Skip to main content
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
. 2021 Oct 22;15:727551. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.727551

Consilience in the Peripheral Sensory Adaptation Response

Willy Wong 1,*
PMCID: PMC8569822  PMID: 34744660

Abstract

Measurements of the peripheral sensory adaptation response were compared to a simple mathematical relationship involving the spontaneous, peak, and steady-state activities. This relationship is based on the geometric mean and is found to be obeyed to good approximation in peripheral sensory units showing a sustained response to prolonged stimulation. From an extensive review of past studies, the geometric mean relationship is shown to be independent of modality and is satisfied in a wide range of animal species. The consilience of evidence, from nearly 100 years of experiments beginning with the work of Edgar Adrian, suggests that this is a fundamental result of neurophysiology.

Keywords: peripheral sensory response, spike frequency, adaptation, consilience, theoretical predictions

1. Introduction

Consilience is the convergence of evidence from different lines of studies or approaches (Whewell, 1840; Wilson, 1999). The unity of science requires results obtained by one approach to concur with evidence obtained by another approach. This principle has found utility in biology where systematic bodies of evidence can be hard to obtain, and has been invoked most prominently in establishing the modern theory of evolution.

Can consilience find application in sensory physiology? While there is little debate that sensory adaptation appears universally amongst the different senses and organisms, there has been no attempt to carry out a quantitative comparison of adaptation responses. Some of the first experiments conducted on sensory nerves were carried out by Nobel Laureate Edgar Adrian. Collaborating with his assistant Yngve Zotterman, Adrian conducted one of his most celebrated experiments: the measurement of rate of impulses from the frog muscle spindle to the stretch of a muscle (Adrian and Zotterman, 1926a). What Adrian found was that the neural activity rises immediately upon initiation of stretch and falls monotonically with time. This is now known as sensory adaptation and is observed nearly universally in all of the senses across many different organisms. A schematic representation of their findings can be found in Figure 1 which includes spontaneous activity prior to the application of the stimulus (SR), the peak activity that occurs at or soon after the presentation of the stimulus (PR), and the steady-state activity after adaptation has stopped (SS).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Peripheral sensory adaptation curve. An idealized sensory adaptation response showing steady-state spontaneous rate (SR) prior to introduction of stimulus, the peak response to the stimulus (PR), and the subsequent new steady-state response (SS).

A particular pattern emerges from Adrian's study when the results are analyzed numerically. By taking the three fixed points in the graph, we observe from his data that the steady-state activity equals the geometric mean of the peak activity and spontaneous activity. This finding is not restricted to a single one of his experiments. In the third instalment of the celebrated 1926 papers (Adrian and Zotterman, 1926b), they measured adaptation in Merkel units in the footpad of a cat to pressure stimuli where a similar result can be found. In equation form, this implies that

SS=PR×SR (1)

Subsequent to Adrian's discoveries, many investigators have studied the peripheral response in other modalities and organisms. While the adaptation response tends to follow the same qualitative shape of Figure 1, the question that remains unanswered is whether they share quantitative similarities. Neurons process and encode many types of information; a diversity of responses can be found at the peripheral level, some of which may even differ from the representation shown in Figure 1. Notable exceptions include the rapidly adapting mechanoreceptors in touch as well as certain units involved in temperature coding. This study focuses on responses that show sustained activity with constant input in isolated peripheral sensory units.

An exhaustive search was carried out on past studies of peripheral sensory adaptation. From these studies, results were analyzed and compared to Equation (1). Despite vastly different mechanisms and modalities, and in organisms from different phyla in Animalia, Equation (1) was found to be obeyed to good approximation. From the perspective of consilience, this demonstrates that Equation (1) is widely applicable and may constitute a new law of neurophysiology. The discovery of this equation came from some recent theoretical advances (Wong, 2020).

2. Results and Discussion

A comprehensive search was carried out in peripheral sensory adaptation yielding thirty six studies which satisfied the conditions set forth in the search criteria (see section 3). A third of the studies were conducted within the past 20 years. At least 15 studies included multiple measurements under different conditions. In total, there are 250 adaptation responses analyzed. The dataset spans eight of the most important sensory modalities including proprioception, touch, taste, hearing, vision, smell, electroreception, and temperature. A total of 13 studies were identified to test the same modality/same animal species combination but were conducted in different labs. One study examined units of both high and low spontaneous activity in the same modality. There are other aspects of the data set worth noting and are discussed later.

We begin the analysis with point-wise comparisons to Equation (1). Table 1 shows the results of 19 studies where a single comparison of spontaneous, peak, and steady-state activities can be made. Equation (1) appears to hold well across different animal species and modalities, although the limited availability of data in each study makes it difficult to draw robust conclusions. Taken together, however, the convergence of evidence is strong. Error between predicted and measured steady-state response is generally within ten percent or less. There are exceptions. The largest source of discrepancy is found in a study on temperature (Merivee et al., 2003) where in two instances the prediction misses the measured value by a considerable amount. Thermoreception is particularly difficult to reconcile and is discussed in more detail later. A number of results (marked by *) are derived from “inverted” responses and are also discussed later.

Table 1.

A summary of different peripheral sensory adaptation results highlighting the relationship between spontaneous, peak, and steady-state activities for different modalities and organisms.

Spontaneous Peak Steady-state Prediction Modality Organism Source
(SR, Hz) (PR, Hz) (SS, Hz) (Hz)
6.1 26.8 13.2 12.8 Proprioception Frog Figure 10B (exp. 12), Adrian and Zotterman, 1926a
3.7 99.7 19.3 19.2 Touch Cat Figure 8 (exp. 6), Adrian and Zotterman, 1926b
15.4 59.9 25.9 30.4 Proprioception Cat Figure 3 (filled circles, prep. 52), Boyd and Roberts, 1953
14.1* 9.1* 11.0* 11.3* Proprioception Cat Figure 3 (open circles, stimulus on, prep. 52), Boyd and Roberts, 1953
11.0* 16.9* 13.8* 13.6* Proprioception Cat Figure 3 (open circles, stimulus off, prep. 52), Boyd and Roberts, 1953
12.9 31.1 18.9 20.0 Proprioception Cat Figure 4 (initial adaptation, prep. 54), Boyd and Roberts, 1953
37.7 151 81.7 75.4 Hearing Cat Figure 6.6 (unit 297-43), Kiang, 1965
60.1 179 100 104 Hearing Cat Figure 6.7 (unit 299-22), Kiang, 1965
28.8 70.7 44.6 45.1 Proprioception Cat Figure 7 (first adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
44.6* 18.1* 33.4* 28.4* Proprioception Cat Figure 7 (second adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
33.4 65.1 43.7 46.6 Proprioception Cat Figure 7 (third adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
43.7* 16.4* 28.0* 26.8* Proprioception Cat Figure 7 (fourth adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
28.0 57.8 42.4 40.2 Proprioception Cat Figure 7 (fifth adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
42.4* 13.4 * 22.9* 23.8* Proprioception Cat Figure 7 (sixth adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
23.9 43.4 32.0 32.2 Proprioception Cat Figure 8 (first adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
32.0* 8.9* 23.3* 16.9 * Proprioception Cat Figure 8 (second adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
23.3 43.4 30.7 31.8 Proprioception Cat Figure 8 (third adaptation) Matthews and Stein, 1969
30.7* 11.2* 22.9* 18.5* Proprioception Cat Figure 8 (fourth adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
22.9 42.0 31.6 31.0 Proprioception Cat Figure 8 (fifth adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
31.6* 7.9* 23.0* 15.8* Proprioception Cat Figure 8 (sixth adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
23.0 42.0 30.9 31.1 Proprioception Cat Figure 8 (seventh adaptation), Matthews and Stein, 1969
6.7 22.6 10.9 12.3 Touch Cat Figure 2B (adaptation), Chambers et al., 1972
10.9* 1.9* 6.7* 4.6* Touch Cat Figure 2B (recovery), Chambers et al., 1972
2.7 16.3 7.4 6.6 Vision Squid Figure 1b, Lange and Hartline, 1974
8.5 67.9 23.6 24.0 Smell Mosquito Figure 2 (top), Davis, 1976
11.7 69.6 30.6 28.5 Smell Mosquito Figure 2 (bottom, middle presentation), Davis, 1976
112 354 223 199 Electroreception Fish Figure 10 (p-unit, first adaptation), Hopkins, 1976
122 359 216 209 Electroreception Fish Figure 10 (p-unit, second adaptation), Hopkins, 1976
95.6 151 143 120 Electroreception Fish Figure 10 (t-unit), Hopkins, 1976
130 639 307 288 Electroreception Fish Figure 12A, Hopkins, 1976
13.6 81.0 29.5 33.2 Taste Rat Figure 2 (initial adaptation), Smith et al., 1978
4.9 27.5 11.6 12.4 Proprioception Crayfish Figure 1, Barrio et al., 1988
12.2 96.1 28.7 34.2 Hearing (warm) Gerbil Figure 1 (upper), Ohlemiller and Siegel, 1998
2.6 51.1 14.5 11.5 Hearing (cool) Gerbil Figure 1 (lower), Ohlemiller and Siegel, 1998
6.1 199 30.5 34.8 Smell Fruit fly Figure 11A (first presentation), De Bruyne et al., 1999
3.8 199 28.8 27.5 Smell Fruit fly Figure 11C (middle presentation), De Bruyne et al., 1999
1.8 9.9 4.0 4.2 Proprioception Cockroach Figure Ai, Ridgel et al., 2000
3.8 199 28.8 27.5 Proprioception Cockroach Figure Aii, Ridgel et al., 2000
17.0 144 33.4 49.4 Temperature Beetle Figure 10A, Merivee et al., 2003
4.6 278 21.2 35.8 Temperature Beetle Figure 10B, Merivee et al., 2003
2.3 333 26.1 27.6 Temperature Beetle Figure 10C, Merivee et al., 2003
2.4 15.0 7.7 6.0 Smell Fish Figure 1a (Lys), Friedrich and Laurent, 2004
4.9 70.0 22.3 18.5 Smell Fish Figure 1c (Trp), Friedrich and Laurent, 2004
3.1 73.2 24.2 15.1 Smell Fish Figure 1c (Trp, 50 min. later), Friedrich and Laurent, 2004
7.8 61.4 19.1 21.9 Temperature Mosquito Figure 2A, Gingl et al., 2005
3.3* 15.0* 7.6* 7.0* Temperature Mosquito Figure 2B, Gingl et al., 2005
0.14* 2.0* 0.58* 0.53* Vision Jellyfish Figure 3D, Garm and Mori, 2009
1.1 2.1 1.5 1.5 Vision Jellyfish Figure 4D, Garm and Mori, 2009
1.5 40.0 7.4 7.7 Water movement Fish Figure 2, Mogdans et al., 2017
5.6 104 28.9 24.1 Water movement Fish Figure 5a, Mogdans et al., 2017
26.5 71.6 42.2 43.6 Water movement Fish Figure 5b, Mogdans et al., 2017
*

Indicates data derived from an inverted adaptation response.

The first two datasets in Table 1 are from Adrian's own pioneering work and are reconstructed using either extrapolated data because the stimulus was not held long-enough for the response to reach steady state (Adrian and Zotterman, 1926a), or by pooling data from other recordings of the same unit (Adrian and Zotterman, 1926b). It would be easy to exclude these studies, but their historical significance cannot be ignored. Of additional interest is a study on hearing which compares the adaptation response of the same unit when cooled or warmed relative to body temperature – see results conducted on gerbil hearing (Ohlemiller and Siegel, 1998). Changes in local temperature result in spike activities differing by at least a factor of two. One could easily imagine that the three values rise or fall uniformly with a change in temperature. Instead, they move in a direction to preserve the equality of the geometric mean. This has possible implications for the generality of the relationship across different animal species where metabolic activity can differ widely. One study was conducted on the visual sensory structures of the jellyfish (rhopalia) by measuring the output of pacemaker cells (Garm and Mori, 2009). The rhopalia modulates the output of these cells determining the basic swim movement in jellyfish (Garm and Mori, 2009; Katsuki and Greenspan, 2013).

The unfortunate circumstance is that spontaneous activity is not always reported or shown. However, this can be overcome if the adaptation response is measured to multiple stimulus levels. Since spontaneous activity is the activity in the absence of stimulation, its value is independent of intensity. As such, Equation (1) predicts a square root relationship between peak and steady-state activity which, on a double-log plot, is a straight line with slope 1/2 and value of intercept dependent on the level of spontaneous activity. See section 3 for mathematical details. Figure 2 shows the results from 14 studies conducted at different levels of intensity. Multiple measurements from the same unit increase the robustness of the findings. In total, this figure comprises over 170 adaptation responses. Figures 2A–H show responses from mechanoreception, (Figures 2I–M) chemoreception, (Figure 2N) thermoreception, and (Figure 2O) photoreception. Regression analysis is detailed in section 3. From the entirety of the data in Figure 2, a single value of slope was found to be 0.662.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Steady-state activity (SS) plotted as a function of peak activity (PR) for different stimulus intensities. In all panels, the dashed line shows the predictions of Equation (1) which is a line with slope one-half on a log-log plot with the value of SR set arbitrarily equal to 4. The actual value of intercept depends on the precise value of the unit's spontaneous activity. (A) Plot of SS vs PR for auditory data taken from a single guinea pig fibre (figure 1, unit GP-17-4), Smith and Zwislocki (1975); (B) the same for auditory data from a single gerbil fibre (figure 1, unit E8F2), Westerman and Smith (1984); (C) results for four separate guinea pig auditory fibres of both high and low spontaneous activity (triangles, figure 1, unit GP31/08; plusses, figure 2, unit GP27/18; circles, figure 1, GP31/13; crosses, figure 2, GP27/04), Yates et al. (1985); (D) the averaged auditory data from ferrets (figure 6), Sumner and Palmer (2012); (E) auditory responses obtained from the saccular nerve fibres of a gold fish (figure 3, increment), Fay (1985); (F) responses from lateral line in fish (figure 6), Mogdans et al. (2017) (G) stretch response in crayfish (figures 1 and 2, both PR and SS are shifted upwards by 0.5 log units), Brown and Stein (1966); (H) stretch response in frog (figure 3, both PR and SS are shifted upwards by 1 log unit), Loewenstein (1956); (I) response of olfactory receptor neurons in fruit flies (figure 3a: crosses, shifted +0.3 log units; figure 5: triangles, shifted +0.1 log units, methyl butyrate; circles, shifted −0.1 log units, methyl butyrate; squares, shifted −0.3 log units, 1-pentanol; plusses, shifted −0.5 log units, propyl acetate), Martelli et al. (2013); (J) taste recordings in fruit fly sensilla (circles, figure 3; crosses, figure 7), Gothilf et al. (1971); (K) taste response in caterpillar (figure 3), Bernays et al. (2002); (L) taste response in blowfly (figure 2a: circles, LiCl, shifted +0.2 log units; triangles, NaCl; crosses, KCl, shifted −0.2 log units; squares, RbCl, shifted −0.4 log units; plusses, CsCl, shifted −0.6 log units), Maes and Harms (1986); (M) same as (L) but for figure 2b, Maes and Harms (1986); (N) response to cooling in beetles (figure 10), Merivee et al. (2003); and (O) vision data from a single ON-centre ganglion cell in the cat. The vision data differ from the other auditory data in that they are derived from pre-adapted luminance values (figure 7: circles, 1 × 10−5 cd m−2, shifted +0.2 log units; plusses, 1 × 10−3 cd m−2; crosses: 1 × 10−1 cd m−2, shifted −0.2 log units), Sakmann and Creutzfeldt (1969).

Of particular interest are Figures 2A–C which show measurements taken from the same animal/modality (guinea pig hearing) conducted in different labs separated by almost 10 years apart. Figure 2C shows the response of four fibres, including two with low spontaneous activity and two with high spontaneous activity. In all cases, the power law relationship is preserved with those units with high spontaneous activity having a higher value of intercept than those with low spontaneous activity. Figures 2A–D show mammalian hearing mediated via the cochlea while Figure 2E shows hearing in the fish mediated via the otolith organs. In Figure 2F, we observe the response of the fish lateral line. Figures 2H,I show data from the response of taste receptors in a blowfly to five alkali salts at different intensities. In total, there are forty eight measurements from two units. Although the data are shown with offset, the actual values overlap indicating that they share a similar slope and intercept which is not surprising given the common value of spontaneous activity. Figure 2O differs from the other studies in that adaptation was conducted on top of an existing pedestal. That is, the retinal ganglion cell was adapted to an existing level of luminance before responding to a further increment. Despite the change in test condition, the quantitative aspects of the response remain unchanged.

Adaptation responses measured from ascending and descending staircases allow for further testing of Equation (1) to pre-adapted levels. A stimulus staircase is a series of ascending and/or descending intensity steps used to probe the response of a unit. Several studies have made use of stimulus staircases including: measurements from warm units in the cat (Handwerker and Neher, 1976) and bat (Schäfer et al., 1988) to increasingly warmer temperatures; recordings from M1 cells for non-image-based vision (i.e., intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells or ipRGC's) in mice to light of increasing levels (Milner and Do, 2017); measurements from olfactory sensory neurons of fruit flies to ascending and descending levels of acetone concentrations (Kim et al., 2011). The data from four studies together with the predicted values are shown in Figure 3. Taken together, this suggests the following generalization of Equation (1):

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Measured versus predicted steady-state activity for ascending and descending staircases. Data from an ascending staircase of temperatures in warm units of cats (crosses, figure 2a), Handwerker and Neher (1976) and bats (squares, figure 2), Schäfer et al. (1988); response of M1 ipRGC in mice to an ascending/descending luminance staircase (open/filled circles, figures 1b/c), Milner and Do (2017); ascending/descending staircases of acetone concentrations from olfactory sensory neurons in fruit flies which show both regular and inverted responses (open/filled triangles, figure 2c), Kim et al. (2011). Finally, the inverted responses from cold fibres to a descending temperature staircase (plusses, figure 2), Schäfer et al. (1989).

SSfinal=PR×SSinitial (2)

This equation includes (1) as a special case.

While adaptation responses commonly conform to the schematic representation shown in Figure 1, there are also circumstances where the response is inverted from its usual representation. The most common form of an “inverted” response is the recovery after the removal of the stimulus. The spike rate falls before returning to the original value prior to the application of the stimulus. Certain units also exhibit a fully inverted response. These are referred to as inhibitory responses, and can be observed in Gingl et al. (2005) where a warm and cool temperature receptor are presented with the same stimulus: The warm unit follows the typical adaptation response to a rise in temperature while the cool unit falls with the same temperature increase. Inhibitory responses are also commonly found in other modalities. Regardless of its shape, the salient point is that Equation (2) is obeyed not just for conventional adaptation but for inverted responses as well. See values marked with * in Table 1 and the response to a descending stimulus staircase for odour (Kim et al., 2011) and temperature (Schäfer et al., 1989) in Figure 3.

All of this suggests that there is a higher organizational principle underlying peripheral sensory adaptation. What sort of theory or model of transduction would be compatible with Equation (2)? There are a number of models, particularly in hearing, that provide good compatibility to experimental data recorded from single unit activity (Sumner et al., 2003; Zilany et al., 2009). However, since the geometric mean relationship appears to hold across different modalities, it is not reasonable to expect a modality-specific model to work with other modalities without additional, possibly ad hoc, assumptions. The model of spike frequency adaptation by Benda et al. (2005) is likely compatible with the geometric mean relationship provided that a suitable form of the firing rate function is assumed together with an appropriate choice of slope for both the peak and steady-state growth functions. Instead, Equation (1) emerges naturally and was first predicted from a theory of sensory processing under development for the past 50 years. The derivation of Equation (1) is provided in Appendix and has been detailed fully elsewhere (Wong, 2020).

Few data were found to be in complete violation of Equation (1). Those that were are most commonly found in thermoreception. Not only are the responses predicted poorly in Table 1 (see results for the beetle), but the slope of the data in Figure 2N falls short of the predicted value of 0.5 (Merivee et al., 2003). In the same study, there are cases where the three fixed points (spontaneous, peak, and steady-state) cannot be easily identified from the adaptation response. Other temperature studies show non-monotonic behaviour with changing stimulus levels (Schäfer et al., 1988, 1989; Heinz et al., 1990). While warming responses tend to fit better than cooling, on the balance thermoreception appears to violate Equation (1). A small number of results from other modalities were also found to be problematic. One study concerns the taste of blowflies (Dethier and Bowdan, 1984); however, the data in this case was obtained by averaging the response of different sensilla resulting in a non-monotonic adaptation curve. Another study, again concerning taste, shows data for sucrose and salt satisfying Equation (1), but not for pheromones (Bowdan, 1984). Apart from temperature, however, entire studies found in violation of Equation (1) were few in numbers.

Much of this investigation has focused on units that adapt slowly and show a sustained response to continued stimulation. Phasic receptors are rapidly adapting units that do not conform to the representation in Figure 1. At steady-state the spike activity is zero. It is believed that phasic units respond to the rate of change of stimulus (Binder et al., 2009). A test can be carried out to see if phasic units obey the geometric mean relationship by presenting a time-varying stimulus to induce a sustained response. This was attempted in Fernández and Goldberg (1976) where the vestibular units of monkeys were subjected to centrifugal forces and an adaptation response was measured to a constant force stimulus. While two of the adaptation responses compare favorably to the equation (unit 213–28: SSmeas = 80.3, SSpred = 86.8; unit 206–18: SSmeas = 125, SSpred = 130), the paper also cites data from units which show far less levels of adaptation (unit 213–28: SSmeas = 97.0, SSpred = 67.6). Moreover, the geometric mean relationship is found only to be satisfied in the activities of isolated sensory units in the absence of interaction from other cells in the neural circuitry. Any neurons that are part of the ascending auditory or visual pathway clearly do not follow the geometric mean relationship, e.g., see the auditory interneuron response (Hildebrandt et al., 2009) or the responses of the H1 neuron in the visual cortex (Maddess and Laughlin, 1985). Even within retinal ganglion cells, if the visual signal overlaps both the centre and surrounding regions (thereby recruiting inhibition), this will facilitate responses which deviate from Equation (1), e.g., Enroth-Cugell and Shapley, 1973.

The main result of this paper is a relationship connecting three fixed points in the adaptation curve. No consideration was made of the time-course of adaptation. And yet, an important point of discussion in the literature is the rate at which adaptation occurs. Adaptation curves are often fitted to a sum of exponentials with different time constants, e.g., Ohlemiller and Siegel, 1998. More recently, it has been proposed that neuronal adaptation can be better modeled using a power-law function of time (Drew and Abbott, 2006). This raises the question whether Equation (1) is compatible with such a formulation. It is important to remember that in both an exponential and a power-law description, a constant offset is required. This offset accounts for the non-zero activity when the stimulus is applied over long periods of time, e.g., see implementation of power law dynamics in Zilany et al. (2009). As such, Equation (1) is not affected by the time-course of adaptation.

There is considerable debate over the origins and role of spontaneous activity (Imaizumi et al., 2018). Spontaneous activity is often thought of as noise within the nervous system when in fact it is clear from Equation (1) that it is likely an integral part of normal sensory function. Spontaneous levels can convey information in the same manner that the peak and steady-state levels convey information about the environment (Eggermont, 2015). There has been much effort towards investigating the origins of spontaneous activity. Although mechanisms can differ, the geometric mean relationship suggest that the functional role of spontaneous activity is the same in all modalities. Equation (1) thus appears to imply that all neurons have non-zero spontaneous activity. While this may seem to contradict observation, it is important to remember that there is a difference between low spontaneous rates and zero activity. Assuming a Poisson model of spike generation, the probability that there are no spikes observed in a unit time interval equals exp(−λ) where λ is the average rate. If λ is sufficiently small, spike events will be rare enough to be considered absent even though it is technically non-zero.

While the analysis in this paper has involved the review of a great number of publications, it is virtually impossible to capture all studies that have included measurements of an adaptation response. And yet, the convergence of evidence already is striking. More than 200 measurements taken from different branches of sensory physiology are shown to be compatible with a single equation. These studies span different experimental preparations, different methods of stimulation, and may even require different techniques to measure the response. That the results would conform, even approximately, to the same mathematical relationship is remarkable and illustrates the true nature of consilience. A number of investigators have also engaged in testing the same modality-species combination. Since they were conducted independently, and are shown to obey the same relationship, this speaks to the reliability of the methodologies used. Table 2 shows a summary organized by animal species.

Table 2.

Summary of studies analyzed organized by animal classification.

Phylum Organism Modality and source
Chordata Bat Temperature (Schäfer et al., 1988)
Cat Touch (Adrian and Zotterman, 1926b; Chambers et al., 1972),
proprioception (Boyd and Roberts, 1953; Matthews and Stein, 1969), hearing (Kiang, 1965),
vision (Sakmann and Creutzfeldt, 1969), temperature (Handwerker and Neher, 1976)
Ferret Hearing (Sumner and Palmer, 2012)
Fish Electroreception (Hopkins, 1976), hearing (Fay, 1985), smell (Friedrich and Laurent, 2004),
movement (Mogdans et al., 2017)
Frog Proprioception (Adrian and Zotterman, 1926a; Loewenstein, 1956)
Gerbil Hearing (Westerman and Smith, 1984; Ohlemiller and Siegel, 1998)
Guinea pig Hearing (Smith and Zwislocki, 1975; Yates et al., 1985)
Mouse Non-image based vision (Milner and Do, 2017)
Pigeon Temperature (Schäfer et al., 1989)
Rat Taste (Smith et al., 1978)
Arthropoda Beetle Temperature (Merivee et al., 2003)
Blowfly Taste (Maes and Harms, 1986)
Caterpillar Taste (Bernays et al., 2002)
Cockroach Proprioception (Ridgel et al., 2000)
Crayfish Proprioception (Brown and Stein, 1966; Barrio et al., 1988)
Fruit fly Smell (De Bruyne et al., 1999; Kim et al., 2011; Martelli et al., 2013),
taste (Gothilf et al., 1971)
Mosquito Smell (Davis, 1976), temperature (Gingl et al., 2005)
Mollusca Squid Vision (Lange and Hartline, 1974)
Cnidaria Jellyfish Vision (Garm and Mori, 2009)

The compilation of studies in this paper also documents the historical development of the sensory sciences which followed the changes in technology unfolding in the twentieth century. For example, the proprioceptive and touch senses were among the first to be investigated as they were the easiest to access and their spike activity slow enough so that reliable spike counts could be achieved even back in Adrian's time using vacuum tube amplifiers (Garson, 2015). With the advent of computers and greater access to more invasive regions, the study of hearing and vision with their higher firing rates soon became possible. Finally, taste, olfaction, and temperature came relatively later due to the difficulty in controlling and maintaining level of stimulation. This may be one reason why the mechanoreception response shows a higher degree of conformity to Equation (1) than chemoreception where the variability can be high. Nevertheless, it is fascinating to observe that the data recorded in 1926 by Adrian and Zottermann hold almost the same fidelity as modern recordings.

Quoting Paul Willis: “Consilience means to use several different lines of inquiry that converge on the same or similar conclusions. The more independent investigations you have that reach the same result, the more confidence you can have that the conclusion is correct. Moreover, if one independent investigation produces a result that is at odds with the consilience of several other investigations, that is an indication that the error is probably in the methods of the adherent investigation, not in the conclusions of the consilience.”1 This work comprises a study of enormous breadth showing, perhaps for the first time, commonalities that exists across almost all sensory modalities and animal species. Evidence includes nearly 100 years of data from eight major sensory modalities, derived from organisms from four major phyla in Animalia (see Table 2). Regardless of mechanism or modality, or from which time period the study was conducted, the consilience of evidence lends proof to a new fundamental result of neurophysiology.

3. Methods

3.1. Data Selection and Processing

A search of peripheral sensory adaptation studies was conducted in academic databases (Google Scholar, Web of Science, and PubMed) casting a wide net using various combinations of keywords including sense, sensory, adapt, adaptation, fibre, unit, receptor, neuron, afferent, tonic, peripheral, action potential, impulse, spike, inter-spike, interval, ISI, frequency, firing, rate, discharge, activity, PST, PSTH, PETH, peri-event, post-stimulus, histogram, spontaneous, coding, pedestal, staircase, recovery, coding. Organism-specific terms like sensillum or sensilla were also used, as were modality specific terms like mechanoreception, chemoreception, thermoreception, electroreception and photoreception. Studies were also found through the tracking of citations. The following inclusion criteria were used:

  1. Measurements conducted on peripheral sensory neurons

  2. Unit stimulated with natural stimuli within its normal sensitivity range

  3. Stimulus onset is near instantaneous; stimulus is of sufficient length to achieve a steady-state response

  4. Spontaneous, peak, and steady-state responses are reported; otherwise, if no spontaneous activity is provided, adaptation response is measured to multiple stimulus levels

In certain cases, restrictions were relaxed to allow a greater number of studies to be included.

Data were digitized and extracted from original publications. The extracted data is available in the Supplementary Material. For certain studies, additional steps were required. For Adrian and Zotterman (1926a), the stimulus was not held long enough to achieve steady-state. Hence, the data from the peak until the removal of stimulus were fitted to an exponential plus offset equation: c1exp[-c2(t-t)]+c3 where t′ is the location of the peak and c1, c2, and c3 are unknown parameters to be determined by a non-linear fitting procedure carried out in MATLAB R2020a (MathWorks) using the function nlinfit. From here the value of c3 was inferred to be the steady-state activity. For Adrian and Zotterman (1926b), the steady-state value was estimated using another experiment conducted on the same unit, but with a slower stimulus ramp (crosses from Figure 8 of Exp. 6). Other studies have shown that the steady-state activity does not depend on the speed of the ramp, e.g., see Boyd and Roberts, 1953. In Loewenstein (1956), the 6% stretch was not included as the steady-state value was not provided. For Brown and Stein (1966), the peak values were obtained from Figure 1, but the steady-state values were obtained from Figure 2. In Yates et al. (1985), only the adaptation values conducted at 5 and 10 dB were extracted for unit GP27/04 as both the 15 and 20 dB experiments show peak responses exceeding the limit of the graph (as noted by the study authors themselves). In Schäfer et al. (1988), the spontaneous activity was missing and only three of four staircase levels were included. For Merivee et al. (2003), many of the responses do not show clear values for spontaneous, peak, and steady-state activities. As such, only the data of Figure 10 was considered. For Garm and Mori (2009), almost all eyes show responses which conform to Equation (1). However some responses took a long time to reach steady-state, and thus only the data from the lower lens eye (Figure 3) and upper lens eye (Figure 4) were included. For Martelli et al. (2013), several of the responses overlapped making it difficult to track their exact values particularly for the lower intensities. In Milner and Do (2017), values were extracted only where there was a clear steady-state level of activity attained, and that the input level lies within the sensitivity range of the unit. This included the third to seventh levels of the luminance staircase in Figure 1B and the fourth to sixth levels in Figure 1C.

It is also instructive to examine why certain datasets could not be included in the analysis. One study used large bin widths to calculate firing rates thereby obscuring the fine structure in adaptation (Baylin, 1979). Large bin widths reduce the noisiness of the response at the expense of reducing the value of peak activity. Recent methods have been developed to optimize the choice of bin width for time-varying rate data, e.g., Shimazaki and Shinomoto, 2007. This method is based on the observation that the spike count per bin accumulated over many trials will converge toward a Poisson distribution. By minimizing the mean integrated square error, an optimal choice of bin width can be found to best estimate the true spike rate. However, use of this method involves taking the entire spike sequence and analyzing it into a number of bins of different sizes, solving for the width that yields minimum estimate error. Many of the adaptation studies cited here were conducted before these methods were available. Fortunately, most studies appear to follow good statistical practice and their results are largely comparable with each other, although the use of adaptive bin sizing would likely improve the estimation of the ‘peak’ in the recovery responses. Some studies have also subtracted away the spontaneous activity from the rate data, e.g., Boyd and Roberts, 1953, rendering some of the results unusable. Finally, a good number of studies show responses that have not yet reached steady-state before the recording was terminated or the stimulus turned off. This is perhaps the single most significant reason why certain datasets could not be included, e.g., Smith and Zwislocki, 1975; Friedrich and Laurent, 2004. Or, if they were included e.g., Maes and Harms, 1986; Martelli et al., 2013, this introduced bias in the analysis. See next discussion.

3.2. Regression Analysis

An analysis of linear least squares was conducted on the data shown in Figure 2. First, a fit was conducted on each dataset separately with the equation

log10(SS)=β0+β1log10(PR) (3)

where β1 is the slope and β0 the intercept. SS is the steady-state activity and PR the peak activity. If the data conforms to equation (1) then β1 = 0.5 and β0 = 0.5log10(SR), i.e., the base 10 logarithm of spontaneous activity. Since SR is independent of intensity, β0 is constant. Table 3 shows the results of the analysis carried out in MATLAB R2020a (MathWorks). The analysis shows that much of the variation in the dependent variable is explained by the independent variable (mostly R2>0.9). The column labelled “Two parameter fit” in Table 3 shows the values of the fitted parameters together with the root-mean-square error (RMSE). In one dataset, panel c (crosses), there were only two points and thus several of the results are marked as “NA”. The confidence interval of the slopes overlapped the predicted value of 0.5 for only 50% of the datasets: there is considerable variation in the values. Therefore, it would be difficult to conclude that all datasets share the same slope.

Table 3.

Regression analysis of Figure 2.

Two parameter fit One parameter fit
Slope Intercept RMSE Intercept RMSE R 2 Panel Source
0.657 0.558 0.0464 0.917 0.0649 0.944 (a) Figure 1, unit GP-17-4, Smith and Zwislocki (1975)
0.404 0.755 0.0191 0.533 0.0350 0.977 (b) Figure 1, unit E8F2, Westerman and Smith (1984)
0.754 −0.0415 0.0149 0.506 0.124 0.998 (c) circles Figure 1, unit GP31/08, Yates et al. (1985)
0.469 1.02 0.0266 0.949 0.0276 0.943 (c) triangles Figure 2, unit GP27/18, Yates et al. (1985)
0.575 0.436 NA 0.608 NA NA (c) crosses Figure 1, GP31/13, Yates et al. (1985)
0.523 0.843 0.0140 0.897 0.0152 0.990 (c) plusses Figure 2, GP27/04, Yates et al. (1985)
0.339 1.27 0.0173 0.829 0.0288 0.886 (d) Figure 6, Sumner and Palmer (2012)
0.691 0.323 0.0473 0.775 0.0646 0.919 (e) Figure 3, increment, Fay (1985)
0.451 0.790 0.0037 0.702 0.0090 0.998 (f) Figure 6, Mogdans et al. (2017)
0.646 0.0431 0.0235 0.288 0.0745 0.994 (g) Figures 1 and 2, Brown and Stein (1966)
0.291 0.590 0.0252 0.320 0.0509 0.856 (h) Figure 3, Loewenstein (1956)
0.740 0.227 0.0477 0.720 0.0917 0.962 (i) crosses Figure 3a, Martelli et al. (2013)
0.750 0.212 0.0950 0.740 0.127 0.877 (i) triangles Figure 5, Martelli et al. (2013)
0.865 0.00965 0.0279 0.740 0.110 0.988 (i) circles Figure 5, Martelli et al. (2013)
0.717 0.402 0.0599 0.803 0.0999 0.951 (i) squares Figure 5, Martelli et al. (2013)
0.857 −0.0127 0.0901 0.692 0.165 0.932 (i) plusses Figure 5, Martelli et al. (2013)
0.291 0.997 0.0279 0.572 0.0440 0.744 (j) circles Figure 3, Gothilf et al. (1971)
0.288 0.858 0.0400 0.391 0.0443 0.294 (j) crosses Figure 7, Gothilf et al. (1971)
0.805 0.0428 0.151 0.791 0.189 0.801 (k) Figure 3, Bernays et al. (2002)
0.706 0.271 0.0127 0.751 0.0487 0.994 (l) crosses Figure 2a, LiCl, Maes and Harms (1986)
0.754 0.135 0.0115 0.731 0.0725 0.997 (l) triangles Figure 2a, NaCl, Maes and Harms (1986)
0.935 −0.347 0.0246 0.686 0.105 0.988 (l) circles Figure 2a, KCl, Maes and Harms (1986)
0.537 0.620 0.0226 0.706 0.0246 0.975 (l) squares Figure 2a, RbCl, Maes and Harms (1986)
0.448 0.761 0.0146 0.638 0.0171 0.964 (l) plusses Figure 2a, CsCl, Maes and Harms (1986)
0.808 −0.352 0.0457 0.355 0.118 0.975 (m) crosses Figure 2b, LiCl, Maes and Harms (1986)
0.754 −0.216 0.0350 0.367 0.123 0.990 (m) triangles Figure 2b, NaCl, Maes and Harms (1986)
0.941 −0.806 0.0533 0.260 0.172 0.977 (m) circles Figure 2b, KCl, Maes and Harms (1986)
1.06 −1.14 0.0426 0.194 0.160 0.979 (m) squares Figure 2b, RbCl, Maes and Harms (1986)
0.579 0.118 0.0431 0.291 0.0479 0.927 (m) plusses Figure 2b, CsCl, Maes and Harms (1986)
0.293 0.630 0.0546 0.160 0.100 0.826 (n) circles Figure 10, Merivee et al. (2003)
0.650 0.439 0.0476 0.787 0.0752 0.966 (o) circles Figure 7, 1 × 10 −5cd m−2, Sakmann and Creutzfeldt (1969)
0.321 1.26 0.0179 0.834 0.0564 0.967 (o) triangles Figure 7, 1 × 10−3 cd m−2, Sakmann and Creutzfeldt (1969)
0.415 0.966 0.0205 0.768 0.0370 0.982 (o) crosses Figure 7, 1 × 10−1 cd m−2, Sakmann and Creutzfeldt (1969)

What are the possible reasons for this discrepancy? Beyond the usual sources of error in measuring spike activity, there is one aspect of the data collection process that has not been addressed. Steady state activity is defined as the activity after which adaptation has stopped. However, as noted in a number of studies, e.g., Yates et al. (1985), adaptation curves take longer to reach steady-state when initiated with larger intensities. The consequence of this is significant. Since adaptation is measured from fixed duration presentations, the value of SS obtained from the final portion of the curve may not have reached steady-state, particularly for the higher intensities. This discrepancy will also increase as intensity is increased. From this, we conclude that there can be systematic overestimation of the slope. For example, both Maes and Harms (1986) and Martelli et al. (2013) show rate responses which have not yet reached steady-state. This is reflected in the upward rise in points at the higher firing frequencies due to the high steady-state values in panels (i) and (m) in Figure 2.

Nevertheless, if we assume that the slopes for all datasets are in fact identical, we can do one of two things. First is to fit each dataset with a regression line with slope fixed at 0.5. See column labelled “One parameter fit” in Table 3. The second is to fit all of the data to a single value of slope. This was accomplished through a simultaneous curve-fit involving 15 equations with 15 adjustable intercepts and a single value of slope. With all datasets weighted equally, the value of slope = 0.662 was obtained.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

WW conceived and conducted the research and wrote the manuscript.

Funding

The author acknowledges a Discovery Grant (458039) from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Appendix

Derivation of Equation (1)

A recent publication detailed the following equations governing the response of peripheral sensory neurons to time-varying stimulation Wong (2020). The theory is based on a mechanism-free approach to sensory information processing:

F=kH (A1)
H=12log(1+β(I+δI)pm) (A2)
dmdt=-a(m-meq) (A3)
meq=(I+δI)p/2 (A4)

where the firing rate response of the neuron F is related to the information or entropy of the stimulus H obtained by sampling a signal with intensity I. m(t) is the sample size and meq the optimal value of the sample size. meq has dependency on stimulus intensity through equation (4). k, β, p, a and δI are fixed parameters. The theory has been shown to work well with many time-varying inputs for different sensory modalities and organisms, see also Norwich (1993). The equations are not difficult to solve, requiring only a solution to a first-order ordinary differential equation. Moreover, they can be solved even more simply numerically using less than ten lines of computer code.

The following is an abbreviated derivation of equation (1); please see Wong (2020) for more details. We begin by solving the response to a step input to obtain the adaptation curve. Given an input that is zero for t < 0 and I for t > 0, equation (3) can be solved to be

m(t)=δIp/2e-at+(I+δI)p/2[1-e-at] (A5)

where the continuity of the solution requires the initial condition to be m(0) = δIp/2. Substituting m(t) into H and F gives the familiar monotonic decay behaviour observed in Figure 1.

The simplicity of equation (1) implies that it is likely the result of some approximation. Consider the case where β (I + δI)p/m ≪ 1 in equation (2). This is satisfied when the parameters are small in value (e.g. β ≪ 1) or when the unit is stimulated with lower intensity values, or a combination of both. In this case, we can approximate (2) through a first-order Taylor series expansion. Together with (1), we obtain

F=(I+δI)pm

where we have set kβ = 2. The values of these constants are not important for this discussion. We are now ready to derive equation (1).

For spontaneous activity, the input intensity is zero and m = δIp/2. At stimulus onset, the intensity has value I and m = δIp/2 through evaluation of equation (5) at t = 0. Finally, for steady-state, we evaluate (5) at t → ∞ to obtain m = (I + δI)p/2. This gives

SR=δIp/2PR=(I+δI)pδIp/2SS=(I+δI)p/2

from which we easily obtain equation (1). A similar method can be used to derive equation (2), as well as a corresponding equation for the inverted response.

Footnotes

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2021.727551/full#supplementary-material

References

  1. Adrian E. D., Zotterman Y. (1926a). The impulses produced by sensory nerve-endings: Part 2. The response of a single end-organ. J. Physiol. 61, 151–171. 10.1113/jphysiol.1926.sp002281 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adrian E. D., Zotterman Y. (1926b). The impulses produced by sensory nerve endings: Part 3. Impulses set up by touch and pressure. J. Physiol. 61, 465–483. 10.1113/jphysiol.1926.sp002308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barrio L., Buño W., Segundo J. (1988). Sensory-synaptic interactions in crayfish stretch receptor neurones. Biol. Cybernet. 59, 385–394. 10.1007/BF00336112 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baylin F. (1979). Temporal patterns and selectivity in the unitary responses of olfactory receptors in the tiger salamander to odor stimulation. J. Gen. Physiol. 74, 17–36. 10.1085/jgp.74.1.17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Benda J., Longtin A., Maler L. (2005). Spike-frequency adaptation separates transient communication signals from background oscillations. J. Neurosci. 25, 2312–2321. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4795-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bernays E., Chapman R., Hartmann T. (2002). A highly sensitive taste receptor cell for pyrrolizidine alkaloids in the lateral galeal sensillum of a polyphagous caterpillar, Estigmene acraea. J. Comp. Physiol. A 188, 715–723. 10.1007/s00359-002-0345-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Binder M. D., Hirokawa N., Windhorst U. (eds.) (2009). Adaptation of Sensory Receptors. Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bowdan E. (1984). Electrophysiological responses of tarsal contact chemoreceptors of the apple maggot fly rhagoletis pomonella to salt, sucrose and oviposition-deterrent pheromone. J. Comp. Physiol. A 154, 143–152. 10.1007/BF00605399 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boyd I., Roberts T. (1953). Proprioceptive discharges from stretch-receptors in the knee-joint of the cat. J. Physiol. 122, 38–58. 10.1113/jphysiol.1953.sp004977 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brown M., Stein R. (1966). Quantitative studies on the slowly adapting stretch receptor of the crayfish. Kybernetik 3, 175–185. 10.1007/BF00290253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chambers M. R., Andres K., Duering M., v, Iggo A. (1972). The structure and function of the slowly adapting type II mechanoreceptor in hairy skin. Q. J. Exp. Physiol. Cogn. Med. Sci. 57, 417–445. 10.1113/expphysiol.1972.sp002177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Davis E. E. (1976). A receptor sensitive to oviposition site attractants on the antennae of the mosquito, Aedes aegypti. J. Insect Physiol. 22, 1371–1376. 10.1016/0022-1910(76)90160-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. De Bruyne M., Clyne P. J., Carlson J. R. (1999). Odor coding in a model olfactory organ: the Drosophila maxillary palp. J. Neurosci. 19, 4520–4532. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04520.1999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dethier V., Bowdan E. (1984). Relations between differential threshold and sugar receptor mechanisms in the blowfly. Behav. Neurosci. 98:791. 10.1037/0735-7044.98.5.791 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Drew P. J., Abbott L. F. (2006). Models and properties of power-law adaptation in neural systems. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 826–833. 10.1152/jn.00134.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Eggermont J. J. (2015). Animal models of spontaneous activity in the healthy and impaired auditory system. Front. Neural Circ. 9:19. 10.3389/fncir.2015.00019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Enroth-Cugell C., Shapley R. (1973). Adaptation and dynamics of cat retinal ganglion cells. J. Physiol. 233, 271–309. 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fay R. R. (1985). Sound intensity processing by the goldfish. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 78, 1296–1309. 10.1121/1.392899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fernández C., Goldberg J. M. (1976). Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. I. Response to static tilts and to long-duration centrifugal force. J. Neurophysiol. 39, 970–984. 10.1152/jn.1976.39.5.970 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Friedrich R. W., Laurent G. (2004). Dynamics of olfactory bulb input and output activity during odor stimulation in zebrafish. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 2658–2669. 10.1152/jn.01143.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Garm A., Mori S. (2009). Multiple photoreceptor systems control the swim pacemaker activity in box jellyfish. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 3951–3960. 10.1242/jeb.031559 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Garson J. (2015). The birth of information in the brain: Edgar Adrian and the vacuum tube. Sci. Context 28, 31–52. 10.1017/S0269889714000313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gingl E., Hinterwirth A., Tichy H. (2005). Sensory representation of temperature in mosquito warm and cold cells. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 176–185. 10.1152/jn.01164.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gothilf S., Galun R., Bar-Zeev M. (1971). Taste reception in the mediterranean fruit fly: electrophysiological and behavioural studies. J. Insect Physiol. 17, 1371–1384. 10.1016/0022-1910(71)90201-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Handwerker H. O., Neher K.-D. (1976). Characteristics of c-fibre receptors in the cat's foot responding to stepwise increase of skin temperature to noxious levels. Pflügers Arch. 365, 221–229. 10.1007/BF01067022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Heinz M., Schäfer K., Braun H. A. (1990). Analysis of facial cold receptor activity in the rat. Brain Res. 521, 289–295. 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91554-T [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hildebrandt K. J., Benda J., Hennig R. M. (2009). The origin of adaptation in the auditory pathway of locusts is specific to cell type and function. J. Neurosci. 29, 2626–2636. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4800-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hopkins C. D. (1976). Stimulus filtering and electroreception: tuberous electroreceptors in three species of gymnotoid fish. J. Comp. Physiol. 111, 171–207. 10.1007/BF00605531 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Imaizumi K., Ruthazer E. S., MacLean J. N., Lee C. C. (eds.) (2018). Spontaneous Activity in the Sensory System. Laussane: Frontiers Media. 10.3389/978-2-88945-478-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Katsuki T., Greenspan R. J. (2013). Jellyfish nervous systems. Curr. Biol. 23, R592–R594. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kiang N. Y.-S. (1965). Discharge Patterns of Single Fibers in the Cat's Auditory Nerve. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kim A. J., Lazar A. A., Slutskiy Y. B. (2011). System identification of Drosophila olfactory sensory neurons. J. Comput. Neurosci. 30, 143–161. 10.1007/s10827-010-0265-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lange G. D., Hartline P. H. (1974). Retinal responses in squid and octopus. J. Comp. Physiol. 93, 19–36. 10.1007/BF00608757 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Loewenstein W. R. (1956). Excitation and changes in adaptation by stretch of mechanoreceptors. J. Physiol. 133, 588–602. 10.1113/jphysiol.1956.sp005611 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Maddess T., Laughlin S. B. (1985). Adaptation of the motion-sensitive neuron H1 is generated locally and governed by contrast frequency. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 225, 251–275. 10.1098/rspb.1985.0061 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Maes F., Harms G. (1986). Neural coding of salt taste quality in the blowfly Calliphora vicina J. Comp. Physiol. A 159, 75–88. 10.1007/BF00612498 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Martelli C., Carlson J. R., Emonet T. (2013). Intensity invariant dynamics and odor-specific latencies in olfactory receptor neuron response. J. Neurosci. 33, 6285–6297. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0426-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Matthews P., Stein R. (1969). The sensitivity of muscle spindle afferents to small sinusoidal changes of length. J. Physiol. 200, 723–743. 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Merivee E., Vanatoa A., Luik A., Rahi M., Sammelselg V., Ploomi A. (2003). Electrophysiological identification of cold receptors on the antennae of the ground beetle Pterostichus aethiops. Physiol. Entomol. 28, 88–96. 10.1046/j.1365-3032.2003.00320.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Milner E. S., Do M. T. H. (2017). A population representation of absolute light intensity in the mammalian retina. Cell 171, 865–876. 10.1016/j.cell.2017.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Mogdans J., Müller C., Frings M., Raap F. (2017). Adaptive responses of peripheral lateral line nerve fibres to sinusoidal wave stimuli. J. Comp. Physiol. A 203, 329–342. 10.1007/s00359-017-1172-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Norwich K. H. (1993). Information, Sensation, and Perception. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ohlemiller K. K., Siegel J. H. (1998). Temporal aspects of the effects of cooling on responses of single auditory nerve fibers. Hear. Res. 123, 78–86. 10.1016/S0378-5955(98)00105-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ridgel A., Frazier S., DiCaprio R., Zill S. (2000). Encoding of forces by cockroach tibial campaniform sensilla: implications in dynamic control of posture and locomotion. J. Comp. Physiol. A 186, 359–374. 10.1007/s003590050436 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sakmann B., Creutzfeldt O. D. (1969). Scotopic and mesopic light adaptation in the cat's retina. Pflügers Arch. 313, 168–185. 10.1007/BF00586245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schäfer K., Braun H. A., Kürten L. (1988). Analysis of cold and warm receptor activity in vampire bats and mice. Pflügers Arch. 412, 188–194. 10.1007/BF00583749 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schäfer K., Necker R., Braun H. A. (1989). Analysis of avian cold receptor function. Brain Res. 501, 66–72. 10.1016/0006-8993(89)91027-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Shimazaki H., Shinomoto S. (2007). A method for selecting the bin size of a time histogram. Neural Comput. 19, 1503–1527. 10.1162/neco.2007.19.6.1503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Smith D. V., Bealer S. L., Van Buskirk R. L. (1978). Adaptation and recovery of the rat chorda tympani response to NaCl. Physiol. Behav. 20, 629–636. 10.1016/0031-9384(78)90256-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Smith R. L., Zwislocki J. (1975). Short-term adaptation and incremental responses of single auditory-nerve fibers. Biol. Cybernet. 17, 169–182. 10.1007/BF00364166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sumner C. J., Lopez-Poveda E. A., O'Mard L. P., Meddis R. (2003). Adaptation in a revised inner-hair cell model. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113, 893–901. 10.1121/1.1515777 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sumner C. J., Palmer A. R. (2012). Auditory nerve fibre responses in the ferret. Eur. J. Neurosci. 36, 2428–2439. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08151.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Westerman L. A., Smith R. L. (1984). Rapid and short-term adaptation in auditory nerve responses. Hear. Res. 15, 249–260. 10.1016/0378-5955(84)90032-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Whewell W. (1840). The Philosophy of the Inductive Sciences, Founded on Their History, 1st Edn. London: West Strand. [Google Scholar]
  55. Wilson E. O. (1999). Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge. Vintage. [Google Scholar]
  56. Wong W. (2020). On the rate coding response of peripheral sensory neurons. Biol. Cybernet. 114, 609–619. 10.1007/s00422-020-00848-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Yates G. K., Robertson D., Johnstone B. M. (1985). Very rapid adaptation in the guinea pig auditory nerve. Hear. Res. 17, 1–12. 10.1016/0378-5955(85)90124-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Zilany M. S., Bruce I. C., Nelson P. C., Carney L. H. (2009). A phenomenological model of the synapse between the inner hair cell and auditory nerve: long-term adaptation with power-law dynamics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 126, 2390–2412. 10.1121/1.3238250 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.


Articles from Frontiers in Human Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES