of individuals with cannabis use in early life was lower (68.1%, 47.3%, 14.7%, respectively), yet still substantial. The prevalence of preadult problematic alcohol use followed a similar but less pronounced pattern. In addition, migration and physical abuse differed between consumption groups (Table 1). Overall, the most frequent risk factor was urbanicity (43.1%), followed by cannabis (38.3%) and physical abuse (16.5%) (Table 1).
Table 1.
n (%) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group | # Risk factors | Cannabis | Alcohol | Urbanicity | Migration | Physical abuse | Sexual abuse |
Preadult | 1 | 21 (100.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) |
Adult | 32 (69.6) | 2 (4.3) | 7 (15.2) | 1 (2.2) | 3 (6.5) | 1 (2.2) | |
Non-polytox | 52 (14.7) | 31 (8.8) | 191 (54.1) | 12 (3.4) | 30 (8.5) | 37 (10.5) | |
Preadult | 2 | 38 (100.0) | 20 (52.6) | 10 (26.3) | 2 (5.3) | 5 (13.2) | 1 (2.6) |
Adult | 56 (87.5) | 21 (32.8) | 29 (45.3) | 7 (10.9) | 9 (14.1) | 6 (9.4) | |
Non-polytox | 79 (47.3) | 38 (22.8) | 105 (62.9) | 28 (16.8) | 47 (28.1) | 37 (22.2) | |
Preadult | ≥3 | 61 (100.0) | 42 (70.0) | 43 (81.1) | 20 (32.8) | 22 (36.1) | 16 (26.7) |
Adult | 55 (91.7) | 35 (59.3) | 41 (73.2) | 19 (31.7) | 24 (40.0) | 22 (36.7) | |
Non-polytox | 77 (68.1) | 57 (50.4) | 98 (88.3) | 44 (38.6) | 63 (55.3) | 44 (38.9) | |
Totala | 471 (38.3) | 246 (20.0) | 524 (43.1) | 133 (10.8) | 203 (16.5) | 164 (13.3) | |
p (χ²) | 4.59e−93 (425.23) | 1.72e−26 (118.65) | 0.58 (1.08) | 0.001 (13.61) | 0.042 (6.35) | 0.421 (1.73) |
Bold values indicate significant differences (p < 0.05) in the prevalence of respective risk factors between the three consumption groups (across number of risk factors).
aCalculation of total percentage also included individuals with 0 risk factors. Underlined p values remain significant after Bonferroni correction (p < 0.05/6 = 0.008). Two-sided Chi²-test applied.
Individuals in the consumption groups differed from each other with respect to age (p = 6.26e−62), age at disease onset (p = 5.87e−38), and premorbid intelligence (corrected for age and medication, p = 0.002), but not medication (p = 0.38; Table 2). Premorbid intelligence of preadult polytoxicomanic individuals was significantly lower than that of adult polytoxicomanic (p = 0.01) and non-polytoxicomanic (p = 0.02), but did not differ between the latter two groups (p > 0.99). A two-way ANOVA confirmed the main effects of environmental risk group and consumption group on age of onset without interaction between the two groups (environmental risks: p = 8.71e−08, consumption: p = 4.66e−12, risks×consumption: p = 0.23). Figure 2B depicts the substantial impact of polytoxicomanic behavior on age at disease onset. In the non-polytoxicomanic individuals, the age at disease onset step-wise decreases with the number of risk factors (p = 1.73e−08). In preadult and adult polytoxicomanic individuals, an increasing number of risk factors had no appreciable further influence on the already early age at disease onset (p = 0.89 and p = 0.30, respectively).
Table 2.
Total | Non-polytox | Adult | Preadult | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD) | ||||
Age (years) | ||||
39.59 (12.71) | 41.98 (12.73) | 34.00 (9.81) | 28.86 (6.86) | 6.26e−62 |
n = 1559 | n = 1192 | n = 213 | n = 154 | |
Age at disease onset (years) | ||||
25.95 (8.81) | 27.21 (9.22) | 22.60 (6.17) | 20.82 (4.66) | 5.87e−38 |
n = 1531 | n = 1170 | n = 210 | n = 151 | |
Chlorpromazine equivalents | ||||
679.25 (670.09) | 669.81 (678.96) | 740.58 (694.05) | 669.71 (558.51) | 0.38 |
n = 1513 | n = 1161 | n = 202 | n = 150 | |
Premorbid intelligence MWT-B-corrected | ||||
0.04 (1.00) | 0.05 (1.04) | 0.13 (0.96) | −0.16 (0.70) | 0.002 |
n = 1440 | n = 1101 | n = 194 | n = 145 |
MWT-B Mehrfachwahl–Wortschatz–Intelligenztest-B; Premobid intelligence: Standardized residuals corrected for Chlorpromazine equivalents and age. Sample sizes vary because of missing data. Bold values indicate significant differences between means of the three consumption groups. Underlined p values remain significant after Bonferroni correction (p < 0.05/4 = 0.0125). Two-sided Welch’s ANOVA (age, age at disease onset, premorbid intelligence) or ANOVA (Chlorpromazine equivalents) applied.
Effects of accumulated preadult environmental risk on other behavioral outcomes
In view of these striking results, we explored the possibility that preadult environmental risk accumulation might be a potent predictor of any other behavioral outcome as well. Building on our previous findings of violent-aggressive behavior increasing with preadult environmental risk [16], we investigated its impact on autoaggressive behavior in form of suicidality. Indeed, with rising number of environmental risks, we also observed a surge in percentage of individuals with suicidal thoughts (p = 0.012, Cochran–Armitage test; OR = 1.5, 95% CI [1.1–2.2], extreme group comparison), suicidal plans (p = 0.004, Cochran–Armitage test; OR = 1.7, 95% CI [1.2–2.3], extreme group comparison) and suicide attempts (Fig. 2C; p = 3.3e−05, Cochran–Armitage test; OR = 2.0, 95% CI [1.4–2.8], extreme group comparison). This surge, however, appears to be explained only by individuals with additional heteroaggression (Fig. 2D).
As another behavioral readout, we assessed whether preadult risk accumulation can also predict severity of autistic features in adulthood (Fig. 2E). For this approach, we even included more risk factors, previously reported to play a role in autistic behavior, i.e., perinatal complications, early neurotrauma, season of birth, paternal age, and number of postnatal infections. Interestingly, we did not observe any association of autistic traits with any environmental risk factor, neither individually (all p > 0.05) nor in accumulation.
Genetic susceptibility to preadult polytoxicomania
In order to overcome the challenges associated with detecting genetic associations in relatively small, but deeply phenotyped, high-quality samples, we developed the novel multiple GWAS–PGAS approach to discover potential first associations of common genetic variants (SNPs) with preadult polytoxicomania. As depicted in Fig. 3A, B, this approach rests on multiple GWAS for polytoxicomania with minor variations in phenotype definitions. Supplementary Fig. 2 presents Manhattan plots for GWAS 1-6. The underlying working hypotheses of the workflow described in Fig. 3C, presume that (i) true genetic signals should be stable across (minor to moderate) changes in sample composition, (ii) true genetic signals are more likely to have neighbors in linkage disequilibrium which show a signal, too (but not entirely excluding that false do as well), and (iii) the here identified genetic associations with polytoxicomania should be independent of schizophrenia diagnosis (although pleiotropic risk genes exist [38] but are a separate matter). Figure 4A, B depicts the overlap structure between different GWAS results at given p value threshold. Importantly, GWAS 1-4 (set1) have the highest number of shared SNPs among each other in the raw (n = 3539-red; Fig. 4A) as well as the clumped/LD-linked results (n = 41-red; Fig. 4B)—convincingly visualized by Jaccard index matrix (Supplementary Fig 3). Notably, clumping and usage of linkage information considerably condense the number of final polytoxicomania-associated SNPs, identifying 41 potentially relevant SNPs (at p < 0.01, with LD neighbors p < 0.05; graphically displayed as red dots in Manhattan plots, Supplementary Fig 2). Out of these, 28 were located in gene-coding regions (±10 kb, Supplementary Table 2). Interestingly, 15 of these genes (out of 20 with human tissue expression data available on Harmonizome [34]) are strongly expressed in brain and/or kidney (Supplementary Fig 4). In addition, gene-wide association results (MAGMAv1.07b) yielded 11 genes potentially associated with preadult polytoxicomanic behavior (Supplementary Table 3). We abstained from deeper interpretation of these genes at this stage because of the still required replication. Known associations, however, listed in Supplementary Tables 2 and 3, are promising.
Discussion
This study discovered a prominent association of polytoxicomania with early accumulation of environmental risk factors, comprising urbanicity, migration, sexual abuse, physical abuse and, as secondary risks, alcohol, and cannabis. In particular, preadult polytoxicomania is >220 times more likely to develop in subjects exposed to ≥3 risks. In addition, polytoxicomania in later life occurs with higher probability after adolescent accumulation of negative environmental impact. Thus, our data show that multiple drug use has obviously remarkable roots in preadult risk factor buildup. In perfect agreement with previous reports (e.g., [39–41]), we find clear escalation in percentage of violent behavior with increasing numbers of drugs consumed. Strikingly, the number of accumulated risk factors appears to be more informative than the type of environmental risk factor. Nonetheless, our data strongly underline the well-known significance of cannabis and alcohol as gateway/starter drugs [29, 42–46].
In fact, all individuals with preadult polytoxicomania reported using cannabis. This confirms a tremendous association between cannabis and preadult polytoxicomania, even though not ultimately proving causality. A more causal perspective is perhaps offered by our data on polytoxicomania arising in adulthood. In adult polytoxicomanic individuals, the preadult risk factors temporally clearly precede the occurrence of polytoxicomania. Also in this group, cannabis is the by far most frequent preadult risk factor (occurring in 70–92% of individuals). Considering this extraordinary impact of cannabis, legalization and commercialization of its use will certainly increase not only incidence and prevalence of psychosis [47–53], but also of polytoxicomania.
Importantly, smoking as the sole risk factor did not increase the chance of developing polytoxicomania and did not influence our accumulation model further. Along these lines, heavy increase in tobacco consumption is highly frequently observed in alcoholics upon alcohol abstinence. This phenomenon is called ‘“Suchtverlagerung” or shift in addictive behaviors and is not associated with generation of polytoxicomania (but causes other severe problems [54]), again emphasizing the “non-specificity” of nicotine consumption in this regard. Nevertheless, it cannot be entirely excluded that tobacco has a minor role as “co-gateway” drug together with cannabis (as cannabis use is tightly associated with smoking status).
Cannabis and alcohol are sometimes seen as potential consequences of early-life stress, like physical and/or sexual abuse, considered as some sort of self-treatment [55]. However, the VIF for all variables in our model was low (all VIF < 1.2), indicating that there was no considerable multicollinearity between the risk factor variables. This suggests an independent input of each variable to the model including independent contributions of cannabis/alcohol and stress-related variables such as physical or sexual abuse.
We show that suicidality, as a severe form of autoaggression, follows a similar stair-pattern, but only in individuals exhibiting heteroaggression as well. Based on all these results, one could speculate that any kind of behavioral traits might be shaped by preadult environmental risk exposure; nevertheless, we did not see any respective influence of the here determined risks on the severity of autistic phenotypes in adulthood. It seems that autistic features are predominantly genetically induced [56, 57]. However, impact of intrauterine damage, e.g., viral infections, on development of autism is well established [58] and, like other potential postnatal risk factors, not recorded here.
The present findings are derived from a large, deeply phenotyped sample of schizophrenic subjects with uniquely comprehensive information on multiple drug consumption, but their translation to the general population has still to ensue with some caution. Nevertheless, preadult polytoxicomania in our sample occurs way before onset of the disease, and may thus reflect general mechanisms of damage to the juvenile brain that predispose to multiple drug consumption rather than any direct association with mental illness. The view of essential generalizability receives support by our studies on violent aggression, where we had the chance to include two general population samples that revealed the same stair-pattern of aggression development with risk accumulation as the 4 independent schizophrenia cohorts [16]. The earlier onset of mental illness in polytoxicomanic individuals shown here emphasizes another devastating role of multiple drug use on brain functions and on outcome in mental disease. Mechanistically, this phenomenon as well as the observed behavioral alterations are likely mediated by epigenetic changes that occur relatively non-specifically in response to accumulated risk factor exposure in early life. These epigenetic changes apparently increase, as others and we have reported previously, the propensity for later substance abuse, depression, violent aggression, and a range of other negative health outcomes [16, 59–61].
On top of the remarkable environmental impact on polytoxicomania, we provide first evidence of a genetic predisposition to this behavioral abnormality as extracted from our novel GWAS–PGAS approach. This novel approach was only possible based on the comprehensive and precise information on drug consumption in our sample, which allowed the accurate classification of just mildly varying phenotypes for respective GWAS. We defined strict criteria for this GWAS–PGAS approach, which is potentially applicable to other genetic association studies as well. Our procedure is grounded on the working hypotheses that true genetic signals remain stable despite mild modifications in phenotype definition and are likely not isolated but in linkage disequilibrium with related signals, and that genetic associations with the target phenotype (here: polytoxicomania) are independent of any other disease diagnosis (here: schizophrenia). Diagnoses-overlapping, i.e., pleiotropic genes [38] were purposely excluded here. The mild variation of phenotype definitions allowed us to identify genetic associations that are more specific for polytoxicomania and not “contaminated” by associations stemming solely from single substance abuse.
We ultimately extracted an interesting set of markers as potential “bouquet of common genetic denominators” for juvenile polytoxicomania, many of them strongly expressed in brain and kidney. Unfortunately, no comparable sample of deeply phenotyped polytoxicomanic individuals is presently available for replication studies—despite intensive worldwide search—but would be crucial to have. Nevertheless, the nature of the obtained first genetic results seems encouraging.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by the Max Planck Society and the DFG Research Center for Nanoscale Microscopy and Molecular Physiology of the Brain (CNMPB). AAS has held a stipend of the IMPRS-Neuroscience MSc-PhD program (DFG Grant GSC 226), VDG has been supported by the IMPRS-Genome Science PhD program.
Author contributions
Concept and supervision of the study: HE Design: AAS & HE. Data acquisition/analysis/interpretation: AAS, VDG, JS, AR, MB and HE. Drafting manuscript: AAS & HE. Drafting display items: AAS & HE. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Data availability
The data supporting the findings of this study are not publicly available because of strict obligation to human data protection laws. The contained information could lead to identification of subjects and compromise research participants’ privacy and consent. Additional information and summary statistics are available on request from the corresponding author (HE).
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41380-021-01069-2.
References
- 1.Toftdahl NG, Nordentoft M, Hjorthøj C. Prevalence of substance use disorders in psychiatric patients: a nationwide Danish population-based study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51:129–40. doi: 10.1007/s00127-015-1104-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mueser KT, Yarnold PR, Rosenberg SD, Swett C, Miles KM, Hill D. Substance use disorder in hospitalized severely mentally ill psychiatric patients: Prevalence, correlates, and subgroups. Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:179–92. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Alaja R, Seppä K, Sillanaukee P, Tienari P, Huyse FJ, Herzog T, et al. Physical and mental comorbidity of substance use disorders in psychiatric consultations. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22:1820–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03987.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Connor JP, Gullo MJ, White A, Kelly AB. Polysubstance use: diagnostic challenges, patterns of use and health. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2014;27:269–75. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hjorthøj C, Østergaard MLD, Benros ME, Toftdahl NG, Erlangsen A, Andersen JT, et al. Association between alcohol and substance use disorders and all-cause and cause-specific mortality in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and unipolar depression: a nationwide, prospective, register-based study. The Lancet. Psychiatry. 2015;2:801–8. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00207-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Conway KP, Vullo GC, Nichter B, Wang J, Compton WM, Iannotti RJ, et al. Prevalence and patterns of polysubstance use in a nationally representative sample of 10th Graders in the United States. J Adolesc Heal. 2013;52:716–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Moss HB, Chen CM, Yi HY. Early adolescent patterns of alcohol, cigarettes, and marijuana polysubstance use and young adult substance use outcomes in a nationally representative sample. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;136:51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.White A, Chan GCK, Quek LH, Connor JP, Saunders JB, Baker P, et al. The topography of multiple drug use among adolescent Australians: findings from the national drug strategy household survey. Addict Behav. 2013;38:2068–73. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bailey AJ, Farmer EJ, Finn PR. Patterns of polysubstance use and simultaneous co-use in high risk young adults. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2019;205:107656. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2019.107656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Brière FN, Fallu JS, Descheneaux A, Janosz M. Predictors and consequences of simultaneous alcohol and cannabis use in adolescents. Addict Behav. 2011;36:785–8. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Collins RL, Ellickson PL, Bell RM. Simultaneous polydrug use among teens: prevalence and predictors. J Subst Abus. 1998;10:233–53. doi: 10.1016/S0899-3289(99)00007-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jongenelis M, Pettigrew S, Lawrence D, Rikkers W. Factors associated with poly drug use in adolescents. Prev Sci. 2019;20:695–704. doi: 10.1007/s11121-019-00993-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Stepniak B, Papiol S, Hammer C, Ramin A, Everts S, Hennig L, et al. Accumulated environmental risk determining age at schizophrenia onset: a deep phenotyping-based study. The Lancet. Psychiatry. 2014;1:444–53. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70379-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Begemann M, Seidel J, Poustka L, Ehrenreich H. Accumulated environmental risk in young refugees—a prospective evaluation. EClinMed. 2020;22:100345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 15.Lederbogen F, Kirsch P, Haddad L, Streit F, Tost H, Schuch P, et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature. 2011;474:498–501. doi: 10.1038/nature10190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mitjans M, Seidel J, Begemann M, Bockhop F, Moya-Higueras J, Bansal V, et al. Violent aggression predicted by multiple pre-adult environmental hits. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24:1549–64. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0043-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Evans GW, Li D, Whipple SS. Cumulative risk and child development. Psychol Bull. 2013;139:1342–96. doi: 10.1037/a0031808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Adelmann PK. Social environmental factors and preteen health-related behaviors. J Adolesc Heal. 2005;36:36–47. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2003.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Racz SJ, Saha S, Trent M, Adger H, Bradshaw CP, Goldweber A, et al. Polysubstance use among minority adolescent males incarcerated for serious offenses. child youth care. Forum. 2016;45:205–20. doi: 10.1007/s10566-015-9334-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sharma S, Mustanski B, Dick D, Bolland J, Kertes DA. Protective factors buffer life stress and behavioral health outcomes among high-risk youth. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2019;47:1289–301. doi: 10.1007/s10802-019-00515-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ribbe K, Friedrichs H, Begemann M, Grube S, Papiol S, Kästner A, et al. The cross-sectional GRAS sample: a comprehensive phenotypical data collection of schizophrenic patients. BMC Psychiatry. 2010;10:91. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-10-91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hancock DB, Guo Y, Reginsson GW, Gaddis NC, Lutz SM, Sherva R, et al. Genome-wide association study across European and African American ancestries identifies a SNP in DNMT3B contributing to nicotine dependence. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23:1911–9. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gelernter J, Sherva R, Koesterer R, Almasy L, Zhao H, Kranzler HR, et al. Genome-wide association study of cocaine dependence and related traits: FAM53B identified as a risk gene. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19:717–23. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Treutlein J, Cichon S, Ridinger M, Wodarz N, Soyka M, Zill P, et al. Genome-wide association study of alcohol dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(Jul):773–84. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mallard TT, Ashenhurst JR, Harden KP, Fromme K. GABRA2, alcohol, and illicit drug use: an event-level model of genetic risk for polysubstance use. J Abnorm Psychol. 2018;127:190–201. doi: 10.1037/abn0000333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sanchez-Roige S, Fontanillas P, Elson SL, Gray JC, De Wit H, MacKillop J, et al. Genome-wide association studies of impulsive personality traits (BIS-11 and UPPS-P) and drug experimentation in up to 22,861 adult research participants identify loci in the CACNA1I and CADM2 genes. J Neurosci. 2019;39:2562–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2662-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Zhang P-W, Ishiguro H, Ohtsuki T, Hess J, Carillo F, Walther D, et al. Human cannabinoid receptor 1: 5′ exons, candidate regulatory regions, polymorphisms, haplotypes and association with polysubstance abuse. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9:916–31. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Waaktaar T, Kan KJ, Torgersen S. The genetic and environmental architecture of substance use development from early adolescence into young adulthood: a longitudinal twin study of comorbidity of alcohol, tobacco and illicit drug use. Addiction. 2018;113:740–8. doi: 10.1111/add.14076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fergusson DM, Boden JM, Horwood LJ. Cannabis use and other illicit drug use: testing the cannabis gateway hypothesis. Addiction. 2006;101:556–69. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01322.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kandel D, Kandel E. The gateway hypothesis of substance abuse: developmental, biological and societal perspectives. Acta Paediatr Int J Paediatr. 2015;104:130–7. doi: 10.1111/apa.12851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lehrl S. Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest: MWT-B. Spitta; 1999.
- 32.Kästner A, Begemann M, Michel TM, Everts S, Stepniak B, Bach C, et al. Autism beyond diagnostic categories: characterization of autistic phenotypes in schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry. 2015;15:1–12. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0494-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hammer C, Stepniak B, Schneider A, Papiol S, Tantra M, Begemann M, et al. Neuropsychiatric disease relevance of circulating anti-NMDA receptor autoantibodies depends on blood-brain barrier integrity. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19:1143–9. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rouillard AD, Gundersen GW, Fernandez NF, Wang Z, Monteiro CD, McDermott MG, et al. The harmonizome: a collection of processed datasets gathered to serve and mine knowledge about genes and proteins. Database (Oxf) 2016;2016:1–16. doi: 10.1093/database/baw100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MAR, Bender D, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:559–75. doi: 10.1086/519795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.de Leeuw CA, Mooij JM, Heskes T, Posthuma D. MAGMA: generalized gene-set analysis of GWAS data. PLoS Comput Biol. 2015;11:1–19. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.R Core Team. R: A Language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Core Team; 2018.
- 38.Lee PH, Anttila V, Won H, Feng YCA, Rosenthal J, Zhu Z, et al. Genomic relationships, novel loci, and pleiotropic mechanisms across eight psychiatric disorders. Cell. 2019;179:1469. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Cuffel BJ, Shumway M, Chouljian TL, Macdonald T. A longitudinal study of substance use and community violence in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1994;182:704–8. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 40.Dornbusch SM, Lin IC, Munroe PT, Bianchi AJ. Adolescent polydrug use and violence in the United States. Int J Adolesc Med Health. 1999;11:197–219. doi: 10.1515/IJAMH.1999.11.3-4.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Beaudoin M, Potvin S, Giguère CE, Discepola SL, Dumais A. Persistent cannabis use as an independent risk factor for violent behaviors in patients with schizophrenia. npj Schizophr. 2020;6:1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41537-020-0104-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Olthuis JV, Darredeau C, Barrett SP. Substance use initiation: the role of simultaneous polysubstance use. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2013;32:67–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-3362.2012.00470.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Secades-Villa R, Garcia-Rodríguez O, Jin CJ, Wang S, Blanco C. Probability and predictors of the cannabis gateway effect: a national study. Int J Drug Policy. 2015;26:135–42. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2014.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ. Does cannabis use encourage other forms of illicit drug use? Addiction. 2000;95:505–20. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.9545053.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hall WD, Lynskey M. Is cannabis a gateway drug? Testing hypotheses about the relationship between cannabis use and the use of other illicit drugs. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2005;24:39–48. doi: 10.1080/09595230500126698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lynskey MT, Heath AC, Bucholz KK, Slutske WS, Madden PAF, Nelson EC, et al. Escalation of drug use in early-onset cannabis users vs co-twin controls. J Am Med Assoc. 2003;289:427–33. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.4.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Van OsJ, Bak M, Hanssen M, Bijl RV, De Graaf R, Verdoux H. Cannabis use and psychosis: a longitudinal population-based study. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;156:319–27. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Di Forti M, Morgan C, Dazzan P, Pariante C, Mondelli V, Marques TR, et al. High-potency cannabis and the risk of psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195:488–91. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.109.064220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Semple DM, McIntosh AM, Lawrie SM. Cannabis as a risk factor for psychosis: systematic review. J Psychopharmacol. 2005;19:187–94. doi: 10.1177/0269881105049040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Arseneault L, Cannon M, Poulton R, Murray R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Cannabis use in adolescence and risk for adult psychosis: Longitudinal prospective study. Br Med J. 2002;325:1212–3. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7374.1212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Murray RM, Hall W. Will legalization and commercialization of cannabis use increase the incidence and prevalence of psychosis? JAMA Psychiatry. 2020;77:777–8. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 52.Gage SH, Hickman M, Zammit S. Association between cannabis and psychosis: epidemiologic evidence. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79:549–56. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Fergusson DM, Poulton R, Smith PF, Boden JM. Cannabis and psychosis. Bmj. 2006;332:172–5. doi: 10.1136/bmj.332.7534.172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hüttner E, Matthies U, Nikolova T, Ehrenreich H. A follow-up study on chromosomal aberrations in lymphocytes of alcoholics during early, medium, and long-term abstinence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1999;23:344–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1999.tb04120.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Temple EC, Driver M, Brown RF. Cannabis use and anxiety: Is stress the missing piece of the puzzle? Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, Bolton P, Simonoff E, Yuzda E, et al. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychol Med. 1995;25:63–77. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700028099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Tick B, Bolton P, Happé F, Rutter M, Rijsdijk F. Heritability of autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of twin studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2016;57:585–95. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Brown AS. Epidemiologic studies of exposure to prenatal infection and risk of schizophrenia and autism. Dev Neurobiol. 2012;72:1272–6. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Lewis CR, Olive MF. Early-life stress interactions with the epigenome: potential mechanisms driving vulnerability toward psychiatric illness. Behav Pharm. 2014;25:341–51. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Vaiserman AM. Epigenetic programming by early‐life stress: evidence from human populations. Dev Dyn. 2015;244:254–65. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.24211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bahari-Javan S, Varbanov H, Halder R, Benito E, Kaurani L, Burkhardt S, et al. HDAC1 links early life stress to schizophrenia-like phenotypes. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2017;114:E4686–94. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1613842114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data supporting the findings of this study are not publicly available because of strict obligation to human data protection laws. The contained information could lead to identification of subjects and compromise research participants’ privacy and consent. Additional information and summary statistics are available on request from the corresponding author (HE).