Abstract
Social cognition and metacognition are frequently impaired in schizophrenia, and these impairments complicate recovery. Recent work suggests that different aspects of metacognition may not be impaired to the same degree. Furthermore, metacognition and the cognitive capacity being monitored need not be similarly impaired. Here, we assessed performance in detecting cues of intentional behaviour as well as metacognition about detecting those cues in schizophrenia. Thirty patients and controls categorized animations of moving dots into those displaying a dyadic interaction demonstrating a chase or no chase and indicated their confidence in these judgments. Perception and metacognition were assessed using signal detection theoretic measures, which were analysed using frequentist and Bayesian statistics. Patients showed a deficit compared to controls in detecting intentionality cues, but showed preserved metacognitive performance into this task. Our study reveals a selective deficit in the perception of intentionality cues, but preserved metacognitive insight into the validity of this perception. It thus appears that impairment of metacognition in schizophrenia varies across cognitive domains - metacognition should not be considered a monolithic stone that is either impaired or unimpaired.
Keywords: Social cognition, Social perception, meta-d’, Experimental task, Self-monitoring
Highlights
-
•
Impaired detection of chasing between two moving objects in schizophrenia
-
•
No impairment in metacognition during this task in schizophrenia
-
•
Preserved metacognition into social perception despite impaired social perception
1. Introduction
Schizophrenia is one of the most disabling psychiatric diseases (Mathers et al., 2008). The poor functioning does not only depend on the characteristic psychotic symptoms, but also on deficits in cognitive functions (Arnon-Ribenfeld et al., 2017; Fett et al., 2011; Green, 1996). One type of cognitive function particularly important for understanding daily functioning in people with schizophrenia is social cognition (Fett et al., 2011; Green et al., 2008; Green and Leitman, 2008), which has been found to be impaired in numerous studies (Green et al., 2015; Savla et al., 2013). Social cognition can be studied for example by assessing the perception of social interactions presented in visual displays: Abstract, interacting moving objects can evoke complex human-like behaviours related to intentions such as chasing each other, wanting something or courting each other (Abell et al., 2000; Bassili, 1976; Blythe et al., 1999; Heider and Simmel, 1944; Santos et al., 2008). Perceiving such displays is associated with increased activity in a network of brain regions involved in social cognition (Blakemore et al., 2003; Castelli et al., 2000; Santos et al., 2010; Schultz et al., 2005). Patients with schizophrenia show impairments in correctly attributing intentions to interacting moving objects in different paradigms (Horan et al., 2009b; Langdon et al., 2017).
Functional outcomes in schizophrenia also crucially depend on the integrity of metacognitive awareness of one's own cognitive capacities (Farrer and Franck, 2007; Frith and Done, 1988; Lysaker et al., 2013, Lysaker et al., 2011; Stephan et al., 2009). A growing number of studies investigated metacognition of perceptual processes. While a recent review has reported small-to-medium sized deficits in metacognition of perception (Rouy et al., 2021), several examples of preserved metacognition of perception exist. For example, one study reported impaired conscious yet preserved unconscious performance monitoring in a low-level visual perception task in schizophrenia (Charles et al., 2017). Another study reported that metacognitive performance monitoring was unrelated to psychosis when perceptual sensitivity was accounted for (Powers et al., 2017). Furthermore, self-reported schizotypy in healthy participants was found to contribute little to variations in metacognitive performance (Rouault et al., 2018). Preserved metacognitive abilities may allow to mitigate the impact of impaired cognitive skills through compensatory strategies (Arnon-Ribenfeld et al., 2017; Davies and Greenwood, 2018; Hasson-Ohayon et al., 2018; Koren et al., 2006; Lysaker et al., 2019, Lysaker et al., 2013, Lysaker et al., 2011). Therefore, a more differentiated investigation may be helpful for understanding self-monitoring deficits in schizophrenia.
Measuring metacognition is difficult as metacognition is not an explicit behaviour. One reliable and bias-free assessment method called meta-d’ (Maniscalco and Lau, 2014, Maniscalco and Lau, 2012) is based on Signal Detection Theory (Green and Swets, 1966; Macmillan and Creelman, 1991). It consists in the combined assessment of discrimination between stimulus alternatives (perceptual performance) and reports of confidence in the discrimination responses. Accurate self-monitoring about perceptual judgments is reflected in higher confidence in true than in false percepts (metacognitive performance) (Fleming and Lau, 2014; Maniscalco and Lau, 2014, Maniscalco and Lau, 2012). In addition, the meta-d’ method allows to measure metacognitive efficiency, a measure of self-monitoring adjusted for differences in perceptual performance (Maniscalco and Lau, 2012). This is particularly useful for distinguishing between cognitive and metacognitive skills in schizophrenia, where cognitive impairments are well known. In fact, the above-mentioned review reported inconclusive metacognitive deficits about perception in schizophrenia when controlling for perceptual performance (Rouy et al., 2021). Another aspect to consider is that metacognitive sensitivity is often higher for percepts about displays in which a target stimulus is present than for percepts about displays without stimulus (Fleming et al., 2010; Kanai et al., 2010). This may be due to the fact that more sensory evidence can be accumulated when reporting the presence rather than the absence of a target. For this reason, a variation of meta-d’ evaluates response-specific metacognitive sensitivity (Maniscalco and Lau, 2014).
To investigate metacognition in social perception, we asked schizophrenia patients and healthy controls to report perceived chasing between two interacting dots. This paradigm has revealed an association between perceived chasing and attribution of animacy in healthy individuals (Schultz et al., 2005) and a deficit in the perception of chasing in participants with autism (David et al., 2014). In addition, participants reported their confidence in their responses. We analysed the data using signal detection measures of performance and metacognition. Based on previous findings, we hypothesized that patients would perform worse than control persons when asked to judge the presence of chasing. Given mixed findings about perceptual metacognition, we were curious to assess metacognitive performance in this task.
2. Methods
Thirty patients (age [mean ± SD] 33.6 ± 10.3 y, 8 females) satisfying DSM-V criteria for schizophrenia (DSM-V 295.90) as determined by consultation with treating psychiatrists and medical records and 30 healthy control subjects (age 34.1 ± 10.9 y, 8 females) individually matched for sex and age (±2 y), participated in this study. Participants’ age ranged from 19 to 60 years and were recruited for the present and another experiment, reported in a previous publication (Muthesius et al., 2020). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were able to speak German sufficiently well to provide written informed consent, and to understand and follow the task instructions. The number of participants was determined by a power analysis optimised for the experiment reported in Muthesius et al. (2020). For this analysis, we assumed an effect size of 0.65 based on a prior study first reporting the task used (Schultz et al., 2019), an alpha error of P = 0.05 and a power of 80%. One patient did not complete the task and dropped out of the study. Patients were in- and outpatients recruited from the Department of Psychiatry at the University Hospital Cologne. Control participants were healthy volunteers recruited among the general population and showed no signs of pre-existent or current psychiatric conditions, as assessed in a clinical interview by a psychiatrist. Details of the sample are provided in Table 1. The study was carried out in compliance with the latest revision of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent. Ethical approval was provided by the University of Cologne Ethics Committee (18–265).
Table 1.
Measure | Patients | Controls |
---|---|---|
Age (y) | 33.6 ± 10.3 | 34.1 ± 10.9 |
Sex (N females; N males) | 8; 22 | 8; 22 |
Disease duration (mo) | 106.9 ± 90.9 | N.A. |
Medication (mg chlorpromazine/day equivalent) | 678.8 ± 416.1 | N.A. |
PANSS total score | 50.6 ± 11.8 | N.A. |
PANSS positive symptoms subscale | 11.73 ± 3.62 | N.A. |
PANSS negative symptoms subscale | 14.13 ± 5.87 | N.A. |
PANSS general symptoms subscale | 24.76 ± 5.55 | N.A. |
CDSS | 0.07 ± 0.25 | N.A. |
CGI | 4.07 ± 0.98 | N.A. |
SOFAS | 56.53 ± 13.81 | N.A. |
Note: CDSS, Calgary Depression Rating Scale for Schizophrenia; CGI, Clinical Global Impression-scale; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale for Schizophrenia; SOFAS, Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale; N.A.: not applicable. Mean ± SD are shown except if otherwise stated.
Patients’ illness duration varied between 0 (first-episode psychosis) and 25 years. Mean disease duration was 8.9 ± 7.6 (median: 6.5) years and widely varied in the sample: Fifteen patients ranged between 0 and 6 years, eight from 6 to 16 years, and seven from 16 to 25 years. Patients were excluded if diagnosed with any comorbid axis-I-disorder, significant medical illness including any past or present neurological disorder or acute substance intoxication, or if there was a risk of acute suicidality measured by item 8 of the Calgary Depression Rating Scale for Schizophrenia (CDSS) (Addington et al., 1993). We did not assess IQ but patients with an explicit diagnosis of mental retardation or learning disabilities were excluded. None of the participants were hospitalized coercively by order of the responsible local authorities. Patients’ symptoms severity were assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale for Schizophrenia (PANSS) (Kay et al., 1987). All patients were under antipsychotic medication determined by the treating psychiatrist. Fourteen patients were treated with one, fourteen patients with two, and two patients with three antipsychotics simultaneously. Additionally, seven patients were treated with antidepressants, four with antiepileptics prescribed for anxiolysis and mood stabilization, and one with low potency neuroleptics. Chlorpromazine equivalent dosage of antipsychotic medication (Table 1) was calculated according to published conversion tables (Gardner et al., 2010; Leucht et al., 2014).
2.1. Experimental task
Participants performed a modified version of a social perception task (Schultz et al., 2005) and reported their confidence in their responses. In a two-alternative forced choice, participants reported whether two moving dots (one coloured red, the other blue; see Fig. 1A) on a visual display chased each other or not. There were four types of trials in this 2 × 2 experimental design, resulting from the combination of two factors: interactive vs. control trials, and low or high level of cross-correlation between dot movements. The four types of trials contained different amounts of sensory evidence of chasing, as shown in Fig. 1B. In interactive trials, the red dot followed the blue dot, and the dots thus chased each other. Control trials were based on the interactive trials, but modified such as to disrupt the chasing (the movements of the red dot were unchanged, but the movements of the blue dot were reversed in time and space compared to the interactive trials; see Schultz et al., 2005). Participants’ task was to discriminate interactive from control trials; their responses were considered correct if they responded “chasing” in interactive trials and if they responded “no chasing” in control trials.
The level of cross-correlation is a parameter in a movement equation (detailed below) and determined the degree of influence that the dots had on each other's position: with a high cross-correlation level, the dots chased each other more and also moved more than at the low cross-correlation level. In interactive trials with high cross-correlation, the dots really chased each other more than in interactive trials with low cross-correlation, because the degree of influence that the dots had on each other's position was higher (see movement equation below). In control trials, the actual chasing was disrupted; however, randomly occurring similarities in the dot trajectories still occurred by chance. These randomly occurring trajectory similarities also influenced the percept of chasing, and were more likely to occur at the high cross-correlation level (for graded reports of perceived chasing at different cross-correlation levels, see Schultz et al., 2005). As a result, participants perceived more chasing in trials with high than with the low cross-correlation levels, in both control and interactive trials. In signal detection terms, observers would thus show a bias towards reporting chasing at the high cross-correlation level.
Our four trial types thus allowed us to assess and compare both discrimination capacity and perceptual bias in patients and control participants. A similar perceptual bias in patients and controls would indicate that patients processed the stimuli similarly to the controls and performed the task as desired. There were ten trials per trial type for a total of forty trials, presented in a randomized order. Each trial started with an animation sequence (4.3 s) followed by participants’ chasing judgment and confidence rating [evaluated on a scale between 1 (=not confident at all) and 4 (=absolutely confident); self-paced].
The movement equation specified a time-series of positions for each object, where the new position of each object was determined by the previous position of both using a multivariate autoregressive process. The algorithm used the following equation to update the position of the dots [x(t+∆t)] at each timepoint:
Here, x(t) are the coordinates of both objects, J is the system's Jacobian controlling the dependencies:
Δt is the time step between two successive positions, β = {1/7, 1/10, 1/2, 2/3} and ω = {1/100, 1/200, 1/50, 1/40} are terms driving the dot movements, W is a constant that scales the random term ∈(t) ~ N(0,Δt), and ρ is the cross-correlation parameter. Critically, the cross-correlation parameter determines the influence of each dot's previous position on the other dot's position: higher values make the red dot move closer to the blue dot and the blue dot move further away from the red dot; this therefore increases interactions between the dots. However, higher values of the cross-correlation parameter also increased the speed of both dots, leading to more motion (for a detailed analysis of the changes in dot speed and separation as a function of changes in cross-correlation level, see Schultz et al., 2005).
2.2. Measurements
Perceptual performance was assessed through the signal detection theory measure d’, which quantified discrimination between trials with chasing and trials without chasing (higher values indicate better discrimination). Perceptual bias was assessed using the measure criterion, or bias, c’. Because a higher value of the cross-correlation parameter made the dots appear to chase each other more (in both the interactive and the control trials, leading to false positive chasing detection), we expected participants to tend to report chasing more frequently at the high cross-correlation level than at the low cross-correlation level, leading to a change in c’ between cross-correlation levels (lower values indicate a tendency to report chasing). The equations used to compute d’ and c’ are reported in the Supplementary materials.
We calculated three measures of metacognitive performance (Maniscalco and Lau, 2014, Maniscalco and Lau, 2012) using the “meta-d’” toolbox (http://www.columbia.edu/~bsm2105/type2sdt/) (Maniscalco and Lau, 2012) implemented in Matlab (MATLAB R2021a, The Mathworks Inc., Natick, USA), with default settings. The first measure is the metacognitive sensitivity meta-d’, a response-bias free measure of how well a participant distinguishes between their correct and incorrect judgments. A participant who gives higher confidence ratings after correct judgments and lower confidence ratings after incorrect judgments has high metacognitive sensitivity. The second measure is the relative metacognitive sensitivity meta-d’/d’, also known as MRatio or metacognitive efficiency, which measures a participant's metacognitive sensitivity given a certain level of task performance. The last measure is the response-specific metacognitive efficiency rs-meta-d’/d’, which involves calculating rs-meta-d’ separately for “yes” and “no” answers (Maniscalco and Lau, 2014). These measures were all estimated for each stimulus cross-correlation level and each participant using the maximum likelihood method, or a simpler sum of squares method in case the maximum likelihood fit did not converge to a solution.
2.3. Statistical analysis
We performed two types of analyses. First, we analysed the dependent variables obtained in each trial of the task performed by our participants: chasing response (response to the task question “did the red dot chase the blue dot?”), response accuracy, confidence rating, and response time. These data were analysed using linear mixed regression models and linear mixed-effects logistic regression models, using Matlab 2021a using the function fitglme.m. All models contained an intercept, fixed effects (dummy variables coding the cross-correlation level and participant group, and for analyses of confidence and response time data, a dummy variable coding whether the response given was correct or not) and random effects (subject number). Models with interaction terms were compared to models without interaction terms using likelihood ratio tests implemented in Matlab's function compare.m. Details of the analyses including the formulas used as well as detailed results are provided in the Supplementary materials.
Second, signal detection theory-based dependent variables were assessed using frequentist and Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVAs implemented in the software JASP, version 0.14.1 (www.jasp-stats.org), using default settings. Reported Bayes Factors (BF10) are odds in favour of the alternative hypothesis, i.e., ratios of the likelihood of the alternative hypothesis (=there is a difference between conditions) to the likelihood of the null hypothesis (=there is no difference between conditions), and are interpreted according to the currently recommended heuristic (Ly et al., 2016) based on Jeffrey's rule (Jeffreys, 1961).
As meta-d’ and derived values are notoriously difficult to estimate from single-participant data with relatively low trial numbers, differences between participant groups can be directly estimated using a hierarchical Bayesian approach (Fleming, 2017). This approach enhances statistical power, incorporates uncertainty in group-level parameter estimates and avoids edge-correction confounds. We used the HMeta-d’ toolbox (https://github.com/metacoglab/HMeta-d) to estimate differences in meta-d’/d’ between patients and controls. This toolbox represents all participants’ data in a hierarchical graphical model, and uses a Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) algorithm to estimate the joint posterior distribution of the model parameters, given the model specification and the data. In accordance with HMeta-d’ toolbox recommendations, early samples of the posterior distributions were discarded and three chains were run in order to diagnose convergence problems. Differences between participant groups were considered significant if the 95% highest-density intervals (HDI; the intervals containing 95% of the MCMC samples) of the posterior distribution of group-level difference parameters did not overlap with zero (Kruschke, 2014).
3. Results
3.1. Perceptual performance
3.1.1. Reports of chasing
First, we assessed whether changes in the stimuli led to the expected changes in perception of chasing in both patients and controls. We expected that participants would perceive chasing between the dots more often in displays in which there were interactions determined by the movement equation (interactive trials), and more often in trials with a higher cross-correlation level, irrespective of the presence of actual chasing (higher cross-correlation level induces a perceptual bias towards perceiving chasing; see Methods). Indeed, a mixed-effects logistic regression (see methods and Supplementary materials) revealed that participants reported chasing more often in the interactive trials (β = 1.54, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [1.41 1.67], t(2396) = 23.22, p < < 0.001) and in trials with high cross-correlation level (β = 1.21, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [1.09 1.34], t(2396) = 18.57, p ≪ 0.001; for full details of the model fit, see Supplementary materials). There was no significant difference between patients and control participants (β = 0.07, SE = 0.16, 95% CI [-0.24 0.38], t(2396) = 0.46, p = 0.65), nor interaction between interactive/control trials and cross-correlation level (β = -0.02, SE = 0.13, 95% CI [-0.24 0.28], t(2394) = 0.15, p = 0.88) or between cross-correlation level and participant group (β = -0.14, SE = 0.12, 95% CI [-0.38 0.11], t(2394) = -1.11, p = 0.27).
To better quantify the perceptual bias towards reporting chasing at higher cross-correlation level, we calculated the Signal Detection Theory measure of criterion or bias (c’, see Methods), expecting lower c’ values at higher cross-correlation level, which is indeed what we observed: c’ was lower in trials with high cross-correlation level than in those with low cross-correlation (Fig. 2A; F(1,58) = 148.7, p ≪ 0.001, ηp2 = 0.72; two-way repeated measures ANOVA). There was no significant difference between patients and control persons (F(1,58) = 0.17, p = 0.68, ηp2 < 0.01) nor interaction between participant group and cross-correlation level (F(1,58) = 0.37, p = 0.55, ηp2 < 0.01). A Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that the best model included only cross-correlation level (BFM = 10.66). Post-hoc tests revealed extremely strong evidence of an effect of cross-correlation level (BF10 > 7.7*1014), and moderately strong evidence towards no difference between patients and controls (BF10 = 0.21). These data suggest that participants of both groups were similarly sensitive to stimulus manipulations.
3.1.2. Accuracy of reports of chasing
Next, we assessed participants’ accuracy at discriminating trials with actual chasing (interactive trials) from trials without actual chasing (control trials), as well as effects of the cross-correlation level on accuracy and differences in accuracy between patients and control participants. Average performance over all trials and participants was 72% correct. A mixed-effects regression model revealed that participants were better than chance (intercept: β = 0.27, SE = 0.02, 95% CI [0.23 0.31], t(2397) = 14.14, p ≪ 0.001), worse in high cross-correlation trials (β = -0.04, SE = 0.02, 95% CI [-0.07 -0.003], t(2397) = -2.16, p < 0.031), and patients were worse than controls (β = -0.06, SE = 0.02, 95% CI [-0.11 -0.016], t(2397) = -2.64, p < 0.01). In the model with interaction term between cross-correlation level and participant group, this interaction was not significant (β = -0.12, SE = 0.11, 95% CI [-0.34 0.09], t(2396) = -1.13, p > 0.2). A mixed-effects logistic regression confirmed that participants made more errors in high cross-correlation level trials (β = -0.12, SE = 0.05, 95% CI [-0.23 -0.01], t(2397) = -2.17, p < 0.03), and that patients made more mistakes than control participants (β = -0.19, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [-0.33 -0.05], t(2397) = -2.60, p < 0.01). In the model with interaction term between cross-correlation level and participant group, this interaction was not significant (β = -0.12, SE = 0.11, 95% CI [-0.34 0.092], t(2396) = -1.13, p = 0.26). See Supplementary Fig. 1 for proportions of correct responses in all conditions.
We then assessed discrimination performance using the signal detection theory measure d’. Patients with schizophrenia were significantly less sensitive (i.e., had lower d’ values) in discriminating chasing from no chasing (Fig. 2B; F(1,58) = 6.45, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.10). There was no significant effect of cross-correlation level (F(1,58) = 1.19, p = 0.28, ηp2 = 0.02) nor interaction between participant group and cross-correlation level (F(1,58) = 0.72, p = 0.40, ηp2 = 0.01). A Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that the best model included only participant group (BFM = 3.43). Post-hoc tests revealed moderate evidence towards no effect of cross-correlation (BF10 = 0.25), and moderately strong evidence of a difference between patients and controls (BF10 = 3.25).
3.1.3. Confidence ratings
A mixed-effects regression model revealed that participants were more confident when they were correct (β = 0.24, SE = 0.04, 95% CI [0.16 0.33], t(2396) = 5.57, p ≪0.001), and more confident in high cross-correlation trials (β = 0.17, SE = 0.04, 95% CI [0.09 0.25], t(2396) = 4.53, p ≪ 0.001). Ratings from patients did not significantly differ from those of controls (β = -0.01, SE = 0.13, 95% CI [-0.26 0.25], t(2396) = -0.04, p > 0.9). A model with interaction terms did not reveal any significant interactions (see Supplementary materials). For distributions of confidence values in all conditions, see Supplementary Fig. 1.
3.1.4. Response times
A mixed-effects regression model revealed that participants responded faster when they gave correct responses (β = -0.88, SE = 0.14, 95% CI [-1.16 -0.61], t(2396) = -6.42, p < < 0.001), and that patients were slower than control participants (β = 0.73, SE = 0.33, 95% CI [0.08 1.38], t(2396) = 2.21, p < 0.027). The cross-correlation level did not significantly influence response times (β = -0.004, SE = 0.12, 95% CI [-0.24 0.23], t(2396) = 0.03, p > 0.9). A model with interaction terms did not reveal any significant interactions (see Supplementary materials). See Supplementary Fig. 1 for response times in all conditions.
3.1.5. Summary of perception findings
In sum, our data show that our task worked as desired: participants were better than chance at the task, and reported chasing more often in interactive trials. As we expected, stimuli created using a high cross-correlation level led to a perceptual bias towards reporting chasing in both patients and control participants. Crucially, patients were worse than control participants at the task, and showed reduced discrimination between interactive and control trials. Patients took longer to respond. Confidence ratings were higher when participants gave correct responses, but surprisingly these ratings were also higher in trials with high cross-correlation level, despite performance being lower in those trials.
3.2. Metacognitive performance
3.2.1. Metacognitive sensitivity
Metacognitive sensitivity (meta-d’) did not differ significantly between patients and controls (Fig. 2C; F(1,58) < 0.01, p = 0.98, ηp2 < 0.001), did not vary with cross-correlation level (F(1,58) = 0.89, p = 0.35, ηp2 = 0.015) and showed no interaction between cross-correlation level and participant groups (F(1,58) = 0.15, p = 0.70, ηp2 = 0.003). A Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that the null model best explained the data (BFM = 5.83; including only the subject factor). Post-hoc tests revealed moderately strong evidence towards no effect of cross-correlation (BF10 = 0.22), and moderately strong evidence towards no difference between patients and controls (BF10 = 0.19).
3.2.2. Metacognitive efficiency
Metacognitive efficiency (meta-d’/d’) did also not differ between patients and controls (Fig. 2D; F(1,58) = 0.61, p = 0.44, ηp2 = 0.011), did not vary with cross-correlation level (F(1,58) = 0.50, p = 0.48, ηp2 = 0.009) and showed no interaction between cross-correlation level and participant groups (F(1,58) = 1.71, p = 0.20, ηp2 = 0.029). The hierarchical Bayesian analysis did not reveal evidence of differences between groups, neither for trials with low nor high cross-correlation (posterior distributions of difference estimates overlapped with 0: mean and 95% highest-density intervals of these distributions were 0.10; [-1.11, 1.21] and 0.61; [-0.53 1.71] for trials with low and high cross-correlation, respectively). See Supplementary Fig. 2 for differences in group posterior distributions of M-ratios obtained by hierarchical Bayesian estimation. These results suggest that there was no difference in metacognitive efficiency between patients and controls.
3.2.3. Response-specific metacognitive efficiency
Response-specific metacognitive efficiency (rs-meta-d’/d’) did not differ significantly between patients and controls (Fig. 2E; F(1,49) = 0.49, p = 0.49, ηp2 = 0.010), did not vary for “yes” vs. “no” responses (F(1,49) < 0.02, p = 0.90, ηp2 < 0.001) nor with cross-correlation level (F(1,49) = 0.16, p = 0.69, ηp2 = 0.003). Interactions between cross-correlation level and participant groups, cross-correlation level and response, and the three-way interaction were not significant (F(1,49) < 1.18, p > 0.28, ηp2 < 0.023). There was a non-significant trend towards an interaction between response and participant groups (F(1,49) = 3.24, p = 0.078, ηp2 = 0.062). Note that rs-meta-d’/d’ could not be estimated for 7 patients and 2 controls because of missing data for either yes or no responses in one or more stimulus conditions. A Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that the null model best explained the data (BFM = 24.10; including only the subject factor). Post-hoc tests revealed moderately strong evidence towards no difference between patients and controls and no effect of cross-correlation or response (BF10 values were respectively: 0.20; 0.11; 0.12).
3.2.4. Summary of metacognition findings
In sum, we found no evidence for differences in metacognitive performance between patients and controls in any measure of metacognition.
4. Discussion
This study investigated perceptual and metacognitive performance in a social perception task in patients with schizophrenia and control persons. Consistent with established findings (Green et al., 2015; Green and Leitman, 2008), patients performed worse than control persons in detecting a social percept in displays of abstract social interactions. However, we found no significant differences in metacognitive performance between patients and controls. Bayesian tests indicated moderate effect sizes for both findings. In the context of recent occasional reports of preserved metacognitive capabilities (Charles et al., 2017; Powers et al., 2017; Rouy et al., 2021), our findings provide additional evidence for preserved metacognitive abilities.
Social perception in schizophrenia has been studied using a wide variety of tasks and measurements, ranging from descriptions of situations of daily life (Corrigan and Green, 1993; Toomey et al., 2002) to the interpretation of displays of interacting abstract objects (Horan et al., 2009b; Langdon et al., 2014, Langdon et al., 2020; Roux et al., 2015). One advantage of displays of interacting abstract objects is that cues for intentional behaviour are solely carried by motion parameters, are independent from object appearance, and can be systematically varied in their quantity. This controlled approach to biological motion allows us to assess the sensitivity to cues about intentions-to-act inscribed in the presented movements irrespective of influences of the objects’ visual appearance or visual context. The psychophysical measures we used here allow a precise quantification of the sensitivity to cues of intentionality and do not depend for example on declarative verbal skills which can be disturbed in persons with schizophrenia.
Patients’ deficit in discriminating between chasing and non-chasing dot displays is unlikely to be due to basic stimulus processing or attention deficits, as we found similar effects of the cross-correlation parameter on perceptual biases in both patients and control persons. This suggests that patients could process the stimuli as we intended and were not simply overwhelmed by the task. The discrimination deficit indicates reduced sensitivity to visual cues for intentional interactions.
Several previous studies had investigated attributions of intentionality to displays of interacting agents in schizophrenia. One study described a deficit in the spontaneous attribution of social meaning to displays of interacting objects (Horan et al., 2009b). Participants verbally described each animation, and these descriptions were rated for the degree of intentionality attributed to the agents. Patients’ descriptions were rated lower in intentionality than those of control persons. This deficit was replicated in subsequent studies (Langdon et al., 2020, Langdon et al., 2017). A chasing detection paradigm with several interacting objects developed by Gao and colleagues (2009) was used in three studies, with two studies reporting preserved detection of cues of intentionality (Langdon et al., 2020, Langdon et al., 2014), while the third reported a deficit (Roux et al., 2015). Our present findings also reveal a deficit in chasing detection with a different display. In sum, a definitive consensus opinion about whether detection of intentionality cues in displays of interacting abstract agents is impaired in schizophrenia or not has not been reached yet.
We assessed metacognition about social perception by combining perceptual decisions (first-order task) and confidence ratings about these decisions. This procedure is considered to be the gold standard to investigate metacognition, because it allows to assess metacognition while taking into account impairments in first-order performance, which are frequent in schizophrenia: A recent meta-analysis found a global deficit of metacognition in schizophrenia driven by studies which did not equate first-order performance between groups, but no conclusive deficit among studies controlling for first-order performance (Rouy et al., 2021). We found no significant impairment in metacognitive performance, whether measured as meta d’, M-ratio or response-specific M-ratio. The data reported in Fig. 2D show an unexpected, non-significant trend towards a reduced metacognitive efficiency in high cross-correlation trials in control participants, but no such trend in patients. This finding fits with the fact that in trials with high cross-correlation, participants reported higher confidence despite performing worse at the task. It seems that high cross-correlation trials induce a percept of chasing separately from actual, mathematically-determined interactions between the moving dots. We currently have no further explanation for this finding; to assess whether this trend is a real effect would require additional experiments, for example using a wider variation of stimuli. Our finding of the absence of significant impairment in metacognitive performance is compatible with the notion that not all types of metacognitive skills are similarly impaired in schizophrenia (see also (Rouy et al., 2021)). Other studies that have controlled for participant performance have also shown preserved components of metacognition in chronic schizophrenia, in the domains of episodic memory (Bacon and Izaute, 2009), detection of auditory signals (Powers et al., 2017), facial emotion recognition (Pinkham et al., 2018) and visual motion perception (Charles et al., 2017; Faivre et al., 2021). For example, Faivre and colleagues (Faivre et al., 2021) reported preserved metacognitive efficiency and sensitivity during perception of visual motion in people with chronic schizophrenia, and equivalent decisional mechanisms in patients and controls.
A more subtle investigation of metacognition consists in assessing the adaptability of metacognitive performance to the stimulus type, such as separate assessment of metacognitive performance for yes and no responses (Maniscalco and Lau, 2014). Indeed, confidence judgments about having perceived a target may be based on the amount of supporting evidence for the target. This is a somewhat different assessment than confidence judgments about not having perceived a target, which is based on the absence of evidence and might thus provide a poorer basis for discerning correct versus incorrect responses. A recent study reported that patients with schizophrenia do not show healthy controls’ adaptability of metacognitive performance to the type of visual motion stimuli (Koizumi et al., 2020). This could indicate that at least in some domains, patients with schizophrenia are atypical not in their metacognitive abilities, but rather in how their metacognitive capacity adapts to the demands of the cognitive function currently engaged. We found a non-significant trend towards a difference in response-specific metacognitive performance between patients and controls: M-ratios tended to be higher for yes- than for no-responses in control persons, as was observed in previous studies (Fleming et al., 2010; Kanai et al., 2010), and interestingly, this trend reversed in patients. While our findings did not reach significance, the adaptability of metacognitive performance to task demands in schizophrenia is a very interesting research topic that we might consider following up in subsequent studies. Assessing response-specific metacognitive performance is a sensitive approach that may allow us to better understand the mechanisms underlying the reported metacognitive difficulties previously reported in schizophrenia.
Some limitations of our study need to be acknowledged. First, our patient sample was small and heterogeneous: participants consisted of in- and outpatients, the sexes were not equally represented, and the duration of their disease varied widely. Second, while basic parameters such as age and sex were individually matched between patients and controls, we did not match the number of years of education and did not measure participants’ IQ. Lastly, our findings relate to processing of abstract stimuli in an experimental task, and thus our conclusions may not extend to real-life situations.
Social cognition and metacognition are challenging for people with schizophrenia, and are an important focus of psychotherapeutic interventions (Horan et al., 2009a; Moritz et al., 2014; Roberts et al., 2003; Wölwer et al., 2014). If preserved, specific metacognitive abilities can be used by patients to reconsider their interpretation of sensory signals they receive. For example, if patients learned to trust their metacognitive insight into a deficient perceptual process, they could learn to compensate or circumvent their deficit and improve the accuracy of their interpretations. Specifically, compensation could be achieved for example by taking more time and/or gathering more information before making a decision; the fact that patients took longer to respond in the present study supports the possibility that such a compensatory mechanism could be at play in the task we investigated here. Our results give some reason for optimism about this process regarding metacognition of social perception.
Declaration of competing interest
The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.
Acknowledgments
The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study. The experiment code, the data and code used for the analysis are posted on https://osf.io/f8k9d/.
Footnotes
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2021.100215.
Contributor Information
Ana Muthesius, Email: ana.muthesius@uk-koeln.de.
Johannes Schultz, Email: johannes.schultz@ukbonn.de.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
References
- Abell F., Happe F., Frith U. Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. J. Cogn. Dev. 2000;15:1–16. [Google Scholar]
- Addington D., Addington J., Maticka-Tyndale E. Assessing depression in schizophrenia: the Calgary depression scale. Br. J. Psychiatry Suppl. 1993:39–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnon-Ribenfeld N., Hasson-Ohayon I., Lavidor M., Atzil-Slonim D., Lysaker P.H. The association between metacognitive abilities and outcome measures among people with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Eur. psychiatr. 2017;46:33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bacon E., Izaute M. Metacognition in schizophrenia: processes underlying patients’ reflections on their own episodic memory. Biol. Psychiatry. 2009;66:1031–1037. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bassili J.N. Temporal and spatial contingencies in the perception of social events. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1976;33:680–685. [Google Scholar]
- Blakemore S.-J., Boyer P., Pachot-Clouard M., Meltzoff A., Segebarth C., Decety J. The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;13:837–844. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.8.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blythe P.W., Group, T.A.B.C.R. Todd P.M., MIller G.F. In: Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart. Gigerenzer G., Todd P.M., editors. Oxford University Press; New York: 1999. How motion reveals intention: categorizing social interactions; pp. 257–285. [Google Scholar]
- Castelli F., Happé F., Frith U., Frith C.D. Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. NeuroImage. 2000;12:314–325. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charles L., Gaillard R., Amado I., Krebs M.-O., Bendjemaa N., Dehaene S. Conscious and unconscious performance monitoring: evidence from patients with schizophrenia. NeuroImage. 2017;144:153–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan P.W., Green M.F. Schizophrenic patients’ sensitivity to social cues: the role of abstraction. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1993;150:589–594. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.4.589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- David N., Schultz J., Milne E., Schunke O., Schöttle D., Münchau A., Siegel M., Vogeley K., Engel A.K. Right temporoparietal gray matter predicts accuracy of social perception in the autism Spectrum. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014;44:1433–1446. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-2008-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies G., Greenwood K. A meta-analytic review of the relationship between neurocognition, metacognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia. J. Ment. Health. 2018;29:496–505. doi: 10.1080/09638237.2018.1521930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faivre N., Roger M., Pereira M., de Gardelle V., Vergnaud J.-C., Passerieux C., Roux P. Confidence in visual motion discrimination is preserved in individuals with schizophrenia. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2021;46:E65–E73. doi: 10.1503/jpn.200022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrer C., Franck N. Self-monitoring in schizophrenia. Curr. Psychiatr. Rev. 2007;3:243–251. doi: 10.2174/157340007782408897. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fett A.-K.J., Viechtbauer W., Dominguez M.-G., Penn D.L., van Os J., Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011;35:573–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming S.M. HMeta-d: hierarchical bayesian estimation of metacognitive efficiency from confidence ratings. Neurosci. Conscious. 2017;2017 doi: 10.1093/nc/nix007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming S.M., Lau H.C. How to measure metacognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming S.M., Weil R.S., Nagy Z., Dolan R.J., Rees G. Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science. 2010;329:1541–1543. doi: 10.1126/science.1191883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith C.D., Done D.J. Towards a neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1988;153:437–443. doi: 10.1192/bjp.153.4.437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao T., Newman G.E., Scholl B.J. The psychophysics of chasing: a case study in the perception of animacy. Cogn. Psychol. 2009;59(2):154–179. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner D.M., Murphy A.L., O’Donnell H., Centorrino F., Baldessarini R.J. International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2010;167:686–693. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09060802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green M.F. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am. J. Psychiatry. 1996;153:321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green M.F., Leitman D.I. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2008;34:670–672. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green D., Swets J. 1966. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. [Google Scholar]
- Green M.F., Penn D.L., Bentall R., Carpenter W.T., Gaebel W., Gur R.C., Kring A.M., Park S., Silverstein S.M., Heinssen R. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr. Bull. 2008;34:1211–1220. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green M.F., Horan W.P., Lee J. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;16:620–631. doi: 10.1038/nrn4005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson-Ohayon I., Goldzweig G., Lavi-Rotenberg A., Luther L., Lysaker P.H. The centrality of cognitive symptoms and metacognition within the interacting network of symptoms, neurocognition, social cognition and metacognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2018;202:260–266. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heider F., Simmel M. An experimental study of apparent behaviour. Am. J. Psychol. 1944;57:243–259. [Google Scholar]
- Horan W.P., Kern R.S., Shokat-Fadai K., Sergi M.J., Wynn J.K., Green M.F. Social cognitive skills training in schizophrenia: an initial efficacy study of stabilized outpatients. Schizophr. Res. 2009;107:47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horan W.P., Nuechterlein K.H., Wynn J.K., Lee J., Castelli F., Green M.F. Disturbances in the spontaneous attribution of social meaning in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 2009;39:635–643. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeffreys H. 3rd ed. OUP Oxford University Press; 1961. The Theory of Probability. [Google Scholar]
- Kanai R., Walsh V., Tseng C. Subjective discriminability of invisibility: a framework for distinguishing perceptual and attentional failures of awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 2010;19:1045–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay S.R., Fiszbein A., Opler L.A. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koizumi A., Hori T., Maniscalco B., Hayase M., Mishima R., Kawashima T., Miyata J., Aso T., Lau H., Takahashi H., Amano K. Atypical spatial frequency dependence of visual metacognition among schizophrenia patients. NeuroImage. 2020;27 doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koren D., Seidman L.J., Goldsmith M., Harvey P.D. Real-world Cognitive–and metacognitive-dysfunction in schizophrenia: a new approach for measuring (and Remediating) more “Right stuff”. Schizophr. Bull. 2006;32:310–326. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbj035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruschke J. 2014. Doing Bayesian Data Analysis: A Tutorial With R, JAGS, and Stan. [Google Scholar]
- Langdon R., McMillan J., Gao T. Detecting motion signals of intent in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2014;219:700–702. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langdon R., Flynn M., Connaughton E., Brüne M. Impairments of spontaneous and deliberative mentalizing co-occur, yet dissociate, in schizophrenia. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2017;56:372–387. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langdon R., Boulton K., Connaughton E., Gao T. Perceiving and attributing intentionality in schizophrenia. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry. 2020;25:269–280. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2020.1777093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leucht S., Samara M., Heres S., Patel M.X., Woods S.W., Davis J.M. Dose equivalents for second-generation antipsychotics: the minimum effective dose method. Schizophr. Bull. 2014;40:314–326. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ly A., Verhagen J., Wagenmakers E.-J. Harold Jeffreys’s default bayes factor hypothesis tests: explanation, extension, and application in psychology. J. Math. Psychol. 2016;14 [Google Scholar]
- Lysaker P.H., Erickson M., Ringer J., Buck K.D., Semerari A., Carcione A., Dimaggio G. Metacognition in schizophrenia: the relationship of mastery to coping, insight, self-esteem, social anxiety, and various facets of neurocognition. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2011;50:412–424. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.2010.02003.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lysaker P.H., Gumley A., Luedtke B., Buck K.D., Ringer J.M., Olesek K., Kukla M., Leonhardt B.L., Popolo R., Dimaggio G. Social cognition and metacognition in schizophrenia: evidence of their independence and linkage with outcomes. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2013;127:239–247. doi: 10.1111/acps.12012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lysaker P.H., Kukla M., Vohs J.L., Schnakenberg Martin A.M., Buck K.D., Hasson Ohayon I. Metacognition and recovery in schizophrenia: from research to the development of metacognitive reflection and insight therapy. J. Exp. Psychopathol. 2019;10 doi: 10.1177/2043808718814992. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Macmillan N.A., Creelman C.D. New York, NY, US; Cambridge University Press: 1991. Detection Theory: A User’s Guide, Detection Theory: A User’s Guide. [Google Scholar]
- Maniscalco B., Lau H. A signal detection theoretic approach for estimating metacognitive sensitivity from confidence ratings. Conscious. Cogn. 2012;21:422–430. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maniscalco B., Lau H. In: The Cognitive Neuroscience of Metacognition. Fleming S.M., Frith C.D., editors. Springer Berlin Heidelberg; Berlin, Heidelberg: 2014. Signal detection theory analysis of type 1 and type 2 data: meta-d', response-specific meta-d', and the unequal variance SDT model; pp. 25–66. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mathers C., World Health Organization. Fat D.M., Boerma J.T. World Health Organization, Geneva; Switzerland: 2008. The Global Burden of Disease: 2004 Update. [Google Scholar]
- Moritz S., Andreou C., Schneider B.C., Wittekind C.E., Menon M., Balzan R.P., Woodward T.S. Sowing the seeds of doubt: a narrative review on metacognitive training in schizophrenia. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2014;34:358–366. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2014.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthesius A., Hölzer S., Grothey A.F., Rubruck J., Vogeley K., Schultz J. New gambling task reveals an atypical relation between social anxiety and social avoidance in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. Open. 2020;1 doi: 10.1093/schizbullopen/sgaa013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pinkham A.E., Klein H.S., Hardaway G.B., Kemp K.C., Harvey P.D. Neural correlates of social cognitive introspective accuracy in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2018;202:166–172. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powers A.R., Mathys C., Corlett P.R. Pavlovian conditioning–induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors. Science. 2017;357:596–600. doi: 10.1126/science.aan3458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts D.L., Penn D.L., Combs D.R. Oxford University Press; 2003. Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT): Group Psychotherapy for Schizophrenia and Other Psychotic Disorders, Clinician Guide. [Google Scholar]
- Rouault M., Seow T., Gillan C.M., Fleming S.M. Psychiatric symptom dimensions are associated with dissociable shifts in metacognition but not task performance. Biol. Psychiatry. 2018;84:443–451. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.12.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roux P., Passerieux C., Ramus F. An eye-tracking investigation of intentional motion perception in patients with schizophrenia. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2015;40:118–125. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rouy M., Saliou P., Nalborczyk L., Pereira M., Roux P., Faivre N. Systematic review and meta-analysis of metacognitive abilities in individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021;126:329–337. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santos N.S., David N., Bente G., Vogeley K. Parametric induction of animacy experience. Conscious. Cogn. 2008;17:425–437. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santos N.S., Kuzmanovic B., David N., Rotarska-Jagiela A., Eickhoff S.B., Shah J.N., Fink G.R., Bente G., Vogeley K. Animated brain: a functional neuroimaging study on animacy experience. NeuroImage. 2010;53:291–302. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savla G.N., Vella L., Armstrong C.C., Penn D.L., Twamley E.W. Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophr. Bull. 2013;39:979–992. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz J., Friston K.J., O’Doherty J., Wolpert D.M., Frith C.D. Activation in posterior superior temporal sulcus parallels parameter inducing the percept of animacy. Neuron. 2005;45:625–635. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz J., Willems T., Gädeke M., Chakkour G., Franke A., Weber B., Hurlemann R. A human subcortical network underlying social avoidance revealed by risky economic choices. elife. 2019;8:1–42. doi: 10.7554/eLife.45249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephan K.E., Friston K.J., Frith C.D. Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophr. Bull. 2009;35:509–527. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toomey R., Schuldberg D., Corrigan P., Green M.F. Nonverbal social perception and symptomatology in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2002;53:83–91. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00177-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wölwer W., Lowe A., Frommann N. Training sozial-kognitiver funktionen – neue Ansätze zur vermeidung psychosozialer behinderungen bei schizophrenen erkrankungen. Fortschr. Neurol. Psychiatr. 2014;82:203–209. doi: 10.1055/s-0034-1366205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.