Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2021 Oct 14;11:20423. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-99438-y

Interindividual differences in environmentally relevant positive trait affect impacts sustainable behavior in everyday life

Kimberly C Doell 1,, Beatrice Conte 1, Tobias Brosch 1
PMCID: PMC8516924  PMID: 34650092

Abstract

Emotions are powerful drivers of human behavior that may make people aware of the urgency to act to mitigate climate change and provide a motivational basis to engage in sustainable action. However, attempts to leverage emotions via climate communications have yielded unsatisfactory results, with many interventions failing to produce the desired behaviors. It is important to understand the underlying affective mechanisms when designing communications, rather than treating emotions as simple behavioral levers that directly impact behavior. Across two field experiments, we show that individual predispositions to experience positive emotions in an environmental context (trait affect) predict pro-environmental actions and corresponding shifts in affective states (towards personal as well as witnessed pro-environmental actions). Moreover, trait affect predicts the individual behavioral impact of positively valenced emotion-based intervention strategies from environmental messages. These findings have important implications for the targeted design of affect-based interventions aiming to promote sustainable behavior and may be of interest within other domains that utilize similar intervention strategies (e.g., within the health domain).

Subject terms: Psychology, Human behaviour

Introduction

Emotions exert a powerful influence on human behavior. They help us detect and understand risks and opportunities, signal that something that is important for our concerns and values is being threatened or supported, and they drive our actions by providing the necessary motivational momentum13. In the context of climate change and sustainable action, emotions can fulfill these functions by making people aware of the urgency to act to mitigate climate change and by providing a motivational basis to engage in sustainable action.

Accumulating research demonstrates how the experience of both emotions and affect influence the willingness to act to promote sustainability. Emotions are generally defined as adaptive reactions that are elicited when an event or object is appraised as relevant to one’s concerns, which often considerably impact subsequent decision-making and behavior; whereas affect is generally a more subtle positive/negative feeling experienced towards an event which can more subtly inform decisions and judgments1,3,4. Together affect and emotional responses are some of the strongest predictors when it comes to predicting a variety of climate change related judgements and behaviors (e.g., risk perceptions, willingness to act, etc.)46.

Overall, people who experience strong affective/emotional reactions toward climate change judge the related risks to be higher and are willing to alter their behavior to a larger extent711. Individual differences in the extent to which people report experiencing specific emotions such as worry, distress, interest, hope, or pride in the context of climate change have all been associated with the willingness to take up mitigation actions1215. Interestingly, such effects have not only been shown for emotions that people actually experience, but also for the emotions people expect to experience1,1619. The anticipation of positive affective reactions can directly motivate sustainable or pro-social behavior when the behavior is expected to be experienced as hedonically pleasurable or morally rewarding; a phenomenon known as “warm glow”20,21. Conversely, a person may avoid specific behaviors because they anticipate negative affective reactions; a phenomenon known as “a cold prickle”22. Anticipated affective reactions have been shown to be important predictors of a range of sustainable behaviors, including various transportation, recycling, and energy-saving behaviors15,2325. Thus, both experienced and anticipated emotions may operate as drivers of sustainable behavior.

Difficulties with leveraging emotions to promote sustainable behavior

Given their enormous potential, it is only logical to try to leverage affect and emotion in an attempt to promote sustainable actions in experimental set-ups and large-scale emotional climate communications. And indeed, messages aiming to induce emotions such as hope, guilt, or anger, have in some cases led to more sustainable intentions and behaviors12,26,27. However, emotion-eliciting messages and interventions have not always been effective, as other studies either failed to produce the desired results or even yielded opposing, unintended boomerang effects. For instance, messages designed to induce hope or general positive affective states have been found to increase willingness to act, to have no effect at all, to decrease climate change risk perception, and, in some cases, to even induce anger and resentment12,2831. Similarly, messages designed to induce fear of climate change have been found to increase willingness to act, to have no effect, and to reduce peoples’ perceived response efficacy3234. Thus, while the experience or the anticipation of “naturally occurring” state emotions or warm glow have been shown to relate to more sustainable behavior, inducing them does not necessarily lead to the same results. As argued previously35, emotions are not simple levers that one can pull in order to promote a desired behavior, and treating them as such often does not work or sometimes even leads to boomerang effects that reduce sustainable behavior36.

Individual differences concerning the elicitation of environmental emotions

We argue here that it is important to consider the inter-individual differences in the mechanisms underlying affect and emotions in the context of sustainable action. Not everyone will experience the same “amount” of emotion, or even the same discrete emotion when encountering a specific stimulus or situation. This tendency to experience such emotions in a predetermined manner (i.e., as a result of being exposed to specific types of stimuli) is called “trait affect”, and changes from individual to individual. One’s concerns, experiences, and values will impact the way that a stimulus is interpreted, and thus influence which emotions are elicited, and to what extent8,37,38. However, attempts to manipulate and induce emotions towards an issue or a behavior which are incompatible with a person’s concern structure may be ineffective or may even boomerang and produce reactance39.

It is thus important to consider individual predispositions to experience emotions, i.e., trait affect, in relation to a given topic or behavior. A recently developed measure of environmentally relevant positive trait affect (from here on referred to as “trait affect”) assesses an individual’s predisposition to experience positive emotions in situations with positive environmental outcomes8. Using items such as “I feel proud when I act in an environmentally friendly manner” and “I feel appreciation towards others when they act in an environmentally friendly manner”, this instrument measures to what extent an individual usually experiences positive affect from their own actions as well as when witnessing other people’s positive behaviors. It moreover reflects the anticipation of positive affect in an environmental context, as a person who in the past has often felt proud after showing pro-environmental behavior is more likely to expect this emotion in the future. Importantly, individual trait affect predicted the impact of an emotion induction on sustainable behaviors in a laboratory-based social dilemma task40: Guilt and pride inductions increased sustainable actions via reductions in consumption and increases in investments, respectively, but only in participants with high levels of trait affect.

Investigating the impact of affect on sustainable behavior in real life

While these results are promising, in order to better understand the dynamics of affect and emotions underlying sustainable behaviors, more research needs to be conducted via real-world field-based experiments. The majority of previous studies have utilized either lab-based methodologies, where individuals react to hypothetical situations, or survey-based approaches, where individuals retrospectively report about their past behavior. Although these experiments provide valuable insights, it is not entirely clear how their results map onto everyday occurrences of environmental behaviors and their emotional antecedents and consequences, as emotions are transient phenomena that change dynamically and continuously throughout the day41. Retrospective self-reports may moreover be influenced by memory biases driven by consistency or social desirability aspects which may potentially inflate correlations between measures of emotions and behavior42.

Here, across two large experiments, we use experience sampling, a field-based methodology where participants were asked to report various environmental actions and their subsequent affective state multiple times a day for several days. Here, we aimed to better understand how interindividual differences in (environmentally relevant) positive trait affect impact both sustainable behaviors, and subsequently experienced affective state. More specifically, in Experiment 1 we show that people with high levels of positive trait affect (i.e., people who tend to experience strong positive emotions in situations with positive environmental outcomes) commit more pro-environmental actions and report greater shifts in subsequent positive affective states. These affective shifts were observed both for pro-environmental actions committed personally and for pro-environmental actions that participants were exposed to by others (i.e., observed first-hand or learned about). These results provide first evidence that changes in affective states may also occur vicariously, when being exposed to the “good” actions of others. Based on these findings, in Experiment 2 we aimed to induce vicarious affective states via positive and negative environmental messages, in order to more causally determine how such stimuli impact environmental behavior as a function of positive trait affect. We observed that in people with high levels of positive trait affect, being exposed to positive environmental messages in the morning increased subsequently experienced positive emotions and resulted in the commission of more pro-environmental actions throughout the rest of the day. However, in a subset of people with low levels of positive trait affect the same exposure resulted in the commission of fewer pro-environmental actions throughout the rest of the day, providing evidence of a message-induced boomerang effect.

Results

Experiment 1: the impact of positive trait affect on environmental behavior and experienced affect in real life

We utilized a novel ecological experience sampling paradigm in which 181 participants (aged 18 to 76, mean = 33.5; 61% female) reported several times per day to what extent they had recently performed or were exposed to environmentally relevant behaviors (ERBs) as well as their current affective state. Participants received messages on their smartphone five times a day for ten days and were asked to report an environmental behavior that occurred within the last hour. They provided a brief description of the behavior and categorized it according to whether it was a committed positive ERB (i.e., with a positive impact on the environment), committed negative ERB, exposed to (i.e., seen, read or heard about) positive ERB, exposed to negative ERB, or nonERB (in case they did not experience an ERB during the relevant interval, they were asked to report a non-environmental behavior). Table 1 illustrates some of the behavioral descriptions provided by participants. Finally, participants reported the valence of their current affective state (from 0 = very negative to 10 = very positive). During participant intake, after providing informed consent, positive trait affect (from the positive outcome affect subscale of the Environmental Trait Affect Questionnaire8), value orientations (using a combination of the Schwartz and Steg Value Scales4345), Social Desirability Scale46, and demographic information were assessed.

Table 1.

Examples of verbatim participant responses by category.

Committed positive Committed negative Exposed to positive Exposed to negative NonERB
Experiment 1

I recycled my bottle

Bought a solar panel and inverter for my house

I took a quick shower

Used plastic bags for garbage

Burning wood

Used energy to watch tv

I saw some people driving Tesla

Watched an interview on tv about carbon credits

Saw somebody throw rubbish from their car

Watched a documentary about water pollution in China

I read a book

Sleeping

Made fun of someone behind his back

Experiment 2

Wash laundry with cold water

I replaced all the lighting in my house with LED bulbs

We had totally vegetarian meal

I took an extra long shower to relax

I tossed trash on the ground because there was no garbage can near by

I threw my cigarette out the car window

Listened to a speech about the green new deal

I read an article about a device in the ocean that will suck up garbage

Saw a sign at the gym about a recycling program

Trump says windmills cause cancer

My dad told me about the coral reefs being destroyed

I had seen an video on world’s most polluted river located in Indonesia

Helped my wife with the dishes

I told a joke to a friend

I danced with my 2 year old

In total, participants responded to 7,161 individual signals (mean response rate of 79%). In a first step, we analyzed to what extent individual differences in trait affect were related to the frequency with which participant committed environmental actions. To this end, we conducted two mixed-effects logistic regression analyses with trait affect as independent variables and positive and negative ERBs as dependent variables, respectively. Biospheric and egoistic value orientations as well as key demographic variables were added as covariates (see8). Results showed that trait affect was positively associated with an increased likelihood to commit positive ERBs (Fig. 1A; Table 2; OR = 1.21, CI = 1.06–1.38, p = 0.004; Tables S2, S3). None of the predictors of interest predicted likelihood to commit negative ERBs (Supplementary Table S3).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The impact of trait affect on environmental behavior and experienced affect in real life in Experiment 1. (A) Line graph illustrating the positive relationship between trait affect and likelihood to commit positive ERBs. (B) Interaction between trait affect and committed positive ERBs compared to NonERBs. (C) Interaction between trait affect and exposed to positive ERBs compared to NonERBs. Vertical dotted lines illustrate where slopes significantly differ from each other as determined by simple slopes analyses. All graphs show predicted values, the grand mean centered values of trait affect (positive outcome ETA) and 95% confidence intervals as estimated from their respective regression models. ERB = environmentally relevant behavior; NonERBs = behaviors reported that were not environmentally relevant.

Table 2.

Multilevel binomial logistic (with logit link) regression model predicting likelihood to commit positive environmental behaviors (i.e. positive ERBs) in Experiment 1.

Predictors Odds ratios 95% CI p
(Intercept) 0.53 0.47–0.60  < 0.001
Positive trait affect 1.21 1.06–1.38 0.004
Biospheric values 1.00 0.91–1.09 0.929
Egoistic values 1.08 0.96–1.22 0.190
Social desirability scale 1.04 1.00–1.07 0.048
Age 1.01 1.00–1.02 0.070
Gender 0.96 0.85–1.07 0.449
Time 0.99 0.99–1.00  < 0.001
Random effects
σ2 3.29
τ00 0.55Participant
τ11 0.00Participant. Time
ρ01 0.82Participant
ICC 0.15
N 180Participant
Observations 7136
Marginal R2 0.019
Conditional R2 0.164

For comparative purposes, a similar model without the covariates is shown in Supplementary Table S2.

ERBS Environmentally relevant behaviors.

We then analyzed to what extent committing or being exposed to environmental actions was related to participants’ affective state, and to what extent individual differences in positive trait affect moderated the relationship with the positive (committed and exposed to) behaviors. To this end, we conducted a linear mixed-effects model with the different ERB types and trait affect as independent variables and current affective state as the dependent variable (Table 3). To assess the ranges of the significance of the trait affect moderation, we conducted a simple slopes analysis using the Johnson–Neyman technique47. Results showed that compared to nonERBs, both committing and being exposed to positive ERBs resulted in a more positive affective state (committed positive ERB: b = 0.59, CI = 0.49–0.68, t(7002) = 12.2, p < 0.001; exposed to positive ERB: b = 0.52, CI = 0.35–0.69, t(6923) = 5.93, p < 0.001), while both committing and being exposed to negative ERBs resulted in a more negative affective state (committed negative ERB: b =  − 0.87, CI =  − 0.99 to − 0.75, t(6970) =  − 14.1, p < 0.001; exposed to negative ERB: b =  − 1.53, CI =  − 1.72 to − 1.33, t(6985) =  − 15.5, p < 0.001). Importantly, individual differences in positive trait affect moderated the effects of the positive behaviors: On the one hand, participants with high levels of trait affect reported positive affective shifts after reporting committing positive ERBs compared to reporting NonERBs (interaction: b = 0.35, CI = 0.26–0.44, t(6989) = 7.69, p < 0.001, Fig. 1B). Participants with low levels of trait affect, on the other hand, reported negative affective shifts after reporting committing positive ERBs compared to reporting NonERBs (as shown by the vertical dotted lines in Fig. 1B). In addition, after being exposed to a positive ERB (compared to NonERBs), participants with high levels of trait affect reported (statistically marginally) larger positive affective shifts (interaction: b = 0.19, CI = 0.00–0.38, p = 0.053, Fig. 1C).

Table 3.

Multilevel linear regression model predicting state affect in Experiment 1.

Predictor Standardized estimates 95% CI p
(Intercept) 6.55 6.36–6.74  < 0.001
Committed positive ERBs 0.59 0.49–0.68  < 0.001
Exposed to positive ERBs 0.52 0.35–0.69  < 0.001
Committed negative ERBs  − 0.87  − 0.99 to − 0.75  < 0.001
Exposed to negative ERBs  − 1.53  − 1.72 to − 1.33  < 0.001
Positive trait affect 0.24 0.04–0.43 0.019
Committed positive ERB × Positive trait affect 0.35 0.26–0.44  < 0.001
Exposed to positive ERB × Positive trait affect 0.19  − 0.00–0.38 0.054
Time  − 0.08  − 0.14 to − 0.02 0.008
Social desirability scale 0.07 0.02–0.13 0.011
Age 0.01  − 0.01–0.03 0.209
Gender  − 0.04  − 0.22–0.15 0.691
Random effects
σ2 2.52
τ00 1.42Participant
τ11 0.09Participant.time
ρ01 0.19Participant
ICC 0.37
N 180Participant
Observations 7130
Marginal R2 0.142
Conditional R2 0.463

For comparative purposes, a similar model without the covariates, and without the interactions, is shown in Supplementary Table S4.

ERBS Environmentally relevant behaviors.

Taken together, Experiment 1 showed that high levels of positive trait affect are associated with (i) a higher number of committed positive ERBs, and (ii) stronger shifts in positive affective state after committing positive ERBs as well as after being exposed to positive ERBs. Additionally, low levels of positive affect were associated with more negative affective shifts after committing positive ERBs. These findings emphasize the importance of interindividual differences in positive affect in the context of real-life sustainable behavior and provide initial evidence for vicariously experienced environmental affect as a potential driver of environmental behavior.

Experiment 2: trait affect and the impact of vicarious environmental affect on environmental behavior

Here we expanded on the role of vicariously induced affect by investigating whether communications about positive (as compared to neutral and negative) environmental actions can lead to an increase in the commission of positive ERBs, while considering to what extent individual differences in trait affect may constitute a boundary condition for this intervention effect. To this end, 331 participants (aged 18 to 64, mean = 36.2; 48% female) received an environmental message each morning. They were either exposed to news about environmentally positive events (e.g., successful “re-greenification” efforts which planted tens of millions of trees; N = 108), environmentally negative events (e.g., how the last several years have been the hottest in history, leading to hundreds of billions of dollars in damages; N = 108), or non-environmental events (e.g., study results concerning the eating habits of snakes; N = 115). To note, these stimuli were designed to mimic real news headlines/articles that people may likely encounter on a regular basis. After reading the news, participants were subsequently asked to rate the intensity of 4 positive emotions (pride, joy, hope, relief) and 4 negative emotions (anger, disgust, guilt, fear) experienced after reading the information. Participants were contacted three times throughout the rest of the day and asked to report to what extent they had recently performed or been exposed to ERBs, similar to Experiment 1. This protocol lasted for three days in total, resulting in responses to 2203 individual signals (mean response rate of 74%). Participants also completed the experimental intake, including providing informed consent, and completed the same set of questionnaires as in Experiment 1.

We first analyzed whether interindividual differences in trait affect influenced the extent to which participants experienced positive affective shifts after reading the environmental news events. We conducted a multiple linear regression with type of news event and trait affect as predictors and the averaged valence of the four experienced positive emotions as a dependent variable (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.91). Significant interaction effects indicated that participants with higher levels of trait affect experienced higher average levels of positive emotions when exposed to environmentally positive news, as well as lower levels of positive emotions when exposed to environmentally negative news (positive compared to non-environmental news interaction with trait affect: b = 6.10, CI = 0.05–0.40, t(323) = 2.49, p = 0.01; negative compared to non-environmental news interaction with trait affect: b =  − 5.88, − 0.41 to − 0.02, t(323) =  − 2.17, p = 0.031; positive compared to non-environmental news: b = 1.20, CI = 1.02–1.38, p < 0.001; negative compared to non-environmental news: b =  − 0.46, CI =  − 0.64 to − 0.29, p < 0.001; Supplementary Table S5).

We next analyzed to what extent exposure to the different types of environmental news had an impact on ERB commission throughout the day, and whether individual trait affect moderated this impact. To this end, we conducted a mixed-effects logistic regression analysis with type of news event and trait affect as predictors and frequency of committed positive ERBs as the dependent variable (Table 4). Significant interactions between news type and trait affect revealed that participants with high trait affect who read a communication about positive environmental news events were more likely to report committing a positive ERB compared to those that read negative environmental news or non-environmental news (see Fig. 2, comparison non-environmental versus positive news OR = 0.75, CI = 0.60–0.95, p = 0.01; comparison negative versus positive news: OR = 0.67, CI = 0.54–0.85, p < 0.001). A simple slopes analysis using the Johnson–Neyman technique allowed us to determine where the slopes of the different groups significantly differed from each other (dotted vertical lines shown in Fig. 2). Participants with high levels of trait affect who were exposed to positive environmental news in the morning had a higher likelihood to commit positive ERBs throughout the rest of the day (compared to participants exposed to negative or non-environmental news). However, participants with low levels of trait affect who were exposed to positive environmental news in the morning had a lower likelihood to commit positive ERBs (again, compared to participants exposed to negative or non-environmental news).

Table 4.

Multilevel binomial logistic regression (with logit link) results predicting likelihood to commit positive ERBs in Experiment 2.

Predictors Odds ratios CI p
(Intercept) 0.72 0.60–0.86  < 0.001
Non-environmental news group 0.90 0.71–1.16 0.429
Negative environmental news group 1.02 0.80–1.31 0.855
Positive trait affect 1.45 1.24–1.69  < 0.001
Non-environmental news group × positive trait affect 0.75 0.60–0.95 0.017
Negative environmental news group × positive trait affect 0.67 0.54–0.85 0.001
Age 1.01 1.00–1.02 0.066
Social desirability scale 1.03 1.00–1.06 0.022
Gender 0.88 0.79–0.97 0.013
Biospheric values 1.12 0.99–1.26 0.070
Egoistic values 1.02 0.91–1.15 0.739
Random effects
σ2 3.29
τ00 pcpID 0.17
ICC 0.05
NpcpID 328
Observations 2190
Marginal R2 0.040
Conditional R2 0.088

The grouping variables are effects coded such that the positive environmental news group represents the “baseline”. For comparative purposes, a similar model without the covariates, and without the interactions, is shown in Supplementary Table S4.

ERBS Environmentally relevant behaviors.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Trait affect, affective environmental news messages, and pro-environmental behaviors in Experiment 2. (A) Interaction between trait affect and positive versus non-environmental news messages on committed positive ERBs. Vertical dotted lines illustrate where slopes significantly differ from each other as determined by simple slopes analyses (it should be noted that the lower cutoff at − 3.11 represents the bottom 2.2% of all participants). (B) Interaction between trait affect and positive versus negative environmental news messages on committed positive ERBs. Vertical dotted lines again illustrate where slopes significantly differ from each other as determined by simple slopes analyses (it should be noted that the lower cutoff at − 0.72 represents the bottom 18.8% of all participants). All graphs show predicted values, grand mean centered values of trait affect, and 95% confidence intervals as estimated from their respective regression models. ERB = environmentally related behavior.

Discussion

Across two experiments we show that inter-individual differences in positive trait affect do not only influence the extent to which positive affect is experienced, but also influence the commission of sustainable behaviors in everyday life. These results may help explain why affective messages which aim to promote sustainability may not have the same effect on everyone12,2931,48. Across both experiments, people with high trait affect showed more pronounced positive affective shifts both after committing and after being exposed to positive environmental actions. At the behavioral level, high trait affect was moreover related to the commission of more pro-environmental behaviors in general (Exp. 1) and to committing more pro-environmental behaviors after being exposed to emotion-inducing communications about pro-environmental news items, even hours later (Exp. 2). Low trait affect, on the other hand, was related to decreased positive affective shifts after committing positive ERBs (Exp. 1) and, in a small percentage of participants, even resulted in the commission of fewer pro-environmental behaviors after exposure to positive environmental news. These results are consistent with previous findings12,31,39 which suggest that in specific participants, manipulations that are incompatible with a person’s affective concern structure may produce reactance, and here, even resulted in an affective boomerang effect that decreased sustainable behaviors overall.

These results have important implications for policy makers and communicators, not only in the climate change domain, but across other domains that utilize similar messaging strategies (e.g., health, financial, pollical, etc.). First, they provide evidence that suggests that message tailoring, a strategy that focuses on individual-level characteristics when designing intervention messages49, is important to induce a desired behavior change. Second, they point to affective predispositions (e.g., trait affect) as a central characteristic that should be the focus when designing such strategies. Third, utilizing messages that are positively valenced likely efficiently sidesteps multiple potential negative repercussions/concerns that have been raised over the use of negatively valenced messages (e.g., increasing depression/demoralization35, drawing from “finite pools of worry”50, etc.). Finally, they demonstrate that the tailoring of affective messages (and the evaluation of their outcomes) is possible, even with relatively simple and straightforward manipulations. Predispositions to experience positive emotions in association with pro-environmental behaviors (i.e., a trait “warm glow”) can be leveraged in order to promote sustainability. However, these interventions must take into account that they will not work for everyone, and even may boomerang for individuals with “incompatible” affective predispositions.

There are still multiple important questions that remain, however. For example, what are the long-term effects of such environmental communications that span over weeks or years? It is possible that repeatedly reminding participants via affective messaging may result in “numbness” or potentially alternate boomerang effects that we did not capture here. Additionally, we did not disentangle behaviors with high- and low-environmental impact. While we did capture both types of behavior inside of our datasets (see Table 1), it is possible that there are different affective mechanisms at play when it comes to promoting high- versus low-impact behavior (e.g.,51). Another potential limitation is that we cannot verify whether the behaviors reported were real (a relevant issue that is congruent with much of the self-report work in this domain). These points should be a focus for future investigation and considered when designing emotion-based interventions. Regardless, our findings provide real-world empirical support for the notion that emotional “one-size fits all”-styled communication strategies are not optimal for promoting pro-environmentalism35.

Our results moreover introduce the distinction between "direct warm glow” based on one’s own positive environmental actions, and a “vicarious warm glow” based on being exposed to others’ positive environmental actions. While Experiment 1 shows that both one’s own and others’ positive environmental actions can result in a positive affective shift, Experiment 2 provides evidence that the vicarious warm glow can be leveraged to promote pro-environmental behaviors, at least in individuals with high levels of trait affect. Humans are an innately social species for whom observational learning powerfully shapes behavior52. Previous research has shown that watching someone else obtain a reward can be experienced as rewarding in itself, especially if that person is perceived as being close and personally relevant53,54. This reward may motivate further pro-social behaviors4,54,55, which in turn result in more (vicariously induced) warm glow20, thus initiating a prosocial feedback loop. Thus, one of the most interesting implications of our results may suggest that such a vicarious response loop may extend to concerns of sustainability and sustainable behaviors. Indeed, previous work has shown that concerns and “objects of care” that are threatened by climate change are personally relevant and can produce strong emotional responses10,50. Consistent with this, here we illustrate for the first time the role of vicarious emotions as an antecedent to, and a consequence of, pro-environmental behavior.

This pattern of results is also consistent with other relevant theoretical and empirical frameworks from positive and environmental psychology, and the affective sciences. Given the benefits associated with natural environments (e.g., nature exposure increases well-being56, restores attention57, and conveys health benefits58), it is not surprising that people are not only connected to nature, but that they would feel a sense of accomplishment, and other positive emotions (e.g., pride) when acting to protect it. According to the positive affect hypothesis, these positive emotions are evolutionarily adaptive, protect us from a variety of mental disorders, and due to their rewarding/positive nature, lead to more life satisfaction and well-being59. This is also well aligned with the Broaden-and-Build theory60, which suggests that positive emotions expand people’s thought patterns thus allowing them to consider new and alternative ways of thinking and behaving. Thus, putting together these frameworks in the context of sustainability, positive emotions arise from personal achievements (e.g., behaving pro-environmentally), broadening the scope of behaviors that may lead to similar positive experiences in the future (i.e., increase anticipation of positive affect in the future), ultimately motivating the commission of further similar behaviors4,15,18,61. Adding our results to these frameworks, it may suggest that exposure to the good deeds of others results in positive affective shifts (i.e., vicariously induced warm glow), which may act to broaden the scope of pro-environmental behaviors and kick-start this virtuous cycle.

Taken together, our findings suggest that environmental emotions and environmental warm glow can be leveraged via interventions to promote pro-environmental actions. Participants with a predisposition to experience positive affect after pro-environmental actions are more likely to engage in them, and to experience more intense affective shifts afterwards, thus receiving an internal reward for an action that primarily benefits others. This reward in turn validates and reinforces expectations to feel good after future pro-environmental behaviors, triggering a positive feedback loop which may result in further pro-environmental behaviors in the future. Our results moreover suggest the intriguing possibility to trigger this feedback loop via vicarious warm glow elicited by other peoples’ pro-environmental actions One promising future research direction would be to develop strategies to increase to what extent positive emotions are experienced after committing or being exposed to pro-environmental actions. However, these strategies need to be adapted to the context and the target audience and to be empirically tested to ensure that no boomerang effects or other non-intended effects occur.

When interpreting the results of the experiments presented here, one needs to consider the potential impact of the experience sampling methodology as a potential driver of behavior change. One previous experiment utilizing experience sampling in participants who were attempting to quit smoking showed that repeatedly reporting one’s behavior can have a positive cathartic effect on mood and anxiety symptoms, and can ultimately lead to a reduction in cigarette cravings62. Similarly, the possibility exists that in our participants the experience sampling directly altered behavior (e.g., “I should ride my bike today so I can report it later and then feel good about it”) or increased awareness about what may not have otherwise been considered as an environmentally relevant behavior (see Table 1). It is however unlikely that these effects had a large confounding impact on the pattern of results related to the link between affect and sustainable action presented above. To go one step further, instead of conceptualizing this aspect as a limitation, it may be reframed as a potential intervention technique, with campaigns that utilize approaches which resemble the experience sampling methodology being an interesting tool to increase self-awareness and promote sustainable actions.

Affective responses play an important role in our reactions to climate change and, if leveraged correctly, may be an important motivating factor to promote sustainable action. By using a real-life experience sampling approach, we were able to gain insights into the interplay of trait affect, experienced affect, and sustainable actions that may not have been possible otherwise. This has yielded important insights about the potential of targeted affective intervention strategies to influence real-world behaviors. Our results help clarify how information campaigns that target positive emotions may have widespread effects on behavior, in both positive and negative directions.

Methods

Experiment 1

Participants for Experiment 1 were recruited online between October 2017 and April 2018 in two large-scale advertisement waves via various forums (Facebook groups, crowdsourcing and citizen science webpages, Reddit, and Amazon’s Mechanical Turk). It should be noted that because we used a self-selection convenience sampling procedure, our sample might not be completely representative. To be included in the study, participants were required to be at least 18 years old, have a personal smart phone with an active data plan, successfully answer 3 attention checks in the questionnaires, and respond to at least half (i.e. 25/50) of the experience sampling messages (see the supplementary materials for a detailed description of the data cleaning). During experiment intake, all participants completed the informed consent, provided demographic information (e.g. age, gender), and completed the questionnaires (i.e. Environmental Trait Affect Questionnaire8, value orientations (using a combination of the Schwartz and Steg Value Scales4345), and the Social Desirability Scale short form46. Finally, participants were required to complete a short training where they received seven examples of different types of environmental behaviors/non-environmental behaviors, and were asked to classify them as a “committed positive environmental behavior”, “committed negative environmental behavior”, “seen/read/heard about positive behavior”, “seen/read/heard about a negative behavior”, or “not environmentally relevant”.

Participants were offered a $10 compensation plus an additional $0.25 for each text message they responded to (maximum of $22.50 USD) in Amazon Online gift cards (or paid via Mechanical Turk). The experience sampling protocol utilized an SMS survey distribution approach, wherein the participants received a hyperlink to a short survey on Qualtrics (Qualtrics.com), via text message, directly to their personal smartphone. Participants were signaled 5 times per day for 10 days randomly between the hours of 9 am and 10 pm. If they did not respond, they were sent a reminder message within 15 min, and after 1 h the signal expired. Participants were first asked to classify their ERB, then give a brief description, and finally rate their current affective state (i.e., mood) on a 11-point scale from “very negative” to “very positive”. Here we report a brief account of the analyses, more detail can be found in the supplementary materials, and in the open-sourced scripts and data which can be found at osf.io/7kmp8. This research was approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences of the University of Geneva, Switzerland and all research was performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

We first analyzed to what extent trait affect differences were related to the frequency with which participants commit positive ERBs. We conducted a mixed effects logistic regression (with logit link; implemented in R with the lme4 package63), which predicts responses to individual messages (Level 1) nested within participants (Level 2), and therefore models between-trial dependencies within participants. All between-participant predictors (including trait affect, biospheric and egoistic value orientations, social desirability, age, time, and gender) were mean-centered across participants so that fixed-effects coefficients could be interpreted relative to the relevant means. Positive ERBs (i.e. the dependent variable) was dummy coded as 1 for the relevant behavior or 0 otherwise (i.e. baseline). Random effects included random intercepts at the participant level and time (i.e., a variable from 1 to 50 according to signal number, which was centered around zero to allow for model convergence) was included as a random slope.

Next, we analyzed to what extent committing or being exposed to ERBs was related to participants’ current affective state, and whether individual differences in positive affect moderated this relationship. To this end, we conducted a linear mixed-effects model, where the fixed effects included each type of ERB reported (committed positive, committed negative, exposed-to positive, exposed-to negative; each predictor was effects-coded with nonERB set to the baseline), positive trait affect, age, gender, social desirability, and time (all centered as in the previous models). Random effects included random intercepts at the participant level and time as a random slope. To test the boundaries of the interactions between trait affect and positive ERBs (i.e. for both committed and exposed to positive ERBs), we additionally conducted a Johnson–Neyman simple slopes analysis (with alpha = 0.05) using the interactions package in R (see Fig. 1B,C).

Experiment 2

Participants for Experiment 2 were recruited online between March 2018 to May 2019 from Mechanical Turk. Similar to Experiment 1, in order to be included in the study, participants were required to be at least 18 years old, have a personal smart phone with an active data plan, successfully answer 3 attention checks in the questionnaires, and respond to at least half (i.e. 6/12) of the experience sampling signals. Participants were offered a $2 compensation plus an additional $0.50 for each message they responded to (totaling $16). Unlike Experiment 1, the experience sampling protocol utilized a cellphone application survey distribution approach (via expiwell.com), wherein participants received a notification on their phone and directly completed the survey inside the application. Once they had successfully completed the intake survey (where they provided informed consent) and training, participants were randomly assigned to one of three experimental groups where they would receive the intervention message (i.e., the positive news, negative news, or non-environmental news) each morning randomly between 8 am and noon. Next, they were asked to rate the intensity to which they felt each of 8 different emotions (on a 100-point slider) including pride, joy, anger, disgust, guilt, fear, hope, and relief. The 4 positive emotions were averaged to create one composite score (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.91). Three times throughout the rest of the day, participants were asked to report an ERB that occurred within the last 1 h, using the same protocol as in Experiment 1. This research was approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences of the University of Geneva, Switzerland and all research was performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

We first analyzed to what extent trait affect related to changes in positive affect after reading each intervention message. We then conducted a multiple linear regression model where the average positive emotion score was the dependent variable and event type (i.e., group) and trait positive affect were included as independent variables. Regression results are shown in Supplementary Table S5.

Next, we analyzed to what extent being exposed to different types of environmentally relevant information each morning interacted with trait affect to influence pro-environmental behavior throughout the rest of the day. Similar to Experiment 1, we conducted a mixed effects logistic regression analysis, which included positive ERB commission as an effects coded dependent variable, as well as type of intervention message and trait positive affect (centered across participants, alongside the other control variables) as independent variables. Random effects included random intercepts at the participant level and time as a random slope. To test the boundaries of the interactions between group and type of message, we additionally conducted a Johnson–Neyman simple slopes analysis (with alpha = 0.05) using the interactions package in R (see Fig. 2).

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information. (212.7KB, docx)

Author contributions

K.D. and T.B. developed the study concept and designed the experiments with input from B.C.; K.D. programmed the experiments and collected the data; K.D. analyzed the data, interpreted the results, wrote and edited the manuscript with constructive feedback provided by B.C. and T.B.

Funding

Funding was provided by Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant No. PYAPP1_160571).

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-99438-y.

References

  • 1.Brosch T, Scherer KR, Grandjean D, Sander D. The impact of emotion on perception, attention, memory, and decision-making. Swiss Med. Wkly. 2013;143:1–10. doi: 10.4414/smw.2013.13786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Frijda NH. The laws of emotion. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lerner JS, Li Y, Valdesolo P, Kassam KS. Emotion and decision making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015;66:799–823. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Brosch T. Affect and emotions as drivers of climate change perception and action: A review. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021;42:15–21. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.van der Linden S. The social-psychological determinants of climate change risk perceptions: Towards a comprehensive model. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015;41:112–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.11.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Xie B, Brewer MB, Hayes BK, McDonald RI, Newell BR. Predicting climate change risk perception and willingness to act. J. Environ. Psychol. 2019;65:101331. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101331. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brosch T, Patel MK, Sander D. Affective influences on energy-related decisions and behaviors. Front. Energy Res. 2014;2:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fenrg.2014.00011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hahnel UJJ, Brosch T. Environmental trait affect. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018;59:94–106. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.08.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Van Der Linden S. Intrinsic motivation and pro-environmental behaviour. Nat. Publ. Gr. 2015;5:612–613. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wang S, Leviston Z, Hurlstone M, Lawrence C, Walker I. Emotions predict policy support: Why it matters how people feel about climate change. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2018;50:25–40. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2018.03.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bolderdijk JW, Steg L, Geller ES, Lehman PK, Postmes T. Comparing the effectiveness of monetary versus moral motives in environmental campaigning. Nat. Clim. Change. 2013;3:413–416. doi: 10.1038/nclimate1767. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hornsey MJ, Fielding KS. A cautionary note about messages of hope: Focusing on progress in reducing carbon emissions weakens mitigation motivation. Glob. Environ. Change. 2016;39:26–34. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2016.04.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Smith N, Leiserowitz A. The role of emotion in global warming policy support and opposition. Risk Anal. 2014;34:937–948. doi: 10.1111/risa.12140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bouman T, et al. When worry about climate change leads to climate action: How values, worry and personal responsibility relate to various climate actions. Glob. Environ. Change. 2020;62:102061. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2020.102061. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bissing-Olson MJ, Fielding KS, Iyer A. Experiences of pride, not guilt, predict pro-environmental behavior when pro-environmental descriptive norms are more positive. J. Environ. Psychol. 2016;45:145–153. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Zeelenberg M, Nelissen RMA, Breugelmans SM, Pieters R. On emotion specificity in decision making: Why feeling is for doing. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2008;3:18–27. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Venhoeven LA, Bolderdijk JW, Steg L. Why acting environmentally-friendly feels good: Exploring the role of self-image. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:1990–1991. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hartmann P, Eisend M, Apaolaza V, D’Souza C. Warm glow vs. altruistic values: How important is intrinsic emotional reward in proenvironmental behavior? J. Environ. Psychol. 2017;52:43–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.05.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Schneider CR, Zaval L, Weber EU, Markowitz EM. The influence of anticipated pride and guilt on pro-environmental decision making. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0188781. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Andreoni J. Impure altruism and donations to public goods: A theory of warm-glow giving. Econ. J. 1990;100:464. doi: 10.2307/2234133. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Jia L, Van Der Linden S. Green but not altruistic warm-glow predicts conservation behavior. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 2020 doi: 10.1111/csp2.211. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Andreoni J. Warm-glow versus cold-prickle: The effects of positive and negative framing on cooperation in experiments. Q. J. Econ. 1995;110:1–21. doi: 10.2307/2118508. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Steg L. Car use: Lust and must. Instrumental, symbolic and affective motives for car use. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 2005;39:147–162. doi: 10.1016/j.tra.2004.07.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Taufik D, Bolderdijk JW, Steg L. Going green? The relative importance of feelings over calculation in driving environmental intent in the Netherlands and the United States. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 2016;22:52–62. doi: 10.1016/j.erss.2016.08.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kraft P, Rise J, Sutton S, Røysamb E. Perceived difficulty in the theory of planned behaviour: Perceived behavioural control or affective attitude? Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2005;44:479–496. doi: 10.1348/014466604X17533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Harth NS, Leach CW, Kessler T. Guilt, anger, and pride about in-group environmental behaviour: Different emotions predict distinct intentions. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013;34:18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.12.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Rees JH, Klug S, Bamberg S. Guilty conscience: motivating pro-environmental behavior by inducing negative moral emotions. Clim. Change. 2015;130:439–452. doi: 10.1007/s10584-014-1278-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Farrow K, Grolleau G, Ibanez L. Social norms and pro-environmental behavior: A review of the evidence. Ecol. Econ. 2017;140:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2017.04.017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Chatelain G, et al. Feel good, stay green: Positive affect promotes pro-environmental behaviors and mitigates compensatory “mental bookkeeping” effects. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018;56:3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.02.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lange F, Dewitte S. Positive affect and pro-environmental behavior: A preregistered experiment. J. Econ. Psychol. 2020;80:102291. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2020.102291. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Myers TA, Nisbet MC, Maibach EW, Leiserowitz AA. A public health frame arouses hopeful emotions about climate change: A Letter. Clim. Change. 2012;113:1105–1112. doi: 10.1007/s10584-012-0513-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ferguson MA, Branscombe NR. Collective guilt mediates the effect of beliefs about global warming on willingness to engage in mitigation behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010;30:135–142. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.11.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.O’Neill S, Nicholson-Cole S. ‘fear won’t do it’: Promoting positive engagement with climate change through visual and iconic representations. Sci. Commun. 2009;30:355–379. doi: 10.1177/1075547008329201. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.So J, Kuang K, Cho H. Reexamining fear appeal models from cognitive appraisal theory and functional emotion theory perspectives. Commun. Monogr. 2016;83:120–144. doi: 10.1080/03637751.2015.1044257. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Chapman DA, Lickel B, Markowitz EM. Reassessing emotion in climate change communication. Nat. Clim. Change. 2017;7:850–852. doi: 10.1038/s41558-017-0021-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hart PS, Nisbet EC. Boomerang effects in science communication: How motivated reasoning and identity cues amplify opinion polarization about climate mitigation policies. Communic. Res. 2012;39:701–723. doi: 10.1177/0093650211416646. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Brosch T, Sander D. Comment: The appraising brain: Towards a neuro-cognitive model of appraisal processes in emotion. Emot. Rev. 2013;5:163–168. doi: 10.1177/1754073912468298. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hornsey MJ, Fielding KS. Understanding (and reducing) inaction on climate change. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2020;14:3–35. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12058. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bessarabova E, Turner MM, Fink EL, Blustein NB. Extending the theory of reactance to guilt appeals: ‘You ain’t guiltin’’ me into nothin"’. J. Psychol. 2015;223:215–224. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Tarditi C, Hahnel UJJ, Jeanmonod N, Sander D, Brosch T. Affective dilemmas: The impact of trait affect and state emotion on sustainable consumption decisions in a social dilemma task. Environ. Behav. 2018;52(1):33–59. doi: 10.1177/0013916518787590. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hollenstein T. This time, its real: Affective flexibility, time scales, feedback loops, and the regulation of emotion. Emot. Rev. 2015;7:308–315. doi: 10.1177/1754073915590621. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Trull TJ, Ebner-Priemer U. The role of ambulatory assessment in psychological science. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2014;23:466–470. doi: 10.1177/0963721414550706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Schwartz, S. H. & Littrell, R. Draft Users Manual: Proper Use of the Schwarz Value Survey. Compiled by Romie F. Littrell. (2009).
  • 44.Schwartz SH. Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1992;25:1–65. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60281-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.de Groot JIM, Steg L. Value orientations and environmental beliefs in five countries: Validity of an instrument to measure egoistic, altruistic and biospheric value orientations. J. Cross. Cult. Psychol. 2007;38:318–332. doi: 10.1177/0022022107300278. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Reynolds WM. Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1982;38:119–126. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198201)38:1&#x0003c;119::AID-JCLP2270380118&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-I. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Potthoff RF. On the Johnson-Neyman technique and some extensions thereof. Psychometrika. 1964;29:241–256. doi: 10.1007/BF02289721. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ibanez L, Moureau N, Roussel S. How do incidental emotions impact pro-environmental behavior? Evidence from the dictator game. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 2017;66:150–155. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2016.04.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Noar SM, Harrington NG, Aldrich RS. The role of message tailoring in the development of persuasive health communication messages. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 2009;33:73–133. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Weber EU. Experience-based and description-based perceptions of long-term risk: Why global warming does not scare us (yet) Clim. Change. 2006;77:103–120. doi: 10.1007/s10584-006-9060-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Van Der Linden S. Warm glow is associated with low-but not high-cost sustainable behaviour. Nat. Sustain. 2018;1:28–30. doi: 10.1038/s41893-017-0001-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Bandura A. Social foundations of thought and action. Health Psychol. Reader. 2012 doi: 10.4135/9781446221129.n6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hackel LM, Zaki J, Van Bavel JJ. Social identity shapes social valuation: Evidence from prosocial behavior and vicarious reward. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017;12:1219–1228. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Mobbs D, et al. A key role for similarity in vicarious reward. Science. 2009;324:900. doi: 10.1126/science.1170539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Fehr E, Fischbacher U. The nature of human altruism. Nature. 2003;425:785–791. doi: 10.1038/nature02043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Dolliver K, Mayer FS, Frantz CM, Bruehlman-Senecal E. Why is nature beneficial? The role of connectedness to nature. Environ. Behav. 2009;41:607–643. doi: 10.1177/0013916508319745. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ohly H, et al. Attention restoration theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health Part B. 2016;19:305–343. doi: 10.1080/10937404.2016.1196155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Martin L, et al. Nature contact, nature connectedness and associations with health, wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviours. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020;68:101389. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101389. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Wood AM, Froh JJ, Geraghty AWA. Gratitude and well-being: A review and theoretical integration. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010;30:890–905. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Fredrickson BL. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 2001;56:218. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Carter DM. Recognizing the role of positive emotions in fostering environmentally responsible behaviors. Ecopsychology. 2011;3:65–69. doi: 10.1089/eco.2010.0071. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Mccarthy DE, Minami H, Yeh VM, Bold KW. An experimental investigation of reactivity to ecological momentary assessment frequency among adults trying to quit smoking. Addiction. 2015;110:1549–1560. doi: 10.1111/add.12996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Bates DE, Mächler H, Bolker VM, Walker KW. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2014;67(1):1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information. (212.7KB, docx)

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES