Abstract
Hormones regulate behavior either through activational effects that facilitate the acute expression of specific behaviors or through organizational effects that shape the development of the nervous system thereby altering adult behavior. Much research has implicated the neuropeptide oxytocin (OXT) in acute modulation of various aspects of social behaviors across vertebrate species, and OXT signaling is associated with the developmental social deficits observed in autism spectrum disorders (ASDs); however, little is known about the role of OXT in the neurodevelopment of the social brain. We show that perturbation of OXT neurons during early zebrafish development led to a loss of dopaminergic neurons, associated with visual processing and reward, and blunted the neuronal response to social stimuli in the adult brain. Ultimately, adult fish whose OXT neurons were ablated in early life, displayed altered functional connectivity within social decision-making brain nuclei both in naive state and in response to social stimulus and became less social. We propose that OXT neurons have an organizational role, namely, to shape forebrain neuroarchitecture during development and to acquire an affiliative response toward conspecifics.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Social behavior is developed over the lifetime of an organism and the neuropeptide oxytocin (OXT) modulates social behaviors across vertebrate species, and is associated with neuro-developmental social deficits such as autism. However, whether OXT plays a role in the developmental maturation of neural systems that are necessary for social behavior remains poorly explored. We show that proper behavioral and neural response to social stimuli depends on a developmental process orchestrated by OXT neurons. Animals whose OXT system is ablated in early life show blunted neuronal and behavioral responses to social stimuli as well as wide ranging disruptions in the functional connectivity of the social brain. We provide a window into the mechanisms underlying OXT-dependent developmental processes that implement adult sociality.
Keywords: development, organizational hypothesis, oxytocin, social decision-making network, sociality, zebrafish
Introduction
Social behavior describes any of a wide group of behaviors by an organism toward members of its species, termed conspecifics (Robinson et al., 2019). The systems underlying these behaviors are thought to be established during development whereas the behavioral manifestations themselves are not immediately put into effect, but become apparent only later in life.
The “organizational hypothesis” claims that hormones can shape the structure of the developing nervous system and as a result alter the adult animal behavior and was first suggested by Phoenix, et al., following their study of the developmental effect of testosterone exposure on subsequent sexual behavior (Phoenix et al., 1959). Organizational effects refer to long-term, irreversible impact of hormones on tissue differentiation that can either directly or indirectly influence physiology, metabolism and behavior. In the context of behavior, organizational signals that can be mediated by neuropeptides, are necessary during critical developmental windows to shape neural systems whose activity will only become relevant at a later stage in the lifetime of the animal. This organizational effect is contrasted with the acute or “activational” effect exerted by hormones, neurotransmitters and neuropeptides on behavior, physiology and metabolism.
The neuropeptide oxytocin (OXT) has a widely-studied activational role in several aspects of social behavior (for review, see Donaldson and Young, 2008; Grinevich et al., 2016; Jurek and Neumann, 2018), including social processing (Gamer et al., 2010; Grinevich and Stoop, 2018), attention (Bartz et al., 2011; Lukas et al., 2011; Shamay-Tsoory and Abu-Akel, 2016), reward (Insel and Shapiro, 1992; Bowen and Neumann, 2017), and prosocial behaviors (Lukas et al., 2011), in rodents and humans. Previously, we showed that this activational role of OXT is evolutionarily conserved, since OXT also modulates perception of visual social cues (Nunes et al., 2020) and social recognition (Ribeiro et al., 2020a,b) in zebrafish.
OXT is thought to have organizational effects as well. As early as 1989, Noonan and colleagues showed that pharmacological treatment of neonatal rats with OXT had long-term effects on behavior in the adult (Noonan et al., 1989). In a more recent example, Keebaugh and Young used selective viral overexpression to modulate OXT receptor (OXTR) levels in the nucleus accumbens of prepubertal female prairie voles and showed that this developmental intervention is necessary and sufficient to induce alloparental behavior in the adult (Keebaugh and Young, 2011). Such results have been described in various mammalian models, behavioral domains, and using pharmacological (e.g., peptide, agonist and antagonist injection) as well as genetic interventions in prenatal and prepubertal animals (for review, see Hammock, 2015; Miller and Caldwell, 2015). Furthermore, human studies of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have strongly suggested that certain features of ASD arise from very early developmental abnormalities which are thought to be linked to OXT signaling, further suggesting an organizational role for OXT in the development of social behavior (Hovey et al., 2014; for review, see Heinrichs et al., 2009; Guastella and Hickie, 2016). However, the mechanisms underlying this organizational mode of action have thus far remained elusive.
It has been well established that OXT communicates with other neurotransmitter systems for proper social behaviors. Dopamine (DA) is mainly involved in reward and reinforcement systems (Love, 2014; Dölen and Malenka, 2014), but also in sensory modulation and attention gating (Love, 2014; Grinevich et al., 2016). Dopaminergic neurons are activated during social interactions and mating, and interact with the OXT system to promote pair-bond formation (Dölen and Malenka, 2014; Johnson et al., 2017) and sociability (Hung et al., 2017), suggesting that OXT and DA are both necessary to promote the sensory and rewarding aspects of social interactions.
Here, we show that in zebrafish, signaling by OXT neurons is required in an organizational manner during early life for the display of social affiliation in adulthood. Furthermore, we go on to show that perturbing this process in a very early developmental time window, equivalent to fetal developmental stages in mammals, has long-lasting effects on specific dopaminergic clusters associated with reward and visual attention/processing, and also leads to wide-ranging changes in brain activity within previously described social processing networks.
Materials and Methods
Experimental model
Zebrafish were raised and bred according to standard protocols. Zebrafish embryos were raised in 100-mm Petri-dishes (50–100 embryos/dish) in E3 medium (5 mm NaCl, 0.16 mm KCl, 0.4 mm CaCL2.2H2O, and 0.4 mm MgSO4.7H2O, with 0.01% methylene blue), and transferred to the main rearing system (Tecniplast) at day 6 (35–40 embryos/3.5 l). Adult zebrafish were kept in mixed sex groups (10 adults/l) in a recirculation life supporting system with the following parameters: 28°C, pH 7.0, conductivity 1000 µS/cm, 14/10 h light/dark cycle. Fishes were fed with a combination of live food (Paramecium caudatum and Artemia salina) and commercial processed dry food (Gemma). Husbandry protocols, water chemistry and health program have been described previously (Borges et al., 2016). All experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with standard operating procedures of the Instituto Gulbenkian de Ciência and Direcção Geral de Alimentação e Veterinária [Direcção Geral de Alimentação e Veterinária (DGAV) permit number 0421/000/000/2015], Portugal, and Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC; protocol number 27100516) of the Weizmann Institute, Israel.
Zebrafish transgenic/mutant lines used in this study: Tg(oxt:EGFP) (Blechman et al., 2011), Tg(oxt:Gal4)wz06 (Anbalagan et al., 2018), Tg(UAS:NTR-mCherry)c264 (Davison et al., 2007), Tg(UAS:sypb-EGFP) (Meyer and Smith, 2006).
Larvae and adult zebrafish (three to six months old) of both sexes were used in this work.
Social affiliation assay
The social preference test followed the protocol described previously (Wircer et al., 2017; Ribeiro et al., 2020a). Briefly, focal zebrafish were given a choice between two side-by-side compartments: one containing a shoal (two males and two females) and an empty one (Fig. 1A,B) during a 10-min test. The stimulus shoal matched the genotype of the focal fish. The stimulus compartment was randomly switched between tests, to control for any place preference possibly induced by the arena or laboratory frames. All compartments were completely sealed to block transmission of chemical and vibrational stimuli and thus, only visual cues were accessible. The experimental test tank was placed over a custom-built infrared LED light box, to increase image quality for subsequent automated video tracking. Fish behavior was recorded from above with either a high-speed camera FLARE (2M360, Io Industries) or a B&W mini surveillance camera (Henelec 300B) connected to a computer, using video recording software (Pinnacle Studio 12). Videos were analyzed with Ethovision XT11.0 (Noldus Inc.). Relevant data were then exported and further analyzed. The two regions of interest (ROIs), empty and shoal, were defined as the 1/10 of the length of the arena immediately adjacent to the empty compartment or the compartment containing the stimulus shoal, respectively. The percentage of cumulative time fish spent in these ROIs (%Tshoal and %Tempty) was used to calculate the social affiliation score [%Tshoal/(%Tshoal + %Tempty)]. This score, also called the sociability score, has been commonly used in rodent studies to measure sociability in the 3-chamber test (Park et al., 2018). Total distance traveled was also measured.
Ontogeny of social affiliation behavior
Social preference was tested at different developmental stages: early larvae (12- d-old), mid larvae (14- to 20- d-old), postflexion metalarvae (21- to 30-d-old), juveniles and adults (from 30- to 90- d-old). The size of the testing arena for early larvae was 3.5 × 6.0 cm; for mid larvae 7.0 × 8.0 cm; for metalarvae 7.0 × 10.0 cm; and for juveniles and adults, it was 20.0 × 20.0 cm. The stimulus shoal matched the developmental stage of the focal fish. Two different experiments were performed: (1) longitudinal, where individual fish were repeatedly tested at each developmental time point; and (2) cross-sectional, where individual fish were tested only once, at a given developmental time point.
From all behavioral experiments, animals were excluded from analysis if they failed to enter either of the ROIs (i.e., shoal or empty) as this results in a social score equals to infinity.
Chemically induced OXT neuronal ablation
Early ablation
Nitroreductase (NTR)-mediated chemically induced OXT neuronal ablation was conducted as previously described (Curado et al., 2007, 2008). Briefly, larvae (∼30–40 per Petri dish) were treated with metronidazole (MTZ; Vetranal catalog #46461, Sigma) dissolved in Danieau buffer to a final concentration of 10 mm for 48 h while being protected from light, to prevent MTZ photoinactivation. After the first 24 h, the MTZ-containing Danieau buffer was replaced by fresh MTZ medium. Control untreated larvae were subjected to the same procedure in Danieau buffer without MTZ, but were also protected from light. After 48 h of treatment, larvae were washed out several times in Danieau buffer.
Adult ablation
We used a modified ablation protocol of 3 × 48 h MTZ treatments (5 mm), as we observed that this increased survival of adult animals. Briefly, adult zebrafish were placed into 2-liter tanks containing MTZ (Veterinary Preparation, Vetmarket, catalog #165228, or 2-methyl-5-nitroimidazole-1-ethanol, TCL Europe, catalog #M0924) dissolved in system water at a final concentration of 5 mm, protected from light. After ∼16 h of treatment, zebrafish were moved to a tank containing fresh system water without MTZ, fed, and allowed to recover for ∼2 h, followed by a second treatment of MTZ 5 mm for 16 h. Animals were then allowed to recover in their home tanks for 48 h. The entire protocol was repeated three times.
Effect of chemically induced OXT neuronal ablation on endogenous OXT cells
Zebrafish treated with MTZ at different time points during development (4- to 6-, 12- to 14-, 20- to 22-, or 90-d-old) were allowed to recover from treatment for 48 h and were then killed to assess the treatment effects on endogenous OXT cells by in situ hybridization (see below) or by direct visualization of the mCherry transgene signal. OXT cells were manually counted using ImageJ software (Schindelin et al., 2012).
Effect of chemically induced OXT neuronal ablation on adult social affiliation
Zebrafish treated with MTZ at 4- to 6-, 12- to 14-, 20- to 22-, or 90-d-old were allowed to grow until adulthood to be tested for social affiliation behavior (see above).
Recovery of endogenous OXT cells following early-life chemically induced OXT neuronal ablation
Zebrafish larvae were ablated at 12–14 days post fertilization (DPF), and then allowed to grow until adulthood. At 90 DPF, animals were killed, brains harvested, and subjected to fluorescent in situ hybridization (see below). The total fluorescence was then quantified in a semi-automated manner (see below).
Time course of NTR-mCherry recovery following 4- to 6-d-old chemically induced OXT neural ablation
Fish were treated with MTZ at 4- to 6-d-old and sampled at different time points: 8-, 12-, 19-, 26-, and 42-d-old. The total fluorescence was then quantified in a semi-automated manner (see below).
Semi-automated method for quantifying total fluorescence
For quantification of images in the recovery experiment we used a semi-automated method for quantifying total fluorescence. Each Z-stack was broken into its component optical planes, and ROIs were selected for the region containing the cells and the background (a region within the tissue with no fluorescent cells). The threshold for “positive” pixels was defined as the 75th percentile value of all the pixels in the background ROI, and all positive pixels per slice were counted. The total number of positive pixels per sample was summed, and then normalized to the maximal value for that cohort. Source code in MATLAB for this procedure is included.
Effect of 4- to 6-d-old chemically induced OXT neuronal ablation on adult neuronal activity [protein S6 (pS6) activation]
To assess the effect of early OXT ablation on adult brain activation patterns in response to a social stimulus, adult zebrafish that had been treated with MTZ at 4- to 6-d-old and untreated controls were exposed individually to either a mixed-sex shoal of conspecifics or an empty tank for 10 min. Immediately after, we blocked the visual stimulus, without disturbing the focal fish, by placing an opaque partition between the experimental and stimulus tanks. After 50 min (to allow for expression of the pS6 neural activation marker), zebrafish were killed and heads were collected and processed for paraffin slice immunofluorescence (see below).
Effect of 4- to 6-d-old chemically induced OXT neuronal ablation on larvae and adult dopaminergic system
Untreated and 4- to 6-d-old MTZ-treated zebrafish larvae were allowed to recover from treatment (48 h) and were either processed immediately for whole-mount larvae TH immunofluorescence (see below) or allowed to grow until adulthood and then processed for paraffin slice TH immunofluorescence (see below).
In situ hybridization
RNA in situ hybridization was performed on both whole larvae and whole adult brains. Larvae or dissected brains were fixed in 4% PFA and in situ hybridization was performed as described previously (Machluf and Levkowitz, 2011; Wircer et al., 2017). An OXT probe was generated using a pGEM plasmid encoding for oxt mRNA (RefSeq NM_178291.2). Following development with Fast Red reagent (Roche, catalog #11496549001), adult brains were embedded in agar and sagittally cut at a thickness of 150 μm on a vibratome, and whole larvae and brain slices were mounted in glycerol and imaged on a Zeiss LSM 800 scanning confocal microscope.
Whole-mount larvae immunofluorescence
Briefly, larvae were euthanized in ice-cold water, transferred to 4% PFA and then incubated overnight at 4°C on a shaker. Then, the PFA was washed out and samples were placed in precooled acetone in a freezer at −20°C for 10 min. The acetone was washed out (PBS 0.1% Triton X-100), and the samples were then incubated in blocking solution (PBS 0.1% Triton X-100 + 1% DMSO + 1% BSA + 5% NGS) for minimum of 2 h at room temperature (RT), followed by incubation with primary antibody overnight at 4°C on a shaker. Primary antibodies used were either mouse anti-TH (MAB 318, Merck Millipore), rabbit anti-EGFP (A11122, ThermoFisher) or guinea pig anti-OXT (T-5021, Peninsula Labs), at a concentration of 1:200. Next, samples were washed repeatedly (minimum of 6 × 15 min washes) with blocking solution and then placed in a blocking solution containing fluorescent secondary antibody (1:200) overnight at 4°C on a shaker. Then, samples were washed in PBS, mounted dorsally on a slide in mounting medium (Aqua-Polymount, Polysciences Inc., catalog #18606-20) and imaged by a Zeiss LSM 800 scanning confocal microscope.
Paraffin adult brain slice immunofluorescence
Zebrafish heads were fixed in 10% buffered formalin for 72 h and decalcified in EDTA (0.5 m, pH 8.0) for 48 h, followed by paraffin inclusion. Coronal slices (6 μm thick) were cut with a microtome. Sectioned slices were then processed for immunofluorescence. After antigen retrieval with Tris-EDTA (10 mm Tris Base, 1 mm EDTA, 0.05% Tween 20) at 95°C for 20 min, slices were washed in TBS-Tx (TBS 0.025% Triton X-100, 3 × 10 min), then incubated with blocking solution (TBS 0.025% Triton X-100 + 1% BSA) for 1 h at RT, followed by an overnight incubation with a primary antibody (1:400 at 4°C). Slides were then washed in TBS-0.025% Triton X-100 (3 × 10 min) and incubated in secondary antibody (1:1000 for 2 h). After washes, slices were incubated with DAPI for 20 min, then rinsed in TBS and mounted with EverBrite Hardset Mounting medium (Biotium). Primary antibodies used were anti-phosphorylation of S6 ribosomal protein, pS6 (Cell signaling S235/236) and anti-tyrosine hydroxylase (TH), anti-TH (mouse monoclonal, MAB 318, Merck Millipore).
Neuronal quantifications
Neuronal pS6 cell quantification
Coronal images were acquired with a commercial Nikon High Content Screening microscope, based on Nilon Ti equipped with a Andor Zyla 4.2 sCMOS camera, using a 20 × 0.75 NA objective, quadruple dichroic filter, and controlled with the Nikon Elements software. To avoid double-imaging of the same cells, we imaged every other slice. We quantified density of pS6-positive cells in 16 brain areas that belong to the social decision-making network (SDMN; O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011): Vv, ventral nucleus of the ventral telencephalic area (V), homologous to the mammalian lateral septum (LS), subdivided into: anterior (Vv_a) and posterior (Vv_p); Vd, dorsal nucleus of V, homologous to the mammalian Nacc, subdivided into: anterior (Vd_a), medial (Vd_m), and posterior (Vd_p); Vc, central nucleus of V, homologous to the mammalian striatum, subdivided into: anterior (Vc_a) and posterior (Vc_p); Dm, medial zone of the dorsal telencephalic area (D), homologous to the mammalian bLAMY; Dl, lateral zone of D, homologous to hippocampus; Dd, dorsal zone of D; Vs, supracommissural nucleus of V, homologous to the mammalian extended amygdala (BNST and meAMY); Vp, postcommissural nucleus of V; Ppa, parvocellular preoptic nucleus, anterior part and Ppp, parvocellular preoptic nucleus, posterior part, both homologous to the mammalian preoptic area (POA); VM, ventromedial thalamic nucleus; VL, ventrolateral thalamic nucleus (Extended Data Fig. 8-1).
For each brain area, about five coronal brain slices were analyzed manually using ImageJ software (Schindelin et al., 2012). Background subtraction and linear adjustments of brightness and contrast levels were performed in the same way for all groups. The brain areas of interest were identified using DAPI to define neuroanatomical boundaries and landmarks identified in the Zebrafish Atlas (Wullimann and Mueller, 2004). In each brain slice, we placed one square of 1000 μm2 in the brain ROI, in each hemisphere, following these criteria: the square was always placed over the highest number of pS6-positive cells, keeping minimum distance from the border and edge of the brain section, a similar strategy that has been used by others (Lorenzi et al., 2017). pS6-positive cells were counted if a nucleus surrounded by the cytoplasm was clearly visible, and if the intensity of the pS6 signal was perceptibly greater than background.
Dopaminergic neuronal quantification
Dopaminergic quantification was performed in both larvae and adult zebrafish, either 4- to 6-d-old MTZ treated or untreated. Briefly, for larvae, whole-mount zebrafish immunofluorescence was performed as described above using primary antibody anti-TH. Larvae were imaged and confocal z-stacks were analyzed by ImageJ. Larva dopaminergic forebrain, pretectum (PrT), and posterior tuberculum (TP) nucleus were identified according to (Sallinen et al., 2009; Tay et al., 2011) and TH+ cells were counted in these three groups.
For adults, heads from both treatment groups (untreated and 4- to 6-d-old MTZ treated) were collected and processed for paraffin embedding, followed by paraffin slice immunofluorescence as described above. Coronal slices were acquired with a SlideScanner Zeiss AxioScan Z1 (Zeiss), and analyzed with Zeiss Zen Lite software. To avoid double-imaging of the same cells, we imaged every other slice. Boundaries separating brain nuclei and subdivisions were identified based on DAPI staining, using as reference a coronal atlas of the zebrafish (Wullimann et al., 1996). Dopaminergic neurons (TH+) were identified using the same criteria as for pS6+ neurons.
TH+ nuclei groups were identified as previously described (Rink and Wullimann, 2001; Panula et al., 2010; Parker et al., 2013; Extended Data Fig. 6-1). We counted TH+ cells in the following clusters: the subpallium area (extending from the rostroventral telencephalon to the dorsocaudal telencephalon, also identified as G2 by Panula et al., 2010), which included a TH cluster containing Vv, Vc, and Vl, and in-between areas, a TH cluster including Vd and the lateral area outside the Vd, and a TH cluster within the Vs (identified by the anterior commissure) and lateral area outside the Vs; TH clusters in the PPa area were divided in two: a more anterior (close to the border of the diencephalon, lateral margin of the ventricle, G3) and a more posterior extending area in the medial area of the PPa (ventral part of the PPa, G4); a TH cluster in the PPr (G7); in the ventromedial and ventrolateral nucleus (Vm, Vl, G6); a TH cluster in the periventricular nucleus of the TP [TP/TP, small cells, ventral to the central posterior thalamic nucleus (CP), G11] and in the posterior tuberal nucleus (PTN; G12; Extended Data Fig. 6-1). Similar to pS6 neuronal quantification described above, we manually analyzed five coronal brain slices for each cluster, in each brain hemisphere, placing one square of 1000 μm2 in the brain ROI with the highest density of cells.
Statistical analysis
Data are represented as mean ± SEM. Normality of the data were tested by D'Agostino and Pearson omnibus and Shapiro–Wilk normality tests. When parametric assumptions were verified, we used parametric statistics; otherwise, we used equivalent non-parametric tests, namely, Mann–Whitney tests to compare the effect of MTZ treatment on OXT mRNA, on the expression of the oxt:GAL4;UAS-NTR-mCherry transgene, and to compare the adult social affiliation score between untreated and MTZ-treated groups at different developmental time points (4- to 6-, 12- to 14-, 22- to 24-, and 90-d-old MTZ treatment). Significance was denoted as p < 0.05, and p values refer to two-tailed tests, unless otherwise noted. The use of one-tailed tests was justified by a priori directionality hypotheses in the following cases: (1) effect of MTZ treatment on OXT mRNA and oxt:GAL4;UAS-NTR-mCherry transgene expression (Fig. 2), since in our system, we expected that MTZ would ablate the NTR-oxytocinergic expressing neurons; (2) effect of MTZ treatment (oxytocinergic ablation) on social affiliation behavior in fish expressing the oxt:GAL4;UAS-NTR-mCherry transgene, and thus causing OXT ablation, since OXT is well known to regulate social behaviors and thus, we expected that oxytocinergic ablation would lead to a decreased social behavior in zebrafish (Fig. 3).
Statistical analysis of behavioral data
One-sample t tests were used to verify whether social affiliation during development was statistically different from chance (0.5). Because we tested eight independent cohorts for the effect of MTZ treatment at 4- to 6-d-old time point (by different researchers in two different laboratories), the total sample size for this time point (n = 210) was much larger than for the other time points (12–14 d old: n = 28; 22–24 d old: n = 19; adult: n = 13). Taking into account all eight cohorts together, the adult social affiliation score of the 4- to 6-d-old MTZ-treated fish were significantly decreased from untreated siblings (p = 0.0012, Mann–Whitney test, n = 112 untreated vs 98 MTZ-treated fish). However, a generalized linear model (GLM) with β regression and planned comparisons, which was used to compare the effect of MTZ treatment on adult social affiliation score of the eight different cohorts (Fig. 3), revealed not only an effect of the MTZ treatment (F(1) = 9.486, p = 0.0021) but also variation among cohorts (F(7) = 2.719, p = 0.0081; Table 1). We therefore performed a Monte Carlo (MC) simulation for a sample size of 30 individuals sampled from the observed data, 15 individuals from each group (untreated and 4- to 6-d-old MTZ treated) with 1000 iterations. This simulation, ensured a more comparable sample size for the 4- to 6-d-old time point treatment relative to other time point treatments. Moreover, it ensured a more representative sample than each cohort independently or the total eight-cohort population. We obtained a MC p value of p < 0.001 (Fig. 3), which means that there was a significantly higher number of simulations in which the social affiliation score of the untreated group was greater than the social affiliation score of the MTZ-treated group, as compared with the number of simulations with the inverse trend, for a sample size of 15 individuals, across all eight cohorts; p value was calculated as the mean social affiliation score of the 1000 iterations for untreated fish divided by the mean scores of the 1000 iterations for treated fish divided by 1000.
Table 1.
Social affiliation behavior (GLM) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
d.f.1 d.f.2 | F ratio | p value | |||
Cohort | 7 Inf | 2.719 | 0.0081** | ||
Treatment | 1 inf | 9.486 | 0.00105** | ||
Cohort:treatment | 7 inf | 1.388 | 0.2053 | ||
Total distance moved (LM) | |||||
Df | Sum Sq | Mean Sq | F value | Pr(>F) | |
Cohort | 7 | 17,444 | 2492.03 | 15.2007 | 8.342e-16*** |
Treatment | 1 | 30 | 30.22 | 0.1843 | 0.6681 |
Cohort:treatment | 7 | 838 | 119.67 | 0.7299 | 0.6468 |
Residuals | 195 | 31,969 | 163.94 |
**p < 0.01,
***p < 0.001.
To determine whether the difference in social affiliation score between untreated and 4- to 6-d-old MTZ-treated fish was because of OXT ablation and not movement impairments caused by MTZ, we used a linear model (LM) to compare the total distance moved (square root transformed) of both groups in the eight independent cohorts (Fig. 3; Table 1). Both GLM with β regression on adult social affiliation score and LM on total distance moved (square root transformed) were also applied to different control cohort fish not expressing oxt:GAL4;UAS-NTR-mCherry transgene (Fig. 3; Table 2). In all of these models (GLM and LM), the explanatory variables were treatment and cohorts for mains effects and interaction.
Table 2.
Social affiliation behavior (GLM) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
d.f.1 d.f.2 | F ratio | p value | |||
Cohort | 3 inf | 7.249 | 0.0001*** | ||
Treatment | 1 inf | 1.241 | 0.2653 | ||
Cohort:treatment | 3 inf | 1.271 | 0.2822 | ||
Total distance moved (LM) | |||||
Df | Sum Sq | Mean Sq | F value | Pr(>F) | |
Cohort | 3 | 15,614.2 | 5204.7 | 58.5513 | <2e-16*** |
Treatment | 1 | 183.3 | 183.3 | 2.0615 | 0.1536 |
Cohort:treatment | 3 | 352.1 | 117.4 | 1.3203 | 0.2710 |
Residuals | 121 | 10,755.9 | 88.9 |
***p < 0.001.
Statistical analysis of neuronal activation (pS6 cell quantification)
To compare neuronal activation between treatments and stimuli, we used a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with a Poisson distribution using the brain identity as the random effect in the statistical model, followed by planned comparisons, since we target areas that are known to belong to the SDMN. We checked whether the data were normally distributed with Shapiro–Wilk tests for each brain area. Since they were not, and because the data are counts, we checked the fit of the data to the Poisson distribution with the “fitdistrplus” package from R (Delignette-Muller and Dutang, 2015). By visual inspection of density and cumulative distribution plots we validated that the Poisson distribution was the most appropriate to our data analyses. As fixed effects we used treatment and stimuli. We analyzed the 16 brain areas independently and corrected the p values of the planned comparison with the false discovery rate (FDR) adjustment method.
Statistical analysis of dopaminergic cell quantification
To compare dopaminergic cell numbers between treatments, and since the data were normally distributed (G7: W = 0.97 685, p = 0.6383; G12: W = 0.9546, p = 0.1459; G2: W = 0.95 265, p = 0.1267; Shapiro–Wilk test), LM was performed to assess the effect of 4- to 6-d-old MTZ treatment in total TH counts of three distinct independently analyzed TH clusters in 8-d-old larvae (Table 3).
Table 3.
BA | Treatment 1 | Treatment 2 | Estimate | SE | z ratio | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Subpallium | Untreated | MTZ treated | −2.889 | 2.622 | −1.102 | 0.278 |
PrT | Untreated | MTZ treated | −8.278 | 2.994 | −2.764 | 0.009 ** |
TP large cells | Untreated | MTZ treated | −1.722 | 0.566 | −3.042 | 0.0045** |
BA, brain area.
**p < 0.01.
The effect of 4- to 6-d-old MTZ treatment on adult dopaminergic system was also assessed with a GLMM with a Poisson regression followed by planned comparisons, comparing the number of TH-positive cells sampled in five squares of 1000 µm2 per TH cluster in eight different adult brain TH clusters. As fixed effects, we used treatment and stimuli. All eight areas were analyzed independently and p values of the planned comparisons were corrected with the FDR adjustment method (Fig. 6; Table 4).
Table 4.
BA | Treatment 1 | Treatment 2 | Estimate | SE | z ratio | p value | p value with FDR correction |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Subpallium (G2) | Untreated | MTZ treated | −0.031 | 0.0562 | −0.552 | 0.5813 | 0.6643 |
PPa (G3) | Untreated | MTZ treated | 0.185 | 0.175 | 1.058 | 0.2901 | 0.3868 |
PPa (G4) | Untreated | MTZ treated | 0.0611 | 0.0533 | 1.147 | 0.2513 | 0.3868 |
PPp (G5) | Untreated | MTZ treated | 0.12 | 0.0747 | 1.603 | 0.1090 | 0.218 |
Vm + Vl (G6) | Untreated | MTZ treated | 0.0143 | 0.0755 | 0.189 | 0.8502 | 0.8502 |
PrT (G7) | Untreated | MTZ treated | 0.201 | 0.0598 | 3.364 | 0.0008 | 0.0048** |
TP (G11) | Untreated | MTZ treated | 0.188 | 0.0724 | 2.597 | 0.0094 | 0.0251* |
PTN (G12) | Untreated | MTZ treated | 0.19 | 0.0586 | 3.239 | 0.0012 | 0.0048** |
BA, brain area; Vm, ventromedial thalamic nucleus; Vl, ventrolateral thalamic nucleus.
*p < 0.05,
**p < 0.01.
Data analysis software
Graphical representations of the data were performed in GraphPad Prism 6.0c software. Shapiro–Wilk normality test, one-sample t test and t tests were performed in GraphPad Prism, and the remaining tests were performed in R programming software, version 3.6.3 (R_core_Team, 2020) with the following packages: Ime4 (Bates et al., 2015) and afex (Singmann et al., 2020) for GLMM with Poisson regression, betareg (Cribari-Neto and Zeileis, 2010). for GLM with β regression, emmeans (Lenth, 2020) for planned comparisons, and the base R package for LM regression and MC simulations (R_core_Team, 2020).
Functional connectivity analysis
Functional connectivity, which has been defined as the temporal coincidence of spatially distant neurophysiological events (Friston, 1994), is typically inferred from temporal correlations between distinct brain areas in human fMRI data from both resting state and task-state studies (Biswal et al., 1997). The rationale for this approach is that areas that show a consistent correlation in their activity are components of the same brain network. Therefore, the concept of functional connectivity is purely correlative in nature, and per se it does not imply a direct causal relationship or the occurrence of structural connectivity between the correlated brain areas (Eickhoff and Müller, 2015). Functional connectivity can also be inferred from correlations across subjects using the expression of molecular markers such as immediate early genes (Teles et al., 2015). Similarly, we computed functional connectivity for our neuronal activity data based on the co-expression of pS6 across different brain regions for the set of individuals of each experimental treatment. For this purpose, we have developed the method described below, which statistically validates the identified networks.
Network reconstruction
To test for functional connectivity of early MTZ-treated versus untreated brains exposed to social stimuli, we reconstructed Pearson correlation matrices using a resampling procedure, similar to the quadratic assignment procedure (QAP; Makagon et al., 2012; Borgatti et al., 2013). Instead of defining a single network, we construct the set of possible networks obtained leaving out some of the specimens' information. More precisely, consider the case of specimens, each with an associated expression vector , where is the number of brain regions. For a given , we consider all the combinations of specimens and compute the corresponding functional graphs. We refer to the collection of graphs obtained in this way as a graph tower , where each of the combinations can be considered as a graph layer. The advantage of this construction is that each layer in the graph tower represents a different instance of the network bootstrapping. In this way, observables computable on a single layer can be bootstrapped across multiple ones. The construction has naturally one parameter, the sampling number , which needs to be chosen on the basis of data-driven considerations. In our experiments, robustness analysis shows that results are robust for values between 10 and 15. To obtain sparse functional connectivity matrices, we threshold each instance following (De Vico Fallani et al., 2017) at a density threshold of , which also corresponds to the minimum network heterogeneity across instances. After thresholding, for each treatment we average the thresholded instances to obtain a single matrix per treatment, which we use in the following analysis.
Detection of robust functional modules
Communities were computed using the Leiden community detection method (Traag et al., 2019) on the averaged treatment matrices. To increase the robustness of the detection, for each condition, we repeat the community detection 400 times. From the 400 candidate partitions we extract the central partition as described previously (Peixoto, 2021) and associate the resulting central partition to the treatment under analysis. To quantitatively characterize differences among partitions, we measure the ratio of total edge weights within a community with that of the edges between communities. More specifically, for a partition with communities we compute the matrix , defined as
where label the modules of and is the edge weight between regions nd . We then compute the ratio of average intra-community to inter-community edge weights as follows:
which measures the ratio of the average weight on the diagonal of to the average diagonal weight. To assess significance of values and of differences between them, we employ a permutation test based on permutating the community labels while preserving the size of the considered communities. We find that all values are significantly different from zero, and so are also the differences in between treatments (p < 0.01).
Strength centrality
As a measure of local integration, we computed also the strength centralities. The first encodes how strongly a node links to its neighbors. while the second total measures how influential a node is at the network level on the basis of its direct connections and how well connected its neighbors are. In Extended Data Fig. 8-4, we report the nodes ranked in decreasing order of strength (weighted degree) centrality.
Results
Early life ablation of oxytocinergic neurons decreases social affiliation
Zebrafish is a highly gregarious species exhibiting well-characterized social behaviors (for review, see Nunes et al., 2017). We quantified the visually-mediated motivation of zebrafish to approach conspecifics, as an indicator of social affiliation, by performing a two-choice preference test measuring the time that individuals spend either near a compartment containing a shoal of conspecifics or an empty one (Ribeiro et al., 2020a,b). When an adult zebrafish is visually presented with both shoal and non-shoal compartments, in a side-by-side configuration, it will spend most of the time in association with the shoal; however, if shoal is not visible, fish tend to explore the entire arena (Fig. 1A,B). In accordance with previous reports (Engeszer et al., 2007; Dreosti et al., 2015), we found social affiliation is an acquired developmental trait that emerges after the third week of life. This was observed both in zebrafish that were repeatedly exposed to the social affiliation behavioral arena throughout development (12-d-old: p = 0.69, n = 15; 14-d-old: p = 0.91, n = 14; 16-d-old: p = 0.73, n = 15; 19-d-old: p = 0.88, n = 17; 22-d-old: p = 0.049, n = 12; 24-d-old: p = 0.07, n = 18; 26-d-old: p = 0.01, n = 17; 28-d-old: p = 0.057, n = 13; 30-d-old: p = 0.17, n = 8; 33-d-old: p < 0.0001, n = 14; 62-d-old: p = 0.002, n = 10; one-sample t test vs theoretical score of 0.5 indicating no preference; Fig. 1C), and in zebrafish that were only exposed to the arena once at a specific developmental time point to avoid habituation to the setup (12-d-old: p = 0.34, n = 16; 19-d-old: p = 0.48, n = 18; 21-d-old: p = 0.85, n = 6; 26-d-old: p = 0.76, n = 10; 36-d-old: p = 0.0007, n = 13; 90-d-old: p < 0.0001, n = 24; one-sample t test vs theoretical score of 0.5 indicating no preference; Fig. 1C).
OXT has long been known to regulate social behaviors across species (Goodson, 2008) and it has been linked to neurodevelopmental disorders that impact social behavior during development and adulthood (Pobbe et al., 2012; Hovey et al., 2014). This makes OXT a good candidate system for investigating neurodevelopmental processes linked to social development. To disentangle the organizational versus activational effects of OXT neurons in the acquisition of adult social affiliation, we used a transgenic line, Tg(oxt:Gal4;UAS:NTR-mCherry), to express NTR protein fused to the mCherry reporter, in OXT neurons (Fig. 1D,E). In the presence of the drug MTZ, NTR produces cytotoxic metabolites, thereby allowing temporally controlled ablation of OXT neurons at different developmental stages (Curado et al., 2007, 2008). We confirmed the expression of the NTR-mCherry fusion protein in the OXT-ergic neuronal population by co-localizing the Tg(oxt:Gal4;UAS:NTR-mCherry) with a transgenic reporter Tg(oxt:EGFP) at different stages of development (8, 14, 24, and 90 d old; Fig. 1E).
In both mammals and fish, OXT is produced by specific cells in the preoptic hypothalamus which can be classified as magnocellular or parvocellular, based on soma size (Sawchenko and Swanson, 1982; Van den Dungen et al., 1982; Knobloch and Grinevich, 2014; Grinevich et al., 2016). We and others have previously shown that zebrafish magnocellular OXT cells are clustered in the anterior-dorsal portion of the cell group, whereas the parvocellular neurons make up the more ventral part of the group (Wircer et al., 2017; Wee et al., 2019). We observed that the NTR-mCherry protein was prominently expressed in the most anterior-dorsal part of the POA (Fig. 1E); thus, it can be considered to localize mainly to magnocellular OXT neurons in both larvae and adult zebrafish. These cells have been shown to project to ventral forebrain in fish (Saito et al., 2004). Also, recent research in rats has shown that these magnocellular cells project to several brains regions including hypothalamus, amygdala, LS, and nucleus accumbens (Zhang et al., 2021), indicating that this projection pattern is evolutionarily conserved. MTZ treatment at all time points was highly effective (>80% decrease) at ablating NTR-mCherry-positive OXT neurons (Fig. 2A,B). To ensure proper ablation of endogenous OXT-ergic cells following MTZ treatment, we also performed fluorescent in situ hybridization with a probe directed against oxt mRNA at 4- to 6-, 12- to 14-, 22- to 24-d-old, and in the adult (Fig. 2C,D). A significant decrease in OXT-expressing cells was observed following MTZ treatment at all tested ages (p = 0.0024 n = 11/10, p = 0.0003 n = 7/7, p = 0.0002 n = 7/8, p = 0.0079 n = 5/4, for 4- to 6-, 12- to 14-, 22- to 24-d-old, and adult ablation untreated/treated, respectively, one tailed Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 2C,D).
To assess the effects of temporal OXT neuronal ablation on adult social affiliation, zebrafish were treated with MTZ at specific time points during development (4- to 6-, 12- to 14-, 22- to 24-d-old) or postpuberty (90-d-old) and social affiliation was assessed in adulthood in comparison to untreated siblings (Fig. 3A,B). We analyzed the social affiliation scores of eight independent cohorts in which OXT neurons were ablated at 4- to 6-d-old and found that despite considerable variation among cohorts during the test, there was a robust effect of MTZ treatment on adult social affiliation (cohorts: p = 0.0081; treatment: p = 0.001; see Materials and Methods, Statistical analysis, for detailed description of the GLM with β regression; Fig. 3C; Table 1). MTZ treatment did not affect overall fish locomotion as measured by the total distance traveled during the trial (cohorts: p = 8.342e-16, treatment: p = 0.668, LM; Fig. 3D; Table 1).
We also observed variation among control adult cohorts not expressing NTR but treated or untreated with MTZ at 4- to 6-d-old, without a main effect of MTZ treatment on either social affiliation score (cohort: p = 0.0001, MTZ treatment: p = 0.2653, GLM with β regression, Fig. 3E; Table 2) or total distance moved (cohort: p < 2e-16, MTZ treatment: p = 0.1536, LM; Fig. 3F; Table 2).
In view of the biological variability observed across cohorts, we further verified the effect of early-life OXT-neuron ablation on social affiliation by using a MC simulation where we randomly sampled 15 treated and 15 untreated 4- to 6-d-old larvae from all eight cohorts and repeated the procedure for 1000 iterations. This analysis also clearly indicated an impairment of adult social affiliation (p < 0.0001, one-tailed Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 3B; see Materials and Methods, Statistical analysis, for details on MC), which was maintained in 12- to 14-d-old MTZ-treated adult fish (p = 0.034, n = 14/14, one-tailed Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 3B).
In contrast to the deficit in social affiliation caused by early life MTZ treatment, ablation of OXT neurons in either 22- to 24- or >90-d-old animals did not affect adult social affiliation (22–24: p = 0.1364, n = 10/9 one-tailed Mann–Whitney test; adults: p = 0.3566, n = 6/7, one-tailed Mann–Whitney test; Fig. 3B). This result coincides with the observation that social affiliation is a developmentally acquired trait which is established before the juvenile stage of development (Fig. 1C; Dreosti et al., 2015).
Taken together, these results show that early developmental ablation of OXT neurons leads to a long-term deficit in social affiliation in adulthood, suggesting that OXT neurons are involved in an early-life developmental process that is required for proper social affiliation later in life.
Early life OXT ablation leads to impairments in specific dopaminergic clusters
Notably, although early-life OXT neuronal ablation induced an impairment in social affiliation, we observed complete recovery of the OXT neural population by adulthood, as ablated fish displayed normal counts of OXT-expressing NTR-mCherry [4- to 6-d-old MTZ-treated (n = 14) vs untreated (n = 15) adult zebrafish: p = 0.33, unpaired t test; Fig. 4A–C]. We next performed a time course analysis for the OXT neural recovery and found that already within one month (i.e., by 42-d-old) from the MTZ treatment there is no discernible difference between treated and control animals (8-d-old, p < 0.0001, n = 15/15; 12-d-old, p < 0.0001, n = 13/7; 19-d-old, p < 0.0001, n = 10/9; 26-d-old, p = 0.0048, n = 12/11; 42-d-old, p = 0.59, n = 9/10; unpaired t test with Welch's correction; Fig. 4D,E). This is in line with previous work showing that zebrafish neurons are capable of regenerating following lesions even in adulthood (Kizil et al., 2012). Conversely, animals in which OXT neurons were ablated in adulthood displayed normal social drive (Fig. 3B) despite the fact that the ablated cell populations had not yet recovered by the time of the behavioral testing (Fig. 2). We verified the recovery of endogenous OXT expression in the oxt:GAL4;UAS-NTR-mCherry transgene, by fluorescent in situ hybridization to oxt mRNA in adult fish subjected to early-life ablation, and found no significant difference from controls [MTZ-treated (n = 4) vs untreated (n = 5) adult zebrafish: p = 0.25, two-tailed Mann–Whitney U test; Fig. 4F,G].
We next hypothesized that the developmental organization of other sociality- modulating neurons might have been affected by early OXT ablation. It has been demonstrated that the dopaminergic neurons regulate social behavior (Dölen and Malenka, 2014; Hung et al., 2017; Johnson et al., 2017). Therefore, we examined whether early perturbation of the OXT neuronal system affected specific TH-positive dopaminergic neuronal clusters, which in zebrafish are anatomically discernible from other TH-positive catecholaminergic cells (Ma, 1994a,b, 1997; Rink and Wullimann, 2001, 2002), and whether these effects persist over the long-term (Figs. 5, 6; Extended Data Fig. 6-1). We observed that already 24 h after ablation of OXT neurons at 4- to 6-d-old, there was a significant decrease in dopaminergic neuronal counts in the PrT (p = 0.009, n = 18/18, LM; Fig. 5A,B; Table 3), and the large neurons of the TP (p = 0.0045, n = 18/18, LM; Fig. 5A,C; Table 3). In contrast, no differences were found in telencephalic DA neurons of the ventral subpallium area (Table 3). The PrT is the teleostean homolog of the superior colliculus, an area known to be involved in gaze control and possibly attention in zebrafish (Antinucci et al., 2019) as well as in mammals (Krauzlis et al., 2004). The TP is the source of the ascending DA system, which is considered analogous to the mammalian ventral tegmental area (VTA; Rink and Wullimann, 2001), which is involved in reward and reinforcement (Morales and Margolis, 2017).
Unlike the ablated OXT cells, which recovered over time, the deficits in dopaminergic neurons observed in early OXT-ablated larvae persisted through adulthood, as adult fish that had been treated with MTZ between 4- and 6-d-old displayed a reduced number of TH-positive dopaminergic cells in the PrT (p = 0.005, n = 7/7; Fig. 6B; Table 4, GLMM). Similarly, OXT ablation caused a decrease in the dopaminergic neurons residing in two subdivisions of the TP, the periventricular nucleus of the TP and the PTN, which are distinguishable only in the adult (TP p = 0.025, n = 7/7; PTN p = 0.005, n = 7/7; Fig. 6C,D; Table 4, GLMM with Poisson regression, planned comparisons and FDR corrections).
To demonstrate that at the time of their ablation, OXT neurons physically interact with the affected dopaminergic neurons, we examined whether OXT neurons form synapses on dopaminergic clusters of the PrT and TP (Fig. 7). We employed a double transgenic line, Tg(oxt:Gal4;UAS:Sypb-EGFP), which genetically expresses the synaptic marker, synaptophysin-GFP in OXT neurons (Anbalagan et al., 2019), and visualized putative synaptic contacts onto TH positive dopaminergic neurons (Fig. 7). To ensure that these are indeed bona-fide OXT release sites, we also co-stained these fish with an anti-OXT antibody (Blechman et al., 2018; Anbalagan et al., 2019; Fig. 7C,D). We found OXT projections forming multiple putative synapses onto the DA neurons of the PrT and the TP (Fig. 7C,D1,D2). We also observed OXT cells directly abutting the TH-positive cells of the TP (Fig. 7D3,D4), raising the possibility that OXT affects nearby dopaminergic cells in a non-synaptic manner as has been shown in other species (Ludwig and Leng, 2006; Son et al., 2013). These results indicate that these two transmitter systems, DA and OXT, are already linked at early developmental stages and that early ablation of OXT neurons irreversibly impairs the development of subsets of dopaminergic neurons, which are key for social affiliation later in life.
Early-life ablation of oxytocinergic neurons leads to impaired social information processing
In view of these results, we next examined whether these long-lasting developmental changes also manifested in the neural processing response to social stimuli in adults, focusing on vertebrate SDMN and mesolimbic reward system (O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011). To this end, early-life ablated (MTZ treated at 4- to 6-d-old) adult zebrafish were exposed to a single visual social stimulus (a shoal of conspecifics comprised of two females and two males) or an empty tank for controls, for 10 min, and their forebrain neuronal activity state was analyzed by immunostaining of phosphorylated ribosomal pS6, a known correlate for neuronal activation (Knight et al., 2012; Fig. 8). We then quantified the number of pS6-positive neurons in several specific brain areas known to be implicated in neural processing of social information and social reward (O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011; Fig. 8; Extended Data Figs. 8-1, 8-2). Results showed that early OXT ablation significantly affected neuronal activity in response to social stimulus in two specific areas: Ppa and in the most anterior part of the ventral nucleus of ventral telencephalon (Vv; GLMM with Poisson regression, planned comparisons, and FDR corrections; Fig. 8B–E; Extended Data Fig. 8-2).
Specifically, we found that while unablated fish exhibited increased activity in the Ppa on exposure to a shoal of conspecifics, this was not observed in early ablated fish (untreated shoal vs untreated empty, p = 0.024, n = 11/7, respectively; MTZ-treated shoal vs MTZ-treated empty, p = 0.88, n = 10/6, respectively; Fig. 8B,C; Extended Data Fig. 8-2). Vv neurons of unablated fish exhibited decreased activity on exposure to a social stimulus, whereas early ablated animals maintained their neuronal activity regardless of the stimulus (untreated shoal vs untreated empty, p = 0.024, n = 11/7, respectively; MTZ-treated shoal vs MTZ-treated empty, p = 0.88, n = 11/7, respectively; Fig. 8D,E; Extended Data Fig. 8-2).
These results show that these two brain areas display deficits in neuronal response to a social stimulus in early OXT-ablated zebrafish. Importantly, the Vv is considered analogous to the mammalian LS, while the Ppa is analogous to the mammalian preoptic hypothalamus (POA; Wullimann and Mueller, 2004; O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011). These areas are core nodes of the social behavior network in all vertebrate species and have a strong reciprocal connection with each other (Wullimann and Mueller, 2004; O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011).
We next examined whether early-life ablation of OXT neurons in the POA altered functional connectivity between forebrain nuclei belonging to the SDMN (O'Connell and Hofmann, 2012), by comparing the correlation matrices of activity levels for the different areas between ablated and non-ablated fish in either basal state or in response to social stimulus (Extended Data Fig. 8-3). We constructed correlation matrices corresponding to each treatment via a resampling scheme, inspired by QAP (Makagon et al., 2012), that additionally allows to choose the threshold for sparsification on the basis of minimal heterogeneity across resampled correlation matrices (see Materials and Methods). To identify functional reconfigurations between different treatments, we identified mesoscale differences in the way the functional networks are organized by their modular (or community) structure. To extract such modules, we performed community detection using the Leiden algorithm (Traag et al., 2019) on the functional connectivity matrix of each treatment. To ensure the robustness of the resulting partitions, we repeated the optimization 400 times per treatment and reconciled the different candidate partitions by considering the central partition (Peixoto, 2021; see final partitions in Fig. 8F). Already by visual inspection it is possible to observe differences in the overall community structure between treatments in the allocation of nodes to communities, and in the relative integration within and across communities. We quantified such integration by calculating the ratio of the total edge weights within communities to the total edge weights between different communities (see Materials and Methods). We found that in the basal state (i.e., when no visual stimulus was presented) the mesoscale functional organization is different between ablated (r = 4.6) and non-ablated fish (r = 7.5), implying that the ablation of OXT neurons in the POA modifies the resting state networks of the forebrain SDMN (Fig. 8F). In particular, ablated fish show a much less segregated network with respect to non-ablated ones. Second, we observed that, in response to the social stimulus (i.e., sight of a conspecific shoal), the functional network of non-ablated fish transitions from a segregated state (r = 7.5) to a more integrated one (r = 3.7), whereas ablated fish showed the opposite pattern, moving from a more integrated basal state (r = 4.6) to a more segregated response network (r = 6.4). All values are significantly different from zero, and also significantly different from each other (p < 0.01). Finally, we completed the network analysis focusing on local differences in terms of network centralities. In particular, we looked at the strength of nodes, representing how strongly a node is connected to its neighbors (Extended Data Fig. 8-4). We found a strong and significant anti-correlation between the ranking of strengths for the ablated versus non-ablated fish in response to the social stimulus (untreated shoal vs MTZ-treated shoal, Spearman r = −0.5, p = 0.01), implying that the hub nodes for non-ablated fish are poorly connected nodes for the ablated and vice versa. Specifically, in untreated fish exposed to the social stimulus, the most connected nodes were the postero-ventral part of the ventral telencephalon (Vvp), followed by the medial zone of the dorsal telencephalic area (Dm), two regions that have been implicated in the regulation of social interactions and motivated behavior in zebrafish (von Trotha et al., 2014; Stednitz et al., 2018). In contrast, both these nodes drop in their centrality rank in the SDMN network of MTZ-treated fish exposed to the social stimulus (from first to fifth and from second to 16th, among 16 nodes, respectively), where the antero-dorsal part of the ventral telencephalon (Vda) and the lateral part of the ventral telencephalon (Vl), two poorly connected nodes in untreated fish (ranking 14th and 16th, out of 16 nodes, respectively), were the most connected nodes (Extended Data Fig. 8-4).
These results indicate that early-life OXT ablation leads to blunted response to shoal-induced changes in brain activity within specific nuclei (Vv and Ppa), and to a wide-ranging alteration in network connectivity spanning several nuclei of the SDMN in both basal state and in response to social stimulus (Fig. 9).
Taken together, our results show that proper response to social stimuli depends on orchestrated co-development of OXT and dopaminergic neurons. We show that during development, OXT has important organizational effects. Early developmental perturbation of OXT neurons led to reduced attraction toward conspecifics, impaired the neurodevelopment of specific dopaminergic neurons, caused a blunted neural response to social stimuli in the forebrain (Ppa and Vv), and altered the connectivity of the SDMN (Fig. 9).
Discussion
Previous research has implicated OXT in various developmental disorders that affect social function in humans, have highlighted its importance in social behavior in animals, and indicated the importance of its communication with other systems, such as DA, serotonin and estrogen for proper social behavior (Heinrichs et al., 2009; Hovey et al., 2014; Grinevich et al., 2016; Grinevich and Stoop, 2018; Rajamani et al., 2018). However, there has been limited investigation into the mechanistic aspects of OXT developmental functions, and how they are linked to its well-described social roles.
Here, we use temporally controlled perturbations of OXT neurons at different developmental timepoints during zebrafish life to show that OXT has a distinct developmental function, namely, to enable proper development of specific downstream dopaminergic clusters known to be involved in visual attention gating and reward (O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011; Antinucci et al., 2019). We also show that in animals where this developmental process was perturbed, the response to social stimuli is affected, and the tendency to shoal with conspecifics is reduced.
The developmentally affected dopaminergic clusters, namely, the PrT (superior colliculus) and the TP (VTA) have been linked in other organisms to social functioning at the level of behavior and, at the molecular level, to the OXT system. Thus, in primates and humans, which like zebrafish are highly dependent on vision for gathering social information, OXTRs are expressed in the superior colliculus (Schorscher-Petcu et al., 2009; Freeman et al., 2014a,b, 2018), and OXT modulates the gaze and oculomotor responses controlled by this nucleus (Leknes et al., 2013; Kret and De Dreu, 2017). This is also true of the VTA, where OXTRs are expressed, with functions to promote sociability (Hung et al., 2017). Notably, rhesus monkeys subjected to early social deprivation displayed a reduction in DA neurons of the VTA (Martin et al., 1991), and mouse knock-out of the autism-associated gene Nlgn3 resulted in impaired OXT signaling in dopaminergic neurons and aberrant activity in the dopaminergic neurons of the VTA (Hörnberg et al., 2020). This further shows how early perturbations to the OXT system can lead to wide-ranging and varied effects on other socially relevant systems in the brain. Whether the changes in dopaminergic neurons, caused the observed changes in social affiliation and neural activity is yet to be directly tested.
In addition to the dopaminergic deficits, we show that in normal fish, neurons of the Ppa (analogous to the mammalian VTA) respond to social stimuli by increased activity, while neurons of the Vv (analogous to the mammalian LS) respond by reducing their activity. In contrast, in developmentally perturbed animals these areas do not change their activity following presentation of the stimulus. Interestingly, the OXTR in the mammalian LS have been implicated in the regulation of social fear (Guzmán et al., 2013; Menon et al., 2018). In zebrafish, the Vv/LS has been functionally associated with social affiliation, mainly through its cholinergic neurons (Stednitz et al., 2018). However, it is interesting to note that in many animal models (e.g., rodents, teleosts, and cartilaginous fish), including zebrafish, the Vv/LS has been shown to contain mainly GABAergic neurons, and to be a source of GABAergic input to the VTA, a putative homolog of the TP, encompassing the TP and PTN, in zebrafish (O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011), and to the POA (O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011; Vega-Quiroga et al., 2018). Thus, it is possible that the observed reduced activity in Vv/LS corresponds to GABAergic neurons that project to the Ppa and TP. These areas are core nodes of the social behavior network in all vertebrate species and have a strong reciprocal connection with each other (Wullimann and Mueller, 2004; O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011). This suggests that social stimuli could promote a disinhibition in these target areas, which would be associated with the rewarding and/or motivational component of social affiliation. As a corollary, in early-ablated animals, where Vv activity remains high in response to social stimuli, the TP would remain inhibited, and the rewarding component of social affiliation would be attenuated, leading to decreased display of this behavior.
In addition, we show that the overall coactivation patterns of forebrain nuclei involved in social processing are altered in developmentally perturbed animals, both at baseline and following presentation of a social stimulus. In other words, we show that the connectivity between the different nodes of the SDMN changes as a result of early life OXT ablation. We submit that the proper maturation of this network at the level of neural architecture is dependent on early-life signaling by OXT neurons.
In summary, our results demonstrate that OXT neurons are required for the developmental acquisition of social affiliation and exert an organizational effect on dopaminergic neuronal populations. Furthermore, our results suggest that this role involves orchestrating the co-development and maturation of several brain systems, such as the midbrain social visual processing and attention systems (the PrT), social decision-making areas in the forebrain and ascending DA centers associated with reward. When the OXT-dependent developmental process is perturbed, neural responses to social stimuli in these regions become compromised, important parts of the SDMN fail to synchronize their activity, and eventually, the propensity to spend time near a shoal of conspecifics is reduced.
Footnotes
We thank all members of Gil Levkowitz's and Rui Oliveira's laboratories for fruitful discussions and Nitzan Konstantin for English editing. We also thank the technical support of Instituto Gulbenkian de Ciência (IGC)'s Advanced Imaging Facility (AIF-UIC), which is supported by the National Portuguese Funding Grant PPBI-POCI-01-0145-FEDER-022122, co-financed by Lisboa Regional Operational Program (Lisboa 2020), under the Portugal 2020 Partnership Agreement, through the European Regional Development Fund (FEDER) and Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT; Portugal); all the staff from the Fish Facility platforms of IGC, Portugal, and Weizmann Institute of Science, Israel, for animal care and valuable advice; Congento, which is supported by the National Portuguese Funding Grant LISBOA-01-0145-FEDER-022170, co-financed by Lisboa Regional Operational Program (Lisboa 2020), under the Portugal 2020 Partnership Agreement, through the FEDER and FCT; IGC's Histopathology Facility for technical support, help, and valuable advices; and the Advanced BioImaging and BioOptics Experimental Platform (ABBE Platform) of the Champalimaud Center for the Unknown (CCU) for all the technical support and help. A.R.N. was supported by the Short-Term EMBO Fellowship ASTF 420-2013, the Weizmann's Dean of Faculty postdoctoral fellowship at the Weizmann Institute, Israel, and by the FCT Grant SFRH/BPD/93317/2013 at IGC. M.G., E.W., and J.B. were supported by the Israel Science Foundation Grant 1511/16, the United States-Israel Binational Science Foundation Grant 2017325, and Sagol Institute for Longevity and Estate of Emile Mimran. G.L. is an incumbent of the Elias Sourasky Professorial Chair. This work was supported by the project LISBOA-01-0145-FEDER-030627 co-funded by the Programa Operational Regional de Lisboa (Lisboa 2020), through Portugal 2020 and the FEDER and by the FCT/Ministério da Ciência, Tecnologia e Ensino Superior (MCTES) through national funds (Programa de Investimento e Despesas de Desenvolvimento da Administração Central).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Anbalagan S, Gordon L, Blechman J, Matsuoka RL, Rajamannar P, Wircer E, Biran J, Reuveny A, Leshkowitz D, Stainier DYR, Levkowitz G (2018) Pituicyte cues regulate the development of permeable neuro-vascular interfaces. Dev Cell 47:711–726.e5. 10.1016/j.devcel.2018.10.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anbalagan S, Blechman J, Gliksberg M, Gordon L, Rotkopf R, Dadosh T, Shimoni E, Levkowitz G (2019) Robo2 regulates synaptic oxytocin content by affecting actin dynamics. Elife 8:e45650. 10.7554/eLife.45650 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antinucci P, Folgueira M, Bianco IH (2019) Pretectal neurons control hunting behaviour. Elife 8:e48114. 10.7554/eLife.48114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Ochsner KN (2011) Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn Sci 15:301–309. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67. [Google Scholar]
- Biswal BB, Van Kylen J, Hyde JS (1997) Simultaneous assessment of flow and BOLD signals in resting-state functional connectivity maps. NMR Biomed 10:165–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blechman J, Amir-Zilberstein L, Gutnick A, Ben-Dor S, Levkowitz G (2011) The metabolic regulator PGC-1α directly controls the expression of the hypothalamic neuropeptide oxytocin. J Neurosci 31:14835–14840. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blechman J, Anbalagan S, Matthews GG, Levkowitz G (2018) Genome editing reveals idiosyncrasy of CNGA2 ion channel-directed antibody immunoreactivity toward oxytocin. Front Cell Dev Biol 6:117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borgatti SP, Everett MG, Johnson JC (2013) Analyzing social networks. Beverley Hills: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Borges AC, Pereira N, Franco M, Vale L, Pereira M, Cunha MV, Amaro A, Albuquerque T, Rebelo M (2016) Implementation of a zebrafish health program in a research facility: a 4-year retrospective study. Zebrafish 13:S115–S125. 10.1089/zeb.2015.1230 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowen MT, Neumann ID (2017) Rebalancing the addicted brain: oxytocin interference with the neural substrates of addiction. Trends Neurosci 40:691–708. 10.1016/j.tins.2017.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cribari-Neto F, Zeileis A (2010) Beta regression in R. Journal of Statistical Software 34:1–24. 10.18637/jss.v034.i02 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Curado S, Anderson RM, Jungblut B, Mumm J, Schroeter E, Stainier DYR (2007) Conditional targeted cell ablation in zebrafish: a new tool for regeneration studies. Dev Dyn 236:1025–1035. 10.1002/dvdy.21100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curado S, Stainier DYR, Anderson RM (2008) Nitroreductase-mediated cell/tissue ablation in zebrafish: a spatially and temporally controlled ablation method with applications in developmental and regeneration studies. Nat Protoc 3:948–954. 10.1038/nprot.2008.58 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davison JM, Akitake CM, Goll MG, Rhee JM, Gosse N, Baier H, Halpern ME, Leach SD, Parsons MJ (2007) Transactivation from Gal4-VP16 transgenic insertions for tissue-specific cell labeling and ablation in zebrafish. Dev Biol 304:811–824. 10.1016/j.ydbio.2007.01.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Vico Fallani F, Latora V, Chavez M (2017) A topological criterion for filtering information in complex brain networks. PLOS Comput Biol 13:e1005305. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005305 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delignette-Muller ML, Dutang C (2015) fitdistrplus: an R package for fitting distributions. J Stat Softw 64:1–34. [Google Scholar]
- Dölen G, Malenka RC (2014) The emerging role of nucleus accumbens oxytocin in social cognition. Biol Psychiatry 76:354–355. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donaldson ZR, Young LJ (2008) Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science 322:900–904. 10.1126/science.1158668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dreosti E, Lopes G, Kampff AR, Wilson SW (2015) Development of social behavior in young zebrafish. Front Neural Circuits 9:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff SB, Müller V (2015) Functional connectivity. In: Brain mapping (Toga AW, ed), pp 187–201. San Diego: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Engeszer RE, Barbiano LD, Ryan MJ, Parichy DM (2007) Timing and plasticity of shoaling behaviour in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Anim Behav 74:1269–1275. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman SM, Inoue K, Smith AL, Goodman MM, Young LJ (2014a) The neuroanatomical distribution of oxytocin receptor binding and mRNA in the male rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta). Psychoneuroendocrinology 45:128–141. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.03.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman SM, Walum H, Inoue K, Smith AL, Goodman MM, Bales KL, Young LJ (2014b) Neuroanatomical distribution of oxytocin and vasopressin 1a receptors in the socially monogamous coppery titi monkey (Callicebus cupreus). Neuroscience 273:12–23. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.04.055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman SM, Palumbo MC, Lawrence RH, Smith AL, Goodman MM, Bales KL (2018) Effect of age and autism spectrum disorder on oxytocin receptor density in the human basal forebrain and midbrain. Transl Psychiatry 8:257. 10.1038/s41398-018-0315-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ (1994) Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: a synthesis. Hum Brain Mapp 2:56–78. 10.1002/hbm.460020107 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gamer M, Zurowski B, Büchel C (2010) Different amygdala subregions mediate valence-related and attentional effects of oxytocin in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:9400–9405. 10.1073/pnas.1000985107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodson JL (2008) Nonapeptides and the evolutionary patterning of sociality. Prog Brain Res 170:3–15. 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00401-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grinevich V, Stoop R (2018) Interplay between oxytocin and sensory systems in the orchestration of socio-emotional behaviors. Neuron 99:887–904. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grinevich V, Knobloch-Bollmann HS, Eliava M, Busnelli M, Chini B (2016) Assembling the puzzle: pathways of oxytocin signaling in the brain. Biol Psychiatry 79:155–164. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guastella AJ, Hickie IB (2016) Oxytocin treatment, circuitry, and autism: a critical review of the literature placing oxytocin into the autism context. Biol Psychiatry 79:234–242. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.06.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guzmán YF, Tronson NC, Jovasevic V, Sato K, Guedea AL, Mizukami H, Nishimori K, Radulovic J (2013) Fear-enhancing effects of septal oxytocin receptors. Nat Neurosci 16:1185–1187. 10.1038/nn.3465 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammock EAD (2015) Developmental perspectives on oxytocin and vasopressin. Neuropsychopharmacology 40:24–42. 10.1038/npp.2014.120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrichs M, von Dawans B, Domes G (2009) Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol 30:548–557. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hörnberg H, Pérez-Garci E, Schreiner D, Hatstatt-Burklé L, Magara F, Baudouin S, Matter A, Nacro K, Pecho-Vrieseling E, Scheiffele P (2020) Rescue of oxytocin response and social behaviour in a mouse model of autism. Nature 584:252–256. 10.1038/s41586-020-2563-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hovey D, Zettergren A, Jonsson L, Melke J, Anckarsäter H, Lichtenstein P, Westberg L (2014) Associations between oxytocin-related genes and autistic-like traits. Soc Neurosci 9:378–386. 10.1080/17470919.2014.897995 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hung LW, Neuner S, Polepalli JS, Beier KT, Wright M, Walsh JJ, Lewis EM, Luo L, Deisseroth K, Dölen G, Malenka RC (2017) Gating of social reward by oxytocin in the ventral tegmental area. Science 357:1406–1411. 10.1126/science.aan4994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel TR, Shapiro LE (1992) Oxytocin receptor distribution reflects social organization in monogamous and polygamous voles. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:5981–5985. 10.1073/pnas.89.13.5981 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson ZV, Walum H, Xiao Y, Riefkohl PC, Young LJ (2017) Oxytocin receptors modulate a social salience neural network in male prairie voles. Horm Behav 87:16–24. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jurek B, Neumann ID (2018) The oxytocin receptor: from intracellular signaling to behavior. Physiol Rev 98:1805–1908. 10.1152/physrev.00031.2017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keebaugh AC, Young LJ (2011) Increasing oxytocin receptor expression in the nucleus accumbens of pre-pubertal female prairie voles enhances alloparental responsiveness and partner preference formation as adults. Horm Behav 60:498–504. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.07.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kizil C, Kaslin J, Kroehne V, Brand M (2012) Adult neurogenesis and brain regeneration in zebrafish. Dev Neurobiol 72:429–461. 10.1002/dneu.20918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knight ZA, Tan K, Birsoy K, Schmidt S, Garrison JL, Wysocki RW, Emiliano A, Ekstrand MI, Friedman JM (2012) Molecular profiling of activated neurons by phosphorylated ribosome capture. Cell 151:1126–1137. 10.1016/j.cell.2012.10.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knobloch HS, Grinevich V (2014) Evolution of oxytocin pathways in the brain of vertebrates. Front Behav Neurosci 8:31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krauzlis RJ, Liston D, Carello CD (2004) Target selection and the superior colliculus: goals, choices and hypotheses. Vision Res 44:1445–1451. 10.1016/j.visres.2004.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kret ME, De Dreu CKW (2017) Pupil-mimicry conditions trust in partners: moderation by oxytocin and group membership. Proc Biol Sci 284:20162554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leknes S, Wessberg J, Ellingsen D-M, Chelnokova O, Olausson H, Laeng B (2013) Oxytocin enhances pupil dilation and sensitivity to 'hidden' emotional expressions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8:741–749. 10.1093/scan/nss062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenth R (2020) emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R package version 1.4.7. [Google Scholar]
- Lorenzi E, Mayer U, Rosa-Salva O, Vallortigara G (2017) Dynamic features of animate motion activate septal and preoptic areas in visually naïve chicks (Gallus gallus). Neuroscience 354:54–68. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Love TM (2014) Oxytocin, motivation and the role of dopamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 119:49–60. 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.06.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludwig M, Leng G (2006) Dendritic peptide release and peptide-dependent behaviours. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:126–136. 10.1038/nrn1845 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukas M, Toth I, Reber SO, Slattery DA, Veenema AH, Neumann ID (2011) The neuropeptide oxytocin facilitates pro-social behavior and prevents social avoidance in rats and mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 36:2159–2168. 10.1038/npp.2011.95 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma PM (1994a) Catecholaminergic systems in the zebrafish. II. Projection pathways and pattern of termination of the locus coeruleus. J Comp Neurol 344:256–269. 10.1002/cne.903440207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma PM (1994b) Catecholaminergic systems in the zebrafish. I. Number, morphology, and histochemical characteristics of neurons in the locus coeruleus. J Comp Neurol 344:242–255. 10.1002/cne.903440206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma PM (1997) Catecholaminergic systems in the zebrafish. III. organization and projection pattern of medullary dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons. J Comp Neurol 381:411–427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Machluf Y, Levkowitz G (2011) Visualization of mRNA expression in the zebrafish embryo. Methods Mol Biol 714:83–102. 10.1007/978-1-61779-005-8_6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makagon MM, McCowan B, Mench JA (2012) How can social network analysis contribute to social behavior research in applied ethology? Appl Anim Behav Sci 138:152–161. 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin LJ, Spicer DM, Lewis MH, Gluck JP, Cork LC (1991) Social deprivation of infant rhesus monkeys alters the chemoarchitecture of the brain: I. Subcortical regions. J Neurosci 11:3344–3358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon R, Grund T, Zoicas I, Althammer F, Fiedler D, Biermeier V, Bosch OJ, Hiraoka Y, Nishimori K, Eliava M, Grinevich V, Neumann ID (2018) Oxytocin signaling in the lateral septum prevents social fear during lactation. Curr Biol 28:1066–1078.e6. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.02.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer MP, Smith SJ (2006) Evidence from in vivo imaging that synaptogenesis guides the growth and branching of axonal arbors by two distinct mechanisms. J Neurosci 26:3604–3614. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0223-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller TV, Caldwell HK (2015) Oxytocin during development: possible organizational effects on behavior. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 6:76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morales M, Margolis EB (2017) Ventral tegmental area: cellular heterogeneity, connectivity and behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 18:73–85. 10.1038/nrn.2016.165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noonan LR, Continella G, Pedersen CA (1989) Neonatal administration of oxytocin increases novelty-induced grooming in the adult rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 33:555–558. 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90386-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nunes AR, Ruhl N, Winberg S, Oliveira RF (2017) Social phenotypes in zebrafish. In: The rights and wrongs of zebrafish: behavioral phenotyping of zebrafish, pp 95–130. Basel: Springer International Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Nunes AR, Carreira L, Anbalagan S, Blechman J, Levkowitz G, Oliveira RF (2020) Perceptual mechanisms of social affiliation in zebrafish. Sci Rep 10:1–14. 10.1038/s41598-020-60154-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connell LA, Hofmann HA (2011) The Vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis. J Comp Neurol 519:3599–3639. 10.1002/cne.22735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connell LA, Hofmann HA (2012) Evolution of a vertebrate social decision-making network. Science 336:1154–1157. 10.1126/science.1218889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panula P, Chen YC, Priyadarshini M, Kudo H, Semenova S, Sundvik M, Sallinen V (2010) The comparative neuroanatomy and neurochemistry of zebrafish CNS systems of relevance to human neuropsychiatric diseases. Neurobiol Dis 40:46–57. 10.1016/j.nbd.2010.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park MJ, Seo BA, Lee B, Shin HS, Kang MG (2018) Stress-induced changes in social dominance are scaled by AMPA-type glutamate receptor phosphorylation in the medial prefrontal cortex. Sci Rep 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parker MO, Brock AJ, Walton RT, Brennan CH (2013) The role of zebrafish (Danio rerio) in dissecting the genetics and neural circuits of executive function. Front Neural Circuits 7:63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peixoto TP (2021) Revealing consensus and dissensus between network partitions. Phys Rev X 11:021003. [Google Scholar]
- Phoenix CH, Goy RW, Geral AA, Young WC (1959) Organizing action of prenatally administered testosterone propionate on the tissues mediating mating behavior in the female guinea pig. Endocrinology 65:369–382. 10.1210/endo-65-3-369 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pobbe RLH, Pearson BL, Defensor EB, Bolivar VJ, Young WS, Lee H-J, Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ, Blanchard RJ (2012) Oxytocin receptor knockout mice display deficits in the expression of autism-related behaviors. Horm Behav 61:436–444. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.10.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R_core_Team (2020) R: a language and environment for statistical computing, Vienna, Austria. Available at https://www.R-project.org/.
- Rajamani KT, Wagner S, Grinevich V, Harony-Nicolas H (2018) Oxytocin as a modulator of synaptic plasticity: implications for neurodevelopmental disorders. Front Synaptic Neurosci 10:17. 10.3389/fnsyn.2018.00017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ribeiro D, Nunes AR, Gliksberg M, Anbalagan S, Levkowitz G, Oliveira RF (2020a) Oxytocin receptor signalling modulates novelty recognition but not social preference in zebrafish. J Neuroendocrinol 32:e12834. 10.1111/jne.12834 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ribeiro D, Nunes AR, Teles M, Anbalagan S, Blechman J, Levkowitz G, Oliveira RF (2020b) Genetic variation in the social environment affects behavioral phenotypes of oxytocin receptor mutants in zebrafish. Elife 9:e56973. 10.7554/eLife.56973 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rink E, Wullimann MF (2001) The teleostean (zebrafish) dopaminergic system ascending to the subpallium (striatum) is located in the basal diencephalon (posterior tuberculum). Brain Res 889:316–330. 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)03174-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rink E, Wullimann MF (2002) Connections of the ventral telencephalon and tyrosine hydroxylase distribution in the zebrafish brain (Danio rerio) lead to identification of an ascending dopaminergic system in a teleost. Brain Res Bull 57:385–387. 10.1016/S0361-9230(01)00696-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson KJ, Bosch OJ, Levkowitz G, Busch KE, Jarman AP, Ludwig M (2019) Social creatures: model animal systems for studying the neuroendocrine mechanisms of social behaviour. J Neuroendocrinol 31:e12807. 10.1111/jne.12807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saito D, Komatsuda M, Urano A (2004) Functional organization of preoptic vasotocin and isotocin neurons in the brain of rainbow trout: central and neurohypophysial projections of single neurons. Neuroscience 124:973–984. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.12.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sallinen V, Torkko V, Sundvik M, Reenilä I, Khrustalyov D, Kaslin J, Panula P (2009) MPTP and MPP+ target specific aminergic cell populations in larval zebrafish. J Neurochem 108:719–731. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05793.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sawchenko PE, Swanson LW (1982) Immunohistochemical identification of neurons in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus that project to the medulla or to the spinal cord in the rat. J Comp Neurol 205:260–272. 10.1002/cne.902050306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, Rueden C, Saalfeld S, Schmid B, Tinevez JY, White DJ, Hartenstein V, Eliceiri K, Tomancak P, Cardona A (2012) Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat Methods 9:676–682. 10.1038/nmeth.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schorscher-Petcu A, Dupré A, Tribollet E (2009) Distribution of vasopressin and oxytocin binding sites in the brain and upper spinal cord of the common marmoset. Neurosci Lett 461:217–222. 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.06.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shamay-Tsoory SG, Abu-Akel A (2016) The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol Psychiatry 79:194–202. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singmann H, Bolker B, Westfall J (2020) afex: analysis of factorial experiments. R Package version 0.27.22. [Google Scholar]
- Son SJ, Filosa JA, Potapenko ES, Biancardi VC, Zheng H, Patel KP, Tobin VA, Ludwig M, Stern JE (2013) Dendritic peptide release mediates interpopulation crosstalk between neurosecretory and preautonomic networks. Neuron 78:1036–1049. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.04.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stednitz SJ, McDermott EM, Ncube D, Tallafuss A, Eisen JS, Washbourne P (2018) Forebrain control of behaviorally driven social orienting in zebrafish. Curr Biol 28:2445–2451.e3. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.06.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tay TL, Ronneberger O, Ryu S, Nitschke R, Driever W (2011) Comprehensive catecholaminergic projectome analysis reveals single-neuron integration of zebrafish ascending and descending dopaminergic systems. Nat Commun 2:171. 10.1038/ncomms1171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teles MC, Almeida O, Lopes J, Oliveira RF (2015) Social interactions elicit rapid shifts in functional connectivity in the social decision-making network of zebrafish. Proc Biol Sci 282:20151099. 10.1098/rspb.2015.1099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traag VA, Waltman L, van Eck NJ (2019) From Louvain to Leiden: guaranteeing well-connected communities. Sci Rep 9:1–12. 10.1038/s41598-019-41695-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Trotha JW, Vernier P, Bally-Cuif L (2014) Emotions and motivated behavior converge on an amygdala-like structure in the zebrafish. Eur J Neurosci 40:3302–3315. 10.1111/ejn.12692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van den Dungen HM, Buijs RM, Pool CW, Terlou M (1982) The distribution of vasotocin and isotocin in the brain of the rainbow trout. J Comp Neurol 212:146–157. 10.1002/cne.902120205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vega-Quiroga I, Yarur HE, Gysling K (2018) Lateral septum stimulation disinhibits dopaminergic neurons in the antero-ventral region of the ventral tegmental area: role of GABA-A alpha 1 receptors. Neuropharmacology 128:76–85. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2017.09.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wee CL, Nikitchenko M, Wang WC, Luks-Morgan SJ, Song E, Gagnon JA, Randlett O, Bianco IH, Lacoste AMB, Glushenkova E, Barrios JP, Schier AF, Kunes S, Engert F, Douglass AD (2019) Zebrafish oxytocin neurons drive nocifensive behavior via brainstem premotor targets. Nat Neurosci 22:1477–1492. 10.1038/s41593-019-0452-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wircer E, Blechman J, Borodovsky N, Tsoory M, Nunes AR, Oliveira RF, Levkowitz G (2017) Homeodomain protein otp affects developmental neuropeptide switching in oxytocin neurons associated with a long-term effect on social behavior. Elife 6:e22170. 10.7554/eLife.22170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wullimann MF, Mueller T (2004) Teleostean and mammalian forebrains contrasted: evidence from genes to behavior. J Comp Neurol 475:143–162. 10.1002/cne.20183 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wullimann MF, Rupp B, Reichert H (1996) Neuroanatomy of the zebrafish brain. Basel: Birkhäuser. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang B, Qiu L, Xiao W, Ni H, Chen L, Wang F, Mai W, Wu J, Bao A, Hu H, Gong H, Duan S, Li A, Gao Z (2021) Reconstruction of the hypothalamo-neurohypophysial system and functional dissection of magnocellular oxytocin neurons in the brain. Neuron 109:331–346.e7. 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.10.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.