Abstract
Recent interest in applying novel imaging techniques to infer optical resolution in compound eyes underscores the difficulty of obtaining direct measures of acuity. A widely used technique exploits the principal pseudopupil, a dark spot on the eye surface representing the ommatidial gaze direction and the number of detector units (ommatidia) viewing that gaze direction. However, dark-pigmented eyes, like those of honeybees, lack a visible pseudopupil. Attempts over almost a century to estimate optical acuity in this species are still debated. Here, we developed a method to visualize a stable, reliable pseudopupil by staining the photoreceptors with fluorescent dyes. We validated this method in several species and found it to outperform the dark pseudopupil for this purpose, even in pale eyes, allowing more precise location of the gaze centre. We then applied this method to estimate the sampling resolution in the frontal part of the eye of the honeybee forager. We found a broad frontal acute zone with interommatidial angles below 2° and a minimum interommatidial angle of 1.3°, a broader, sharper frontal acute zone than previously reported. Our study provides a new method to directly measure the sampling resolution in most compound eyes of living animals.
Subject terms: Retina, Animal physiology, Zoology
Introduction
(a) Quantification of visual acuity in compound eyes
The compound eyes of arthropods are comprised of densely packed tube-like optical units known as ommatidia, each with a lens that focuses light from a narrow region of visual space onto underlying photoreceptors. Together, the ommatidia sample the region of space viewed by each eye and allow animals to resolve information in different locations of their environment. At a single ommatidium level, optical resolution of the most common type of compound eye, the apposition eye, can be well predicted from measured or inferred parameters such as photoreceptor diameter, focal length and facet diameter1–3. However, even the best quality image is only useful if it is matched by an angular sampling strategy to appropriately exploit the potential resolution1–3. The independent sampling of space by individual ommatidia in apposition eyes provides a large number of free parameters for local variations in sampling strategy. The diameter of individual facet lenses, as well as the angular density of the ommatidia, and how they are geometrically juxtaposed, all change radically among species, sexes and across eye regions within the same compound eye1,4. These characteristics determine differences in the ability of the eye (or a local region of an eye) to sample the environment and thus to resolve different objects.
Over the last century many different methods have been used to measure or infer the sampling resolution of compound eyes. In living animals, indirect measures have exploited the behavioural responses to variously fine spatial patterns in either freely moving (e.g.5–10) or tethered insects (e.g.11–13) as well as direct electrophysiological measures from the first optic ganglion14,15. Estimates have also been obtained from a number of different methods applied to fixed (non-living)16–19 and living20 animals to estimate the gaze direction of individual ommatidia based on the anatomy of the eye.
Each of these techniques, however, have inevitable pitfalls. Behavioural estimates, for example, are easily confounded by the motivation of the animal to solve a difficult task. Electrophysiological measures are technically very challenging and often neglect differences in resolution across the eye in favour of recording from the most easily accessed regions. Anatomical measures based on samples preserved in fixative are limited by the non-physiological state of the eye as well as artefacts such as shrinkage due to chemical treatments, which may be severe even with optimal fixation and embedding21. For many years the ‘pseudopupil method’ has provided a more direct approach for measuring the axial directions of ommatidia in living insects. This method most often relies on the principal pseudopupil, a dark spot which results from light absorption by the pigment cells surrounding the crystalline cone and the photoreceptors close to the direction of gaze22. This dark pseudopupil shifts its position as the eye rotates, and changes shape according to the density of receptors sampling the local viewing direction. Mapping the pseudopupil’s positional shift for a known rotational angle allows estimation of the sampling resolution, as measured by the divergence angle between neighbouring ommatidia (known as the interommatidial angle, Δφ). This angle describes the local ommatidial packing density, that is, the number of ommatidia that view a specific narrow region of visual space. However, attempts to estimate the visual acuity in this way has, in many insects, been frustrated by the lack of a clearly identifiable principal pseudopupil due to pigments in pigment cells underneath the cornea that make the eyes appear uniformly dark.
(b) Visual acuity in honeybees
In this context, the honeybee, an important model in neuroethology and cognition23, has for almost a century been the focus of debate concerning the sampling resolution of its darkly pigmented eyes and many attempts have been made to measure it, with inconsistent results (15; also reviewed in24). This exemplifies the implicit difficulties of obtaining reliable measures of optical acuity in darkly pigmented insect eyes. In the honeybee (as in many Hymenoptera) this is further complicated by the vertically elongated, oval-shaped eye, resulting from different sampling in the vertical and horizontal directions, leading to some confusion in defining the interommatidial angle along these dimensions which differs among authors22.
The first attempts to measure the sampling resolution of the honeybee eye date back to 1928, when Baumgärtner applied anatomical reconstruction of the ommatidial axes based on histological sections16. He found a resolution maximum close to the equator of the eye and 60° away from the frontal visual field, with a vertical resolution, Δφv, just below 1°, and with a horizontal row separation, Δφh, of around 2.6°16,25. These numbers, however, require allowance for the hexagonal nature of the ommatidial lattice, where Δφ = [Δφh2 + Δφv2]0.5 (for a detailed overview of this formula, see22). Once this allowance has been made a Δφ of 1.64° can be calculated. Around the same time, Hecht and Wolf5 studied the reaction of crawling bees to grating patterns of different stripe widths and found a resolution agreeing with Baumgärtner’s anatomically-derived value. A similar value for the vertical resolution, around 1°, was then shown in a later study by Del Portillo26 by reconstructing the surface of the cornea from anatomical sections. In 1971 Wiitanen and Varela27 estimated Δφ from histological sections and obtained a minimum of 1.5°, averaged from the vertical and horizontal components. In subsequent work, a larger value of 2° was supported by analysis of freely-flying honeybees trained with sugar rewards in a Y maze: by varying the density of gratings Srinivasan and Lehrer6 found a resolution limit for gratings with bars of 2° width, a value also confirmed by a later study in walking bees28.
In the honeybee, the more direct pseudopupil method is challenged by screening pigment in the eye. In 1973 Kirschfeld29 used antidromic illumination and measured the radiation of ommatidial axes at different focal planes above the cornea to estimate an average Δφ of 1.8° in the frontal eye. Unpublished PhD dissertation work by Seidl30 also used antidromic illumination and estimated a minimum Δφ of 2.1° horizontally and 1.2° vertically in the middle-frontal part of the eye. However later work by Horridge24 claimed that Seidl’s method included errors in interpretation of the data such that the minimum Δφ were actually isotropic at around 1.7°, and hence may agree more closely with Kirschfeld’s estimate for frontal acuity29.
To add to this confusion, a recent paper18 exploited X-ray micro computed tomography techniques (µCT) to investigate the visual properties of the honeybee eye, obtaining a minimum inter-facet angle (a measure that approximates the average Δφ along all 3 axes of the ommatidial rows) of about 1°, but not at the front of the eye—instead at an elevation of 30° and an azimuth of 60°. This conflicts with our own intracellular electrophysiological measures of resolution in honeybee photoreceptors31, which suggested that acuity is highest directly at the front of the field of view.
The disparity between these various attempts in both the value and location of highest visual acuity underscores the need for a more reliable and accurate method to measure Δφ across a living eye, particularly in dark-pigmented eyes like those of the honeybee. To solve these difficulties and controversies we here describe a new method, whereby uptake of fluorophores by the photoreceptors induces a fluorescent pseudopupil—even in darkly pigmented insect eyes. We applied this technique to several insect species, including some in which we could also observe a dark principal corneal pseudopupil when viewed with incident illumination. We hereafter refer to this as the ‘dark’ pseudopupil to differentiate it from our bright induced fluorescent pseudopupil and from a variety of other pseudopupil types that have previously been observed in different insect species (see Methods section). This allowed us to confirm that the induced fluorescent pseudopupil provides a reliable estimate for the gaze direction of ommatidia in living (intact) animals. We then applied this technique to accurately map Δφ in the frontal visual fields of honeybee foragers. Our data confirm the presence of an intense frontal acute zone in these insects, with Δφ reaching a minimum of 1.3° and remaining below 2° across a large region of the frontal eye (e.g. traversing 40° of azimuth and 60° of elevation). Taken together with our own recent electrophysiological estimates of photoreceptor acceptance angles down to 1.6° within this frontal acute zone, we confirm that the frontal resolution of honeybee forager eyes is similar to that found in many dipteran flies.
Results
(a) Induction of the fluorescent pseudopupil
Dark principal pseudopupils result from absorption of orthodromic light by the rhabdoms and surrounding primary pigment cells22. This gives rise to the appearance of a dark spot centred on the photoreceptors directed towards the observer (Fig. 1a). This becomes visible by contrast with light substantially scattered by secondary pigment cells just below the cornea22. To visualise the fluorescent pseudopupil, application of the combination of dimethylsulfoxide (DMSO), ringer solution and a fluorescent neuronal tracer (either Neurobiotin 488 or Lucifer Yellow) appears to retrogradely label the photoreceptors (or at least their rhabdomeres) in the living eye. Short-wavelength light focused by the insect’s own cornea onto the cells below, then presumably excites longer wavelength fluorescence within any structures that take up the dye, and at least some of this light then propagates back along the incident path (Fig. 1b). This can then be selectively visualized using an appropriate fluorescence filter set (see Methods). Figure 1c illustrates an example of such a fluorescent pseudopupil induced in a dragonfly, Aeshna cyanea. By rotating the eye, this induced fluorescent pseudopupil clearly moves across the eye surface (see Supplementary Information video 1, part 1), also changing in its shape and dimensions in response to variations in the local photoreceptor (i.e. ommatidial) angular density, just as seen in the natural dark pseudopupil22.
Does the signal we detect as the fluorescent pseudopupil truly originate from the photoreceptors, or could it be due to uptake of the fluorophore by other cells within the ommatidium? This is an important issue to resolve if we are to trust that the centre of this pseudopupil defines the true gaze direction of the ommatidia and can thus be used as alternative to the dark (principal) pseudopupil to map visual acuity in species where the latter is invisible.
The eyes of dipteran flies are ideal for testing this hypothesis, since Franceschini32 previously showed via corneal neutralization that the majority of the refracting power in fly ommatidia is due to curvature of the outer corneal facets. The distinctive trapezoid pattern of the distal tips of the rhabdomeres in each dipteran ommatidium can then be imaged using a conventional microscope by simply neutralizing the refracting power of the cornea with a drop of high-refractive index oil and illuminating the retina from behind with light introduced through an aperture cut into the back of the head, i.e. antidromic illumination32. We adapted this approach to epifluorescence illumination in a scanning laser confocal microscope. We first applied the same method as in Fig. 1 to induce a fluorescent pseudopupil in the clear eye of the dipteran hoverfly, Eristalis tenax (Fig. 2a–d, about 20 min after application of the dye). We then applied a drop of glycerol to both sides of a glass coverslip placed in contact with the eye to neutralise the corneal refraction. This allowed us to use a long working distance, high numerical aperture glycerol immersion objective to obtain high resolution optical sections from different focal planes, from the cornea to the distal parts of the fluorescing photoreceptors (Fig. 2a–d). At high magnification the typical dipteran trapezoidal pattern was then clearly visible against a completely dark background (Fig. 2c). This shows that it is primarily the rhabdomeres, rather than other organelles or cells within each ommatidium, that initially take up large amounts of the fluorophore. It is worth noting, however, that in the hoverfly the fluorescence subsequently spreads to a large number of supporting cells just beneath the cornea. After around three hours, the induced fluorescent pseudopupil may be swamped by fluorescence from the entire corneal surface of the eye as viewed from any angle (Fig. 2e, and Supplementary Information video 1, part 2).
(b) Fluorescence allows for a better estimate of the pseudopupil centre
Can the induced fluorescent pseudopupil reliably locate the direction of gaze as successfully as the dark pseudopupil observed in some compound eyes? Given that we know from the evidence presented above that the dye is selectively taken up by the rhabdomeres, we would predict that only light focused directly onto their tips should excite fluorescence. The resulting pseudopupil should then be confined to the centre of a region spanned by the dark pseudopupil (which is broader because light is also absorbed by the surrounding pigment granules from off-axis rays). To test this, we stained the photoreceptors of a solitary bee, Anthophora sp., in which the majority of the corneal surface appears pale due to strongly scattering secondary pigment so that a well contrasted dark pseudopupil can easily be seen from many viewing angles (Fig. 3a,b). We captured sequential images using first orthoscopic illumination with white light to visualise the dark principal pseudopupil (Fig. 3b) and then epifluorescence illumination to detect the fluorescent pseudopupil (Fig. 3c) so that we could then construct a false-colour montage (Fig. 3d). This demonstrates that the fluorescent pseudopupil is coincident with the darkest part of the principal pseudopupil. We quantified this further by constructing the intensity profiles across the pseudopupil centres (Fig. 3e). This shows that the two share a common centre but for the fluorescence image also reveals a sharper fall-off in brightness away from the pupil centre.
The narrower intensity profile of the induced fluorescent pseudopupil presumably reflects the double-pass nature of epifluorescence, where the resulting image depends both on excitation light being focused onto the fluorophores from the viewpoint of the observer, while off-axis light ends up absorbed by surrounding pigment cells without exciting fluorescence (Fig. 1b). Emission is then collected as for the luminous pseudopupil formed by antidromic illumination22. By contrast, the principal pseudopupil is dark over a langer area due to the same off-axis absorption of orthodromic light, but this time expanding the visible dark patch rather than exciting fluorescence (Fig. 1a). As also noted previously for luminous pseudopupils induced by antidromic illumination22,29, the width of the fluorescent area can be adjusted to a certain degree by the experimenter by varying the numerical aperture (NA) of the lens, either via switching objectives or by closing the iris diaphragm (Fig. 4), although this is a trade-off, due to decreased resolving power.
The small fluorescent pseudopupil allows easier determination of its centre compared to the dark pseudopupil, which can otherwise be problematic in determining angular sampling resolution22. Often, very broad pseudopupils are seen in species with intense acute zones where large numbers of photoreceptors look in almost the same direction (e.g. in dragonflies:33). These broad acute-zone pseudopupils are rarely circularly symmetric due to differences in the rate at which acuity falls off in different directions away from the centre. This can make it very difficult to determine the pseudopupil centre. For example, A. cyanea has a very broad, dark pseudopupil in the frontal part of the eye (Fig. 1c). However the induced fluorescent pseudopupil shows a much more easily identifiable centre (Fig. 1c and Supplementary Information video 1, part 1).
(c) Eye maps for the honeybee forager compound eye confirm higher sampling resolution that prior estimates
The position of the pseudopupil can be used to determine Δφ and thus the local sampling resolution of the eye22,34. This method involves estimating the shift in the pseudopupil centre (i.e. number of ommatidia that it moves across) for a known rotation of the eye. This has been used in a number of species, to obtain full maps of the change in Δφ across the field of view of the eye, and thus provide valuable and unique information on how compound eyes sample the environment (e.g. in halictid and xylocopid bees:35–37).
Given the sparseness of data (and past disagreements) on the sampling resolution of the honeybee forager due to the impossibility of visualising the dark principal pseudopupil in their heavily pigmented eyes, we selected this species as an ideal test for the practicality of our induced fluorescent pseudopupil method to map part of the eye (see Fig. 5 and Supplementary Information video 1, part 3). We rotated the head of a honeybee forager on a goniometer in 10° steps to scan a portion of the eye from −40° to + 40° in latitude and from −10° to 60° in longitude. The averaged 3D contour maps of Δφ obtained (Fig. 6) revealed a frontal acute zone with a substantially higher resolution (i.e. lower Δφ) across the frontal eye region than was previously described using the antidromic pseudopupil, anatomical or behavioural methods6,16,26–30. Along the equator, Δφ stays below 1.7° to at least 40° of azimuth. Along the vertical orientation, resolution falls off more rapidly as one moves into the dorsal visual field, with frontal values of Δφ already exceeding 2° by 20° of elevation. Resolution below the equator is more impressive, with Δφ remaining below 1.9° down to −30° across the entire frontal region of the visual world. The centre of this acute zone corresponds to the frontal head axis, with a minimum Δφ of just below 1.3°. Interestingly, this corresponds well with the region of the eye where we previously obtained the smallest acceptance angles, around 1.6°, using electrophysiological recordings31. Note that according to earlier maps of the visual fields38, this is a region of the eye with roughly 10° of binocular overlap (shown as a dashed line in Fig. 6). Hence, at the very front of the visual fields, objects viewed by the area of maximal acuity (Δφ < 1.5°) would be seen by both eyes.
Discussion
We used fluorescent dyes (Lucifer Yellow or Neurobiotin 488) to retrogradely stain the photoreceptors in compound eyes of living, intact, insects. We successfully obtained a clear, bright, pseudopupil that is visible both in clear compound eyes (Aeshna cyanea, Eristalis tenax, Anthophora sp.) and in dark, pigmented, compound eyes (Apis mellifera). Our data support the potential of this method for reliable pseudopupil analysis in a wide variety of compound eyes that have previously been problematic.
Although this method allowed us to obtain excellent pseudopupils, we observed a high variation in performance among species used and among samples. In particular, species with less pigmented eyes such as Eristalis tenax show the fastest uptake, with an induced fluorescent pseudopupil visible within 30 min, if kept in the dark. In dark, heavily pigmented eyes like those of Apis mellifera, the fluorescent pseudopupil took up to 12 h to be visible. Besides the presence of pigments, other factors may influence the penetration and uptake of the dyes, so future experimenters will need to optimise the dissection and application of the fluorophore for their selected target species. For example, we noticed differences in the rate of uptake depending on whether glands and tracheal tubes were removed, as well as how much the sample was subsequently exposed to light.
It has previously been reported that light might facilitate and speed up the endocytotic uptake of Lucifer Yellow by photoreceptors when injected through the retina39. We certainly observed a large increase in brightness of the eye during exposure to light while imaging under the epifluorescence microscope. Thus, in order to acquire images and map the pseudopupil across large areas of the eye it is important to minimize the exposure time, and to minimize the light intensity of both the microscope and the laboratory environment. Note that the dye diffuses throughout the haemolymph and other structures in the eye will also appear fluorescent (e.g. antennal pedicel and ocelli).
As we mentioned earlier, if left incubating for a long enough time, even in the dark (from about 3–4 h in the dark at RT for the drone fly Eristalis), the entire eye will end up glowing (Fig. 2e), frustrating further attempts to image the pseudopupil. We observed this for both Lucifer Yellow and for Neurobiotin 488. In future it may be worth additional experimentation with other neuronal tracers or fluorophores to see if these provide better control of this non-specific uptake.
While the light-driven uptake by other eye structures limits the time during which the induced fluorescent pseudopupil can be used to map visual acuity, the resulting fluorescence of a fully fluorescent eye may also be useful for other applications. For example, by constructing confocal image stacks of the corneal surface, we were able to obtain a complete 3-dimensional model of the corneal facets (Fig. 2e and Supplementary Information video 1, part 2). Accurate models of the whole corneal surface of the eye (and to a limited extent, of sub-corneal structures) could thus be obtained with a relatively simple procedure from a living eye, without fixing the head, similar to the structural illumination microscopy method used in non-fixed eyes. In this respect, this method may be superior for counting facet numbers, measuring facet diameters or for mapping corneal axes, compared with alternatives such as µCT imaging of fixed tissue18,19, or construction of facet maps from replicas made using nail polish that is peeled off the corneal surface40–42.
The method we present here establishes a basis for future comparative studies aimed at estimating the sampling resolution of dark, pigmented, compound eyes in intact animals. Compared to other techniques that physiologically record the sampling power of the eye (e.g. pattern electroretinogram14,15), the advantage of having a functional pseudopupil in a living eye is that it provides the easiest and most accurate method for mapping variations in visual acuity across the entire compound eye. Since many compound eyes show large variations in acuity across the eye, with specific acute zones associated with distinct differences in behaviour1, creating such maps is important for studying visual ecology and the evolution of compound eyes. Previous attempts to create such maps in dark-pigmented eyes have used antidromic illumination7,17,30,43 where light is passed through the back of the head of the insect. However, this preparation is often ex vivo and the head is prone to desiccation and thus to possible artefacts coming from misalignments between the light path and the ommatidial axis. Alternatively, in fixed compound eyes the ommatidial axis can be estimated anatomically. However, even if fixation is perfect, anatomical estimates are limited by departures from basic assumptions based on ideal eye geometry. The gaze direction for a single ommatidium is determined by the specific position of the tips of the receptors which lay together in different numbers and patterns depending on the type of compound eye and species (see for a review44). In an ‘ideal’ eye, each rhabdom is oriented such that its tip is co-axial with the overlying crystalline cone and the corneal lens. The angle between the receptor axes in two adjacent ommatidia (Δφ) then provides a measure of the sampling frequency for that particular area of the eye. However, the actual axis of the rhabdom does not always coincide with the axis estimated from a simple anatomical reconstruction of the cornea and lenses (e.g. an axis perpendicular to the corneal surface). The latter, obtained from a fixed and sectioned eye, can deviate significantly from the receptor axis, particularly away from the centre of the field of view and in eyes with large local variations in acuity22,45. This was elegantly illustrated by Stavenga (Fig. 23 in22) for a damselfly, where the centre of the corneal reflection obtained by coaxial illumination of the eye rarely coincides with that of the dark pseudopupil. This problem is further exacerbated in sectioned material by possible distortions due to compression at the microtome knife edge.
As an alternative to histological sectioning, detailed morphological reconstruction of whole fixed eyes with X-ray micro computed tomography techniques (µCT) have recently been used in an attempt to map ommatidial axes in a number of species46. However, as with anatomical methods, only highly detailed maps of inter-facet angles can be made with this method. In order to overcome this limitation, Bagheri and colleagues19 used µCT but developed semi-automatic software to plot the rhabdom axis rather than the facet axis. Although these techniques are leading edge technologies for samples that cannot be kept alive in the lab or that are only available as museum specimens, or even fossils (e.g.47), they are complicated by the same assumptions as other anatomical methods. We suggest that our fluorescent pseudopupil provides a more direct and reliable result under normal physiological conditions.
Our application of this technique to the honeybee forager (Apis mellifera) resolves disparities between numerous past studies that have rarely used anything like a direct measure of the Δφ (for a review see15). Both Kirshfeld29 and later Seidl30 used the antidromic illumination technique to visualize luminous pseudopupils in honeybees and then estimated a minimum Δφ in the frontal part of the eye of 1.8° and 1.65° respectively (average of horizontal and vertical values). We found a similar minimum in a broad region in the frontal part of the eye, particularly in the ventral visual field, but were also able to map the centre of this acute zone, with Δφ below 1.3°. This confirms that the frontal part of the eye has a much better resolution than previously estimated (Fig. 6). Recently, Taylor and colleagues18 used µCT to map the inter-facet angles of the eyes, as a proxy for estimating Δφ. They found frontal inter-facet angles of around 1.6°, approximately 20% larger than we report here. However, this was not the area of maximum acuity that they report. They found a region with inter-facet angles down to 1° (i.e. even smaller than we report for the frontal eye fields), but this region was centred 60° more laterally, and 30° more dorsally than the directly frontal acute zone we have identified. A similar minimum was also found by Baumgärtner16, again 60° away from the anterior axis of the eye, but around the equator. However, as Taylor and colleagues do acknowledge, these techniques have limitations that arise from skewed rhabdom orientations. Any such skewness is, of course, fully accounted for when directly imaging the pseudopupil. Hence, we assert that our map definitively identifies the area of maximum acuity to be in the frontal visual field, in agreement with our recent electrophysiological determinations of photoreceptor acuity measured across the eye31. The location of this acute zone makes perfect sense considering that honeybees frontally fixate objects of interest, such as the hive entrance or flowers. It is also an eye region that is clearly involved in frontal pattern discrimination (e.g.6,48,49).
Methods
(a) Animals
Insects (Apis mellifera, Eristalis tenax, Aeshna cyanea and Anthophora sp.) were collected in the field between May and November 2019, anaesthetised on ice for a few minutes and then immobilized in a pipette tip cut at its narrow end using hot beeswax and violin rosin (1:1). The head was tilted forwards by 45° in order to gain access to the back of the head and the mouthparts were fixed with wax. We then visualised the pseudopupil in these species using both the traditional pseudopupil method and our novel fluorescence method.
(b) The traditional pseudopupil method
This method has allowed reliable estimates of Δφ, and even mapping of this angle over the entire eye, for a large range of species (see below). The method relies on the fact that a prominent dark pseudopupil is visible on the cornea when incident light from the gaze direction of the observer (e.g. for an eye viewed through a microscope or camera) is either absorbed or reflected by structures within the ommatidia.
In many insects, such axial incident light is absorbed by pigment cells surrounding the crystalline cone and the rhabdomeres of ommatidia directed towards the viewer and the corneal facet lenses of these ommatidia thus appear darker than those of more distant ommatidia receiving light from other directions22,45 (Fig. 1a). Since each photoreceptor samples an area of space that overlaps with its neighbouring ommatidia, this dark area extends across a small region of nearby ommatidia, giving the appearance of a dark ‘pupil’, by analogy to that in a camera eye. Unlike a real pupil however, this “pseudopupil” moves as the eye is rotated in front of a fixed viewpoint. As mentioned above, by measuring the shift in the centre of the pseudopupil for known small rotations of the eye, it is thus possible to map the ommatidial axes (and thus Δφ) across the entire eye and to correlate this with changes in ommatidial facet diameter. Dark principal pseudopupils are seen in the apposition compound eyes of many species of insects and crustaceans (and have been exploited to measure Δφ, e.g. in mantids:45,50; dragonflies:33; carpenter bees:36,37; hornets:51; and fiddler crabs:52).
In some species, the pseudopupil is not dark (as a result of light absorption) but is instead bright under coaxial illumination due to reflection of incident light from structures within the ommatidia, such as a tapetum lining the back of the eye beneath the rhabdoms (as in butterflies:53,54) or due to the presence of other reflective structures within the ommatidia such as screening pigments (e.g. water striders:55; flies:22; nocturnal bees:35). Just as with their darker counterparts, these luminous pseudopupils have proved particularly useful for mapping Δφ in apposition eyes (e.g. butterflies:34,56; flies:57–60; water striders:55; nocturnal bees:35).
While the pseudopupil method provides a ‘gold standard’ technique for directly measuring sampling resolution in many species, its application to others is made difficult by the presence of large amounts of dark light-absorbing screening pigments below the cornea. This makes it difficult or impossible to distinguish the pseudopupil, unless antidromic illumination is used, i.e. by shining strong light through the head of the animal7,29,38, an approach which brings with it a suite of other problems.
(c) A novel, fluorescence-based pseudopupil method
To visualise the fluorescent pseudopupil, the cuticle on the back of one eye was cut using a scalpel and set to one side. Glands (if present) and tracheal tubes were also gently removed. The haemolymph was partially removed with tissue paper and about 20 µl of a freshly made mixture of 1:1 Dimethylsulfoxide (Sigma-Aldrich Sweden AB) and Ringer’s solution was applied on the open head capsule. The insect was then left in the dark for 20 min at RT. A few small crystals of a fluorescent dye (we used either Neurobiotin 488 (BioNordika Sweden AB) or Lucifer Yellow (Sigma-Aldrich Sweden AB)) were then applied on the open head capsule before the head capsule was resealed by replacing the removed section of cuticle and waxing it in place to avoid desiccation. The insect was then left to incubate in a box containing wet tissue paper for 15 min-1 h (Eristalis tenax and Aeshna cyanea) or up to 6–12 h (Anthophora sp. and Apis mellifera) and kept in the dark at RT, to allow infiltration of the fluorophore.
(d) Imaging
After incubation, each insect was imaged using either epi-illumination (for the fluorescent pseudopupil) or by using incident light in those species where the dark pseudopupil was visible (Anthophora & Aeshna), in a fluorescence stereomicroscope (Nikon SMZ18, BergmanLabora AB, Sweden) equipped with a GFP-B filter cube and Plan Apochromatic objectives: either 0.5× (working distance, WD, 71 mm), 1× (WD 60 mm) or 2× (WD 20 mm). Successful fluorescence labelling was seen as groups of ommatidia whose receptors were directed towards the objective lens, and clearly visible as a fluorescent patch (Figs. 1, 5), which then moved across the surface of the eye as the sample was slowly rotated along azimuth and/or elevation (Supplementary Information, video 1, part 1 and 3).
In order to acquire the images necessary to reconstruct a map of Δφ across the compound eye, single animals were put on a Leitz goniometer and similar methods as used by Rutowski and Warrant34 were then applied (these methods, in turn, were based on those developed by Land and Eckert57—see34 for a full description of the methods). Briefly, the insect was carefully positioned at the centre of the goniometer with the surface of the back of the head parallel to the stage of the microscope (i.e. horizontal) and the anterior centre of the head aligned with the zeros of both the latitude (elevation) and longitude (azimuth) axes of the goniometer. The goniometer allowed us to tilt the head around its centre in both latitude and longitude. Before starting to acquire images, small fluorescent crystals (Lucifer Yellow, Sigma-Aldrich Sweden AB) were sprinkled on the surface of the eye with the use of a fine paintbrush to provide visible landmarks for subsequent off-line analysis. Images were acquired after moving the goniometer along lines of both azimuth (from −10° to + 60°) and elevation (−40° to + 40°), in 10° steps.
Images were then captured using a Nikon SMZ18 fluorescence stereomicroscope (Nikon, BergmanLabora AB, Sweden) that had been modified by a 180° reversal of the imaging head and rotation of the objective turret to align the episcopic light source (Sola light engine SM-5-LCR-SB Lumencor®, USA) coaxially with the imaging pathway of a cooled sCMOS camera (Andor Zyla 5.5, Oxford Instruments) coupled to NIS Elements AR software (version 4.50, Nikon, BergmanLabora AB, Sweden).
In order to acquire Fig. 1c and 5 and video 1 (part 1 and 3), animals were mounted in a custom-built goniometer constructed from 2 motorized precision rotation stages (KPRMTE/M, Thorlabs Inc., USA) mounted on a manual translation stage (all components from Thorlabs Inc., USA). This allowed continuous or stepwise rotation of the sample both along azimuth and elevation. To acquire Fig. 2 and video 1 (part 2), images were acquired with a Leica SP8 DLS confocal microscope (Leica Microsystems A/S, Denmark).
(e) Analysis of eye maps
For each image captured after rotating the goniometer, the centre of the pseudopupil was obtained in facet row-based coordinates (see33), using the fluorescent landmarks on the eye. The average pseudopupil diameter at each location was measured by manually drawing a straight line through 5–8 ommatidia in focus near the centre of the pseudopupil using the Nikon software tool (NIS-Elements AR, version 4.50, Nikon, BergmanLabora AB, Sweden). A text file recording the centre of the pseudopupil (in facet row-based coordinates) for each combination of latitude and longitude, together with averaged facet diameter at the same location, were then fed into custom-built software (Facet version 4.0.0 for Mac OSX 2017,33) that allowed us to calculate the local average Δφ (within a rhomboidal eye surface area 5 facets long and 5 facets wide along the two facet rows defining the coordinate axes). The software then generated 3D projection plots (representing the 3D space around the eye) with the calculated Δφ and facet diameter given at each latitude and longitude. From these, contour maps showing changes in Δφ and facet diameter within the visual field of the eye could be visualised as 3D projection plots. Note that the Δφ obtained by this method represents the average of the angles subtended by nearest neighbouring ommatidia along all 3 hexagonally oriented rows of the eye, as used in a number of earlier studies33–36,54,55,58.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We thank Ola Gustafsson and the Microscopy Facility at the Department of Biology, Lund University.
Author contributions
Conceptualization, methodology, and writing: E.R., E.J.W. and D.O.C.; experiments and analysis: E.R. and D.O.C.; visualisation: E.R.; funding acquisition: E.R., E.J.W. and D.O.C.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Lund University. This research was supported by the Swedish Research Council (VR 2018-03452 to DOC and VR 2016-04014 to EJW) and Formas (project grant 2018-01218 to DOC and ER).
Data availability
The software Facet version 4.0.1 for Mac OSX 2017 is available from https://github.com/insectvision/Facet. Raw image data for Figure 6 are available on 10.5281/zenodo.5570599.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
The original online version of this Article was revised: In the original version of this Article a previous rendition of Figure 2 was published.
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Change history
11/9/2021
A Correction to this paper has been published: 10.1038/s41598-021-01737-x
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-00407-2.
References
- 1.Land, M. F. Variations in the structure and design of compound eyes. in Facets of Vision (eds. Stavenga, D.G., Hardie, R.C.). 90–111. (Springer, 1989).
- 2.Land MF. Visual acuity in insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 1997;42:147–177. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.42.1.147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Warrant EJ, McIntyre PD. Arthropod eye design and the physical limits to spatial resolving power. Prog. Neurobiol. 1993;40:413–461. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(93)90017-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cronin T, Johnsen S, Marshall NJ, Warrant EJ. Visual Ecology. Princeton University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hecht S, Wolf E. The visual acuity of the honey bee. J. Gen. Physiol. 1929;12:727–760. doi: 10.1085/jgp.12.6.727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Srinivasan MV, Lehrer M. Spatial acuity of honeybee vision and its spectral properties. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 1988;162:159–172. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Spaethe J, Chittka L. Interindividual variation of eye optics and single object resolution in bumblebees. J. Exp. Biol. 2003;206:3447–3453. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Macuda T, Gegear RJ, Laverty TM, Timney B. Behavioural assessment of visual acuity in bumblebees (Bombus impatiens) J. Exp. Biol. 2001;204:559–564. doi: 10.1242/jeb.204.3.559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Chakravarthi A, Baird E, Dacke M, Kelber A. Spatial vision in Bombus terrestris. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2016;10:17. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Chakravarthi A, Rajus S, Kelber A, Dacke M, Baird E. Differences in spatial resolution and contrast sensitivity of flight control in the honeybees Apis cerana and Apis mellifera. J. Exp. Biol. 2018;221:jeb184267. doi: 10.1242/jeb.184267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Buchner, E. Behavioral analysis of spatial vision in insects. in Photoreception and Vision in Invertebrates (ed. Ali, M. A.). 561–621 (Plenum, 1984).
- 12.Wehner, R. Spatial vision in arthropods. In Handbook of Sensory Physiology. Vol. VII 6C (ed. Autrum, H.). 287–616. (Springer, 1981).
- 13.Lawson KKK, Srinivasan MV. Contrast sensitivity and visual acuity of Queensland fruit flies (Bactrocera tryoni) J. Comp. Physiol. A. 2020;206:419–428. doi: 10.1007/s00359-020-01404-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ogawa Y, Ryan LA, Palavalli-Nettimi R, Seeger O, Hart NS, Narendra A. Spatial resolving power and contrast sensitivity are adapted for ambient light conditions in Australian Myrmecia ants. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2019;7:18. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2019.00018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ryan LA, Cunningham R, Hart NS, Ogawa Y. The buzz around spatial resolving power and contrast sensitivity in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Vis. Res. 2020;169:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2020.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Baumgärtner HD. Formensinn und die Sehschärfe der Bienen. Z. vgl. Physiol. 1928;7:56–143. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bergman M, Rutowski RL. Eye morphology and visual acuity in the pipevine swallowtail (Battus philenor) studied with a new method of measuring interommatidial angles. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2016;117:646–654. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Taylor GJ, Tichit P, Schmidt MD, Bodey AJ, Rau C, Baird E. Bumblebee visual allometry results in locally improved resolution and globally improved sensitivity. Elife. 2019;8:1. doi: 10.7554/elife.40613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Bagheri ZM, Jessop AL, Kato S, Partridge JC, Shaw J, Ogawa Y, Hemmi JM. A new method for mapping spatial resolution in compound eyes suggests two visual streaks in fiddler crabs. J. Exp. Biol. 2020;223:jeb210195. doi: 10.1242/jeb.210195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.van Hateren JH, Hardie RC, Rudolph A, Laughlin SB, Stavenga DG. The bright zone, a specialized dorsal eye region in the male blowfly Chrysomyia megacephala. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 1989;164:297–308. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bekkouche BMB, Fritz HKM, Rigosi E, O’Carroll DC. Comparison of transparency and shrinkage during clearing of insect brains using media with tunable refractive Index. Front. Neuroanat. 2020;14:599282. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2020.599282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Stavenga, D. G. Pseudopupils of compound eyes. in Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. VII/6A (ed. Autrum, H.) 357–439 (Springer, 1979).
- 23.Srinivasan MV. Honey bees as a model for vision, perception, and cognition. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2010;55:267–284. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.010908.164537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Horridge A. The spatial resolutions of the apposition compound eye and its neuro-sensory feature detectors: Observation versus theory. J. Insect Physiol. 2005;51:243–266. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2004.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Giger, A.D. Honeybee Vision: Analysis of Pattern Orientation. PhD thesis, Australian National University. 10.25911/5D7639C8D3689 (1996).
- 26.del Portillo J. Beziehungen zwischen den Öffnungswinkeln der Ommatidien, Krümmung und Gestalt der Insektenaugen und ihrer funktionellen Aufgabe. Z. Vgl. Physiol. 1936;23:100–145. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wiitanen W, Varela FG. Analysis of the organization and overlap of the visual fields in the compound eye of the honeybee (Apis mellifera) J. Gen. Physiol. 1971;57:303–325. doi: 10.1085/jgp.57.3.303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Warrant EJ, Porombka T, Kirchner WH. Neural image enhancement allows honey bees to see at night. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 1996;263:1521–1526. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kirchner K. Optomotorische Reaktionen der Biene auf bewegte ‘Polarisations-Muster’. Z. Naturforsch. 1973;28c:329–338. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Seidl, R. Die Sehfelder und Ommatidien-Divergenzwinkel von Arbeiterin, Königin und Drohn der Honigbiene (Apis mellifica). PhD thesis, Technische Hochschule Darmstadt (1982).
- 31.Rigosi E, Wiederman SD, O’Carroll DC. Visual acuity of the honey bee retina and the limits for feature detection. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:45972. doi: 10.1038/srep45972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Franceschini N. Sampling of the visual environment by the compound eye of the fly: Fundamentals and applications. in Photoreceptor Optics. (ed. Snyder, A.W., Menzel, R.) 98–125 (Springer, 1975).
- 33.Sherk TE. Development of the compound eyes of dragonflies (odonata). III. Adult compound eyes. J. Exp. Zool. 1978;203:61–80. doi: 10.1002/jez.1402030107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rutowski RL, Warrant EJ. Visual field structure in a butterfly Asterocampa leilia (Lepidoptera, Nymphalidae): Dimensions and regional variation in acuity. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 2002;188:1–12. doi: 10.1007/s00359-001-0273-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Warrant EJ, Kelber A, Gislén A, Greiner B, Ribi W, Wcislo WT. Nocturnal vision and landmark orientation in a tropical halictid bee. Curr. Biol. 2004;14:1309–1318. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.07.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Somanathan H, Kelber A, Borges RM, Wallén R, Warrant EJ. Visual ecology of Indian carpenter bees II: Adaptations of eyes and ocelli to nocturnal and diurnal lifestyles. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 2009;195:571–583. doi: 10.1007/s00359-009-0432-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Somanathan H, Borges RM, Warrant EJ, Kelber A. Visual adaptations for mate detection in the male carpenter bee Xylocopa tenuiscapa. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0168452. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0168452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Seidl R, Kaiser W. Visual field size, binocular domain and the ommatidial array of the compound eyes in worker honey bees. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 1981;143:17–26. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wilcox M, Franceschini N. Illumination induces dye incorporation in photoreceptor cells. Science. 1984;225:851–854. doi: 10.1126/science.6206565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ribi WA. A unique hymenopteran compound eye. The retina fine structure of the digger wasp Sphex cognatus Smith (Hymenoptera, Sphecidae) Zool. Jb. Anat. 1978;100:299–342. [Google Scholar]
- 41.van Praagh JP, Ribi WA, Wehrhahn C, Wittmann D. Drone bees fixate the queen with the dorsal frontal part of their compound eyes. J. Comp. Physiol. 1980;136:263–266. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Greiner B, Ribi WA, Warrant EJ. Retinal and optical adaptations for nocturnal vision in the halictid bee Megalopta genalis. Cell Tissue Re. 2004;316:377–390. doi: 10.1007/s00441-004-0883-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Dyer AG, Streinzer M, Garcia J. Flower detection and acuity of the Australian native stingless bee Tetragonula carbonaria Sm. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 2016;202:629–639. doi: 10.1007/s00359-016-1107-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Nilsson, D.-E. Optics and evolution of the compound eye. in Facets of Vision (ed. Stavenga, D.G., Hardie, R.C.) 30–75. (Springer, 1989).
- 45.Horridge GA. The separation of visual axes in apposition compound eyes. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 1978;285:1–59. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1978.0093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Baird E, Taylor G. X-ray micro computed-tomography. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:R289–R291. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.01.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Taylor GJ, Hall SA, Gren JA, Baird E. Exploring the visual world of fossilized and modern fungus gnat eyes (Diptera: Keroplatidae) with X-ray microtomography. J. R. Soc. Interface. 2020;17:20190750. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2019.0750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Avarguès-Weber A, Giurfa M. Conceptual learning by miniature brains. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2013;280:20131907. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.1907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Lehrer M. Looking all around: Honeybees use different cues in different eye regions. J. Exp. Biol. 1998;201:3275–3292. doi: 10.1242/jeb.201.24.3275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rossel S. Regional differences in photoreceptor performance in the eye of the praying mantis. J. Comp. Physiol. 1979;131:95–112. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kelber A, Jonsson F, Wallén R, Warrant EJ, Kornfeldt T, Baird E. Hornets can fly at night without obvious adaptations of eyes and ocelli. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e21892. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Smolka J, Hemmi JM. Topography of vision and behaviour. J. Exp. Biol. 2009;212:3522–3532. doi: 10.1242/jeb.032359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Stavenga DG. Reflections on colourful ommatidia of butterfly eyes. J. Exp. Biol. 2002;205:1077–1085. doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.8.1077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Stavenga DG. Colour in the eyes of insects. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 2002;188:337–348. doi: 10.1007/s00359-002-0307-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Dahmen H. Eye specialisations in water striders: An adaptation to life in a flat world. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 1991;169:623–632. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Rutowski RL, Gislén L, Warrant EJ. Visual acuity and sensitivity increase allometrically with body size in butterflies. Arth. Struct. Devel. 2009;38:91–100. doi: 10.1016/j.asd.2008.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Land MF, Eckert H. Maps of the acute zones of fly eyes. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 1985;156:525–538. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Warrant, E. J. The design of compound eyes and the illumination of natural habitats. in Ecology of Sensing (eds. Barth, F.G., Schmid, A.). 187–213 (Springer, 2001).
- 59.Warrant, E. J. Matched filtering and the ecology of vision in insects. in The Ecology of Animal Senses—Matched Filters for Economical Sensing (Eds. von der Emde, G., Warrant, E. J.) 143–167. (Springer, 2016).
- 60.Straw AD, Warrant EJ, O’Carroll DC. A ‘bright zone’ in male hoverfly (Eristalis tenax) eyes and associated faster motion detection and increased contrast sensitivity. J. Exp. Biol. 2006;209:4339–4354. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Varela FG, Wiitanen W. The optics of the compound eye of the honeybee (Apis mellifera) J. Gen. Physiol. 1970;55:336–358. doi: 10.1085/jgp.55.3.336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The software Facet version 4.0.1 for Mac OSX 2017 is available from https://github.com/insectvision/Facet. Raw image data for Figure 6 are available on 10.5281/zenodo.5570599.