Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 May 9.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatr Res. 2021 Apr 30;91(4):962–969. doi: 10.1038/s41390-021-01554-y

Lateralized neonatal EEG coherence during sleep predicts language outcome.

Renée A Shellhaas 1, Ronald D Chervin 2,3, John DE Barks 1, Fauziya Hassan 1,3, Martha D Carlson 1, Joseph W Burns 4
PMCID: PMC8556395  NIHMSID: NIHMS1693594  PMID: 33931736

Abstract

Background:

Enriched language exposure may benefit infants in the neonatal intensive care unit. We hypothesized that changes in neonatal EEG coherence during sleep, in response to maternal voice exposure, predict language development.

Methods:

Convalescent neonates underwent 12-hour polysomnography. A recording of the mother’s voice was randomized to continuous playback in the first or second 6 hours. We calculated the imaginary coherence (ICOH – a measure of functional connectivity) between EEG leads. Spearman correlations were computed between ICOH and 18-month Bayley-III Language scores.

Results:

Thirty-five neonates were included (N=18 33-to-<35 weeks gestation; N=17 ≥35 weeks). Predictive value of ICOH during neonatal non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep was left-lateralized and varied with gestational age and voice playback. ICOH in the left-hemispheric (C3-Cz; T3-Cz) channels across multiple EEG frequency bands was associated with 18-month language scores (rho= −0.34 to −0.48). The association was driven by neonates born at 33-34 weeks gestation, and a trend suggested a possible effect of maternal voice at some EEG frequencies. Right-hemisphere ICOH (C4-Cz; T4-Cz) was not associated with language outcome.

Conclusion:

Left-hemispheric EEG functional connectivity during neonatal NREM sleep shows early signs of physiologic asymmetry that may predict language development. We speculate that sleep analyses could have unique prognostic value.

Introduction

During the first half of the third trimester, important cerebellar and white matter growth occurs, followed at 34 to 40 weeks postmenstrual age by a rapid acceleration of cortical gray matter development(1). During these critical periods of brain development, neonates who require intensive care are exposed to a hospital environment.

Many modern neonatal intensive care units (NICUs) now provide care for individual infants in private rooms. While clinical and architectural endeavors target reductions in ambient noise in the NICU(2, 3), the impact of noise reduction on sick infants remains unclear. A quiet environment may provide increased opportunity for sleep, but decreased language exposure in the private room could have adverse consequences(4).

Animal models demonstrate the time-specific role of the acoustic environment during auditory cortex organization and suggest that sound deprivation can result in abnormal structure and function of the auditory system(5-7). Conversely, animal data also suggest that auditory environment enrichment may have beneficial effects on neuronal development(8). A study of human infants born at <30 weeks gestation demonstrated that abnormalities of the nonprimary auditory cortex, demonstrated on serial brain MRI, were associated with lower language scores by age 2 years(9).

We hypothesized that enriched language exposure through playback of maternal voice recordings may benefit infants in the NICU. Our initial data demonstrated that neonates born at ≥35 weeks, but not those born at 33-34 weeks, had increased wakefulness in response to their mother’s voice, and also that exposure to the mother’s voice during sleep protected against awakening after bursts of loud noises(10). Moreover, our previous work demonstrated that quantitative sleep and electroencephalogram (EEG) markers shortly after birth may have prognostic value, in that they can show associations with 18-month neurodevelopmental outcomes(11). Others have suggested that neonatal EEG coherence – a measure of functional connectivity – reflects brain maturity(12, 13) and that there are measurable differences in neonatal EEG coherence between infants with hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy and controls(14). Initial studies of developmentally-focused NICU care suggest that methods of NICU care delivery can alter EEG coherence among preterm infants and that neurophysiological effects may persist in childhood(15, 16). Thus, we hypothesized that neonatal EEG coherence changes in response to maternal voice may predict neurodevelopmental outcomes, especially in the language domain.

Methods

This study was approved by a Michigan Medicine Institutional Review Board. A parent of every enrolled infant provided written informed consent.

Subjects:

Late-preterm and term newborns admitted to a level IV single-patient room NICU were recruited for participation in this study. Inclusion criteria were: birth at ≥33 weeks to 41 weeks gestation, need for NICU admission, and stable vital signs in an open crib. Exclusion criteria were: congenital brain abnormalities, airway abnormalities that were likely to cause sleep-disordered breathing, suspected or known genetic abnormalities that would alter developmental trajectories, severe encephalopathy that altered sleep-wake cycling, and abnormal neonatal hearing screen (brainstem auditory evoked responses). The clinical and demographic profile of included infants were presented elsewhere,(10) though the 18-month outcomes have not been presented previously. Our previous work delineated a clear difference in response to maternal voice recording for infants born at 33-34 weeks gestation compared with infants born at ≥35 weeks gestation(10), Thus we planned the present analyses to compare these two age groups.

Measurements:

Every infant underwent a 12-hour polysomnogram at the bedside in the NICU. Recordings were generally started after the infant’s evening feed (around 8pm). A registered polysomnographic technologist experienced with pediatric studies remained at the bedside throughout the study, to ensure high quality recordings. Details of the polysomnographic recording procedures and scoring have been presented elsewhere(10). The EEG leads were placed according to the international 10-20 system, modified for neonates(17, 18) and EEG was sampled at 256 Hz. Clinical care and feeding regimens were maintained during the polysomnogram. A continuous voice playback of the infant’s mother reading children’s books(19, 20) was randomized 1:1 to be played for the first or second 6-hour segment of the polysomnogram. The playback device was placed on a bedside table 1-2 feet from the infant.

All infants were invited to return for follow-up neurodevelopmental assessments with Bayley-III and neurological examinations at 18 months corrected age. The developmental outcome examiners were blinded to the advanced EEG analysis results.

Analysis:

We calculated Spearman correlations between clinical and polysomnographic variables and Bayley-III language scores. We then computed complex coherence between the EEG leads over sequential 3-second time epochs throughout the polysomnograms. Our previous work suggested that quantitative features of non-rapid eye movement (NREM, previously referred to as quiet) sleep were most predictive of outcomes among sick newborns(11). Thus, we focused our analyses on NREM sleep epochs; other sleep and wake stages were analyzed secondarily. Specifically, we evaluated mean coherence between electrode pairs. As we hypothesized that coherence associated with language exposure would be lateralized, we focused our analyses on the central and temporal electrode pairs (C3-Cz and C4-Cz; T3-Cz and T4-Cz). Central electrodes are typically available in clinical polysomnography; as language function is primarily localized to the temporal lobe we also analyzed data from the temporal electrodes. We did not employ artifact reduction, nor did we omit segments with poor signal quality. However, to render analyses generalizable to other NICUs, we relied on polysomnogram-defined sleep-wake stages for our analyses.

The coherence calculation used Welch’s method to reduce the variance of the estimates(21). We used the imaginary part of the coherence (ICOH) as a metric to mitigate the impact of volume conduction between leads(22). To examine the effect of maternal voice playback and sleep stage, we identified the associated time epochs. A bootstrap estimate of the mean ICOH for each lead pair was computed for the specified frequency range, sleep stage, and playback condition for each participant.

To evaluate associations with 18-month outcomes, we computed Spearman correlations between mean ICOH and Bayley-III Scores. To evaluate the difference in Spearman correlations during vs. without maternal voice playback, we calculated 10,000 bootstrap samples of the original data by sampling the ICOH and Bayley language data with replacement. For each sampling, the Spearman correlation was computed with and without maternal voice playback, and the difference of the correlation coefficients was computed. This procedure produced a bootstrap distribution for each correlation coefficient and the difference of the correlation coefficients such that the null hypothesis was rejected if zero was outside the 95% confidence interval of the distribution.

All analyses were conducted using Matlab (MathWorks, Natick, MA) and statistical significance was defined as p-value <0.05. The priority in this novel application of coherence analysis to neonatal sleep was to identify associations that may provide insight into neonatal sleep physiology. For this reason, we did not adjust for multiple comparisons.

Results

Forty-seven infants were enrolled, one was later excluded due to a genetic diagnosis associated with developmental delay, and 35 returned for 18-month follow-up (N=18 at 33-to-<35 weeks gestation, N=17 at ≥35 weeks). Three did not have maternal voice playback exposure so were excluded from part of the analysis. Clinical and demographic data are presented in Table 1.

Table 1:

Clinical, demographic, and sleep profiles of 35 newborn infants

Clinical and Demographic Data (mean ± standard deviation unless otherwise indicated)
Full sample
N=35
33-34 weeks gestation
N=18
≥35 weeks gestation
N=17
Gestational age at birth (weeks) 35.7±0.8 34.0±0.4 37.5±1.5
Legal age at time of polysomnography (days) 5.9±3.5 5.3±2.5 6.6±5.7
Postmenstrual age at time of polysomnography (weeks) 36.5±1.3 34.7±0.6 38.5±1.5
Birth weight (grams) 2584±57 2172±292 3021±631
Sex Female 18, Male 17 Female 11, Male 7 Female 8, Male 9
5-minute Apgar score Median 9 [IQR 8, 9] Median 9 [IQR 8,9] Median 8 [IQR 7, 9]
Neurological exam (Thompson) score Median 0 [IQR 0, 2] Median 0 [IQR 0, 2] Median 0 [IQR 0,2]
SNAPPE-II score Median 5 [IQR 0, 16] Median 0 [IQR 0, 5] Median 7 [IRQ 5, 18]
Primary NICU diagnosis Prematurity N=20 Prematurity N=17 Prematurity N=3
Respiratory diagnoses N=8 Respiratory diagnoses N=1 Respiratory diagnoses N=7
Hypoglycemia N=4 Hypoglycemia N=4
Other N=3a Other N=3
Sleep Data
Polysomnogram Summary Results Normal N=13 Normal N=61 Normal N=7
Primary sleep apnea of infancy N=12 Primary sleep apnea of infancy N=6 Primary sleep apnea of infancy N=6
Central sleep apnea N=2 Central sleep apnea N=1 Central sleep apnea N=1
Obstructive sleep apnea N=2 Obstructive sleep apnea N=2 Obstructive sleep apnea N=0
Obstructive & Central sleep apnea N=3 Obstructive & Central sleep apnea N=1 Obstructive & Central sleep apnea N=2
Hypoventilation N=1 Hypoventilation N=1 Hypoventilation N=0
Unspecified sleep disturbance N=2 Unspecified sleep disturbance N=1 Unspecified sleep disturbance N=1
Apnea-Hypopnea Index (AHI) 22.8±14.8 27.0±17.7 18.3±9.5
 REMb sleep AHI 33.0±19.2 37.5±20.6 29.1±17.1
 NREMc sleep AHI 14.1±16.3 19.0±21.3 8.8±4.5
Obstructive Apnea Index 3.8±6.0 5.9±7.8 1.5±1.1
Central Apnea Index 6.9±8.7 9.1±11.4 4.7±2.3
Hypopnea Index 12.1±6.4 12.0±5.7 12.2±7.2
% time with O2 saturation <90% 5.4±9.8 3.7±4.8 7.1±13.3
% of Total Sleep Time Spent in REMb Sleep 49.2±6.9 49.0±4.3 49.4±8.9
% of Total Sleep Time Spent in NREMc Sleep 33.1±7.2 35.5±5.8 30.6±7.8
% of Total Sleep Time Spent in Indeterminate Sleep 17.7±5.2 15.5±4.4 20.0±5.2
% of Total Recording Time Spent Awake 14.0±5.8 12.9±5.8 17.4±4.9
Outcome Data (Bayley-III Scores)
Language score 97.5±4.4 97.2±16.7 97.8±11.9
Cognitive score 102.6±7.1 102.2±10.9 102.9±12.8
Motor score 103.5±2.1 104.0±12.0 103±9.1
a

Other diagnoses included sepsis (N=1), hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy (N=1), and observation due to maternal myasthenia gravis (N=1)

b

REM=Rapid Eye Movement

c

NREM=Non-Rapid Eye Movement

Bayley-III scores were, on average, about 5 points (1/3 of a standard deviation) lower for the language subscale than for cognitive and motor subscales (97.5±4.4, compared to 102.6±7.1 and 103.5±2.1). None of the clinical or standard polysomnographic variables listed in Table 1, from the neonatal period, were associated with 18-month Bayley-III scores (p>0.15 for each association).

The predictive value of ICOH during neonatal NREM sleep was lateralized, and varied with gestational age. ICOH in the left-hemispheric channels across multiple EEG frequency bands was associated with 18-month language scores (C3-Cz: rho= −0.15 to −0.43, p<0.05; T3-Cz: rho=0.08 to −0.49, p<0.05; figure 1). However, right hemisphere ICOH (C4-Cz and T4-Cz) was not associated with language outcome (table 2). The association for left hemisphere ICOH was isolated to neonates born at <35 weeks gestation.

Figure 1:

Figure 1:

Among 35 neonates, the imaginary component of the coherence (ICOH) between left central and temporal EEG electrodes (C3-Cz and T3-Cz) was predictive of 18-month language outcome, especially during playback of a maternal voice recording (Panels A, C, E, and G). There were no such associations of right central and temporal (C4-Cz and T4-Cz) ICOH and outcome, with or without maternal voice playback (Panels B, D, F, and H).

Table 2:

Association between EEG coherence during NREMa sleep and 18-month language outcomes

Association between LEFT hemisphere ICOHb (C3-Cz and T3-Cz EEG electrodes) during neonatal
NREM sleep and 18-month language outcomes
EEG
Frequency
Maternal
Voice
Playback
C3-Cz ICOH all subjects C3-Cz ICOH
GAc 33 to <35 weeks
C3-Cz ICOH
GA ≥35 weeks
rho p-value rho p-value rho p-value
.33-2 Hz off −0.167 0.337 −0.245 0.327 −0.057 0.829
2-4 Hz off −0.379 0.025 * −0.287 0.249 −0.464 0.061
4-8 Hz off −0.194 0.265 −0.22 0.380 −0.209 0.421
8-12 Hz off −0.343 0.044 * −0.529 0.024 * −0.073 0.782
12-16 Hz off −0.263 0.126 −0.485 0.041 * 0.060 0.818
16-30 Hz off −0.199 0.252 −0.234 0.350 −0.132 0.615
0.33-2 Hz on −0.287 0.111 −0.232 0.307 −0.350 0.201
2-4 Hz on −0.441 0.012 * −0.415 0.097 −0.474 0.075
4-8 Hz on −0.259 0.152 −0.434 0.082 −0.036 0.899
8-12 Hz on −0.371 0.037 * −0.637 0.006 * 0.066 0.814
12-16 Hz on −0.419 0.017 * −0.660 0.004 * −0.165 0.557
16-30 Hz on −0.434 0.013 * −0.600 0.011 * −0.273 0.326
EEG
Frequency
Maternal
Voice
Playback
T3-Cz ICOH all subjects T3-Cz ICOH
GAc 33 to <35 weeks
T3-Cz ICOH
GA ≥35 weeks
rho p-value rho p-value rho p-value
0.33-2 Hz off 0.118 0.501 0.059 0.816 0.119 0.648
2-4 Hz off −0.318 0.063 −0.302 0.223 −0.232 0.369
4-8 Hz off −0.359 0.034 * −0.326 0.187 −0.427 0.088
8-12 Hz off −0.498 0.002 * −0.532 0.023 * −0.403 0.108
12-16 Hz off −0.321 0.060 −0.417 0.085 −0.207 0.426
16-30 Hz off −0.361 0.033 * −0.243 0.331 −0.421 0.093
0.33-2 Hz on 0.038 0.838 0.011 0.966 −0.002 0.995
2-4 Hz on −0.284 0.115 −0.364 0.151 −0.219 0.433
4-8 Hz on −0.383 0.031 * −0.414 0.098 −0.364 0.182
8-12 Hz on −0.507 0.003 * −0.656 0.004 * −0.283 0.306
12-16 Hz on −0.480 0.005 * −0.536 0.027 * −0.418 0.121
16-30 Hz on −0.534 0.002 * −0.570 0.017 * −0.474 0.075
Association between RIGHT hemisphere ICOH (C4-Cz and T4-Cz EEG electrodes) during neonatal
NREM sleep and 18-month language outcomes
EEG
Frequency
Maternal
Voice
Playback
C4-Cz ICOH all subjects C4-Cz ICOH
GA 33 to <35 weeks
C4-Cz ICOH
GA ≥35 weeks
rho p-value rho p-value rho p-value
0.33-2 Hz off 0.065 0.709 0.206 0.412 −0.161 0.537
2-4 Hz off −0.085 0.625 −0.033 0.896 −0.196 0.452
4-8 Hz off −0.256 0.138 −0.092 0.716 −0.332 0.193
8-12 Hz off −0.253 0.142 −0.248 0.320 −0.164 0.530
12-16 Hz off −0.327 0.055 −0.456 0.057 −0.124 0.635
16-30 Hz off −0.279 0.105 −0.394 0.106 −0.037 0.888
0.33-2 Hz on −0.143 0.435 −0.089 0.736 −0.285 0.303
2-4 Hz on −0.148 0.418 −0.246 0.342 −0.03 0.914
4-8 Hz on −0.270 0.135 −0.218 0.402 −0.291 0.293
8-12 Hz on −0.154 0.400 −0.277 0.283 0.066 0.814
12-16 Hz on −0.182 0.319 −0.345 0.174 0.115 0.684
16-30 Hz on −0.322 0.073 −0.465 0.060 −0.147 0.601
EEG
Frequency
Maternal
Voice
Playback
T4-Cz ICOH all subjects T4-Cz ICOH
GA 33 to <35 weeks
T4-Cz ICOH
GA ≥35 weeks
rho p-value rho p-value rho p-value
0.33-2 Hz off 0.033 0.850 0.004 0.987 −0.015 0.955
2-4 Hz off −0.183 0.293 −0.142 0.575 −0.202 0.438
4-8 Hz off −0.145 0.405 −0.027 0.916 −0.219 0.399
8-12 Hz off −0.173 0.320 −0.299 0.228 0.034 0.896
12-16 Hz off −0.247 0.152 −0.387 0.113 −0.034 0.896
16-30 Hz off −0.166 0.340 −0.419 0.083 0.150 0.565
0.33-2 Hz on −0.139 0.447 −0.166 0.524 −0.203 0.469
2-4 Hz on −0.333 0.063 −0.515 0.034 * −0.104 0.712
4-8 Hz on −0.108 0.558 −0.194 0.455 −0.036 0.899
8-12 Hz on −0.046 0.801 −0.283 0.272 0.341 0.214
12-16 Hz on −0.067 0.715 −0.312 0.222 0.228 0.414
16-30 Hz on −0.033 0.859 −0.434 0.082 0.456 0.088
a

NREM = non-rapid eye movement

b

ICOH = imaginary part of the coherence (presented as average ICOH across each frequency range)

c

GA = gestational age

*

denotes p<0.05

In contrast, the associations between EEG ICOH and Bayley-III scores during REM sleep, indeterminate sleep, or wakefulness were less consistent, though still left lateralized (supplemental table). There was no association between EEG ICOH in these central and temporal channels and the Bayley-III cognitive or motor scale scores.

The bootstrap distributions of the difference of the correlation coefficients with and without voice playback for the T3-Cz electrode pair are displayed in Figure 2. The correlation coefficients were most different at the higher frequency bands), while they did not appear to change in response to maternal voice playback at the lower frequencies.

Figure 2:

Figure 2:

Histograms from the bootstrap analyses of the difference in Spearman correlation of the imaginary component of the coherence (ICOH) during NREM sleep, using the T3-Cz electrode pair, and Bayley-III language scores, with and without maternal voice playback, for 32 infants (panel A: 4 to <8Hz; panel B: 8 to <12Hz; panel C: 12 to <16Hz; panel D: 16-30Hz). The vertical blue lines indicate the 2.5 and 97.5 percentile values. Differences in ICOH with and without maternal voice playback were greater for higher EEG frequencies.

Discussion

Results of this cross-sectional polysomnographic study of 35 neonates in an intensive care unit suggest that patterns of EEG functional connectivity during NREM sleep show left-lateralized predictive value for language development at 18 months. These results were distinct from those for 18-month cognitive and motor outcomes, which showed no associations with EEG functional connectivity at these electrode positions, and therefore suggest plausible underlying neuroanatomic specificity, as opposed to spurious results. Functional connectivity at certain frequencies was most predictive in the subset of infants born at 33 to <35 weeks gestation. In addition, a trend suggested that for some frequencies, playback of the mother’s voice may enhance the predictive value of the concurrent ICOH for language outcomes. In the absence of strong prognostic value from traditional neonatal clinical data, neurologic exam scores, or standard polysomnographic variables, we speculate that digital analyses of the sleep EEG could have unique value.

Language development is a complex and dynamic process that is vulnerable to disruption during the time that most sick neonates are cared for in the NICU. Among 90 very preterm infants (<30 weeks) without severe brain injury who were studied with serial brain MRI and 2-year follow-up, disturbance of the maturation of nonprimary cortex (but not primary cortex) was associated with abnormal language development(9). A landmark report suggested that for preterm infants, the private NICU room environment is associated with reduction of expected sulcal asymmetry in the auditory cortex by term equivalent age and that this manifests as poorer language development at age 2 years(4). However, others reported that there was no discernible difference in white matter diffusion parameters between infants cared for in private rooms versus an open bay NICU and that these MRI parameters did not predict 2-year language scores(9). Here, we demonstrate that a functional measure of brain development – the EEG ICOH – may also measure relevant brain maturation that relates to language development for some preterm infants. We also add that this measure may change with enriched exposure to the maternal voice.

Others have reported decreased heart rate and respiratory rate for preterm infants (30 to <37 weeks gestation) during exposure to recorded lullabies and taped maternal voice, with an effect that was most pronounced for infants with higher gestational ages(23). For the present cohort, exposure to the maternal voice playback insulated neonates from arousals associated with peak NICU noise epochs. The impact of voice exposure on immediate polysomnographic data was highest in late-preterm and term infants(10). Yet, the present analyses raise the possibility that functional connectivity changes in response to the same voice exposure may predict language outcomes for preterm infants rather than late-preterm and term infants. The impact of maternal voice exposure may not be isolated to late-preterm and term neonates, but instead may differ across stages of brain maturation. Others have reported lateralized maturational EEG changes in a nonlinear spectral measure for preterm neonates who were offered skin to skin contact over eight weeks(24). Such findings, combined with those presented here, underscore that specialization of the brain already exists in preterm infants – responses to tactile sensory stimuli localize to the right hemisphere while auditory stimuli localize to the left hemisphere.

Importantly, auditory stimulation is but one of multiple sensory modalities that influence brain development. All neonates, even those who appear healthy after birth, express age-specific consequences of brain maturation related to maternal-placental-fetal factors. This is particularly a consideration for preterm neonates – even late preterm infants – and their immature brain maturation at birth. There remains a gap in developmental care research with regard to individual infants’ responses to interventions relative to genetic vulnerability or resilience and adversities that are not detectable by contemporary maternal care and surveillance. These variables may alter infants’ responses to a particular enrichment paradigm (such as sound or tactile input) according to the effect on the cortical region required for the response.

We report results from 35 neonates who underwent gold-standard bedside polysomnography and 18-month developmental follow-up. Our study focused on detailed analysis of each newborn’s sleep physiology; the sample size was, by necessity, limited. In this novel application of coherence analyses in neonates, we prioritized identification of new insight into sleep physiology and neurodevelopment, and did not adjust for multiple comparisons. Results therefore merit replication. Some additional limitations merit discussion. We analyzed the potential impact of changes in the acoustic environment on neonatal EEG-sleep data, but we acknowledge that other factors also influence sleep in the NICU (e.g. handling(25), medications, and light exposure). We are unable to account for these other factors in the present analysis. We did not measure exposure to live spoken words in the home prior to conducting the Bayley-III exams. The home language environment, and other factors, may have influenced the developmental trajectories of participants in this study. Future work should include more comprehensive evaluation and statistical adjustment for socioeconomic status and other factors that may relate to developmental outcomes; such measures were beyond the scope of this initial study.

Conclusions:

Clearly predictive, non-invasive measures of neonatal brain development are of utmost importance to clinicians and parents alike. We demonstrate for the first time that lateralized neonatal EEG coherence during quiet sleep is associated with 18-month language development. Whether coherence is a fixed feature (e.g. reflects a subtle, unchangeable functional brain abnormality) or could be improved with enriched language exposure and careful attention to early language development remains an open question. Importantly, our findings were most robust for the youngest neonates in this study. Language exposure interventions likely need to be tailored to infants’ gestational and post-menstrual age. We have no clear explanation for why impact of the maternal voice might be more rather than less prominent in a window that lies well before the end of fetal development. We speculate however that such a finding, if confirmed, could have implications for language exposure in utero, not just after premature delivery. The optimal timing of language exposure, the influence of that exposure on sleep, and the association with developmental trajectory should be key priorities for continued research.

Supplementary Material

1

Impact statement:

  • During neonatal non-rapid eye movement sleep, EEG functional connectivity predicts future language development.

  • Left temporal and central EEG coherence –specifically the imaginary component of coherence – is predictive, whereas the same analysis from the right hemisphere is not.

  • These results appear to vary according to the infant’s gestational age, and a trend suggests they may be enhanced by measuring functional connectivity during exposure to the mother’s voice.

  • These findings identify early evidence of physiologic differentiation within the cerebral hemispheres and raise the possibility that neonatal non-rapid eye movement sleep has a role to play in language development.

Acknowledgements:

The authors deeply appreciate the infants and parents for their interest in research and enthusiasm for this work. They also thank the clinical research coordinators and sleep technologists whose tireless attention to detail and care for each of the participating infants was integral to this study.

Statement of financial support:

This study was supported by NIH and the Michigan Institute for Clinical and Health Research (R21HD083409; UL1TR002240).

Footnotes

Author disclosures: Dr. Shellhaas’ research is supported by grants from NIH, PCORI, the Pediatric Epilepsy Research Foundation, and the University of Michigan. She serves as a consultant for the Epilepsy Study Consortium and as an Associate Editor for Neurology. She receives royalties from UpToDate for authorship of topics related to neonatal seizures.

Dr. Chervin has had financial relationships with the American Academy of Sleep Medicine, the Associated Professional Sleep Societies, UpToDate, and Cambridge University Press; has been a member of the boards for the International Pediatric Sleep Association and the not-for-profit Sweet Dreamzzz; is a member of an advisory board for the not-for-profit Pajama Program; receives research support from the NIH; is named in patents and copyrighted material, owned by the University of Michigan, that concern identification and treatment of sleep disorders; and has received royalties from Zansors.

Dr. Barks receives grant funding from NIH.

Dr. Hassan has previously served as a consultant for Biogen, and has received research funding from Jazz pharmaceuticals.

Dr. Carlson has nothing to disclose.

Dr. Burns receives grant funding from NIH and is named in patents, owned by Michigan Technological University and the University of Michigan, that concern identification and treatment of sleep disorders.

Category of study: Clinical

Consent statement: A parent of every infant provided written informed consent to participate in the study.

References

  • 1.Matthews LG, et al. Brain growth in the NICU: critical periods of tissue-specific expansion. Pediatr Res. 2018;83(5):976–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.White RD. Designing environments for developmental care. Clin Perinatol. 2011;38(4):745–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.White RD. Recommended standards for newborn ICU design, 9th edition. J Perinatol. 2020;40(Suppl 1):2–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pineda RG, et al. Alterations in brain structure and neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm infants hospitalized in different neonatal intensive care unit envrionments. Journal of Pediatrics. 2014;164(1):52–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.de Villers-Sidani E, Chang EF, Bao S, Merzenich MM. Critcal period window for spetral tuning defned in the primay auditory cortex (A1) in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(1):180–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mowery TM, Kotak VC, Sanes DH. Transient Hearing Loss Within a Critical Period Causes Persistent Changes to Cellular Properties in Adult Auditory Cortex. Cerebral cortex (New York, NY : 1991). 2015;25(8):2083–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Oliver DL, Izquierdo MA, Malmierca MS. Persistent effects of early augmented acoustic environment on the auditory brainstem. Neuroscience. 2011;184:75–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bose M, et al. Effect of the environment on the dendritic morphology of the rat auditory cortex. Synapse (New York, NY). 2010;64(2):97–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Monson BB, et al. Differential Rates of Perinatal Maturation of Human Primary and Nonprimary Auditory Cortex. eNeuro. 2018;5(1). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Shellhaas RA, Burns JW, Barks JDE, Hassan F, Chervin RD. Maternal Voice and Infant Sleep in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit. Pediatrics. 2019;144(3). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Shellhaas RA, Burns JW, Hassan F, Carlson MD, Barks JDE, Chervin RD. Neonatal sleep-wake analyses predict 18-month neurodevelopmental outcomes. Sleep. 2017;40(11):zsx144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Meijer EJ, et al. Functional connectivity in preterm infants derived from EEG coherence analysis. European journal of paediatric neurology : EJPN : official journal of the European Paediatric Neurology Society. 2014;18(6):780–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.González JJ, et al. Assessment of electroencephalographic functional connectivity in term and preterm neonates. Clinical neurophysiology : official journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2011;122(4):696–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.McLaren J, Holmes GL, Berg MT. Functional Connectivity in Term Neonates With Hypoxic-Ischemic Encephalopathy Undergoing Therapeutic Hypothermia. Pediatr Neurol. 2019;94:74–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.McAnulty G, et al. School-age effects of the newborn individualized developmental care and assessment program for preterm infants with intrauterine growth restriction: preliminary findings. BMC Pediatr. 2013;13:25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Myers MM, et al. Family Nurture Intervention in preterm infants alters frontal cortical functional connectivity assessed by EEG coherence. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104(7):670–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1991;8(2):200–2. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Shellhaas RA, et al. The American Clinical Neurophysiology Society's Guideline on Continuous Electroencephalography Monitoring in Neonates. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;28(6):611–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Fox M Time for Bed: HMH Books for Young Readers; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wood A The Napping House: HMH Books for Young Readers; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Welch PD. The Use of fast Fourier transform for the estimation of power spectra: a method based on time averaging over short, modified periodograms. IEEE Transactions on Audio Electroacoustics. 1967;AU-15(2):70–3. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nolte G, et al. Identifying true brain interaction from EEG data using the imaginary part of coherency. Clinical neurophysiology : official journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2004;115(10):2292–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wirth L, et al. Effects of standardized acoustic stimulation in premature infants: a randomized controlled trial. J Perinatol. 2016;36(6):486–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kaffashi F, Scher MS, Ludington-Hoe SM, Loparo KA. An analysis of the kangaroo care intervention using neonatal EEG complexity: a preliminary study. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2013;124:238–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Levy J, et al. Impact of hands-on care on infant sleep in the neonatal intensive care unit. Pediatr Pulmonol. 2017;52(1):84–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES