Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2021 Nov 1;11:21416. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-00850-1

No aftereffects of high current density 10 Hz and 20 Hz tACS on sensorimotor alpha and beta oscillations

Louis-Philippe Lafleur 1, Audrey Murray 2,3,4, Manon Desforges 5,6, Kevin Pacheco-Barrios 7, Felipe Fregni 7, Sara Tremblay 5,6, Dave Saint-Amour 2,3, Jean-François Lepage 8, Hugo Théoret 1,3,
PMCID: PMC8560917  PMID: 34725379

Abstract

Application of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) is thought to modulate ongoing brain oscillations in a frequency-dependent manner. However, recent studies report various and sometimes inconsistent results regarding its capacity to induce changes in cortical activity beyond the stimulation period. Here, thirty healthy volunteers participated in a randomized, cross-over, sham-controlled, double-blind study using EEG to measure the offline effects of tACS on alpha and beta power. Sham and high current density tACS (1 mA; 10 Hz and 20 Hz; 0.32 mA/cm2) were applied for 20 min over bilateral sensorimotor areas and EEG was recorded at rest before and after stimulation for 20 min. Bilateral tACS was not associated with significant changes in local alpha and beta power frequencies at stimulation sites (C3 and C4 electrodes). Overall, the present results fail to provide evidence that bilateral tACS with high current density applied over sensorimotor regions at 10 and 20 Hz reliably modulates offline brain oscillation power at the stimulation site. These results may have implications for the design and implementation of future protocols aiming to induce sustained changes in brain activity, including in clinical populations.

Subject terms: Excitability, Motor cortex

Introduction

The endogenous oscillations of the brain are associated with specific cognitive functions and are believed to play an important role in regimenting communication between cortical and subcortical areas1,2. Considering their putative association with various cognitive states and pathophysiological disorders3,4, there is high interest in modulating these oscillations non-invasively. Among non-invasive brain stimulation tools, transcranial alternative current stimulation (tACS) appears particularly well-suited for this task, as it consists in the application of a weak (< 2 mA) sinusoidal electric current passing through the scalp that entrains cortical oscillations at a specific frequency in the nearby cortex5,6.

Several studies have shown that tACS can modulate cognition7, perception8, corticospinal excitability9 and cortical oscillations6 during the stimulation period (on-line effects). For example, studies using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) have shown frequency-specific tACS effects on corticospinal excitability911. Similarly, administration of tACS in the alpha band (8–12 Hz) to the occipital and parietal cortex has been shown to entrain oscillations in this frequency range6,12. Furthermore, the immediate effects of tACS are not restricted to the vicinity of the stimulated sites, as changes in resting-state fMRI connectivity have been reported during alpha13,14, beta15 and gamma16 tACS.

While the neurophysiological consequences of online tACS may explain its documented influence on behavior1722, one outstanding question is whether tACS can induce changes that outlast the stimulation period (off-line effects). Although the precise mechanism of its action is not completely understood, it is believed that tACS-induced oscillation entrainment and neuronal synchronization may lead to lasting neuroplastic changes5,12,23,24. The most consistent finding for offline tACS has been that stimulation applied at alpha frequencies over occipital areas can increase alpha power12,2427 for up to 70 min after the end of stimulation28. Recent studies, however, reported no aftereffect in the alpha band following alpha tACS over occipital29,30, parietal31, somatosensory32 and frontal32 cortex. Beta tACS has also been shown to increase beta power following stimulation of visual27 and parietal cortex33, although Berger and collaborators34 found no effect of 20 Hz stimulation on beta power.

Alpha and beta oscillations are known to be predominant in sensorimotor cortex, peaking at approximately 10 Hz and 20 Hz, respectively35,36. Studies using TMS to assess tACS-induced changes in corticospinal excitability have provided conflicting results. Whereas some studies have reported offline effects of alpha or beta tACS on corticospinal excitability11,37,38, most studies have shown no aftereffects19,3943 or mixed results21,44,45.

The aftereffects of alpha and beta tACS in sensorimotor cortex have also been investigated using EEG and MEG, and here again results have been inconsistent. Wach and collaborators46 and Sugata et al.47 found no evidence for offline modulation of MEG alpha and beta oscillations following 10 Hz and 20 Hz tACS with almost identical stimulation parameters. In both of these studies, tACS was administered for ten minutes in a C3-supraorbital montage with an intensity of 1mA46,47. Similarly, a recent EEG study reported no aftereffects in the alpha and beta frequencies following 10 min of 10 Hz and 20 Hz tACS applied for 10 min in a C3-supraorbital montage at 1mA48. The induction of significant tACS aftereffects in sensorimotor cortex, however, has been reported in a recent study that used a different stimulation approach. Wischnewski and collaborators49 applied 20 Hz tACS using high-definition stimulation50, where a small round electrode (3.14 cm2) positioned over C3 was surrounded by four return electrodes at T7, F3, Cz and P3. Stimulation at 20 Hz was applied for 15 min at an intensity of 2 mA. Resting-state oscillations in the beta frequency were significantly increased for up to 60 min after the end of stimulation, and the effects were restricted to the stimulated frequency and electrode49.

Taking into consideration the fact that tACS modulation of resting-state EEG in sensorimotor cortex has been reported for high49 but not low4648 current densities and that TMS evidence shows that tACS-induced changes corticospinal excitability are also dependent on current intensity and density11,51, the present study was designed to probe the effects of high current density tACS on sensorimotor alpha and beta oscillations. To this end, 10 Hz, 20 Hz, and sham stimulation were applied with a current density of 0.32 mA/cm2 in 30 healthy participants in a randomized, cross-over, sham-controlled, double-blind protocol. Stimulating electrodes (3.14 cm2) were positioned over C3 and C4 in a bilateral protocol that has been previously shown to induce aftereffects in alpha and beta frequencies34,52,53. Resting-state EEG was acquired with the same electrodes used for stimulation for 5 min prior to tACS and for 20 min after the end of stimulation.

Results

Participant blinding

Participants were asked if they believed they had received active or sham stimulation after each session. For all testing sessions, 72% of participants correctly identified sham stimulation, while 83% and 93% of participants correctly identified as « active » the 10 Hz and 20 Hz stimulation conditions, respectively. Cochran’s Q test did not reveal significant differences between conditions (χ2 (2) = 4.91; p = 0.086). To remove the bias of previous responses given by each participant through the sessions, an additional analysis was performed on the first visit data only. Nine participants out of ten correctly identified 10 Hz stimulation as active, 10 participants of ten correctly identified 20 Hz stimulation as active, and 7 participants of ten correctly identified sham stimulation as inactive at their first visit. Fisher’s exact test revealed no significant difference between groups for the proportion of correct answers (p = 0.29).

Alpha and beta power

For α-power at electrode site C3 (Fig. 1a), there was no main effect of Condition (F1.69, 49.14 = 1.70; p = 0.20; η2partial = 0.06), no main effect of Time (F2.15, 62.29 = 2.35; p = 0.10; η2partial = 0.08) and no Condition X Time interaction (F4.39, 127.18 = 0.83; p = 0.52; η2partial = 0.03).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Effects of tACS on alpha and beta oscillations. Log-transformed alpha power at electrode (a) C3 and (b) C4 following stimulation. Log-transformed beta power at electrode (c) C3 and (d) C4 following stimulation. Error bars represent SEM.

For α-power at electrode site C4 (Fig. 1b), there was no main effect of Condition (F1.81, 52.52 = 0.40; p = 0.66; η2partial = 0.01), no main effect of Time (F1.89, 54.87 = 1.86; p = 0.17; η2partial = 0.06) and no Condition X Time interaction (F3.53, 102.25 = 1.97; p = 0.11; η2partial = 0.06).

For β-power at electrode site C3 (Fig. 1c), there was no main effect of Condition (F1.34, 38.80 = 1.87; p = 0.18; η2partial = 0.06), no main effect of Time (F2.20, 63.81 = 0.84; p = 0.45; η2partial = 0.03) of and no Condition X Time interaction (F2.89, 83.77 = 0.95; p = 0.42; η2partial = 0.03).

For β-power at electrode site C4 (Fig. 1d), there was no main effect of Condition (F1.43, 41.36 = 1.03; p = 0.34; η2partial = 0.03), no main effect of Time (F1.66, 48.02 = 0.29; p = 0.71; η2partial = 0.01) and no Condition X Time interaction. (F4.28, 124.24 = 2.39; p = 0.05; η2partial = 0.08).

Pearson’s correlations were computed to determine if baseline alpha or beta activity was associated with the strength of the tACS effect in the alpha and beta frequencies. There was no statistically significant correlation between baseline alpha or beta power and tACS effects on post stimulation alpha ratios (T1/T0, T2/T0, T3/T0, T4/T0) at electrodes C3 and C4 (all r between -0.08 and -0.33 and all p > 0.07 for alpha; all r between -0.09 and -0.30 and all p > 0.10 for beta). Individual data normalized to pre-tACS values are shown in Fig. 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Individual response to tACS normalized to baseline values. Individual alpha and beta log-transformed power following 10 Hz, 20 Hz, or sham stimulation at electrode C3. Note that one participant with high power values is not presented in the figure for clarity but was included in the statistical analysis.

Bayesian repeated measures ANOVAs were also conducted to quantify the plausibility of both the null and the alternative hypotheses, permitting interpretation of null findings. In Bayesian inference, the likelihood of the data is considered under both hypotheses, and these probabilities are compared via the Bayes factor (BF). According to Lee and Wagenmakers’ classification54 (see also Stefan et al.55), the level of evidence is deemed inconclusive/anecdotal for BF between 0.33 and 3, moderate for BF < 0.33 or > 3, and strong for BF < 0.01 or > 10. Following the JASP guidelines, BF comparing the null model against all other models were computed and each experimental effect was obtained by calculating the inclusion BF across matched models. Results revealed relatively strong evidence supporting the null hypothesis (H0) over H1 for the Condition X Time interaction under both α and β frequencies and both electrodes C3 and C4 (all BF values were lower than 0.01 (from 0.003 to 0.006)). For more complete Bayesian analysis, see Table S1-S8, and Table S9 for a summary of the classification.

Electrical field simulation of the tACS protocol using the Finite-Element Method (FEM) showed focal and strong fields occurring bilaterally in the sensorimotor areas (precentral and postcentral gyri) with a peak electrical field strength of 0.268 V/m. As intended, the electrical fields were in phase between both hemispheres and confirmed the focal distribution over central areas (Fig. 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Electrical field model. (a) Normal component of the electrical field (electrical field strength in V/m) and its neuroanatomical distribution. (b) Representation of the electrical field vectors for the same model results.

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to determine whether tACS applied at fixed frequencies of 10 Hz and 20 Hz over sensorimotor areas can induce oscillatory aftereffects in the stimulated areas. To this end, tACS was administered bilaterally with high current densities. Compared to sham stimulation, active tACS did not modulate alpha or beta oscillations. Bayesian analyses further support the absence of effects on alpha and beta power post-tACS. Given the magnitude of the Bayes factor, it appears reasonable to assume that the present results not only represent ‘absence of evidence’, but also ‘evidence of absence’ of aftereffects.

A simple explanation for the present results is that tACS does not induce robust, reliable aftereffects in oscillatory power. This is supported by studies suggesting that tACS applied at common stimulation intensities does not induce enough intracranial current to entrain neuronal populations5658. Furthermore, it has been suggested that stimulation of peripheral nerves in the skin, rather than direct neuronal stimulation, explains neuronal entrainment59 (see Kasten et al.60 for an opposite view). Even if enough current does reach cortical neurons, the absence of significant aftereffects could be explained by the high levels of inter-individual variability associated with transcranial electrical stimulation (tES6165). Kasten and collaborators60 recently reported that three individual factors accounted for up to 87% of the variance in the strength of tACS induced aftereffects: strength of the intracranial electric field, location of the electric field with respect to the stimulation target, and match between stimulation and peak endogenous frequencies. These data suggest that individual factors such as brain anatomy have a considerable impact on the size of tACS-induced neuronal modulation60 and may thus significantly influence group-level effects. Individual data in the present study clearly show that significant inter-individual variability occurred in the response to both 10 Hz and 20 Hz tACS.

An alternative explanation for the present negative findings is that specific methodological factors prevented optimal stimulation effects. For example, the stimulation frequency was not matched to individual endogenous peak frequencies. For stimulation of occipital cortex, most studies have used individual alpha frequency (IAF), which resulted in predominantly significant aftereffects24,2628,66 whereas fixed intensity stimulation has been less successful29,32. In motor cortex, fixed intensity stimulation has provided inconsistent results with some studies reporting aftereffects47,49 while others have not46,48. It is therefore possible that if tACS had been administered at individual alpha and beta frequencies in the present study, aftereffects would have been observed. However, it should be noted that using IAF does not necessarily lead to a perfect match between stimulation frequency and peak oscillatory activity. Indeed, Stecher et al.26 reported an average mismatch between the IAF stimulation frequency and post-stimulation alpha peak of 0.8 Hz that reached 2.5 Hz in some participants. Vossen et al.24 also reported that stimulation frequency and IAF can show discrepancies in the order of -1.5 Hz to 3.0 Hz. Interestingly, it was found that the strength of aftereffects is negatively correlated with the match between stimulation frequency and IAF, suggesting that a closer match between endogenous alpha oscillation peak frequency and stimulation frequency leads to weaker aftereffects24. More recently, Kasten and collaborators60 provided contradictory evidence, showing that the mismatch between IAF and stimulation frequency is a contributing factor to tACS aftereffects (less mismatch leads to greater power increases). Taken together, the available data do not provide a definitive answer as to whether stimulating at individual peak frequencies increases stimulation effects. Studies directly comparing the efficacy of fixed- and individual-intensity protocols are needed to determine whether tACS is more effective when the stimulation frequency is matched with baseline peak frequencies.

Another factor that has been shown to modulate the effects of tES is electrode size and placement, as well as stimulation intensity. Most previous studies assessing the effects of alpha or beta tACS on cortical oscillations have used at least one large stimulation electrode (35 cm2) with relatively low stimulation intensity (≈ 1 mA)12,24,25,27,30,4648. For both EEG49,51 and TMS11,51 measures of tACS aftereffects, however, higher stimulation intensities (and current densities) have been associated with stronger effects. In the present study, tACS was applied at an intensity of 1 mA but with much smaller electrodes than what is usually used. Thus, despite using a current density that was significantly higher than what was used in most previous studies, aftereffects were not found following stimulation. One possible explanation for this is that the effect of tES intensity is non-linear. For example, increasing tDCS stimulation duration from 13 to 26 minutes67 or current intensity from 1 to 2 mA68 has been shown to reverse the direction of aftereffects. More recently, De Koninck and collaborators69 reported that 1 mA IAF tACS induced stronger aftereffects in alpha power than stimulation at 4–6 mA. Thus, it is possible that increasing current density had the paradoxical effect of reducing the ability of tACS to produce aftereffects in sensorimotor cortex. It should be noted, however, that similar off-line protocols to the one used in the present study (bilateral, 1 mA, 3.14 cm2 electrode size) have been shown to modulate brain oscillations. For example, Berger and collaborators34 reported that 20 min of bilateral alpha-tACS (P3-P4) increased alpha oscillations while Hsu et al.53 reported that bilateral theta-tACS (F3-F4) increased beta oscillations. Nevertheless, FEM data show that most of the current in the present study was distributed over the sensorimotor areas. Furthermore, despite higher current densities being delivered owing to smaller electrode surface, electrical field strengths were within the range of previously reported models, albeit near the upper limit30,44,70. This suggests that the stimulation protocol used in the present study was efficient in targeting sensorimotor areas with relatively high current strength.

Another feature of tES protocols that may have an effect on physiological response is the state of the brain at the time of stimulation. It has been repeatedly shown that stimulation protocols are state-dependent71. For example, the effects of tACS on alpha oscillations have been shown to disappear when participants have their eyes closed during the experiment, which could be explained by increased endogenous alpha activity25. Here again, there is variability in the studies that have assessed the aftereffects of alpha and beta tACS with regards to state-dependency. In some studies, participants were required to perform a task that was relevant to the experimental question during stimulation29,3134. In other studies, a simple task was performed throughout the entire experiment (before, during, and after stimulation) to maintain vigilance levels12,2426,28,30. In the present study, participants were asked to watch a documentary film during the entire experiment, as previously described66. It is possible that passive film viewing was not engaging enough for participants, which led to fatigue and increases in alpha power. Alpha power at baseline, however, was not correlated with the strength of the aftereffects in the alpha frequency, suggesting that pre-stimulation alpha levels were not related to the efficacy of tACS in inducing aftereffects.

In conclusion, the present data do not support the idea that administration of higher current density by using smaller electrodes leads to robust aftereffects in oscillatory activity following tACS. The important interindividual variability and selection of specific stimulation parameters may account for the negative findings. As a result, individually tailored stimulation protocols60 may increase the reliability and efficacy of tACS protocols and help determine what specific factors contribute to overall response.

Methods

Participants

Thirty healthy right-handed (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory: 90.28 ± 10.67) volunteers (20 females, 18–37 years, mean age = 23.50 ± 3.86) were recruited via public ads to take part in the experiment. Exclusion criteria were history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, history of head injury resulting in loss of consciousness, cardiac pacemaker, presence of intracranial metal implant, tinnitus, history of seizures, history of fainting, substance abuse, and other contraindications to TMS and tACS72. This study conformed to the standards set by the Declaration of Helsinki and all the procedures were approved by the Comité d’éthique de la recherche en arts et sciences (CÉRAS) of the Université de Montréal. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Procedure

In a fully within-subject, counterbalanced and double-blind design, participants took part in three experimental sessions, each separated by at least 72 h: (1) 10 Hz tACS bilateral stimulation; (2) 20 Hz tACS bilateral stimulation; (3) sham stimulation. During experimental sessions, participants were conformably seated in a chair located in an electrically shielded cabin. Electrodes were mounted on a Starstim (Neuroelectrics, Barcelona, Spain) headcap placed on the head of the participant. Once the headcap was in place, electrode gel (SignaGel, Parker Laboratories, Fairfield, USA) was applied on the contact surface between the electrode and the scalp to decrease impedance and improve conductivity. Each session lasted approximately 45 min and consisted of three blocks: (1) Baseline EEG recording for 5 min; (2) 10 Hz tACS, 20 Hz tACS or sham stimulation for 20 min; (3) EEG recording for 20 min (Fig. 4a). During the experimental sessions, participants watched an episode of the British Broadcasting Corporation’s “Planet Earth” on a computer screen located 60 cm from the participant. This was used to maintain attention without producing overt emotional response.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Experimental design. (a) Five minutes of resting-state EEG were acquired before tACS. Immediately following the end of stimulation, resting-state EEG was recorded for an additional 20 min (b) Stimulating and recording electrodes were positioned over C3 and C4.

tACS and EEG recordings

tACS was delivered for 20 min with a Starstim 8 tCS-EEG system (Neuroelectrics, Barcelona, Spain) at an intensity of 1 mA peak-to-peak. Hybrid tCS and EEG Pistim Ag–AgCl pellet electrodes with a 12 mm diameter and 3.14 cm2 circular contact area were used for stimulation with a current density of 0.32 mA/cm2. Stimulation was administered with two electrodes positioned on C3 and C4 (Fig. 4b). Stimulation intensity was progressively ramped up for the first 30 s of stimulation and ramped down for the last 30 s of stimulation. For sham stimulation, the current was turned off after the ramp up period for the remainder of the stimulation session. Electrode impedance was verified before each session and stimulation started only when it was below 10kΩ. The system was set up to stop stimulation if impedance exceeded 15 kΩ, which did not occur in the present experiment.

EEG was recorded using the same system and electrodes at a sampling rate of 500 Hz and analyzed offline. Signal was obtained from eight electrodes (F3, F4, Fz, C3, C4, Cz, P3, P4) mounted on a neoprene headcap in accordance with the international 10–20 EEG system. Online electrical reference earclips consisted of two opposed Ag/AgCl pellets of 8 mm diameter on the right ear.

EEG data analysis

The offline analysis of the EEG data was performed using BrainVision Analyzer 2.2 (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). Continuous raw data were low-pass filtered at 50 Hz (fourth-order Butterworth, zero-phase shift) and high-pass filtered at 0.5 Hz. A 60 Hz notch filter was applied to attenuate electrical interference. Data were re-referenced to electrode Fz and downsampled at 256 Hz. A 60 Hz notch filter was applied to attenuate electrical interference. EEG segments were separated into five-minute periods. The first segment corresponded to baseline (T0; pre-stimulation), and the four others to post-stimulation recordings (T1; 0–5 min, T2; 5–10 min, T3; 10–15 min, T4; 15–20 min).

All segments were then split into 1 s epochs, and segments contaminated by eye blinks or muscle movements were excluded using a semiautomatic artifact detection algorithm (min–max 100 µV criterion). A minimum number of 100 EEG clean segments per time interval for each stimulation condition was set as a criterion for a participant to be included in the analysis. No participant was excluded on this basis, with most participants having more than 250 clean segments in each time interval. For power analysis, Fast Fourier Transforms (FFT) were computed on individual epochs with 1 Hz frequency resolution using a Hanning window function (10%). Epochs were averaged for each time interval and condition and the mean power activity (µV2) was extracted for alpha (α) and beta (β) frequencies, corresponding to the average power between 8–12 Hz, and 13–30 Hz, respectively. Power data were log-transformed using a natural logarithm to meet the normality assumption required for analysis of variance since raw data and their residuals for the ANOVA model were not normally distributed. To avoid negative values, a value of 1 was added to all raw data before being log-transformed.

Electrical field simulation

The SimNIBS 2 software pipeline was used for electrical field simulation73, based on a finite-element method (FEM) that allows for precise calculations of electric fields in complex geometrical shapes such as a human head. tACS electrical distribution was simulated under a quasi-static regime assumption74,75 since at relatively low frequencies of tDCS (< 1 kHz, in the present study 10 and 20 Hz) the electrical fields can be separated into spatial and temporal components. In the case of a sinusoidal current with a specific frequency and amplitude, the electric field will vary in time and with the same frequency and phase of the input current74,75. As a result, the electrical field obtained at peak currents can be simulated, with the temporal variations of the current scaling the field with no changes of its distribution in the brain.

The spatial component was calculated by solving Laplace's equation for the electrostatic potential φ

·(σφ)=0

using Dirichlet boundary conditions at the electrodes (σ representing the ohmic conductivity, and ∇ the divergence and gradient differential vector operators, respectively). The FEM solver76 used the Galerkin method based on tetrahedral first order elements, and the residuals for the conjugate gradient solver were required to be < 10−9.

The electric field vector (E) was then determined by the numerical differentiation of φ.

E=-φ

The current density J was determined via Ohm's law (J = delta*E). The electrostatic potential and the field values were scaled such that a current unit (i.e., 1) was passing through the electrodes.

Statistical analysis

Repeated measures analysis of variance (rmANOVA), with factors Condition (10 Hz, 20 Hz, sham) and Time (T0, T1, T2, T3, T4) was used separately to test for changes in α and β EEG power. This analysis was conducted for the two electrodes of interest (C3 and C4) at each of the two frequency bands. In the case of significant interaction effects, post-hoc analyses were performed with Bonferroni corrections to control for multiple comparisons, and non-sphericity was adjusted using Greenhouse-Weisser correction when required. To determine whether baseline alpha or beta activity is associated with the effects of tACS, Pearson’s correlation coefficients were computed with the log-transformed baseline power values and T1/T0, T2/T0, T3/T0 and T4/T0 power ratios for electrodes C3 and C4. No participant data were removed from analysis.

Additional Bayesian statistical analyses were tested with the JASP package (version 0.14.177) to quantify the plausibility of alternative H1 versus the null H0 hypotheses. Bayesian repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted using JASP default priors, and effects are reported as the Bayes factor for the inclusion (BFincl) of a particular effect, calculated as the ratio between the likelihood of the data given the model compared to the model without that specific effect (see Keysers et al.78 for detailed description of BFincl).

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Tables. (35.4KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (RGPIN-2016-05079). LPL was supported by a scholarship from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada.

Author contributions

Conception and design: L.P.L., D.S.A., J.F.L., H.T.; Data acquisition and analysis: L.P.L., A.M., M.D., K.P.C., F.F.; Data Interpretation: L.P.L., A.M., F.F., D.S.A., J.F.L, H.T.; Drafting of the manuscript: L.P.L., A.M., K.P.C; All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-00850-1.

References

  • 1.Thut G, Miniussi C. New insights into rhythmic brain activity from TMS-EEG studies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009;13:182–189. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Siegel M, Donner TH, Engel AK. Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;13:121–134. doi: 10.1038/nrn3137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Buzsaki G, Draguhn A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science. 2004;304:1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neural synchrony in brain disorders: Relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron. 2006;52:155–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Herrmann CS, Rach S, Neuling T, Struber D. Transcranial alternating current stimulation: A review of the underlying mechanisms and modulation of cognitive processes. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:279. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Helfrich RF, et al. Entrainment of brain oscillations by transcranial alternating current stimulation. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Vosskuhl J, Huster RJ, Herrmann CS. Increase in short-term memory capacity induced by down-regulating individual theta frequency via transcranial alternating current stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015;9:257. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Helfrich RF, et al. Selective modulation of interhemispheric functional connectivity by HD-tACS shapes perception. PLoS Biol. 2014;12:e1002031. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Feurra M, et al. Frequency-dependent tuning of the human motor system induced by transcranial oscillatory potentials. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:12165–12170. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0978-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Feurra M, et al. State-dependent effects of transcranial oscillatory currents on the motor system: What you think matters. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:17483–17489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1414-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wischnewski M, Schutter DJLG, Nitsche MA. Effects of beta-tACS on corticospinal excitability: A meta-analysis. Brain Stimul. 2019;12:1381–1389. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.07.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Zaehle T, Rach S, Herrmann CS. Transcranial alternating current stimulation enhances individual alpha activity in human EEG. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e13766. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bächinger M, et al. Concurrent tACS-fMRI reveals causal influence of power synchronized neural activity on resting state fMRI connectivity. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:4766–4777. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1756-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gundlach C, et al. Reduction of somatosensory functional connectivity by transcranial alternating current stimulation at endogenous mu-frequency. Neuroimage. 2020;221:117175. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Weinrich CA, et al. Modulation of long-range connectivity patterns via frequency-specific stimulation of human cortex. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:3061–3068. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Moisa M, Polania R, Grueschow M, Ruff CC. Brain network mechanisms underlying motor enhancement by transcranial entrainment of gamma oscillations. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:12053–12065. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2044-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Pogosyan A, Gaynor LD, Eusebio A, Brown P. Boosting cortical activity at Beta-band frequencies slows movement in humans. Curr. Biol. 2009;19:1637–1641. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Joundi RA, Jenkinson N, Brittain JS, Aziz TZ, Brown P. Driving oscillatory activity in the human cortex enhances motor performance. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:403–407. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wach C, et al. Effects of 10 Hz and 20 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on motor functions and motor cortical excitability. Behav. Brain. Res. 2013;241:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.11.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Pollok B, Boysen AC, Krause V. The effect of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) at alpha and beta frequency on motor learning. Behav. Brain Res. 2015;293:234–240. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cappon D, D’Ostilio K, Garraux G, Rothwell J, Bisiacchi P. Effects of 10 Hz and 20 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation on automatic motor control. Brain Stimul. 2016;9:518–524. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Heise K-F, Monteiro TS, Leunissen I, Mantini D, Swinnen SP. Distinct online and offline effects of alpha and beta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on continuous bimanual performance and task-set switching. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:3144. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-39900-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Veniero D, Vossen A, Gross J, Thut G. Lasting EEG/MEG aftereffects of rhythmic transcranial brain stimulation: Level of control over oscillatory network activity. Front. Cell Neurosci. 2015;9:477. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Vossen A, Gross J, Thut G. Alpha power increase after transcranial alternating current stimulation at alpha frequency (alpha-tACS) reflects plastic changes rather than entrainment. Brain Stimul. 2015;8:499–508. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Neuling T, Rach S, Herrmann CS. Orchestrating neuronal networks: Sustained after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation depend upon brain states. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:161. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Stecher HI, Pollok TM, Strüber D, Sobotka F, Herrmann CST. minutes of α-tACS and ambient illumination independently modulate EEG α-power. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017;11:257. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Nakazono H, et al. Transcranial alternating current stimulation of α but not β frequency sharpens multiple visual functions. Brain Stimul. 2020;13:343–352. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.10.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kasten FH, Dowsett J, Herrmann CS. Sustained aftereffect of α-tACSl up to 70 min after stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10:245. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Clayton MS, Yeung N, Cohen Kadosh R. The effects of 10 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation on audiovisual task switching. Front. Neurosci. 2018;12:67. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Stecher HI, Herrmann CS. Absence of alpha-tACS aftereffects in darkness reveals importance of taking derivations of stimulation frequency and individual alpha variability into account. Front. Psychol. 2018;9:984. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ronconi L, Melcher D, Junghöfer M, Wolters CH, Busch NA. Testing the effect of tACS over parietal cortex in modulating endogenous alpha rhythm and temporal integration windows in visual perception. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2020 doi: 10.1111/ejn.15017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.May ES, et al. Modulating brain rhythms of pain using transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) - a sham-controlled study in healthy human participants. J. Pain. 2021;9:S1526–5900. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2021.03.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Battaglini L, Ghiani A, Casco C, Ronconi L. Parietal tACS at beta frequency improves vision in a crowding regime. Neuroimage. 2020;208:116451. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Berger A, Pixa NH, Steinberg F, Doppelmayer M. Brain oscillatory and hemodynamic activity in a bimanual coordination task following transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS): A combined EEG-fNIRS study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018;12:67. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Salmelin R, Hari R. Characterization of spontaneous MEG rhythms in healthy adults. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1994;91:237–248. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)90187-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ritter P, Moosmann M, Villringer A. Rolandic alpha and beta EEG rhythms' strengths are inversely related to fMRI-BOLD signal in primary somatosensory and motor cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:1168–11687. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Zaghi S, et al. Inhibition of motor cortex excitability with 15Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) Neurosci. Lett. 2010;479:211–214. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gallasch E, Rafolt D, Postruznik M, Fresnoza S, Christova M. Decrease of motor cortex excitability following exposure to a 20 Hz magnetic field as generated by a rotating permanent magnet. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2018;129:1397–1402. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2018.03.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Antal A, et al. Comparatively weak after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on cortical excitability in humans. Brain Stimul. 2008;1:97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2007.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Rjosk V, et al. Transcranial alternating current stimulation at beta frequency: Lack of immediate effects on excitation and interhemispheric inhibition of the human motor cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10:560. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Nowak M, et al. Driving human motor cortical oscillations leads to behaviorally relevant changes in local GABAA inhibition: A tACS-TMS study. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:4481–4492. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0098-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Schilberg L, et al. Phase of beta-frequency tACS over primary motor cortex modulates corticospinal excitability. Cortex. 2018;103:142–152. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Pozdniakov I, Vorobiova AN, Galli G, Rossi S, Feurra M. Online and offline effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation of the primary motor cortex. Sci. Rep. 2021;11:3854. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-83449-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Heise KF, et al. Evaluation of a modified high-definition electrode montage for transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) of pre-central areas. Brain Stimul. 2016;9:700–704. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lafleur LP, et al. Neurophysiological aftereffects of 10Hz and 20Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation over bilateral sensorimotor cortex. Brain Res. 2020;1727:146542. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Wach C, et al. The effect of 10 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on corticomuscular coherence. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:511. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Sugata H, et al. Modulation of motor learning capacity by transcranial alternating current stimulation. Neuroscience. 2018;391:131–139. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Harada T, et al. Off-line effects of alpha-frequency transcranial alternating current stimulation on a visuomotor learning task. Brain Behav. 2020;10:e01754. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wischnewski M, et al. NMDA receptor-mediated motor cortex plasticity after 20 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation. Cereb. Cortex. 2019;29:2924–2931. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Datta A, et al. Gyri-precise head model of transcranial direct current stimulation: Improved spatial focality using a ring electrode versus conventional rectangular pad. Brain Stimul. 2009;2:201–207. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2009.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Cancelli A, et al. Cortical inhibition and excitation by bilateral transcranial alternating current stimulation. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2015;33:105–114. doi: 10.3233/RNN-140411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.D'Atri A, et al. Bilateral 5 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation on fronto-temporal areas modulates resting-state EEG. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:15672. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16003-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hsu WY, Zanto TP, van Schouwenburg MR, Gazzaley A. Enhancement of multitasking performance and neural oscillations by transcranial alternating current stimulation. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0178579. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0178579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Lee MD, Wagenmakers EJ. Bayesian Cognitive Modeling: A Practical Course. Cambridge University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Stefan AM, et al. A tutorial on Bayes Factor Design Analysis using an informed prior. Behav. Res. 2019;51:1042–1058. doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-01189-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Lafon B, et al. Low frequency transcranial electrical stimulation does not entrain sleep rhythms measured by human intracranial recordings. Nat. Commun. 2017;8:1199. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01045-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Vöröslakos M, et al. Direct effects of transcranial electric stimulation on brain circuits in rats and humans. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:483. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-02928-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Huang Y, et al. Measurements and models of electric fields in the in vivo human brain during transcranial electric stimulation. Elife. 2017;6:e18834. doi: 10.7554/eLife.18834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Asamoah B, Khatoun A, Mc Laughlin M. tACS motor system effects can be caused by transcutaneous stimulation of peripheral nerves. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:266. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-08183-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Kasten FH, Duecker K, Maack MC, Meiser A, Herrmann CS. Integrating electric field modeling and neuroimaging to explain inter-individual variability of tACS effects. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:5427. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13417-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Krause B, Cohen Kadosh R. Not all brains are created equal: The relevance of individual differences in responsiveness to transcranial electrical stimulation. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2014;8:25. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Wiethoff S, Hamada M, Rothwell JC. Variability in response to transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor cortex. Brain Stimul. 2014;7:468–475. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Chew T, Ho KA, Loo CK. Inter- and intra-individual variability in response to transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) at varying current intensities. Brain Stimul. 2015;8:1130–1137. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Horvath JC, Forte JD, Carter O. Evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) generates little-to-no reliable neurophysiologic effect beyond MEP amplitude modulation in healthy human subjects: A systematic review. Neuropsychologia. 2015;66:213–236. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Lopez-Alonso V, Cheeran B, Rio-Rodriguez D, Fernandez-Del-Olmo M. Inter-individual variability in response to non-invasive brain stimulation paradigms. Brain Stimul. 2014;7:372–380. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Gundlach C, Müller MM, Nierhaus T, Villringer A, Sehm B. Modulation of somatosensory alpha rhythm by transcranial alternating current stimulation at mu-frequency. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017;11:432. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Monte-Silva K, et al. Induction of late LTP-like plasticity in the human motor cortex by repeated non-invasive brain stimulation. Brain Stimul. 2013;6:424–432. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Batsikadze G, Moliadze V, Paulus W, Kuo MF, Nitsche MA. Partially non-linear stimulation intensity-dependent effects of direct current stimulation on motor cortex excitability in humans. J. Physiol. 2013;591:1987–2000. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.249730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.De Koninck BP, Guay S, Blais H, De Beaumont L. Parametric study of transcranial alternating current stimulation for brain alpha power modulation. Brain Commun. 2021;3:fcab010. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcab010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Antonenko D, Grittner U, Puonti O, Flöel A, Thielscher A. Estimation of individually induced e-field strength during transcranial electric stimulation using the head circumference. Brain Stimul. 2021;14:1055–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2021.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Silvanto J, Muggleton N, Walsh V. State-dependency in brain stimulation studies of perception and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008;12:447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A, Safety of TMS Consensus Group Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2009;120:2008–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Thielscher A, Antunes A, Saturnino GB. Field modeling for transcranial magnetic stimulation: A useful tool to understand the physiological effects of TMS? Annu. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2015;215:222–225. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2015.7318340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Miranda PC, Mekonnen A, Salvador R, Basser PJ. Predicting the electric field distribution in the brain for the treatment of glioblastoma. Phys. Med. Biol. 2014;59:4137–4147. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/59/15/4137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Opitz A, et al. Spatiotemporal structure of intracranial electric fields induced by transcranial electric stimulation in humans and nonhuman primates. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:31236. doi: 10.1038/srep31236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Geuzaine C. GetDP: A general finite-element solver for the de Rham complex. Proc. Appl. Math. Mech. 2007;7:1010603–1010604. doi: 10.1002/pamm.200700750. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.JASP Team. JASP (Version 0.14.1) [Computer software]. https://jasp-stats.org (2020).
  • 78.Keysers C, Gazzola V, Wagenmakers EJ. Using Bayes factor hypothesis testing in neuroscience to establish evidence of absence. Nat. Neurosci. 2020;23:788–799. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0660-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Tables. (35.4KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES