Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2021 Apr 13;45(4):506–517. doi: 10.1007/s11031-021-09878-2

Compassion protects against vital exhaustion and negative emotionality

Aino Saarinen 1,2,, Liisa Keltikangas-Järvinen 2, Essi Viding 3, Henrik Dobewall 1, Kaisa Kaseva 4, Terho Lehtimäki 5,6, Olli Raitakari 7,8,9, Mirka Hintsanen 1
PMCID: PMC8550749  PMID: 34720257

Abstract

We investigated (i) the predictive relationships of compassion with negative emotionality (a marker of susceptibility to stress) and vital exhaustion (a marker of chronic stress response) and (ii) the effect of compassion on the developmental courses of negative emotionality and vital exhaustion over a follow-up from early adulthood to middle age. We used the prospective Young Finns data (n = 1031–1495, aged 20–50). Compassion was evaluated in 1997, 2001, and 2012; and vital exhaustion and negative emotionality in 2001, 2007, and 2012. The predictive paths from compassion to vital exhaustion and negative emotionality were stronger than vice versa: high compassion predicted lower vital exhaustion and lower negative emotionality. The effect of high compassion on lower vital exhaustion and lower negative emotionality was evident from early adulthood to middle age. Overall, high compassion appears to protect against dimensions of stress from early adulthood to middle age, whereas this study found no evidence that dimensions of stress could reduce disposition to feel compassion for others’ distress over a long-term follow-up.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s11031-021-09878-2.

Keywords: Compassion, Personality, Stress, Psychosocial stress, Longitudinal

Introduction

The widely used classification for mental disorders, DSM-5 (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition), lists stress responses as a crucial trigger for almost all psychiatric disorders (American Psychiatric Association (APA), 2013). Additionally, stress responses also predisposes to the incidence of a wide range of somatic diseases, such as coronary heart disease (Steptoe & Kivimäki, 2012), cancer (Chida et al., 2008), inflammatory diseases (Black, 2002), and gastrointestinal disorders (Mönnikes et al., 2001). Taken together, stress responses compose a major risk for both mental and somatic health.

Consequently, a wide variety of stress-reducing interventions have been developed, ranging from practical life-style interventions (Merrill et al., 2007) to cognitive-behavioral therapies and hypnotic treatments (Bryant et al., 2005). In the recent years, an increasing amount of research has been directed to the development of compassion-cultivating interventions aiming to reduce stress (e.g. Abelson et al., 2014; Gilbert, 2014; Jazaieri et al., 2013). By definition, compassion includes an affective component (i.e. feeling concern for other’s distress) and a motivational component (i.e. the desire to alleviate other’s distress) (Gilbert, 2019; Goetz et al., 2010).

Stress can be measured from a variety of perspectives: amount of exposure to stressors, susceptibility to stress responses, or actualized stress responses (Epel et al., 2018). Stress responses, in turn, can refer to psychological, physiological, or neural responses and to short-term (acute) or long-term responses (chronic responses leading to allostatic load) (Epel et al., 2018). In this study, we aimed to investigate the relationships of compassion with two stress-related dimensions: vital exhaustion and negative emotionality. Vital exhaustion refers to a marker of chronic stress responses (Koertge et al., 2002). High vital exhaustion is found to associate with altered cardiac reactivity to task-induced stress (Keltikangas-Järvinen & Heponiemi, 2004), reduced habituation of free cortisol responses to repeated acute psychosocial stress (Kudielka et al., 2006), increased s-prolactin (Halford et al., 2003), and altered cortisol levels and higher perceived stress (Nicolson & van Diest, 2000). Hence, vital exhaustion refers to chronically elevated stress responses that remain despite there would not be any current (acute) exposure to stress. Negative emotionality, in turn, is a marker of dispositional susceptibility to stress, i.e. a relatively stable personality trait predisposing to elevated stress levels in response to even comparatively mild environmental stressors (Buss & Plomin, 1984; Ebner & Singewald, 2017; Strelau & Angleitner, 1991). There is evidence that high negative emotionality predicts higher stress at work (Hintsanen et al., 2011), elevated cortisol levels in response to conflicts (Laurent & Powers, 2007), and higher depression and anxiety (Skipstein et al., 2012).

There are several theoretical models about the relationship between compassion and stress responses. Firstly, compassion includes a non-judgemental attitude toward others’ negative affective states that is proposed to simultaneously enhance affective regulation and tolerance of one’s own distress (Gilbert, 2019; Kirby & Gilbert, 2017). Secondly, feelings of social belongingness are suggested to compose a fundamental evolutionary need for the human being (Brown & Brown, 2017). A lack of compassionate feelings may interfere with forming and maintaining balanced social relationships and cause distress via social rejection and interpersonal conflicts (Brown & Brown, 2017). Thirdly, it has been postulated that compassionate inter-dependence between family members and close acquaintances activates socioemotional brain networks (e.g. amygdala and insula) and triggers oxytocin release that, in turn, is suggested to increase social pleasure and have inhibitory effects on sympathetic stress responses (Brown & Brown, 2017). In addition, the “Tend and Befriend” theory postulates that, in case of threat, human beings are prone to respond in a compassionate way (i.e. to affiliate and comfort each other). This compassionate response, in turn, is proposed to associate with an “affiliative neurocircuitry” that triggers oxytocin (i.e. increased caring behavior toward in-group members) and endogenous opioids (i.e. increased responses to social pain and pleasure) (Taylor, 2011).

Regarding previous literature, it appears that experiencing compassion for others may also reduce one’s own stress responses. To date, compassion-based interventions are shown to predict lower psychological stress responses over a 2-week follow-up (Matos et al., 2017), lower self-reported psychological stress responses over a 2-month follow-up (Brito-Pons et al., 2018), and lower self-reported stress reactivity to psychosocial stressors over a 9-month follow-up (Engert et al., 2017). Regarding physiological correlates of stress, compassion-based interventions are found to predict reduced C-reactive protein (CRP) levels over a 6-week follow-up (Pace et al., 2013) and lower inflammatory responses to acute stress after a 6-week follow-up (Pace et al., 2009). Moreover, compassion-based interventions are found to predict other stress-related qualities: for example, reduced worry over a 9-week follow-up (Jazaieri et al., 2014) and decreased negative emotionality after a 1-day-long intervention (Klimecki et al., 2014). Finally, compassion-focused therapy interventions are found to reduce rumination (i.e. to move attentional focus from one’s own worries to the needs of others) (Frostadottir & Dorjee, 2019). Regarding negative affect, however, three intervention studies found that compassion training did not reduce negative affect (Engen & Singer, 2015; Klimecki et al., 2013, 2014).

A major limitation of the compassion-based intervention studies (e.g. loving-kindness meditation), however, is that they provide only partial evidence of compassion’s contribution to dimensions of stress. That is, it remains uncertain whether it is increased compassion or some other element of the intervention that has the alleviating effect on dimensions of stress. A second prominent limitation of the compassion-based intervention studies is that they are not able to elucidate the direction of relationships between compassion and stress in everyday life: that is, whether dimensions of stress could also predict a decrease in compassion. Consequently, it is necessary to investigate the relationship of compassion with dimensions of stress using large population-based datasets with long prospective follow-ups that provide possibilities to control a wider set of covariates and to use such statistical analyses that increase understanding about predictive relationships between compassion and stress.

Many pieces of previous evidence suggest that compassion may predict alterations in stress levels. Specifically, compassionate states and dispositional compassion are related to various biomarkers for stress reactivity (Cosley et al., 2010; Stellar et al., 2015). High compassion may also promote psychological well-being (Saarinen et al., 2019) and protect against the development of hypertension (Saarinen et al., 2020) that is closely related to physiological stress. Furthermore, it is known that compassionate feelings commonly lead to behavioral acts that aim to alleviate others’ suffering such as affiliative behavior (Gilbert, 2017; Gilbert & Mascaro, 2017) that, in turn, can buffer against stress responses (DeVries et al., 2003). Finally, high compassion associates with more favorable health behaviors (Gluschkoff et al., 2018) that, in turn, may enhance stress coping. To the best of our knowledge, no study has investigated the relationship of compassion and vital exhaustion. Regarding negative affect, three studies have found that dispositional compassion for others’ suffering may not associate with negative affect (Arimitsu et al., 2019; López et al., 2018; Medvedev et al., 2020).

Several pathways describing how dimensions of stress might predict lower level of compassion for others have also been proposed. First, some degree of psychological well-being is suggested to be a necessary requirement to be able to feel compassion for others’ suffering. For example, in environments with high levels of stress and life dissatisfaction (e.g. workplace bullying), individuals are more prone to display aggression toward others and to dating violence (e.g. Hauge et al., 2009; Hershcovis et al., 2007; Lewis & Fremouw, 2001). Secondly, stress hormones are found to affect the processing of social cues and to be involved in the initiation of social aggression (Montoya et al., 2012) that, in turn, correlate strongly with a lower level of compassion (García et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2012). Finally, neurobiological studies suggest that stressor exposure may affect the functioning of such brain regions that are responsible for the processing of socioemotional cues and compassionate feelings. For example, exposure to stressors is linked to alterations in the plasticity of amygdala (Roozendaal et al., 2009) and altered opioid release in the nucleus accumbens (Marinelli et al., 2004). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens, in turn, are found to be involved when experiencing compassion for others (Valk et al., 2017; Weng et al., 2013, 2018).

Not only do we lack clarity regarding the direction of the association between compassion and dimensions of stress, we also do not have a good understanding of how stable this association is across different ages. In compassion-based intervention studies, the follow-ups have ranged from a few hours (Abelson et al., 2014) to 9 months (Engert et al., 2017). The non-intervention studies investigating the association between compassion and stress have been cross-sectional (e.g. Cosley et al., 2010; Stellar et al., 2015). Even though cross-sectional studies provide possibilities to investigate the state-level correlations between compassion and stress, prospective follow-ups are vital for investigating the stability of the association between compassion and dimensions of stress across different ages and developmental transitions. There is evidence that the effect of dispositional traits on stress responses may vary over age (Mroczek & Almeida, 2004). To date, it has remained unclear whether the association of compassion with dimensions of stress could change across development or be evident from early adulthood to middle age.

The first aim of the current study was to investigate the predictive relationships of compassion with negative emotionality (i.e. a marker of susceptibility to stress) and vital exhaustion (i.e. a marker of chronic stress responses). That is, whether compassion predicts changes in the levels of negative emotionality and vital exhaustion or vice versa. The second aim was to investigate the effect of compassion on the developmental courses of negative emotionality and vital exhaustion over a long-term prospective follow-up, ranging from early adulthood to middle age. We used population-based data with an over 10-year prospective follow-up and several measurement points. The data also provided possibilities to take into consideration several potential confounders: age, sex, participants’ and their parents socioeconomic factors (level of income and educational level), and agreeableness. On the basis of previous theoretical models (Brown & Brown, 2017; Gilbert, 2019; Kirby & Gilbert, 2017; Taylor, 2011), we had the following research hypotheses: (1) high compassion predicts lower negative emotionality and vital exhaustion over the follow-up, and (2) the predictive pathways are more likely to proceed from high compassion to lower levels of negative emotionality and vital exhaustion than vice versa.

Methods

Participants

The participants were members of the prospective longitudinal Young Finns Study. The participants were selected randomly from six birth cohorts (born in 1962, 1965, 1968, 1971, 1974, and 1977) from the population register of the Social Insurance Institution. The original sample included 3596 participants at the baseline measurement in 1980. Since then, eight follow-up measurements have been conducted between 1983 and 2012 (when participants were aged 35‒50 years). The Young Finns Study was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Additionally, all the Finnish universities with medical schools approved the study design. Before participation, all the participants or their parents (for participants aged below 12 years) provided written informed consent after the nature of the procedures had been fully explained. The design and methods of the Young Finns Study is described more precisely elsewhere (Raitakari et al., 2008).

For the current study, compassion was assessed in 1997, 2001, and 2012; vital exhaustion in 2001, 2007, and 2012; negative emotionality in 1997, 2001, 2007, and 2012; agreeableness in 2007 and 2012; parents’ level of income and educational level in 1980; and participants’ level of income and educational level in 2011. We included in the analyses all the participants who had data available on all study variables in at least one of the measurement times (e.g. data on vital exhaustion in 2001, 2007, or 2012; data on negative emotionality in 1997, 2001, 2007, or 2012; data on compassion in 1997, 2001, or 2012; data on parents’ socioeconomic factors in 1980; data on participants’ socioeconomic factors in 2011). The final data included 1031–1495 participants. The study design is illustrated in Supplementary Table 1.

Attrition was examined by comparing the study variables between the included (n = 1573) and excluded (n = 2023) participants. Included participants (vs. excluded participants) had slightly lower levels of negative emotionality and vital exhaustion, were slightly older, and had more likely high educational level in adulthood and more likely parents with high educational level. Women were more likely to participate than men. There was no attrition bias in compassion. The results of these attrition analyses are reported with more detail in the Supplementary Table 3.

Measures

Compassion

Compassion was evaluated with the Compassion scale of the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) (Cloninger et al., 1994). The Compassion scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.85–0.87 in 1997, 2001, and 2012) consists of 10 self-rating items (e.g. ‘It gives me pleasure to see my enemies suffer’ [reverse scored]; “It gives me pleasure to help others, even if they have treated me badly”). The items are responded with a 5-point Likert-scale (1 = completely disagree; 5 = completely agree). The correlations between the single items and the mean score of compassion were significant in all measurement years (r = 0.48–0.81 in 1997; r = 0.47–0.80 in 2001; and r = 0.43–0.76 in 2012). For each measurement time, we calculated the mean score of compassion for all the participants who had responded to at least 50% of the items. There were no statistical differences between complete vs. incomplete responders of compassion in negative emotionality or vital exhaustion in any measurement year. The validity of the Compassion scale has been described with more details elsewhere (see Saarinen et al., 2019).

Stress-related dimensions

Negative emotionality was measured with the Negative Emotionality scale of the Emotionality, Activity, and Sociability Temperament Survey (EAS) (Buss, 1991). The scale consists of 12 items (e.g. “I’m easily frightened”; “I’m irritated a great deal more than people are aware of”) that are responded with a 5-point scale (1 = totally disagree; 5 = totally agree). In our data, the internal consistency of the scale was good (Cronbach’s α = 0.80–0.82 in 1997, 2001, 2007, and 2012). The correlations between single items and the mean score of negative emotionality were significant in all measurement years (r = 0.37–0.72 in 1997; r = 0.39–0.69 in 2001; r = 0.31–0.71 in 2007; and r = 0.35–0.71 in 2012). We calculated the mean score of the items for all the participants who had responded to at least 50% of the items.

Vital exhaustion was measured with The Maastricht Vital Exhaustion Questionnaire (Appels et al., 1987) that includes 21 items (e.g. “Do you ever wake up with a feeling of exhaustion and fatigue?”; “Do little things irritate you more lately then used do?”; “Do you feel you want to give up trying?”) that are responded with a 3-point scale (0 = no; 1 = I’m not sure; 2 = yes). We calculated the mean score of the items for all the participants who had responded to at least 50% of the items. In our data, the internal consistency of the scale was good (Cronbach’s α = 0.88–0.89 in 2001, 2007, and 2012). The correlations between the mean score and single items of vital exhaustion were statistically significant and comparatively strong in all measurement years(r = 0.41–72 in 2001; r = 0.44–0.76 in 2007; and r = 0.44–0.74 in 2012).

Using factor analysis, we examined whether the items of the Maastricht Vital Exhaustion Questionnaire (MVEQ) and the Negative Emotionality scale of the Emotionality, Activity, and Sociability Temperament Survey (EAS) are loaded to different factors. We conducted factor analysis with two factors and Promax rotation. The results of the factor analysis are presented in Supplementary Table 2. The results showed that, except for two cross-loadings (the items 11 and 17 of the MVEQ), the items of the two questionnaires were loaded to separate factors. This indicated that vital exhaustion and negative emotionality have only minor conceptual overlap. In addition, test–retest correlations showed that negative emotionality was comparatively stable over the follow-up [r(1997–2001) = 0.68; r(2001–2007) = 0.68; r(2007–2012) = 0.73; r(1997–2012) = 0.56]. Test–retest correlations of vital exhaustion were somewhat lower for vital exhaustion [r(2001–2007) = 0.55; r(2007–2012) = 0.63; r(2001–2012) = 0.50] than for negative emotionality. This suggested that vital exhaustion may measure a somewhat varying but still fairly long-standing state of chronic stress whereas negative emotionality may refer to a more stable disposition to stress responses.

Covariates

Covariates included age, sex, participants’ and their parents’ socioeconomic factors, and agreeableness in adulthood. Participants’ and their parents’ socioeconomic factors included education level and level of income. Parents’ and participants’ educational level was classified into 3 categories (1 = comprehensive school, i.e. the first 9 years of school; 2 = high school or occupational school; 3 = academic, i.e. university or college). In case mother’s and father’s educational levels were different, we used the higher educational level. Parents’ level of income was classified into three categories (1 = low; 2 = average; 3 = high). Participants’ level of income was assessed with a 13-point scale (1 = less than 5000€ per year; 13 = more than 60,000€ per year). The socioeconomic factors were included in the analyses as separate variables.

Agreeableness was measured with the Finnish version of the Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Five‐Factor Inventory (NEO‐FFI) (see Rantanen et al., 2007). The agreeableness scale included 12 self-rating statements (e.g. “I would rather cooperate than compete with others”) with a 5-point response scale (1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree). All the items were reversed so that higher values referred to higher agreeableness. The internal consistency of the scale was good (Cronbach’s α = 0.79–0.80 in 2007 and 2012). For each measurement time, we calculated the mean score of the items for all the participants who had responded to at least 50% of the items. Next, we calculated the mean score of year 2007 and 2012 scores of agreeableness for all the participants with data available on 2007 and/or 2012.

Statistical analyses

We conducted statistical analyses using the version 16.0 of Stata SE. The predictive relationships of compassion with negative emotionality and vital exhaustion were examined using structural equation models with cross-lagged panel design (MLMV estimation). We used the scores of compassion, negative emotionality, and vital exhaustion in 2001 and 2012. We conducted 4 models: (1) a model including only stability coefficients (the variables at each time point were predicted by the same variables at the previous time point) and covariances between compassion, negative emotionality, and vital exhaustion at each time point; (2) a model that included the predictive coefficients from negative emotionality and vital exhaustion at each time point to compassion at the next timepoint; and (3) a model that included predictive coefficients in the opposite direction (i.e. from compassion at each time point to negative emotionality and vital exhaustion at the next time point); (4) a model with the predictive coefficients in both directions. All the SEM models were controlled for age, sex, participants’ and their parents’ socioeconomic factors (level of income, educational level). As additional analysis, we included also agreeableness as covariate.

The statistical fit of the models 1‒4 was evaluated by comparing the values of the χ2 test of absolute model fit, Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Root-Mean-Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC). Adequate values of the CFI range over 0.95, and the value of RMSEA should be below 0.06 (Hu & Bentler, 1999). Moreover, lower scores of the χ2 test of absolute model fit and the BIC indicate better model-fit (Schreiber et al., 2006). The selection of best-fitting model was done on the basis of the χ2 test of absolute model fit (in case the χ2 test value between two models was not statistically significantly different, the “more simple” model was selected).

The longitudinal associations of compassion with negative emotionality and vital exhaustion were investigated with growth curve modeling (MLMV estimation). The growth curve models estimate both fixed effects (regression coefficients) and random effects (individual-level variation in the intercept and residual variance, i.e. within-individual variation over the follow-up). In order to reduce potential multicollinearity in the multilevel models, age was centered to the age of the youngest age cohort in the first measurement year of the outcome variable (i.e. with age of 20 years when predicting negative emotionality in 1997–2012, and with the age of 24 years when predicting vital exhaustion in 2001–2012). As previous studies have found curvilinear trends in the course of compassion over age (Hintsanen et al., 2019), we included age-squared in the growth-curve models.

As additional analysis, we included also agreeableness as covariate (in fixed effects). This analysis was conducted because there is previous evidence that compassion for others correlates strongly with agreeableness (but not with other Big Five traits such as neuroticism) (Di Fabio & Saklofske, 2021; Kim et al., 2020; Pommier et al., 2020). Hence, this analysis was conducted to test whether the effect of compassion on dimensions of stress is confounded by agreeableness.

We estimated separate growth curve trajectories for vital exhaustion (in 2001–2012) and negative emotionality (in 1997–2012). We predicted the curve of vital exhaustion by the compassion score of year 2001; and we predicted the curve of negative emotionality by the compassion score of year 1997 (i.e. the first measurement year of the outcome variable). Intercept, age, age-squared, compassion and its age-interactions, gender, and participants’ and their parents’ level of income and educational level were set as fixed effects.

Results

The means, standard deviations, and frequencies of the study variables are shown in Table 1. Correlation coefficients between the study variables are presented in Supplementary Table 4.

Table 1.

The means, standard deviations (SD), and frequencies of the study variables

Variable (measurement year) Mean/frequency (%) SD Range
Compassiona 3.68 0.63 1–5
Vital exhaustiona 0.41 0.36 1–5
Negative emotionalitya 2.57 0.59 1–5
Gender (Female) 885 (56.26)
Age in 2001 31.66 5.02 24–49
Parental educational level
 Comprehensive school 503 (32.0)
 High school or occupational school 654 (41.6)
 Academic level (university or college) 416 (26.5)
Parents’ level of income
 Low 388 (24.7)
 Average 824 (52.4)
 High 361 (23.0)
Participants’ educational level
 Comprehensive school 39 (2.5)
 High school or occupational school 832 (52.9)
 Academic level (university or college) 702 (44.6)
Participants’ level of income 7.45 3.07 1–13

aIn Table 1, we reported the year 2001 values of compassion, vital exhaustion, and negative emotionality

The results of the structural equation models are presented in Table 2. The results showed that there was no difference in the statistical fit (p = 0.224) between Model 1 (no predictive paths) and Model 2 (predictive paths from negative emotionality and vital exhaustion to compassion). Instead, Model 3 (predictive paths from compassion to negative emotionality and vital exhaustion) had significantly better statistical fit than Model 1 or Model 2 (p < 0.00001). There was not significant difference in the statistical fit between Models 3 and 4 (predictive paths in both directions) (p = 0.456). Hence, as adding more statistical parameters to the model (i.e. predictive paths in both directions) did not improve statistical fit significantly, Model 3 (with fewer parameters) was selected. Taken together, Model 3 had clearly better statistical fit (RMSEA = 0.043, CFI = 0.979) than Model 1 and 2 and slightly better fit than Model 4. This indicated that the predictive paths from compassion to negative emotionality and vital exhaustion were slightly stronger than in the opposite direction. The predictive relationships (in Model 3) are illustrated in Fig. 1. In Model 3, the predictor variables explained approximately 47% of variance of compassion, 39% of variance of negative emotionality, and 23% of variance of vital exhaustion.

Table 2.

The goodness-of-fit statistics for the longitudinal models on the predictive relationships of compassion with negative emotionality and vital exhaustion

χ2 value df p RMSEA CFI BIC Model comparisons
χ2 difference test df p
Model 1 115.466 24  < .001 0.051 0.969 35,606.369
Model 2 112.470 22  < .001 0.052 0.969 35,617.994 χ2(2 vs. 1) = 2.996 2 0.224
Model 3 82.193 22  < .001 0.043 0.979 35,587.717 χ2(3 vs. 1) = 33.273 2  < .00001
Model 4 80.625 20  < .001 0.045 0.979 35,600.768 χ2(4 vs. 3) = 1.569 2 0.456

Model 1: No cross-lagged predictive paths

Model 2: Predictive paths from negative emotionality and vital exhaustion to compassion

Model 3: Predictive paths from compassion to negative emotionality and vital exhaustion

Model 4: Predictive paths in both directions

RMSEA the root mean square error of approximation, CFI the Comparative Fit Index, BIC the Bayesian information criterion. n = 1495

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The illustration of Model 3: Standardized predictive pathways from compassion to negative emotionality and vital exhaustion. For clarity, control variables (age, sex and participants’ and their parents’ level of income and educational level) were excluded from this figure. *p < .05, n = 1495

In the additional analysis, we included agreeableness in adulthood as covariate. The results were largely replicated. That is, Model 3 had higher goodness-of-fit when compared to Model 1 [χ2(Model 3 vs. 1) = 33.716, p < 0.00001] or Model 2 [χ2(Model 3 vs. 1) = 30.036]. Further, adding predictive paths in both directions did not improve the model significantly [χ2(Model 3 vs. 1 = 2.116), p = 0.347]. Thus, also the additional analysis indicated that there were stronger predictive pathways from high compassion to lower levels of negative emotionality and vital exhaustion than vice versa.

The results of the growth curve models are presented in Table 3. High compassion predicted lower vital exhaustion. There were no significant interactions between compassion and age or age-squared, indicating that the effect of high compassion on lower vital exhaustion was evident over the whole age range (between the ages of 24–49 years). The results showed that high compassion predicted lower negative emotionality. Further, the interaction between compassion and age was significant, suggesting that the effect of compassion on negative emotionality became weaker over age but was evident from early adulthood to middle age (see Fig. 2). All these findings were adjusted for sex and participants’ and their parents’ level of income and educational level. The findings are illustrated in Fig. 2.

Table 3.

Results of multilevel models with longitudinal design

Vital exhaustion (n = 1031) Negative emotionality (n = 1212)
B 95% CI B 95% CI
Fixed effects
 Intercept 1.167* 0.672, 1.661 2.675* 2.252, 3.098
 Compassion − 0.273* − 0.398, − 0.148 − 0.636* − 0.740, − 0.532
 Age − 0.024 − 0.091, 0.043 − 0.096* − 0.142, − 0.051
 Age squared 0.000 − 0.002, 0.003 0.001 0.000, 0.003
 Age*Compassion 0.006 − 0.013, 0.024 0.021* 0.008, 0.033
 Age squared*Compassion 0.000 − 0.001, 0.001 0.000 − 0.001, 0.000
Random effects
 Variance of intercept 0.583* 0.549; 0.618 0.649* 0.618; 0.682
 Residual variance 0.571* 0.552; 0.589 0.559* 0.545; 0.573

All the models were adjusted for sex and participants’ and their parents’ level of income and educational level

Estimates (B) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) of compassion and age, when predicting standardized scores of negative emotionality and vital exhaustion

*p < .05

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Model-predicted values with 95% confidence intervals (marked with gray color) of (i) vital exhaustion and (ii) negative emotionality over age separately for participants with low (− 1 SD), average, and high (+ 1 SD) levels of compassion. Adjusted for sex and participants’ and their parents’ level of income and educational level

In the additional analysis, we included also agreeableness in adulthood as covariate. All the significant associations of compassion with negative emotionality and vital exhaustion remained. The results are shown with more details in Supplementary Table 5.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this study was the first to investigate the relationship of compassion with stress-related dimensions (i.e. vital exhaustion and negative emotionality) over a long prospective follow-up of over a decade with several measurement points. The results showed that the predictive paths from compassion to negative emotionality and vital exhaustion were significantly stronger than vice versa. This indicates that high compassion is likely to protect against negative emotionality and vital exhaustion, whereas high negative emotionality or high vital exhaustion may not reduce one’s disposition to feel compassion for others over a long-term follow-up. Furthermore, the findings showed that the protective effect of high compassion against negative emotionality and vital exhaustion is evident from early adulthood to middle age.

This study was the first to demonstrate the protective role of compassion against vital exhaustion that is a marker of chronic and repeated stress responses (Keltikangas-Järvinen & Heponiemi, 2004; Koertge et al., 2002; Kudielka et al., 2006). Many theoretical mechanisms have been proposed to explain the pathway from compassionate feelings to lower stress responses. Firstly, by definition, compassion includes an element of non-judgemental attitude toward others’ distress that is postulated to stabilize self-regulation and enhance tolerance also toward one’s own affective states such as distress (Gilbert, 2019; Kirby & Gilbert, 2017). Secondly, it has been emphasized that feelings of social belongingness compose a fundamental evolutionary need for the human being, and lack of compassion may disrupt with maintaining social networks and cause distress via social rejection (Brown & Brown, 2017).

Thirdly, it has been proposed that compassionate inter-dependence between community members (e.g. family members or friendships) activates socioemotional brain networks and triggers oxytocin release that, in turn, is suggested to have inhibitory effects on sympathetic stress responses (Brown & Brown, 2017). This is also in accordance with the “Tend and Befriend” theory (Taylor, 2011). In line with this proposed mechanism, high compassion is related to lower cortisol reactivity, lower heart rate, higher heart rate variability, and a lower risk for hypertension in individuals at familial risk for hypertension (Cosley et al., 2010; Saarinen et al., 2020; Stellar et al., 2015). Hence, compassion may protect against physiological disturbations in the cardiac and neuroendocrinological stress-regulation systems. However, since this study did not include physiological indicators, more research is needed.

A number of previous studies have found that compassion training may not reduce negative affect (Engen & Singer, 2015; Klimecki et al., 2013, 2014) and that dispositional compassion for others’ suffering may not associate with negative affect in questionnaire-based studies (Arimitsu et al., 2019; López et al., 2018; Medvedev et al., 2020). This study, however, demonstrated that high compassion predicts decreased levels of vital exhaustion and negative emotionality over long-term follow-up. The differences between previous findings and our results may be explained by our larger sample size, substantially longer prospective follow-up, and more sophisticated statistical analyses that enabled to differentiate between correlations and predictive pathways between compassion, vital exhaustion, and negative emotionality. Overall, the findings of the current study provide evidence that experiencing compassion for others may not require unstressed psychosocial circumstances but experiencing compassion may reduce stress responses to psychosocial circumstances.

Previously, negative emotionality is found to be a marker of dispositional susceptibility to stress (Strelau & Angleitner, 1991) and to predict higher stress responses at work (Hintsanen et al., 2011) and elevated cortisol levels in response to conflicts (Laurent & Powers, 2007). In this light, the association between high compassion and lower negative emotionality suggests that compassion may predict a higher threshold to experience stress. This may be explained by the findings that compassion is related to better social functioning (Goetz et al., 2010) and may, thus, improve the quality of social relationships. In addition, compassion may protect against stress susceptibility via more adaptive coping strategies. For example, compassion training predicts less frequent use of expressive suppression and higher acceptance when facing psychosocial stressors (Jazaieri et al., 2018). Finally, compassion-focused therapy reduces rumination (e.g. Frostadottir & Dorjee, 2019), indicating that highly compassionate individuals may not be prone to direct their attention to distressing thoughts.

Regarding study limitations, as this was not a genetically informative study, we were not able to study the potential roles of genetic predispositions or gene-environment interactions in driving the association between compassion and stress-related characteristics. It has been found, for example, that oxytocin-receptor genes may affect both empathy and stress reactivity (Rodrigues et al., 2009). Further, the functioning of parental empathy-related brain networks is found to predict offspring’s reactivity to stress (Abraham et al., 2018), implying that there may be heritable aspects in the compassion-stress connection. Longitudinal, multivariate twin data would be able to shed further light on the interaction between compassion and genetic and environmental factors when predicting stress responses.

Secondly, we could not control life events or other environmental factors during the follow-up. On the basis of test–retest correlations, vital exhaustion may change over years to some degree, and it is possible that some favorable life events during the follow-up may alleviate its influence on compassion. Nevertheless, vital exhaustion is commonly regarded as a marker of chronic stress (see e.g. Koertge et al., 2002) and may thus not likely appear or disappear over single exposures to stress. Further, increasing compassion is found to increase also prosocial behavior toward others (Weng et al., 2015) that, in turn, may result in social interactions and situations that are less stressful.

Finally, because this study focused on a developmental period from early adulthood to middle age, it is not able to elucidate how compassion development in early childhood calibrates stress resilience later on. This would be an interesting line of enquiry in the future. To date, there is evidence that children with callous-unemotional traits may exhibit reduced physiological responses to stress (Stadler et al., 2011; von Polier et al., 2013).

This study had also a variety of strengths. Firstly, we had an exceptionally long prospective follow-up: a 15-year follow-up of compassion and negative emotionality and a 10-year follow-up of vital exhaustion. This provided possibilities to investigate the relationship between compassion and stress-related dimensions from early adulthood to middle age. Moreover, we had a fairly large population-based sample. Finally, we used widely used and validated measures of compassion, vital exhaustion, and negative emotionality.

Regarding practical implications, a recent meta-analysis highlighted that interventions for chronic stress (burnout) have focused on the adverse antecedents of stress, whereas positive and preventive factors have remained less investigated (Ahola et al., 2017). Additionally, the meta-analysis pointed out that interventions for chronic stress have most commonly included analyzing one’s mental processes (e.g. cognitive-behavioral therapy, psychodrama, cognitive coping training) and been ineffective for alleviating chronic stress (Ahola et al., 2017). Our findings, in turn, suggest that promoting compassion (i.e. directing attention from the self to others) might protect against vital exhaustion and negative emotionality. Currently, a large proportion of compassion-focused interventions have been provided for individuals in their adolescence or early adulthood (see e.g. Kirby, 2017; Reddy et al., 2013) but our findings provide a promising piece of evidence that increasing compassion training might protect against vital exhaustion also in middle age. There is encouraging evidence that compassion can be cultivated with short-term cost-effective interventions, even within circa 2 months (Jazaieri et al., 2013). Finally, since vital exhaustion is particularly prevalent in individuals with high experienced job demands and low experienced job control (Schuller et al., 2014) and in individuals with work overcommitment (Preckel et al., 2005), there could be also organisation-level interventions enhancing compassion between coworkers in occupational organisations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Author contributions

AS and MH designed the research questions and drafted the manuscript. AS conducted the statistical analyses. MH, KK, and HD assisted with the data analyses. LK-J, EV, HD, KK, TL, OR, and MH contributed to the interpretation of the results and collaborated with writing the manuscript. LK-J, MH, TL, and OR contributed to the data collection.

Funding

Open access funding provided by University of Helsinki including Helsinki University Central Hospital.. This study was supported financially by the Academy of Finland (M.H., Grant Number 308676). The Young Finns Study has been financially supported by the Academy of Finland: Grants 322098, 286284, 134309 (Eye), 126925, 121584, 124282, 129378 (Salve), 117787 (Gendi), and 41071 (Skidi); the Social Insurance Institution of Finland; Competitive State Research Financing of the Expert Responsibility area of Kuopio, Tampere and Turku University Hospitals (Grant X51001); Juho Vainio Foundation; Paavo Nurmi Foundation; Finnish Foundation for Cardiovascular Research; Finnish Cultural Foundation; The Sigrid Juselius Foundation; Tampere Tuberculosis Foundation; Emil Aaltonen Foundation; Yrjö Jahnsson Foundation; Signe and Ane Gyllenberg Foundation; Diabetes Research Foundation of Finnish Diabetes Association; EU Horizon 2020 (Grant 755320 for TAXINOMISIS and Grant 848146 for To Aition); European Research Council (Grant 742927 for MULTIEPIGEN project); Tampere University Hospital Supporting Foundation and Finnish Society of Clinical Chemistry.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.

Ethical approval

The study was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Additionally, all the Finnish universities with medical schools approved the study design. Before participation, all the participants or their parents (for participants aged below 12 years) provided written informed consent after the nature of the procedures had been fully explained.

Footnotes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Abelson JL, Erickson TM, Mayer SE, Crocker J, Briggs H, Lopez-Duran NL, Liberzon I. Brief cognitive intervention can modulate neuroendocrine stress responses to the Trier Social Stress Test: Buffering effects of a compassionate goal orientation. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;44:60–70. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abraham E, Raz G, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Empathy networks in the parental brain and their long-term effects on children's stress reactivity and behavior adaptation. Neuropsychologia. 2018;116:75–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ahola K, Toppinen-Tanner S, Seppänen J. Interventions to alleviate burnout symptoms and to support return to work among employees with burnout: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Burnout Research. 2017;4:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.burn.2017.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®) (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
  5. Arimitsu K, Hitokoto H, Kind S, Hofmann SG. Differences in compassion, well-being, and social anxiety between Japan and the USA. Mindfulness. 2019;10(5):854–862. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-1045-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Appels APWM, Höppener P, Mulder P. A questionnaire to assess premonitory symptoms of myocardial infarction. International Journal of Cardiology. 1987;17:15–24. doi: 10.1016/0167-5273(87)90029-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Black PH. Stress and the inflammatory response: A review of neurogenic inflammation. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2002;16:622–653. doi: 10.1016/S0889-1591(02)00021-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brito-Pons G, Campos D, Cebolla A. Implicit or explicit compassion? Effects of compassion cultivation training and comparison with mindfulness-based stress reduction. Mindfulness. 2018;9:1494–1508. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-0898-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brown SL, Brown RM. Compassionate neurobiology and health. The Oxford handbook of compassion science. Oxford University Press; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  10. Buss AH. The EAS theory of temperament. In: Strelau J, Angleitner A, editors. Explorations in temperament: International perspectives on theory and measurement. New York: Plenum Press; 1991. pp. 43–60. [Google Scholar]
  11. Buss AH, Plomin R. Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bryant RA, Moulds ML, Guthrie RM, Nixon RD. The additive benefit of hypnosis and cognitive-behavioral therapy in treating acute stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;73:334. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chida Y, Hamer M, Wardle J, Steptoe A. Do stress-related psychosocial factors contribute to cancer incidence and survival. Nature Reviews. Clinical Oncology. 2008;5:466. doi: 10.1038/ncponc1134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Švrakić DM. The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University; 1994. pp. 19–28. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cosley BJ, McCoy SK, Saslow LR, Epel ES. Is compassion for others stress buffering? Consequences of compassion and social support for physiological reactivity to stress. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2010;46:816–823. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.04.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. DeVries AC, Glasper ER, Detillion CE. Social modulation of stress responses. Physiology & Behavior. 2003;79:399–407. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00152-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Di Fabio A, Saklofske DH. The relationship of compassion and self-compassion with personality and emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences. 2021;169:110109. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ebner K, Singewald N. Individual differences in stress susceptibility and stress inhibitory mechanisms. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2017;14:54–64. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.11.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Engen HG, Singer T. Compassion-based emotion regulation up-regulates experienced positive affect and associated neural networks. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2015;10(9):1291–1301. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Engert V, Kok BE, Papassotiriou I, Chrousos GP, Singer T. Specific reduction in cortisol stress reactivity after social but not attention-based mental training. Sci Adv. 2017;3:e1700495. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Epel ES, Crosswell AD, Mayer SE, Prather AA, Slavich GM, Puterman E, Mendes WB. More than a feeling: A unified view of stress measurement for population science. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2018;49:146–169. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Frostadottir AD, Dorjee D. Effects of mindfulness based cognitive therapy (MBCT) and compassion focused therapy (CFT) on symptom change, mindfulness, self-compassion, and rumination in clients with depression, anxiety, and stress. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019;10:1099. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. García Ó, Aluja A, García LF, Escorial S, Blanch A. Zuckerman-Kuhlman-Aluja Personality Questionnaire (ZKA-PQ) and Cloninger’s Temperament and Character Inventory Revised (TCI-R): A comparative study. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2012;53:247–257. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2012.00943.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gilbert P. The origins and nature of compassion focused therapy. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2014;53:6–41. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gilbert P. Compassion: Concepts, research and applications. Gilbert. London: Routledge; 2017. pp. 31–68. [Google Scholar]
  26. Gilbert P. Explorations into the nature and function of compassion. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2019;28:108–114. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gilbert P, Mascaro J. Compassion: Fears, blocks, and resistances: An evolutionary investigation. In: Seppälä EM, Simon-Thomas E, Brown SL, Worline MC, Cameron L, Doty JR, editors. The Oxford handbook of compassion science. Oxford University Press; 2017. pp. 399–420. [Google Scholar]
  28. Goetz JL, Keltner D, Simon-Thomas E. Compassion: an evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136:351–374. doi: 10.1037/a0018807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gluschkoff K, Pulkki-Råback L, Elovainio M, Saarinen A, Tammelin T, Hirvensalo M, Lehtimäki T, Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Raitakari O, Hintsanen M. Is it good to be good? Dispositional compassion and health behaviors. Annals Behavioral Medicine. 2018;53:665–673. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Halford C, Anderzén I, Arnetz B. Endocrine measures of stress and self-rated health: A longitudinal study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2003;55:317–320. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00634-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hauge LJ, Skogstad A, Einarsen S. Individual and situational predictors of workplace bullying: Why do perpetrators engage in the bullying of others. Work & Stress. 2009;23:349–358. doi: 10.1080/02678370903395568. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hershcovis MS, Turner N, Barling J, Arnold KA, Dupré KE, Inness M, LeBlanc MM, Sivanathan N. Predicting workplace aggression: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2007;92:228–238. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.1.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hintsanen M, Hintsa T, Widell A, Kivimäki M, Raitakari OT, Keltkangas-Järvinen L. Negative emotionality, activity, and sociability temperaments predicting long-term job strain and effort–reward imbalance: A 15-year prospective follow-up study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2011;71:90–96. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hintsanen M, Gluschkoff K, Dobewall H, Cloninger CR, Keltner D, Saarinen A, Wesolowska K, Volanen SM, Raitakari OT, Pulkki-Råback L. Parent–child-relationship quality predicts offspring dispositional compassion in adulthood: A prospective follow-up study over three decades. Developmental Psychology. 2019;55:216. doi: 10.1037/dev0000633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jazaieri H, Jinpa GT, McGonigal K, Rosenberg EL, Finkelstein J, Simon-Thomas E, Cullen M, Doty JR, Gross JJ, Goldin PR. Enhancing compassion: A randomized controlled trial of a compassion cultivation training program. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2013;14:1113–1126. doi: 10.1007/s10902-012-9373-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Jazaieri H, McGonigal K, Jinpa T, Doty JR, Gross JJ, Goldin PR. A randomized controlled trial of compassion cultivation training: Effects on mindfulness, affect, and emotion regulation. Motivation and Emotion. 2014;38:23–35. doi: 10.1007/s11031-013-9368-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jazaieri H, McGonigal K, Lee IA, Jinpa T, Doty JR, Gross JJ, Goldin PR. Altering the trajectory of affect and affect regulation: The impact of compassion training. Mindfulness. 2018;9:283–293. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0773-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Heponiemi T. Vital exhaustion, temperament, and cardiac reactivity in task-induced stress. Biological Psychology. 2004;65:121–135. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00112-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kim JJ, Gerrish R, Gilbert P, Kirby JN. Stressed, depressed, and rank obsessed: Individual differences in compassion and neuroticism predispose towards rank-based depressive symptomatology. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 2020 doi: 10.1111/papt.12270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kirby JN. Compassion interventions: The programmes, the evidence, and implications for research and practice. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice. 2017;90(3):432–455. doi: 10.1111/papt.12104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kirby J, Gilbert P. The emergence of the compassion focused therapies. In: Gilbert P, editor. Compassion: Concepts, research and applications. Routledge; 2017. pp. 258–285. [Google Scholar]
  43. Klimecki OM, Leiberg S, Lamm C, Singer T. Functional neural plasticity and associated changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23:1552–1561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Klimecki OM, Leiberg S, Ricard M, Singer T. Differential pattern of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2014;9:873–879. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Koertge J, Al-Khalili F, Ahnve S, Janszky I, Svane B, Schenck-Gustafsson K. Cortisol and vital exhaustion in relation to significant coronary artery stenosis in middle-aged women with acute coronary syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27(8):893–906. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00002-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kudielka BM, von Känel R, Preckel D, Zgraggen L, Mischler K, Fischer JE. Exhaustion is associated with reduced habituation of free cortisol responses to repeated acute psychosocial stress. Biological Psychology. 2006;72:147–153. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Laurent H, Powers S. Emotion regulation in emerging adult couples: Temperament, attachment, and HPA response to conflict. Biological Psychology. 2007;76:61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lee J, Min SK, Kim K-H, Kim B, Cho SJ, Lee S-H, Choi TK, Suh S-Y. Differences in temperament and character dimensions of personality between patients with Hwa-byung, an anger syndrome, and patients with major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2012;138:110–116. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lewis SF, Fremouw W. Dating violence: A critical review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review. 2001;21:105–127. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(99)00042-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. López A, Sanderman R, Ranchor AV, Schroevers MJ. Compassion for others and self-compassion: Levels, correlates, and relationship with psychological well-being. Mindfulness. 2018;9(1):325–331. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0777-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Marinelli PW, Quirion R, Gianoulakis C. An in vivo profile of β-endorphin release in the arcuate nucleus and nucleus accumbens following exposure to stress or alcohol. Neuroscience. 2004;127:777–784. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.05.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Matos M, Duarte C, Duarte J, Pinto-Gouveia J, Petrocchi N, Basran J, Gilbert P. Psychological and physiological effects of compassionate mind training: A pilot randomized controlled study. Mindfulness. 2017;8:1699–1712. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0745-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Medvedev ON, Cervin M, Barcaccia B, Siegert RJ, Roemer A, Krägeloh CU. Network analysis of mindfulness facets, affect, compassion, and distress. Mindfulness. 2020 doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01555-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Merrill RM, Aldana SG, Greenlaw RL, Diehl HA, Salberg A. The effects of an intensive lifestyle modification program on sleep and stress disorders. The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging. 2007;11:242. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Montoya ER, Terburg D, Bos PA, Van Honk J. Testosterone, cortisol, and serotonin as key regulators of social aggression: A review and theoretical perspective. Motivation and Emotion. 2012;36:65–73. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9264-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mönnikes H, Tebbe JJ, Hildebrandt M, Arck P, Osmanoglou E, Rose M, Klapp B, Wiedenmann B, Heymann-Mönnikes I. Role of stress in functional gastrointestinal disorders. Digestive Disease. 2001;19:201–211. doi: 10.1159/000050681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mroczek DK, Almeida DM. The effect of daily stress, personality, and age on daily negative emotionality. Journal of Personality. 2004;72:355–378. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00265.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nicolson NA, van Diest R. Salivary cortisol patterns in vital exhaustion. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2000;49:335–342. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(00)00166-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pace TW, Negi LT, Adame DD, Cole SP, Sivilli TI, Brown TD, Issa MJ, Raison CL. Effect of compassion meditation on neuroendocrine, innate immune and behavioral responses to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:87–98. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pace TW, Negi LT, Dodson-Lavelle B, Ozawa-de Silva B, Reddy SD, Cole SP, Danese A, Craighead LW, Raison CL. Engagement with cognitively-based compassion training is associated with reduced salivary C-reactive protein from before to after training in foster care program adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:294–299. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pommier E, Neff KD, Tóth-Király I. The development and validation of the Compassion Scale. Assessment. 2020;27(1):21–39. doi: 10.1177/1073191119874108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Preckel D, Von Känel R, Kudielka BM, Fischer JE. Overcommitment to work is associated with vital exhaustion. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health. 2005;78(2):117–122. doi: 10.1007/s00420-004-0572-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Raitakari OT, Juonala M, Rönnemaa T, Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Räsänen L, Pietikäinen M, Hutri-Kähönen N, Taittonen L, Jokinen E, Marniemi J, Jula A. Cohort profile: The cardiovascular risk in Young Finns Study. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2008;37:1220–1226. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rantanen J, Metsäpelto RL, Feldt T, Pulkkinen LEA, Kokko K. Long-term stability in the Big Five personality traits in adulthood. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2007;48:511–518. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2007.00609.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Reddy SD, Negi LT, Dodson-Lavelle B, Ozawa-de Silva B, Pace TW, Cole SP, Raison CL, Craighead LW. Cognitive-based compassion training: A promising prevention strategy for at-risk adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2013;22:219–230. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9571-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Rodrigues SM, Saslow LR, Garcia N, John OP, Keltner D. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106:21437–21441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909579106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Roozendaal B, McEwen BS, Chattarji S. Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:423. doi: 10.1038/nrn2651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Saarinen AI, Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Pulkki-Råback L, Cloninger CR, Elovainio M, Lehtimäki T, Raitakari O, Hintsanen M. The relationship of dispositional compassion with well-being: A study with a 15-year prospective follow-up. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2019 doi: 10.1080/17439760.2019.1663251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Saarinen AI, Keltikangas-Järvinen L, Hintsa T, Pulkki-Råback L, Ravaja N, Lehtimäki T, Raitakari O, Hintsanen M. Does compassion predict blood pressure and hypertension? The modifying role of familial risk for hypertension. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2020 doi: 10.1007/s12529-020-09886-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Schreiber, J. B., Nora, A., Stage, F. K., Barlow, E. A., & King, J. (2006). Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: A review. Journal of Educational Research, 99, 323– 338.
  71. Schuller K, Roesler U, Rau R. Self-reported job characteristics and negative spillover from work to private life as mediators between expert-rated job characteristics and vital exhaustion. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology. 2014;23(2):177–189. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2012.727555. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Skipstein A, Janson H, Kjeldsen A, Nilsen W, Mathiesen KS. Trajectories of maternal symptoms of depression and anxiety over 13 years: The influence of stress, social support, and maternal temperament. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:1120. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-1120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Stadler C, Kroeger A, Weyers P, Grasmann D, Horschinek M, Freitag C, Clement HW. Cortisol reactivity in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and disruptive behavior problems: The impact of callous unemotional traits. Psychiatry Research. 2011;187:204–209. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Stellar JE, Cohen A, Oveis C, Keltner D. Affective and physiological responses to the suffering of others: Compassion and vagal activity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2015;108:572–585. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Steptoe A, Kivimäki M. Stress and cardiovascular disease. Nature Reviews. Cardiology. 2012;9:360. doi: 10.1038/nrcardio.2012.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Strelau J, Angleitner A. Explorations in temperament: International perspectives on theory and measurement. Plenum Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  77. Taylor SE. Tend and befriend theory. In: Van Lange PA, Kruglanski AW, Higgins ET, editors. Handbook of theories of social psychology. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 2011. pp. 32–49. [Google Scholar]
  78. Valk SL, Bernhardt BC, Trautwein FM, Böckler A, Kanske P, Guizard N, Collins DL, Singer T. Structural plasticity of the social brain: Differential change after socio-affective and cognitive mental training. Science Advances. 2017;3:e1700489. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. von Polier GG, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K, Wiesler K, Rieke J, Heinzel-Gutenbrunner M, Bachmann CJ, Vloet TD. Reduced cortisol in boys with early-onset conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits. BioMed Research International. 2013 doi: 10.1155/2013/349530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Weng HY, Fox AS, Shackman AJ, Stodola DE, Caldwell JZ, Olson MC, Rogers GM, Davidson RJ. Compassion training alters altruism and neural responses to suffering. Psychological Science. 2013;24:1171–1180. doi: 10.1177/0956797612469537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Weng HY, Fox AS, Hessenthaler HC, Stodola DE, Davidson RJ. The role of compassion in altruistic helping and punishment behavior. PLoS ONE. 2015 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0143794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Weng HY, Lapate RC, Stodola DE, Rogers GM, Davidson RJ. Visual attention to suffering after compassion training is associated with decreased amygdala responses. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Motivation and Emotion are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES