Abstract
Methodological studies investigating transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (lDLPFC) in paediatric populations are limited. Therefore, we investigated in a paediatric population whether stimulation success of multichannel tDCS over the lDLPFC depends on concurrent task performance and individual head anatomy. In a randomised, sham-controlled, double-blind crossover study 22 healthy participants (10–17 years) received 2 mA multichannel anodal tDCS (atDCS) over the lDLPFC with and without a 2-back working memory (WM) task. After stimulation, the 2-back task and a Flanker task were performed. Resting state and task-related EEG were recorded. In 16 participants we calculated the individual electric field (E-field) distribution. Performance and neurophysiological activity in the 2-back task were not affected by atDCS. atDCS reduced reaction times in the Flanker task, independent of whether atDCS had been combined with the 2-back task. Flanker task related beta oscillation increased following stimulation without 2-back task performance. atDCS effects were not correlated with the E-field. We found no effect of multichannel atDCS over the lDLPFC on WM in children/adolescents but a transfer effect on interference control. While this effect on behaviour was independent of concurrent task performance, neurophysiological activity might be more sensitive to cognitive activation during stimulation. However, our results are limited by the small sample size, the lack of an active control group and variations in WM performance.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Cognitive neuroscience
Introduction
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a promising neuromodulatory technique in research and clinical application1 (for review see2). Although the use of tDCS in children and adolescents is increasingly being investigated, important insights into how tDCS affects and interacts with the developing brain are missing. Previous studies in the motor cortex show that findings on tDCS effects obtained in adults cannot simply be assumed to be valid for tDCS application in children and adolescents3,4. Compared to adults, children show different conductivity of the skull tissue, different white and gray matter content and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) volume as well as a smaller brain-scalp distance, all of which influence the electric-field (E-field) distribution5,6. However, due to differences in cortical architecture, receptor distribution and anatomical factors, it is not clear if findings from motor cortex are transferable to other cortical areas7.
The left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is an area often used as target region for electrical stimulation, due to its role for various cognitive and executive functions such as working memory (WM) or decision making8,9. tDCS was shown to modulate neuronal activity in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (lDLPFC) and associated cognitive functions in adults10,11 and in clinical samples in children and adolescents12,13. However, to date, no methodological studies have been conducted on the effects of tDCS over the lDLPFC in normally developed children and adolescents.
While tDCS studies show partially no linear dependency between applied current intensity and stimulation effects14–16, studies modelling the individual current flow inside the brain indicate that higher target field strengths may lead to stronger stimulation effects17,18. Previous studies that stimulated the lDLPFC using tDCS have mostly used classical bipolar montages, which produce a relative diffuse electric field (E-field) and poor targeting19,20. An alternative could be optimised multichannel montages, for which modelling studies predict a comparatively strong but also focal stimulation of the target area21,22. For adults, an increased effectiveness of an optimised multichannel montage has already been shown for motor cortex stimulation23 as well as lDLPFC stimulation24.
Studies in adults suggest that cognitive engagement during stimulation has an impact on the type and strength of tDCS after-effects25,26. Regarding lDLPFC stimulation, both a superiority of online26,27 and of offline stimulation have been proposed28,29. Especially in the context of a potential therapeutic use of tDCS, it is relevant to generate broad, transferring effects. According to the flexible hub theory, the general enhancement of the lDLPFC as an after-effect of tDCS might improve different executive functions, based on the current task related demands30,31. Unfortunately, online and offline stimulation is rarely compared within the same study.
An important tool to illustrate stimulation effects is provided by neurophysiological correlates such as event-related potentials (ERPs) and event-related and resting state oscillatory power recorded by EEG. Studies in adults demonstrate effects of anodal tDCS (atDCS) over the lDLPFC on neurophysiological activity. atDCS led to increased task related N2 and P3 amplitudes32,33. Additionally, theta and alpha oscillatory power have been shown to be influenced by tDCS24,34. Also, changes in resting state oscillatory power have been demonstrated following atDCS33,35. However, several studies did not prove an effect of tDCS over the lDLPFC on resting state oscillatory power36–38.
The current study aimed at investigating the previously mentioned factors using multichannel atDCS over the lDLPFC in children and adolescents (10–17 years). We explored whether atDCS effects are influenced by concurrent task performance during stimulation and individual anatomy. As outcomes we used behavioural and neurophysiological variables. We expected atDCS to lead to improved performance and corresponding EEG correlates in a 2-back WM task (target task). Further, we expected stimulation effects to be influenced by concurrent task performance during stimulation. To investigate atDCS transfer effects, we used a Flanker task (non-target task) that investigates interference control39. Following the flexible hub theory, we expected that atDCS also improves Flanker task performance and related neurophysiological activity. Furthermore, we assumed that the effects of atDCS are modulated by individual anatomy, with higher E-field distributions in the target area leading to stronger atDCS effects. Because respective research in typically developing children and adolescents is lacking, we also investigated aspects of tolerability for multichannel atDCS.
Materials and methods
Participants
The study was approved by the local ethics committee of the Medical Faculty, Kiel University, Kiel, Germany and was carried out in accordance with the latest revision of the Declaration of Helsinki (Clinical trial registration number: German Clinical Trials Register DRKS00008207, 31/08/2017). This study is part of the EU research project STIPED (Stimulation in pediatrics; grant agreement No 731827). All participants and their parents gave written informed consent prior to the experiment. In total, 34 participants were recruited. After a screening procedure we included 29 healthy children and adolescents between 10 and 18 years. Due to study dropout and technical problems during data recording, a total of 22 participants were included in the final data analyses (14 females, mean age: 15.18 years, SD: 1.9, Fig. 1a). Except for one case, the reason given for study termination was that study participation was too time-consuming. One 13-year old girl developed an epileptic disease during her study participation and had to be excluded40. Exclusion criteria were an IQ score < 80 (CFT-20-R, Grundintelligenztest Skala 2—Revision)41, abnormalities during pregnancy or birth, past or present chronic medical conditions, epileptic seizure(s) in the past or in the family, substance consumption or regular medication, any body electronic devices or implants and pregnancy. Health and social impairments were further assessed using the CBCL (Child Behavior Checklist42), FBB-ADHS (Fremdbeurteilungsbogen für Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörungen43) and SRS (Social Responsiveness Scale44). All participants’ characteristics can be found in Table 1. We did not control for handedness since previous studies and metanalyses report no effect of handedness on tDCS effects above the lDLPFC10–12,45.
Table 1.
Mean ± standard deviation | Exclusion criteria | |
---|---|---|
Sex | 14 female, 8 male | |
Age | 15.12 years ± 1.9 | 10 < age > 18 |
CFT-20-R | 106.68 ± 8.91 | IQ < 80 |
CBCL Competence | 59.86 ± 7.66 | T < 37 |
CBCL Problems | 48 ± 8.14 | T > 69 |
FBB-ADHS | 0.19 ± 0.18 | KW > 0.5 |
SRS | 45 ± 9.39 | T > 60 |
CFT-20-R Grundintelligenztest Skala 2-Revision, CBCL child behavior checklist, FBB-ADHS Fremdbeurteilungsbogen für Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörungen, SRS social responsiveness scale.
Experimental design
We used a randomised, sham-controlled, double-blind, crossover study design (see Fig. 2a). Participants underwent six experimental sessions: one screening and baseline measurement (T1) followed by four stimulation sessions (T2–T5) and one optional MRI session (T6). In the stimulation sessions each participant received four different stimulation conditions in randomised order: atDCS with or without concurrent 2-back task performance and sham tDCS with or without concurrent 2-back task performance. The minimum period between stimulation sessions for a single participant was 7 days. At the start of each stimulation session, participants filled in a diary, asking for any adverse events since the last session and their current mood and motivation. Next, we recorded a resting state EEG (2 min eyes open, 2 min eyes closed), followed by 20 min stimulation. In case of concurrent task performance during stimulation, the 2-back task started after 2.5 min of tDCS and ended 2.5 min before the end of stimulation. During non-concurrent stimulation, participants were instructed to sit relaxed with opened eyes. After stimulation, we recorded a second resting state EEG (2 min eyes open, 2 min eyes closed). Afterwards participants performed the 2-back task, a Flanker task and a Continuous Performance Task (CPT) during EEG recording. Eventually, participants filled in a questionnaire on safety, tolerability and blinding of stimulation. The present study is limited to the analysis of the stimulation sessions (T2–T5). Of the tasks performed, the 2-back and Flanker task were evaluated, while the CPT was performed for later comparisons with patient groups.
Tasks and stimuli
All tasks were programmed using the software Presentation® (Version 20.0, Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Berkeley, CA, https://www.neurobs.com/). In the 2-back participants had to decide whether a currently presented picture was identical to the picture shown two steps back (Fig. 2c). Pictures were taken from the Stark Lab Mnemonic Similarity Task (MST46). To make the task more demanding, we included lure pictures. Participants had to press the right (non-target trial) or the left (target trial) mouse button in each trial. The task lasted approximately 16 min and contained 366 trials with 30% target trials. Each trial consisted of 500 ms picture presentation followed by fixation cross presentation jittered between 1600 and 2000 ms duration, resulting in a trial duration of 2100–2500 ms. This 2-back task was previously included in our study with healthy adults24 and is based on the verbal WM n-back task47,48. At baseline measurement (T1) and at each stimulation visit (T2–T5) participants performed a short training (23 trials) to ensure the task was understood correctly. The training was repeated until participants achieved an accuracy of at least 15 (65%) correctly answered trials.
In the Flanker task (Fig. 2c), stimuli consisted of five arrows. This task is a modified version of the Eriksen Flanker task39 and was previously used in our tDCS study with ADHD patients49. Participants had to indicate via button press if the middle target arrow pointed to the right or to the left. The outer arrows served as distractors. The task had about 16 min duration and 528 trials in total with 50% of the trials being congruent and incongruent. Stimuli were presented for 60 ms with an inter stimulus interval of 1676 ms and a trial duration of 1736 ms. Again, prior to the task participants performed a short training at each visit.
Accuracy and RT for both tasks were analysed using the computing environment R50 (version 3.6.1). As measurement of accuracy in the 2-back task we computed d′ scores51. RT were analysed for target trial hits. For the Flanker task accuracy was defined as proportion of correct responses for congruent and incongruent trials separately. RT as well were analysed for correct responses for congruent and incongruent trials.
Transcranial direct current stimulation
We applied 2 mA atDCS over the lDLPFC using a Starstim 32 stimulator (Neuroelectrics, Barcelona, Spain). Five 3.14 cm2 circular PiStim electrodes were positioned at AF3 (897 µA), AF7 (284 µA), F3 (819 µA), Fp2 (-1000 µA) and T7 (-1000 µA), filled with EEG electrode gel. Electrodes were positioned using a head cap following the 10–10 system (Fig. 2b). For anodal stimulation current was ramped up for 30 s at the beginning and down during 30 s at the end of stimulation, for sham stimulation current was ramped up and immediately down for 60 s at the beginning and end of the stimulation.
Montage optimisation
The multichannel montage used in this study was derived from an optimisation algorithm applied to a template head model (Colin head model)19. The optimisation was conducted using the Stimweaver algorithm52. This method determines the montage that minimise the least squares difference between a weighted target E-field map and the weighted E-field induced by the montage. In this study, we focused on the component of the E-field normal to the cortical surface (En) of the head model, since this component is thought to induce the strongest polarisations in the pyramidal neurons, aligned along this direction (lambda-E model)52. For the target map we created a maximum excitation region (maximum weight, 10), and a positive target En field of + 0.50 V/m on the left hemisphere based on Brodmann area 46/left DLPFC. The target En-field on the remaining areas was set to 0 V/m with a lower weight (2). The electrode positions were selected from those available in Neuroelectrics PRO cap (64 positions of the 10–10 EEG system). The maximum current in each electrode was constrained to 1.0 mA and the total injected current was limited to a maximum of 2.0 mA. The distribution of En in the cortical surface of the template head model is shown in Fig. 3a.
EEG recording and preprocessing
EEG was recorded throughout the whole stimulation sessions (T2–T5), i.e. resting state EEG prior to stimulation, EEG during stimulation, resting state EEG after stimulation, EEG during task performance after stimulation (2-back and Flanker task). All EEG data was analysed except for EEG recording during stimulation. EEG was recorded with a rate of 500 S/s and a bandwidth of 0–125 Hz (DC coupled) from 32 channels during the stimulation sessions (see Fig. 2b) using the Starstim 32 stimulator, NG PiStim electrodes and NIC software v2.0.10 and v2.0.11 (Neuroelectrics, Barcelona, Spain). The electrode positions corresponded to the international 10–10 standard system, with the reference and ground electrodes located on the right mastoid. Electrode impedances were kept below 10 kΩ.
We preprocessed EEG data using BrainVisionAnalyzer 2 (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). The data was re-referenced to the common average and filtered (120 Hz low-pass, 0.53 Hz high-pass filter, 50 Hz notch filter). We applied a semiautomatic raw data inspection, an independent component analysis and, following segmentation, a semiautomatic artefact rejection to remove artefacts. For the 2-back task a mean number of 191 trials (SD: 35) and for the Flanker task a mean number of 210 trials (SD: 37) was included in the analyses. Further information on trial numbers for both tasks can be found in Tables S2 and S3 in the supplementary.
EEG data was further analysed using the Fieldtrip toolbox53. Due to a limited number of target hits in the 2-back task for several participants we restricted our analysis of EEG correlates to correct rejection trials in the 2-back task. However, trial numbers for 2-back target hit trials and results of corresponding ERP analyses can be found in the supplementary. In the 2-back task, we analysed the N2 (200–270 ms post stimulus) in the FCz electrode as region of interest (ROI) and the P3 (250–500 ms post stimulus) component in the Pz electrode as ROI. In the Flanker task, ERPs and time–frequency representations were analysed for correctly answered incongruent trials. Here, we analysed the N2 component (250–350 ms post stimulus) in the FCz electrode and P3 (350–600 ms post stimulus) component in the Pz electrode. ROIs were based on previous investigations of task related ERPs54–56. The time windows of all component were defined based on visual inspection of the grand average ERP from all participants.
For task related oscillatory activity we performed a time–frequency analysis with a Hanning taper in steps of 2 Hz for the theta (4–6 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz) and beta frequency band (14–30 Hz). For all frequency bands event-related synchronization/desynchronization (ERS/ERD)57 was computed with respect to a pre stimulus baseline (− 250 to 0 ms). In the 2-back task we analysed the post stimulus interval from 0 to 1000 ms, in the Flanker task we analysed the post stimulus interval from 0 to 700 ms.
Resting state EEG was Fourier-transformed with a moving Hanning window in steps of 0.5 Hz for the theta (4–7.5 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz) and beta (12.5–30 Hz) frequency band and averaged in every participant. Next, we computed pre to post stimulation changes in frequency power for all frequency bands.
Calculation of individual E-field distribution
From the included 22 participants 16 individuals underwent structural head scanning on a 3 T Philips Achieva scanner, during which the following sequences were acquired: a T1-weighted scan (1 mm3, TR = 2530 ms, TE = 3.5 ms, TI = 1100 ms, FA = 7°, fast water excitation), a T2-weighted scan (1 mm3, TR = 3200 ms, TE = 300 ms, no fat suppression), and a diffusion MRI (dMRI) scan (2 mm3, TR = 6300 ms, TE = 51 ms, 67 directions, b = 1000). Each participants MRI was segmented using an in-house implementation combining extra-cerebral tissue segmentations from a new segmentation approach, which will be included in a future version of the open-source simulation toolbox SimNIBS58, with brain tissue segmentations and cortical gray matter surface reconstructions from FreeSurfer59. Finite element head models were then generated, including representations of the scalp, skull, CSF, gray matter and white matter. The models also contained representations of Pistim electrodes (1 cm radius, cylindrical Ag/AgCl electrodes) placed in 61 positions of the 10–10 EEG system. The head models of the template head model and 3 participants are shown in Fig. 3e–h. For the electrodes, only the conductive gel underneath the metal connector was represented in the head model. Unless otherwise stated, the scalp, skull, and CSF were modelled as isotropic with conductivities of 0.33 S/m, 0.008 S/m, and 1.79 S/m, respectively, which are appropriate values for the DC-low frequency values used in transcranial current stimulation19. The gray and white matter were modelled as anisotropic (volume normalization)60, with isotropic conductivity values used for diffusion tensor scaling of 0.40–0.15 S/m, for the gray matter—white matter19. The E-field distribution induced by the common optimised montage was calculated for each participant (see Fig. 3b–d). All E-field calculations were performed in COMSOL, using second- order tetrahedral mesh elements to solve Laplace’s equation. For each participant the surface-average value of the normal component of the E-field was calculated for the lDLPFC (Brodmann area 46), as region targeted by the optimisation. Figure 1b shows the relationship between participants age and the normal component of the E-field.
Questionnaire on tolerability and participant blinding of atDCS
For assessment of side effects and blinding effectiveness we used a standardised safety questionnaire61. Participants rated the incidence and intensity (0 = not experienced to 3 = strongly experienced) of the six most common tDCS side effects on a 4-point scale. Additionally, following each stimulation application, the participants gave their opinion as to whether they had received anodal or sham stimulation.
Statistical analyses
Behavioural data
Throughout all analyses, results were regarded as statistically significant with a two-tailed p value < 0.05. Differences in accuracy and RT were analysed using linear mixed effects models (LME). The models of 2-back task performance during stimulation included the fixed factors stimulation (sham, anodal) and age and their interaction as well as visit (atDCS session 1 to 4). The models of 2-back task performance after stimulation included the additional fixed factor atDCS + target task (atDCS with or without concurrent 2-back task performance). For the Flanker task we computed two models with the fixed factors stimulation, trial (congruent or incongruent), atDCS + target task, age and all corresponding interactions, except for the four-way interaction, as well as visit. In all models we included a random intercept. In case of violations of assumptions, we computed robust linear mixed models using the robustlmm package62.
Neurophysiological data
Analyses of neurophysiological data were performed using the Fieldtrip toolbox53. Differences in ERPs of correct rejection 2-back trials and of incongruent Flanker trials were investigated using two-way repeated measurement ANOVAs with the factors stimulation and atDCS + target task based on the respective ROIs and time intervals. Regarding task related ERD/ERS and resting state post–pre frequency changes, we used two-way repeated measurement ANOVAs with the factors stimulation and atDCS + target task with a cluster based approach. This non-parametric approach solves the problem of multiple comparisons by cluster correction and avoids assumptions on normally distributed data. In all ANOVAs we restricted our analysis to the main effect of stimulation and the interaction of stimulation and atDCS + target task. Due to the number of ANOVAs computed for EEG analysis we used a Bonferroni-Holm correction to adjust the alpha level. Following the ANOVAs, in case of a significant main or interaction effect paired t-tests for the specific significant time window and cluster electrodes or ROI were conducted.
Correlation of E-field and stimulation effects
We computed correlations between stimulation effects (difference between the sham and anodal condition) and the individual normal E-field values in the stimulation target area. Due to the high number of correlations a Bonferroni-Holm Alpha correction was performed.
Tolerability and blinding of tDCS
Differences in experience of side effects between sham and anodal stimulation were investigated using Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. Blinding effectiveness was assessed using a Pearson’s chi-squared test.
Results
Behavioural data
Mean accuracy and RT values for both tasks are displayed in Table 2. Additionally, Fig. 4 displays 2-back accuracy during and after stimulation. Results of the LMM of 2-back d′ scores and RT are summarized in Table 3. Neither for 2-back performance during nor after stimulation we found a significant main effect of or interaction with stimulation on d′ scores or RT. There was a significant effect of age for d′ scores during stimulation, showing an increase in d′ scores with increasing age (β = 0.162, t (19.8) = 2.22, p = 0.038). For 2-back RT after stimulation we found a reduction of RT at the third stimulation visit compared to the overall RT mean (β = − 24.386, t (57) = − 2.55, p = 0.013).
Table 2.
Task | Time point | Outcome | Stimulation condition | |
---|---|---|---|---|
sham | anodal | |||
2-back | During | d′ | 2.38 (0.78) | 2.35 (0.71) |
Accuracy | 67.02 (18.01) | 65.75 (22.64) | ||
RT | 580.21 (155.71) | 577.43 (173.37) | ||
After | d′ | 2.23 (0.78) | 2.21 (0.81) | |
Accuracy | 64.18 (22.94) | 64.37 (23.57) | ||
RT | 594.12 (197.29) | 566.71 (189.41) | ||
Flanker | After | Accuracy | 92.83 (11.35) | 94.07 (6.66) |
RT | 521.43 (129.44) | 512.08 (126.82) |
RT reaction time.
Table 3.
Predictors | Accuracy | Reaction time | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | t value | p value | β | t value | p value | |
During stimulation | ||||||
Stimulation (sham vs. anodal) | − 0.016 | − 0.258 | 0.799 | − 2.068 | − 0.299 | 0.769 |
Age | 0.162 | 2.223 | 0.038 | 22.041 | 1.256 | 0.224 |
Visit 1 | − 0.172 | − 1.234 | 0.231 | − 3.995 | − 0.244 | 0.810 |
Visit 2 | 0.025 | 0.202 | 0.842 | − 16.325 | − 1.140 | 0.269 |
Visit 3 | 0.019 | 0.152 | 0.880 | 26.069 | 1.775 | 0.093 |
Stimulation × age | − 0.001 | − 0.023 | 0.982 | − 0.144 | − 0.038 | 0.970 |
After stimulation | ||||||
Stimulation (sham vs. anodal) | − 0.016 | − 0.485 | 0.634 | − 8.478 | − 1.545 | 0.128 |
Task (con vs. non-con) | 0.049 | 1.453 | 0.162 | 11.058 | 2.005 | 0.050 |
Age | 0.033 | 0.720 | 0.479 | − 4.857 | − 0.632 | 0.529 |
Visit 1 | 0.000 | 0.003 | 0.997 | 19.157 | 1.914 | 0.061 |
Visit 2 | 0.088 | 1.492 | 0.151 | 0.693 | 0.073 | 0.942 |
Visit 3 | 0.039 | 0.673 | 0.510 | − 24.386 | − 2.553 | 0.013 |
Stimulation × task | 0.007 | 0.215 | 0.832 | − 4.193 | − 0.760 | 0.450 |
Stimulation × age | − 0.034 | − 1.825 | 0.086 | − 2.307 | − 0.757 | 0.453 |
Concurrent × age | 0.009 | 0.448 | 0.659 | − 0.505 | − 0.166 | 0.869 |
Stimulation × task × age | 0.004 | 0.233 | 0.818 | − 1.580 | − 0.510 | 0.612 |
Significant results are printed in bold.
Analysis of the Flanker task showed no stimulation effect on accuracy but an effect of stimulation on RT (β = − 6.438, t (57) = − 2.47, p = 0.015; see Table 4). Following anodal stimulation, RT were generally reduced compared to sham stimulation (see Fig. 5a). Importantly, there was no trade-off between RT and accuracy, meaning while RT were reduced following anodal compared to sham stimulation, accuracy did not decrease. Furthermore, we found an interference effect reflected in a significant effect of trial for both accuracy (β = − 2.145, t (138) = − 10.42, p < 0.001) and RT (β = 35.701, t (138) = 13.81, p < 0.001): Compared to congruent trials accuracy scores were lower and RT were higher for incongruent trials. Besides, we found a significant interaction of trial and age for RT (β = 3.298, t (138) = 2.36, p = 0.019). While RT decreased with increasing age for both trial types, this decrease was steeper for congruent than for incongruent trials.
Table 4.
Predictors | Accuracy | Reaction time | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | t value | p value | β | t value | p value | |
Stimulation (sham vs. anodal) | 0.103 | 0.498 | 0.619 | − 6.438 | − 2.472 | 0.015 |
Task (con vs. non-con) | − 0.129 | − 0.622 | 0.535 | − 0.428 | − 0.163 | 0.870 |
Trial (congr. vs. incongr.) | − 2.145 | − 10.420 | < 0.001 | 35.700 | 13.804 | < 0.001 |
Age | 0.168 | 0.720 | 0.473 | 3.601 | 0.987 | 0.325 |
Visit 1 | − 0.546 | − 1.460 | 0.147 | 8.616 | 1.813 | 0.072 |
Visit 2 | − 0.275 | − 0.766 | 0.445 | 7.161 | 1.583 | 0.116 |
Visit 3 | 0.354 | 0.982 | 0.328 | − 6.078 | − 1.340 | 0.182 |
Stimulation × task | − 0.004 | − 0.021 | 0.983 | 0.738 | 0.282 | 0.778 |
Stimulation × trial | − 0.025 | − 0.123 | 0.902 | 0.185 | 0.072 | 0.943 |
Concurrent × trial | − 0.298 | − 1.447 | 0.150 | 0.110 | 0.043 | 0.966 |
Stimulation × age | − 0.225 | − 1.961 | 0.052 | − 0.328 | − 0.226 | 0.821 |
Task × age | 0.185 | 1.607 | 0.110 | 1.864 | 1.287 | 0.200 |
Trial × age | 0.057 | 0.511 | 0.610 | 3.298 | 2.364 | 0.019 |
Stimulation × task × trial | − 0.295 | − 1.432 | 0.154 | 1.479 | 0.571 | 0.569 |
Stimulation × task × age | − 0.014 | − 0.119 | 0.905 | − 2.092 | − 1.421 | 0.157 |
Stimulation × trial × age | 0.004 | 0.032 | 0.975 | 0.085 | 0.061 | 0.951 |
Task × trial × age | 0.114 | 1.023 | 0.308 | − 1.175 | − 0.842 | 0.401 |
Significant results are printed in bold.
Neurophysiological data
The ANOVAs of 2-back and Flanker ERPs did not show a significant effect of stimulation or a significant stimulation*atDCS + target task interaction.
Similarly, our analyses of 2-back task related alpha, theta and beta ERD/ERS revealed no significant stimulation effect or stimulation*atDCS + target task interaction. For the Flanker task ERD/ERS our analyses did not show a main effect of stimulation but a significant interaction effect of stimulation*atDCS + target task for beta ERD/ERS in an area covering the stimulation target area (AF7, Fp1, AF3, C5, Fpz, F3, F7, FC5, T7; 430–700 ms; see Fig. 6c). Pairwise comparisons showed increased oscillatory beta power following anodal compared to sham stimulation in the non-concurrent atDCS + target task condition (t = − 2.744, p = 0.014; see Fig. 6a,d,e). Additionally, following anodal stimulation beta power was increased for non-concurrent compared to the concurrent tDCS + target task condition (t = − 4.16, p = 0.002).
Two-way repeated measures ANOVAs of resting state frequency data did not show a significant stimulation effect for any frequency band. Also, the interaction of stimulation*atDCS + target task was not significant in all frequency bands.
Correlation of E-field and stimulation effects
We did not find a significant correlation between the individual normal component of the E-field and stimulation effects on behavioural or neurophysiological outcomes. Especially the significant stimulation effect on Flanker RT was not correlated with the individual E-field component (see Fig. 5b), neither for the concurrent stimulation condition (r = − 0.13, p = 0.612) nor for the non-concurrent atDCS + target task condition (r = 0.13, p = 0.619). The same was true for the stimulation effect on Flanker task related beta oscillatory power and the individual E-field component for the concurrent (r = − 0.15, p = 0.578) and non-concurrent atDCS + target task condition (r = 0.06, p = 0.816; see Fig. 6b).
Additionally, participants age and the individual E-field component were not correlated (see Fig. 1b).
Tolerability and blinding of tDCS
For the conditions sham tDCS without concurrent task (χ2 (1, N = 22) = 0.18, p = 0.669), sham tDCS with concurrent task (χ2 (1, N = 22) = 1.81, p = 0.179) and atDCS with concurrent task (χ2 (1, N = 20) = 2.91, p = 0.088) participants were unable to guess better than chance whether they had received anodal or sham stimulation, while for the atDCS without concurrent task condition the rate of correct assumptions was significantly higher than guess probability (χ2 (1, N = 22) = 4.54, p = 0.033). The intensity of perceived side effects was generally low (see Supplemental Table S1). Only the intensity of perceived itching during stimulation was significantly higher under anodal compared to sham stimulation (z = 2.52, p = 0.012).
Discussion
The goal of the present study was to examine the effects of multichannel atDCS targeting the lDLPFC in children and adolescents, investigating the influence of concurrent task performance during stimulation and individual anatomy. We could show that (1) atDCS did not affect WM performance in a 2-back task (target task). Additionally, atDCS did not affect neurophysiological activity in this 2-back task (note, analyses for 2-back neurophysiological activity were based on correct rejection trials since there were not enough trials to calculate ERP components for hit targets). (2) In a Flanker task (non-target task) we found a reduction of RT following atDCS. (3) This effect was independent of whether atDCS had been combined with a cognitive task (2-back task). However, increased Flanker task related beta oscillation was observed only following stimulation without concurrent task performance. (4) The individual E-field was not correlated with stimulation effects. (5) The stimulation led to minor side effects.
Our partly null finding could be due to a suboptimal combination of stimulation parameters. Based on computational modelling, a multichannel montage leads to a more focused stimulation while a classical bipolar montage leads to a comparatively diffuse current flow in the brain20,22. However, it is not clear whether a more focal stimulation also causes stronger tDCS effects. WM is a cognitive process based on a widely distributed neural network63,64. A multichannel montage may not be able to sufficiently activate this underlying network due to its focality, which may lead to reduced effects on performance, while larger electrodes allow simultaneous activation of different relevant brain regions65.
Furthermore, the 2 mA total injected current used in this study may not have been sufficient in every participant in terms of induced E-field strength in the target area to lead to detectable tDCS effects66. More important than simply increasing the current intensity seems to be the individualisation of the stimulation in order to ensure that a sufficient and at the same time safe current intensity is applied to each participant22,67. The multi-channel montage we used was optimised for the target region lDLPFC, but only based on a standard brain. A next step would be to individualise the optimisation with respect to the participant specific neuroanatomy.
Regarding WM performance, imaging studies prove that the activation of the DLPFC is dependent on cognitive load68, with higher cognitive load leading to stronger neural activation and potentially stronger tDCS effects. However, the DLPFC activation is related to the individual WM capacity69 and shows a nonmonotonic, inverted-U response to WM load70. Therefore, an excessive cognitive load could also be an inhibitor for a possible tDCS effect34. Compared to a standard 2-back task, the task we used was complicated by the requirement to respond to each stimulus, not only target stimuli, and the use of lures. The 2-back accuracy during stimulation was shown to be dependent on the age of participants, which may indicate a possible overload in younger children. Thus, it can be hypothesised that some participants were overchallenged by the task and the optimum of their DLPFC activation had already been exceeded.
In terms of neurophysiological activity related to WM performance, we cannot draw definite conclusions since we were not able to analyse target hit trials in the 2-back task. Here, EEG analyses were based on correct rejection trials that do not represent the primary measure of n-back tasks.
Results of the resting-state EEG recordings showed that theta, alpha and beta power did not differ significantly between anodal and sham tDCS conditions. These results correspond to previous studies in adults demonstrating no atDCS effect on resting state neurophysiological activity36–38. The missing atDCS influence on resting state EEG might be due to a state dependency of stimulation effects71,72. Besides, resting state EEG was recorded directly after termination of stimulation. This might have been too early for long term tDCS after-effects to occur. Future studies should include additional resting state EEG recordings following task performances after stimulation. In this way, long-term effects could be revealed, as they have already been found in previous studies33,73,74.
In contrast to the 2-back task, we found an atDCS effect on behavioural and neurophysiological outcomes in the non-target Flanker task. Our results are consistent with previous studies in adults that also found reduced RT in a Flanker task after atDCS over the lDLPFC32,75. Still, it should be emphasised that the stimulation effects in the Flanker task were small and limited to RT and specific neuronal oscillations. As for WM functioning, the functions relevant for the Flanker task, such as interference control, sustained attention and response inhibition, are based on a network of different brain areas. This network includes the DLPFC and the anterior cingulate cortex as critical hubs76. We cannot exclude the possibility that our multichannel montage activated the circuits involved in the Flanker task more strongly than the 2-back related WM network due to the distribution of the electrodes on the skull.
Interestingly, the atDCS effect on Flanker task performance occurred regardless of whether atDCS had been combined with a cognitive task or not. The fact that improvements in Flanker task performance were also found after atDCS with concurrent 2-back task performance argues against task-specific effects of stimulation, as assumed by the activity selectivity model77,78. The Flanker task is mainly applied to investigate interference control, but also relies on WM processes. Working memory, in turn, is composed of different executive functions79,80. While the 2-back task particularly requires an updating component81, the Flanker task especially requires an inhibition component82. Both WM components seem to be closely connected functionally83,84 and structurally85, which is why a transfer of stimulation effects is conceivable. Additionally, following the flexible hub theory, a general enhancement of the lDLPFC activity could contribute to the improvement in different tasks30,31. At the same time, the changes in Flanker task performance cannot be clearly attributed to stimulation in isolation since this task was performed as second task after the application of stimulation and 2-back task performance. This increased strain on the target area lDLPFC through the stimulation and the following execution of the 2-back task after stimulation might have had effects on the neuroplasticity and thus on the performance in the Flanker task86.
Besides the behavioural changes, we found increased Flanker task related beta activity. In the prefrontal cortex a connection of beta oscillations with WM, interference control and distraction prevention has been observed before87–90. Studies in adults suggest an activity selectivity of neurophysiological changes following tDCS combined with task performance38,91. In our study, the 2-back task during stimulation could also have led to more selective activation, which may have counteracted a transfer effect. However, in this case, neurophysiological activity seems to be more sensitive to this influence than behavioural activity. Therefore, regarding our hypothesis that the effects of stimulation depend on the performance of a task during stimulation, we cannot draw a definite conclusion.
We did not find a connection between atDCS effects and calculations of the individual E-field. This could be because we computed the individual E-field only for a subsample, which may have masked possible correlations. Besides, we determined the participant-specific stimulation effect only in relation to the sham condition. Other studies that have investigated the relationship between individual E-field and tDCS effects have mostly determined the stimulation effect in relation to a pre-stimulation baseline17,92. In this way, tDCS effects can be examined more precisely, for example by considering the participants day-specific individual performance level.
In children and adolescents, a 2 mA multichannel montage has not been investigated so far. In the current study, all participants reported only mild side effects directly following stimulation. Nevertheless, our study demonstrates the importance to adapt atDCS for applications in a paediatric group, also regarding safety aspects. The case of a female participant, who developed an epileptic disease during her participation in this study, shows that the screening procedure needs to ask more specifically for neurological abnormalities40,93. Additionally, our study demonstrates the importance to proactively ask for adverse events following stimulation sessions, as it is procedure in clinical trials.
There are some limitations in our study. Due to a relatively large drop-out, we were only able to include a comparatively small sample (n = 22) in the evaluation. Furthermore, the E-field distribution was only computed for a subsample. In particular, the lack of correlation between individual E-field distributions and atDCS effects can therefore only be interpreted to a limited extent. To draw stronger conclusions regarding this relationship in children and adolescents, future studies should examine a larger sample size. At the same time, experiments on children should start with small samples to avoid exposing a large population to potential risks. Furthermore, the acquisition of a task baseline at each visit would have been an additional control for intraindividual differences. Especially the investigation of the influence of individual E-field on atDCS effects would have benefited from such a baseline. Still, we were able to determine stimulation effects with regards to sham stimulation. Another limitation is that participants were not effectively blinded whether they had received anodal or sham stimulation in the non-concurrent atDCS condition. We used a control task to differentiate stimulation effects more clearly. However, using an additional active stimulation control condition (e.g. cathodal tDCS) would have been beneficial. In this way, stimulation effects could have been more clearly delineated and effects attributable to lack of blinding more clearly excluded. However, the atDCS effect on Flanker task performance was independent of whether atDCS was applied with concurrent or non-concurrent task performance. Besides, no stimulation effect in the target 2-back task was observed.
Our study demonstrates limited but at the same time transfer effects of multichannel atDCS targeting the lDLPFC in children and adolescents. While our behavioural data argues against an activity selectivity effect of stimulation, neurophysiological activity might be more sensitive regarding concurrent task performance than behavioural outcomes. Further, tDCS studies in children and adolescents should use screening procedures adapted to this age group and inquire about adverse events after each stimulation session, to prevent dangers from the stimulation.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by European Union’s H2020 program under Grant Agreement No 731827 (STIPED). We thank Dr. Saskia Helbling, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany for assistance with statistical analysis for EEG in the FieldTrip toolbox.
Author contributions
All authors contributed extensively to the work presented in this paper. M.Sp., M.Si. and V.M. designed the experiment. M.Sp. administered the experiment, collected, and analysed the data. K.P. and C.M. contributed to the collection of the data. R.S., O.P., M.C.B., G.R. and A.T. performed modelling. M.Sp., R.S. and V.M. wrote the manuscript. M.Sp., H.B., C.B.-Z., A.P.-K. and K.K. developed the tasks for the study. C.B., T.S. and A.D. supported data analysis. A.P. and F.N. contributed to manuscript writing and extensive reviewing. All authors discussed the results and implications and commented on the manuscript at all stages. All authors reviewed the manuscript. All authors have approved the submitted version and agreed both to be personally accountable for the author’s own contributions and to ensure that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Competing interests
RS and MCB are employees at Neuroelectrics. GR is a co-founder of Neuroelectrics, a Company that manufactures tDCS and EEG technology. The remaining authors don’t have any competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors jointly supervised this work: Michael Siniatchkin and Vera Moliadze.
Contributor Information
Maike Splittgerber, Email: splittgerber@med-psych.uni-kiel.de.
Vera Moliadze, Email: moliadze@med-psych.uni-kiel.de.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-00933-z.
References
- 1.Ciullo V, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation and cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders. Systematic review of the evidence and future directions. Neuroscientist. 2020 doi: 10.1177/1073858420936167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Woods AJ, et al. A technical guide to tDCS, and related non-invasive brain stimulation tools. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2016;127:1031–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Moliadze V, et al. Stimulation intensities of transcranial direct current stimulation have to be adjusted in children and adolescents. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2015;126:1392–1399. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.10.142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Moliadze V, et al. ImA cathodal tDCS shows excitatory effects in children and adolescents. Insights from TMS evoked N100 potential. Brain Res. Bull. 2018;140:43–51. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2018.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kessler SK, et al. Dosage considerations for transcranial direct current stimulation in children. A computational modeling study. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e76112. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Beauchamp MS, et al. The developmental trajectory of brain-scalp distance from birth through childhood. Implications for functional neuroimaging. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e24981. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Knotkova H, Nitsche MA, Bikson M, Woods AJ. Practical Guide to Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation. Springer; 2019. pp. 133–165. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Heekeren HR, Marrett S, Bandettini PA, Ungerleider LG. A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain. Nature. 2004;431:859–862. doi: 10.1038/nature02966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.D’Esposito M, et al. The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature. 1995;378:279–281. doi: 10.1038/378279a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wörsching J, et al. Imaging transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the prefrontal cortex-correlation or causality in stimulation-mediated effects? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016;69:333–356. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Dedoncker J, Brunoni AR, Baeken C, Vanderhasselt M-A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in healthy and neuropsychiatric samples. Influence of stimulation parameters. Brain Stimul. 2016;9:501–517. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lee JC, Kenney-Jung DL, Blacker CJ, Doruk Camsari D, Lewis CP. Transcranial direct current stimulation in child and adolescent psychiatric disorders. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2019;28:61–78. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2018.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sotnikova A, Soff C, Tagliazucchi E, Becker K, Siniatchkin M. Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates neuronal networks in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Brain Topogr. 2017;30:656–672. doi: 10.1007/s10548-017-0552-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Batsikadze G, Moliadze V, Paulus W, Kuo M-F, Nitsche MA. Partially non-linear stimulation intensity-dependent effects of direct current stimulation on motor cortex excitability in humans. J. Physiol. 2013;591:1987–2000. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.249730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mosayebi Samani M, Agboada D, Jamil A, Kuo M-F, Nitsche MA. Titrating the neuroplastic effects of cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the primary motor cortex. Cortex. 2019;119:350–361. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ehrhardt SE, Filmer HL, Wards Y, Mattingley JB, Dux PE. The influence of tDCS intensity on decision-making training and transfer outcomes. J. Neurophysiol. 2021;125:385–397. doi: 10.1152/jn.00423.2020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kim J-H, et al. Inconsistent outcomes of transcranial direct current stimulation may originate from anatomical differences among individuals. Electric field simulation using individual MRI data. Neurosci. Lett. 2014;564:6–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.01.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Antonenko D, et al. Towards precise brain stimulation. Is electric field simulation related to neuromodulation? Brain Stimul. 2019;12:1159–1168. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.03.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Miranda PC, Mekonnen A, Salvador R, Ruffini G. The electric field in the cortex during transcranial current stimulation. Neuroimage. 2013;70:48–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Laakso I, et al. Electric fields of motor and frontal tDCS in a standard brain space. A computer simulation study. Neuroimage. 2016;137:140–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Saturnino GB, Siebner HR, Thielscher A, Madsen KH. Accessibility of cortical regions to focal TES. Dependence on spatial position, safety, and practical constraints. Neuroimage. 2019;203:116183. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Salvador, R. et al. In Brain and Human Body Modeling 2020, (eds Makarov S. N. et al.) (Cham (CH), 2021).
- 23.Fischer DB, et al. Multifocal tDCS targeting the resting state motor network increases cortical excitability beyond traditional tDCS targeting unilateral motor cortex. Neuroimage. 2017;157:34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Splittgerber M, et al. Individual baseline performance and electrode montage impact on the effects of anodal tDCS over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020;14:974. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Trumbo MC, et al. Enhanced working memory performance via transcranial direct current stimulation. The possibility of near and far transfer. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93:85–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gill J, Shah-Basak PP, Hamilton R. It’s the thought that counts. Examining the task-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on executive function. Brain Stimul. 2015;8:253–259. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Martin DM, Liu R, Alonzo A, Green M, Loo CK. Use of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to enhance cognitive training. Effect of timing of stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 2014;232:3345–3351. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4022-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hill AT, Fitzgerald PB, Hoy KE. Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory. A systematic review and meta-analysis of findings from healthy and neuropsychiatric populations. Brain Stimul. 2016;9:197–208. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Friehs MA, Frings C. Offline beats online. Transcranial direct current stimulation timing influences on working memory. NeuroReport. 2019;30:795–799. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000001272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Zanto TP, Gazzaley A. Fronto-parietal network. Flexible hub of cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013;17:602–603. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cole MW, et al. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:1348–1355. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Dubreuil-Vall L, Chau P, Ruffini G, Widge AS, Camprodon JA. tDCS to the left DLPFC modulates cognitive and physiological correlates of executive function in a state-dependent manner. Brain Stimul. 2019;12:1456–1463. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Keeser D, et al. Prefrontal direct current stimulation modulates resting EEG and event-related potentials in healthy subjects. A standardized low resolution tomography (sLORETA) study. Neuroimage. 2011;55:644–657. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hoy KE, et al. Testing the limits Investigating the effect of tDCS dose on working memory enhancement in healthy controls. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:1777–1784. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Boonstra TW, Nikolin S, Meisener A-C, Martin DM, Loo CK. Change in mean frequency of resting-state electroencephalography after transcranial direct current stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10:270. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Horvath JC, Forte JD, Carter O. Evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) generates little-to-no reliable neurophysiologic effect beyond MEP amplitude modulation in healthy human subjects. A systematic review. Neuropsychologia. 2015;66:213–236. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Gordon PC, et al. Modulation of cortical responses by transcranial direct current stimulation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. A resting-state EEG and TMS-EEG study. Brain Stimul. 2018;11:1024–1032. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hill AT, Rogasch NC, Fitzgerald PB, Hoy KE. Impact of concurrent task performance on transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)—Induced changes in cortical physiology and working memory. Cortex. 2019;113:37–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 1974;16:143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Splittgerber M, et al. First generalized tonic clonic seizure in the context of pediatric tDCS—A case report. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2019 doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2019.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Weiß RH. CFT 20-R. Grundintelligenztest Skala 2—Revision. Hogrefe; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Döpfner M, Plück J, Kinnen C. CBCL/6-18R, TRF/6-18R, YSR/11-18R. Deutsche Schulalter-Formen der Child Behavior Checklist von Thomas M. Achenbach. Hogrefe; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Döpfner M, Görtz-Dorten A, Lehmkuhl G. DISYPS-II, Diagnostik-System für psychische Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter nach ICD-10 und DSM-IV. Huber; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) Western Psychological Services; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Barbey AK, Koenigs M, Grafman J. Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex. 2013;49:1195–1205. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Stark SM, Kirwan CB, Stark CEL. Mnemonic similarity task. A tool for assessing hippocampal integrity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019;23:938–951. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kirchner WK. Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J. Exp. Psychol. 1958;55:352–358. doi: 10.1037/h0043688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Jonides J, et al. Verbal working memory load affects regional brain activation as measured by PET. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1997;9:462–475. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Breitling C, et al. Improving interference control in ADHD patients with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2016;10:72. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2016.00072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2019).
- 51.Macmillan NA, Creelman CD. Response bias. Characteristics of detection theory, threshold theory, and “nonparametric” indexes. Psychol. Bull. 1990;107:401–413. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.3.401. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Ruffini G, Fox MD, Ripolles O, Miranda PC, Pascual-Leone A. Optimization of multifocal transcranial current stimulation for weighted cortical pattern targeting from realistic modeling of electric fields. Neuroimage. 2014;89:216–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen J-M. FieldTrip. Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011;2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Pfueller U, et al. Maturation of P300 amplitude and short-term learning as reflected by P300 habituation between trial blocks in children. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2011;79:184–194. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Folstein JR, van Petten C. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP. A review. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:152–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Danielmeier C, Wessel JR, Steinhauser M, Ullsperger M. Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict. Psychophysiology. 2009;46:1288–1298. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00860.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Pfurtscheller G, da Silva FHL. Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization. Basic principles. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1999;110:1842–1857. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00141-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Puonti O, et al. Accurate and robust whole-head segmentation from magnetic resonance images for individualized head modeling. Neuroimage. 2020;219:117044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Fischl B, et al. Whole brain segmentation. Neuron. 2002;33:341–355. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Opitz A, Windhoff M, Heidemann RM, Turner R, Thielscher A. How the brain tissue shapes the electric field induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroimage. 2011;58:849–859. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Antal A, et al. Low intensity transcranial electric stimulation. Safety, ethical, legal regulatory and application guidelines. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017;128:1774–1809. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Koller M. Robustlmm: An R package for robust estimation of linear mixed-effects models. J. Stat. Soft. 2016 doi: 10.18637/jss.v075.i06. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Rottschy C, et al. Modelling neural correlates of working memory. A coordinate-based meta-analysis. Neuroimage. 2012;60:830–846. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Eriksson J, Vogel EK, Lansner A, Bergström F, Nyberg L. Neurocognitive architecture of working memory. Neuron. 2015;88:33–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Savic B, Müri R, Meier B. High definition transcranial direct current stimulation does not modulate implicit task sequence learning and consolidation. Neuroscience. 2019;414:77–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.06.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Vöröslakos M, et al. Direct effects of transcranial electric stimulation on brain circuits in rats and humans. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:483. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-02928-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Evans C, et al. Dose-controlled tDCS reduces electric field intensity variability at a cortical target site. Brain Stimul. 2020;13:125–136. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Manoach DS, et al. Prefrontal cortex fMRI signal changes are correlated with working memory load. NeuroReport. 1997 doi: 10.1097/00001756-199701200-00033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.van Snellenberg JX, et al. Dynamic shifts in brain network activation during supracapacity working memory task performance. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;36:1245–1264. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Callicott JH, et al. Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. Cereb. Cortex (New York) 1999;9:20–26. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.1.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Hsu T-Y, Juan C-H, Tseng P. Individual differences and state-dependent responses in transcranial direct current stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10:643. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Silvanto J, Muggleton NG, Cowey A, Walsh V. Neural adaptation reveals state-dependent effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2007;25:1874–1881. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05440.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology. 2001;57:1899–1901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Stagg CJ, Antal A, Nitsche MA. Physiology of transcranial direct current stimulation. J. ECT. 2018;34:144–152. doi: 10.1097/YCT.0000000000000510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Karuza EA, et al. Mapping the parameter space of tDCS and cognitive control via manipulation of current polarity and intensity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10:665. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Fan J, Flombaum JI, McCandliss BD, Thomas KM, Posner MI. Cognitive and brain consequences of conflict. Neuroimage. 2003;18:42–57. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Fertonani A, Miniussi C. Transcranial electrical stimulation. What we know and do not know about mechanisms. Neuroscientist. 2017;23:109–123. doi: 10.1177/1073858416631966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Bikson M, Rahman A. Origins of specificity during tDCS. Anatomical, activity-selective, and input-bias mechanisms. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:688. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013 doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Baddeley A. Working memory. Theories, models, and controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2012;63:1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Collette F, van der Linden M. Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2002;26:105–125. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00063-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Sanders AF, Lamers JM. The Eriksen flanker effect revisited. Acta Physiol. (Oxf.) 2002;109:41–56. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00048-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Szmalec A, Verbruggen F, Vandierendonck A, Kemps E. Control of interference during working memory updating. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2011;37:137–151. doi: 10.1037/a0020365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Scharinger C, Soutschek A, Schubert T, Gerjets P. When flanker meets the n-back. What EEG and pupil dilation data reveal about the interplay between the two central-executive working memory functions inhibition and updating. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:1293–1304. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Nee DE, et al. A meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. Cereb. Cortex (New York). 2013 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Vosskuhl J, Strüber D, Herrmann CS. Non-invasive brain stimulation. A paradigm shift in understanding brain oscillations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018;12:211. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Tafuro A, Ambrosini E, Puccioni O, Vallesi A. Brain oscillations in cognitive control. A cross-sectional study with a spatial stroop task. Neuropsychologia. 2019;133:107190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Ambrosini E, Vallesi A. Domain-general stroop performance and hemispheric asymmetries. A resting-state EEG study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017;29:769–779. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Miller EK, Lundqvist M, Bastos AM. Working memory 2.0. Neuron. 2018;100:463–475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Swann N, et al. Intracranial EEG reveals a time- and frequency-specific role for the right inferior frontal gyrus and primary motor cortex in stopping initiated responses. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:12675–12685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3359-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Pisoni A, et al. Cognitive enhancement induced by anodal tDCS drives circuit-specific cortical plasticity. Cereb. Cortex (New York) 2018;28:1132–1140. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Laakso I, Mikkonen M, Koyama S, Hirata A, Tanaka S. Can electric fields explain inter-individual variability in transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor cortex? Sci. Rep. 2019;9:626. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-37226-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Sierawska A, et al. First epileptic seizure and initial diagnosis of juvenile myoclonus epilepsy (JME) in a transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) study—Ethical analysis of a clinical case. Neuroethics. 2020;13:347–351. doi: 10.1007/s12152-020-09444-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.