Abstract
Background
The Cognitive Reserve (CR) theory posits that brains with higher reserve can cope with more cerebral damage to minimize clinical manifestations. The aim of this study was to examine the effect of education (CR proxy) on brain structure and function in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) patients and in cognitively healthy elderly (HC) individuals.
Methods
Fifty-seven AD patients, 57 aMCI patients and 48 HCs were included to investigate the relationships between education years and gray matter volume (GMV), regional homogeneity (ReHo) and functional connectivity (FC) in brain regions to show associations with both structure and function. Taking the severity of the disease into account, we further assessed the relationships in AD stratified analyses.
Results
In AD group, the GMV of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and ReHo in the left inferior temporal cortex (ITC) were inversely associated with education years, after adjustment for age, sex, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), and total intracranial volume or head motion parameters. Seed-based FC analyses revealed that education years were negatively correlated with the FC between the left anterior ITC and left mid frontal cortex as well as right superior frontal cortex and right angular gyrus. Stratified analyses results indicated that this negative relation between education and GMV, ReHo, FC was mainly present in mild AD, which was attenuated in moderate AD and aMCI groups.
Conclusions
Our results support the CR theory, and suggest that CR may be protective against AD related brain pathology at the early stage of clinical dementia. These findings could provide the locus of CR-related functional brain mechanisms and a specific time-window for therapeutic interventions to help AD patients to cope better with the brain pathological damage by increasing CR.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s12883-021-02445-9.
Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, Cognitive reserve, Education, Gray matter volume, Resting-state fMRI
Background
The reserve theory is established to interpret the apparent discrepancy between brain pathology and its clinical manifestations in different individuals. The theory posits that certain life experiences enable some people to withstand the brain pathology more effectively compared with others through enhancing the adaption of neural networks [1, 2]. Since the changes in brain recruitment associated with reserve are a normal response to increased task demands, this definition suggests that cognitive reserve is present in both healthy individuals and those with brain damage [3]. The cognitive/functional brain processes that support CR may already be present before the onset of brain pathologies. Most studies support the reserve hypothesis in neurodegenerative disorders. In Alzheimer’s disease (AD), patients with higher cognitive reserve (CR) can tolerate greater brain pathological damage at a particular degree of cognitive impairment than those with lower CR, such as coping with decreased cerebral 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) metabolism [4–8], cortical thinning, or gray matter volume (GMV) atrophy [4, 6, 9–11]; having more severe white matter damage [12, 13]; and also primary AD pathologies such as greater amyloid-beta (Aβ) accumulation and increased tau deposition [14–16].
Reserve is an abstract construct that cannot be measured directly. Several findings demonstrated that education was positively correlated with cognition level and thus influenced the risk of dementia. It is suggested that education should be deemed as the main indirect evaluation index of CR, though other indexes such as occupation or IQ may also be considered [17–19]. Higher educated individuals seem to withstand more severe brain damage. Although it is not a perfect measurement of CR, education has been used widely with diverse populations and in various studies, thus reinforcing the rationale for testing its connection to dementia. A crucial question is whether our measurement of brain changes could be used as CR-related biomarkers of AD pathology. In a recent study, cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) Aβ42, CSF tau, FDG-PET, and structural and functional MRI have been suggested as biomarkers of AD-related pathology [20]. Therefore the measures of AD-related pathology we used here were MRI-based measures of brain structure and function.
To test the CR hypothesis, in the present study, we investigated whether higher education is associated with comparatively lower regional GMV, lower regional homogeneity (ReHo) and decreased functional connectivity (FC) at a given level of global cognitive function in AD. We hypothesized that despite more severe brain structural and functional damage, highly educated elderly AD individuals could preserve cognitive ability comparable to poorly educated patients.
Methods
Participants
The total sample in this study was composed of 162 participants (57 AD patients, 57 aMCI patients, and 48 HCs). The AD and aMCI patients were recruited from the Dysmnesia Outpatient Department at the first Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University in Hefei, Anhui Province, China between 2017 and 2019. The HCs were recruited from the local community or were the spouses of the patients in the study. The diagnosis of AD in the study was established according to the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke-Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS-ADRDA) criteria of “probable” or “possible” AD [21, 22] and that patients showed medial temporal atrophy on structural magnetic resonance imaging [22, 23]. The diagnosis of aMCI was established according to the Petersen criteria [24, 25] and patients had a Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) score of 0.5. All aMCI subjects had memory complaints confirmed by neuropsychological testing, while absence of dementia. 32 aMCI patients (single domain) exhibit memory problems only, whereas 25 aMCI patients (multiple domain) also have impaired visuospatial ability. The criteria for HC comprised the following: (1) normal general physical status; (2) normal global cognitive function as measured by the MMSE (score range between 24 and 30); (3) CDR memory score of 0; and (4) without memory complaints. Exclusion criteria for all participants included: history of other neurological or psychiatric diseases or head injury with loss of consciousness, use of sedative drugs in the last 24 h before the neuropsychological assessment, drug or alcohol addiction, prior chronic exposure to neurotoxic substances.
Neuropsychological assessment
All participates were right-handed and underwent clinical evaluation and neuropsychological assessment. Educational attainment was assessed in terms of the highest year of schooling completed. The following neuropsychological test battery was administered to each subject for the purpose of establishing a clinical diagnosis [26]. (i) General cognitive function was assessed by the MMSE [27] and CDR score [28]. The severity of dementia was assessed by a neuropsychologist using both the CDR scale and the MMSE, defining the initial severity level of AD, which corresponded to mild (CDR 1), moderate (CDR 2), or severe dementia (CDR 3) [15]. (ii) The Chinese version of the auditory verbal learning test (AVLT) was used to evaluate episodic memory ability [29]. (iii) The verbal fluency test (VFT) was used to assess semantic memory ability [30]. (iv) The Digit Span Forward and Backward tests were used to evaluate working memory ability [31]. All participants gave their written informed consent and the study was approved by the Medical Research Ethics Committee of the first Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University.
Neuroimaging protocol acquisition
All participants were scanned on a General Electric Discovery MR750w 3.0 T scanner (General Electric, Waukesha, WI, USA) with a 24-channel head coil. MRI examinations were performed within 2 days after the completion of the cognitive tests. The MR imaging protocol included routine T2-weighted and fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images, three-dimensional (3D) high-resolution T1-weighted structure images, and resting-state functional MRI (rs-fMRI) images. Routine T2-weighted and FLAIR images were used to exclude subjects with organic brain diseases. 3D high-resolution T1-weighted structure images were acquired using the brain volume (BRAVO) sequence (TR = 8.5 ms; TE = 3.2 ms; TI = 450 ms; flip angle = 12°; 188 slices; slice thickness = 1 mm; no gap; FOV = 256 × 256 mm2; matrix = 256 × 256). The rs-fMRI scans were performed with an echo planar imaging sequence (TR = 2000 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 90°; slice thickness = 3 mm; slice gap = 1 mm; FOV = 220× 220 mm2; matrix = 64 × 64). A custom-built head coil cushion and earplugs were used to minimize head motion and dampen scanner noise. During the scans, subjects were instructed to keep their eyes closed, relax, and move as little as possible while not falling asleep.
White matter hyperintensity evaluation
The severity of white matter hyperintensity (WMH) was assessed visually on axial FLAIR sequences according to the modified Fazekas scale [32, 33]. This scale divided WMH into periventricular and deep categories. Periventricular WMH was graded according to the following patterns: 0 = absent, 1 = caps or a pencil-thin lining, 2 = a smooth halo, and 3 = irregular WMH extending into the deep white matter. Deep WMH was graded according to the following patterns: 0 = absent or single punctate foci, 1 = multiple punctate foci, 2 = beginning confluence of foci, and 3 = large fused foci. The total scores were acquired by adding the periventricular and deep WMH scores together. Two radiologists assessed and scored the WMH burden independently.
Voxel-based Morphometry analysis
The 3D T1-weighted structural images were processed using the VBM8 toolbox (http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm.html) in Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM8; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). First, all structural images were visually inspected to screen for artifacts or gross anatomical abnormalities, after which they were segmented into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid using the standard segmentation model. After an initial affine registration of the gray matter concentration map into Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, the gray matter concentration images were nonlinearly warped using the diffeomorphic anatomical registration through exponential Lie algebra (DARTEL) technique and then resampled to a voxel size of 1.5 mm × 1.5 mm × 1.5 mm [34]. The gray matter volume (GMV) map was obtained by multiplying the gray matter concentration map by the non-linear determinants that were derived from the spatial normalization step. Finally, the resultant GMV images were smoothed with a 8-mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel.
fMRI data preprocessing
Resting-state BOLD data were preprocessed using SPM and Data Processing & Analysis for Brain Imaging (DPABI, http://rfmri.org/dpabi). The first ten volumes for each participant were discarded to allow the signal to reach equilibrium and the participants to adapt to the scanning noise [35]. The 175 remaining volumes were corrected for the acquisition time delay between slices. Then, realignment was performed to correct the motion between time points. Head motion parameters were computed by estimating the translation in each direction and the angular rotation on each axis for each volume. All participants’ BOLD data were within the defined motion thresholds (i.e., translational or rotational motion parameters less than 3.0 mm or 3.0°). We also calculated frame-wise displacement (FD), which indexes the volume-to-volume changes in head position. Several nuisance covariates (the linear drift, the estimated motion parameters based on the Friston-24 model, the spike volumes with FD > 0.5, the white matter signal, and the cerebrospinal fluid signal) were regressed out from the data. The datasets were then band-pass filtered using a frequency range of 0.01 to 0.08 Hz. In the normalization step, individual structural images were firstly co-registered with the mean functional image, and then the transformed structural images were segmented and normalized to the MNI space using the DARTEL technique. Finally, each filtered functional volume was spatially normalized to MNI space using the deformation parameters estimated during the above step and resampled into a 3-mm cubic voxel.
ReHo analysis
ReHo assesses local intrinsic FC, being defined as the Kendall’s coefficient of concordance (KCC) for the time series of a given voxel with those of its 26 nearest neighboring voxels [36]. The KCC can be computed by the following formula:
where W is the KCC among given voxels, ranging from 0 to 1; Ri is the sum rank of the ith time point; is the mean of Ri; K is the number of time series within a measured cluster (K = 27, one given voxel plus its 26 neighbors), and n is the number of ranks (n = 175). Then, we normalized the ReHo of each voxel by dividing it by the mean ReHo value of the whole brain. Finally, each ReHo map was spatially smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 6 mm × 6 mm × 6 mm full width at half maximum.
Resting-state FC (rsFC) analyses
Prior to the rsFC analysis, the preprocessed data were additionally smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 6 mm × 6 mm × 6 mm full width at half maximum after spatial normalization. The rsFC was examined with a seed-based correlation approach. For the purpose of FC analyses, the regions showing a significant correlation with years of education from both the GMV and ReHo analyses were used as seeds. For each individual data set, Pearson’s correlation coefficients between the mean time series of each seed regions of interest (ROI) and time series of each voxel in the rest of the brain were computed and converted to z-values using Fisher’s r-to-z transformation to improve the normality.
Statistical analysis
Demographics and clinical characteristics [age, years of education, MMSE score, total intracranial volume, Frame-wise displacement] were compared in AD, aMCI, and HC groups using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Group difference in sex was tested by using Pearson’s Chi-square tests. Each cognitive composite score was computed by converting raw scores on each component of neuropsychological tests to z scores, using the mean and SD for all participants in the study, and averaging the z scores to yield the composite score.
For VBM analyses, voxel wise correlations were computed between education years and the GMV images using the multiple regression analysis of SPM8 in the AD, aMCI, and HC groups, respectively. The same analysis was performed for ReHo and seed-based rsFC. All analyses were controlled for age, sex, MMSE, and total intracranial volume (TIV) or head motion parameters. Considering that the cerebrovascular load may affect the mechanism of structural and functional connectivity of CR, fazekas score was added as a new covariate within the model. Multiple comparisons were corrected using a voxel-level false discovery rate (FDR) method with a significance level of P < 0.05. In addition, clusters surviving P < 0.05 corrected for family-wise error (FWE) were also considered significant. The regions that showed significant correlations were then considered ROI and the mean GMV and ReHo values in these regions were extracted for the subsequent linear regression analysis.
Given the severity of the dementia symptoms in AD, we further assessed the relationships between years of education and ROI-based neuroimaging variables (GMV, ReHo, and rsFC) in stratified analyses controlling for age, sex, MMSE, and TIV or head motion parameters. Between-group analyses were performed to investigate group difference of ROI-based neuroimaging variables (GMV, ReHo, and rsFC). All p values were two-tailed; P < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant. Finally, the Pearson correlation analyses were used to identify correlations between ROI-based neuroimaging variables (GMV, ReHo, and rsFC) with clinical cognitive function scores. All these statistical analyses were performed by using the SPSS 23.0 software package (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). All p values were two-tailed; P < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.
Results
Demographic and clinical characteristics
Demographics, clinical characteristics and neuropsychological scores are listed in Table 1. The three groups were well matched in age (one-way ANOVA, F = 0.491, P = 0.613), sex (Chi-square test, χ2 = 0.878, P = 0.645), and frame-wise displacement (one-way ANOVA, F = 0.768, P = 0.466). There were significant differences in MMSE score (one-way ANOVA, F = 255.546, P < 0.001), years of education (one-way ANOVA, F = 18.805, P < 0.001), and TIV (one-way ANOVA, F = 3.884, P = 0.023).
Table 1.
Characteristics | AD (n = 57) |
aMCI (n = 57) |
HC (n = 48) |
p Value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Women (%) | 31 (54) | 35 (61) | 30(63) | 0.645b |
Age (years) | 67.74 (8.28) | 66.33 (7.47) | 66.75 (7.41) | 0.613a |
Years of education | 6.14 (5.38) | 9.53 (4.71) | 11.69(3.48) | <0.001a |
MMSE score | 15.28 (5.22) | 26.35 (1.58) | 28.60(1.11) | <0.001a |
Clinical Dementia Rating Scale | ||||
1 (%) | 21 (37) | |||
2 (%) | 22 (39) | |||
3 (%) | 14 (25) | |||
TIV (cm3) | 1314.27 (109.09) |
1352.93 (99.70) |
1368.67 (103.03) |
0.023a |
Frame-wise displacement (mm) | 0.21(0.14) | 0.22(0.16) | 0.18(0.11) | 0.466a |
Fazekas score | 2(1–3) | 1(0.5–2.5) | 1(1–2) | 0.229c |
neuropsychological tests | ||||
AVLT (immediate) | 2.01(1.62) | 5.89(2) | 9.04(1.76) | <0.001a |
AVLT (delay) | 0(0–0) | 4.46(3.08) | 10.13(2.77) | <0.001c |
AVLT (recognize) | 10(6–12) | 12(11–14) | 14(13.5–15) | <0.001c |
VFT (Animals) | 9(7–10) | 13(10.5–14.5) | 18.13(3.93) | <0.001c |
DSpF | 5(4–6) | 7(6–8) | 8(7–8) | <0.001c |
DSpB | 3(2–3) | 4(3–5) | 5(4–6) | <0.001c |
cognitive functionscore | ||||
Episodic memoryd | −2.70(1.65) | 0.27(1.58) | 2.88(1.22) | <0.001a |
Semantic memoryd | −0.85(0.63) | 0.04(− 0.43–0.32) | 1.0(0.74) | <0.001c |
Working memoryd | −1.48(1.63) | 0.29(1.24) | 1.42(1.32) | <0.001a |
Datas with normal distribution are expressed as mean (SD) or as percentage
Datas with non-normal distribution are expressed as median (interquartile range)
aGroup difference was tested by using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA)
bGroup difference was tested by using Pearson’s Chi-square test
cGroup difference was tested by using Kruskal-Wallis test
dEach cognitive composite score was computed by converting raw scores on each component of neuropsychological tests to z scores, using the mean and SD for all participants in the study, and averaging the z scores to yield the composite score
Abbreviations: AD Alzheimer’s disease; aMCI amnestic mild cognitive impairment;, healthy control; MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination; TIV total intracranial volume; SD standard deviations; AVLT auditory verbal learning test; VFT verbal fluency test; DSpF Digit Span Forward; DSpB Digit Span Backward
Correlations between GMV and education years
Controlling for age, sex, MMSE, and TIV, significant negative correlations were observed between education years and the GMV of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) in AD patients (see Fig. 1, Table 2.). No significant negative correlations were observed in aMCI patients or HCs, and no positive correlations were observed in any of the groups. After adding the fazekas score as the new covariate, the similar results were obtained (see Fig. S1, Table S1.).
Table 2.
Region | Number of voxels | t-score | MNI coordinates | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | |||
Negative correlations between years of education and GMV | |||||
dorsal anterior cingulate cortex | 291 | −5.25 | 0 | 20 | 33 |
Negative correlations between years of education and ReHo | |||||
left anterior part of inferior temporal cortex | 65 | −6 | −57 | − 12 | −33 |
left posterior part of inferior temporal cortex | 59 | −4.69 | −51 | −30 | −21 |
Negative correlations between years of education and left aITC rsFC | |||||
right angular gyrus | 32 | −4.79 | 45 | −66 | 42 |
left mid frontal cortex | 27 | −5.57 | −39 | 15 | 57 |
right superior frontal cortex | 16 | −4.95 | 24 | 27 | 54 |
Structural MR (GMV): Adjusted for age, sex, MMSE, and TIV
Resting-state fMRI (ReHo and rsFC): Adjusted for age, sex, MMSE, and head motion parameters
Coordinates (x,y,z) are given in MNI standard space. All regions listed are statistically significant at p<0.05 level (FDR corrected)
Abbreviations: GMV gray matter volume; ReHo Regional Homogeneity; rsFC resting-state functional connectivity; MNI Montreal Neurological Institute; aITC anterior part of inferior temporal cortex; MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination; TIV total intracranial volume
Correlations between ReHo and education years
Controlling for age, sex, MMSE, and head motion parameters, education years were significantly negatively correlated with the ReHo values of the left inferior temporal cortex (ITC) in AD patients (see Fig. 2, Table 2.). No significant negative correlations were observed in aMCI patients or HCs, and no positive correlations were observed in any of the groups. The similar results were also observed after adding the fazekas score as the new covariate (see Fig. S2, Table S1.).
.
Correlations between seed-based FC and education years
The dACC and ITC were then used as seeds for the whole brain connectivity analyses. Thus we obtained the FC maps and the FC maps were then correlated with education years. Controlling for age, sex, MMSE, and head motion parameters, significant negative correlations were observed between education years and the connectivity of the left anterior ITC (aITC) with the left mid frontal cortex, as well with the right superior frontal cortex and right angular gyrus in AD patients (see Fig. 3, Table 2.). No significant negative correlations were observed in the dACC and posterior ITC (pITC) based FC and no positive correlations were found. The similar results were found after adding the fazekas score as the new covariate (see Fig. S3, Table S1.).
Correlations between the values extracted from ROI and education years in AD stratified analysis
Table 3 displays the stratified analysis of the correlations between education years and MRI-based biomarkers of AD-related pathology (GMV, ReHo, and FC values of ROI) according to the disease severity (CDR scores). In the mild dementia group, all biomarkers we used were inversely associated with years of education (P < 0.01). This inverse relationship was attenuated and not significant in patients with moderate or severe dementia, or in patients with aMCI (see Fig. 4).
Table 3.
AD-related pathology biomarkers | Relationships with years of education | ||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HC | aMCI | CDR1 (mild dementia) |
CDR2 (moderate dementia) |
CDR3 (severe dementia) |
|||||||||||
β (SE) |
p | B | β (SE) |
p | B | β (SE) |
p | B | β (SE) |
p | B | β (SE) |
p | B | |
GMV of dACC |
−0.001 (0.002) |
0.7 | −0.062 |
− 0.003 (0.002) |
0.038 * |
− 0.258 |
− 0.007 (0.002) |
0.006** | − 0.753 |
− 0.006 (0.005) |
0.233 | − 0.569 |
−0.005 (0.004) |
0.239 | −0.568 |
ReHo of left aITC |
0.008 (0.005) |
0.108 | 0.261 |
−0.003 (0.003) |
0.372 | −0.127 |
−0.018 (0.004) |
0.000*** | −0.739 |
− 0.038 (0.012) |
0.006** | −1.485 |
− 0.007 (0.005) |
0.247 | − 0.62 |
ReHo of left pITC |
0.003 (0.006) |
0.649 | 0.076 |
−0.008 (0.004) |
0.037 * |
−0.284 |
−0.025 (0.006) |
0.002** | −0.831 |
− 0.003 (0.014) |
0.829 | − 0.101 |
−0.011 (0.01) |
0.305 | −0.586 |
left aITC rsFC | |||||||||||||||
right angular gyrus |
0.002 (0.01) |
0.814 | 0.038 |
−0.004 (0.009) |
0.662 | −0.061 |
−0.034 (0.01) |
0.004** | −0.72 |
− 0.041 (0.026) |
0.127 | − 0.899 |
−0.036 (0.012) |
0.017 * |
−1.043 |
left mid frontal cortex |
0.013 (0.008) |
0.098 | 0.271 |
0.002 (0.008) |
0.763 | 0.039 |
−0.031 (0.008) |
0.002** | −0.717 |
−0.035 (0.019) |
0.081 | −0.873 |
0.003 (0.011) |
0.78 | 0.103 |
right superior frontal cortex |
0.002 (0.009) |
0.835 | 0.032 |
−0.002 (0.007) |
0.786 | −0.038 |
−0.029 (0.009) |
0.007** | −0.658 |
− 0.05 (0.021) |
0.029* | −1.244 |
0.013 (0.012) |
0.317 | 0.372 |
Structural MR (GMV): Adjusted for age, sex, MMSE, and TIV
Resting-state fMRI (ReHo and rsFC): Adjusted for age, sex, MMSE, and head motion parameters
Abbreviations: AD Alzheimer’s disease; aMCI amnestic mild cognitive impairment; HC healthy control; CDR Clinical Dementia Rating; SE standard error; GMV gray matter volume; ReHo Regional Homogeneity; rsFC resting-state functional connectivity; dACC dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; aITC anterior part of inferior temporal cortex; pITC posterior part of inferior temporal cortex; MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination; TIV total intracranial volume
β stand for unstandardized coefficients, B stand for standardized coefficients (Beta)
P< 0.05*, P< 0.01**, P< 0.001***
Between-group differences of the values extracted from ROI
There were significant differences in GMV of dACC among groups, with significantly lower volumes in different AD groups (mild, moderate and severe group) compared to HC (p < 0.05), and lower volumes in severe AD than aMCI (p < 0.05). For both L-aITC and L-pITC, ReHo values were lower in severe AD compared to HC and aMCI groups (p < 0.05). For the FC between the left anterior ITC and left mid frontal cortex, right superior frontal cortex and right angular gyrus, the rs-FC values were lower in severe AD compared to HC and aMCI groups(p < 0.05) (see Fig. 5).
Correlations between the values extracted from ROI and cognitive function
Pearson correlation analyses were used to examine the relationship between ROI-based neuroimaging variables and clinical cognitive function scores. In HC group, GMV of dACC was significantly negatively correlated with semantic memory(r = − 0.407, p = 0.006), but not in the episodic and working memory (see Table S2.). In AD group, GMV of dACC was significantly positively correlated with semantic memory (r = 0.453, p = 0.001) and episodic memory (r = 0.334, p = 0.015). ReHo values of the left pITC were significantly positively correlated with episodic memory (r = 0.391, p = 0.004), and the rs-FC values of the left aITC connected with the right angular gyrus were also significantly positively correlated with episodic memory (r = 0.312, p = 0.023). The rs-FC values of the left aITC connect with the left middle frontal cortex were significantly positively correlated with episodic memory (r = 0.338, p = 0.013) and semantic memory (r = 0.277, p = 0.045) (see Table 4.). There were no significant correlations between ROI-based neuroimaging variables and clinical cognitive function scores in aMCI group (see Table S3.).
Table 4.
Cognitive system | Episodic memory | Semantic memory | Working memory | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | r | p | r | p | |
aGMV of dACC | 0.334 | 0.015* | 0.453 | 0.001** | 0.205 | 0.141 |
bReHo of left aITC | 0.172 | 0.217 | 0.049 | 0.728 | 0.021 | 0.884 |
bReHo of left pITC | 0.391 | 0.004** | 0.2 | 0.151 | 0.102 | 0.466 |
Left aITC rsFCb | ||||||
right angular gyrus | 0.312 | 0.023* | 0.082 | 0.562 | 0.089 | 0.525 |
Left middle frontal cortex | 0.338 | 0.013* | 0.277 | 0.045* | 0.084 | 0.552 |
right superior frontal cortex | 0.108 | 0.44 | −0.035 | 0.805 | −0.128 | 0.36 |
The relationship between neuroimaging variables and clinical cognitive function scores were assessed by using Pearson correlation analysis
aAdjusted for age, sex, education, and TIV
bAdjusted for age, sex, education, and head motion parameters
P< 0.05*, P< 0.01**, P< 0.001***
Abbreviations: AD Alzheimer’s disease; GMV gray matter volume; ReHo Regional Homogeneity; rsFC resting-state functional connectivity; dACC dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; aITC anterior part of inferior temporal cortex; pITC posterior part of inferior temporal cortex; TIV total intracranial volume; ROI region of interest
Discussion
In this study, we found that highly educated AD patients showed greater gray matter volume atrophy of the dACC, lower ReHo values of left ITC, and decreased FC of the left aITC with the left mid frontal cortex, right superior frontal cortex and right angular gyrus. Meanwhile, we found GMV of dACC shrinked, ReHo values of left ITC decreased, and FC of the left aITC with the left mid frontal cortex, right superior frontal cortex and right angular gyrus decreased with the progression of AD. This suggested brain shrinkage, reduction of ReHo and FC values mean more severe brain damage. As a result, it is reasonable to assume that with the same cognitive function level, individuals with higher cognitive reserve had more severe pathology damage in brain structure and function. These results may seem counterintuitive; at a comparable clinical symptom severity degree, AD neuropathology may be more severe in high educated persons. One probable explanation is that this kind of education background may provide more skills to compensate for the clinical symptoms of the disorder. A series of studies confirmed that biomarkers were more abnormal in higher CR individuals than lower CR individuals at the same degree of cognitive function [7, 11, 14, 37–44]. Whereas, underlying neural mechanism is still unclear. We speculate that intellectual activity may keep neuronal health and enhance self-repair process by stimulating neuronal activity.
Our first finding was that more highly educated AD patients can tolerate more gray matter volume atrophy of the dACC than less educated AD individuals while they have the same degree of cognitive function. This result gives solid evidence for the perspective that higher educated individuals have a relatively higher CR and indicates that ACC may engage in cognitive reserve processes. This gray matter finding is consistent with the previous study which reported that compared to monolinguals, bilinguals have increased gray matter in an extensive cluster along the ACC, starting from its dorsal region and extending towards its ventral region [45]. As one of the brain areas underlying executive control, ACC is more highly stimulated in bilingual speakers and this may result in greater cognitive reserve that compensates for the brain atrophy found in normal aging [46, 47]. Some evidences also showed that bilingualism delays the onset of dementia [48, 49]. Guzmán-Vélez et al. suggested that education and bilingualism may use a similar mechanism to compensate for brain damage, perhaps because a second language is often learned through formal education [50]. That is to say, subjects with a high educational background are more likely to have bilingualism ability. As a result, it is plausible to suppose that early and lifelong education induces beneficial neural changes upon brain structures mediating cognitive control, specifically the ACC. Functionally, the ACC is indirectly related to episodic memory through its influence on related functions such as cognitive control, conflict resolution, motivation, and perseverance [51]. Some researchers raised that cognitive control may be a part of reserve [52]. Previous studies have suggested that ACC is responsible for integrating various input information and regulating the processing in other brain regions [53–55]. We speculate that education induces neural plasticity and provides greater reserve to tolerate greater gray matter volume atrophy [56]. This may also explain why there were no significant correlations observed in HCs and aMCI patients; as education likely improve brain plasticity by developing neural connections rather than increasing gray matter volume in normal cognitive and preclinical AD individuals.
As an important rs-fMRI metric, ReHo has been proposed to be able to effectively quantify synchronization among BOLD time series of a given voxel and its nearest neighboring voxels [57], and could provide valuable spatiotemporal information from a neurobiological perspective [58]. Increasing evidence suggests that ReHo can be used as an imaging biomarker to monitor and/or identify AD pathology and ReHo has also been shown to significantly correlate with cognitive performance [59, 60]. Some experts find that decreases and increases in ReHo values can coexist across multiple brain regions in the patients with the same cognitive diagnosis [61]. This might suggest that it might be too early to conclude an overall effect regarding changes in ReHo value, as dynamics of cerebral blood flow could be highly affected by a wide spectrum of degenerative states and possible interregional reciprocal changes. Our analyses suggested that higher educated AD individuals have relatively lower ReHo values of left ITC. The decrease in ReHo of left ITC in the sever AD group indicated an ongoing profound disconnection process. It is known that in the AD brain, the density of neurofibrillary tangles in the inferior temporal cortex is high and is significantly correlated with dementia severity or neuronal loss [62]. In AD, starting from the inferior temporal gyrus, tau pathology spreads gradually throughout the cortex, resulting in cognitive impairment [63]. The neurofibrillary tangles and tau deposition may lead to decreases in the amplitude and/or phase discordance of BOLD signals within individual neuron clusters. Another study indicated that highly educated prodromal AD patients showed more severe hypometabolism and relative hypermetabolism than individuals with poor education in the left inferior gyri and right superior frontal gyri, respectively [7]. Hypometabolismin the left inferior gyri might be related to low ReHo values. A recent study showed that in the left inferior temporal gyrus, higher levels of education were related to thinner cortical thickness (greater cortical atrophy) [9]. Therefore, we speculate that the left inferior temporal gyrus may play an important role in CR in AD patients.
Besides, we found that higher education was related to the reduced connectivity of the left aITC with the left mid frontal cortex, right superior frontal cortex, and the right angular gyrus in AD. The ongoing profound disconnection process in the left aITC may affect its functional connectivity with the rest of the brain. The left mid frontal cortex is an area known to allocate and coordinate cognitive resources in the working memory [64–67], and is thought to be involved in Chinese reading and reading acquisition [68]. The right angular gyrus is also an area known to be susceptible to AD pathology. The AD-related pathological susceptibility and cognitive related characteristics of these significantly correlated regions in our study indicated that these regions may be components of neural reserve. In summary, our present findings suggested that AD patients with high education can withstand the AD pathology process than those with low levels of education due to neural reserve.
The second finding of this study was that higher educated mild AD patients were better able to withstand greater gray matter volume atrophy, and decreased ReHo values and FC compared with lower educated mild AD patients at the same level of the cognitive performance. Interestingly, the differences in brain structure and function related to education seem to weaken in the more severe AD brains [69]. The result is in line with previous studies that demonstrate the notion that CR mainly occurs at the initial stage of the disease [15, 70, 71]. With the progression of clinical symptoms, the protective impact of education on the brain exits but diminishes. We assume that there exists a fixed critical threshold, at which brain reserve can withstand the brain damage. Once exceeding the threshold, brain reserve capacity will be depleted, leading to specific clinical symptoms. In AD brain, synapses gradually reduce, and the point where synapses reduced to the specific number might be the critical threshold. We therefore suggest that brains with high synaptic density can cope better with AD pathology process before reaching the threshold. There is an abundance of evidence for the threshold effect that higher educated AD individuals can mask dementia manifestations initially, but once going through the threshold, cognition will decline more rapidly [15, 18, 72–75].
Our findings suggest that the protective impact of education mainly occurs in the earlier stages of the AD and a reasonable time-window for therapeutic interventions should therefore be chosen. In mild AD and aMCI, improvement of CR may help resist AD dementia, but when the threshold is reached, CR cannot withstand AD-related brain pathology anymore and intervention will be inefficient. In addition, the aim of most clinical trials is to evaluate the progression of cognitive impairment between drug or intervention group and untreated group. Taking the discrepancy of different level of CR-related cognitive decline into account, clinicians should select different treatment strategies separately for high and low CR in order to improve the curative effect.
In healthy control group, no significant correlations between brain structure and function and education levels were found. One possible reason might be the subject selection, i.e., some subjects with apolipoprotein E4 (APOE4) and others without APOE4. The APOE4 is one of the risk factors for AD [76]. Higher levels of education and higher levels of midlife cognitive activities forestall amyloid deposition by a yet unkown mechanism, which is evident in APOE4 carriers, as APOE4 carriers have more amyloid at the earlier stage and accumulate faster compared with non-APOE4 carriers [77, 78]. Thus, we should consider the effect of APOE4 on cognitively normal subjects. Moreover, ceiling effects [3] and the relatively small sample size may have influenced the result. The MMSE scores in HC group are affected by ceiling effects as many healthy control individuals have the maximum scores of 30. However, cognitive level of these individuals are not equal because their true cognitive state may be significantly higher than 30 [73].
At last, we examined the relation between ROI-based neuroimaging variables and clinical cognitive function scores. In HC group, greater gray matter volume of dACC was associated with poorer semantic memory. This indirectly demonstrated our speculation that education may increase brain reserve by inducing neural plasticity in cognitively healthy elderly individuals. It was an active process. However in AD group, gray matter volume atrophy of dACC was associated with poor semantic memory and episodic memory. We also found that lower ReHo and rs-FC values were associated with poorer semantic memory and episodic memory. This indicates that the later stages of AD dementia is a passive process, the protective role of education on cognition remains but diminishes and patients with higher education can tolerate more severe brain structural and functional damage.
Our study has some limitations. Firstly, the design of this study is a cross sectional study; additional longitudinal studies are necessary to better understand the relationship between brain structure and function characteristics and education. Secondly, our patients were recruited at specialized memory clinics, a multi-center research is necessary to validate the results across different stages of AD. Thirdly, MRI variables we used in this study cannot capture the whole aspect of brain pathology. MRI variables should include different structural measures, such as regional volumes, cortical thickness, measures of white matter integrity, as well as measures of functional networks and molecular markers of pathology. However, it is also true that neuroimaging variables we used here may capture a portion of CR. Fourth, we tested education as a proxy of cognitive reserve but did not evaluate the combined effect of occupation, premorbid IQ, leisure activities, socioeconomic status (SES) or early life linguistic ability. It is difficult to isolate the contribution of formal education to cognitive reserve from that of other variables. People with lower educational levels probably had lower IQ and came from a lower socioeconomic background which is associated with worse nutrition and lack of access to adequate health care, which can also influence brain health. Indeed, it is difficult to disentangle the role of each component of cognitive reserve on shaping the relationship between brain structure and function. Although each aspect might play an independent and additive role, we believe that years of education is a reasonable proxy that reflect the overall consequences of a brain that was not adequately stimulated during childhood. However, it is necessary to measure and account for occupation, SES or other contributors of cognitive reserve instead of only education to offer a more accurate estimate of cognitive reserve for the future research. Finally, subjects with available APOE genotype data were small, so we did not consider the influence of APOE4 on cognitive decline. However, unless APOE genotype is associated with education, it would not have any impact on our findings.
Our study also has obviously strengths. Firstly, without ionizing radiation and invasion, and less expensive than PET, MRI can provide more information about brain properties, so we chose MRI-based biomarkers to explore the correlation between education and AD neuroimaging. Secondly, the integration of brain function and structure holds great promise in improving our understanding of cognition. Indeed, our analysis highlights the importance of considering AD severity in modeling cognitive decline. Thirdly, MRI-based biomarkers are useful to assess the effect of cognitive interventions. Two patients with the same clinical symptoms could differ in MRI-based biomarkers. Understanding the disassociation between cognition and pathology is crucial for successfully preventing AD-related neuropathology and improving our understanding of AD.
Conclusions
Our results indicate that education is protective against cognitive deterioration, providing support for the CR theory, which predicts that at the same level of clinical symptom severity, higher educated patients can compensate for AD neuropathological manifestations more effectively. Exploring the neural basis of CR might improve our understanding of CR-associated brain structural and functional changes and contribute to targeted intervention. Transcranial direct current or magnetic stimulating the CR-related brain targets might increase CR and slow down the progression of AD.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the many collaborators for their work in setting up the study and implementing the protocol. We would like to thank our patients for their role in the research design.
Abbreviations
- CR
Cognitive reserve
- AD
Alzheimer’s disease
- aMCI
Amnestic mild cognitive impairment
- HC
Cognitively healthy elderly
- GMV
Gray matter volume
- ReHo
Regional homogeneity
- FC
Functional connectivity
- dACC
Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex
- ITC
Inferior temporal cortex
- MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
- FDG-PET
18F-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography
- CSF
Cerebrospinal fluid
- fMRI
Functional MRI
- CDR
Clinical Dementia Rating
- AVLT
Auditory verbal learning test
- VFT
Verbal fluency test
- rs-fMRI
Resting-state functional MRI
- MNI
Montreal Neurological Institute
- FD
Frame-wise displacement
- ROI
Regions of interest
- TIV
Total intracranial volume
- FDR
False discovery rate
- aITC
Anterior inferior temporal cortex
Authors’ contributions
YY and WK conceptualized and designed the study. ZW was responsible for conducting the analyses, preparing the first draft of the manuscript, and preparing the manuscript for submission. LXS, LXH, WH, LM, GZ, WX, TY, ZS were responsible for data collection and initial data preprocessing. All authors contributed to and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
The work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant numbers81771817, 81901726), the National Key R&D Program of China (grant number: 2016YFC1305904) and the Province Natural Science Foundation Project of Anhui (grant number: 1608085MH169).
The funding bodies had no role in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable requests.
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This study is approved by the Medical Research Ethics Committee of the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, China, according to the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was provided by all subjects.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Kai Wang, Email: wangkai1964@126.com.
Yongqiang Yu, Email: cjr.yuyongqiang@vip.163.com.
References
- 1.Stern Y, Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Bartres-Faz D, Belleville S, Cantilon M, Chetelat G, et al. Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer's & dementia : the journal of the Alzheimer's Association. 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 2.Perneczky R, Kempermann G, Korczyn AD, Matthews FE, Ikram MA, Scarmeas N, Chetelat G, Stern Y, Ewers M. Translational research on reserve against neurodegenerative disease: consensus report of the international conference on cognitive Reserve in the Dementias and the Alzheimer's Association reserve, resilience and protective factors professional interest area working groups. BMC Med. 2019;17(1):47. doi: 10.1186/s12916-019-1283-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J International Neuropsychological Society : JINS. 2002;8(3):448–460. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702813248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Perneczky R, Drzezga A, Diehl-Schmid J, Schmid G, Wohlschlager A, Kars S, Grimmer T, Wagenpfeil S, Monsch A, Kurz A. Schooling mediates brain reserve in Alzheimer's disease: findings of fluoro-deoxy-glucose-positron emission tomography. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006;77(9):1060–1063. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.094714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cohen AD, Price JC, Weissfeld LA, James J, Rosario BL, Bi W, Nebes RD, Saxton JA, Snitz BE, Aizenstein HA, et al. Basal cerebral metabolism may modulate the cognitive effects of Abeta in mild cognitive impairment: an example of brain reserve. J Neurosci. 2009;29(47):14770–14778. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3669-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bastin C, Yakushev I, Bahri MA, Fellgiebel A, Eustache F, Landeau B, Scheurich A, Feyers D, Collette F, Chetelat G, et al. Cognitive reserve impacts on inter-individual variability in resting-state cerebral metabolism in normal aging. NeuroImage. 2012;63(2):713–722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Morbelli S, Perneczky R, Drzezga A, Frisoni GB, Caroli A, van Berckel BN, Ossenkoppele R, Guedj E, Didic M, Brugnolo A, et al. Metabolic networks underlying cognitive reserve in prodromal Alzheimer disease: a European Alzheimer disease consortium project. J Nuclear Med. 2013;54(6):894–902. doi: 10.2967/jnumed.112.113928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ewers M, Brendel M, Rizk-Jackson A, Rominger A, Bartenstein P, Schuff N, Weiner MW. Reduced FDG-PET brain metabolism and executive function predict clinical progression in elderly healthy subjects. NeuroImage Clinical. 2014;4:45–52. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lee DH, Lee P, Seo SW, Roh JH, Oh M, Oh JS, Oh SJ, Kim JS, Jeong Y. Neural substrates of cognitive reserve in Alzheimer's disease spectrum and normal aging. NeuroImage. 2019;186:690–702. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Boots EA, Schultz SA, Almeida RP, Oh JM, Koscik RL, Dowling MN, Gallagher CL, Carlsson CM, Rowley HA, Bendlin BB, et al. Occupational complexity and cognitive Reserve in a Middle-Aged Cohort at risk for Alzheimer's disease. Arch Clinical Neuropsychol. 2015;30(7):634–642. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acv041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Querbes O, Aubry F, Pariente J, Lotterie JA, Demonet JF, Duret V, Puel M, Berry I, Fort JC, Celsis P. Early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease using cortical thickness: impact of cognitive reserve. Brain. 2009;132(Pt 8):2036–2047. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Brickman AM, Siedlecki KL, Muraskin J, Manly JJ, Luchsinger JA, Yeung LK, Brown TR, DeCarli C, Stern Y. White matter hyperintensities and cognition: testing the reserve hypothesis. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32(9):1588–1598. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.10.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Piras F, Cherubini A, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G. Education mediates microstructural changes in bilateral hippocampus. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(2):282–289. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rentz DM, Locascio JJ, Becker JA, Moran EK, Eng E, Buckner RL, Sperling RA, Johnson KA. Cognition, reserve, and amyloid deposition in normal aging. Ann Neurol. 2010;67(3):353–364. doi: 10.1002/ana.21904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dumurgier J, Paquet C, Benisty S, Kiffel C, Lidy C, Mouton-Liger F, Chabriat H, Laplanche JL, Hugon J. Inverse association between CSF Abeta 42 levels and years of education in mild form of Alzheimer's disease: the cognitive reserve theory. Neurobiol Dis. 2010;40(2):456–459. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2010.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Vemuri P, Lesnick TG, Przybelski SA, Knopman DS, Preboske GM, Kantarci K, Raman MR, Machulda MM, Mielke MM, Lowe VJ, et al. Vascular and amyloid pathologies are independent predictors of cognitive decline in normal elderly. Brain. 2015;138(Pt 3):761–771. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Landeau B, La Joie R, Mevel K, Mezenge F, Perrotin A, Desgranges B, Bartres-Faz D, Eustache F, Chetelat G. Relationships between years of education and gray matter volume, metabolism and functional connectivity in healthy elders. NeuroImage. 2013;83:450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurology. 2012;11(11):1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Adam S, Bonsang E, Grotz C, Perelman S. Occupational activity and cognitive reserve: implications in terms of prevention of cognitive aging and Alzheimer's disease. Clin Interv Aging. 2013;8:377–390. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S39921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Jack CR, Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Shaw LM, Aisen PS, Weiner MW, Petersen RC, Trojanowski JQ. Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer's pathological cascade. Lancet Neurology. 2010;9(1):119–128. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1984;34(7):939–944. doi: 10.1212/WNL.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR, Jr, Kawas CH, Klunk WE, Koroshetz WJ, Manly JJ, Mayeux R, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2011;7(3):263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman HH, Fox NC, Gamst A, Holtzman DM, Jagust WJ, Petersen RC, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2011;7(3):270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Petersen RC, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Donohue MC, Gamst AC, Harvey DJ, Jack CR, Jr, Jagust WJ, Shaw LM, Toga AW, et al. Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative (ADNI): clinical characterization. Neurology. 2010;74(3):201–209. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181cb3e25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56(3):303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wu X, Geng Z, Zhou S, Bai T, Wei L, Ji GJ, Zhu W, Yu Y, Tian Y, Wang K. Brain structural correlates of odor identification in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease revealed by magnetic resonance imaging and a Chinese olfactory identification test. Front Neurosci. 2019;13:842. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Morris JC. The clinical dementia rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43(11):2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/WNL.43.11.2412-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zhao Q, Lv Y, Zhou Y, Hong Z, Guo Q. Short-term delayed recall of auditory verbal learning test is equivalent to long-term delayed recall for identifying amnestic mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e51157. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Pasquier F, Lebert F, Grymonprez L, Petit H. Verbal fluency in dementia of frontal lobe type and dementia of Alzheimer type. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1995;58(1):81–84. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.58.1.81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kalbe E, Kessler J, Calabrese P, Smith R, Passmore AP, Brand M, Bullock R. DemTect: a new, sensitive cognitive screening test to support the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and early dementia. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry. 2004;19(2):136–143. doi: 10.1002/gps.1042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Fazekas F, Chawluk JB, Alavi A, Hurtig HI, Zimmerman RA. MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer's dementia and normal aging. AJR Am J Roentgenol. 1987;149(2):351–356. doi: 10.2214/ajr.149.2.351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Helenius J, Mayasi Y, Henninger N. White matter hyperintensity lesion burden is associated with the infarct volume and 90-day outcome in small subcortical infarcts. Acta Neurol Scand. 2017;135(5):585–592. doi: 10.1111/ane.12670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Zhu J, Zhuo C, Qin W, Xu Y, Xu L, Liu X, Yu C. Altered resting-state cerebral blood flow and its connectivity in schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 2015;63:28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Zhu J, Zhang Y, Zhang B, Yang Y, Wang Y, Zhang C, Zhao W, Zhu DM, Yu Y. Abnormal coupling among spontaneous brain activity metrics and cognitive deficits in major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2019;252:74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.04.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Zang Y, Jiang T, Lu Y, He Y, Tian L. Regional homogeneity approach to fMRI data analysis. NeuroImage. 2004;22(1):394–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stern Y, Alexander GE, Prohovnik I, Mayeux R. Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol. 1992;32(3):371–375. doi: 10.1002/ana.410320311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kemppainen NM, Aalto S, Karrasch M, Nagren K, Savisto N, Oikonen V, Viitanen M, Parkkola R, Rinne JO. Cognitive reserve hypothesis: Pittsburgh compound B and fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography in relation to education in mild Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol. 2008;63(1):112–118. doi: 10.1002/ana.21212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Serra L, Cercignani M, Petrosini L, Basile B, Perri R, Fadda L, Spano B, Marra C, Giubilei F, Carlesimo GA, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive reserve in Alzheimer disease. Rejuvenation Res. 2011;14(2):143–151. doi: 10.1089/rej.2010.1103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ewers M, Insel PS, Stern Y, Weiner MW. Cognitive reserve associated with FDG-PET in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2013;80(13):1194–1201. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31828970c2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Perneczky R, Diehl-Schmid J, Pohl C, Drzezga A, Kurz A. Non-fluent progressive aphasia: cerebral metabolic patterns and brain reserve. Brain Res. 2007;1133(1):178–185. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Borroni B, Premi E, Bozzali M, Padovani A. Reserve mechanisms in neurodegenerative diseases: from bench to bedside and back again. Curr Med Chem. 2012;19(36):6112–6118. doi: 10.2174/0929867311209066112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hoenig MC, Bischof GN, Hammes J, Faber J, Fliessbach K, van Eimeren T, Drzezga A. Tau pathology and cognitive reserve in Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;57:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Liu Y, Julkunen V, Paajanen T, Westman E, Wahlund LO, Aitken A, Sobow T, Mecocci P, Tsolaki M, Vellas B, et al. Education increases reserve against Alzheimer's disease--evidence from structural MRI analysis. Neuroradiology. 2012;54(9):929–938. doi: 10.1007/s00234-012-1005-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Abutalebi J, Guidi L, Borsa V, Canini M, Della Rosa PA, Parris BA, Weekes BS. Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia. 2015;69:201–210. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Borsa VM, Perani D, Della Rosa PA, Videsott G, Guidi L, Weekes BS, Franceschini R, Abutalebi J. Bilingualism and healthy aging: aging effects and neural maintenance. Neuropsychologia. 2018;111:51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Abutalebi J, Della Rosa PA, Green DW, Hernandez M, Scifo P, Keim R, Cappa SF, Costa A. Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(9):2076–2086. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gollan TH, Salmon DP, Montoya RI, Galasko DR. Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(14):3826–3830. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Craik FI, Bialystok E, Freedman M. Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology. 2010;75(19):1726–1729. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fc2a1c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Guzman-Velez E, Tranel D. Does bilingualism contribute to cognitive reserve? Cognitive and neural perspectives. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(1):139–150. doi: 10.1037/neu0000105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Carter CS, van Veen V. Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: an update of theory and data. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007;7(4):367–379. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.4.367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Stern Y, Zarahn E, Habeck C, Holtzer R, Rakitin BC, Kumar A, Flynn J, Steffener J, Brown T. A common neural network for cognitive reserve in verbal and object working memory in young but not old. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(4):959–967. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4(6):215–222. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Shackman AJ, Salomons TV, Slagter HA, Fox AS, Winter JJ, Davidson RJ. The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12(3):154–167. doi: 10.1038/nrn2994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Metzler-Baddeley C, Jones DK, Steventon J, Westacott L, Aggleton JP, O'Sullivan MJ. Cingulum microstructure predicts cognitive control in older age and mild cognitive impairment. J Neurosci. 2012;32(49):17612–17619. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3299-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Kim JP, Seo SW, Shin HY, Ye BS, Yang JJ, Kim C, Kang M, Jeon S, Kim HJ, Cho H, et al. Effects of education on aging-related cortical thinning among cognitively normal individuals. Neurology. 2015;85(9):806–812. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Tu MC, Hsu YH, Yang JJ, Huang WH, Deng JF, Lin SY, Lin CY, Kuo LW. Attention and Functional Connectivity Among Patients With Early-Stage Subcortical Ischemic Vascular Disease and Alzheimer's Disease. Frontiers in aging neuroscience. 2020;12:239. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Jiang L, Zuo XN. Regional homogeneity: a multimodal, multiscale neuroimaging marker of the human Connectome. The Neuroscientist. 2016;22(5):486–505. doi: 10.1177/1073858415595004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Zhang Z, Liu Y, Jiang T, Zhou B, An N, Dai H, Wang P, Niu Y, Wang L, Zhang X. Altered spontaneous activity in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment revealed by regional homogeneity. NeuroImage. 2012;59(2):1429–1440. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Liu Y, Yu C, Zhang X, Liu J, Duan Y, Alexander-Bloch AF, Liu B, Jiang T, Bullmore E. Impaired long distance functional connectivity and weighted network architecture in Alzheimer's disease. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(6):1422–1435. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Cai S, Wang Y, Kang Y, Wang H, Kim H, von Deneen KM, Huang M, Jiang Y, Huang L. Differentiated regional homogeneity in progressive mild cognitive impairment: a study with post hoc label. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Dement. 2018;33(6):373–384. doi: 10.1177/1533317518778513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Okamura N, Furumoto S, Fodero-Tavoletti MT, Mulligan RS, Harada R, Yates P, Pejoska S, Kudo Y, Masters CL, Yanai K, et al. Non-invasive assessment of Alzheimer's disease neurofibrillary pathology using 18F-THK5105 PET. Brain. 2014;137(Pt 6):1762–1771. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Franzmeier N, Rubinski A, Neitzel J, Kim Y, Damm A, Na DL, Kim HJ, Lyoo CH, Cho H, Finsterwalder S, et al. Functional connectivity associated with tau levels in ageing, Alzheimer's, and small vessel disease. Brain. 2019;142(4):1093–1107. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Chen HC, Vaid J, Bortfeld H, Boas DA. Optical imaging of phonological processing in two distinct orthographies. Exp Brain Res. 2008;184(3):427–433. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1200-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Liu Y, Dunlap S, Fiez J, Perfetti C. Evidence for neural accommodation to a writing system following learning. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28(11):1223–1234. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Petrides M, Alivisatos B, Meyer E, Evans AC. Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90(3):878–882. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.3.878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Deco G, Rolls ET. Horwitz B: "what" and "where" in visual working memory: a computational neurodynamical perspective for integrating FMRI and single-neuron data. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(4):683–701. doi: 10.1162/089892904323057380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Siok WT, Niu Z, Jin Z, Perfetti CA, Tan LH. A structural-functional basis for dyslexia in the cortex of Chinese readers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(14):5561–5566. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801750105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Koepsell TD, Kurland BF, Harel O, Johnson EA, Zhou XH, Kukull WA. Education, cognitive function, and severity of neuropathology in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2008;70(19 Pt 2):1732–1739. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000284603.85621.aa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Serra L, Musicco M, Cercignani M, Torso M, Spano B, Mastropasqua C, Giulietti G, Marra C, Bruno G, Koch G, et al. Cognitive reserve and the risk for Alzheimer's disease: a longitudinal study. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36(2):592–600. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Thow ME, Summers MJ, Saunders NL, Summers JJ, Ritchie K, Vickers JC. Further education improves cognitive reserve and triggers improvement in selective cognitive functions in older adults: the Tasmanian healthy brain project. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2018;10:22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2017.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Barulli D, Stern Y. Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(10):502–509. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Robitaille A, van den Hout A, Machado RJM, Bennett DA, Cukic I, Deary IJ, Hofer SM, Hoogendijk EO, Huisman M, Johansson B, et al. Transitions across cognitive states and death among older adults in relation to education: a multistate survival model using data from six longitudinal studies. Alzheimer's & dementia. 2018;14(4):462–472. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Stern Y, Albert S, Tang MX, Tsai WY. Rate of memory decline in AD is related to education and occupation: cognitive reserve? Neurology. 1999;53(9):1942–1947. doi: 10.1212/WNL.53.9.1942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Stern Y, Tang MX, Denaro J, Mayeux R. Increased risk of mortality in Alzheimer's disease patients with more advanced educational and occupational attainment. Ann Neurol. 1995;37(5):590–595. doi: 10.1002/ana.410370508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Belloy ME, Napolioni V, Greicius MD. A quarter century of APOE and Alzheimer's disease: Progress to date and the path forward. Neuron. 2019;101(5):820–838. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Jack CR, Jr, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Knopman DS, Vemuri P, Mielke MM, Lowe V, Senjem ML, Gunter JL, Machulda MM, et al. Age, sex, and APOE epsilon4 effects on memory, brain structure, and beta-amyloid across the adult life span. JAMA neurology. 2015;72(5):511–519. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.4821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Jansen WJ, Ossenkoppele R, Knol DL, Tijms BM, Scheltens P, Verhey FR, Visser PJ, Aalten P, Aarsland D, Alcolea D, et al. Prevalence of cerebral amyloid pathology in persons without dementia: a meta-analysis. Jama. 2015;313(19):1924–1938. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.4668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable requests.