Abstract
The mating behavior of teleost fish consists of a sequence of stereotyped actions. By observing mating of zebrafish under high-speed video, we analyzed and characterized a behavioral cascade leading to successful fertilization. When paired, a male zebrafish engages the female by oscillating his body in high frequency (quivering). In response, the female pauses swimming and bends her body (freezing). Subsequently, the male contorts his trunk to enfold the female’s trunk. This behavior is known as wrap around. Here, we found that wrap around behavior consists of two previously unidentified components. After both sexes contort their trunks, the male adjusts until his trunk compresses the female’s dorsal fin (hooking). After hooking, the male trunk slides away from the female’s dorsal fin, simultaneously sliding his pectoral fin across the female’s gravid belly, stimulating egg release (squeezing/spawning). Orchestrated coordination of spawning presumably increases fertilization success. Surgical removal of the female dorsal fin inhibited hooking and the transition to squeezing. In a neuromuscular mutant where males lack quivering, female freezing and subsequent courtship behaviors were absent. We thus identified traits of zebrafish mating behavior and clarified their roles in successful mating.
Subject terms: Physiology, Zoology
Introduction
Zebrafish is a freshwater teleost whose natural habitat is shallow water in South Asia1,2. As a widely used model organism in biomedical research3, the mechanism of zebrafish reproduction has been studied for both ethological and practical reasons, such as maximizing egg collection for experiments4. In natural environments, zebrafish spawn in shallow water with aquatic vegetation1,2. In the laboratory, mating tanks are designed to emulate the natural environment and elicit spawning behaviors5. Upon successful mating, a single female zebrafish produces up to 200–300 eggs in a single spawning session6. Depending on the quality of eggs and sperm, typically 58–78% of released eggs become fertilized7.
The description of mating behavior in zebrafish is variable among the literature2,8–10, and recent technologies now offer the opportunity to better understand the intricacies of this behavioral cascade. One commonality, however, is how activation of (room) daylight initiates a male’s pursuit toward a female into the shallow area of the mating tank. Heretofore, mating behavior has generally been characterized by the following steps: undulate, chase, escort, encircle, quivering, pin, wrap around, and spawning5,8,11. In context, male and female fish swim back and forth within their habitat for tens of minutes (undulate). At some point, the male begins to follow the female closely (chase). The male appears to guide the female to spawning sites by repeatedly swimming between the female and the shallow areas of the mating tank (escort). The pair swim together in circles (encircle), and the male oscillates his body close to the female (quivering). Occasionally, the pair appear to court while in contact with the wall (pin). The male enfolds the female with his trunk (wrap around), and both sexes release their gametes (spawning). Kang et al. called the wrap around step grasping, highlighting the role of the male pectoral fin positioning, in conjunction with trunk alignment between the pair12.
The details of each mating behavior step, and their specific contributions toward successful fertilization, have not been clarified to the extent possible. Here, we explored zebrafish mating behavior in fine detail and classified our new observations within the existing step (Fig. 1A); we focus on the significance of individual steps using surgical manipulations and mutants.
Materials and methods
Fish lines and maintenance
The RIKEN-WT strain and the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor ε subunit knockout (εKO) line (ZFIN ID:ZDB-ALT-201215-9) were used in the present study. Zebrafish were maintained in self-circulating systems at Osaka Medical and Pharmaceutical University (OMPU) and Jichi Medical University (JMU). This study was carried out in compliance with the ARRIVE guidelines. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. All experimental protocols were approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees at OMPU and JMU.
Observation of mating and counting of eggs
Zebrafish were kept under long day conditions (8 AM–22 PM light). On the night preceding the observation, a pair consisting of one male and one female fish (3–4 months old; n = 10 unique pairs were used) were placed in a mating tank (Tecniplast, Buguggiate, Italy), separated by a divider. Standard length and body weight were 30.3 ± 0.9 mm, 630 ± 73 mg for males, 28.0 ± 1.6 mm, 406 ± 78 mg for females. The following morning, the divider was removed at 8 AM and mating behaviors were documented over the following hour. Mating behaviors were recorded for 60 min by both a home video camera HDR-CX420 (Sony, Tokyo, Japan) and a web camera DC-NCR300U (Hanwha Q CELLS Japan, Tokyo, Japan), positioned to capture both a horizontal and vertical view of the mating tank. Quivering, hooking, and spawning during the initial 30 min were manually identified and quantified off line, based on the slow-motion replay of the recorded videos. At 9 AM, we collected all eggs, then counted total eggs and fertilized eggs at 5–7 h post fertilization (hpf). Any eggs developed past the epiboly stage13 met criteria as fertilized eggs. In experiments using εKOs, we extended the mating behavioral observation to 3 h, to account for the possibility that the mutant pairs exhibited delayed spawning (requiring > 1 h).
Dorsal fin removal
After anesthetizing adult female fish (3–4 months old; 10 fish) in 0.04% tricaine (Tokyo Chemical Industry, Tokyo, Japan), dorsal fins of female zebrafish were removed (> 80% of the original length) with surgical scissors. After 2 days of recovery in a tank, the surgically altered female and an intact male were paired for mating. Mating behaviors were recorded for 1 h or 3 h (εKO).
High-speed video analysis of mating behavior
High-speed image capturing of wild type (WT) pairs, WT male and dorsal fin-removed female pairs, and WT and εKO zebrafish pairs were performed with a Photron camera (Photron, Tokyo, Japan) at 1000 frames/s. For the analyses on quivering, captured images were saved in JPEG format and processed with ImageJ. For each male performing quivering behavior, we measured the series of head angles as described in Zempo et al.14 as a proxy for plotting trajectory over time. We calculated and statistically analyzed amplitudes between the minimum and the maximal peaks of head angles during the initial 110 ms of quivering.
Statistical analysis
In Figs. 2E–I and 3D, E, unpaired t test (two-tailed) was performed for statistical analysis. For correlative analyses, Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient was calculated and the statistical significance was analyzed by Student's t-distribution.
Results
We used a high-speed camera (1000 frames/sec) to capture behavior immediately preceding spawning and examined coordination of mating sequences leading to successful fertilization (Fig. 1A, B, Movies 1 and 2). Based on the framework of Sessa et al.5 and Kang et al.12, we classified steps of mating as shown in Fig. 1A, with modifications as described below.
Following, and perhaps in response to, quivering by the male, the female stalls swimming and twists her trunk slightly; we call this step freezing (Fig. 1C and Supplemental Fig. 1). We propose separating the period formally known as “wrap around” into two separate steps based on the forward-then-inverse direction of male trunk movement relative to the female trunk. We call the first step, in recharacterizing wrap around, hooking. Hooking involves the male contorting his trunk, wrapping it around the female’s trunk, and sliding it rostrally (Fig. 1C). As reported in Kang et al.12, one pectoral fin of the male was also positioned under the female belly. After contacting the female dorsal fin, the male appears to apply mechanical pressure to the trunk of the female with his trunk and pectoral fin (Movies 1 and 2). Notably, the female moves forward during this process, allowing the male trunk and pectoral fin to shift caudally along the female body. We call this step squeezing, the timing of which partially overlaps with that of spawning. Males presumably release sperm during or immediately after squeezing which facilitates one completed cycle of mating.
To investigate the importance of these newly found behavior steps, we first analyzed mating episodes, clutch size, and fertilization success in mating between WT zebrafish. Time course analysis showed that mating episodes (one episode defined by one set of sequential quivering, hooking and spawning behaviors) concentrated in the initial 30 min following the initial quivering behavior; mating episodes were sparce after 40 min (Fig. 1D). During spawning, the number of eggs released per unit time also showed a similar pattern (Fig. 1D). Given this time course, we quantified behavioral events for 30 min in subsequent analyses, while eggs were collected and counted after the 60 min pairing session. For each mating pair, the total number of eggs was divided by the number of separate spawning behaviors occurring in a 30 min period to determine an average number of eggs released by a single spawning; this average value was relatively reliable (low variance) with only a few outliers (Fig. 1E; 14.9 ± 4.3).
Spawning consists of gamete release by a male and a female. While the released eggs can be visualized, release of sperm could not be visualized even under high-speed camera. We reasoned that the percentage of fertilized eggs in released eggs correlate with coordinated spawning, which needs to occur in a short time window for successful fertilization.
We plotted the number of released eggs against that of fertilized eggs from multiple pairing sessions (Fig. 1F). The plot showed a strong, positive correlation (r = 0.81), however, the fertilization success rate was fairly constant (r = 0.075) when plotted against the total number of released eggs (Fig. 1G). These data suggest that the coordinated timing of spawning inferred from the fertilization rate were consistently observed and reliable.
Next, we examined how steps preceding spawning affected fertilization. Slow-motion videos showed that quivering constantly preceded hooking, which was regularly followed by spawning. In statistical analysis, the number of separate hooking bouts observed in 30 min correlated reliably with that of quivering (Fig. 2A, r = 0.85). Hooking showed moderately strong correlation with spawning (Fig. 2B, r = 0.68). These data suggest that the sequential steps of quivering, hooking, and spawning are tightly linked, and follow through once initiated.
To examine the significance of hooking, we contrived a method to specifically inhibit it. We predicted the female dorsal fin as a key for transition from hooking to squeezing because it seemed to constitute an integral element of the behavior (Fig. 1). Therefore, we surgically removed the dorsal fin from n=10 female zebrafish, of which we observed following their recovery period (Movie 3, Fig. 2C,D). We observed that without the female dorsal fin intact, the male trunk slipped rostrally and appeared to miss the opportunity to engage in squeezing. In response to such maladapted hooking and absent squeezing, the female terminated the behavioral mating sequence by evading the male without spawning.
We compared the number of spawning episodes exhibited by the same mating pairs, both before and after the female’s dorsal fin removal. The results showed a dramatical decrease after the fin removal (Fig. 2E; 25.1 ± 5.1 vs 3.5 ± 1.9; n=10), suggesting that hooking is critical for spawning. When the total released egg number was compared, it was also strongly decreased (Fig. 2F; 267 ± 49 vs 67 ± 31). Interestingly, among the small number of released eggs obtained after surgery, the fertilization rate was also reduced (Fig. 2G, 68 ± 5.7% vs 35 ± 11%), which suggested that the successful hooking is important for coordinating the timing of spawning between the male and female, as well as for adjusting the relative location of their gamete release.
We examined whether the dorsal fin removal also affects upstream steps in the behavioral mating sequence. The number of quivering bouts did not change between before, and after, the fin removal (Fig. 2H; 12.2 ± 3.9 vs 21.6 ± 7.8), suggesting that the failed hooking did not lead to compensatory increase of quivering. Quivering after surgery did lead to freezing, and subsequently to the incomplete hooking endeavor. The percentage of pairs proceeding from quivering to hooking (note hooking in these pairs ended uncompleted) did not change after the fin removal (Fig. 2I).
We next examined how quivering affects its downstream steps. We recently established a KO zebrafish line of the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (AChR) ε subunit14. While the locomotion of εKOs were normal in many respects14, we found that the quivering of the KO male was compromised and used it as a tool to specifically impair quivering (Fig. 3A, Movie 4).
We analyzed the head angles during quivering using a high-speed camera (Fig. 3A). In WT pairs, the head angles plotted against time showed clear and regular peaks in accordance with previous studies11 (Fig. 3B). εKOs in contrast showed much reduced change in head angles. In addition, the pattern of the head turn was variable among individual males and many of them seemed uncoordinated because timing, direction, and degree of head angle change was unpredictable, unlike that of a WT zebrafish.
When the head angles shown in Fig. 3B were averaged (Fig. 3C), a clear oscillating pattern was evident in the WT male. In contrast, peaks were not recognizable in the εKO male, suggesting inconsistent phases. Because the regular oscillation could not be determined in many εKOs, the angle amplitude was determined automatically (see methods) and compared. The amplitude was strongly suppressed in εKOs compared to WTs (Fig. 3D; 48.9 ± 4.0 vs 24.0 ± 2.2 degrees).
Presumably due to the compromised quivering, pairs of the εKO male and the WT female did not lay eggs, even after 3 h of pairing (Fig. 3E). In these pairs, female did not show freezing response, and males did not display hooking behavior. We checked the fertility of εKO males by in vitro fertilization, using sperm from testis of a sacrificed εKO male and eggs from a WT female (Supplemental Fig. 2). Fertilization of eggs from this cross verified the quality of sperm in εKO males. In contrast, WT male and εKO female pairs showed typical mating behaviors and laid eggs, suggesting that the freezing performed by the εKO female was strong enough to cause following steps (Movie 5). Therefore, the εKO mutation appears to interrupt successful mating events by inhibiting the quivering courtship behavior in εKO males, yet not by inhibiting freezing or spawning behaviors by females.
Discussion
In the present study, based on high-speed motion analysis, we defined three previously uncharacterized steps of mating between the quivering and spawning behaviors: freezing, hooking and squeezing (Fig. 1A). For an efficient fertilization, it is important to synchronize the timing of spawning. We evaluated the synchronized spawning inferred from the fertilization rate, and examined the contribution of key steps using surgical intervention and genetic mutants (Fig. 4).
In response to quivering stimulations from WT males, WT female fish displayed S-shaped bending (freezing). WT females did not show freezing behavior to εKO males with insufficient quivering (Fig. 3, Movie 4). Freezing behavior may facilitate grasping of the female. Thus freezing as a receptive behavior may be equivalent to lordosis in female mice or rats15,16.
Behaviors similar to freezing have been reported in some teleosts. Female killifish (Fundulus heteroclitus) show S-shaped bending during spawning behavior, described as early as in 190717. Later works suggested the S-shaped bending of killifish can be induced by injecting hormones understood to trigger reproductive behaviors, arginine vasopressin or oxytocin18, in the absence of quivering. S-shaped bending is also observed in the spawning behavior of medaka (Oryzias latipes), and the behavior in medaka also occurs in response to oxytocin (Atonin)19. Therefore, freezing behavior in zebrafish might be controlled by the same suite of reproductive hormones.
It is noteworthy, however, that in killifish and medaka, not only females but also males show S-shape bending during spawning, which is also induced by arginine vasopressin or oxytocin18,19. In contrast, male zebrafish never showed freezing or S-shaped bending during mating behavior. Moreover, the timing of S-shape bending in the mating process is different between medaka and zebrafish. Therefore, the significance and the control mechanism of the S-shaped bending are likely different between teleost species and there is more understanding to be uncovered regarding the courtship or mating behaviors of these traditionally medicinal and genetic model organisms.
Following quivering, the male fish transitioned to hooking. Removal of the female dorsal fin inhibited hooking (Fig. 2). However failed hooking occasionally led to spawning. These females spawned eggs in a shallow area in the tank (Fig. 4). Proximity to the floor may allow male fish to hold females without hooking.
Even when spawning occurred, fertilization rate in released eggs was strongly reduced after surgery (Fig. 2G). This is in sharp contrast to mating between WT pairs (Fig. 1F, G), in which the fertilization rate was independent from the clutch size. The release of eggs and sperm must be coordinated with regard to both timing and proximity. Our finding stresses the importance of hooking for triggering spawning behaviors.
A previous study suggested that surgically removing the pair of male pectoral fins, which are important for the grasping of the female, results in strikingly decreased fertilization rate12. In combination with the current study, males’ pectoral fins and females’ dorsal fins both seem important for coordinating fertilization. Any disturbance to the sequence of hooking and squeezing results in an erroneous coordination of spawning between the pair.
We analyzed quivering quantitatively, which has not been done previously. During quivering, the WT male directly stimulated the body of the female by repeatedly turning his head 48.9 ± 4.0 degrees. In addition to zebrafish, quivering behavior has been reported in other teleosts including medaka20, Mexican cavefish21, cichlid fish22, or salmoninae23. Medaka and Mexican cave fish males display quivering immediately preceding spawning. Thus, quivering in these species may be involved in inducing egg release by stimulating females in the same fashion as in zebrafish. In contrast, quivering in cichlid (A. burtoni) plays a different role22. A male cichlid fish escorts a female to his territory by displaying quivering in front of the female. After entering the spawning site, the pair encircle each other several times and lay eggs. Therefore, rather than inducing spawning, quivering in cichlids may function to attract females. All in all, quivering have varied functions across species. In the present study, using a mutant with impaired quivering, we showed that a male’s quivering behavior is necessary for the subsequent freezing behavior of females in zebrafish.
The εKO mutant does not express muscle-type nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (AChR) in fast muscles14. Although neuromuscular junctions in fast muscles are synaptically silenced, the εKO fish compensate their locomotion defects by rewiring motor neurons and converting slow muscles to fast muscles. Regardless of this neurological compensation, the εKO male fish showed defective quivering behavior. We speculate that quivering in the εKO males was weak and uncoordinated because of their smaller number of functional muscles compared to the WTs12. Indeed, while spontaneous swimming speed of εKOs is comparable to WTs14, the initial phase of escape response is weaker as measured by the amplitude of the head movement. Thus, the rapid succession of contractions during quivering may require increased muscle power, coordination, or energy compared to standard swimming. For the same reason, εKO male may not be able to perform squeezing either. However, it is difficult to examine this possibility because εKO male may not even attempt squeezing, which follows quivering in normal mating.
Conversely, εKO females paired with WT males showed typically successful spawning behavior in spite of the smaller number of functional muscles in εKO females14. This result suggests that female zebrafish need fewer functional muscle cells than males to complete squeezing, presumably due to the passive nature of the egg release by females. In medaka, females spawn simply by the quivering stimulation without squeezing20. When medaka pairs were disturbed during quivering by tapping on the tank, quivering stimulation longer than 4 sec was sufficient for female to start releasing eggs. Moreover, after spawning was initiated, females did not stop releasing eggs even when separated from males. One possible mechanism underlying this difference between zebrafish and medaka is the ovarian contraction, reported in several teleosts including medaka21, guppy22, killifish23 among others. The ovary contraction, induced by applied acetylcholine24, plays an important role in releasing eggs. In medaka, acetylcholine induces ovary contraction even after removal of the abdominal wall25, which suggests a direct stimulation of the ovarian smooth muscle. The ovary contraction has not been reported in zebrafish. Likewise, Mexican cavefish display wrap around behavior and squeezing-like behavior26, in which ovary contraction has not been reported. It is therefore a reasonable hypothesis that teleost species require one of two mechanisms to release eggs: squeezing behavior or ovary contraction. A curious next step in this investigation will involve examining whether the ovary of zebrafish or Mexican cavefish contract in response to acetylcholine. In addition, morphological analysis on smooth muscles in the ovary wall of various teleost species, including zebrafish and Mexican cavefish, will also provide important information to further understand the mechanism of egg release during spawning behavior.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We thank Drs. Kataaki Okubo and Tory Williams for critical reading of the manuscript. We are grateful to Ms. Seiko Ohkuma at JMU for her excellent care of zebrafish used in this study. This study was supported by JICHI MEDICAL UNIVERSITY YOUNG INVESTIGATOR AWARD.
Author contributions
B.Z. and F.O. conceptualized and designed the study; B.Z., N.T., and E.D. performed experiments; N.T. and E.D. prepared Figs. 1, 2 and 4; B.Z. prepared Fig. 3 and Movies; B.Z. and N.T. analyzed data; B.Z. and F.O. wrote the manuscript; F.O. supervised the study, provided resources.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Buntaro Zempo, Email: bzempo@jichi.ac.jp.
Fumihito Ono, Email: fumihito.ono@ompu.ac.jp.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-99638-6.
References
- 1.Lawrence C. The husbandry of zebrafish (Danio rerio): a review. Aquaculture. 2007;269:1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.aquaculture.2007.04.077. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Spence R, Gerlach G, Lawrence C, Smith C. The behaviour and ecology of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Biol. Rev. 2008;83:13–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00030.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rosenthal N, Ashburner M. Headwaters of the zebrafish-emergence of a new model vertebrate. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2002;3:717–724. doi: 10.1038/nrg891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hoo, J. Y., Kumari, Y., Shaikh, M. F., Hue, S. M. & Goh, B. H. Zebrafish: a versatile animal model for fertility research. BioMed Res. Int. (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 5.Sessa AK, et al. The effect of a depth gradient on the mating behavior, oviposition site preference, and embryo production in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Zebrafish. 2008;5:335–339. doi: 10.1089/zeb.2008.0535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Nasiadka A, Clark MD. Zebrafish breeding in the laboratory environment. ILAR J. 2012;53:161–168. doi: 10.1093/ilar.53.2.161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Riesco MF, Valcarce DG, Martínez-Vázquez JM, Robles V. Effect of low sperm quality on progeny: a study on zebrafish as model species. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-47702-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Darrow KO, Harris WA. Characterization and development of courtship in zebrafish, Danio rerio. Zebrafish. 2004;1:40–45. doi: 10.1089/154585404774101662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Pradhan A, Olsson PE. Zebrafish sexual behavior: role of sex steroid hormones and prostaglandins. Behav. Brain Funct. 2015;11:1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12993-015-0068-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Yong L, Thet Z, Zhu Y. Genetic editing of the androgen receptor contributes to impaired male courtship behavior in zebrafish. J. Exp. Biol. 2017;220:3017–3021. doi: 10.1242/jeb.161596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kalueff, A. V. et al. Towards a comprehensive catalog of zebrafish behavior 1.0 and beyond. Zebrafish10, 70–86 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 12.Kang J, Nachtrab G, Poss KD. Local Dkk1 crosstalk from breeding ornaments impedes regeneration of injured male zebrafish fins. Dev. Cell. 2013;27:19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.devcel.2013.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kimmel CB, Ballard WW, Kimmel SR, Ullmann B, Schilling TF. Stages of embryonic development of the zebrafish. Dev. Dyn. 1995;203:253–310. doi: 10.1002/aja.1002030302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Zempo, B., Yamamoto, Y., Williams, T. & Ono, F. Synaptic silencing of fast muscle is compensated by rewired innervation of slow muscle. Sci. Adv.6, eaax8382 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 15.Sakuma Y. Estradiol-sensitive projection neurons in the female rat preoptic area. Front. Neurosci. 2015;9:67. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Etgen AM, Chu HP, Fiber JM, Karkanias GB, Morales JM. Hormonal integration of neurochemical and sensory signals governing female reproductive behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 1999;105:93–103. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00085-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Newman HH. Spawning behavior and sexual dimorphism in Fundulus heteroclitus and allied fish. Biol. Bull. 1907;12:314–348. doi: 10.2307/1535681. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wilhelmi AE, Pickford GE, Sawyer WH. Initiation of the spawning reflex response in Fundulus by the administration of fish and mammalian neurohypophysial preparations and synthetic oxytocin. Endocrinology. 1955;57:243–252. doi: 10.1210/endo-57-2-243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Egami N. Preliminary note on the induction of the spawning reflex and oviposition in oryzias latipes by the administration of neurohypophyseal substances. Zool. Soc. Japan. 1959;32:13–17. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ono, Y. & Uematsu, T. Mating ethogram in Oryzias latipes. J. Fac. Sci. HOKKAIDO Univ. Ser. VI. Zool.13, 197–202 (1957).
- 21.Simon, V., Hyacinthe, C. & Rétaux, S. Breeding behavior in the blind Mexican cavefish and its river-dwelling conspecific. PLoS ONE14, e0212591 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 22.Juntti SA, Hilliard AT, Kent KR, Kumer A, Nguyen A, Jimenez MA, Loveland JL, Mourrain P, Fernald RD. A neural basis for control of cichlid female reproductive behavior by prostaglandin F2α. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:943–949. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.01.067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Esteve M. Observations of spawning behaviour in Salmoninae: Salmo, Oncorhynchus and Salvelinus. Rev. Fish Biol. Fish. 2005;15:1–21. doi: 10.1007/s11160-005-7434-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Uematsu, K. An analysis of sufficient stimuli for the oviposition in the medaka Oryzias latipes. J. Fac. Appl. Biol. Sci. Hiroshima Univ.29, 109–116 (1990).
- 25.Robinson EJ, Rugh R. The reproductive processes of the fish, Oryzias latipes. Biol. Bull. 1943;84:115–125. doi: 10.2307/1538054. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kujala GA. Corticosteroid and neurohypophyseal hormone control of parturition in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 1978;36:286–296. doi: 10.1016/0016-6480(78)90035-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bartley SM, Kippax JB, Taylor MH. Spontaneous ovarian contractions in Fundulus heteroclitus. J. Exp. Zool. 1992;264:219–224. doi: 10.1002/jez.1402640215. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bills ML, Callaway SB, Kessler AT, Taylor MH. Ovarian contraction in various teleost families. J. Fish Biol. 2008;72:671–680. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2007.01757.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Nambu M. Induction of oviposition by iajeotion or histamine serotonin in the fish, Oryzias latipes. Annot. Zool. Jpn. 1971;44:15–18. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.