Abstract
One hallmark feature of Parkinson’s disease (PD) is Lewy body pathology associated with misfolded alpha-synuclein. Previous studies have shown that striatal injection of alpha-synuclein preformed fibrils (PFF) can induce misfolding and aggregation of native alpha-synuclein in a prion-like manner, leading to cell death and motor dysfunction in mouse models. Here, we tested whether alpha-synuclein PFFs injected into the medial prefrontal cortex results in deficits in interval timing, a cognitive task which is disrupted in human PD patients and in rodent models of PD. We injected PFF or monomers of human alpha-synuclein into the medial prefrontal cortex of mice pre-injected with adeno-associated virus (AAV) coding for overexpression of human alpha-synuclein or control protein. Despite notable medial prefrontal cortical synucleinopathy, we did not observe consistent deficits in fixed-interval timing. These results suggest that cortical alpha-synuclein does not reliably disrupt fixed-interval timing.
Keywords: alpha-synuclein, preformed fibrils, prion, interval timing
Introduction:
Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a devastating neurodegenerative disorder affecting both movement and cognitive function. PD is characterized by Lewy body pathology, which consists of alpha-synuclein aggregation as well as progressive neurodegeneration of the dopaminergic and cholinergic systems[31, 35, 42]. This synucleinopathy can affect cognitive abilities as patients with cognitive impairment often have Lewy bodies and Lewy neurites in the frontal and temporal cortex[5]. Whereas motor symptoms of PD can be managed by dopaminergic medications and deep brain stimulation, cognitive issues related to PD are more difficult to treat[33]. The only FDA-approved medication for symptomatic treatment of cognitive issues in PD is rivastigmine, an acetylcholinesterase inhibitor[42].
One reliable approach to inducing synucleinopathy in rodent models is via preformed fibrils (PFF) of alpha-synuclein. These are a specific alpha-synuclein fibrillar conformation that may facilitate misfolding and aggregation of alpha-synuclein monomers. PFFs can act like prions in animal models in that they trigger native alpha-synuclein to fold abnormally and can spread trans-synaptically within neuronal circuits [1, 18, 19, 22–24, 38]. Furthermore, when human alpha-synuclein is overexpressed using adeno-associated virus (AAV) in mice, alpha-synuclein PFF pathology progresses more rapidly[36]. This model can produce reliable cellular and motor deficits when injected in mice[8]. However, studies have reported mixed results regarding cognitive deficits[8, 19].
We investigated cognitive function by injecting human alpha-synuclein PFF bilaterally into the mouse medial prefrontal cortex. To test cognitive function, we used interval timing, which is reliably disrupted in human PD patients and rodent models[6, 25, 33], and depends on the medial prefrontal cortex[12, 21, 28, 40]. We used a fixed-interval timing task that requires mice to estimate an interval of 12 seconds and requires working memory for temporal rules and attention to the passage of time[31]. Critically, we have found that fixed-interval timing is reliably disrupted in rodent PD models, including VTA dopaminergic depletion and dopaminergic disruptions in medial prefrontal cortex [7, 17, 20, 28, 32, 40, 43]. Here, we tested the specific hypothesis that cortical alpha-synuclein PFFs disrupt fixed-interval timing in mice. Our findings provide insight into cortical alpha-synuclein PFFs in rodent PD models.
Materials and Methods:
Mice:
Fourteen wild-type (WT) C57/BL6J male mice (000664; Jackson Laboratories) were used in this study. All procedures were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee (#707239) at the University of Iowa, in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.
Alpha-synuclein overexpression:
Human alpha-synuclein PFF and monomer solutions were injected into the medial prefrontal cortex of 9-month-old mice. At 3 months, animals were pre-injected with a viral vector designed to overexpress either human alpha-synuclein (hSNCA) or control protein (green fluorescent protein, or GFP) in targeted regions. Both vectors (CAG-hSNCA-WPRE and CAG-GFP-WPRE) were driven by CAG promoters with recombinant AAV2/6 vectors (serotype 2 genome/serotype 6 capsid), produced and purified by Vector BioLabs (Philadelphia, PA, USA). Titers of AAV were ~1013 GC/ml by qPCR. Alpha-synuclein PFFs and monomers were a gift from the West lab and made according to previously published protocols[1]. Briefly, human alpha-synuclein, encoded in the inducible bacterial expression plasmid pRK172 and purified by maxi-prep (Qiagen), was transformed into chemically-competent BL21 (DE3) Codon Plus cells (Clontech). A single colony was selected for growth in terrific broth (Fisher) supplemented with 100 μg/mL ampicillin (Sigma). Growth was monitored to log-phase protein production induced by isopropyl β-D-1-thiogalactopyranoside (IPTG) for 2 hours at 37°C, and pellets were collected at OD600=0.80. Bacterial pastes were weighed and ~5 g were combined with 25 mL of lysis buffer (750 mM NaCl, 10 mM Tris (pH 7.6), 1 mM EDTA, 1 mM PMSF, and 1× bacterial protease inhibitor cocktail, RPI). Homogenates were sonicated at 70% power (Fisher Model 500 Dismembrator) for 1 minute 20 seconds (5-second sonication pulses with 25 seconds of off time, on ice), and tubes were placed in boiling water for 15 minutes. After ~15 minutes on ice, samples were centrifuged for 25 minutes at ~10,000g for 25 minutes at 4°C, twice. Supernatants were loaded into SnakeSkin dialysis tubing (3.5 kDa MWCO, Fisher) and placed into 4 L of ice-cold buffer (10 mM Tris (pH 7.6) with 50 mM NaCl, 1 mM EDTA, PMSF), stirring overnight. Next, supernatants were centrifuged at 100,000 g for 1 h at 4°C, and concentrated to ~5 mL using Amicon Ultra-15 3.5 kDa MWCO columns. The concentrate was then passed through a HiLoad 16/600 Superdex Column, 1 ×120 mL (GE Healthcare) with running buffer (10 mM Tris (pH 7.6) with 50 mM NaCl, 1 mM EDTA). Fractions containing alpha-synuclein protein, as assessed by SDS-PAGE and Coomassie stain, were combined and loaded into SnakeSkin dialysis tubing (5 kDa MWCO, Fisher) and placed into 4 L of ice-cold buffer (10 mM Tris (pH 7.6) with 25 mM NaCl, 1 mM EDTA, PMSF), stirring overnight. After dialysis the fractions were loaded at one time into a HiPrep Q HP 16/10 Column, 1 × 20 mL (GE Healthcare) with loading buffer (10 mM Tris (pH 7.6), 25 mM NaCl, l mM EDTA, 1 mM PMSF) and eluted with a gradient application of high-salt buffer (10 mM Tris (pH 7.6), 1 M NaCl, 1 mM EDTA, 1 mM PMSF). Samples containing alpha-synuclein were identified by SDS-PAGE and Coomassie blot, and dialyzed into the same dialysis buffer (excluding PMSF and EDTA) and concentrated as before. Concentration of protein (monomer, undiluted) was determined by BCA assay (Pierce), and purity was assessed by SDS-PAGE and Coomassie blot. Monomer protein diluted to a 5mg/mL solution of human alpha-synuclein protein (buffered with 50 mM Tris (pH 7.5) and 150 mM KCl) in 1.5-mL polypropylene centrifuge tubes were shaken at 700 RPM in an Eppendorf thermomixer for 7 days at 37°C. The solution becomes turbid during the incubation. For sonication of the fibrils, a 1/8-inch probe tip was inserted to a few millimeters from the bottom of the tube, and 30% power (Fisher Scientific Sonic Dismembrator FB120110) was applied for the indicated time (15–240 seconds) in 15-second intervals with 1 sonication pulse and a 1-second wait. Between 15-second intervals, fibrils rested in a metal block at room temperature for 2 minutes to dissipate heat in the solution. Since sonication aerosolizes fibrils, all sonication steps were performed in a biosafety-level-2 cabinet, with the operator wearing disposable wrist guards and double gloves. The sonicated fibrils were then aliquoted and frozen in liquid nitrogen at −80°C, and thawed immediately prior to experiments. For all procedures, monomer alpha-synuclein and fibrillar alpha-synuclein preparations were kept on ice. Cleanup and inactivation of the fibrils on contaminated surfaces was accomplished using a 1%-SDS solution as described[4, 37]. Antibody against tyrosine hydroxylase was purchased from Millipore (AB152). Antibody against phosphorylated alpha-synuclein (p-S129) was purchased from abcam (ab51253). Secondary antibodies (goat anti-rabbit IgG and goat anti-mouse IgG) conjugated to Alexa Fluor 488 and 568, were purchased from Thermo Fisher.
Mouse fixed-interval timing:
At 3 months, mice were trained to perform a fixed interval-timing task with a 12-second interval[20, 41]. Operant chambers (MedAssociates) were equipped with a nosepoke response port, a yellow LED stimulus light (ENV-313W), a pellet dispenser (ENV-203-20), and a house light (ENV-315W). Behavioral chambers were housed in sound-attenuating chambers (MedAssociates). All behavioral responses, including nosepokes and access to pellet receptacles, were recorded with infrared sensors. First, animals learned to make nosepokes to receive rewards (20-mg rodent purified pellets, F0071, Bio-Serv). After fixed-ratio training (FR1), animals were trained in a 12-second fixed-interval timing task in which rewards were delivered for the first nosepoke after a 12-second interval (Figure 2A). The house light was turned on to signal the start of the 12-second interval. Early responses were not rewarded. Responses after 12 seconds resulted in termination of the trial and delivery of the reward. Rewarded nosepokes were signaled by turning off the house light. Each trial was followed by a 30±6-second pseudorandom inter-trial interval that concluded with the house light turning on, signaling the beginning of the next trial. All sessions were 60 minutes long. Mice consumed 1–1.5 g of sucrose pellets during each behavioral session, and additional food was provided 1–2 hours after each behavioral session in the home cage. Single housing and a 12-hour light/dark cycle were used; all experiments took place during the light cycle. For motivation, mice were maintained at 80%–85% of their baseline body weight during the course of these experiments. Responses were summed into time-response histograms with 1-second bins, 0–18 seconds after the trial start. Plotting was done using the MATLAB function ksdensity.m to estimate the probability-density function of time-response histograms with a bandwidth of 1, normalized to maximum response rate and averaged across animals. We analyzed 10 days of fixed-interval timing performance and quantified behavior by these metrics: 1) the curvature of time-response histograms (1 for a positive curve, and 0 for a flat line[12, 15, 20, 28, 41]); 2) the “start” time of response, calculated using single-trial analysis of trial-by-trial changes between low and high response rates[9, 13] (1–12 seconds); 3) the coefficient of variance (CV) of the start time; and 4) the aggregate number of responses. The median of each of these metrics was calculated over 10 days of behavior and compared between groups by a non-parametric Wilcoxon test.
Novel object recognition task:
Mice were habituated in the arena (40cm × 40cm) for 10 minutes in day one, followed by another hour of habituation in day 2. After habituation on day 2, two identical objects were placed into the arena, and mice were brought into the arena and were allowed to explore the objects for 10 minutes. One hour later, one of the objects is replaced with a novel object and mice were brought into the arena for another 10 minutes. The time spent exploring the familiar and the novel object is recorded, and a discrimination index was calculated as (NT−FT)/(NT+FT), where NT represents time spent with the novel object, and FT represents time spent with the familiar object. Objects and the arena were cleaned by 70% alcohol between each session of exploration. The objects deployed for mice were counterbalanced.
Open field:
Mice were placed in a clean open field container (40 × 40 cm) surrounded by plexiglass walls in a quiet, well-lit room. Motor and exploratory activity was video recorded and tracked for 10 min using the ANY-maze software (Wood Dale, IL 60191). Total distance travelled, and total time spent in the center area and in the areas close to the walls were calculated by the ANY-maze software. Thigmotaxis was calculated by dividing the total amount of time spent in the areas close to the wall by the time spent in both regions (edge and the center area). The container was sanitized with 70% alcohol between each trial and allowed to air dry between each trial.
Stereotaxic surgical procedures:
After 3-month-old mice were trained in the 12-second fixed-interval timing task, they received stereotactical injections of either AAV-hSNCA or AAV-GFP targeting bilateral medial prefrontal cortex, ventral tegmental area (VTA), and basal forebrain. Briefly, mice were anesthetized using ketamine (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg). A surgical level of anesthesia was maintained, with ketamine supplements (10 mg/kg) given hourly (or as needed) and regular monitoring for stable respiratory rate and absent toe pinch response. Mice were placed in the stereotactic equipment with non-rupturing ear bars. A heating pad was used to prevent hypothermia. Under aseptic surgical conditions, the skull was leveled between bregma and lambda. A Hamilton syringe was lowered to the target areas at the rate of 1 mm/minute, and after a 5-minute delay, AAV was injected slowly at the rate of 0.1 ul/min (0.5 ul per injection site: coordinates from bregma: medial prefrontal cortex: AP: +1.8, ML ± 0.5, DV −1.8; VTA: AP: +3.3, ML ± 1.1, DV −4.6 at 10 degree angled; basal forebrain: AP: −0.7, ML ± 1.8, DV −4.0 and 5.0). Ten minutes after the injection was completed, the syringe was pulled out slowly (2 mm/minute). Following this AAV injection, a second surgery was performed 6 months later to inject either alpha-synuclein PFFs or alpha-synuclein monomers into bilateral medial prefrontal cortex (2 μl per injection site (5mg/ml): coordinates from the bregma: medial prefrontal cortex: AP: +1.8, ML ± 0.5, DV −1.8). Mice that were pre-injected with AAV-hSNCA received PFFs, while mice pre-injected with AAV-GFP received monomers.
Histology:
At 15 months when experiments were complete, mice were euthanized by injections of 100 mg/kg ketamine. All mice were intracardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde. The brain was removed and post-fixed in paraformaldehyde overnight and immersed in 30% sucrose until the brains sank. Sections (40 μm) were made on a cryostat (Leica) and stored in cryoprotectant (50% PBS, 30% ethylene glycol, 20% glycerol) at −20°C. For immunohistochemistry, free-floating sections were washed with PBS then PBST (0.03% Triton-X100 in PBS) and incubated in 2% normal goat serum for 1 hour at room temperature, followed by primary antibody incubation at 4°C overnight. After washing with PBST, sections were incubated with secondary antibody for 1 hour. After washing with PBST, then PBS, sections were mounted onto slides with mounting media containing DAPI (Invitrogen P36962). Sections were stained for phosphorylated synuclein at the serine-129 residue (which is linked with enhanced pathogenicity, Abcam, ab51253), tyrosine hydroxylase (Millpore-AB152), and choline-acetyl-transferase (Millipore, AB144P). Images were captured on an Olympus VS120 Microscope. All fluorescent immunohistochemistry images were pseudocolored to represent multiple fluorophores.
Statistics:
All procedures were reviewed by the Biomedical Research and Epidemiology Core. Central tendency was reported as median, and variability was reported by interquartile range. Comparisons were made by non-parametric Wilcoxon rank sum tests. The null hypothesis was tested with Bayes Factor[27].
Results
Alpha-synuclein PFFs in medial prefrontal cortex
We injected 9-month-old mice with human alpha-synuclein PFFs into the medial prefrontal cortex. Of note, at 3 months old, mice were pre-injected with AAV-hSNCA into the medial prefrontal cortex, VTA, and basal forebrain, which are areas of prominent synuclein pathology in humans. Controls received injections of AAV-GFP in the same locations at 3 months, and alpha-synuclein monomers into medial prefrontal cortex at 9 months. Phosphorylated-alpha-synuclein- (p-syn; S129) positive aggregates with fibrillar morphology were found around the PFF injection site in bilateral medial prefrontal cortex (Figure 1B, C, J), with spread to neighboring prefrontal regions in most mice. We confirmed bilateral pathology at the injection site in all PFF-injected mice. There was no evidence of p-syn-positive cells with fibrillar pathology in control mice (Figure 1D, E). In some mice, we found p-syn-positive cells in the basal forebrain where we overexpressed human alpha-synuclein though AAV, but there were no somas with clear fibrillar pathology (Figure 1L). In the VTA and substantia nigra, we found scattered cells with fibrillar p-synuclein (0–2 positive soma per section) (Figure 1F). Given the lack of significant fibril spread to this region, we stained for tyrosine hydroxylase in two mice per group. As expected, we saw no gross differences in TH staining in these mice (Figure 1G, H), in line with the lack of significant fibrillar spread to this region. Unexpectedly, we found moderate amounts of p-synuclein in a fibrillar pattern in somas and neurites in the temporal cortex, including the entorhinal cortex (Figure 1I). These regions were not preinjected with virus coding for human alpha-synuclein. We also found soma with fibrillar staining in the striatum, though the density was more limited compared with the medial frontal cortex (Figure 1K).
Cortical alpha-synuclein PFFs do not affect interval timing
Because injection of PFFs into the striatum can disrupt motor behaviors[16], we tested the hypothesis that cortical PFFs can disrupt cognitive behaviors. We turned to interval timing, which is reliably disrupted in human PD patients[30, 33], correlated with cognitive dysfunction in these patients, and disrupted in two rodent PD models that disrupt the medial prefrontal cortex[20, 32]. One reliable metric of interval timing that is disrupted by dopaminergic manipulation is the curvature of time-response histograms, which uses the cumulative distribution to measure deviations from a flat line, ranging from −1 to 1, with 0 being a flat line[28]. Strikingly, we found that despite observing clear synucleinopathy in the medial prefrontal cortex, there was no consistent deficit in the curvature of time-response histograms (Median(IQR): Control 0.18(0.17–0.22), PFF 0.18(0.14–0.24); p>0.99; Bayes Factor 2.8), start times (Control 5.5(5.1–6.0), PFF 6.0(5.3–6.0); p=0.7; Bayes Factor 2.5), start time CVs (Control 0.32(0.30–0.33), PFF 0.34(0.33–0.36); p=0.10; Bayes Factor 1.4), or nosepokes (Control 190(96–272), PFF 148(92–192); p=0.71; Bayes Factor 2.0) (Figure 2). There was no reliable difference in any metric of timing that we could measure. These data provide evidence that fibrillar synuclein in the medial prefrontal cortex does not reliably impair fixed-interval timing in mice. Furthermore, we found no consistent differences in NOR behavior (Discrimination Index, Median(IQR): Control 0.03(−0.02–0.27), PFF −0.22(−0.37–0.09); p=0.46; Bayes Factor 2.2; Figure 3A) [18], or the open-field assay (Ratio of the time spent close to wall as an indicator for thigmotaxis, Median(IQR): Control 0.92(0.84–0.98), PFF 0.89(0.75–0.97); p=0.62; Bayes Factor 2.0; Figure 3B; Total distance travelled, Median(IQR): Control 21m(12–25), PFF 18m(13–20); p=0.80; Bayes Factor 2.7; Figure 3C).
Discussion
We tested the hypothesis that medial prefrontal cortical alpha-synuclein PFFs affect interval-timing in rodent models. Despite evident synucleinopathy in this region, we did not find consistent evidence of deficits in a fixed-interval timing task that is reliably impaired in rodent PD models. We note that interval timing is reliably impaired in humans with PD, and in several animal models of PD, and depends on medial prefrontal cortex[31, 43]. Recent work from our group found that medicated PD patients have increased variance of responses during interval timing[6, 43]. Furthermore, past work has consistently shown that disrupting midbrain dopamine[20, 26, 28, 34], cortical dopamine[29, 30], or striatal dopamine reliably impairs interval timing[10, 11]. Given these data, it is notable that mice with cortical PFFs have no measurable timing impairments.
There are at least three possibilities for our results. First, cortical cells may be less vulnerable to alpha-synuclein fibrils compared with other brain regions. Blumenstock et al. showed abnormal morphology in cortical pyramidal cells following PFF injections in the striatum after five months[3]. In that study, fibrils were derived from mouse alpha-synuclein, which has been shown in some cases to cause more spread and pathology compared to human fibrils used in our study[1]. Although we did see extensive alpha-synuclein pathology in the medial prefrontal cortex, it is possible that more widespread pathology in the cortex would be required to see a deficit in this task. For example, the specific population of cortical cells affected by PFFs in our study may not be the same population that is necessary for temporal control of action, or may not be sufficiently dysfunctional. Future studies evaluating distinct subpopulations may improve our understanding of the role of cortical subpopulations in this task. It is also possible that alpha-synuclein perturbations in the cortex act distinctly from subcortical alpha-synuclein[39]. Second, interval-timing deficits in highly trained mice may depend on pathology in subcortical projection neurons, such as dopaminergic projection neurons in the VTA, as we have shown previously[2, 20, 28]. Although prefrontal regions are critical for temporal control of action[43], other cortical or subcortical brain networks may compensate for cortical deficits in our study. Although we found spread to subcortical regions such as the striatum in our PFF injections, the most prominent fibrillar pathology was in the medial prefrontal cortex (Figure 1). In this model, there was almost no fibrillar staining of synuclein seen in the VTA and substantia nigra. Although it is possible that the apparent lack of spread could be secondary to cell loss, this is unlikely, given the lack of behavioral deficits or qualitative changes in TH staining. Future studies might disentangle these possibilities in describing the role of cortical alpha-synuclein in cognitive function.
Our study has limitations. First, our control animals received both AAV-GFP and human alpha-synuclein monomer injections, with no separate vehicle-only control. However, our curvature metrics from control mice closely match our prior work, suggesting these manipulations did not alter interval timing [20, 41]. Second, we did not extensively evaluate dopaminergic cell loss in this mouse model. As there was no behavioral difference between groups, we chose not to quantitatively evaluate dopaminergic cell loss in the VTA or substantia nigra. Qualitatively, there was no evidence of a substantial decrease in TH staining in the sections we examined, consistent with our hypothesis that dopaminergic cell loss is key to interval-timing deficits in mouse models of PD. Third, we only used one measure of timing (12-second fixed-interval timing). Other aspects of timing could be influenced by cortical alpha-synuclein pathology. Our injection model may not have induced sufficient synucleinopathy to impair medial prefrontal function (Figure 1). Finally, we only performed a limited number of tasks. We found no difference in overall distance traveled during open field, suggesting there was no difference in exploratory behavior/hyperactivity. We also found no difference in thigmotaxis, suggesting there was no difference in anxiety-like behavior between PFF and control mice. No significant difference in the novel object recognition assay was detected between PFF and control mice as well. While we include these data for completeness, our primary conclusions are drawn from interval timing in which control mice performed similarly to prior studies[14, 20, 41]. Further studies will be necessary to determine if other tasks mediated by cortex are influenced by cortical PFFs, or if deficits are more likely to be driven by selectively vulnerable cells in other brain regions.
Despite these limitations, our data provide evidence that injection of human alpha-synuclein PFFs into the medial prefrontal cortex does not reliably disrupt interval timing, which is essential for guiding the choice of injection site and fibril species in future investigations using alpha-synuclein models of synucleinopathy.
Highlights.
Cortical injection of alpha-synuclein preformed fibrils (PFF) induces diffuse synucleinopathy
Cortical injection of PFFs does not affect interval timing in mice
Medial prefrontal cortical synucleinopathy does not reliably disrupt interval timing
Acknowledgements:
QZ is supported by NIH/NINDS R25 NS079173, the NIH/NINDS NeuroNext Fellowship, and the physician scientist training program at University of Iowa. QZ is a trainee of the University of Iowa Clinical Neuroscientist Training Program (CNS-TP). NN is supported by NIH R01 NS100849-A1. GMA is supported by NIH K08 NS109287 and the Iowa Neuroscience Institute.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References:
- [1].Abdelmotilib H, Maltbie T, Delic V, Liu Z, Hu X, Fraser KB, Moehle MS, Stoyka L, Anabtawi N, Krendelchtchikova V, Volpicelli-Daley LA, West A, alpha-Synuclein fibril-induced inclusion spread in rats and mice correlates with dopaminergic Neurodegeneration, Neurobiol Dis 105 (2017) 84–98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Alberico SL, Cassell MD, Narayanan NS, The Vulnerable Ventral Tegmental Area in Parkinson’s Disease, Basal Ganglia 5 (2015) 51–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Blumenstock S, Rodrigues EF, Peters F, Blazquez-Llorca L, Schmidt F, Giese A, Herms J, Seeding and transgenic overexpression of alpha-synuclein triggers dendritic spine pathology in the neocortex, EMBO Mol Med 9 (2017) 716–731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Bousset L, Brundin P, Bockmann A, Meier B, Melki R, An Efficient Procedure for Removal and Inactivation of Alpha-Synuclein Assemblies from Laboratory Materials, J Parkinsons Dis 6 (2016) 143–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Braak H, Del Tredici K, Rub U, de Vos RA, Jansen Steur EN, Braak E, Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson’s disease, Neurobiol Aging 24 (2003) 197–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Buhusi CV, Meck WH, What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing, Nat Rev Neurosci 6 (2005) 755–765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Buhusi CV, Reyes MB, Gathers CA, Oprisan SA, Buhusi M, Inactivation of the Medial-Prefrontal Cortex Impairs Interval Timing Precision, but Not Timing Accuracy or Scalar Timing in a Peak-Interval Procedure in Rats, Front Integr Neurosci 12 (2018) 20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Chung HK, Ho HA, Perez-Acuna D, Lee SJ, Modeling alpha-Synuclein Propagation with Preformed Fibril Injections, J Mov Disord 12 (2019) 139–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Church RM, Meck WH, Gibbon J, Application of scalar timing theory to individual trials, J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 20 (1994) 135–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].De Corte BJ, Wagner LM, Matell MS, Narayanan NS, Striatal dopamine and the temporal control of behavior, Behav Brain Res 356 (2019) 375–379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Drew MR, Simpson EH, Kellendonk C, Herzberg WG, Lipatova O, Fairhurst S, Kandel ER, Malapani C, Balsam PD, Transient overexpression of striatal D2 receptors impairs operant motivation and interval timing, J Neurosci 27 (2007) 7731–7739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Emmons EB, De Corte BJ, Kim Y, Parker KL, Matell MS, Narayanan NS, Rodent Medial Frontal Control of Temporal Processing in the Dorsomedial Striatum, J Neurosci 37 (2017) 8718–8733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Emmons EB, Kennedy M, Kim Y, Narayanan NS, Corticostriatal stimulation compensates for medial frontal inactivation during interval timing, Sci Rep 9 (2019) 14371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Emmons EB, Ruggiero RN, Kelley RM, Parker KL, Narayanan NS, Corticostriatal Field Potentials Are Modulated at Delta and Theta Frequencies during Interval-Timing Task in Rodents, Front Psychol 7 (2016) 459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Fry W, Kelleher RT, Cook L, A mathematical index of performance on fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement, J Exp Anal Behav 3 (1960) 193–199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Henderson MX, Cornblath EJ, Darwich A, Zhang B, Brown H, Gathagan RJ, Sandler RM, Bassett DS, Trojanowski JQ, Lee VMY, Spread of alpha-synuclein pathology through the brain connectome is modulated by selective vulnerability and predicted by network analysis, Nat Neurosci 22 (2019) 1248–1257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Kim J, Ghim JW, Lee JH, Jung MW, Neural correlates of interval timing in rodent prefrontal cortex, J Neurosci 33 (2013) 13834–13847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Kim S, Kwon SH, Kam TI, Panicker N, Karuppagounder SS, Lee S, Lee JH, Kim WR, Kook M, Foss CA, Shen C, Lee H, Kulkarni S, Pasricha PJ, Lee G, Pomper MG, Dawson VL, Dawson TM, Ko HS, Transneuronal Propagation of Pathologic alpha-Synuclein from the Gut to the Brain Models Parkinson’s Disease, Neuron (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Kim S, Kwon SH, Kam TI, Panicker N, Karuppagounder SS, Lee S, Lee JH, Kim WR, Kook M, Foss CA, Shen C, Lee H, Kulkarni S, Pasricha PJ, Lee G, Pomper MG, Dawson VL, Dawson TM, Ko HS, Transneuronal Propagation of Pathologic alpha-Synuclein from the Gut to the Brain Models Parkinson’s Disease, Neuron 103 (2019) 627–641 e627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Kim YC, Han SW, Alberico SL, Ruggiero RN, De Corte B, Chen KH, Narayanan NS, Optogenetic Stimulation of Frontal D1 Neurons Compensates for Impaired Temporal Control of Action in Dopamine-Depleted Mice, Curr Biol 27 (2017) 39–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Kim YC, Narayanan NS, Prefrontal D1 Dopamine-Receptor Neurons and Delta Resonance in Interval Timing, Cereb Cortex 29 (2019) 2051–2060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Luk KC, Kehm V, Carroll J, Zhang B, O’Brien P, Trojanowski JQ, Lee VM, Pathological alpha-synuclein transmission initiates Parkinson-like neurodegeneration in nontransgenic mice, Science 338 (2012) 949–953. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Luk KC, Kehm VM, Zhang B, O’Brien P, Trojanowski JQ, Lee VM, Intracerebral inoculation of pathological alpha-synuclein initiates a rapidly progressive neurodegenerative alpha-synucleinopathy in mice, J Exp Med 209 (2012) 975–986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Luk KC, Song C, O’Brien P, Stieber A, Branch JR, Brunden KR, Trojanowski JQ, Lee VM, Exogenous alpha-synuclein fibrils seed the formation of Lewy body-like intracellular inclusions in cultured cells, Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106 (2009) 20051–20056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Malapani C, Rakitin B, Levy R, Meck WH, Deweer B, Dubois B, Gibbon J, Coupled temporal memories in Parkinson’s disease: a dopamine-related dysfunction, J Cogn Neurosci 10 (1998) 316–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Meck WH, Neuroanatomical localization of an internal clock: a functional link between mesolimbic, nigrostriatal, and mesocortical dopaminergic systems, Brain Res 1109 (2006) 93–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].Morey RD, Rouder JN, Bayes factor approaches for testing interval null hypotheses, Psychol Methods 16 (2011) 406419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Narayanan NS, Land BB, Solder JE, Deisseroth K, DiLeone RJ, Prefrontal D1 dopamine signaling is required for temporal control, Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109 (2012) 20726–20731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Parker KL, Chen KH, Kingyon JR, Cavanagh JF, Narayanan NS, D1-dependent 4 Hz oscillations and ramping activity in rodent medial frontal cortex during interval timing, J Neurosci 34 (2014) 16774–16783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Parker KL, Chen KH, Kingyon JR, Cavanagh JF, Narayanan NS, Medial frontal approximately 4-Hz activity in humans and rodents is attenuated in PD patients and in rodents with cortical dopamine depletion, J Neurophysiol 114 (2015) 1310–1320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Parker KL, Lamichhane D, Caetano MS, Narayanan NS, Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease and timing deficits, Front Integr Neurosci 7 (2013) 75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Parker KL, Ruggiero RN, Narayanan NS, Infusion of D1 Dopamine Receptor Agonist into Medial Frontal Cortex Disrupts Neural Correlates of Interval Timing, Front Behav Neurosci 9 (2015) 294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Singh A, Cole RC, Espinoza AI, Evans A, Cao S, Cavanagh JF, Narayanan NS, Timing variability and midfrontal ~4 Hz rhythms correlate with cognition in Parkinson’s disease, NPJ Parkinsons Dis 7 (2021) 14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Soares S, Atallah BV, Paton JJ, Midbrain dopamine neurons control judgment of time, Science 354 (2016) 1273–1277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].Spillantini MG, Schmidt ML, Lee VM, Trojanowski JQ, Jakes R, Goedert M, Alpha-synuclein in Lewy bodies, Nature 388 (1997) 839–840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Thakur P, Breger LS, Lundblad M, Wan OW, Mattsson B, Luk KC, Lee VMY, Trojanowski JQ, Bjorklund A, Modeling Parkinson’s disease pathology by combination of fibril seeds and alpha-synuclein overexpression in the rat brain, Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114 (2017) E8284–E8293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Thomzig A, Wagenfuhr K, Daus ML, Joncic M, Schulz-Schaeffer WJ, Thanheiser M, Mielke M, Beekes M, Decontamination of medical devices from pathological amyloid-beta-, tau- and alpha-synuclein aggregates, Acta Neuropathol Commun 2 (2014) 151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Volpicelli-Daley LA, Luk KC, Patel TP, Tanik SA, Riddle DM, Stieber A, Meaney DF, Trojanowski JQ, Lee VM, Exogenous alpha-synuclein fibrils induce Lewy body pathology leading to synaptic dysfunction and neuron death, Neuron 72 (2011) 57–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].Wagner LM, Nathwani SM, Ten Eyck PP, Aldridge GM, Local cortical overexpression of human wild-type alpha-synuclein leads to increased dendritic spine density in mouse, Neurosci Lett 733 (2020) 135051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Xu M, Zhang SY, Dan Y, Poo MM, Representation of interval timing by temporally scalable firing patterns in rat prefrontal cortex, Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111 (2014) 480–485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [41].Zhang Q, Jung D, Larson T, Kim Y, Narayanan NS, Scopolamine and Medial Frontal Stimulus-Processing during Interval Timing, Neuroscience 414 (2019) 219–227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Zhang Q, Kim YC, Narayanan NS, Disease-modifying therapeutic directions for Lewy-Body dementias, Front Neurosci 9 (2015) 293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [43].Zhang Q, Weber MA, Narayanan NS, Medial prefrontal cortex and the temporal control of action, Int Rev Neurobiol 158 (2021) 421–441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]