Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2021 Nov 12;16(11):e0258829. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258829

Predictors of school students’ leisure-time physical activity: An extended trans-contextual model using Bayesian path analysis

Juho Polet 1, Jekaterina Schneider 1, Mary Hassandra 2, Taru Lintunen 1, Arto Laukkanen 1, Nelli Hankonen 3, Mirja Hirvensalo 1, Tuija H Tammelin 4, Kyra Hamilton 5, Martin S Hagger 1,6,*
Editor: Lambros Lazuras7
PMCID: PMC8589162  PMID: 34767569

Abstract

The present study aimed to examine effects of motivational and social cognition constructs on children’s leisure-time physical activity participation alongside constructs representing implicit processes using an extended trans-contextual model. The study adopted a correlational prospective design. Secondary-school students (N = 502) completed self-report measures of perceived autonomy support from physical education (PE) teachers, autonomous motivation in PE and leisure-time contexts, and social cognition constructs (attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control), intentions, trait self-control, habits, and past behavior in a leisure-time physical activity context. Five weeks later, students (N = 298) self-reported their leisure-time physical activity participation. Bayesian path analyses supported two key premises of the model: perceived autonomy support was related to autonomous motivation in PE, and autonomous motivation in PE was related to autonomous motivation in leisure time. Indirect effects indicated that both forms of autonomous motivation were related to social cognition constructs and intentions. However, intention was not related to leisure-time physical activity participation, so model variables reflecting motivational processes did not account for substantive variance in physical activity participation. Self-control, attitudes, and past behavior were direct predictors of intentions and leisure-time physical activity participation. There were indirect effects of autonomous motivation in leisure time on intentions and physical activity participation mediated by self-control. Specifying informative priors for key model relations using Bayesian analysis yielded greater precision for some model effects. Findings raise some questions on the predictive validity of constructs from the original trans-contextual model in the current sample, but highlight the value of extending the model to incorporate additional constructs representing non-conscious processes.

Introduction

Research indicates that low levels of physical activity have deleterious effects on the health of young people [1]. However, children and adolescents in many nations are not sufficiently active to confer health benefits and reduce disease risk [2]. As a consequence, national and international health organizations have developed guidelines and national strategies aimed at promoting physical activity in this population [3]. Given the imperative for promoting physical activity among young people, public health organizations and educators have sought to identify optimally effective strategies to enhance physical activity in this population, and the contexts in which these strategies will have maximal reach.

Physical education (PE) has been suggested as a potentially useful existing network that can be utilized to deliver interventions promoting physical activity both inside school, and, importantly, outside school, in children and adolescents [4]. Researchers have, therefore, aimed to explore the potentially efficacious strategies delivered in PE to promote increased physical activity outside of school. Such an endeavor necessitates an understanding of the determinants of children and adolescents’ physical activity participation in a PE context and, importantly, whether those determinants relate to physical activity participation outside of school in students’ leisure time [5]. Understanding how factors linked to engagement in physical activity in school relate to physical activity performed in another context, leisure time, is critical to informing potential strategies delivered in PE that promote physical activity participation in children and adolescents in their leisure time. Such an approach is also consistent with one of the key pedagogical aims of PE to provide young people with the necessary skills to lead an active lifestyle [6].

The trans-contextual model

The trans-contextual model [5] was developed to provide a theoretical explanation of the constructs and associated processes that link engagement in physical activity in school PE with physical activity participation in leisure time. Specifically, the model draws on multiple theories to outline relations between school students’ motivation toward physical activity in PE and their motives and beliefs toward, and actual participation in, physical activity in their leisure time. The model integrates core constructs and processes from self-determination theory [7], the hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation [8], and the theory of planned behavior [9]. Next, we outline the key premises of the model relating to the determinants of children and adolescents’ leisure-time physical activity and the processes involved.

Based on self-determination theory, the first premise of the trans-contextual model focuses on the origins of school students’ motivation toward activities in PE, and how their motivation relates to their behavior in PE. The model predicts that the social environment in educational settings fostered by social agents and leaders (e.g., PE teachers) will determine the type or form of motivation students experience when performing tasks (e.g., physical activities in PE) and, importantly, their persistence on tasks. Central to the theory is the distinction between autonomous and controlled forms of motivation [10]. Autonomous motivation is a form of motivation reflecting self-endorsed reasons for acting and autonomously motivated individuals tend to persist on tasks and exhibit behavioral persistence. Fostering autonomous motivation through the display of autonomy-supportive behaviors by social agents such as PE teachers will promote autonomous motivation toward physical activities performed in PE. Students who perceive their PE teacher as displaying behaviors that support their autonomy are more likely to report autonomous motivation toward activities in PE [11]. This prediction forms the basis of the first premise of the trans-contextual model: students’ perceived autonomy support from their teachers in PE will relate to their autonomous motivation toward physical activity in a PE context.

A central prediction of the trans-contextual model is that there will be a trans-contextual relationship between students’ autonomous motivation toward physical activities across PE and leisure-time contexts. This prediction is based on Vallerand’s [8] hierarchical model, which describes the process by which motivation is transferred across contexts. Vallerand proposed that individuals experiencing autonomous motivation toward activities in one context will also cite autonomous motives toward similar behaviors in other related contexts. This forms the second premise of the trans-contextual model: school students’ level of autonomous motivation toward physical activities in a PE context will be related to their autonomous motivation toward physical activities performed outside of school in their leisure time.

A final prediction of the trans-contextual model is that autonomous motivation toward physical activities in a leisure-time context will be related to students’ beliefs and intentions toward, and future participation in, leisure-time physical activity. If an individual has experienced a behavior as autonomously motivated, it is likely to be internalized and integrated into the individual’s repertoire of behaviors that satisfy their psychological need for autonomy. They are therefore more likely to actively seek out opportunities to engage in the behavior in future. To do so, they need to align their system of beliefs and intentions involved in the decision to perform that behavior in future. In the trans-contextual model, this process is represented by associations between forms of motivation from self-determination theory and the sets of beliefs from the theory of planned behavior, a leading social cognition theory [9]. Autonomously-motivated individuals are likely to form intentions to perform the behavior in future, and report favorable attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control, the immediate belief-based determinants of intentions [1214]. This forms the third premise of the trans-contextual model: students’ autonomous motivation toward physical activity in leisure time will be related to their future participation in leisure-time physical activity mediated by the belief-based social cognition determinants (attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control) and their intentions toward participating in leisure-time physical activity in future.

The key premises of the trans-contextual model have received substantial empirical support [5, 1517]. Furthermore, a meta-analysis of studies applying the model in PE and leisure-time physical activity contexts provides converging evidence supporting model predictions across multiple studies [18].

Extending the model

While the trans-contextual model has displayed utility in identifying the determinants of leisure-time physical activity participation, it is not without limitations. One limitation is the exclusive focus on motivational and social cognition determinants of leisure-time physical activity participation without regard for the influence of implicit beliefs and motives that affect individuals’ behavior beyond their awareness [19]. In contrast, dual-process theories of motivation and social cognition propose that individuals’ behavior is determined by constructs that reflect conscious, reasoned decision making (e.g., autonomous motivation, social cognition constructs) but also by constructs that reflect implicit decision-making that impact behavior with little reasoned deliberation. Such constructs include implicit attitudes, habits, and individual difference constructs [2023]. Non-conscious processes are adaptive because they lead to effective, efficient decision-making when reasoned deliberation is unnecessary or particularly costly [24]. Constructs reflecting these non-conscious processes are proposed to impact behavior directly without mediation by intentions, independent of the reasoned processes. These constructs may, therefore, account for the additional variance in leisure-time physical activity participation in the trans-contextual model and serve to provide a more comprehensive prediction of physical activity motivation and behavior in PE and leisure time.

Prominent behavioral determinants that reflect non-conscious processes are habit, trait self-control, and affective attitudes. Focusing on habit, although research has historically considered effects of past behavior as a viable proxy for habitual effects [25], recent research has focused on habit as a psychological construct [21, 26]. Theories of habit suggest that habitual behaviors are a function of behavioral experiences in the presence of consistent environmental, situational, or internal cues, and are often experienced as automatic, effortless, and highly accessible [26]. These components have been captured by self-report measures of habit, which are meta-cognitive measures which tap individuals’ experience of target behaviors as ‘unthinking’ and ‘automatic’ [21]. Such measures have been shown to predict behavior independent of intention-mediated measures [2730], and are also associated with action accessibility and behavioral performance in stable contexts [31, 32].

While habits are expected to predict intentions and behavior, they may also be related to motives that form part of the ‘motivational sequence’ proposed in the trans-contextual model. Individuals that hold autonomous motives toward behaviors like leisure-time physical activity, are more likely to persist with those behaviors, because the behavior fulfils psychological needs and leads to adaptive outcomes in the absence of external contingencies. This has been supported in the research literature on self-determination theory with consistent links between autonomous motivation and physical activity [33]. Related to this, autonomously motivated individuals are also more likely to form habits for those behaviors because they are likely to perform those behaviors in a consistent fashion with high frequency and in stable contexts–the key conditions under which habits form [34, 35]. This has been supported by previous research on the autonomous motivation-habit relationship [36]. Based on these premises, it follows that autonomous motivation toward physical activity in PE and leisure-time contexts is expected to be indirectly related to intentions and physical activity behavior mediated by habits. This proposed effect will be independent of the ‘motivational sequence’ proposed in the model, consistent with the dual-process approaches outlined previously.

Trait self-control reflects individual differences in capacities and self-regulatory skills that enable individuals to resist impulses and temptations, and engage in sustained, effortful behavior to attain long-term goal-directed outcomes [37]. Trait self-control has been consistently related to adaptive behaviors, including physical activity, across multiple contexts and populations [38]. Research has also demonstrated that behavioral effects of trait self-control may be direct, independent of intentions [39]. Such effects reflect generalized tendencies to engage in adaptive behaviors without the need for deliberation or consideration. However, a case has also been made for effects of trait self-control on behavior mediated by intentions [39]. Such effects reflect situations where individuals have to actively engage in effortful deliberation to overcome a maladaptive behavior, or engage in a new behavior, that requires deliberation. Effects of trait self-control in motivational and social cognition theories may, therefore, relate to behavior via two pathways, directly, and indirectly through intentions. Research incorporating trait self-control in the model has supported these dual effects, with direct and intention-mediated effects on physical activity participation [39].

While self-control has been identified as an independent determinant of intentions and behavior, there is also research that has linked self-control, and self-regulatory processes in general, with the forms of motivation implicated in the motivational sequence of the trans-contextual model. For example, research has highlighted that individuals reporting self-determined motives are less likely to be vulnerable to self-control failure and ego-depletion [4042], and more likely to report intentions toward, and participate in, future behaviors, including physical activity [36, 39, 43]. These findings are consistent with the self-determination theory hypothesis that autonomous motivation is ‘energizing’ and individuals with autonomous motives toward behaviors are likely to report greater capacity to perform the behavior, and hence greater self-control [36]. This hypothesis also aligns with the premise of the trans-contextual model that autonomous motives lead individuals to mobilize their resources to perform need-satisfying behaviors in future. Consistent with these proposals, it is reasonable to expect that autonomous motivation will be indirectly related to leisure-time physical activity intentions and behavior through self-control.

There is also research demonstrating that attitudes may predict behavior directly, and such direct effects may also reflect non-conscious decision making [44, 45]. The original conceptualization of the theory of planned behavior specifies that attitudes represent cognitive reflections on future participation in a target behavior and should relate to behavior mediated by intentions. However, direct effects of attitudes have been identified [45], and have been attributed to the affective or emotional component of attitude. Research separating the cognitive and affective attitude components has demonstrated independent effects. The affective component is proposed to encompass visceral approach or avoidance responses learned through behavioral experience [44]. Direct effects of attitude on behavior may reflect a further spontaneous, automatic process, which affects behavior independent of intentions.

The present study

In the present study, we aimed to extend the trans-contextual model by including self-control, habits, and attitudes as additional direct determinants of physical activity intentions and behavior participation. This extension is expected to provide additional information on the determinants of leisure-time physical activity behavior, particularly effects of constructs representing non-conscious processes not accounted for in the original model. We also applied the Bayesian analytic approach to test model effects using informative prior values for key model effects derived from a meta-analysis of the model [18]. We also capitalized on previous research on self-reported habit [36, 46] and trait self-control [38] to specify informative priors for these parameters in our test of extensions to the model. We expected to see a reduced level of uncertainty in the distributions of the parameters of the model specified for the current data when informative priors for key model parameters derived from the meta-analysis are specified, reflected in narrowed credibility intervals, compared to the distributions when non-informative priors are specified.

Specifically, the study adopted survey methods and a five-week prospective design with measures of motivational and social cognition constructs, habit, trait-self-control, and past physical activity participation taken at an initial occasion, and self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation taken at follow-up, five weeks later. This time period was selected to provide reasonable medium-term prediction, which exceeds typical time frames in model tests [47]. In addition to testing effects of the motivational and social cognition determinants from the trans-contextual model on students’ intentions toward, and actual participation in, leisure-time physical activity, we also tested direct effects of the constructs reflecting non-conscious processes as direct determinants of leisure-time physical activity participation: self-reported habit, trait self-control, and attitudes. Further, effects of autonomous motivation in both contexts were expected to be indirectly related to physical activity intentions and behavior in leisure time mediated by both habit and self-control. Such effects represent processes by which self-determined motivation promotes behavioral enactment by promoting greater perceived self-regulatory capacity [42] and experience of the behavior as automatic [48]. Finally, we also expected model effects to hold in the presence of past behavior, and that there would be an indirect effect of past physical activity behavior on leisure-time physical activity mediated by habit. Such a relationship would illustrate the extent to which past behavior is a function of habit formation, consistent with previous theory and research [27, 48]. The specific predictions of the proposed model, including direct and indirect effects in the proposed model are summarized in Table 1 and Fig 1.

Table 1. Summary of hypothesized direct and indirect effects in the extended trans-contextual model.

Hypothesis (H) Independent variable Dependent variable Mediator(s) Informative priors
β σ2
Direct effects
H1 PAS Aut. mot. (PE)a 0.42 0.10
H2 Aut. mot. (PE) Aut. mot. (LT)a 0.56 0.17
H3 PAS Aut. mot. (LT)a 0.29 0.18
H4 Aut. mot. (LT) Attitudea 0.60 0.12
H5 Aut. mot. (LT) Subjective norma 0.26 0.26
H6 Aut. mot. (LT) PBCa 0.51 0.19
H7 Aut. mot. (LT) Intentiona 0.31 0.13
H8 Attitude Intentiona 0.68 0.09
H9 Subjective norm Intentiona 0.42 0.25
H10 PBC Intentiona 0.63 0.28
H11 Habit Intention
H12 Self-control Intention
H13 Intention Phys. act.a 0.60 0.20
H14 Attitude Phys. act.a 0.43 0.21
H15 PBC Phys. act.a 0.43 0.21
H16 Habit Phys. act.b 0.43 0.13
H17 Self-control Phys. act.c 0.26 0.09
H18 Aut. mot. (LT) Phys. act.
H19 Aut. mot. (LT) Habitd 0.20 0.43
H20 Aut. mot. (LT) Self-control
H21 Past behavior Habit
H22 Past behavior Phys. act.
Indirect effects
H23 PAS Aut. mot. (LT) Aut. mot. (PE)
H24 Aut. mot. (PE) Intention Aut. mot. (LT)
Attitude
H25 Aut. mot. (PE) Intention Aut. mot. (LT)
Sub. norm.
H26 Aut. mot. (PE) Intention Aut. mot. (LT)
PBC
H27 Aut. mot. (PE) Phys. act. Aut. mot. (LT)
Attitude
Intention
H28 Aut. mot. (PE) Phys. act. Aut. mot. (LT)
Sub. norm.
Intention
H29 Aut. mot. (PE) Phys. act. Aut. mot. (LT)
PBC
Intention
H30 Aut. mot. (PE) Phys. act. Aut. mot. (LT)
PBC
H31 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention Attitude
H32 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention Sub. norm.
H33 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention PBC
H34 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention Habit
H35 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention Self-control
H36 Aut. mot. (LT) Phys. act. Attitude
Intention
H37 Aut. mot. (LT) Phys. act. Sub. norm.
Intention
H38 Aut. mot. (LT) Phys. act. PBC
Intention
H39 Aut. mot. (LT) Habit Phys. act
H40 Aut. mot. (LT) Self-control Phys. act
H41 Past beh. Habit Phys. act

Note. aPrior values derived from Hagger and Chatzisarantis [18];

bPrior values derived from Gardner et al. [49];

cPrior values derived from de Ridder et al. [38];

dPrior value derived from Kaushal et al. [36]. PAS = Perceived autonomy support; Aut. mot. = Autonomous motivation; PE = Physical education context; LT = Leisure-time context; PBC = Perceived behavioral control; Sub. norm = Subjective norm; Phys. act = Self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation; Past. beh. = Past leisure-time physical activity behavior.

Fig 1. Hypothesized relations among constructs of the extended trans-contextual model.

Fig 1

Effects of past behavior on all other model constructs have been omitted for clarity.

Method

Participants

Participants were lower secondary school students (N = 502, 43.82% female; M age = 14.52, SD = 0.71) recruited from selected schools across Jyväskylä, Finland with support from the City Education Department. The University institutional review board and Education Department approved the study protocol prior to data collection. Informed consent was sought from the head teacher of each school, and, subsequently, PE teachers and eligible students’ parents or legal guardians via the schools’ online administration and communication software or via email or post. Opt-in consent was sought from the head teachers and PE teachers, while opt-out consent was sought from students’ parents and legal guardians. Qualified full-time PE teachers teaching regular PE lessons in lower secondary schools were eligible to participate in the study and were asked to select one of their PE classes to take part. Students in grades 7 to 9 (typical ages 13 to 15 years) in lower secondary schools were eligible to participate. Students with existing physical or mental health conditions that prevented participation in PE lessons, regular leisure-time physical activity, or completing surveys were excluded.

Design and procedure

Data for this study was collected as part of a larger randomized controlled trial (trial registration: ISRCTN39374060; PETALS). The trial adopted a cluster-randomized, waitlist control, single-group intervention design with randomization by school. The trial comprised a teacher training phase and an implementation phase; full details of the intervention design and content have been published previously [50]. Secondary school PE teachers (N = 29) from 11 secondary schools and their students (N = 502) were invited to participate in the study. The pool of potentially eligible students numbered approximately 5000 across the 11 schools. Baseline data was collected prior to the teacher training phase and participants completed self-report questionnaires assessing demographic, psychological, and behavioral measures. The baseline data collection period was followed by the teacher training phase (12 hours over two weeks) and the implementation phase (one month), after which post-intervention data was collected comprising the same self-report questionnaires as at baseline. Follow-up data was further collected one, three, and six months post-intervention. The present study used measures of motivation and social cognition constructs and leisure-time physical activity participation taken at baseline and post-intervention leisure-time physical activity participation controlling intervention effects at baseline. Data for the present study were collected between September and December 2018.

Measures

Measures of study constructs were adapted from instruments used in previous applications of the trans-contextual model. Measures included in the surveys were: perceived autonomy support from PE teachers [51]; autonomous motivation derived from items measuring self-determined forms of motivation from the perceived locus of causality scales for the PE and leisure-time physical activity contexts [52]; intentions, attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control from the theory of planned behavior [53]; self-reported habit [21] and trait self-control [54]; and self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation [55]. All self-report measures were previously translated from English to Finnish using a back-translation process by two bilingual researchers. All measures used in the current research exhibited acceptable construct validity in previous research applying confirmatory factor analyses. Items tapping each construct exhibited satisfactory factor loadings, average variance extracted, and composite reliability estimates [5, 46, 56]. Furthermore, we also conducted a pilot study in which the validity of the translated trans-contextual measures was tested in the target population [57]. This study used single-indicator latent variable model using omega reliability estimates to control for measurement error. Simulation research using full structural equation models and single-indicator models has revealed little difference in the parameter estimates in models either approach [58]. These data provided support for the use of these measures in this population, particularly the construct and predictive validity of the measures in the context of the trans-contextual model. Full details of the measures used are available in Appendix A in S1 File.

Perceived autonomy support

Students’ perceived autonomy support from their PE teacher was measured using items from the Perceived Autonomy Support Scale for Exercise Settings [51]. The scale comprised 13 items (e.g., “I feel that my PE teacher provides me with choices and options to …”) with responses provided on seven-point scales (1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree).

Autonomous motivation

Autonomous motivation toward in-school and out-of-school physical activities was measured using items from the Perceived Locus of Causality Questionnaire [52]. Two items measured identified regulation (e.g., “I do PE/physical activity because it is important to me to do well in PE/physical activity”) and two items measured intrinsic motivation (e.g., “I do PE/physical activity because it is fun”). Responses were provided on seven-point scales (1 = not true for me and 7 = very true for me). For each of the PE and leisure-time contexts, a composite autonomous motivation score was computed by averaging scores on the identified regulation and intrinsic motivation items.

Theory of planned behavior constructs

Measures of students’ attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and intentions with respect to their future participation in leisure-time physical activity were developed according to guidelines [53]. Attitudes were measured on three items in response to a common stem: “Participating in physical activity in the next five weeks will be…”, with responses provided on seven-point scales (e.g., 1 = unenjoyable and 7 = enjoyable). Subjective norms (e.g., “Most people who are important to me think I should do active sports and/or vigorous physical activities during my leisure time in the next five weeks”), perceived behavioral control (e.g., “I am confident I could do active sports and/or vigorous physical activities during my leisure time in the next five weeks”), and intentions (e.g., “I intend to do active sports and/or vigorous physical activities during my leisure time in the next five weeks”) were measured using two items each with responses provided on seven-point scales (e.g., 1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree).

Habit

Habit was measured using automaticity items from the Self-Reported Habit Index [21] which focuses on personal experience of the behavior as ‘automatic’ and excludes items related to past behavior. The scale comprised four items (e.g., “Physical activity is something I do without thinking”) with responses provided on seven-point scales (1 = completely disagree and 7 = completely agree).

Trait self-control

Students’ trait self-control was measured using the 10-item Self-Discipline Scale (e.g., “I tend to carry out my plans”) from the IPIP-HEXACO scales [59] with responses provided on four-point scales (1 = not like me at all and 4 = very much like me).

Behavior

Past leisure-time physical activity at baseline and leisure-time physical activity participation at follow-up was measured using the short form of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ; 55). The IPAQ comprises four items recording the frequency (number of days) and duration (minutes per day) of engagement in moderate and vigorous physical activity, walking, and sitting over the past seven days. IPAQ data were processed according to established guidelines [60]. The procedure gives an estimate of physical activity in MET-minutes per week with higher MET-minute values indicating higher level of physical activity engagement. Full details of calculations used to produce physical activity estimates are presented in Appendix B in S1 File. Internal consistency values for the short-form IPAQ exceed guideline cut-off values and scores on the scale demonstrate reasonable agreement with the long form in previous research [55].

Data analysis

The proposed hypotheses of the extended trans-contextual model (see Table 1 and Fig 1) were tested using Bayesian path analytic models estimated with the Mplus 7.31 statistical software. We computed composite scales study measures by computing an average of the items for each construct. We controlled for the effects of the intervention in the model by including effects of a binary variable representing intervention group membership (1 = allocated autonomy support intervention group, 0 = allocated to control group) on follow-up leisure-time physical activity participation. We controlled for effects of gender as a binary variable (0 = female, 1 = male) and age as a continuous variable by estimating effects of these variables on all other constructs in the model. Missing data for the model components were imputed using full information maximum likelihood (FIML) in Mplus.

A Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) simulation process using Gibbs’ algorithm [61] was applied to estimate our Bayesian path models in the present study. The first 50% of the iterations (N = 100,000) was used as a burn-in phase with the remainder used to test the specified model parameters. We established convergence of the parameter estimates in the Bayesian models according to the Gelman-Rubin [62] criterion based on a potential scale reduction (PSR) value of 1.01. Our analysis required estimation of two models. We first estimated a model that adopted the non-informative default priors available in Mplus to estimate model parameters. The defaults assumed a normal distribution with a mean of zero and a variance value essentially equivalent to infinity (1010). In our second model, we applied informative prior values taken from previous research [18, 36, 38, 49] to estimate model parameters. Specifically, priors for relations among the trans-contextual model constructs were taken from Hagger and Chatzisarantis’ [18] meta-analysis. The prior value for the effect of self-reported habit on leisure-time physical activity participation was derived from Gardner et al.’s [46] meta-analysis of self-reported habit in physical activity. The prior value for the effect of trait self-control on leisure-time physical activity participation was taken from de Ridder et al.’s [38] meta-analysis of trait self-control in health behaviors. The prior value for the effect of autonomous motivation in leisure time on habit was taken from Kaushal et al.’s [36] integrated model test. Finally, we used non-informative prior values for remaining parameters without user-specified priors.

We used multiple published criteria to establish the goodness-of-fit of our proposed models with the data across the iterations of the Bayesian analysis [61]. These criteria included the 95% confidence interval of the difference in the goodness-of-fit chi-square value across the observed and replicated models, as well as the posterior predictive p-value (PPP). The goodness-of-fit chi-square value should have a positive upper limit, a negative lower limit, and be centered about zero, and the PPP value should be greater than .05 and approach .50, for well-fitting models. In addition, we also report the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), a relative fit index which allows researchers to identify the model with the greatest parsimony relative to fit as it includes a term that ‘penalizes’ overfitting. Cut-off values of .95 or greater for the CFI and TLI, and .06 for the RMSEA, have been proposed as indicative of good model fit. Furthermore, the 90% confidence intervals of each index should ideally exceed the cut-off values.

With respect to model parameters, a point estimate and 95% posterior credibility interval was produced for each parameter in the models. A non-zero credibility interval for a parameter provides confirmatory support for the proposed effect in the model. Point estimates and credibility intervals were also produced for the proposed indirect effects in the models [63]. In addition, we also expected that specification of informative prior values in the second model would lead to increased precision in the point estimates. To demonstrate changes in precision, we followed Yuan and McKinnon’s method which evaluates the extent to which the posterior credibility intervals about each parameter estimate is narrowed with the introduction of informative priors. If the width of the confidence intervals is reduced, we have sharp confirmation that inclusion of the prior values alongside the current data leads to increased precision in estimates. Data, syntax, and output files for our analyses are available online: https://osf.io/z8axj.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Of the participants who completed the initial survey (N = 502), 370 provided complete data at baseline and 298 participants (50% female; M age = 14.51, SD = 0.70) provided data for analysis after the second survey (19.46% attrition rate). Attrition was due to school absences. Percentage of missing data for the psychological constructs over time was low (M = 1.4%; range = 0.0% to 4.0%) and Little’s MCAR test (χ2 = 63.882, df = 70, p = .683) suggested the data were missing at random. In addition, there were no significant differences between those who completed study measures at both time points and those who did not on gender distribution, (χ2(1) = 0.403, p = .526), age (t(368) = -0.463, p = .643), or baseline physical activity (t(368) = -1.103, p = .271). We also conducted a one-way MANOVA to examine differences in psychological variables between those who completed study measures at both time points and those who did not, which was not significant (F(9, 360) = 0.775, p = .639; Wilks’ Λ = .981; partial η2 = .019). All constructs exhibited adequate internal consistency. Means, standard deviations, omega internal consistency coefficients and zero-order intercorrelations among study constructs are presented in Appendix C in S1 File.

Path analyses

Bayesian path analytic models using non-informative (Bayesian posterior predictive χ2 95% CI = [-26.087, 57.542]; PPP = 0.225; BIC = 9202.749) and informative priors for key model relationships (Bayesian posterior predictive χ2 95% CI = [-18.211, 67.825]; prior PPP = 0.126; BIC = 9081.715) exhibited adequate goodness-of-fit with the data. In addition, the BIC indicated that the model that included informative priors exhibited better fit than the model with non-informative priors. Parameter estimates and 95% credibility intervals for the analysis with non-informative priors (Model 1) and the analysis including informative priors for key model relationships (Model 2) are presented in Table 2 and Fig 2.

Table 2. Parameter estimates (β) with 95% credibility intervals for hypothesized effects from the Bayesian path analyses of the extended trans-contextual model for leisure-time physical activity.

H Independent variable Dependent variable Mediator(s) Model 1 Model 2 %diff
β 95% CrI β 95% CrI
LL UL LL UL
Direct effects
H1 PAS Aut. mot. (PE)† 2.403*** 1.485 3.018 1.222*** 0.625 1.634 -34.18
H2 Aut. mot. (PE) Aut. mot. (LT) 0.449*** 0.364 0.536 0.452*** 0.366 0.537 -0.58
H3 PAS Aut. mot. (LT) 0.140* 0.012 0.264 0.138* 0.011 0.265 0.79
H4 Aut. mot. (LT) Attitude 0.433*** 0.343 0.523 0.437*** 0.348 0.526 -1.11
H5 Aut. mot. (LT) Sub. norm 0.164* 0.007 0.321 0.166* 0.012 0.322 -1.27
H6 Aut. mot. (LT) PBC 0.221*** 0.119 0.322 0.225*** 0.125 0.326 -0.99
H7 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention 0.410*** 0.304 0.516 0.403*** 0.298 0.507 -1.42
H8 Attitude Intention 0.166** 0.057 0.275 0.182*** 0.076 0.289 -2.29
H9 Sub. norm Intention 0.118*** 0.058 0.178 0.118*** 0.059 0.178 -0.83
H10 PBC Intention 0.325*** 0.227 0.422 0.324*** 0.228 0.420 -1.54
H11 Habit Intention 0.057 -0.029 0.144 0.058 -0.028 0.144 -0.58
H12 Self-control Intention 0.217* 0.032 0.400 0.214* 0.030 0.399 0.27
H13 Intention Phys. act. -0.001 -0.062 0.060 0.000 -0.060 0.061 -0.82
H14 Attitude Phys. act. 0.092** 0.032 0.151 0.093** 0.034 0.152 -0.84
H15 PBC Phys. act. -0.029 -0.085 0.026 -0.029 -0.084 0.026 -0.90
H16 Habit Phys. act. -0.005 -0.051 0.040 -0.004 -0.049 0.042 0.00
H17 Self-control Phys. act. 0.104* 0.003 0.206 0.107* 0.007 0.207 -1.48
H18 Aut. mot. (LT) Phys. act. -0.017 -0.079 0.045 -0.019 -0.080 0.043 -0.81
H19 Aut. mot. (LT) Habit 0.121 -1.592 2.637 0.532*** 0.392 0.671 -93.40
H20 Aut. mot. (LT) Self-control 0.108*** 0.046 0.171 0.138*** 0.071 0.207 8.80
H21 Past behavior Habit 1.521*** -3.089 0.658 0.799*** 0.397 1.206 -78.41
H22 Past beh. (LT) Phys. act. 0.675*** 0.519 0.829 0.672*** 0.518 0.827 -0.32
Indirect effects
H23 PAS Aut. mot. (LT) Aut. mot. (PE) 1.059*** 0.643 1.449 0.547*** 0.276 0.777 -37.84
H24 Aut. mot. (PE) Intention Aut. mot. (LT) 0.032** 0.011 0.057 0.035*** 0.014 0.061 2.17
Attitude
H25 Aut. mot. (PE) Intention Aut. mot. (LT) 0.008* 0.000 0.020 0.008* 0.001 0.020 -5.00
Sub. norm.
H26 Aut. mot. (PE) Intention Aut. mot. (LT) 0.032*** 0.015 0.053 0.032*** 0.016 0.054 0.00
PBC
H27 Aut. mot. (PE) Phys. act. Aut. mot. (LT) 0.000 -0.002 0.002 0.000 -0.002 0.002 0.00
Attitude
Intention
H28 Aut. mot. (PE) Phys. act. Aut. mot. (LT) 0.000 -0.001 0.001 0.000 -0.001 0.001 0.00
Sub. norm.
Intention
H29 Aut. mot. (PE) Phys. act. Aut. mot. (LT) 0.000 -0.002 0.002 0.000 -0.002 0.002 0.00
PBC
Intention
H30 Aut. mot. (PE) Phys. act. Aut. mot. (LT) -0.003 -0.009 0.003 -0.003 -0.009 0.003 0.00
PBC
H31 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention Attitude 0.071** 0.024 0.123 0.079*** 0.032 0.131 0.00
H32 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention Sub. norm. 0.018* 0.001 0.043 0.019* 0.001 0.044 2.38
H33 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention PBC 0.071*** 0.035 0.115 0.072*** 0.037 0.115 -2.50
H34 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention Habit 0.003 -0.117 0.240 0.030 -0.015 0.079 -73.67
H35 Aut. mot. (LT) Intention Self-control 0.022* 0.003 0.052 0.028* 0.004 0.064 22.45
H36 Aut. mot. (LT) Phys. act. Attitude 0.000 -0.005 0.005 0.000 -0.005 0.005 0.00
Intention
H37 Aut. mot. (LT) Phys. act. Sub. norm. 0.000 -0.001 0.001 0.000 -0.001 0.001 0.00
Intention
H38 Aut. mot. (LT) Phys. act. PBC 0.000 -0.005 0.005 0.000 -0.005 0.005 0.00
Intention
H39 Aut. mot. (LT) Phys. act Habit 0.000 -0.071 0.056 -0.002 -0.027 0.023 -60.63
H40 Aut. mot. (LT) Phys. act Self-control 0.011 0.000 0.026 0.014* 0.001 0.033 23.08
H41 Past beh. Phys. act Habit -0.002 -0.127 0.108 -0.003 -0.042 0.035 -67.23

Note. †Parameters with informative priors. Model 1 = Bayesian path model with non-informative priors; Model 2 = Bayesian path model including informative priors; H = Hypothesis; β = Parameter estimate; 95% CrI = 95% credibility interval of path coefficient; %diff = Percent difference in 95% credibility interval of path coefficients of path analysis including informative priors for specified model relationships compared to analysis using non-informative priors (negative numbers indicate a narrowing of credibility intervals when using informative priors); PAS = Perceived autonomy support; Aut. mot. = Autonomous motivation; PE = Physical education contexts; LT = Leisure-time context; PBC = Perceived behavioral control; Sub. norm = Subjective norm; Phys. act = Self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation; Past. Beh. = Past leisure-time physical activity behavior.

*p < .05

**p < .01

***p < .001.

Fig 2. Parameter estimates from the Bayesian path analysis of the extended trans-contextual model for leisure-time physical activity including informative priors.

Fig 2

PE = Physical education context; LT = Leisure time context. Model parameters omitted for clarity: past physical activity behavior→perceived autonomy support, β = 0.390, 95% CI [0.116, 0.664], p = .003; past physical activity behavior→autonomous motivation (PE), β = 0.368, 95% CI [-0.076, 0.799], p = .051; past physical activity behavior→autonomous motivation (LT), β = 1.382, 95% CI [1.109, 1.654], p < .001; past physical activity behavior→attitude, β = 0.194, 95% CI [-0.109, 0.495], p = .106; past physical activity behavior→subjective norms, β = 0.337, 95% CI [-0.193, 0.867], p = .103; past physical activity behavior→perceived behavioral control, β = 0.593, 95% CI [0.249, 0.938], p < .001; past physical activity behavior→intention, β = 0.293, 95% CI [0.014, 0.574], p = .019; past physical activity behavior→habit, β = 0.799, 95% CI [0.397, 1.206], p < .001; past physical activity behavior→self-control, β = 0.153, 95% CI [-0.041, 0.348], p = .061; past physical activity behavior→physical activity behavior, β = 0.672, 95% CI [0.518, 0.827], p = .001.

It is important to note that we did not conduct an a priori statistical power analysis for the current study as the main purpose of the trial was to test the effects of the intervention [50]. Nevertheless, we conducted a posteriori power analysis to check whether we had adequate power to test the current model. Our analysis was based on MacCallum, Browne, and Sugawara’s [64] statistical power determination based on the RMSEA. The final sample size (N = 298), a null hypothesis RMSEA of 0 and a study hypothesis RMSEA of 0.064, alpha set at 0.05, and 16 degrees of freedom were inputs for the analysis, which was conducted using the Webpower package in R [65]. The resulting power estimate of 0.805 indicated that we had sufficient statistical power to detect effects of the stipulated size.

Assuming the selected priors derived from meta-analyses were indicative of the population point estimates and distributions of effects among model constructs, we expected that Model 2 would yield greater precision in model parameter estimates compared to Model 1 [63]. This was evaluated by examining the extent to which the credibility intervals about each parameter estimate differed across the models (Table 2). Results indicated that the width of the credibility intervals was narrowed for a few of the effects in Model 2 relative to Model 1, but not by a substantial margin in most cases. The adequate fit of both models suggests that including informative priors in the analyses for key model relationships did not have a substantial bearing on the pattern of effects in the model. Nevertheless, given that some parameters were more precise, particularly the direct effects of perceived autonomy support on autonomous motivation in PE and autonomous motivation in PE on autonomous motivation in leisure time, and the indirect effects of autonomous motivation in PE on autonomous motivation in leisure time and intention, we elected to evaluate our hypothesis tests based on the model using informative priors (Model 2). If the posterior distribution for each effect, represented by the credibility intervals about the coefficients, did not include zero, then the effect was considered supported and the posterior probability of a non-zero value for the coefficient exceeds 0.975.

In terms of direct effects, we found a non-zero effect of perceived autonomy support in PE on autonomous motivation in PE (H1; β = 1.222, 95% CI [0.6425, 1.634], p < .001). There was also a non-zero trans-contextual effect of autonomous motivation in PE on autonomous motivation in leisure time (H2; β = 0.452, 95% CI [0.366, 0.537], p < .001), and perceived autonomy support in PE also had a non-zero effect on autonomous motivation in leisure time (H3; β = 0.138, 95% CI [0.011, 0.265], p < .001). We also found non-zero effects of autonomous motivation in leisure time on attitudes (H4; β = 0.437, 95% CI [0.348, 0.526], p < .001), subjective norms (H5; β = 0.166, 95% CI [0.012, 0.322], p = .017), and perceived behavioral control (H6; β = 0.225, 95% CI [0.125, 0.326], p < .001). There were also non-zero effects of attitudes (H8; β = 0.182, 95% CI [0.076, 0.289], p < .001), subjective norms (H9; β = 0.118, 95% CI [0.059, 0.178], p < .001), and perceived behavioral control (H10; β = 0.324, 95% CI [0.228, 0.420], p < .001) on intentions. Moreover, there were non-zero effects of autonomous motivation in leisure time (H7; β = 0.403, 95% CI [0.298, 0.507], p < .001) and trait self-control (H12; β = 0.214, 95% CI [0.030, 0.399], p = .012) on intentions, but the effect of habit on intention (H11) was no different from zero. We found non-zero effects of attitude (H14; β = 0.093, 95% CI [0.034, 0.152], p = .001), trait self-control (H17; β = 0.107, 95% CI [0.007, 0.207], p = .018), and past physical activity participation (H22; β = 0.672, 95% CI [0.518, 0.827], p < .001) on leisure-time physical activity participation at follow-up, while effects of intention (H13), perceived behavioral control (H15), habit (H16), and autonomous motivation in leisure time (H18) were no different from zero. In addition, there were non-zero effects of autonomous motivation in leisure time on habit (H19; β = 0.532, 95% CI [0.392, 0.671], p < .001) and trait self-control (H20; β = 0.138, 95% CI [0.071, 0.207], p < .001). Finally, we found a non-zero effect of past behavior on habit (H21; β = 0.799, 95% CI [0.397, 1.206], p < .001). All effects were small-to-medium in size.

Focusing on the indirect effects, we found non-zero indirect effects of perceived autonomy support on autonomous motivation in leisure time mediated by autonomous motivation in PE (H23; β = 0.547, 95% CI [0.276, 0.777], p < .001). There were also non-zero effects of autonomous motivation in PE on intentions mediated by autonomous motivation in leisure time and attitude (H24; β = 0.035, 95% CI [0.014, 0.061], p < .001), subjective norms (H25; β = 0.008, 95% CI [0.001, 0.020], p = .017), and perceived behavioral control (H26; β = 0.032, 95% CI [0.016, 0.054], p < .001). Similarly, there were also non-zero indirect effects of autonomous motivation in leisure time on intentions mediated by attitude (H31; β = 0.079, 95% CI [0.032, 0.131], p < .001), subjective norms (H32; β = 0.019, 95% CI [0.001, 0.044], p = .017), and perceived behavioral control (H33; β = 0.072, 95% CI [0.037, 0.115], p < .001). There were also non-zero indirect effects of autonomous motivation in leisure-time on intention (H35; β = 0.028, 95% CI [0.004, 0.064], p = .012) and physical activity participation (H40; β = 0.014, 95% CI [0.001, 0.033], p = .018) mediated by trait self-control, but indirect effects mediated by habit were no different from zero (H34, H39). However, indirect effects of autonomous motivation in the PE (H27−H30) and leisure-time (H36−H38) contexts through the social cognition constructs on leisure-time physical activity participation were no different from zero, primarily because the intention-behavior relationship was also no different from zero. The indirect effect of past behavior on physical activity participation mediated by habit (H41) was also no different from zero. Finally, effects of the intervention on leisure-time physical activity participation, and effects of age on model constructs, were no different from zero. However, we found non-zero effects of gender on autonomous motivation in PE (β = -.401, 95% CI [-0.717, -0.099], p = .004), with girls experiencing higher levels than boys, autonomous motivation in leisure time (β = 0.256, 95% CI [0.055, 0.458], p = .007), and attitudes (β = 0.210, 95% CI [0.016, 0.402], p = .017).

Discussion

The purpose of the current research was to examine the determinants of lower secondary school students’ leisure-time physical activity participation using an extended version of the trans-contextual model [5]. Specifically, the model was augmented to include two constructs that reflect non-conscious processes as predictors of leisure-time physical activity participation: self-reported habit [21] and trait self-control [37]. In addition, attitude was also set as a direct predictor of leisure-time physical activity participation, representing a further non-conscious process [44, 45]. Hypothesized relations among the extended trans-contextual model constructs were tested using a two-wave prospective survey design in a convenience sample of lower secondary school students. Data were analyzed using two Bayesian path analytic models: one specifying non-informative priors and one in which informed priors for key relations in the model derived from previous research were specified. Results indicated adequate fit of both models with the data. Perceived autonomy support predicted autonomous motivation in PE and leisure-time contexts, autonomous motivation in PE predicted autonomous motivation in a leisure-time context, and autonomous motivation in a leisure-time context predicted social cognition constructs (attitudes, perceived behavioral control) and intentions toward leisure-time physical activity participation. There were also indirect effects of perceived autonomy support on autonomous motivation in leisure time mediated by autonomous motivation in PE, and of autonomous motivation in PE and leisure time on intentions through the social cognition constructs. In contrast, the hypothesized indirect effects of autonomous motivation in both contexts on leisure-time physical activity participation were not supported, primarily due to effects of intention and perceived behavioral control on behavior that were no different from zero. However, attitudes and trait self-control predicted both intentions and behavior. Furthermore, there were indirect effects of autonomous motivation in leisure-time on intentions and physical activity participation mediated by self-control, but not habit. The Bayesian analytic approach demonstrated that the model was tenable with the model incorporating informative prior knowledge demonstrating better fit with the data and more precision for some of the parameter estimates.

Overall, current results supported hypotheses relating to the first two premises of the trans-contextual model, that is, the premises specifying effects of perceived autonomy support on autonomous motivation in PE, and the trans-contextual effects of autonomous motivation across PE and leisure time context [5, 18]. It also provided support for the effects of autonomous motivation in leisure time on intentions to participate in leisure-time physical activity mediated by the attitude, subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control constructs from the theory of planned behavior. However, there was scant evidence for the third premise, due to an intention-physical activity participation relationship that was no different from zero. These findings suggest that, in the current sample, the trans-contextual model is effective in identifying motivational and social cognition determinants of secondary school students’ intentions to participate in leisure-time physical activity, and the processes involved, but not their actual participation. We propose four possible interpretations of the current findings. First, results may raise questions on the effectiveness of the trans-contextual model in identifying the determinants of leisure-time physical activity participation. There have been occasions where studies on the motivational and social cognition constructs in multi-theory, integrated models have failed to yield non-zero effects for the primary predicted determinants of behavior [17, 20, 66]. Nevertheless, such occasions are rare, and are contrary to the substantive body of meta-analytic evidence applying the trans-contextual model [18] and other integrated models that have supported effects more broadly and in multiple populations and contexts [67, 68]. Therefore, it may be premature to use the current data as a basis for rejecting the trans-contextual model.

A second interpretation may be that some of the hypothesized effects in the model were attenuated due to contextual factors that affected relations among constructs, particularly the intention-behavior relationship. It is important to note that the intention-behavior relationship is integral to the model as it is a key link in the ‘motivational sequence’ by which perceived autonomy support in PE and autonomous motivation in both contexts relates to leisure-time physical activity participation. An intention-behavior relationship that is no different from zero, therefore, suggests that the indirect effect of autonomy support and autonomous motivation in both contexts on behavior, a key premise of the model, is not supported. This should not, however, invalidate the model. Rather it may signpost potential contextual or environmental factors that lead to effects in the model are attenuated. For example, research has shown that extraneous constructs moderate the intention-behavior relationship [69].

One possibility is that the current research was conducted in the context of an intervention. However, correlations of the intervention with key model constructs, particularly intentions and follow-up physical activity participation were no different from zero. In fact, the only effects of the intervention on variables from the current study were on perceived autonomy support and attitudes at baseline, and these effects were opposite to the predicted direction and were taken prior to the intervention. Furthermore, we also controlled for intervention effects in the current model, so reported effects were independent of intervention effects. This leaves the possibility of other extraneous constructs attenuating the intention-physical activity participation relationship in the current study. It is possible, for example, that students’ intentions were particularly unstable or inconsistent with their subsequent behavior, given research that has confirmed these intention properties moderate these relations [69]. However, this possibility remains speculative as we have no data on intention stability or consistency, nor do we have any contextual or demographic information that would explain such inconsistencies.

A third explanation may be that participation in leisure-time physical activity in the current sample of school students was largely determined by constructs that reflect individual-level non-conscious processes, that is, constructs that impact behavior directly independent of intentions. That the only determinants of leisure-time physical activity participation in the current study were past physical activity participation, attitude, and trait self-control is consistent with this interpretation. Focusing first on the direct effect of trait self-control on behavior, this construct is proposed to reflect non-conscious processes insofar as those endorsing it are purported to exhibit adaptive self-regulatory skills that assist in pursuing goal-directed behaviors and help resist temptations to engage in alternative behaviors that may derail pursuit of the behavior [39, 43]. On the surface, such an effect implies that individuals applying such skills must engage in active, effortful decision making to ensure focus on the target behavior and manage distractions, a conscious process. This may be the case for behaviors with which the individual has little experience. However, where the individual has substantive experience and has engaged in such active deliberation over the management of the behavior and application of their skills, they are likely to have well-learned behavioral scripts or schemas stored in memory to manage distractions and maintain behavioral engagement, obviating the need for such conscious deliberation. This is consistent with research suggesting that individuals with good trait self-control are highly effective in managing their environment so as not to be encumbered by distractions and to ensure that the cues to their desired behavior are omnipresent [38]. While this mechanistic explanation is speculative, it may explain the direct effect of trait self-control on behavior in the current model and provides justification to explore the role of this constructs within the trans-contextual model.

A fourth and final interpretation is that social environmental factors may have contributed to the weak intention-behavior relationship. The high availability of inactive highly-appealing pastimes available to young people (e.g., computer games) and social norms within families and peer groups to engage in inactive pastimes may have contributed to failure of students to engage in physical activity even if they had autonomous motives and intentions to do so. This is consistent with the current data in which students’ average intentions to engage in physical activity in their leisure time was above the scale mid-point (M = 5.651, SD = 1.282). The effects of peer norms are especially strong in this age group, so young people with intentions to be active may find that they are superseded by their need to conform. These premises are consistent with ecological models that stress environmental influences [e.g., 70], and research suggesting that such influences are important predictors of behavior beyond social cognition determinants [e.g., 71]. Analogously, if a child has low or no intention to participate in physical activity, they may still be compelled to spontaneously do so if their peer groups decides to have a ‘kick about’ with a football in their local park. The current study did not measure environmental influences, so such determinants cannot be empirically verified from the current data and should be considered speculative. Nevertheless, it points to the potential importance of incorporating constructs that reflect these environmental determinants within integrated models such as the trans-contextual model.

Turning to the direct effect of attitude on leisure-time physical activity participation, current findings are consistent with previous research that has found a direct effect of attitude components on behavior in multiple health contexts [44, 45]. Such effects might represent affective motives to engage in a behavior learned through positive or negative experiences that coincide with the behavior [72]. As a consequence, the anticipation of rewarding affective responses may be reasons why children and adolescents engage in physical activities outside of school without the need for reasoned decision making. Such an effect has not been identified in previous research adopting the trans-contextual model, but has been consistently identified in research applying the theory of planned behavior in health behavior contexts, including physical activity [45, 72].

With respect to the direct effect of past physical activity behavior, current findings corroborate previous research reporting effects of past behavior on subsequent behavior in social cognition theories [e.g., 14, 25, 47, 73]. This research demonstrates that past behavior accounts for substantive variance in behavior and often attenuates effects of other constructs. The inclusion of past behavior in social cognition models is important as it provides an indication of the sufficiency of the theory [9, 73]. The absence of effects of theory constructs other than past behavior provides an indication that the theory may be inadequate as a means to explain behavior beyond the stability of the behavior itself. Although in the case of the current research, the exclusion of past behavior did not restore effects of other constructs such as intention on behavior.

So, what might the large-sized effect of past behavior represent? Researchers have suggested that past behavior may model effects of unmeasured constructs in tests of these theories [9, 25]. Given social cognition theories incorporate constructs that reflect reasoned, deliberative processes, past behavior effects may model effects of constructs representing non-conscious processes such as habits and implicit beliefs. The substantive effect of past physical activity behavior on leisure-time physical activity participation in the current study suggests that lower secondary school students’ physical activity in their leisure time may be a function of these kinds of constructs. Current findings suggest, however, that habit may not be among these determinants, given that the independent effect of self-reported habit on leisure-time physical activity participation was no different from zero, and habit did not mediate effects of past behavior on physical activity participation. Although it must be stressed that the current measure of habit focused exclusively on automaticity, one aspect of habit, and may not have sufficiently captured all habitual influences [e.g., 74]. The current study did not include measures that capture other aspects of habit such as context stability and accessibility of relevant cues to the behavior [26]. In addition, we did not measure other constructs that may reflect these non-conscious processes, such as implicitly held beliefs developed through past experiences of the behavior covarying with evaluations [24]. Research has suggested that measures of implicit beliefs predict behavior, including physical activity participation, independent of intentions [20] and may also mediate effects of past behavior on subsequent behavior [28]. The effects of past behavior in the current study may, therefore, indicate that physical activity behavior in leisure time may be a function of unmeasured constructs reflecting implicit processes, but such an inference is speculative and requires empirical verification.

Finally, consistent with previous research on self-determination theory, we found an indirect effect of autonomous motivation in leisure-time on both intentions and behavior mediated by trait self-control. Research on self-determination theory suggests that autonomous motivation is associated with better self-regulatory capacity and resilience in the face of self-control resource depletion [40, 42]. These findings are an important augmentation of trans-contextual model as they provide an alternative process by which individuals enact leisure-time physical activity in the absence of the ‘motivational sequence’ outlined in the original model. This finding lends additional support to the ‘energizing’ effect of autonomous motivation–students in the current study were more likely to report greater self-control if they perceived their behavior to be autonomous. As self-control was measured as a trait and was not specific to physical activity in the current study, the self-control-autonomous motivation relationship in the current study may, in fact, reflect a general tendency for autonomously motivated individuals to report greater self-control. This needs to be corroborated at the trait level, such as examining relations between causality orientations from self-determination theory and trait self-control, and examine whether such individual differences are behaviorally relevant [75].

The current research also illustrates the value of adopting a Bayesian analytic approach to combine prior knowledge of the distributions of model effects with the observed distributions to produce precise estimates and variability among model constructs. This was demonstrated by the narrowing of the credibility intervals about some of the model parameters. Importantly, the data used for the informative priors was highly reliable given they were derived from meta-analyses of multiple studies with large samples sizes. It is, however, also important to note that although the informative priors for the trans-contextual model effects were a meta-analysis of studies on samples of school students with similar profile to the participants in the current study [18], priors for the effects of the additional variables, self-reported habit and trait self-control were derived from research from multiple populations and mostly adult samples [38, 46]. Therefore, the priors were not directly comparable to the current sample. Nevertheless, current findings may be of value as a source of informative priors for future applications of the extended trans-contextual model. Consistent with the Bayesian approach, the current study should form part of an ongoing iterative research process that yields increasingly precise estimates of effects in the model.

Strengths, limitations and recommendations for future research

Strengths of the current study include (1) a focus on the determinants of lower secondary school students’ leisure-time physical activity participation, a priority area of research; (2) the application of an extended trans-contextual model, an integrative multi-theory approach that provided a priori hypotheses on the relations among the determinants and leisure-time physical activity participation; (3) adoption of a two-wave prospective design using validated measures of model determinants and behavior; and (4) application of Bayesian analytic procedures that enabled utilization of prior knowledge to arrive at precise estimates of model effects. However, it is also important to note limitations of the current research that may affect interpretation of the findings and the extent to which they can be generalized.

While we endeavored to incorporate additional constructs representing non-conscious determinants of leisure-time physical activity participation in the current study, our measures did not encompass a full range of candidate determinants. For example, the current study did not include measures of implicit cognition and motivation with respect to school students’ leisure-time physical activity participation. Given that measures of constructs such as implicit beliefs and autonomous motivation have been shown to predict behavior directly independent of intentions in adult samples [20, 28, 76], future tests of the extended trans-contextual model should consider incorporating measures of these constructs as predictors of leisure-time physical activity participation. This is particularly important given the lack of effects of the intentional or motivational constructs on leisure-time physical activity participation in the current study, and inclusion of implicit beliefs may assist in providing an explanation of the effects of past behavior.

We also did not include the beliefs that underpin the attitude and subjective norm constructs [9]. Their effects on intentions and behavior are typically mediated by the direct attitude and subjective norm measures. Similarly, we did not include constructs related to socioecological environment that may determine behavior, and whose effects on behavior may be mediated by the social cognition constructs in the model [70]. There is precedence for the indirect effect of these beliefs and socio-ecological constructs in the model. Research has demonstrated that beliefs and socio-ecological factors relating to context and environment are related to the social cognition constructs that predict health behavior, and those constructs mediate the effects of the beliefs and socio-ecological factors on behavior [77, 78]. While the constructs in the current model are proposed to account for the effects of these variables, such influences need empirical verification and serve as an avenue for future research.

In addition, current data are correlational, which limits the extent to which we could infer causal relations among the extended trans-contextual model constructs. As with many model tests, including those of the trans-contextual model, causal effects are inferred from theory not the data [18]. Future research should consider the adoption of panel designs that permit modeling of temporal change and direction among trans-contextual model constructs over time through cross-lagged effects [12]. Such designs should also consider examining measuring model constructs over longer periods of time to test the capacity of the model to account for long-term change in its constructs and physical activity behavior, see Jacobs et al. [79] for an example. In addition, intervention and experimental designs that adopt appropriate behavior change techniques [80, 81] are needed to test the effect of manipulating the constructs found to have a direct effect on leisure-time physical activity participation [82]. For example, interventions targeting attitudes should seek to promote enjoyment and positive affect through positive experiences of physical activity, and interventions targeting self-discipline should seek to provide self-regulatory skills that promote better control over impulses to spend excessive time on leisure-time alternatives to physical activity (e.g., video games, watching television) and identify and manage barriers.

Conclusion

The current research is the first to test an extended version of the trans-contextual model to identify determinants of leisure-time physical activity participation in lower secondary school students. Results indicate that the traditional motivational and social cognition constructs are effective in predicting leisure-time physical activity intentions, but not actual behavior. However, we found direct effects of trait self-control and attitude on leisure-time physical activity participation, suggesting that students’ physical activity participation was determined by constructs representing non-conscious processes. A further innovation of the current research is the application of a Bayesian analytic approach to update the effects and variability estimates of model parameters based on previous meta-analytic findings. Results raise questions over the effectiveness of the original trans-contextual model constructs in determining leisure-time physical activity participation, at least in the current societal context in which the physical environment may not support engagement in physical activity and offers various competing, non-active alternatives (e.g., video games). However, current findings highlight the potential of including additional constructs representing non-conscious processes. However, these data should not be considered unequivocal evidence to support rejection of the model as unmeasured moderator variables may have affected model effects. Further replication of the extended trans-contextual model predictions in larger samples is warranted.

Supporting information

S1 File

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Pauliina Hietanen, Noora Kilpeläinen, Sampsa Löppönen, Stina Seppänen, and Miika Tuominen for their assistance with data collection. We are grateful to Jari Villberg for his assistance with the data analysis. Jekaterina Schneider is now with the Centre for Appearance Research at University of the West of England, Bristol, UK.

Data Availability

Data, analysis scripts and output, study materials, and supplemental materials are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/z8axj/).

Funding Statement

This project was supported by the Finnish Ministry of Education and Culture under grant number OKM/62/626/2016) awarded to MSH and TL; and a Finland Distinguished Professor (FiDiPro) award from Business Finland under grant number 1801/31/2105 awarded to MSH. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Kurdaningsih SV, Sudargo T, Lusmilasari L. Physical activity and sedentary lifestyle towards teenagers’ overweight/obesity status. Int J Comm Med Pub Health. 2017;3(3):630–5. 10.18203/2394-6040.ijcmph20160623 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Guinhouya BC, Samouda H, de Beaufort C. Level of physical activity among children and adolescents in Europe: A review of physical activity assessed objectively by accelerometry. Pub Health. 2013;127(4):301–11. 10.1016/j.puhe.2013.01.020 doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2013.01.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Pate RR, Dowda M. Raising an active and healthy generation: A comprehensive public health initiative. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2018;7(1):3–14. 10.1249/JES.0000000000000171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Powell E, Woodfield LA, Nevill AM. Increasing physical activity levels in primary school physical education: The SHARP Principles Model. Prev Med Reports. 2016;3:7–13. 10.1016/j.pmedr.2015.11.007 doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2015.11.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NLD, Culverhouse T, Biddle SJH. The processes by which perceived autonomy support in physical education promotes leisure-time physical activity intentions and behavior: A trans-contextual model. J Educ Psychol. 2003;95(4):784–95. 10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.784 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Trudeau F, Shephard RJ. Physical education, school physical activity, school sports and academic performance. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2008;5(1):10. 10.1186/1479-5868-5-10 doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-5-10 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Deci EL, Ryan RM. Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1985. doi: 10.1097/00007691-198512000-00010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Vallerand RJ. Towards a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1997;29:271–360. 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60019-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1991;50(2):179–211. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NLD. Advances in self-determination theory research in sport and exercise. 2007;8(5):597–9. 10.1016/j.psychsport.2007.06.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lim BSC, Wang CKJ. Perceived autonomy support, behavioural regulations in physical education and physical activity intention. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2009;10(1):52–60. 10.1016/j.psychsport.2008.06.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Chan DKC, Zhang L, Lee ASY, Hagger MS. Reciprocal relations between autonomous motivation from self-determination theory and social cognition constructs from the theory of planned behavior: A cross-lagged panel design in sport injury prevention. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2020;48:101660. 10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101660 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Chatzisarantis NLD, Hagger MS, Wang CKJ, Thøgersen-Ntoumani C. The effects of social identity and perceived autonomy support on health behaviour within the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Curr Psychol. 2009;28(1):55–68. 10.1007/s12144-009-9043-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chatzisarantis NLD, Hagger MS, Brickell T. Using the construct of perceived autonomy support to understand social influence within the theory of planned behavior. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2008;9:27–44. 10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.12.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Shen B, McCaughtry N, Martin J. Urban adolescents’ exercise intentions and behaviors: An exploratory study of a trans-contextual model. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2008;33(4):841–58. 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.09.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Chan DKC, Dimmock JA, Donovan RJ, Hardcastle S, Lentillon-Kaestner V, Hagger MS. Self-determined motivation in sport predicts motivation and intention of anti-doping behaviors: A perspective from the trans-contextual Model. J Sci Med Sport. 2015;18(3):315–22. 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.04.001 doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kalajas-Tilga H, Hein V, Koka A, Tilga H, Raudsepp L, Hagger MS. Application of the trans-contextual model to predict change in leisure time physical activity. Psychol Health. 2021. 10.1080/08870446.2020.1869741 doi: 10.1080/08870446.2020.1869741 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NLD. The trans-contextual model of autonomous motivation in education: Conceptual and empirical issues and meta-analysis. Rev Educ Res. 2016;86(2):360–407. 10.3102/0034654315585005 doi: 10.3102/0034654315585005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Strack F, Deutsch R. Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2004;8:220–47. 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1 doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hagger MS, Trost N, Keech J, Chan DKC, Hamilton K. Predicting sugar consumption: Application of an integrated dual-process, dual-phase model. Appetite. 2017;116:147–56. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.04.032 doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.04.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Verplanken B, Orbell S. Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2003;33:1313–30. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bogg T. Conscientiousness, the transtheoretical model of change, and exercise: A neo-socioanalytic integration of trait and social-cognitive frameworks in the prediction of behavior. J Pers. 2008;76(4):775–802. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00504.x doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00504.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Chatzisarantis NLD, Hagger MS. Influences of personality traits and continuation intentions on physical activity participation within the theory of planned behaviour. Psychol Health. 2008;23(3):347–67. 10.1080/14768320601185866 doi: 10.1080/14768320601185866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hagger MS. Redefining habits and linking habits with other implicit processes. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2020;46:101606. 10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.101606 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ouellette JA, Wood W. Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychol Bull. 1998;124(1):54–74. 10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.54 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Wood W. Habit in personality and social psychology. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2017;21(4):389–403. 10.1177/1088868317720362 doi: 10.1177/1088868317720362 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.van Bree RJH, van Stralen MM, Mudde AN, Bolman C, de Vries H, Lechner L. Habit as mediator of the relationship between prior and later physical activity: A longitudinal study in older adults. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2015;19(1):95–102. 10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.03.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hamilton K, Gibbs I, Keech JJ, Hagger MS. Reasoned and implicit processes in heavy episodic drinking: An integrated dual process model. Br J Health Psychol. 2020;25(1):189–209. 10.1111/BJHP.12401 doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hamilton K, Kirkpatrick A, Rebar A, Hagger MS. Child sun safety: Application of an integrated behavior change model. Health Psychol. 2017;36(9):916–26. 10.1037/hea0000533 doi: 10.1037/hea0000533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tak NI, te Velde SJ, Oenema A, Van der Horst K, Timperio A, Crawford D, et al. The association between home environmental variables and soft drink consumption among adolescents. Exploration of mediation by individual cognitions and habit strength. Appetite. 2011;56(2):503–10. 10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.013 doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Orbell S, Verplanken B. The automatic component of habit in health behavior: Habit as cue-contingent automaticity. Health Psychol. 2010;29(4):374–83. 10.1037/a0019596 doi: 10.1037/a0019596 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Danner UN, Aarts H, de Vries NK. Habit vs. intention in the prediction of future behaviour: The role of frequency, context stability and mental accessibility of past behaviour. Br J Soc Psychol. 2008;47(2):245–65. 10.1348/014466607X230876 doi: 10.1348/014466607X230876 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Teixeira PJ, Carraca E, Markland DA, Silva M, Ryan RM. Exercise, physical activity, and self-determination theory: A systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9(1):78. 10.1186/1479-5868-9-78 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wood W, Rünger D. Psychology of habit. Ann Rev Psychol. 2016;67(1):289–314. 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033417 doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033417 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hagger MS. Habit and physical activity: Theoretical advances, practical implications, and agenda for future research. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2019;42:118–29. 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.12.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kaushal N, Bérubé B, Hagger MS, Bherer L. Investigating the role of self-control beliefs in predicting exercise behavior: A longitudinal study. Br J Health Psychol. 2021. 10.1111/bjhp.12525 doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Pers. 2004;72(2):271–324. 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.de Ridder DTD, Lensvelt-Mulders G, Finkenauer C, Stok FM, Baumeister RF. Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2012;16(1):76–99. 10.1177/1088868311418749 doi: 10.1177/1088868311418749 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hagger MS, Hankonen N, Kangro E- M, Lintunen T, Pagaduan J, Polet J, et al. Trait self-control, social cognition constructs, and intentions: Correlational evidence for mediation and moderation effects in diverse health behaviors. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2019;11(3):407–37. 10.1111/aphw.12153 doi: 10.1111/aphw.12153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Moller AC, Deci EL, Ryan RM. Choice and ego depletion: The moderating role of autonomy. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2006;32:1024–36. 10.1177/0146167206288008 doi: 10.1177/0146167206288008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Muraven M. Autonomous self-control is less depleting. J Res Pers. 2008;42:763–70. 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.08.002 doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.08.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Muraven M, Gagne M, Rosman H. Helpful self-control: Autonomy support, vitality, and depletion. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2008;44:573–85. 10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.008 doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hagger MS, Gucciardi DF, Turrell A, Hamilton K. Self-control and health-related behavior: The role of implicit self-control, trait self-control, and lay beliefs in self-control. Br J Health Psychol. 2019;24(4):764–86. 10.1111/bjhp.12378 doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lawton R, Conner MT, McEachan R. Desire or reason: Predicting health behaviors from affective and cognitive attitudes. Health Psychol. 2009;28(1):56–65. 10.1037/a0013424 doi: 10.1037/a0013424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Conner MT, McEachan R, Taylor N, O’Hara J, Lawton R. Role of affective attitudes and anticipated affective reactions in predicting health behaviors. Health Psychol. 2015;34(6):642–52. 10.1037/hea0000143 doi: 10.1037/hea0000143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gardner B, de Bruijn G- J, Lally P. A systematic review and meta-analysis of applications of the self-report habit index to nutrition and physical activity behaviours. Ann Behav Med. 2011;42(2):174–87. 10.1007/s12160-011-9282-0 doi: 10.1007/s12160-011-9282-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hagger MS, Polet J, Lintunen T. The reasoned action approach applied to health behavior: Role of past behavior and test of some key moderators using meta-analytic structural equation modeling. Soc Sci Med. 2018;213:85–94. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.07.038 doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.07.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Phipps D, Hagger MS, Hamilton K. Predicting limiting ‘free sugar’ consumption using an integrated model of health behavior. Appetite. 2020;150:104668. 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104668 doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Gardner B, Abraham C, Lally P, de Bruijn G-J. Towards parsimony in habit measurement: Testing the convergent and predictive validity of an automaticity subscale of the self-report habit index. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9(1):102. 10.1186/1479-5868-9-102 doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-9-102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Polet J, Hassandra M, Lintunen T, Laukkanen A, Hankonen N, Hirvensalo M, et al. Using physical education to promote out-of school physical activity in lower secondary school students—A randomized controlled trial protocol. BMC Pub Health. 2019;19:157. 10.1186/s12889-019-6478-x doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-6478-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NLD, Hein V, Pihu M, Soós I, Karsai I. The perceived autonomy support scale for exercise settings (PASSES): Development, validity, and cross-cultural invariance in young people. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2007;8(5):632–53. 10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.09.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ryan RM, Connell JP. Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;57:749–61. 10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.749 doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ajzen I. Constructing a TPB questionnaire: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts; 2002. updated September 1, 2002. Available from: http://people.umass.edu/~aizen/pdf/tpb.measurement.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.IPIP. Self-discipline scale from the NEO-PI-R. 2017. Available from: https://ipip.ori.org/newNEOKey.htm#Self-Discipline.
  • 55.Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjostrom M, Bauman AE, Booth ML, Ainsworth BE, et al. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2003;35(8):1381–95. 10.1249/01.mss.0000078924.61453.fb doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hagger MS, Hamilton K. Grit and self-discipline as predictors of effort and academic attainment. Br J Educ Psychol. 2019;89(2):324–42. 10.1111/bjep.12241 doi: 10.1111/bjep.12241 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Polet J, Lintunen T, Schneider J, Hagger MS. Predicting change in middle school students’ leisure-time physical activity participation: A prospective test of the trans-contextual model. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2020;50(9):512–23. 10.1111/jasp.12691 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Savalei V. A comparison of several approaches for controlling measurement error in small samples. Psychol Methods. 2019;24(3):352–70. 10.1037/met0000181 doi: 10.1037/met0000181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Ashton MC, Lee K, Goldberg LR. The IPIP-HEXACO scales: An alternative, public-domain measure of the personality constructs in the HEXACO model. Pers Individ Diff. 2007;42:1515–26. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.10.027 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.The IPAQ group. International Physical Activity Questionnaire. 2012. Available from: http://www.ipaq.ki.se/.
  • 61.Muthén BO, Asparouhov T. Bayesian structural equation modeling: A more flexible representation of substantive theory. Psychol Methods. 2012;17:313–35. 10.1037/a0026802 doi: 10.1037/a0026802 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Gelman A, Rubin DB. Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Stat Sci. 1992;7:457–511. 10.1214/ss/1177011136 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Yuan Y, MacKinnon DP. Bayesian mediation analysis. Psychol Methods. 2009;14(4):301–22. 10.1037/a0016972 doi: 10.1037/a0016972 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.MacCallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM. Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psych Methods. 1996;1(2):130–49. 10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Zhang Z, Yuan K-H, editors. Practical statistical power analysis using Webpower and R. Granger, IN: ISDSA Press; 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Brown DJ, Hagger MS, Morrissey S, Hamilton K. Predicting fruit and vegetable consumption in long-haul heavy goods vehicle drivers: Application of a multi-theory, dual-phase model and the contribution of past behaviour. Appetite. 2017;121(1):326–36. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.11.106 doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.11.106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NLD. Integrating the theory of planned behaviour and self-determination theory in health behaviour: A meta-analysis. Br J Health Psychol. 2009;14(2):275–302. 10.1348/135910708X373959 doi: 10.1348/135910708X373959 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Hamilton K, Cox S, White KM. Testing a model of physical activity among mothers and fathers of young children: Integrating self-determined motivation, planning, and theory of planned behavior. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2012;34(1):124–45. 10.1123/jsep.34.1.124 doi: 10.1123/jsep.34.1.124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Cooke R, Sheeran P. Moderation of cognition-intention and cognition-behaviour relations: A meta-analysis of properties of variables from the theory of planned behaviour. Br J Soc Psychol. 2004;43(2):159–86. 10.1348/0144666041501688 doi: 10.1348/0144666041501688 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Salmon J, Hesketh KD, Arundell L, Downing KL, Biddle SJH. Changing behavior using ecological models. In: Hagger MS, Cameron LD, Hamilton K, Hankonen N, Lintunen T, editors. The handbook of behavior change. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2020. p. 237–50. 10.1017/97811086773180.017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Zhang CQ, Wong MC- Y, Zhang R, Hamilton K, Hagger MS. Adolescent sugar-sweetened beverage consumption: An extended health action process approach. Appetite. 2019;141:104332. 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104332 doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Conroy DE, Berry TR. Automatic affective evaluations of physical activity. Ex Sport Sci Rev. 2017;45(4):230–7. 10.1249/JES.0000000000000120. doi: 10.1249/JES.0000000000000120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Chatzisarantis NLD, Hagger MS, Smith B, Phoenix C. The influences of continuation intentions on the execution of social behaviour within the theory of planned behaviour. Br J Soc Psychol. 2004;43(4):551–83. 10.1348/0144666042565399 doi: 10.1348/0144666042565399 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Hagger MS, Rebar AL, Mullan BA, Lipp OV, Chatzisarantis NLD. The subjective experience of habit captured by self-report indexes may lead to inaccuracies in the measurement of habitual action. Health Psychol Rev. 2015;9(3):296–302. 10.1080/17437199.2014.959728 doi: 10.1080/17437199.2014.959728 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Hagger MS, Hamilton K. General causality orientations in self-determination theory: Meta-analysis and test of a process model. Eur J Pers. 2020. 10.1177/0890207020962330 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Arnautovska U, Fleig L, O’Callaghan F, Hamilton K. Older adults’ physical activity: The integration of autonomous motivation and theory of planned behaviour constructs. Aust Psychol. 2019;54(1):46–54. 10.1111/ap.12346 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Hagger MS, Hamilton K. Effects of socio-structural variables in the theory of planned behavior: A mediation model in multiple samples and behaviors. Psychol Health. 2021;36(3):307–33. 10.1080/08870446.2020.1784420 doi: 10.1080/08870446.2020.1784420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NLD, Biddle SJH, Orbell S. Antecedents of children’s physical activity intentions and behaviour: Predictive validity and longitudinal effects. Psychol Health. 2001;16(4):391–407. 10.1080/08870440108405515 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Jacobs N, Hagger MS, Streukens S, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Claes N. Testing an integrated model of the theory of planned behaviour and self-determination theory for different energy-balance related behaviours and intervention intensities. Br J Health Psychol. 2011;16(1):113–34. 10.1348/135910710X519305 doi: 10.1348/135910710X519305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Hardcastle SJ, Fortier MS, Blake N, Hagger MS. Identifying content-based and relational techniques to change behavior in motivational interviewing. 2017;11(1):1–16. 10.1080/17437199.2016.1190659 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Knittle K, Heino MTJ, Marques MM, Stenius M, Beattie M, Ehbrecht F, et al. The compendium of self-enactable techniques to change and self-manage motivation and behaviour v1. 0. Nat Hum Behav. 2020;4:215–23. 10.1038/s41562-019-0798-9 doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0798-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Hagger MS, Cameron LD, Hamilton K, Hankonen N, Lintunen T, editors. The handbook of behavior change. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2020. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Lambros Lazuras

19 Mar 2021

PONE-D-21-02994

Predictors of School Students’ Leisure-Time Physical Activity: An Extended Trans-Contextual Model Using Bayesian Path Analysis

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Hagger,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Two expert reviewers with a track record of publications in the TCM and physical activity have reviewed your manuscript and provided detailed comments. I concur with their evaluation and have decided that minor revisions are needed before your manuscript is considered for publication. 

Please submit your revised manuscript by May 01 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Lambros Lazuras

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

  1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

3. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

4. Thank you for submitting the above manuscript to PLOS ONE. During our internal evaluation of the manuscript, we found significant text overlap between your submission and the following previously published work, of which you are an author.

- https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S1041608018301468?via%3Dihub ("Motivational predictors of students' participation in out-of-school learning activities and academic attainment in science: An application of the trans-contextual model using Bayesian path analysis" by Hagger and Hamilton, 2018)

We would like to make you aware that copying extracts from previous publications, especially outside the methods section, word-for-word is unacceptable. In addition, the reproduction of text from published reports has implications for the copyright that may apply to the publications.

Please revise the manuscript to rephrase the duplicated text, cite your sources, and provide details as to how the current manuscript advances on previous work. Please note that further consideration is dependent on the submission of a manuscript that addresses these concerns about the overlap in text with published work.

We will carefully review your manuscript upon resubmission, so please ensure that your revision is thorough.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: General comments

This is an interesting manuscript testing an extended trans-contextual model to examine effects of motivational and social cognition constructs alongside the potential influence of implicit processes in influencing leisure time physical activity behavior among secondary school students. The study is based on sound theory with extended and clear theoretical explanations of the psychological mechanisms tested starting from the influence of PE teachers’ behavior on the psychological mechanisms operating to influence intentions and involvement in leisure time physical activity. Other positive aspects of the manuscript involve the prospective design used with behavior measured five weeks after assessment of the psychological constructs, the Bayesian method of path analyses conducted, and the testing of implicit processes added to the model. Overall, the study is well designed, clearly described in terms of the theoretical processes tested and the instruments used, statistical analysis is clearly reported, and the discussion covers in a satisfactory way study findings while the authors also state study limitations with sufficient analysis. Overall, I have been delighted reading the current manuscript and I believe it adds significant knowledge in the extant literature on motivational processes among secondary school students’ participation in leisure time physical activity.

Specific comments

Measures, Results. The authors have not reported confirmatory factor analysis results for the measurement tools used to measures the variables of the theory of planned behavior, habit, and trait self-control or other evidence supportive of the validity of scores derived from the instruments used in the present study. Therefore, it would strengthen the manuscript if the above information was presently added.

Reviewer #2: The present study investigated an extension of the trans-contextual

model (TCM) of motivation incorporating constructs related to implicit

process. It is a very interesting paper, technically well-written

providing sufficient contribution to the understanding of the effect

of school physical education on out-of-school physical activity. Below

there are some minor comments for authors to further increase the

clarity of their argumentation.

In the proposed model, the implicit processes constructs are not

somehow related to the TCM. In my understanding the core of the TCM is

that influences through a sequence of effects out-of-school physical

activity. In previous extensions of the model, the added variables

were clearly affected by autonomy supportive climate in physical

education and assisted in explaining more variance or introduced a new

pathway through which physical education climate influences

out-of-school physical activity. In the present study, this is not

apparent. The extensions of the model seem to work outside the core

hypotheses of the model. In fact, it seems that the added variables

address the intention-behavior gap, rather than contribute to the

motivational sequence proposed by the model. In this sense, it is not

clear to me how the proposed extension of the model helps us

understand the effect of physical education motivational climate on

out-of-school physical activity, which is the core aim of TCM (at

least as it is applied in the physical education-leisure time

association). In this sense, I would expect to see paths linking the

school environment (i.e., climate, motivational regulations) to

constructs such as trait self-control; there is evidence in the

literature linking autonomous motivation to self-regulation and

metacognition for instance. In this line, I would expect to see a path

from leisure-time autonomous motivation on habits; i.e., autonomously

motivated students would endorse more often habitual physical activity

or hold relevant beliefs.

Personally, I don’t see the lack of effect of intention on behavior as

a limitation of the TCM. The core and unique in the literature

premises of the model (i.e., trans-contextual effects of motivation,

effect of motivation on proximal to behavior social cognitive

variables) have been supported in the present study. The

intention-behavior gap, in my mind, is a limitation of the TPB and

had has been extensively studied in the literature. In this sense, I

don’t think that the authors should discard the value of TCM, as it

seems to be done in the discussion section.

In this line, I think that the second and third explanations offered

focus on the intention-behavior gap, rather than the utility of the

TCM. If the focus of the paper is on better understanding the

intention-behavior gap, then what is the value of testing the whole TCM?

Also, I am not sure I agree with the arguments presented in p. 26

about the spontaneous participation of children in physical activity.

And the reason for this is that if children hold positive views about

physical activity they will be actively involve in systematic physical

activity. If not, then spontaneous participation may be for external

to physical activity reasons, i.e., pressure by peers etc. In this

case the value of participation is questionable. I am not aware of any

evidence suggesting that such spontaneous participation in physical

activity will eventually result in more systematic participation in

the future. In this sense, the whole concept of implicit processes

should be conceived with caution about its contribution in

understanding systematic physical activity.

I found really interesting the discussion on the association of past

behavior and habits. Drawing back to my previous comment, I would

expect a path linking these two variables. This association may

provide, in my mind, a perfect bridge between explicit and implicit

processes associated with physical activity.

Overall, I think that a stronger association is needed of the added

variables with the core model.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Nov 12;16(11):e0258829. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258829.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


21 May 2021

RESPONSES TO REVIEWER 1’S COMMENTS

REVIEWER’S COMMENT 1: General comments

This is an interesting manuscript testing an extended trans-contextual model to examine effects of motivational and social cognition constructs alongside the potential influence of implicit processes in influencing leisure time physical activity behavior among secondary school students. The study is based on sound theory with extended and clear theoretical explanations of the psychological mechanisms tested starting from the influence of PE teachers’ behavior on the psychological mechanisms operating to influence intentions and involvement in leisure time physical activity. Other positive aspects of the manuscript involve the prospective design used with behavior measured five weeks after assessment of the psychological constructs, the Bayesian method of path analyses conducted, and the testing of implicit processes added to the model. Overall, the study is well designed, clearly described in terms of the theoretical processes tested and the instruments used, statistical analysis is clearly reported, and the discussion covers in a satisfactory way study findings while the authors also state study limitations with sufficient analysis. Overall, I have been delighted reading the current manuscript and I believe it adds significant knowledge in the extant literature on motivational processes among secondary school students’ participation in leisure time physical activity.

AUTHORS’ RESPONSE: We thank the Reviewer for the positive feedback on our manuscript and for highlighting its strengths and contribution. We have responded to their comments below, and have used the tracked changes function to mark the changes we have made to the revised manuscript.

REVIEWER’S COMMENT 2: Specific comments

Measures, Results. The authors have not reported confirmatory factor analysis results for the measurement tools used to measures the variables of the theory of planned behavior, habit, and trait self-control or other evidence supportive of the validity of scores derived from the instruments used in the present study. Therefore, it would strengthen the manuscript if the above information was presently added.

AUTHORS’ RESPONSE: The Reviewer raises an important point regarding the importance of confirmatory evidence for validity of the measures used to tap the model constructs. We did not conduct a full structural equation model in the current analysis because the complexity of the model relative to the sample size would have presented problems with convergence and stability of the factors, so such a model was contraindicated in this context. We did not conduct a confirmatory factor analysis for the same reasons. However, all measures were adapted directly from previous analyses using the trans-contextual model and the measures of habit and self-control have also been validated in numerous previous analyses. These analyses lend support for the construct validity of these measures through confirmatory factor analyses (e.g., factor loadings, average variance extracted, composite reliability). In addition, we conducted pilot analyses to test the validity of the measures used in the target population. These analyses used a single-indicator latent variable approach using omega reliability estimates to control for measurement error (note: simulation research using full structural equation models and single-indicator models has revealed little difference in the parameter estimates in models in either approach). These data provided support for the use of these measures in this population, particularly the predictive validity of the measures in the context of the trans-contextual model. We have cited these sources of validity in the revised manuscript (please see pages 13, lines 275-283 of the revised manuscript).

RESPONSES TO REVIEWER 2’S COMMENTS

REVIEWER’S COMMENT 1: The present study investigated an extension of the trans-contextual model (TCM) of motivation incorporating constructs related to implicit process. It is a very interesting paper, technically well-written providing sufficient contribution to the understanding of the effect of school physical education on out-of-school physical activity. Below there are some minor comments for authors to further increase the clarity of their argumentation.

AUTHORS’ RESPONSE: We are grateful to the Reviewer for providing positive feedback and constructive comments on our manuscript. We have responded to each comment in the point-by-point list below, and have used the track changes function to identify the changes we have made as a result of the revision process.

REVIEWER’S COMMENT 2: In the proposed model, the implicit processes constructs are not somehow related to the TCM. In my understanding the core of the TCM is that influences through a sequence of effects out-of-school physical activity. In previous extensions of the model, the added variables were clearly affected by autonomy supportive climate in physical education and assisted in explaining more variance or introduced a new pathway through which physical education climate influences out-of-school physical activity. In the present study, this is not apparent. The extensions of the model seem to work outside the core hypotheses of the model. In fact, it seems that the added variables address the intention-behavior gap, rather than contribute to the motivational sequence proposed by the model. In this sense, it is not clear to me how the proposed extension of the model helps us understand the effect of physical education motivational climate on out-of school physical activity, which is the core aim of TCM (at least as it is applied in the physical education-leisure time association). In this sense, I would expect to see paths linking the school environment (i.e., climate, motivational regulations) to constructs such as trait self-control; there is evidence in the literature linking autonomous motivation to self-regulation and metacognition for instance. In this line, I would expect to see a path from leisure-time autonomous motivation on habits; i.e., autonomously motivated students would endorse more often habitual physical activity or hold relevant beliefs.

AUTHORS’ RESPONSE: We thank the Reviewer for identifying this important issue. We agree that the original proposition of the TCM was to examine the processes by which autonomy support in PE relates to autonomous motivation toward physical activity in leisure time and intention toward, and actual participation in, leisure time physical activity. It does so by integrating constructs and hypotheses from self-determination theory (SDT) and the theory of planned behavior (TPB). The intention-behavior relationship, and the limitations thereof, are, as a consequence, integral to these processes. Knowledge of factors that may facilitate or inhibit the enactment of intentions, is, therefore, also integral to the TCM, so any constructs that may affect this relationship is also important to the model. In addition, the ultimate goal of the TCM is to explain variance in students’ leisure-time physical activity. As a consequence, augmenting the model to include constructs that represent other, parallel processes that may explain variance in physical activity in this population make important additions to the model and assist in its explanatory value. This is consistent with the suggestions made by Ajzen (1991) who indicated that the TPB is open to the inclusion of other constructs provided they make an independent contribution to the explanation of behavior. Other theorists, particularly those who have concerned themselves with the integration of theories (e.g., Hagger & Hamilton, 2020; Montaño & Kasprzyk, 2015), have made similar suggestions and highlighted that theories should be regarded as ‘living things’ and ‘works in progress’ that are not axiomatic or immutable, but open to modification and expansion provided those additions have conceptual value and, as Ajzen proposed, stand up to empirical rigor in facilitating explanation of behavior. The modified version of the TCM satisfies these criteria. The added constructs (habit and trait self-control) are consistent theoretically in that they outline alternative processes by which students may enact leisure-time physical activity, independent of the process outlined in the original model. These implicit processes are those that have been outlined in other social cognition models that have the TPB at their roots and previous integrated models (e.g., Hagger et al., 2017; Hamilton et al., 2020; van Bree et al., 2015). On the issue of relations between the motivational constructs from SDT and the added habit and self-control constructs in the extended model, we originally included correlations between these constructs as free parameters in the model, although we did not specify hypothesized relations among them. However, we agree with the Reviewer that these represent interesting and important additions. So we have included hypotheses for direct effects of autonomous motivation in leisure time on both habit (H19) and self-control (H20), and included them in the model. We have also specified, consistent with the Reviewer’s comments, potential indirect effects for autonomous motivation on intentions and behavior through these variables (see hypotheses H34, H35 and H39, H40 in the revised manuscript). These have been included in Tables 1 and 2, and specified in Figures 1 and 2. We have also included a summary of the conceptual basis for these effects in the Introduction section (please see page 7, lines 134-148 and page 8, lines 163-176) and the hypothesis section at the end of the Introduction (please see page 10, lines 210-219), reported the Results for these effects (please see page 24, lines 438-441), and explained their relevance to the TCM in the Discussion section (please see pages 31-32, lines 609-624).

REVIEWER’S COMMENT 3: Personally, I don’t see the lack of effect of intention on behavior as a limitation of the TCM. The core and unique in the literature premises of the model (i.e., trans contextual effects of motivation, effect of motivation on proximal to behavior social cognitive variables) have been supported in the present study. The intention-behavior gap, in my mind, is a limitation of the TPB and had has been extensively studied in the literature. In this sense, I don’t think that the authors should discard the value of TCM, as it seems to be done in the discussion section.

AUTHORS’ RESPONSE: As we stated in our response to the previous comment, the intention-behavior ‘gap’ is a key prediction of the TPB. It is also integral to the ‘motivational sequence’ and third premise of the TCM (Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2016). The intention-behavior relationship should not, therefore, be disregarded in the context of the TCM. As the Reviewer notes in the previous comment, the TCM is concerned, through the proposed ‘motivational sequence’ with the processes that relate to intentions toward, and participation in, physical activity in leisure time. The intention-behavior relation is integral to this sequence, and so its rejection therefore would lead to a rejection of the motivational sequence, at least as a means to explain the motivational processes that lead to behavior. Consideration of non-deliberative predictors of physical activity behavior within the TCM is, therefore, important because it identifies potential determinants of physical activity participation in situations where the traditional TCM components may ‘fail’ to predict behavior. This would mean that the TCM is better equipped to explain instances (e.g., populations, contexts) in which intentions do not predict behavior and the motivational sequence does not account for variance in the key dependent variable of the model. We have, however, been careful not to dismiss the TCM outright, but rather highlight that for certain populations and in certain contexts, the lack of association between intention and behavior may mean that alternative processes that act in parallel to the motivational sequence may be accounting for behavior (please see page 26-27, lines 492-500 of the revised manuscript).

REVIEWER’S COMMENT 4: In this line, I think that the second and third explanations offered focus on the intention-behavior gap, rather than the utility of the TCM. If the focus of the paper is on better understanding the intention-behavior gap, then what is the value of testing the whole TCM?

AUTHORS’ RESPONSE: As we outlined in our responses to the previous comments, the intention-behavior gap is integral to the motivational sequence of the TCM, and therefore any research on the TCM should be concerned with the intention-behavior relation because, if the association is small or no different from zero, as is the case in the current research, it means that the motivational sequence is not effective as a description of the processes that lead to leisure-time physical activity participation. Therefore, means to explain why this is the case in the context of the TCM adds value to the model when its traditional components do not provide an adequate account for variance in physical activity. Please see our changes to the manuscript, noted in our response to the comment above, with regard to this issue. We also provide additional discussion of the importance of the additions in the Discussion section on page 27, lines 503-510 of the revised manuscript.

REVIEWER’S COMMENT 5: Also, I am not sure I agree with the arguments presented in p. 26 about the spontaneous participation of children in physical activity. And the reason for this is that if children hold positive views about physical activity they will be actively involve in systematic physical activity. If not, then spontaneous participation may be for external to physical activity reasons, i.e., pressure by peers etc. In this case the value of participation is questionable. I am not aware of any evidence suggesting that such spontaneous participation in physical activity will eventually result in more systematic participation in the future. In this sense, the whole concept of implicit processes should be conceived with caution about its contribution in understanding systematic physical activity.

AUTHORS’ RESPONSE: We agree, and we have removed the reference to spontaneous participation of children in physical activity and reformulated the manuscript excerpt as follows: “Such effects might represent affective motives to engage in a behavior learned through positive or negative experiences that coincide with the behavior (Conroy & Berry, 2017). As a consequence, the anticipation of rewarding affective responses may be reasons why children and adolescents engage in physical activities outside of school without the need for reasoned decision making”. Please see our addition to the revised manuscript on pages 29-30, lines 568-575.

REVIEWER’S COMMENT 6: I found really interesting the discussion on the association of past behavior and habits. Drawing back to my previous comment, I would expect a path linking these two variables. This association may provide, in my mind, a perfect bridge between explicit and implicit processes associated with physical activity.

AUTHORS’ RESPONSE: Thanks for this comment. We appreciate the need to provide links between previous experience and habit, to demonstrate that performing the behavior frequently in the past informs whether participants reflect that their behavior experienced as ‘automatic’ and determined without extensive deliberation. In fact, we included past behavior as a predictor of habit in the model. For clarity, we did not illustrate this in Figure 2, but mention it in the figure caption. However, we agree that it may be important to specify this indirect effect as it sheds light on a key process in the model, that the link between past and future behavior is mediated by habit. So we have included this as an additional hypothesis in the model in the Introduction (please see page 10, lines 210-219) and also in Tables 1 and 2 (H41), the Results (please see page 25, lines 445-446, and Table 2), and Discussion (please see page 26, lines 475-477 and page 30, lines 593-596) sections of the revised manuscript.

REVIEWER’S COMMENT 7: Overall, I think that a stronger association is needed of the added variables with the core model.

AUTHORS’ RESPONSE: We thank the Reviewer for the previous comments. These justifications have now been provided, and we have noted the location of the additions in our responses to the above comments.

REFERENCES

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 179-211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2016). The trans-contextual model of autonomous motivation in education: Conceptual and empirical issues and meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 86(2), 360-407. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654315585005

Hagger, M. S., & Hamilton, K. (2020). Changing behavior using integrated theories. In M. S. Hagger, L. D. Cameron, K. Hamilton, N. Hankonen & T. Lintunen (Eds.), The Handbook of Behavior Change (pp. 208-224). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/97811086773180.015

Hagger, M. S., Trost, N., Keech, J., Chan, D. K. C., & Hamilton, K. (2017). Predicting sugar consumption: Application of an integrated dual-process, dual-phase model. Appetite, 116, 147-156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.04.032

Hamilton, K., Gibbs, I., Keech, J. J., & Hagger, M. S. (2020). Reasoned and implicit processes in heavy episodic drinking: An integrated dual process model. British Journal of Health Psychology, 25(1), 189-209. https://doi.org/10.1111/BJHP.12401

Montaño, D. E., & Kasprzyk, D. (2015). Theory of reasoned action, theory of planned behavior, and the integrated behavioral model. In K. Glanz, B. K. Rimer & K. Viswanath (Eds.), Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice (5th ed., pp. 95-124). Jossey-Bass.

van Bree, R. J. H., van Stralen, M. M., Mudde, A. N., Bolman, C., de Vries, H., & Lechner, L. (2015). Habit as mediator of the relationship between prior and later physical activity: A longitudinal study in older adults. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 19(1), 95-102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.03.006

Attachment

Submitted filename: Responses_to_reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Lambros Lazuras

7 Oct 2021

Predictors of School Students’ Leisure-Time Physical Activity: An Extended Trans-Contextual Model Using Bayesian Path Analysis

PONE-D-21-02994R1

Dear Dr. Hagger,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Lambros Lazuras

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Lambros Lazuras

26 Oct 2021

PONE-D-21-02994R1

Predictors of School Students’ Leisure-Time Physical Activity: An Extended Trans-Contextual Model Using Bayesian Path Analysis

Dear Dr. Hagger:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Lambros Lazuras

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Responses_to_reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    Data, analysis scripts and output, study materials, and supplemental materials are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/z8axj/).


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES