Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 Oct 8;10:e73425. doi: 10.7554/eLife.73425

Intergenerational adaptations to stress are evolutionarily conserved, stress-specific, and have deleterious trade-offs

Nicholas O Burton 1,2,3,, Alexandra Willis 4, Kinsey Fisher 5, Fabian Braukmann 2, Jonathan Price 2, Lewis Stevens 6,7, L Ryan Baugh 5,8, Aaron Reinke 4, Eric A Miska 2,7,9
Editors: Diethard Tautz10, Patricia J Wittkopp11
PMCID: PMC8570697  PMID: 34622777

Abstract

Despite reports of parental exposure to stress promoting physiological adaptations in progeny in diverse organisms, there remains considerable debate over the significance and evolutionary conservation of such multigenerational effects. Here, we investigate four independent models of intergenerational adaptations to stress in Caenorhabditis elegans – bacterial infection, eukaryotic infection, osmotic stress, and nutrient stress – across multiple species. We found that all four intergenerational physiological adaptations are conserved in at least one other species, that they are stress -specific, and that they have deleterious tradeoffs in mismatched environments. By profiling the effects of parental bacterial infection and osmotic stress exposure on progeny gene expression across species, we established a core set of 587 genes that exhibited a greater than twofold intergenerational change in expression in response to stress in C. elegans and at least one other species, as well as a set of 37 highly conserved genes that exhibited a greater than twofold intergenerational change in expression in all four species tested. Furthermore, we provide evidence suggesting that presumed adaptive and deleterious intergenerational effects are molecularly related at the gene expression level. Lastly, we found that none of the effects we detected of these stresses on C. elegans F1 progeny gene expression persisted transgenerationally three generations after stress exposure. We conclude that intergenerational responses to stress play a substantial and evolutionarily conserved role in regulating animal physiology and that the vast majority of the effects of parental stress on progeny gene expression are reversible and not maintained transgenerationally.

Research organism: C. elegans

Introduction

Multigenerational effects of a parent’s environment on progeny have been reported to contribute to numerous organismal phenotypes and pathologies in species ranging from plants to mammals (Agrawal et al., 1999; Bozler et al., 2019; Burton et al., 2020; Burton et al., 2017; Dantzer et al., 2013; Dias and Ressler, 2014; Hibshman et al., 2016; Houri-Zeevi et al., 2020; Jordan et al., 2019; Kaletsky et al., 2020; Kishimoto et al., 2017; Klosin et al., 2017; Luna et al., 2012; Ma et al., 2019; Moore et al., 2019; Öst et al., 2014; Palominos et al., 2017; Posner et al., 2019; Veenendaal et al., 2013; Vellichirammal et al., 2017; Webster et al., 2018; Wibowo et al., 2016; Willis et al., 2021). These effects on progeny include many notable observations of intergenerational (lasting 1–2 generations) adaptive changes in phenotypically plastic traits such as the development of wings in pea aphids (Vellichirammal et al., 2017), helmet formation in Daphnia (Agrawal et al., 1999), accelerated growth rate in red squirrels (Dantzer et al., 2013), and physiological adaptations to osmotic stress and pathogen infection in both Arabidopsis (Luna et al., 2012; Wibowo et al., 2016) and Caenorhabditis elegans (Burton et al., 2020; Burton et al., 2017). These intergenerational adaptive changes in development and physiology, which include effects that are sometimes interchangeably described as parental effects, can lead to substantial increases in organismal survival, with up to 50-fold increases in the survival of offspring from stressed parents being reported when compared to the offspring from naive parents (Burton et al., 2020). While many of the most studied intergenerational effects of a parent’s environment on offspring have been identified in plants and invertebrates, intergenerational effects have also been reported in mammals (Dantzer et al., 2013; Dias and Ressler, 2014). Similar to findings in plants and invertebrates, some observations of intergenerational effects in mammals have been found to be physiologically adaptive (Dantzer et al., 2013), but many others, such as observations of fetal programming in humans (de Gusmão Correia et al., 2012; Langley-Evans, 2006; Schulz, 2010) and studies of the Dutch Hunger Winter (Veenendaal et al., 2013), have been reported to be deleterious. Nonetheless, even for these presumed deleterious intergenerational effects, it has been hypothesized that under different conditions the intergenerational effects of fetal programming, such as the effects caused by the Dutch Hunger Winter, might be considered physiologically adaptive (Hales and Barker, 2001; Hales and Barker, 1992).

If intergenerational responses to environmental stresses represent evolutionarily conserved processes, if they are general or stress-specific effects, and whether adaptive and deleterious intergenerational effects are molecularly related remains unknown. Furthermore, multiple different studies have recently reported that some environmental stresses elicit changes in progeny physiology and gene expression that persist for three or more generations, also known as transgenerational effects (Kaletsky et al., 2020; Klosin et al., 2017; Ma et al., 2019; Moore et al., 2019; Posner et al., 2019; Webster et al., 2018). However, if intergenerational effects (lasting 1–2 generations) and transgenerational effects (lasting 3+ generations) represent related or largely separable phenomena remains unclear. Answering these questions is critically important not only in understanding the role that multigenerational effects play in evolution, but also in understanding how such effects might contribute to multiple human pathologies that have been linked to the effects of a parent’s environment on offspring, such as Type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease (Langley-Evans, 2006).

Here, we investigated the evolutionary conservation, stress specificity, and potential tradeoffs of four independent models of intergenerational adaptations to stress in C. elegans – bacterial infection, eukaryotic infection, nutrient stress, and osmotic stress. We found that all four models of intergenerational adaptive effects are conserved in at least one other species, but that all exhibited a different pattern of evolutionary conservation. Each intergenerational adaptive effect was stress -specific and multiple intergenerational adaptive effects exhibited deleterious tradeoffs in mismatched environments or environments where multiple stresses were present simultaneously. By profiling the effects of multiple different stresses on offspring gene expression across species we identified a set of 37 genes that exhibited intergenerational changes in gene expression in response to stress in all species tested. In addition, we found that an inversion in the expression of a key gene involved in the intergenerational response to bacterial infection, rhy-1, from increased expression to decreased expression in the offspring of stressed parents, correlates with an inversion of an adaptive intergenerational response to bacterial infection in C. elegans and C. kamaaina to a deleterious intergenerational effect in C. briggsae. Lastly, we report that none of the effects of multiple different stresses on F1 gene expression that we detected here persisted transgenerationally into F3 progeny in C. elegans. Our findings demonstrate that intergenerational adaptive responses to stress are evolutionarily conserved, stress -specific, and are predominantly not maintained transgenerationally. In addition, our findings suggest that the mechanisms that mediate intergenerational adaptive responses in some species might be related to the mechanisms that mediate intergenerational deleterious effects in other species.

Results

Intergenerational adaptations to stress are evolutionarily conserved

To test if any of the intergenerational adaptations to stress that have been reported in C. elegans are evolutionarily conserved in other species we focused on four recently described intergenerational adaptations to abiotic and biotic stresses – osmotic stress (Burton et al., 2017), nutrient stress (Hibshman et al., 2016; Jordan et al., 2019), Pseudomonas vranonvensis infection (bacterial) (Burton et al., 2020), and Nematocida parisii infection (eukaryotic – microsporidia) (Willis et al., 2021). All of these stresses are exclusively intergenerational and did not persist beyond two generations in any experimental setup previously analyzed (Burton et al., 2017; Burton et al., 2020; Willis et al., 2021). We tested if these four intergenerational adaptive responses were conserved in four different species of Caenorhabditis (C. briggsae, C. elegans, C. kamaaina, and C. tropicalis) that shared a last common ancestor approximately 30 million years ago and have diverged to the point of having approximately 0.05 substitutions per site at the nucleotide level (Figure 1A; Cutter, 2008). These species were chosen because they represent multiple independent branches of the Elegans group (Figure 1A) and because we could probe the conservation of underlying mechanisms using established genetics approaches.

Figure 1. Intergenerational adaptations to multiple stresses are evolutionarily conserved in multiple species of Caenorhabditis.

(A) Phylogenetic tree of the Elegans group of Caenorhabditis species adapted from Stevens et al., 2020. Scale represents substitutions per site. (B) Percent of wild-type C. elegans (N2), C. kamaaina (QG122), C. briggsae (AF16), and C. tropicalis (JU1373) animals surviving after 24 hr on plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3–4 experiments of >100 animals. (C) Percent of C. kamaaina wild-type (QG122) animals surviving after 24 hr of exposure to P. vranovensis. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (D) Percent of wild-type animals mobile and developing at 500 mM NaCl after 24 hr. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (E) Percent of wild-type and Cbr-gpdh-2(syb2973) mutant C. briggsae (AF16) mobile and developing after 24 hr at 500 mM NaCl. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (F) Percent of animals exhibiting detectable infection by N. parisii as determined by DY96 staining after 72 hr for C. elegans and C. briggsae, or 96 hr for C. kamaaina and C. tropicalis. Data presented as mean values ± s.e.m. n = 3–4 experiments of 83–202 animals. (G) Boxplots for length of L1 progeny from P0 parents that were subject to the HB101 dose series. Larvae were measured using Wormsizer. Boxplots show median length with four quartiles. n = 3–8 experiments of 50–200 animals. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.0001, ****p < 0.0001.

Figure 1—source data 1. Statistics source data for Figure 1.
elife-73425-fig1-data1.xlsx (745.1KB, xlsx)

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Intergenerational responses to environmental stress are conserved in wild isolates of Caenorhabditis species.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Percent of wild-type C. elegans (N2) and C. briggsae (JU1348) animals surviving after 24 hr on plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (B) Percent of wild-type C. elegans (N2) and C. briggsae (SX1125) animals surviving after 24 hr on plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (C) Percent of wild-type C. elegans isolates surviving after 24 hr on plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (D) Percent of wild-type C. kamaaina animals mobile and developing at 500 mM NaCl after 24 hr. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (E–G) Percent of animals exhibiting detectable infection by N. parisii as determined by DY96 staining after 72 hr for C. elegans and C. briggsae, or 96 hr for C. kamaaina and C. tropicalis. Data presented as mean values ± s.e.m. n = 3 experiments of 68–115 animals (E), 27–102 animals (F), and 38–104 animals ( G). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.

We exposed parents of all four species to P. vranovensis and subsequently studied their offspring’s survival rate in response to future P. vranovensis exposure. We found that parental exposure to the bacterial pathogen P. vranovensis protected offspring from future infection in both C. elegans and C. kamaaina (Figure 1B) and that this adaptive intergenerational effect in C. kamaaina required the same stress response genes (cysl-1 and rhy-1) as previously reported for C. elegans (Burton et al., 2020; Figure 1C), indicating that these animals intergenerationally adapt to infection via a similar and potentially conserved mechanism. By contrast, we found that naive C. briggsae animals were more resistant to P. vranovensis than any of the other species tested, but exposure of C. briggsae parents to P. vranovensis caused greater than 99% of offspring to die upon future exposure to P. vranovensis (Figure 1B). We confirmed that parental P. vranovensis exposure resulted in an adaptive intergenerational effect for C. elegans but a deleterious intergenerational effect for C. briggsae by testing multiple additional wild isolates of both species (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A-C). Parental exposure to P. vranovensis had no observable effect on offspring response to infection in C. tropicalis (Figure 1B). We conclude that parental exposure to P. vranovensis causes substantial changes in offspring susceptibility to future P. vranovensis exposure in multiple species, but whether those effects are protective or deleterious for offspring is species-dependent.

Using a similar approach to investigate intergenerational adaptive responses to other stresses, we found that parental exposure to mild osmotic stress protected offspring from future osmotic stress in C. elegans, C. briggsae, and C. kamaaina, but again not in C. tropicalis (Figure 1D). This intergenerational adaptation to osmotic stress in C. briggsae and C. kamaaina required the glycerol-3-phosphate dehydrogenase gpdh-2 (Figure 1E and Figure 1—figure supplement 1D), similar to previous observations for C. elegans (Burton et al., 2017) and indicating that these adaptations are regulated by similar and likely evolutionarily conserved mechanisms.

We then sought to test if intergenerational resistance to infection by the eukaryotic pathogen N. parisii is similarly conserved in Caenorhabditis species. N. parisii is a common natural pathogen of both C. elegans and C. briggsae (Zhang et al., 2016). Here, we show that N. parisii can also infect C. kamaaina and C. tropicalis (Figure 1—figure supplement 1E-G). By investigating the effects of parental N. parisii infection on offspring across species, we found that parental exposure of C. elegans and C. briggsae to N. parisii protected offspring from future infection (Figure 1F). By contrast, parental exposure of C. kamaaina and C. tropicalis to N. parisii had no observable effect on offspring infection rate (Figure 1F).

Lastly, we investigated the intergenerational effects of nutrient stress on offspring. We found that parental nutrient stress by food deprivation resulted in larger offspring in both C. elegans and C. tropicalis, which is predicted to be adaptive (Hibshman et al., 2016), but had minimal effects on offspring size in C. briggsae and C. kamaaina (Figure 1G). Collectively, our findings indicate that all four reported intergenerational adaptive effects in C. elegans are conserved in at least one other species but all four show a different pattern of conservation, which is consistent with each response being regulated by distinct molecular mechanisms (Burton et al., 2020; Burton et al., 2017; Hibshman et al., 2016; Jordan et al., 2019; Willis et al., 2021).

Parental exposure to environmental stresses leads to common and stress-specific gene expression changes in offspring across species

Of the four intergenerational models investigated here, parental exposure of C. elegans to osmotic stress and P. vranovensis infection were previously reported to cause substantial changes in offspring gene expression, including the increased expression of genes that are required for the observed intergenerational adaptations (Burton et al., 2020; Burton et al., 2017). These effects of parental stress exposure on offspring gene expression resemble a subset of the transcriptional stress response observed in parental animals and could potentially prime offspring to respond to the same stress (Burton et al., 2020). Here, we exposed C. elegans, C. briggsae, C. kamaaina, and C. tropicalis to either osmotic stress or P. vranovensis infection and subsequently performed RNA-seq on offspring to test: (1) if the specific heritable changes in gene expression in response to each of these stresses are conserved across species and (2) if any changes in gene expression correlate with the phenotypic differences in intergenerational responses to stress we observed in the different species. This analysis allowed us to compare the effects of parental stress on offspring gene expression of 7587 single-copy orthologs that are conserved across all four species (Supplementary file 1).

Consistent with previous observations in C. elegans, we found that parental exposure to P. vranovensis resulted in substantial changes in offspring gene expression in all four species we investigated (Figure 2 and Supplementary file 2). Of the 7587 single-copy orthologs shared between the four species, we identified 367 genes that exhibited a greater than twofold change in expression in the offspring of infected animals in C. elegans (padj <0.01) and at least one other species (Figure 2D and Supplementary file 2). Furthermore, we found that 35 genes exhibited a greater than twofold change in expression (padj <0.01) in the offspring of parents exposed to P. vranovensis in all four species (Figure 2D and Table 1). These data indicate that parental exposure to the bacterial pathogen P. vranovensis leads to changes in offspring gene expression at a common set of stress–response genes in diverse species of Caenorhabditis.

Figure 2. Parental exposure to P. vranovensis and osmotic stress have overlapping effects on offspring gene expression across multiple species.

(A) Average fold change of 7587 single-copy ortholog genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans and C. briggsae parents fed P. vranovensis BIGb0446 when compared to parents fed E. coli HB101. Average fold change from three replicates. Red dots represent genes that exhibit >twofold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in both species. (B) Average fold change of 7587 single-copy ortholog genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans and C. kamaaina parents fed P. vranovensis BIGb0446 when compared to parents fed E. coli HB101. Average fold change from three replicates. Red dots represent genes that exhibit >twofold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in both species. (C) Average fold change of 7587 single-copy ortholog genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans and C. tropicalis parents fed P. vranovensis BIGb0446 when compared to parents fed E. coli HB101. Average fold change from three replicates. Red dots represent genes that exhibit > twofold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in both species (D) Venn diagram of the number of genes that exhibit overlapping >2 fold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in F1 progeny of animals exposed to P. vranovensis BIGb0446 in each species. (E) Normalized counts of reads matching orthologs of rhy-1 in the F1 offspring of parents fed either E. coli HB101 or P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Data from Supplementary file 2. n = 3 replicates. (F) Average fold change of 7587 single-copy ortholog genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans and C. briggsae parents grown at 300 mM NaCl when compared to parents grown at 50 mM NaCl. Average fold change from three replicates. Red dots represent genes that exhibit >twofold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in both species. (G) Average fold change of 7587 single-copy ortholog genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans and C. kamaaina parents grown at 300 mM NaCl when compared to parents grown at 50 mM NaCl. Average fold change from three replicates. Red dots represent genes that exhibit >twofold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in both species in both species. (H) Average fold change of 7587 single-copy ortholog genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans and C. tropicalis parents grown at 300 mM NaCl when compared to parents grown at 50 mM NaCl. Average fold change from three replicates. Red dots represent genes exhibit >twofold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in both species. (I) Venn diagram of the number of genes that exhibit overlapping >twofold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in F1 progeny of animals grown at 300 mM NaCl in each species. (J) Normalized counts of reads matching orthologs of gpdh-1 in the F1 progeny of parents grown at either 300 mM NaCl or 50 mM NaCl. Data from Supplementary file 3. n = 3 replicates. (K) Average fold change for 7587 ortholog genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans parents fed P. vranovensis or exposed to 300 mM NaCl when compared to naive parents. Average fold change from three replicates. Red dots – genes that change in expression in response to both stresses. Blue dots – genes that change in expression in response to only osmotic stress. Green dots – genes that change in expression in response to only P. vranovensis. (L) Average fold change of 7512 single-copy ortholog genes in F3 progeny of C. elegans and C. kamaaina fed P. vranovensis BIGb0446 when compared to those fed E. coli HB101. Average fold change from three replicates. Blue dots represent genes that exhibited >twofold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in C. elegans. Green dots represent genes that exhibited >twofold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in C. kamaaina. (M) Average fold change of 7512 single-copy ortholog genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans and C. kamaaina parents grown at 300 mM NaCl when compared to parents grown at 50 mM NaCl. Average fold change from three replicates. Green dots represent genes that exhibited >twofold (padj <0.01) changes in expression in C. kamaaina. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.0001, ****p < 0.0001.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Differences in developmental timing are insufficient to explain a majority of the observed differences in gene expression in the offspring of stressed parents.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) PCA of gene expression from Boeck et al., 2016 compared to RNA-seq data reported in the study. Time points of development are in minutes, t60 = 60 min postfertilization. (B) Percentage of genes differentially expressed in the offspring of parents exposed to different stresses that exhibit DESeq2 normalized counts that fall within or outside one standard deviation of the average normalized counts observed throughout all developmental time points from Boeck et al., 2016. (C) Total number of genes differentially expressed in the offspring of parents exposed to different stresses that exhibit DESeq2 normalized counts that fall within or outside one standard deviation of the average normalized counts observed throughout all developmental time points from Boeck et al., 2016. (D) rhy-1 normalized counts from all time points during development from Boeck et al., 2016, the offspring of parents exposed to E. coli HB101 (this study), or the offspring of parents exposed to P. vranovensis (this study). (E) cysl-2 normalized counts from all time points during development from Boeck et al., 2016, the offspring of parents exposed to E. coli HB101 (this study), or the offspring of parents exposed to P. vranovensis (this study).

Table 1. Complete list of genes that exhibited a greater than twofold change in expression in the F1 progeny of parents exposed to P. vranovensis or osmotic stress in all four species tested.

Genes that change in F1 progeny of all species exposed to P. vranovensis
Predicted function
C18A11.1 Unknown
R13A1.5 Unknown
D1053.3 Unknown
pmp-5 ATP-binding activity and ATPase-coupled transmembrane transporter activity, ortholog of human ABCD4
C39E9.8 Unknown
nit-1 Nitrilase ortholog – predicted to enable hydrolase activity
lips-10 Lipase related
srr-6 Serpentine receptor, class R
Y51B9A.6 Predicted to enable transmembrane transporter activity
gst-33 Glutathione S-transferase
ptr-8 Patched domain containing, ortholog of human PTCHD1, PTCHD3, and PTCHD4
ZC443.1 Predicted to enable D-threo-aldose 1-dehydrogenase activity
cri-2 Conserved regulator of innate immunity, ortholog of human TIMP2
Y42G9A.3 Unknown
ttr-21 Transthyretin-related, involved in response to Gram-negative bacteria
F45E4.5 Involved in defense against Gram-negative bacteria
C42D4.1 Domain of unknown function DUF148
asp-14 Predicted to enable aspartic-type endopeptidase activity. Involved in innate immune response
cyp-32B1 Cytochrome P450 family. Ortholog of human CYP4V2
nas-10 Predicted to enable metalloendopeptidase activity and zinc ion-binding activity
W01F3.2 Unknown
nhr-11 Nuclear hormone receptor
F26G1.2 Unknown
F48E3.2 Predicted to enable transmembrane transporter activity
hpo-26 Unknown, hypersensitive to pore forming toxin
R05H10.1 Unknown
C08E8.4 Involved in innate immune response
C11G10.1 Unknown
Y73F4A.2 Unknown, DOMON domain containing
bigr-1 Predicted to enable hydrolase activity
nlp-33 Neuropeptide like, involved in innate immune response
far-3 Predicted to enable lipid-binding activity
Genes that change in F1 progeny of all species exposed to both osmotic stress and P. vranovensis
C30B5.6 Unknown
hphd-1 Predicted to enable hydroxyacid–oxoacid transhydrogenase activity. Ortholog of human ADHFE1
C42D4.3 Unknown, DB module and domain of unknown function DB
Genes that change in F1 progeny of all species exposed to osmotic stress
ttr-15 Transthyretin-like family
F08F3.4 Predicted to enable catalytic activity. Involved in innate immune response.Ortholog of human TDH

We performed the same analysis on the offspring of all four species from parents exposed to osmotic stress. From this analysis, we observed that parental exposure to osmotic stress resulted in 235 genes exhibiting differential expression in both C. elegans and C. briggsae offspring (Figure 2F–J and Supplementary file 3). In addition, we found that these changes in gene expression were largely distinct from the gene expression changes observed in the offspring of parents exposed to P. vranovensis (Figure 2K and Supplementary files 2 and 3), indicating that different parental stresses have distinct effects on offspring gene expression. However, parental exposure to C. kamaaina and C. tropicalis to osmotic stress resulted in approximately fivefold fewer changes in offspring gene expression (Figure 2G–H and Supplementary file 3). In total five genes (C30B5.6, hphd-1, C42D4.3, ttr-15, and F08F3.4) exhibited differential expression in the offspring of parents exposed to osmotic stress in all four species (Figure 2I and Table 1) and three of these five (C30B5.6, hphd-1, and C42D4.3) were also observed to change in the offspring of parents exposed to P. vranovensis (Table 1).

Pairing gene expression and phenotypic data across species significantly enriches for genes required for intergenerational adaptations

To further probe how parental exposure to environmental stresses affects offspring gene expression, we first analyzed the gene ontology of the 37 genes that exhibit changes in expression in the offspring of stressed parents in all four species using g:Profiler (Raudvere et al., 2019). We found that these 37 genes were significantly enriched for extracellular proteins (p < 2.278 × 10–3). However, no additional commonalities were identified and none of these 37 genes have previously been linked to adaptations to P. vranovensis infection or osmotic stress.

We found that different species exhibit different intergenerational responses to both P. vranovensis infection and osmotic stress (Figure 1). We hypothesized that the effects of parental exposure to environmental stresses on offspring gene expression might correlate with how offspring phenotypically respond to stress. Parental exposure of C. elegans and C. kamaaina to P. vranovensis led to increased progeny resistance to future P. vranovensis exposure (Figure 1B). By contrast, parental exposure of C. briggsae to P. vranovensis led to increased offspring susceptibility to P. vranovensis (Figure 1B). We hypothesized that differences in the expression of genes previously reported to be required for adaptation to P. vranovensis, such as the acyltransferase rhy-1, might underlie these differences between species. We therefore investigated whether any genes exhibited specific changes in expression in C. elegans and C. kamaaina that were either absent or inverted in C. briggsae. We found that of the 562 genes that exhibited a greater than twofold change in expression in the offspring of parents exposed to P. vranovensis in C. elegans, only 54 also exhibited a greater than twofold intergenerational change in expression in C. kamaaina (Supplementary file 2). From this refined list of 54 genes, 17 genes either did not exhibit a change in C. briggsae or changed in the opposite direction (Table 2).

Table 2. Complete list of genes that exhibited a consistent and greater than twofold change in expression in the F1 progeny of parents exposed to P. vranovensisor osmotic stress in only species that intergenerationally adapted to stress.

Genes listed for P. vranovensis were identified by comparing genes that change consistently in C. elegans and C. kamaaina, but not C. briggsae. Genes listed for osmotic stress were identified by comparing genes that change consistently in C.elegans, C. briggsae, and C. kamaaina, but not in C. tropicalis. Bold font indicates genes that have already been demonstrated to be involved in C. elegans adaption to these stresses.

Genes that change consistently in F1 progeny of only species that adapt to P. vranovensis Predicted function
daf-18 Lipid phosphatase, homologous to human PTEN tumor suppressor
gst-38 Glutathione S-transferase
H04M03.3 Predicted to enable oxidoreductase activity.
oops-1 Oocyte partner of SPE-11
F09G8.10 Unknown
glb-1 Globin -related
F57H12.6 Unknown
elo-6 Predicted to enable transferase activity, transferring acyl groups, ortholog of human ELOVL3 and ELOVL6
cpr-5 Predicted to enable cysteine-type peptidase activity
xpo-2 Exportin involved in nuclear export, ortholog of human CSE1L
cysl-1 Cysteine synthase known to be involved in adaptation to P. vranovensis
rhy-1 Regulator of HIF-1 known to be involved in adaptation to P. vranovensis
cdc-25.1 Homolog of human CDC25 phosphatase
imb-1 Importin beta family, ortholog of human KPNB1
VZK882L.2 Unknown
cysl-2 Cysteine synthase known to be involved in adaptation to P. vranovensis
cyk-7 Involved in intercellular bridge organization
Genes that change consistently in F1 progeny of only species that adapt to osmotic stress
T05F1.9 Unknown
grl-21 Unknown, Ground-like domain containing
gpdh-1 Glycerol-3-phosphate dehydrogenase known to be involved in osmotic stress resposne
T22B7.3 Amidinotransferase, ortholog of human DDAH1 and DDAH2

Consistent with our hypothesis that intergenerational gene expression changes across species might correlate with their phenotypic responses, we found that all three genes previously reported to be required for the intergenerational adaptation to P. vranovensis (rhy-1, cysl-1, and cysl-2 – Burton et al., 2020) were among the 17 genes that exhibited differential expression in C. elegans and C. kamaaina but not in C. briggsae. This overlap is significantly above what is expected by chance (P < 1.337e−−08 – hypergeometric probability). We conclude that the effects of parental exposure to P. vranovensis on offspring gene expression correlate with their phenotypic response. Furthermore, we propose that this new list of 17 genes (Table 2) is likely to be enriched in additional conserved genes required for this intergenerational response to pathogen infection. This list includes multiple highly conserved genes including multiple factors involved in nuclear transport (imb-1 and xpo-2), the CDC25 phosphatase ortholog cdc-25.1, and the PTEN tumor suppressor ortholog daf-18.

Notably, of the revised list of 17 genes, we identified a single gene that exhibited a greater than twofold increase in expression in C. elegans and C. kamaaina F1 progeny but had an inverted greater than twofold decrease in expression in C. briggsae F1 progeny. That gene is rhy-1 (Figure 2E), one of the three genes known to be required for animals to intergenerationally adapt to P. vranovensis infection (Burton et al., 2020). This directional change of rhy-1 expression in progeny of animals exposed to P. vranovensis correlates with the observation that parental exposure to P. vranovensis results in enhanced pathogen resistance in offspring in C. elegans and C. kamaaina but has a strong deleterious effect on pathogen resistance in C. briggsae (Figure 1B). Collectively, these findings suggest that molecular mechanisms underlying adaptive and deleterious effects in different species might be related and dependent on the direction of changes in gene expression of specific stress–response genes.

We performed the same analysis pairing our transcriptional data with our phenotypic data for the intergenerational response to osmotic stress. We found that C. elegans, C. briggsae, and C. kamaaina intergenerationally adapted to osmotic stress, but C. tropicalis did not (Figure 1D). We therefore identified genes that were differentially expressed in the F1 offspring of C. elegans, C. briggsae, and C. kamaaina exposed to osmotic stress, but not in C. tropicalis. From this analysis, we identified four genes (T05F1.9, grl-21, gpdh-1, and T22B7.3) that are specifically differentially expressed in the three species that adapt to osmotic stress but not in C. tropicalis (Table 2); this list of genes includes the glycerol-3-phosphate dehydrogenase gpdh-1 which is one of the most upregulated genes in response to osmotic stress and is known to be required for animals to properly respond to osmotic stress (Lamitina et al., 2006). These results suggest that, similar to our observations for P. vranovensis infection, different patterns in the expression of known osmotic stress response genes correlate with different intergenerational phenotypic responses to osmotic stress.

Differences in gene expression in the offspring of stressed parents could be due to programmed changes in expression in response to stress or due to indirect effects caused by changes in developmental timing. To confirm that the embryos from all conditions were collected at the same developmental stage we compared our RNA-seq findings to a time-resolved transcriptome of C. elegans development (Boeck et al., 2016). Consistent with our visual observations that a vast majority of offspring collected were in the comma stage of embryo development, we found that the gene expression profiles of all offspring from both naive and stressed parents overlapped strongly with the 330- to 450-min time points of development (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). In addition, we found that approximately 50% of all genes that were differentially expressed in the offspring of stressed parents when compared to naive parents exhibited a change in gene expression that was more than one standard deviation outside their average expression across all time points of embryo development (Figure 2—figure supplement 1B-C). We similarly found that many of the genes known to be required for intergenerational responses to stress exhibit expression that is outside the range of expression observed at any time point of early development (Figure 2—figure supplement 1D-E). These results suggest that a majority of the expression differences we observed in the offspring of stressed parents were not due to differences in developmental timing.

The effects of parental bacterial infection and osmotic stress on offspring gene expression are not maintained transgenerationally

Determining whether the effects of parental exposure to stress on offspring gene expression are reversible after one generation or if any changes in gene expression persist transgenerationally is a critical and largely unanswered question in the field of multigenerational effects. To test if any of the intergenerational changes in gene expression that we observed persist transgenerationally, we performed RNA-seq of F3 progeny of C. elegans exposed to both P. vranovensis and osmotic stress. We found that none of the 1515 genes that exhibited differential expression in F1 progeny for either P. vranovensis infection or osmotic stress were also differentially expressed in C. elegans F3 progeny (Figure 2L and M and Supplementary file 4). We conclude that, at minimum, the vast majority of intergenerational effects of these stresses on gene expression in C. elegans do not persist transgenerationally.

We hypothesized that transgenerational effects on gene expression could potentially be more robust in other species. We therefore performed the same analysis on F3 gene expression in response to both P. vranovensis infection and osmotic stress in a second species that intergenerationally adapts to both stresses, C. kamaaina. We again found that none of the genes that exhibited differential expression in F1 progeny of parents exposed to P. vranovensis were also differentially expressed in F3 progeny (Figure 2L and Supplementary file 4). We did, however, identify two genes, the C. kamaaina orthologs of C. elegans hphd-1 and C09B8.4, that exhibited differential expression in both the F1 and F3 progeny of parents exposed to osmotic stress (Figure 2M and Supplementary file 4). It is possible that these two genes represent true transgenerational effects on gene expression, but given that these effects were not also observed in C. elegans and that only two genes were identified out of thousands of possible gene comparisons using a false discovery cutoff of 1 %, we cannot rule out that these two genes are false positives. Collectively, our results suggest that neither of these biotic or abiotic stresses that elicit robust intergenerational changes in gene expression cause similar transgenerational changes in gene expression under the same conditions in multiple different species. We note, however, that it remains possible that transgenerational effects of these stresses could persist through other mechanisms, could affect the expression of genes that are not clearly conserved between species, or could exert weaker effects on broad classes of genes that would not be detectable at any specific individual loci as was reported for the transgenerational effects of starvation and loss of COMPASS complex function on gene expression in C. elegans (Greer et al., 2011; Webster et al., 2018). Furthermore, it is possible that transgenerational effects on gene expression in C. elegans are restricted to germ cells (Buckley et al., 2012; Houri-Zeevi et al., 2020; Posner et al., 2019) or to a small number of cells and are not detectable when profiling gene expression in somatic tissue from whole animals.

Intergenerational responses to stress can have deleterious tradeoffs

Intergenerational changes in animal physiology that protect offspring from future exposure to stress could be stress-specific or could converge on a broadly stress-resistant state. If intergenerational adaptive effects are stress-specific, then it is expected that parental exposure to a given stress will protect offspring from that same stress but potentially come at the expense of fitness in mismatched environments. If intergenerational adaptations to stress converge on a generally more stress-resistant state, then parental exposure to one stress might protect offspring against many different types of stress. To determine if the intergenerational effects we investigated here represent specific or general responses, we assayed how parental C. elegans exposure to osmotic stress, P. vranovensis infection, and N. parisii infection, either alone or in combination, affected offspring responses to mismatched stresses. We found that parental exposure to P. vranovensis did not affect the ability of animals to intergenerationally adapt to osmotic stress (Figure 3A). By contrast, parental exposure to osmotic stress completely eliminated the ability of animals to intergenerationally adapt to P. vranovensis (Figure 3B). This effect is unlikely to be due to the effects of osmotic stress on P. vranovensis itself, as mutant animals that constitutively activate the osmotic stress response (osm-8) were also completely unable to adapt to P. vranovensis infection (Figure 3C; Rohlfing et al., 2011). We conclude that animals’ intergenerational responses to P. vranovensis and osmotic stress are stress-specific, consistent with our observation that parental exposure to these two stresses resulted in distinct changes in offspring gene expression (Figure 2K).

Figure 3. Intergenerational adaptations to stress are stress-specific and have deleterious tradeoffs.

Figure 3.

(A) Percent of wild-type C. elegans mobile and developing at 500 mM NaCl after 24 hr. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (B) Percent of wild-type C. elegans surviving after 24 hr of exposure to P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (C) Percent of wild-type and osm-8(n1518) C. elegans surviving after 24 hr of exposure to P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (D) Percent of wild-type C. elegans mobile and developing at 420 mM NaCl after 48 hr. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (E) N. parisii parasite burden of individual C. elegans after 72 hr (as determined by percentage fluorescence from DY96-stained spores after 72 hr). Data presented as mean values ± s.e.m. n = 4 experiments of 25 animals (F) N. parisii parasite burden of individual C. elegans after 72 hr (as determined by percentage fluorescence from DY96-stained spores after 72 hr). Data presented as mean values ± s.e.m. n = 3 experiments of 25 animals. (G) Percent of wild-type C. elegans surviving after 24 hr of exposure to P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Data presented as mean values ± s.e.m. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.0001, ****p < 0.0001.

Figure 3—source data 1. Statistics source data for Figure 3.

We performed a similar analysis comparing animals’ intergenerational response to osmotic stress and the eukaryotic pathogen N. parisii. We previously reported that L1 parental infection with N. parisii results in progeny that is more sensitive to osmotic stress (Willis et al., 2021). Here, we found that L4 parental exposure of C. elegans to N. parisii had a small, but not significant effect on offspring response to osmotic stress (Figure 3D). However, similar to our observations for osmotic stress and bacterial infection, we found that parental exposure to both osmotic stress and N. parisii infection simultaneously resulted in offspring that were less protected against future N. parisii infection than when parents are exposed to N. parisii alone (Figure 3E). Collectively, these data further support the conclusion that intergenerational responses to infection and osmotic stress are stress-specific and suggest that intergenerational adaptations to osmotic stress might come at the expense of animals’ ability to properly respond to bacterial or eukaryotic infections when either is paired with osmotic stress.

To compare animals’ intergenerational responses to bacterial infection and eukaryotic infection, we performed a similar comparative analysis. We found that parental exposure to P. vranovnesis had no observable effect on offspring response to N. parisii either alone or when both pathogens were present simultaneously (Figure 1F). Similarly, we found that parental exposure to N. parisii had no observable effect on offspring response to P. vranovensis either alone or when both pathogens were present at the same time (Figure 1G). We conclude that intergenerational adaptations to osmotic stress, P. vranovensis infection and N. parisii infection are largely stress-specific.

Intergenerational responses to Pseudomonas pathogens are distinct from other bacterial pathogens

To further probe the specificity of intergenerational responses to stress, we also sought to determine if the substantial changes in pathogen resistance and gene expression observed in C. elegans offspring from parents exposed to the bacterial pathogen P. vranovensis were specific to this pathogen or part of a general response to bacterial pathogens. We previously found that the transcriptional response to P. vranovensis in F1 progeny is distinct from the response to P. aeruginosa (Burton et al., 2020). To further probe the specificity of this intergenerational response, we first screened wild bacterial isolates from France (Samuel et al., 2016) and the United Kingdom (Supplementary file 5) for those that are potential natural pathogens of C. elegans and that also intergenerationally affect C. elegans survival or growth rate. From this analysis, we identified a new Pseudomonas isolate, Pseudomonas sp. 15C5, where parental exposure to Pseudomonas sp. 15C5 enhanced offspring growth rate in response to future exposure to Pseudomonas sp. 15C5 (Figure 4A). This intergenerational effect resembled C. elegans intergenerational adaptation to P. vranovensis and we found that parental exposure to either isolate of Pseudomonas protected offspring from future exposure to the other Pseudomonas isolate (Figure 4A–B). To test if Pseudomonas sp. 15C5 was a new isolate of P. vranovensis or a distinct species of Pseudomonas, we performed both 16S rRNA sequencing and sequenced the gene rpoD of Pseudomonas sp. 15C5. From this analysis, we found that Pseudomonas sp. 15C5 is not an isolate of P. vranovensis and is most similar to Pseudomonas putida – 99.49% identical 16S rRNA and 98.86% identical rpoD by BLAST (Supplementary file 6). These results indicate that parental exposure to multiple different Pseudomonas species can protect offspring from future pathogen exposure. We note, however, that other pathogenic species of Pseudomonas, such as P. aeruginosa, did not cross protect against P. vranovensis (Burton et al., 2020), indicating that not all pathogenic species of Pseudomonas result in the same intergenerational in offspring pathogen resistance.

Figure 4. Many of the intergenerational effects of parental exposure to bacterial pathogens on offspring gene expression are pathogen specific.

(A) Percent of wild-type C. elegans that developed to the L4 larval stage after 48 hr of feeding on Pseudomonas sp. 15C5. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (B) Percent of wild-type C. elegans surviving after 24 hr of exposure to P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (C) Percent of wild-type C. elegans that developed to the L4 larval stage after 48 hr of feeding on Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (D) Percent of wild-type C. elegans that developed to the L4 larval stage after 48 hr of feeding on Serratia plymuthica BUR1537. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. (E) Average fold change of genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans fed either Pseudomonas sp. 15C5 or P. vranovensis BIGb0446 when compared to parents fed E. coli HB101. Average fold change from three replicates. Red dots represent genes that exhibit statistically significant (padj <0.01) changes in the F1 offspring of parents fed both Pseudomonas sp. 15C5 and P. vranovensis BIGb0446. (F) Average fold change of genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans fed either Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469 or P. vranovensis BIGb0446 when compared to parents fed E. coli HB101. Average fold change from three replicates. Red dots represent genes that exhibit statistically significant (padj <0.01) changes in the F1 offspring of parents fed both Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469 and P. vranovensis BIGb0446. (G) Average fold change of genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans fed either S. plymuthica BUR1537 or P. vranovensis BIGb0446 when compared to parents fed E. coli HB101. Average fold change from three replicates. Red dots represent genes that exhibit statistically significant (padj <0.01) changes in the F1 offspring of parents fed both S. plymuthica BUR1537 and P. vranovensis BIGb0446. (H) Venn diagram of the number of genes that exhibit overlapping statistically significant (padj <0.01) changes in expression in F1 progeny of C. elegans parents fed each different bacterial species. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.0001.

Figure 4—source data 1. Statistics source data for Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Parental exposure to Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469 and S. plymuthica BUR1537 does not protect offspring from P. vranovensis.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Percent of wild-type C. elegans (N2) animals surviving after 24 hr on plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Data presented as mean values ± s.d. n = 3 experiments of >100 animals. * p < 0.05, **** p < 0.0001.

In addition to these intergenerational adaptive effects, we also identified two bacterial isolates that activate pathogen–response pathways, Serretia plymutica BUR1537 and Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469 (Samuel et al., 2016; Hellberg et al., 2015), that resulted in intergenerational deleterious effects (Figure 4C–D). Parental exposure of animals to these potential bacterial pathogens did not intergenerationally protect animals against P. vranovensis (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). We conclude that parental exposure to some species of Pseudomonas can protect offspring from other species of Pseudomonas, but that these effects are likely specific to a subset of Pseudomonas species and not part of a broad response to Gram-negative bacterial pathogens.

To determine how different parental bacterial infections affect offspring gene expression patterns, we profiled gene expression in the offspring of C. elegans parents exposed to each of P. vranovensis BIGb0427, Pseudomonas sp. 15C5, Serretia plymuthica BUR1537, and Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469. We found that only 28 genes exhibit differential expression in the offspring of parents exposed to all four potential pathogens (Figure 4E–H). However, we identified 309 genes that are specifically differentially expressed in the offspring of parents exposed to P. vranovensis and Pseudomonas sp. 15C5 but not in the offspring of parents exposed to S. plymuthica BUR1537 or Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469 (Figure 4H and Supplementary file 7). We conclude that parental exposure to bacterial pathogens that elicit enhanced offspring resistance to P. vranovensis resulted in distinct changes in offspring gene expression that are not observed when parents are exposed to other Gram-negative bacterial pathogens. Collectively, our results suggest that a majority of the intergenerational effects of a parent’s environment on offspring gene expression are both stress and pathogen-specific.

Discussion

Overall, our findings support the conclusion that some types of intergenerational effects are conserved while others diverge, not unlike other important aspects of biology that vary between the species investigated here such as mode of reproduction – C. elegans, C. briggsae, and C. tropicalis are hermaphroditic while C. kamaaina is an exclusively male–female species. Specifically, our findings provide some of the first evidence that the mechanisms underlying intergenerational effects of a parent’s environment on offspring are evolutionarily conserved among different species. While these findings are restricted to the genus Caenorhabditis, to our knowledge they represent the first observation that intergenerational responses to stress are conserved across any evolutionary distance, and they provide a base from which we can compare the numerous different reported observations of multigenerational effects in C. elegans to similar intergenerational responses to stress in more distantly related species. For example, we found that only a subset of the many transcriptional changes that are detectable in the offspring of stressed C. elegans parents are conserved in any other species investigated. In addition, we used our analysis to identify 37 genes that exhibited intergenerational regulation of expression in response to the specific stresses of P. vranovensis infection or osmotic stress in all species studied (Figure 3). We propose that these genes might be particularly tuned for intergenerational regulation and might similarly be involved in intergenerational responses to stress in more distantly related species, including species outside the Caenorhabditis genus.

Notably, we found that the expression of these 37 genes in the offspring of parents exposed to either P. vranovensis infection or osmotic stress were still differentially expressed in C. tropicalis even though parental exposure to these stresses did not appear to affect offspring stress resistance in either assay (Figures 1 and 2). We hypothesize that the molecular consequences of parental stress on offspring, such as changes in the expression of stress–response genes, might be more easily identifiable than the specific physiological consequences of parental stress on offspring. In this case, we might not have detected the unique phenotypic effects of parental exposure to stress on offspring in C. tropicalis using our assay conditions, but such effects might still exist in this species and be related to those observed in other species. Future studies of the phenotypic effects of parental stress on offspring across species will likely shed significant light on how similar molecular mechanisms can mediate different intergenerational responses to stress across evolution.

Consistent with the hypothesis that parental exposure to the same stress might elicit distinct phenotypic effects on offspring in different species via evolutionarily related mechanisms, we found that parental exposure of C. briggsae to P. vranovensis had a strong deleterious effect on offspring pathogen resistance even though parental exposure of C. elegans and C. kamaaina to P. vranovensis resulted in increased offspring resistance to P. vranovensis (Figure 1B). This inversion of an intergenerational effect from a presumed adaptive effect to a presumed deleterious effect correlated with an inversion in the expression of specific pathogen–response genes that were previously reported to be required for animals to intergenerationally adapt to P. vranovensis, such as rhy-1 which exhibits increased expression in C. elegans and C. kamaaina offspring from infected parents but decreased expression in C. briggsae offspring from infected parents (Figure 2E). To our knowledge, these findings are the first to suggest that the molecular mechanisms underlying presumed adaptive and deleterious intergenerational effects in different species are evolutionarily related at the gene expression level. These findings suggest that similar observations of presumed intergenerational deleterious effects in diverse species, such as fetal programming in humans, might also be molecularly related to intergenerational adaptive effects in other species. Alternatively, our findings suggest that presumed intergenerational deleterious effects might in fact represent deleterious tradeoffs that are adaptive in other contexts. We expect that a more complete consideration of the evolution of intergenerational effects and the potential relationship between adaptive and deleterious effects will play an important role in understanding how intergenerational effects contribute to organismal resilience in changing environments, what role such effects play in evolution, and how such effects contribute to multiple human pathologies associated with a parent’s environment (Langley-Evans, 2006).

Lastly, the extent to which intergenerational and transgenerational responses to environmental stress represent related, independent, or even mutually exclusive phenomena represents a major outstanding question in the field of multigenerational effects. Evolutionary modeling of intergenerational and transgenerational effects has suggested that different ecological pressures favor the evolution of either intergenerational or transgenerational responses under different conditions. Specifically, it has been suggested that intergenerational effects are favored when offspring environmental conditions are predictable from the parental environment (Dey et al., 2016; Lind et al., 2020; Proulx et al., 2019; Uller, 2008). Furthermore, it has been speculated that intergenerational adaptations to stress will have costs (Uller, 2008). These costs, such as the costs we observed for animals intergenerational adaptation to osmotic stress (Figure 3), are likely to strongly favor the loss or active erasure of intergenerational effects if the parental environment improves to avoid potential deleterious effects when a stress is no longer present. By contrast, transgenerational effects were found to predominantly be favored when parental environmental cues are unreliable and the maintenance of information across many generations might be worth the potential costs (Uller et al., 2015).

Our findings in this study support either a model in which intergenerational and transgenerational effects represent potentially distinct phenomena or a model in which transgenerational effects only persist or occur under certain conditions with the vast majority of the effects of parental stress on offspring gene expression being lost or actively erased after one generation under other conditions. We strongly suspect that future studies into the mechanisms regulating these intergenerational effects will shed significant light on how intergenerational effects on gene expression are lost and/or erased. In addition, we expect that similar studies of transgenerational effects will potentially elucidate the circumstances under which animals decide if environmental information might be worth maintaining transgenerationally despite any potential tradeoffs and if the growing number of transgenerational effects observed in C. elegans are similarly evolutionarily conserved.

Lastly, future studies of intergenerational effects will be critical in determining the extent to which the mechanisms that mediate intergenerational effects are conserved outside of Caenorhabditis and if similar mechanisms to those uncovered in C. elegans mediate the numerous different adaptive and deleterious intergenerational effects that have been reported in diverse taxa ranging from the intergenerational development of wings in aphids (Vellichirammal et al., 2017) to fetal programming and the role it plays in disease in humans (Langley-Evans, 2006).

Materials and methods

Strains

C. elegans strains were cultured and maintained at 20°C unless noted otherwise. The Bristol strain N2 was the wild-type strain. Wild-isolate strains used in the main figures of this study: N2 (C. elegans), AF16 (C. briggsae), JU1373 (C. tropicalis), and QG122 (C. kamaaina). Wild-isolate strains used in figure supplements of this study: MY1 (C. elegans), PS2025 (C. elegans), CX11262 (C. elegans), JU440 (C. elegans), JU778 (C. elegans), JU1213 (C. elegans), LKC34 (C. elegans), JU1491 (C. elegans), EG4724 (C. elegans), KR314 (C. elegans), SX1125 (C. briggsae), and JU1348 (C. briggsae). Mutant alleles used in this study: osm-8(n1518) and Cbr-gpdh-2(syb2973).

P. vranovensis survival assays

P. vranovensis BIGb0446 or Pseudomonas sp. 15C5 was cultured in LB at 37 °C overnight. 1 ml of overnight culture was seeded onto 50 mm NGM agar plates and dried in a laminar flow hood (bacterial lawns completely covered the plate such that animals could not avoid the pathogen). All plates seeded with BIGb0446 or 15C5 were used the same day they were seeded. Young adult animals were placed onto 50 mm NGM agar plates seeded with 1 ml either E. coli HB101, P. vranovensis BIGb446, or Pseudomonas sp. 15C5 for 24 h at room temperature (22 °C). Embryos from these animals were collected by bleaching and placed onto fresh NGM agar plates seeded with BIGb0446. Percent surviving were counted after 24 hr at room temperature (22 °C) unless otherwise noted.

Osmotic stress and P. vranovensis multiple stress adaptation assays

Young adult animals that were grown on NGM agar plates seeded with E. coli HB101 were collected and transferred to new 50 mM NaCl control plates seeded with E. coli HB101, 300 mM NaCl plates seeded with E. coli HB101, 50 mM NaCl control plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446, or 300 mM NaCl plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Animals were grown for 24 hr at room temperature (22 °C). Embryos from these animals were collected by bleaching and transferred to new 500 mM NaCl plates seeded with E. coli HB101 or 50 mM NaCl plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Percent of animals developing or surviving was scored after 24 hr at room temperature as previously described in Burton et al., 2017 and Burton et al., 2020.

Preparation of N. parisii spores

Spores were prepared as described previously (Willis et al., 2021). In brief, large populations of C. elegans N2 were infected with microsporidia spores. Infected worms were harvested and mechanically disrupted using 1 mm diameter Zirconia beads (BioSpec). Resulting lysate was filtered through 5 μm filters (Millipore Sigma) to remove nematode debris. Spore preparations were tested for contamination and those free of contaminating bacteria were stored at −80 °C.

N. parisii infection assays and multiple stress adaptation assays

P0 populations of 2500 animals were mixed with 1 ml of 10× saturated E. coli OP50-1 or P. vranovensis and a low dose of N. parisii spores (see Table 3) and plated on a 10 cm plate. This low dose limited the detrimental effects on animal fertility that are observed with higher doses, while ensuring most animals were still infected. F1 populations of 1000 animals were mixed with 400 μl of 10× saturated E. coli OP50-1 and a high dose of N. parisii spores (see Table 3) and plated on a 6 cm plate.

Table 3. Details of N. parisii doses employed.

N. parisii dose Plate concentration (spores/cm2) Millions of spores used
6 cm plate 10 cm plate
Low ~32,000 2.5
High ~88,000 2.5

To test for inherited immunity to N. parisii in C. elegans, C. briggsae, C. tropicalis, and C. kamaaina, synchronized animals were infected from the L1 larval stage with a low dose of N. parisii. C. elegans and C. briggsae were grown for 72 hr at 21°C; C. tropicalis and C. kamaaina were grown for 96 hr at 21°C. Ten percent of total P0 animals were fixed in acetone for DY96 staining, as described below. Embryos from the remaining animals were collected by bleaching and synchronized by hatching overnight in M9. Resulting F1 animals were infected from the L1 larval stage with a high dose of N. parisii. C. elegans and C. briggsae were fixed at 72 hr postinfection (hpi) at 21°C; C. tropicalis and C. kamaaina were fixed at 96 hpi at 21°C.

For multiple stress adaptation assays using N. parisii and osmotic stress, animals were grown on NGM agar plates seeded with 10× saturated E. coli OP50-1 until the L4 stage. Next, animals were collected and mixed with 1 ml of either E. coli OP50-1 alone or supplemented with a low dose of N. parisii spores and plated on either 50 mM NaCl or 250 mM NaCl plates. Animals were grown for 24 hr at 21 °C. Embryos from these animals were collected by bleaching. To test adaptation to osmotic stress, 2000 F1 embryos were transferred to 420 mM NaCl plates seeded with E. coli OP50-1. Percentage of animals hatched was scored after 48 hr at 21 °C, as previously described in Burton et al., 2017 and Burton et al., 2020. To test adaptation to N. parisii, the remaining embryos were synchronized by hatching overnight in M9. Resulting F1 animals were either not infected as controls, or infected at the L1 larval stage with a high dose of N. parisii. Animals were fixed after 72 hr at 21 °C for DY96 staining and analysis.

For multiple stress adaptation assays using N. parisii and P. vranovensis, animals were grown on NGM agar plates seeded with E. coli OP50-1 until the L4/young adult stage. Next, animals were collected and mixed with 1 ml of either E. coli OP50-1 alone or E. coli OP50-1 supplemented with a low dose of N. parisii spores, or 1 ml of P. vranovensis BIGb0446 alone or P. vranovensis BIGb0446 supplemented with a low dose of N. parisii spores. Animals were plated on NGM and grown for 24 hr at 21 °C. Embryos from these animals were collected by bleaching. To test adaptation to P. vranovensis, 2000 F1 embryos were transferred to new NGM plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Percentage of animals surviving was scored after 24 hr at 21 °C as previously described in Burton et al., 2017 and Burton et al., 2020. To test adaptation to N. parisii, the remaining embryos were synchronized by hatching overnight in M9. Resulting F1 animals were either not infected as controls, or infected from the earliest larval stage with a high dose of N. parisii. Animals were fixed after 72 hr at 21 °C for DY96 staining and analysis.

Fixation and staining of N. parisii infection

Worms were washed off plates with M9 and fixed in 1 ml acetone for 10 min at room temperature, or overnight at 4 °C. Fixed animals were washed twice in 1 ml PBST (phosphate-buffered saline [PBS] containing 0.1 % Tween-20) before staining. Microsporidia spores were visualized with the chitin-binding dye Direct Yellow (DY96). For DY96 staining alone, animals were resuspended in 500 μl staining solution (PBST, 0.1 % sodium dodecyl sulfate, 20 μg/ml DY96), and rotated at 21 °C for 30 min in the dark. DY96-stained worms were resuspended in 20 μl EverBrite Mounting Medium (Biotium) and mounted on slides for imaging. Note: to pellet worms during fixation and staining protocols, animals were centrifuged for 30 s at 10,000 × g.

Image analysis of N. parisii infection

Worms were imaged with an Axioimager 2 (Zeiss). DY96-stained worms were imaged to determine number of embryos per worm. Worms possessing any quantity of intracellular DY96-stained microsporidia were considered infected. Precise microsporidia burdens were determined using ImageJ/FIJI (Schindelin et al., 2012). For this, each worm was defined as an individual ‘region of interest’ and fluorescence from GFP (DY96-stained microsporidia) subject to ‘threshold’ and ‘measure area percentage’ functions on ImageJ. Images were thresholded to capture the brighter signal from microsporidia spores, while eliminating the dimmer GFP signal from worm embryos. Final values are given as % fluorescence for single animals.

Preparation of OP50 for plating worms

One colony of E. coli strain OP50 was added to 100 ml of LB and grown overnight at room temperature then stored at 4 °C. One or five drops of HB101 were added to 6 or 10 cm plates of NGM, respectively, to use for growing worm strains and recovering them from starvation.

Preparation of HB101 for liquid culture

One colony of E. coli strain HB101 was added to a 5 ml starter culture of LB with streptomycin and grown for 24 hr at 37 °C. The starter cultures were then added to a 1 l culture of TB and grown for another 24 hr at 37 °C. The bacteria was centrifuged for 10 min at 5000 rpm to form a pellet. After being weighed, the bacteria was then resuspended in S-complete to create a 10× (250 mg/ml) stock that was stored at 4 °C. Further dilutions with S-complete were used to create the dilutions for each condition in this experiment.

Dietary restriction/dilution series cultures

For C. elegans, C. briggsae, and C. tropicalis, 10 L4 hermaphrodite worms were picked onto three 10 cm plates seeded with OP50, and for C. kamaaina 10 L4 females and ~20 males were picked onto three 10 cm plates. For all species, adults were removed after 24 hr. C. elegans and C. briggsae were grown for 96 hr before bleaching and C. tropicalis and C. kamaaina were grown for 120 hr before bleaching due to slower growth and longer generation time. After bleaching, worms were aliquoted into 100 ml cultures of S-complete at one worm/100 μl with a concentration of 25 , 12.5 , 6.25 , 3.13 , or 1.6 mg/ml of HB101 and kept in 500 ml flasks in shaking incubators at 20 °C and 180 rpm. Worms were grown in these cultures for 96 hr (C. elegans), 102 hr (C. briggsae), or 120 hr (C. tropicalis and C. kamaaina) before being bleached and prepared for starvation cultures. Due to slow development and inability to properly scale up in liquid culture, 1.6 mg/ml cultures for C. briggsae and 1.6 and 3.13 mg/ml cultures for C. kamaaina were excluded from the rest of this experiment.

Starvation cultures

After bleach, embryos were placed into 5 ml virgin S-basal cultures in 16 mm glass test tubes on a roller drum at 20 °C at one worm/μl. Worms were aliquoted out of this culture using micropipettes for further assays.

Measuring L1 size

Twenty-four hours after bleach (~12 hr after hatch), 1000 L1s were pipetted out of the starvation cultures, spun down in 15 ml plastic conical tubes by centrifuge for 1 min at 3000 rpm then plated onto unseeded 10 cm NGM plates. L1s were imaged with a Zeiss Discovery. V20 stereomicroscope at ×77 and measured using Wormsizer (Moore et al., 2013). Ad libitum concentration was defined as 25 mg/ml and dietary restriction concentration was determined based on what concentration of HB101 produced the largest average L1 size for each strain. For C. elegans, this was 3.13 mg/ml, and eightfold dilution from ad libitum and consistent with previous determinations for dietary restriction in C. elegans (Hibshman et al., 2016). For C. briggsae, peak L1 size varied between 12.5 and 6.25 mg/ml depending on replicate. We chose to use 6.25 mg/ml as the dietary restriction concentration to be consistent with replicates that were already being processed. The peak L1 size and determination of dietary restriction for C. tropicalis were 6.13 mg/ml. C. kamaaina did not show a significant change in L1 size across conditions and was ultimately excluded from the brood size assay due to difficulty interpreting effects of starvation on brood size in a male–female strain.

L1 size statistics

A linear mixed effects model was performed on the L1 size data to see if there was a significant effect of HB101 concentration on L1 size. The lme4 package in R studio was used to perform this linear mixed effects test. The function lmer() was used on data from each species, for example: • lmer(length~ condition + (1 | replicate) + (1 | replicate:condition), data = C_elegans), ‘length’ is the length in microns of each individual worm, ‘condition’ is the fixed effect of the concentration of HB101, ‘1 | replicate’ is the addition of the random effect of replicate to the model, ‘1 | replicate:condition' is the addition of the random effect per combination of replicate and condition, and ‘data’ is the primary spreadsheet restricted by the species of interest.

Gene orthology inference among species

To identify one-to-one orthologs across the four species, we downloaded protein and GFF3 files for C. elegans, C. briggsae, and C. tropicalis genomes from WormBase (Harris et al., 2020) (version WS275) and for the C. kamaaina genome from caenorhabditis.org (version v1). We assessed gene set completeness using BUSCO (Simão et al., 2015) (version 4.0.6; using the parameter -m proteins) using the ‘nematoda_odb10’ lineage dataset. For each species, we selected the longest isoform for each protein-coding gene using the agat_sp_keep_longest_isoform.pl script from AGAT (Jacques Dainat, 2021) (version 0.4.0). Filtered protein files were clustered into orthologous groups (OGs) using OrthoFinder (Emms and Kelly, 2019) (version 2.4.0; using the parameter -og) and one-to-one OGs were selected.

F1 and F3 sample collection for RNA-seq

Young adult animals grown on NGM agar plates seeded with E. coli HB101 were collected and transferred to new plates seeded with either control plates (50 mM NaCl) seeded with E. coli HB101, P. vranovensis BIGb0446, P. vranovensis BIGb0427, S. plymuthica BUR1537, Pseudomonas sp. 15C5, Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469, or plates containing 300 mM NaCl seeded with E. coli HB101. Animals were grown for 24 hr at room temperature (22 °C). Embryos from these animals were collected by bleaching and immediately frozen in 1 ml Trizol.

Analysis of RNA-seq data

RNA libraries were prepared and sequenced by BGI TECH SOLUTIONS using 100PE DNBseq Eukaryotic Transcriptome service. Quality controlled and adapter trimming of RNA reads were performed using fastp-v4.20.0 (Chen et al., 2018) (--qualified_quality_phred 20 --unqualified_percent_limit 40 --length_required 50 --low_complexity_filter --complexity_threshold 30 --detect_adapter_for_pe --correction --trim_poly_g --trim_poly_x \ --trim_front1 2 --trim_tail1 2 --trim_front2 2 --trim_tail2 2) (1). Next, reads were aligned using STAR-2.7.1a (Dobin et al., 2013) (--alignSJoverhangMin 8 --alignSJDBoverhangMin 1 --outFilterMismatchNmax 999 --outFilterMismatchNoverReadLmax 0.04 --alignIntronMin 10 --alignIntronMax 1000000 --alignMatesGapMax 1000000 --outFilterType BySJout --outFilterMultimapNmax 10000 --winAnchorMultimapNmax 50 --outMultimapperOrder Random) (2) against the genome of C. elegans WS275, C. briggsae WS275, C. tropicalis WS275, and the C. kamaaina genome obtained from caenorhabditis.org. Read counts were obtained using subread-2.0.0 (-M -O -p --fraction -F GTF -a -t exon -g gene_id) (Liao et al., 2014) (3) using the annotation for C. elegans PRJNA13758.WS275, C. briggsae PRJNA10731.WS275, C. tropicalis PRJNA53597.WS275, and C. kamaaina Caenorhabditis_kamaaina_QG2077_v1. Counts were imported into R and differential gene expression analysis was performed with DESeq2 (FDR < 0.01) (Love et al., 2014).

For comparisons made between different species, genes were subsetted to include only those 7587 single-copy ortholog groups that were identified between the four species. In addition to the 7203 genes that were identified as single-copy ortholog groups by OrthoFinder, the 7587 contain an additional 385 ortholog groups that were identified as having more than one ortholog in one out four of the species but where all but one of the multiple orthologs had no observable expression in any of the samples collected.

For the comparison between the stress response and gene expression during embryo development, data were downloaded from Boeck et al., 2016 and imported in R with raw counts from this study. The range of embryo expression for each gene was considered as one standard deviation ± the mean of regularized log normalized counts across all embryo time points. DEGs from the stress experiments where the regularized log normalized counts for one or both of the comparison samples (for all replicates) were outside of the embryo range were considered unlikely to be caused by developmental timing.

L4+ developmental rate assays

Young adult animals that were grown on NGM agar plates seeded with E. coli HB101 were collected and transferred to new plates seeded with E. coli HB101, Pseudomonas sp. 15C5, S. plymuthica BUR1537, or Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469. Animals were grown for 24 hr at room temperature (22 °C). Embryos from these animals were collected by bleaching and transferred to new plates seeded with 1 ml of E. coli HB101 Pseudomonas sp. 15C5, S. plymuthica BUR1537, or Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469. Percent of animals that reached the L4 larval stage was scored after either 48 or 72 hr at 22 °C.

Identification of Pseudomonas sp. 15 C5 and S. plymuthica BUR1537

Samples of rotting fruit and vegetation were collected from around Cambridge (UK) in 50 ml vials. For isolation of wild bacteria, the samples were homogenized and resuspended in M9 and plated on LB Agar, Nutrient Agar, or Actinomycete Isolation Agar plates and grown at either 37 °C or 30 °C for 24 hr. Single colonies were isolated from the plates and grown in LB or Nutrient Broth at the same temperature overnight. Stocks were frozen and stored at −80 °C in 20 % glycerol. One thousand five hundred and thirty-seven total isolates were obtained and frozen. C. elegans embryos were placed onto NGM agar plates seeded with each of the 1537 bacterial isolates. Bacterial isolates that caused substantial delays in animal development or lethality were further analyzed for isolates where parental exposure to the isolate for 24 hr modified offspring phenotype when compared to offspring from parents fed the normal laboratory diet of E. coli HB101. Bacterial genus and species were identified by 16 S rRNA profiling and sequencing.

RNAi in C. kamaaina

dsDNA corresponding to the C. kamaaina orthologs of cysl-1, rhy-1, mek-2, and gpdh-2 was synthesized and cloned into the L4440 vector by GENEWIZ (Takeley, UK). Vectors were transformed in E. coli HT115. C. kamaaina embryos were collected by bleaching and placed onto NGM agar plates containing 1 mM IPTG that were seeded with E. coli HT115 transformed with either the L4440 empty vector or each of the new vectors and grown at room temperature (22 °C) for 48 hr. After 48 hr, animals were transferred to new 50 mM NaCl control plates seeded with E. coli HB101, 300 mM NaCl plates seeded with E. coli HB101, or 50 mM NaCl control plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Animals were grown for 24 hr at room temperature (22  °C). Embryos from these animals were collected by bleaching and transferred to new 500 mM NaCl plates seeded with E. coli HB101 or 50 mM NaCl plates seeded with P. vranovensis BIGb0446. Percent of animals developing or surviving was scored after 24 hr at room temperature as previously described in Burton et al., 2017 and Burton et al., 2020.

Statistics and reproducibility

Sample sizes for experiments involving C. elegans were selected based on similar studies from the literature and all animals from each genotype and condition were grouped and analyzed randomly. All replicate numbers listed in figure legends represent biological replicates of independent animals cultured separately, collected separately, and analyzed separately. Unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test was used for Figures 1B, D, F, 2E, J, 4C and D, and Figure 1—figure supplement 1F-G. Two-way ANOVA was used for Figures 1C, E, 3A–G, and Figure 1—figure supplement 1A-E. One-way ANOVA was used for Figure 4A and B, and Figure 4—figure supplement 1. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001. The experiments were not randomized. The investigators were not blinded to allocation during experiments and outcome assessment.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Buck Samuel and Marie-Anne Felix for bacterial isolates. We also thank Matt Rockman and Luke Noble for prepublication access to the genome of C. kamaaina. We would also like to thank Marie-Anne Felix and the Caenorhabditis Genetic Center, which is funded by the NIH National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), for Caenorhabditis strains. NOB is funded by a Next Generation Fellowship from the Centre for Trophoblast Research. KF and LRB were funded by the National Institutes of Health (GM117408, LRB). AW and AR were funded by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (Grant #522691522691) and an Alfred P Sloan Research Fellowship FG2019-12040 (to AWR). This work was also supported by Cancer Research UK (C13474/A18583, C6946/A14492) and the Wellcome Trust (104640/Z/14/Z, 092096/Z/10/Z) grants to EAM.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Nicholas O Burton, Email: nick.burton@vai.org.

Diethard Tautz, Max-Planck Institute for Evolutionary Biology, Germany.

Patricia J Wittkopp, University of Michigan, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Centre Trophoblast Research Next Generation fellowship to Nicholas O Burton.

  • National Institutes of Health to Kinsey Fisher, L Ryan Baugh.

  • National Institutes of Health GM117408 to L Ryan Baugh.

  • Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Grant #522691522691 to Alexandra Willis, Aaron W Reinke.

  • Alfred P. Sloan Foundation FG2019-12040 to Aaron Reinke.

  • Cancer Research UK C13474/A18583 to Eric A Miska.

  • Cancer Research UK C6946/A14492 to Eric A Miska.

  • Wellcome Trust 104640/Z/14/Z to Eric A Miska.

  • Wellcome Trust 092096/Z/10/Z to Eric A Miska.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. List of 7587 single-copy orthologous genes conserved among C. elegans, C. briggsae, C. kamaaina, and C. tropicalis.
elife-73425-supp1.xlsx (354.2KB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 2. Expression of single-copy orthologous genes in F1 progeny of animals exposed to P. vranovensis.
elife-73425-supp2.xlsx (6.5MB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 3. Expression of single-copy orthologous genes in F1 progeny of animals exposed to osmotic stress.
elife-73425-supp3.xlsx (7.2MB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 4. Expression of single-copy orthologous genes in F3 progeny of animals exposed to P. vranovensis and osmotic stress.
elife-73425-supp4.xlsx (4.5MB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 5. List of bacteria isolated from United Kingdom.
elife-73425-supp5.xlsx (61.7KB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 6. PCR sequences of Pseudomonas 15C5 16 S rRNA and rpoD.
elife-73425-supp6.txt (1.6KB, txt)
Supplementary file 7. Expression of single-copy orthologous genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans exposed to P. vranovensis, Pseudomonas sp. 15C5, Serratia plymuthica BUR1537, or Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469.
elife-73425-supp7.xlsx (20.5MB, xlsx)
Transparent reporting form

Data availability

RNA-seq data that support the findings of this study have been deposited at NCBI GEO and are available under the accession code GSE173987.

The following dataset was generated:

Burton N, Price J, Braukmann F, Miska E. 2021. Parental exposure to environmental stress results in evolutionarily conserved intergenerational changes in offspring gene expression. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus. GSE173987

The following previously published datasets were used:

Boeck M. 2016. The time-resolved transcriptome of C. elegans. NCBI Sequence Read Archive - Supplemental Table 1. PMC5052054

References

  1. Agrawal AA, Laforsch C, Tollrian R. Transgenerational induction of defences in animals and plants. Nature. 1999;401:60–63. doi: 10.1038/43425. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Boeck ME, Huynh C, Gevirtzman L, Thompson OA, Wang G, Kasper DM, Reinke V, Hillier LW, Waterston RH. The time-resolved transcriptome of C. elegans. Genome Research. 2016;26:1441–1450. doi: 10.1101/gr.202663.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bozler J, Kacsoh BZ, Bosco G. Transgeneratonal inheritance of ethanol preference is caused by maternal NPF repression. eLife. 2019;8:e45391. doi: 10.7554/eLife.45391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Buckley BA, Burkhart KB, Gu SG, Spracklin G, Kershner A, Fritz H, Kimble J, Fire A, Kennedy S. A nuclear Argonaute promotes multigenerational epigenetic inheritance and germline immortality. Nature. 2012;489:447–451. doi: 10.1038/nature11352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Burton NO, Furuta T, Webster AK, Kaplan REW, Baugh LR, Arur S, Horvitz HR. sulin-like signalling to the maternal germline controls progeny response to osmotic stress. Nature Cell Biology. 2017;19:252–257. doi: 10.1038/ncb3470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Burton NO, Riccio C, Dallaire A, Price J, Jenkins B, Koulman A, Miska EA. Cysteine synthases CYSL-1 and CYSL-2 mediate C. elegans heritable adaptation to P. vranovensis infection. Nature Communications. 2020;11:1741. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-15555-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chen S, Zhou Y, Chen Y, Gu J. fastp: an ultra-fast all-in-one FASTQ preprocessor. Bioinformatics. 2018;34:i884–i890. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bty560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cutter AD. Divergence Times in Caenorhabditis and Drosophila Inferred from Direct Estimates of the Neutral Mutation Rate. Molecular Biology and Evolution. 2008;25:778–786. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msn024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dantzer B, Newman AEM, Boonstra R, Palme R, Boutin S, Humphries MM, McAdam AG. Density Triggers Maternal Hormones That Increase Adaptive Offspring Growth in a Wild Mammal. Science. 2013;340:1215–1217. doi: 10.1126/science.1235765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. de Gusmão Correia ML, Volpato AM, Águila MB, Mandarim-de-Lacerda CA. Developmental origins of health and disease: experimental and human evidence of fetal programming for metabolic syndrome. Journal of Human Hypertension. 2012;26:405–419. doi: 10.1038/jhh.2011.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dey S, Proulx SR, Teotónio H. Adaptation to Temporally Fluctuating Environments by the Evolution of Maternal Effects. PLOS Biology. 2016;14:e1002388. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dias BG, Ressler KJ. Parental olfactory experience influences behavior and neural structure in subsequent generations. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:89–96. doi: 10.1038/nn.3594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dobin A, Davis CA, Schlesinger F, Drenkow J, Zaleski C, Jha S, Batut P, Chaisson M, Gingeras TR. STAR: ultrafast universal RNA-seq aligner. Bioinformatics. 2013;29:15–21. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bts635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Emms DM, Kelly S. OrthoFinder: phylogenetic orthology inference for comparative genomics. Genome Biology. 2019;20:238. doi: 10.1186/s13059-019-1832-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Greer EL, Maures TJ, Ucar D, Hauswirth AG, Mancini E, Lim JP, Benayoun BA, Shi Y, Brunet A. Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance of longevity in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature. 2011;479:365–371. doi: 10.1038/nature10572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hales CN, Barker DJP. Type 2 (non-insulin-dependent) diabetes mellitus: the thrifty phenotype hypothesis. Diabetologia. 1992;35:595–601. doi: 10.1007/BF00400248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hales CN, Barker DJP. The thrifty phenotype hypothesis: Type 2 diabetes. British Medical Bulletin. 2001;60:5–20. doi: 10.1093/bmb/60.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Harris TW, Arnaboldi V, Cain S, Chan J, Chen WJ, Cho J, Davis P, Gao S, Grove CA, Kishore R, Lee RYN, Muller H-M, Nakamura C, Nuin P, Paulini M, Raciti D, Rodgers FH, Russell M, Schindelman G, Auken KV, Wang Q, Williams G, Wright AJ, Yook K, Howe KL, Schedl T, Stein L, Sternberg PW. WormBase: a modern Model Organism Information Resource. Nucleic Acids Research. 2020;48:D762–D767. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkz920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hellberg JE, Matilla MA, Salmond GPC. The broad-spectrum antibiotic, zeamine, kills the nematode worm Caenorhabditis elegans. Frontiers in Microbiology. 2015;6:137. doi: 10.3389/fmicb.2015.00137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hibshman JD, Hung A, Baugh LR. Maternal Diet and Insulin-Like Signaling Control Intergenerational Plasticity of Progeny Size and Starvation Resistance. PLOS Genetics. 2016;12:e1006396. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Houri-Zeevi L, Korem Kohanim Y, Antonova O, Rechavi O. Three Rules Explain Transgenerational Small RNA Inheritance in C. elegans. Cell. 2020;182:1186–1197. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2020.07.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jacques Dainat Darío Hereñú, Pascal Pucholt. NBISweden/AGAT: AGAT-v0.6.2 2021
  23. Jordan JM, Hibshman JD, Webster AK, Kaplan REW, Leinroth A, Guzman R, Maxwell CS, Chitrakar R, Bowman EA, Fry AL, Hubbard EJA, Baugh LR. sulin/IGF Signaling and Vitellogenin Provisioning Mediate Intergenerational Adaptation to Nutrient Stress. Current Biology. 2019;29:2380–2388. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.05.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kaletsky R, Moore RS, Vrla GD, Parsons LR, Gitai Z, Murphy CT. C. elegans interprets bacterial non-coding RNAs to learn pathogenic avoidance. Nature. 2020;586:445–451. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2699-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kishimoto S, Uno M, Okabe E, Nono M, Nishida E. Environmental stresses induce transgenerationally inheritable survival advantages via germline-to-soma communication in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature Communications. 2017;8:14031. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Klosin A, Casas E, Hidalgo-Carcedo C, Vavouri T, Lehner B. Transgenerational transmission of environmental information in C. elegans. Science. 2017;356:320–323. doi: 10.1126/science.aah6412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lamitina T, Huang CG, Strange K. Genome-wide RNAi screening identifies protein damage as a regulator of osmoprotective gene expression. PNAS. 2006;103:12173–12178. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602987103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Langley-Evans SC. Developmental programming of health and disease. The Proceedings of the Nutrition Society. 2006;65:97–105. doi: 10.1079/pns2005478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Liao Y, Smyth GK, Shi W. featureCounts: an efficient general purpose program for assigning sequence reads to genomic features. Bioinformatics. 2014;30:923–930. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btt656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lind MI, Zwoinska MK, Andersson J, Carlsson H, Krieg T, Larva T, Maklakov AA. Environmental variation mediates the evolution of anticipatory parental effects. Evolution Letters. 2020;4:371–381. doi: 10.1002/evl3.177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Love MI, Huber W, Anders S. Moderated estimation of fold change and dispersion for RNA-seq data with DESeq2. Genome Biology. 2014;15:550. doi: 10.1186/s13059-014-0550-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Luna E, Bruce TJA, Roberts MR, Flors V, Ton J. Next-generation systemic acquired resistance. Plant Physiology. 2012;158:844–853. doi: 10.1104/pp.111.187468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ma C, Niu R, Huang T, Shao L-W, Peng Y, Ding W, Wang Y, Jia G, He C, Li C-Y, He A, Liu Y. N6-methyldeoxyadenine is a transgenerational epigenetic signal for mitochondrial stress adaptation. Nature Cell Biology. 2019;21:319–327. doi: 10.1038/s41556-018-0238-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Moore BT, Jordan JM, Baugh LR. WormSizer: high-throughput analysis of nematode size and shape. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e57142. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Moore RS, Kaletsky R, Murphy CT. Piwi/PRG-1 Argonaute and TGF-β Mediate Transgenerational Learned Pathogenic Avoidance. Cell. 2019;177:1827–1841. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.05.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Öst A, Lempradl A, Casas E, Weigert M, Tiko T, Deniz M, Pantano L, Boenisch U, Itskov PM, Stoeckius M, Ruf M, Rajewsky N, Reuter G, Iovino N, Ribeiro C, Alenius M, Heyne S, Vavouri T, Pospisilik JA. Paternal Diet Defines Offspring Chromatin State and Intergenerational Obesity. Cell. 2014;159:1352–1364. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Palominos MF, Verdugo L, Gabaldon C, Pollak B, Ortíz-Severín J, Varas MA, Chávez FP, Calixto A. Transgenerational Diapause as an Avoidance Strategy against Bacterial Pathogens in Caenorhabditis elegans. MBio. 2017;8:e01234-17. doi: 10.1128/mBio.01234-17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Posner R, Toker IA, Antonova O, Star E, Anava S, Azmon E, Hendricks M, Bracha S, Gingold H, Rechavi O. Neuronal Small RNAs Control Behavior Transgenerationally. Cell. 2019;177:1814–1826. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.04.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Proulx SR, Dey S, Guzella T, Teotónio H. How differing modes of non-genetic inheritance affect population viability in fluctuating environments. Ecology Letters. 2019;22:1767–1775. doi: 10.1111/ele.13355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Raudvere U, Kolberg L, Kuzmin I, Arak T, Adler P, Peterson H, Vilo J. g:Profiler: a web server for functional enrichment analysis and conversions of gene lists (2019 update. Nucleic Acids Research. 2019;47:W191–W198. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkz369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rohlfing AK, Miteva Y, Moronetti L, He L, Lamitina T. The Caenorhabditis elegans mucin-like protein OSM-8 negatively regulates osmosensitive physiology via the transmembrane protein PTR-23. PLOS Genetics. 2011;7:e1001267. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1001267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Samuel BS, Rowedder H, Braendle C, Félix MA, Ruvkun G. Caenorhabditis elegans responses to bacteria from its natural habitats. PNAS. 2016;113:E3941–E3949. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1607183113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, Rueden C, Saalfeld S, Schmid B, Tinevez JY, White DJ, Hartenstein V, Eliceiri K, Tomancak P, Cardona A. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nature Methods. 2012;9:676–682. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Schulz LC. The Dutch Hunger Winter and the developmental origins of health and disease. PNAS. 2010;107:16757–16758. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1012911107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Simão FA, Waterhouse RM, Ioannidis P, Kriventseva EV, Zdobnov EM. BUSCO: assessing genome assembly and annotation completeness with single-copy orthologs. Bioinformatics. 2015;31:3210–3212. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btv351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Stevens L, Rooke S, Falzon LC, Machuka EM, Momanyi K, Murungi MK, Njoroge SM, Odinga CO, Ogendo A, Ogola J, Fèvre EM, Blaxter M. The Genome of Caenorhabditis bovis. Current Biology. 2020;30:1023–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.01.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Uller T. Developmental plasticity and the evolution of parental effects. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2008;23:432–438. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Uller T, English S, Pen I. When is incomplete epigenetic resetting in germ cells favoured by natural selection? Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 2015;282:20150682. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.0682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Veenendaal M, Painter R, de Rooij S, Bossuyt P, van der Post J, Gluckman P, Hanson M, Roseboom T. Transgenerational effects of prenatal exposure to the 1944-45 Dutch famine. BJOG. 2013;120:548–554. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.12136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Vellichirammal NN, Gupta P, Hall TA, Brisson JA. Ecdysone signaling underlies the pea aphid transgenerational wing polyphenism. PNAS. 2017;114:1419–1423. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1617640114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Webster AK, Jordan JM, Hibshman JD, Chitrakar R, Baugh LR. Transgenerational Effects of Extended Dauer Diapause on Starvation Survival and Gene Expression Plasticity in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 2018;210:263–274. doi: 10.1534/genetics.118.301250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Wibowo A, Becker C, Marconi G, Durr J, Price J, Hagmann J, Papareddy R, Putra H, Kageyama J, Becker J, Weigel D, Gutierrez-Marcos J. Hyperosmotic stress memory in Arabidopsis is mediated by distinct epigenetically labile sites in the genome and is restricted in the male germline by DNA glycosylase activity. eLife. 2016;5:e13546. doi: 10.7554/eLife.13546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Willis AR, Zhao W, Sukhdeo R, Wadi L, El Jarkass HT, Claycomb JM, Reinke AW. A parental transcriptional response to microsporidia infection induces inherited immunity in offspring. Science Advances. 2021;7:eabf3114. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abf3114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Zhang G, Sachse M, Prevost MC, Luallen RJ, Troemel ER, Félix MA. A Large Collection of Novel Nematode-Infecting Microsporidia and Their Diverse Interactions with Caenorhabditis elegans and Other Related Nematodes. PLOS Pathogens. 2016;12:e1006093. doi: 10.1371/journal.ppat.1006093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Diethard Tautz1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

The authors have systematically studied the question of an evolutionary conservation of stress responses in different species of nematodes. They identify some common and stress-specific pathways that lead to modifications in offspring of stressed parents. The study provides much needed experimental data on the question of how such epigenetic changes could contribute to evolutionary adaptive processes.

Decision letter after peer review:

[Editors’ note: the authors submitted for reconsideration following the decision after peer review. What follows is the decision letter after the first round of review.]

Thank you for submitting the paper "Intergenerational adaptations to stress are evolutionarily conserved, stress specific, and have deleterious trade-offs" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and a Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Alexei Maklakov (Reviewer #2).

We are sorry to say that, after consultation with the reviewers, we have decided that this work will not be considered further for publication by eLife at this stage.

As you will see from the reviews below, all three reviewers acknowledge the great potential of your approach. However, the transcriptome data that are the basis for the bulk of the conclusions are not fully adequate and too superficially analysed. It would seem very important to have at least the parental expression data as a reference and address the problem that only a subset of the embryos shows a response (see comment of reviewer 1). Further, with only three biological replicates, it would be necessary to use a more stringent cut-off for calling significant genes, or to use independent confirmation experiments. A more explicit pathway analysis should be also included. Also, the discussion (and possibly further analysis) of genes showing transgenerational effects should be enhanced. Since it is not clear whether the additional RNASeq data can be generated in a short time, we decided to reject at this point, but we would be happy to reconsider a significantly updated version.

Reviewer #1:

The general idea of comparing response patterns to stress in the offspring generation is new and very interesting. However, the data that are presented are in several ways preliminary. The phenotype comparisons are mostly convincing, although statistical treatments are partly unclear, given that each "replicate" includes itself many individuals. The transcriptomic data are minimal (only three replicates) and lack comparison to the stress responses in the parental animals. The analysis of the transcriptome data is limited to counting overlaps between significantly changed genes, without deeper discussion of the genes and pathways that are affected. The top response genes that are directly tested have been discovered before. Hence, while interesting patterns are evident from the data, this work largely confirms prior work, including that described in Burton et al. 2020.

The title and the discussion claim evolutionary conservation, but the actual results support this only partly. This needs to be stated more carefully. Also, given that the species used are rather closely related, one should relate the level of "conservation" to other conserved processes between these species.

While the term "intergenerational" is increasingly often used in these studies, the authors focus essentially on classic parental effects. A more systematic comparison with further generations would be useful to judge whether epigenetic programming might occur, or whether it is a pure F1 effect. In fact, given that there is at least a small subset of genes that show transgenerational effects, one should have expected a much deeper analysis of these genes.

The RNASeq data are produced on batches of embryos, on which only a subset (e.g. 50% for the infections) would show an effect. It would be very important to obtain single-embryo data to better understand why some react and some not.

The analysis of the RNASeq data is too superficial. Given that there are only three biological replicates, in a situation where much variance is to be expected (see comment above), one has to consider the data as highly underpowered. Hence, it would be necessary to include not only a p-value cut-off, but also a "fold-expression change" cut-off (e.g. at least 2-fold or even more). This would lead to rather different numbers and possibly also to different conclusions.

Reviewer #2:

Transgenerational effects (TE) (usually defined as multigenerational effects lasting for at least three generations) generated a lot of interest in recent years but the adaptive value of such effects is unclear. In order to understand the scope for adaptive TE we need to understand (i) whether such effects are common; (ii) whether they are stress-specific; and (iii) if there are trade-offs with respect to performance in different environments. The last point is particularly important because F1, F2 and F3 descendants may encounter very different environments. On the other hand, intergenerational effects (lasting for one or two generations) are relatively common and can play an important role in evolutionary processes. However, we do not know whether intergenerational and transgenerational effects have same underlying mechanisms.

This study makes a big step towards resolving these questions and strongly advances our understanding of both phenomena. Much of the previous work on mechanisms of multigenerational effects has been conducted in C. elegans and this works uses the same approach. They focus on bacterial infection, Microsporidia infection, larval starvation and osmotic stress. I did not quite understand why the authors chose to focus on P. vranovensis rather than P. aeruginosa P14 that has been used in previous studies of transgenerational effects in C. elegans. However, this is a minor point because I guess they were interested in broad transgenerational responses to bacterial infection rather than in strain-specific ones. The authors used different Caenorhabditis species, which is another strength of this study in addition to using multiple stresses.

They found 279 genes that exhibited intergenerational changes in all C species tested, but most interestingly, they show that a reversal in gene expression corresponds to a reversal in response to bacterial infection (beneficial in two species and deleterious on one). This is very intriguing! This was further supported by similar observations of osmotic stress response.

They also report that intergenerational effects are stress-specific and there have deleterious effects in mismatched environments, and, importantly, when worms were subject to multiple stresses. It is quite likely that offspring will experience a range of environments and that several environmental stresses will be present simultaneously in nature. I really liked this aspect of this work as I think that tests in different environments, especially environments with multiple stresses, are often lacking, which limits the generality of the conclusions.

Another interesting piece of the puzzle is that beneficial and deleterious effects could be mediated by the same mechanisms. It would be interesting to explore this further. However, this is not a real criticism of this work. I think that the authors collected an impressive dataset already and every good study generates new research questions.

Given these findings, I was particularly keen to see what comes of transgenerational effects. The general answer was that there aren't many, and the authors conclude that all intergenerational effects that they studied are largely reversible and that intergenerational and transgenerational effects represent distinct phenomena. While I think that this is a very important finding, I am not sure whether we can conclude that intergenerational and transgenerational effects are not related.

In my view, an alternative interpretation is that intergenerational effects are common while transgenerational effects are rare. Because intergenerational effects are stress-specific, transgenerational effects could be stress-specific as well.

Perhaps different mechanisms regulate intergenerational responses to, say, different forms of starvation (e.g. compare opposing transgenerational responses to prolonged larval starvation (Rechavi et al. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2014.06.020) and rather short adulthood starvation (Ivimey-Cook et al. 2021 https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.0701)). Perhaps some (most?) forms of starvation generate only intergenerational responses and do not generate transgenerational responses. But some do. Those forms of starvation that generate both intergenerational and transgenerational effects could do so via same mechanisms and represent the same phenomenon. I am by no means saying this is the case, but I am not sure that the absence of evidence of transgenerational effects in this study necessarily suggests that inter- and trans-generational effects are different phenomena.

The only concern real concern was the lack of phenotypic data on F3 beyond gene expression. Ideally, I would like to see tests of pathogen avoidance and starvation resistance in F3. However, given the amount of work that went into this study, the lack of strong signature of potential transgenerational effects in gene expression, and the fact that most of these effects were shown previously to last only one generation, I do not think this is crucial.

It would be very interesting to compare gene expression and other phenotypic responses in F1 and F3 between P. vranovensis and PA14. Also, it would be interesting to test the adaptive value of intergenerational and transgenerational effects after exposure to both strains in different environments. This is would be very informative and help with understanding the evolutionary significance of transgenerational epigenetic inheritance of pathogen avoidance as reported previously. Why response to P. vranovensis is erased while response to PA14 is maintained for four generations? Are nematodes more likely to encounter one species than the other? Again, however, this is not something necessary for this study.

The main strengths of this paper are (i) use of multiple stresses; (ii) use of multiple species; (iii) tests in different environments; and (iv) simultaneous evaluation of intergenerational and transgenerational responses. This study is first of a kind, and it provides several important answers, while highlighting clear paths for future work.

Excellent work and I think it will generate a lot of interest in the community, definitely want to see it published in eLife.

I am not fully convinced about their interpretation of whether inter- transgenerational effects are separate phenomena but happy to be convinced otherwise.

Reviewer #3:

In this manuscript, the authors address whether the mechanisms mediating intergenerational effects are conserved in evolution. This question is important not only to frame this phenomenon in an evolutionary context, but to address several interlinked questions: is there a mechanism in common between adaptive versus deleterious effects? What makes some effects last one instead of several generations? What is the ecological relevance for those mechanisms? Using Caenorhabditis elegans as a model of reference, they compare four types of intergenerational effects on additional three Caenorhabditis species.

The authors used previously characterized models of intergenerational inheritance, focusing on those that are likely to have adaptive significance. This is relevant, because the adaptive relevance of other published examples of inter- and transgenerational inheritance is not clear. They used functional studies to probe for conservation of mechanisms for bacterial infection and resistance to osmolarity stress, which is a major strength of this study. The data supports the claim of conservation in some types of intergenerational inheritance and divergence in others. One major question addressed in this manuscript is whether there is a potential overarching mechanism that confers stress-resistance across generations. Their experiments convincingly show that this is not the case, but that instead, there are stress-specific mechanisms responsible for intergenerational inheritance.

The authors highlighted the discovery of 279 highly conserved genes that exhibited intergenerational in gene expression. The manuscript would be strengthened if these genes were shown to have functional relevance.

It is interesting that C. elegans can pass the P. vavronensis resistance to the offspring, but not C. briggsae. Is this because C. briggsae is already expressing cysl-1 in the parental generation? What would happen if cysl-1 would be knocked out in this species? Would it lack resistance to the pathogen?

The authors showed that C. elegans and C. briggsae seem to share intergenerational effects of resistance to N. parisii in other species. It would be interesting to know if the mechanisms are conserved, but unfortunately this information is lacking. Similarly, it would be interesting to know if gdph-2 mediates resistance to osmolarity stress in C. kamaaina.

The supplementary tables would be more useful if the three-letter name would be included as well.

Indicate in Figure 3C that these are RNAi experiments. Also for Figure 3C, spell out "EV" in the legend of the figure. I suppose it means 'empty vector'?

The font size in the figures is pretty small and therefore difficult to read.

eLife. 2021 Oct 8;10:e73425. doi: 10.7554/eLife.73425.sa2

Author response


[Editors’ note: the authors resubmitted a revised version of the paper for consideration. What follows is the authors’ response to the first round of review.]

Reviewer #1:

The general idea of comparing response patterns to stress in the offspring generation is new and very interesting.

We thank Reviewer 1 for their time and thoughtful comments. We agree that these comparisons are new and very interesting and have added multiple revised analyses to the manuscript based on the reviewer comments that we think will further enhance the impact of and conclusions made in this study.

However, the data that are presented are in several ways preliminary. The phenotype comparisons are mostly convincing, although statistical treatments are partly unclear, given that each "replicate" includes itself many individuals.

The statistical treatments for groups of individuals are the same as in Burton et al., 2017, Burton et al., 2020, and Willis et al., 2021 which include the original reports of the intergenerational responses studied here. Replicates that include many individuals are relatively common when working with C. elegans and are usually compared using ANOVA or student’s t-tests (depending on the number of comparisons) to analyze the variation in batch effects as well as differences between populations of animals.

We believe this ability to assay hundreds or even thousands of animals, in total, for each comparison in this study makes our data substantially stronger and more reliable. However we are happy to perform any additional statistical tests the reviewer might want to see.

The transcriptomic data are minimal (only three replicates)

To address this comment we compared our original three replicates of RNA-seq from F1 animals from C. elegans parents exposed to P. vranovensis BIGb0446 to a second independent three replicates of F1 animals from C. elegans parents exposed to a second P. vranovensis isolate (BIGb0427 – the data for this second P. vranovensis isolate was already part of Figure 4 of this manuscript).

By comparing these three new replicates to our previous findings from three original replicates we found that 515 of the 562 genes that exhibited a >2-fold change and were significant at padj <0.01 in the original three replicates were also changed at >2-fold and padj <0.01 in the new three replicates. We believe our findings that 91.6% of genes change >2-fold and remain significant at padj<0.01 even when the number of replicates is doubled (and a different isolate of P. vranovensis is used!) suggests that adding additional replicates would not substantially change the conclusions of this manuscript.

We would also like to highlight, as above, that because this analysis was done on populations of thousands of similarly staged animals, as opposed to individuals, that this further reduces the variability between replicates. In addition, much of our transcriptomic data from each species was then compared across species and genes were only analyzed for those that changed in multiple different species which themselves each represent a separate three additional replicates [ie genes that change in all 4 species analyzed have to exhibit significant (>2-fold, padj <0.01) changes across 12 total replicates].

Our new findings comparing six replicates did not substantially change the number of genes identified when compared to using three replicates, and the fact that for all of the main conclusions of this manuscript each set of triplicates from one species was then compared across 9 additional replicates from three other species from pools of thousands of animals makes us very confident that our results are robust and highly reproducible.

… and lack comparison to the stress responses in the parental animals.

We agree with Reviewer 1 that comparisons to parental animals are interesting and important. Comparisons of F1 progeny gene expression patterns to parental animals were not included here because such comparisons were previously published in some of our original reports of these intergenerational effects (For example, see Burton et al., 2020). In summary, we found that most, but not all, of the effects on gene expression in F1 animals were also detected in parental animals. However, the transcriptional responses only turn on in F1 animals post gastrulation and do not appear to be due to the simple deposition of parental mRNAs into embryos (Burton et al., 2020).

We have updated the text to highlight these findings.

The analysis of the transcriptome data is limited to counting overlaps between significantly changed genes, without deeper discussion of the genes and pathways that are affected.

In the revised manuscript we have completely redone all of the transcriptomic analysis to use a stricter set of cutoffs for significance – both padj <0.01 and requiring a >2-fold change in expression based on the helpful comments of Reviewer 1 – which we agree with – see below.

As part of this new analysis we have now also included a deeper discussion of the genes that exhibited similar changes across species, including using g:Profiler to examine the genes that exhibited changes across all four species.

In addition, we have now paired our phenotypic and transcriptomic data across species to identify 19 new genes that we predict are highly likely to be involved in intergenerational responses to stress based on their expression patterns across species. These 19 genes come out of highly filtered analyses across species that identified a total of 23 genes that change only in species that adapt to P. vranovensis or osmotic stress and not in species that do not adapt.

Interestingly, this analysis identified nearly all of the previously known genes involved in intergenerational adaptations to these stresses including rhy-1, cysl-1, cysl-2 and gpdh-1. Thus, we predict the remaining 19 genes that came out of this analysis are highly likely to be involved in the responses to these stresses. Furthermore, in the revised text we highlight that our new list of 19 genes includes multiple conserved factors that are required for animal viability including genes involved in nuclear transport (imb-1 and xpo-2), the CDC25 phosphatase ortholog cdc-25.1, and the PTEN tumor suppressor ortholog daf-18. This new analysis will likely form the basis for future investigations into the mechanisms underlying these exciting intergenerational effects.

We believe this additional analysis greatly improves this manuscript. We are also happy to include any specific additional analysis the reviewer would like to see.

The top response genes that are directly tested have been discovered before. Hence, while interesting patterns are evident from the data, this work largely confirms prior work, including that described in Burton et al. 2020.

We have revised the text to highlight that the aims of this particular study were to determine if multigenerational responses to stress were evolutionarily conserved at any level, as well as to determine the potential costs of such effects and the specificity of the responses. Questions that were not addressed in any previous study of multigenerational effects, including Burton et al., 2020.

Because of the aims of this study we believe it was critical to focus on genes that had an established role in these intergenerational responses in C. elegans and to compare and contrast the behavior and requirement of these genes in intergenerational responses in other species. (Although we note that this newly revised manuscript, we have now also reported 19 new top response genes – see above).

In addition to our original goals, in this study we were able to determine the extent to which intergenerational transcriptional responses are conserved and the extent to which intergenerational transcriptional changes persist trans-generationally (which we find to be effectively not at all using our revised stricter analysis). We believe these findings are not only novel, but perhaps will be surprising to much of the intergenerational and transgenerational field and have a major impact on both how multigenerational studies are interpreted and how they are conducted in the future. This is especially the case for studies in C. elegans which is one of the leading model organisms to study the mechanisms underlying both intergenerational and transgenerational responses to stress.

For example, we note that several landmark studies of transgenerational effects (persisting into F3 or later generations) in C. elegans performed RNA-seq on F1 progeny (For example, Moore et al., Cell 2019 or Ma et al., Nature Cell Biology 2019). Our new findings reported here suggest that it is possible that none of the transcriptional effects detected in F1 animals will persist in F3 progeny. Furthermore, our studies demonstrate the importance of comparing C. elegans transcriptional effects to related Caenorhabditis species as we found that only a subset of the effects detected in C. elegans are conserved in any other Caenorhabditis species. (Such comparisons are important for determining if and to what extent observations of intergenerational and/or transgenerational effects observed in C. elegans represent conserved phenomena).

For all of these reasons we believe our data is highly exciting, will be of broad interest to the field, and represent novel and potentially unexpected findings that were not previously reported in any prior work including Burton et al., 2020.

The title and the discussion claim evolutionary conservation, but the actual results support this only partly. This needs to be stated more carefully. Also, given that the species used are rather closely related, one should relate the level of "conservation" to other conserved processes between these species.

As reviewer 1 highlights, the relationship between members of a single genus is potentially small, however we would like to also highlight that worms, and Caenorhabditis species in particular, evolve much more rapidly that most species. As an example for the genomic relationship between the species used in this manuscript we would like to highlight Figure 2 from Karin Kiontke's 2004 paper (https://www.pnas.org/content/101/24/9003#sec-2) showing that the divergence between Caenorhabditis species is exceptionally large and they estimate that C. elegans relationship to C. kamaaina is much larger than the divergence between humans and mice for example, and is somewhere closer to the relationship between humans and zebra fish which might not be considered to be closely related.

Nonetheless, we have updated the text of this manuscript to more explicitly state the level of relationship between the four species investigated here (approximately 0.05 substitutions per site at the nucleotide level and an estimated 30 million years of evolution). We have also added a comparison to other process that diverge between these four species such as mode of reproduction (C. elegans, C. briggsae, and C. tropicalis are hermaphrodites but C. kamaaina is an exclusively male-female species). Lastly, we have added a statement to the text to reflect reviewer 3’s summary “The data supports the claim of conservation in some types of intergenerational inheritance and divergence in others.” We believe such a statement accurately reflects the data presented in this manuscript.

Regardless of the exact relationship between these species, we also note that the decision to focus on species within the genus Caenorhabditis was a conscious decision as part of our study plan because, to our knowledge, there have been no documented cases of conservation of intergenerational (or transgenerational) responses to stress in any species across any distance of relationship. Furthermore, a greater evolutionary distance would have made the transcriptional comparisons of single copy orthologs across species that we performed here more difficult as there would be fewer clear single copy orthologues due to more frequent gene duplications/deletions at greater evolutionary distances.

When we began this work it was not clear that these type of phenomena, especially the many such effects that have been described in C. elegans, would be conserved at all and thus we believe one of the highlights of this paper is that we can now show that these intergenerational effects are indeed conserved, at minimum, within many species of Caenorhabditis. Furthermore, this study creates a solid foundation to investigate these types of effects in more distantly related species.

For example, our studies using only these four related species demonstrate that pairing phenotypic and transcriptomic data for the response to bacterial infection can quickly narrow down the >1000 transcriptional changes that are detectable in C. elegans to a list of only 19 genes that includes all three genes previously reported to be involved in this adaptation. This finding not only predicts that the remaining genes among these 19 are highly likely to be enriched for those involved in this intergenerational adaptation but also suggests that these genes might be particularly sensitive to intergenerational regulation and thus studies in more distantly related species could potentially focus on this much smaller subset of genes.

In summary, we believe that many important and novel conclusions about the relative conservation of these types of effects can be made by comparing species within a single genus.

While the term "intergenerational" is increasingly often used in these studies, the authors focus essentially on classic parental effects.

We have modified the text to state that several of these intergenerational effects can also be described as parental effects.

We chose to use the term intergenerational here not only due to its widespread use in the literature to describe F1 effects (For example, see reviews – Perez and Lehner, Nature Cell Biology, 2019 or Miska and Ferguson-Smith. Science, 2016), but also because the term intergenerational encompasses both parental effects on F1 animals and effects that persist for two generations (and affect F2 progeny). This was important to us because some of the effects studied here, such as the response to P. vranovensis, have been found to persist for two generations under certain conditions (Burton et al., 2020) and thus cannot be described as exclusively parental in nature.

In addition to these reasons, and because we agree with the general sentiment of Reviewer 1 that the historical use of many different terms to describe different multigenerational effects can lead to much confusion, we have also recently co-authored a substantial review of non-genetic inheritance that goes in depth into the history of the terms intergenerational and transgenerational and highlights that the definition of the term intergenerational has come to include, but is not restricted to, effects that were classically described as parental effects (for a more in-depth discussion of these terms and the history of their use see the dedicated section “What are intergenerational and transgenerational effects” in our new review Burton and Greer, Seminars in Cell and Developmental Biology 2021).

Regardless, we are aware that these terms have historically been used with what are sometimes different definitions (the same is true for the term transgenerational), and thus we are also happy to change any instance of the word intergenerational in the text to parental effect should it be desired by the reviewers as we agree that many of the effects described here could also be defined as parental effects and the change in terms would not impact the exciting underlying biology or the novelty/impact of our findings.

A more systematic comparison with further generations would be useful to judge whether epigenetic programming might occur, or whether it is a pure F1 effect.

Our previous studies of these effects have already performed phenotypic comparisons with further generations (F2, F3, and beyond) and found that each of these effects are intergenerational (or parental) in nature and none of these phenotypic effects persist beyond two generations (F2) in any experimental set up we have analyzed (Burton et al., 2017; 2020, Willis et al., 2021). We have updated the text to highlight these previous analyses.

With regards to “epigenetic programming” vs. “pure F1 effect” – we do not believe these terms/effects are mutually exclusive and we do not believe the duration an effect persists for can be used to imply a specific underlying mechanism. For example, RNAi inheritance in C. elegans, one of the classically cited mechanisms mediating epigenetic inheritance in C. elegans, only persists into F1 animals and not further when the RNAi targets somatically expressed genes. (Burton et al., 2011) –[RNAi inheritance beyond F1 animals has predominantly only been observed in germ cells when silencing germline expressed genes in C. elegans (Buckley et al., 2012)]. Similarly, in Arabidopsis, intergenerational (F1 only) adaptations to stress appear to be dependent on histone modifications at stress response genes that are heritable/programmed (For example, Luna et al., 2012). We believe these effects, and others, appear to represent examples of epigenetic programming despite being pure F1 effects. (Many similar effects are reviewed in Burton and Greer, Seminars in Cell and Developmental Biology, 2021).

Along these same lines, effects that persist for more than one generation/transgenerational effects are not always clearly due to epigenetic programming. For example, there are observations of transgenerational effects in mice that are now thought to be due to the repeated transfer of microbiomes from parents to offspring (See– Sonnenburg et al., Nature, 2016 or the review by Perez and Lehner, Nature Cell Biology 2019).

While there is perhaps some debate as to what exactly qualifies as “epigenetic”, there are a growing number of examples of F1 effects of stress that are transmitted via germ cells and/or appear to represent programmed changes in gene expression that only last for a single generation and then are erased/lost after one generation across many different species. And some of these effects have been reported to require similar molecular machinery as those that persist for more than one generation.

Thus, while F1 effects could be mediated by mechanisms that are not classically considered epigenetic, we believe it is important to not rule out the possibility that such effects might be epigenetic in nature.

In fact, given that there is at least a small subset of genes that show transgenerational effects, one should have expected a much deeper analysis of these genes.

In this revised manuscript we have updated our transcriptomic analysis to be more strict on what qualifies for significance in line with Reviewer 1s comment below which we completely agree with – see below, we now required a >2-fold change in addition to a padj <0.01.

Using this revised analysis we found that none of the >1,500 changes in gene expression that were observed in F1 progeny in C. elegans persisted into the F3 progeny (includes both the response to P. vranovensis and osmotic stress). Thus our revised analysis eliminated the few transgenerational effects that did occur when the >2-fold filter was not applied and thus no deeper analysis was done.

We have, however, updated the discussion to highlight that our analysis further suggests that transgenerational effects might only occur under specific conditions, might be more rare than previously estimated, or might be mutually exclusive with intergenerational effects. In addition, we have now performed a deeper analysis on the genes that change in F1 progeny which we are very excited about given the significant, poorly understood, and apparently relatively common role intergenerational effects appear to play in organismal survival in stressful environments.

The RNASeq data are produced on batches of embryos, on which only a subset (e.g. 50% for the infections) would show an effect. It would be very important to obtain single-embryo data to better understand why some react and some not.

We agree with reviewer 1 that understanding why some, but not all offspring survive in our assays is a very interesting biological question.

In an effort to investigate if all of the embryos of parents exposed to both osmotic stress and P. vranovensis infection exhibit the observed intergenerational effects on gene expression or if only some embryos responded and not others, we have previously used GFP reporters of major stress response genes such as sod-5::GFP for osmotic stress and an endogenously tagged copy of cysl-2::GFP for P. vranovensis infection – see Burton et al., 2017 or Burton et al., 2020. This analysis, which included endogenously tagged genes, let us quantify relative gene expression responses at the single-embryo level.

We found that nearly 100% of F1 animals exhibit the increased expression of these genes in offspring at high levels even though only approximately 50% of these animals show survival in these assays and these effects (see imaging results in Burton et al., 2017 and Burton et al., 2020). These findings indicate that essentially all of the F1 animals appear to elicit, at least to some extent, the changes in gene expression that depend on a parent’s environment.

There are several possible reasons why the changes in gene expression in some F1 embryos are insufficient to lead to survival under the very stressful conditions analyzed here. We would be happy to add a discussion of the possible reasons for this to the manuscript if the reviewers would prefer this (there are many possibilities, and this type of inter-individual variability is arguably an entire field of biology). Alternatively, we could also highlight some excellent reviews of the broader field of inter-individual variability in stress responses that have been written. However, we have not added these in this current version as we felt such discussion might distract from the main conclusions of this manuscript.

The analysis of the RNASeq data is too superficial. Given that there are only three biological replicates, in a situation where much variance is to be expected (see comment above), one has to consider the data as highly underpowered. Hence, it would be necessary to include not only a p-value cutoff, but also a "fold-expression change" cutoff (e.g. at least 2-fold or even more). This would lead to rather different numbers and possibly also to different conclusions.

We agree and thank reviewer 1 for this comment. We have reanalyzed all of our transcriptomic data in this manuscript to include a >2-fold expression change cutoff. This has changed the exact numbers in the new version of this manuscript but has not changed any of the broader conclusions. In fact, this new analysis has perhaps strengthened our claims that using these more stringent cutoffs can be used to more confidently predict likely genes involved in intergenerational responses to stress.

Reviewer #2:

Transgenerational effects (TE) (usually defined as multigenerational effects lasting for at least three generations) generated a lot of interest in recent years but the adaptive value of such effects is unclear. In order to understand the scope for adaptive TE we need to understand (i) whether such effects are common; (ii) whether they are stress-specific; and (iii) if there are trade-offs with respect to performance in different environments. The last point is particularly important because F1, F2 and F3 descendants may encounter very different environments. On the other hand, intergenerational effects (lasting for one or two generations) are relatively common and can play an important role in evolutionary processes. However, we do not know whether intergenerational and transgenerational effects have same underlying mechanisms.

This study makes a big step towards resolving these questions and strongly advances our understanding of both phenomena. Much of the previous work on mechanisms of multigenerational effects has been conducted in C. elegans and this works uses the same approach. They focus on bacterial infection, Microsporidia infection, larval starvation and osmotic stress. I did not quite understand why the authors chose to focus on P. vranovensis rather than P. aeruginosa P14 that has been used in previous studies of transgenerational effects in C. elegans. However, this is a minor point because I guess they were interested in broad transgenerational responses to bacterial infection rather than in strain-specific ones. The authors used different Caenorhabditis species, which is another strength of this study in addition to using multiple stresses.

We thank the reviewer for these comments. We’d like to briefly highlight that P. vranovensis was also shown to elicit the same transgenerational effects as P. aeruginosa in the bioRxiv version of the same papers that reported transgenerational effects for P. aeruginosa (Kaletsky et al., 2020 – GRb0427 is an isolate of P. vranovensis).

It is not clear to us why this result was not included in the final published version of this manuscript, but we in fact used P. vranovensis for these studies in part because of this bioRxiv paper and because we failed to detect any robust intergenerational effects using P. aeruginosa PA14 in any of our assays – including at the RNA-seq level (unpublished).

Nonetheless, we have since confirmed with Coleen Murphy’s lab that they do find P. vranovensis elicits the same transgenerational effect on behaviour as P. aeruginosa. We expect that future investigations into the conditions under which P. vranovensis elicits effects that are lost/erased after 1 generation and the conditions under which effects might be maintained for more than 3 generations will be highly interesting.

They found 279 genes that exhibited intergenerational changes in all C species tested, but most interestingly, they show that a reversal in gene expression corresponds to a reversal in response to bacterial infection (beneficial in two species and deleterious on one). This is very intriguing! This was further supported by similar observations of osmotic stress response.

We thank Reviewer 2 for their excitement, and we agree that these findings were highly exciting.

They also report that intergenerational effects are stress-specific and there have deleterious effects in mismatched environments, and, importantly, when worms were subject to multiple stresses. It is quite likely that offspring will experience a range of environments and that several environmental stresses will be present simultaneously in nature. I really liked this aspect of this work as I think that tests in different environments, especially environments with multiple stresses, are often lacking, which limits the generality of the conclusions.

Another interesting piece of the puzzle is that beneficial and deleterious effects could be mediated by the same mechanisms. It would be interesting to explore this further. However, this is not a real criticism of this work. I think that the authors collected an impressive dataset already and every good study generates new research questions.

Given these findings, I was particularly keen to see what comes of transgenerational effects. The general answer was that there aren't many, and the authors conclude that all intergenerational effects that they studied are largely reversible and that intergenerational and transgenerational effects represent distinct phenomena. While I think that this is a very important finding, I am not sure whether we can conclude that intergenerational and transgenerational effects are not related.

In my view, an alternative interpretation is that intergenerational effects are common while transgenerational effects are rare. Because intergenerational effects are stress-specific, transgenerational effects could be stress-specific as well.

We agree with reviewer 2 that our findings suggest that intergenerational effects are common and transgenerational effects are either rare in comparison or only occur under specific conditions. We have updated the text to include this interpretation.

Perhaps different mechanisms regulate intergenerational responses to, say, different forms of starvation (e.g. compare opposing transgenerational responses to prolonged larval starvation (Rechavi et al. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2014.06.020) and rather short adulthood starvation (Ivimey-Cook et al. 2021 https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.0701)). Perhaps some (most?) forms of starvation generate only intergenerational responses and do not generate transgenerational responses. But some do. Those forms of starvation that generate both intergenerational and transgenerational effects could do so via same mechanisms and represent the same phenomenon. I am by no means saying this is the case, but I am not sure that the absence of evidence of transgenerational effects in this study necessarily suggests that inter- and trans-generational effects are different phenomena.

We agree and, similar to above, have updated the text accordingly to state that it is also very possible that transgenerational effects only occur under certain conditions.

The only concern real concern was the lack of phenotypic data on F3 beyond gene expression. Ideally, I would like to see tests of pathogen avoidance and starvation resistance in F3. However, given the amount of work that went into this study, the lack of strong signature of potential transgenerational effects in gene expression, and the fact that most of these effects were shown previously to last only one generation, I do not think this is crucial.

It would be very interesting to compare gene expression and other phenotypic responses in F1 and F3 between P. vranovensis and PA14. Also, it would be interesting to test the adaptive value of intergenerational and transgenerational effects after exposure to both strains in different environments. This is would be very informative and help with understanding the evolutionary significance of transgenerational epigenetic inheritance of pathogen avoidance as reported previously. Why response to P. vranovensis is erased while response to PA14 is maintained for four generations? Are nematodes more likely to encounter one species than the other? Again, however, this is not something necessary for this study.

We completely agree with Reviewer 2 and have indeed attempted these experiments both in Burton et al., 2020 and in unpublished results.

With regards to the transgenerational F3 effects, as mentioned above, P. vranovensis has been reported to elicit the same transgenerational effect as P. aeruginosa PA14 – at least as reported in the Kaletsky et al., 2020 bioRxiv version of the manuscript from the same studies. (GRb0427 is an isolate of P. vranovensis).

To date, however, in our laboratory we have been unable to detect any transgenerational effects for either P. vranovensis or P. aeruginosa infection on gene expression data from RNA-seq experiments (data from this manuscript and unpublished data).

It is not yet clear why this is the case, but we note that the RNA-seq analysis from the transgenerational PA14 studies (published in Moore et al., Cell 2019) was performed on F1 animals and thus was looking at intergenerational effects – to our knowledge no RNA-seq on F3 progeny from animals exposed to PA14 has ever been published. Thus, as it stands there is no existing F3 gene expression studies done using PA14 for us to compare our results to, but it remains possible that PA14 does not elicit specific effects on F3 gene expression when analyzed by RNA-seq.

For F1 effects we have published a gene expression comparison for P. vranovensis and P. aeruginosa F1 effects in a previous manuscript (Burton et al. 2020) and will add a mention of this to the text. Briefly, we detected very few F1 effects on gene expression when exposing adults to P. aeruginosa for 24 hours and parental infection by P. aeruginosa did not result in protection for offspring from P. vranovensis infection (Burton et al., 2020). We concluded that the intergenerational adaptation to P. vranovensis was not initiated by P. aeruginosa and was at least somewhat specific to P. vranovensis as well as the new species of Pseudomonas described in this manuscript which does cross protect.

The main strengths of this paper are (i) use of multiple stresses; (ii) use of multiple species; (iii) tests in different environments; and (iv) simultaneous evaluation of intergenerational and transgenerational responses. This study is first of a kind, and it provides several important answers, while highlighting clear paths for future work.

Excellent work and I think it will generate a lot of interest in the community, definitely want to see it published in eLife.

We agree with Reviewer 2 and thank them for their kind comments.

I am not fully convinced about their interpretation of whether inter- transgenerational effects are separate phenomena but happy to be convinced otherwise.

We will update the text of the manuscript to highlight that it is also possible that transgenerational effects represent effects that are comparatively rare when compared to intergenerational effects as suggested by Reviewer 2 as we agree this is a valid and potentially even likely interpretation.

Reviewer #3:

In this manuscript, the authors address whether the mechanisms mediating intergenerational effects are conserved in evolution. This question is important not only to frame this phenomenon in an evolutionary context, but to address several interlinked questions: is there a mechanism in common between adaptive versus deleterious effects? What makes some effects last one instead of several generations? What is the ecological relevance for those mechanisms? Using Caenorhabditis elegans as a model of reference, they compare four types of intergenerational effects on additional three Caenorhabditis species.

The authors used previously characterized models of intergenerational inheritance, focusing on those that are likely to have adaptive significance. This is relevant, because the adaptive relevance of other published examples of inter- and transgenerational inheritance is not clear. They used functional studies to probe for conservation of mechanisms for bacterial infection and resistance to osmolarity stress, which is a major strength of this study. The data supports the claim of conservation in some types of intergenerational inheritance and divergence in others. One major question addressed in this manuscript is whether there is a potential overarching mechanism that confers stress-resistance across generations. Their experiments convincingly show that this is not the case, but that instead, there are stress-specific mechanisms responsible for intergenerational inheritance.

We agree and thank Reviewer 3 for their kind comments.

The authors highlighted the discovery of 279 highly conserved genes that exhibited intergenerational in gene expression. The manuscript would be strengthened if these genes were shown to have functional relevance.

We note that, in line with Reviewer 1s comments, the revised manuscript has added a >2-fold change filter to call significant gene expression changes. Due to this enhanced filter there are now 37 genes, instead of 279, that change in all species tested. In addition to these 37 genes, we have also identified 19 additional genes that specifically change >2-fold in species that adapt to P. vranovensis infection but do not change, or change in opposite directions, in species that do not adapt to this pathogen. Furthermore, we identified four genes that change in species that adapt to osmotic stress but not in those that do not adapt.

With regards to functional relevance, we found that all three genes previously identified as functionally required for the adaptation to P. vranovensis (cysl-1, cysl-2, and rhy-1) appear among the newly revised list of 19 genes that specifically change in species that adapt (C. elegans and C. kamaaina) and not others that exhibit a deleterious intergenerational effect (C. briggsae). Similarly, among the four genes that specifically change in species that adapt to osmotic stress (elegans, briggsae and kamaaina) is gpdh-1 which has previously been found to be functionally involved in adapting to osmotic stress. These data indicate that our newly revised lists generated by pairing comparative transcriptomics with our phenotypic analysis across species are highly enriched in functionally relevant genes (p < 1.337e-08 – hypergeometric probability).

We think testing the functional relevance of the remaining 19 genes on these lists will be very exciting but we have not included such analysis in this study for several reasons including the fact that many of these genes cannot be tested in the same way as cysl-1/cysl-2/rhy-1 because they are either required for embryonic viability and thus no viable mutants exist (imb-1, xpo-2, and cdc-25.1) or because they exhibit consistently decreased expression across all species (for example, daf-18) which is unlike cysl-1/cysl-2/rhy-1 which exhibit increased expression and thus could behave in phenotypically distinct ways rather than simply being required for the adaptation.

In addition, we felt such analysis was not required for, and might potentially distract from, the main aims of this study to focus on the study of the evolutionary conservation, tradeoffs, and specificity of intergenerational responses.

It is interesting that C. elegans can pass the P. vavronensis resistance to the offspring, but not C. briggsae. Is this because C. briggsae is already expressing cysl-1 in the parental generation? What would happen if cysl-1 would be knocked out in this species? Would it lack resistance to the pathogen?

We agree with reviewer 3 that given the requirement of cysl-1 in both C. elegans and C. kamaaina for the intergenerational adaptation to P. vranovensis, it would be interesting to see if increased cysl-1 expression might explain the increased naïve resistance of C. briggsae. We have now created a cysl-1 KO in C. briggsae. We found that cysl-1 C. briggsae mutants remain resistant to P. vranovensis even in the absence of cysl-1.

It is likely that other genes also act as modifiers of the response to P. vranovensis and cysl-1 activity alone is not the sole determinant of resistance in C. briggsae. Consistent with this hypothesis we have recently performed screens on C. elegans cysl-1 and cysl-2 mutants and identified mutations that render C. elegans resistant to P. vranovensis even in the absence of cysl-1 (unpublished).

As this result does not affect the conclusions of this manuscript and because the reason for C. briggsae’s naïve resistance to P. vranovensis remains unknown we have not included this result in this version of the manuscript but are happy to include it if the reviewers found it appropriate.

The authors showed that C. elegans and C. briggsae seem to share intergenerational effects of resistance to N. parisii in other species. It would be interesting to know if the mechanisms are conserved, but unfortunately this information is lacking. Similarly, it would be interesting to know if gdph-2 mediates resistance to osmolarity stress in C. kamaaina.

This new manuscript now includes RNAi data demonstrating that gpdh-2 is also required in C. kamaaina for proper adaptation to osmotic stress (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D).

The exact mechanism by which C. elegans adapts to N. parisii infection is not yet known so we were unable to directly test if the mechanism is conserved in C. briggsae. We have, however, previously found that loss of parental lin-35 can promote offspring resistance to N. parisii (Willis et al., 2021). We therefore created a lin-35 deletion mutant in C. briggsae to test if this same effect occurred in C. briggsae but this mutant was incredibly sick and was lost after only a few generations when no more fertile offspring were produced. We have therefore been unable to test this role of lin-35 in C. briggsae.

The supplementary tables would be more useful if the three-letter name would be included as well.

We agree and have updated the supplementary tables to include this.

Indicate in Figure 3C that these are RNAi experiments.

Done.

Also for Figure 3C, spell out "EV" in the legend of the figure. I suppose it means 'empty vector'?

EV does mean Empty Vector and we have updated the figure to now say “Empty Vector”.

The font size in the figures is pretty small and therefore difficult to read.

We have increased the font size in the figures.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Burton N, Price J, Braukmann F, Miska E. 2021. Parental exposure to environmental stress results in evolutionarily conserved intergenerational changes in offspring gene expression. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus. GSE173987
    2. Boeck M. 2016. The time-resolved transcriptome of C. elegans. NCBI Sequence Read Archive - Supplemental Table 1. PMC5052054

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Statistics source data for Figure 1.
    elife-73425-fig1-data1.xlsx (745.1KB, xlsx)
    Figure 3—source data 1. Statistics source data for Figure 3.
    Figure 4—source data 1. Statistics source data for Figure 4.
    Supplementary file 1. List of 7587 single-copy orthologous genes conserved among C. elegans, C. briggsae, C. kamaaina, and C. tropicalis.
    elife-73425-supp1.xlsx (354.2KB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 2. Expression of single-copy orthologous genes in F1 progeny of animals exposed to P. vranovensis.
    elife-73425-supp2.xlsx (6.5MB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 3. Expression of single-copy orthologous genes in F1 progeny of animals exposed to osmotic stress.
    elife-73425-supp3.xlsx (7.2MB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 4. Expression of single-copy orthologous genes in F3 progeny of animals exposed to P. vranovensis and osmotic stress.
    elife-73425-supp4.xlsx (4.5MB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 5. List of bacteria isolated from United Kingdom.
    elife-73425-supp5.xlsx (61.7KB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 6. PCR sequences of Pseudomonas 15C5 16 S rRNA and rpoD.
    elife-73425-supp6.txt (1.6KB, txt)
    Supplementary file 7. Expression of single-copy orthologous genes in F1 progeny of C. elegans exposed to P. vranovensis, Pseudomonas sp. 15C5, Serratia plymuthica BUR1537, or Aeromonas sp. BIGb0469.
    elife-73425-supp7.xlsx (20.5MB, xlsx)
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    RNA-seq data that support the findings of this study have been deposited at NCBI GEO and are available under the accession code GSE173987.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Burton N, Price J, Braukmann F, Miska E. 2021. Parental exposure to environmental stress results in evolutionarily conserved intergenerational changes in offspring gene expression. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus. GSE173987

    The following previously published datasets were used:

    Boeck M. 2016. The time-resolved transcriptome of C. elegans. NCBI Sequence Read Archive - Supplemental Table 1. PMC5052054


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES