Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Drug Alcohol Depend. 2021 Sep 24;228:109066. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2021.109066

Advancing our Understanding of the Intersection between Emotion Regulation and Alcohol and Drug Use Problems: Dyadic Analysis in Couples with Intimate Partner Violence and Alcohol Use Disorder

Nicole H Weiss 1, Jasara Hogan 2, Meagan Brem 3, Andrea A Massa 4, Charli M Kirby 2, Julianne C Flanagan 2,4
PMCID: PMC8595860  NIHMSID: NIHMS1745549  PMID: 34610519

Abstract

Background:

An extensive body of literature has linked emotion regulation to alcohol and drug use problems, including among populations characterized by intimate partner violence (IPV). Advancing this research, the goal of the current study was to examine cognitive emotion regulation strategies and alcohol and drug use problems within a dyadic framework. Specifically, we examined actor and partner effects of maladaptive and adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies on alcohol and drug use problems.

Methods:

Eighty-four romantic couples (N = 168 total participants) participating in a randomized controlled trial who reported physical IPV and an alcohol use disorder completed self-reported measures of cognitive emotion regulation as well as alcohol and drug use problems.

Results:

Regarding actor effects, men’s greater use of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies was positively related to their alcohol use problems (p < .001), whereas both women’s (p = .02) and men’s (p = .047) greater use of adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies was negatively related to their own alcohol use problems. In terms of partner effects, men’s greater use of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies (p = .001) and less use of adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies (p = .017) was related to their partners’ greater drug use problems.

Conclusions:

Findings replicate and extend existing work in this area, showing that men’s and women’s emotion regulation strategies are related to their own alcohol and drug use problems and that men’s cognitive emotion regulation strategies are also related to their partners’ drug use problems.

Keywords: Intimate partner violence, cognitive emotion regulation, alcohol use problems, drug use problems, dyadic analysis

1. Introduction

Intimate partner violence (IPV) is a pervasive and devastating public health concern, experienced by approximately 1 in 3 women and 1 in 4 men during their lifetime (Smith et al., 2018) and is associated with staggering physical, psychological, economic, and societal costs (Brown et al., 2008; Rivara et al., 2007; Walby, 2009). Alcohol and drug use problems are well-established and clinically significant risk factors (Jewkes, 2002; Stith et al., 2004) and consequences (Ahmadabadi et al., 2019; El-Bassel et al., 2005; Plichta, 2004) of IPV. Between 20% and 50% of all incidents of IPV occur when one or both partners are under the influence of alcohol or drugs (Hart, 2007). As described in the myopia model (Noel et al., 2013; Steele & Josephs, 1990), alcohol and drug intoxication may impair behavioral inhibition (Fillmore, 2003) and narrow attention (Bayless & Harvey, 2017), enabling violent acts (Giancola et al., 2011; Giancola et al., 2010). Alcohol and drug use problems are also commonly cited consequences of IPV, with longitudinal studies documenting higher rates of alcohol (Martino et al., 2005) and drug (Roberts et al., 2003) use problems following IPV. Prevailing models of alcohol and drug use problems, such as negative reinforcement (Baker et al., 2004) and self-medication (Khantzian, 1997), suggest that avoidance of arousal and anxiety related to IPV victimization is a central motive underlying the etiology and maintenance of alcohol and drug use problems in this population (Stewart et al., 1998). It is perhaps not surprising then that prevalence rates of alcohol and drug use disorders are significantly higher for those with IPV victimization (7.3% and 2.7%, respectively) versus those without victimization histories (2.3% and 0.4%, respectively; Okuda et al., 2011). Notably, alcohol and drug use problems among IPV-victimized individuals is linked to numerous negative outcomes including psychological distress (Sullivan & Holt, 2008), risky behaviors (Campbell et al., 2008), legal problems (Oberleitner et al., 2013), and economic strain (Peterson et al., 2018). High prevalence rates and significant impacts of alcohol and drug use problems in populations characterized by IPV underscore the importance of research in this area.

A clinically significant factor to study in relation to alcohol and drug use problems among IPV populations is emotion regulation. As defined here, emotion regulation refers to the conscious processes through which individuals monitor, evaluate, and modify emotional information (Gross, 1998). Individuals may use a variety of cognitive emotion regulation strategies to achieve this goal (Garnefski et al., 2001). Putatively adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies, including acceptance (i.e., thoughts of accepting what you have experienced and resigning yourself to what has happened), positive reappraisal (i.e., thoughts of creating a positive meaning to the event in terms of personal growth), refocus on planning (i.e., thinking about what steps to take and how to handle the negative event), positive refocusing (i.e., thinking about joyful and pleasant issues instead of thinking about the actual event), and putting into perspective (i.e., thoughts of brushing aside the seriousness of the event/emphasizing the relativity when comparing it to other events), are generally associated with better health outcomes across a wide range of contexts (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2006b, 2007; Garnefski et al., 2001; Garnefski et al., 2005). Conversely, putatively maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies, including self-blame (i.e., thoughts of putting the blame for what you have experienced on yourself), rumination (i.e., thinking about the feelings and thoughts associated with the negative event), catastrophizing (i.e., thoughts of explicitly emphasizing the terror of what you have experienced), and other-blame (i.e., thoughts of putting the blame for what you have experienced on the environment or another person), are linked to poorer health outcomes (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2006b, 2007; Garnefski et al., 2001; Garnefski et al., 2005).

Past research has linked emotion regulation to alcohol and drug use problems (for reviews, see Weiss, Sullivan, et al., 2015; Weiss, Tull, et al., 2015), including among IPV populations (Ortiz et al., 2015; Weiss et al., 2018; Weiss et al., in press). For instance, IPV-victimized women with (vs. without) alcohol and drug use disorders have been found to report higher emotion dysregulation (Weiss et al., 2018). Moreover, maladaptive emotion regulation strategies are significantly associated with greater alcohol and drug use, whereas adaptive emotion regulation strategies are significantly related to less alcohol and drug use (Weiss et al., 2017). One explanation for these findings is that adaptive emotion regulation strategies lead to greater reductions in negative affect than maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (Webb et al., 2012). The affective processing model of negative reinforcement posits that escape from and avoidance of negative affect are prepotent motives for alcohol and drug use (Baker et al., 2004). Indeed, the experience of negative affect and the desire to avoid negative affect are chief motives for alcohol and drug use (Khantzian, 1997). Thus, reliance on maladaptive emotion regulation strategies may increase future motivations for alcohol and drug use, whereas adaptive emotion regulation strategies may reduce future motivations for alcohol and drug use (Cooper et al., 2016).

One critical limitation of the existing research in this area is that examination of the relations of emotion regulation to alcohol and drug use problems has occurred among only one partner in the IPV dyad. Recent statistical advances made it possible to account for the dyadic context of IPV through the use of Actor-Partner Interdependence Models (APIM; Kenny & Cook, 1999; Kenny et al., 2006). APIM allows researchers to account for both actor effects (e.g., how much one’s alcohol and drug use problems are predicted by their own cognitive emotion regulation strategies) and partner effects (e.g., how much one’s alcohol and drug use problems are influenced by their partner’s cognitive emotion regulation strategies). Substantive literature highlights the role of situational factors (e.g., interpersonal influences) on alcohol and drug use problems (Wallace, 1999), with at least one study finding that alcohol and drug use was more common among IPV-victimized women on days when they saw their male partners (Sullivan et al., 2016). However, research in this area has just recently begun to consider the effects of both partners’ behaviors on alcohol and drug use problems (Patton & Fisher, 2018), and none of this work has explored the influence of emotion regulation. Within an IPV context, one may be more likely to engage in problematic alcohol or drug use when their partner exhibits more maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and less likely to misuse alcohol or drugs when their partner exhibits more adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies. For instance, use of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies may increase one’s negative affect (Lavy & van den Hout, 1994); in turn, one’s partner may feel less emotionally connected or experience stress-exacerbating conflict, driving them to use alcohol or drugs (Levitt & Cooper, 2010; Windle & Windle, 2019). Conversely, use of adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies may mitigate the likelihood of partner alcohol and drug use problems, perhaps by eliciting in them feelings of support, shown to buffer against the development of alcohol and drug use problems (Jarnecke & South, 2014). These partner effects may be stronger for women partners versus men partners (Cranford et al., 2015; Levitt & Cooper, 2010; Windle & Windle, 2019).

Advancing current research, the goal of the present study was to examine emotion regulation and alcohol and drug use problems within a dyadic framework. Specifically, using a sample of couples with IPV, we explored actor and partner effects of maladaptive and adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies on alcohol and drug use problems. Consistent with existing research in IPV samples (Ortiz et al., 2015; Weiss et al., 2018; Weiss et al., in press), we expected that for both men and women, actor maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies would positively associate with actor alcohol and drug use problems and that actor adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies would negatively associate with actor alcohol and drug use problems. Additionally, we hypothesized that for both men and women, actor maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies would positively relate to partner alcohol and drug use problems and actor adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies would negatively relate to partner alcohol and drug use problems. Finally, we expected stronger effects for women (Cranford et al., 2015; Levitt & Cooper, 2010; Windle & Windle, 2019).

2. Material and Methods

2.1. Procedures and Participants

Eighty-four couples (168 total participants) were enrolled in a larger, ongoing randomized controlled trial that examined the effects of oxytocin on alcohol craving and aggression between partners in the laboratory. This study only examined data collected at baseline prior to medication administration. Participants were eligible to participate in the larger study if both members of the couple (1) were between ages 18 and 75 years, (2) were fluent in English, (3) endorsed at least one instance of mild or moderate physical intimate partner violence in their current relationship as assessed by the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2; Straus et al., 1996, 2003), and (4) agreed to participate in the study. Additionally, at least one member of the couple was required to meet Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (Amercian Psychiatric Assocation, 2013) criteria for alcohol use disorder (AUD). Interested participants were excluded from the study if they (1) were pregnant or breastfeeding, (2) endorsed current/past psychiatric or medical conditions that could interfere with neuroendocrine functioning, (3) endorsed current suicidal ideation and intent, (4) reported a history of severe physical or sexual intimate partner violence within their current relationship as defined by the CTS2 (Straus et al., 1996, 2003), (5) began psychotropic medication within the four weeks prior to screening, (6) evidenced acute alcohol withdrawal as indicated by a score of 8 or greater on the Clinical Institute Withdrawal Assessment of Alcohol Scale, Revised (Bernstein et al., 2003), or (7) endorsed a history of epilepsy or seizures. Due to inadequate power to test the effects of sex constellations within couples, same-sex couples were excluded from analyses. Thus, 78 different-sex couples comprised the final sample for the present study.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Emotion regulation was assessed using the 18-item Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire – Short (CERQ-Short; Garnefski & Kraaij, 2006a).

The CERQ-Short assesses participants’ use of specific adaptive and maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies in response to stressful events. The Adaptive Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies subscale score was calculated by summing 10 items across the CERQ-Short’s five, two-item, adaptive emotion regulation subscales: Acceptance (e.g., “I think that I have to accept that this has happened”), Positive Refocusing (e.g., “I think of pleasant things that have nothing to do with it”), Refocus on Planning (e.g., “I think about a plan of what I can do best”), Positive Reappraisal (e.g., “I think that I can become a stronger person as a result of what has happened”), and Putting into Perspective (e.g., “I think that it hasn’t been too bad compared to other things”). The Maladaptive Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies subscale score was calculated by summing 8 items across the CERQ-Short’s four, two-item, maladaptive emotion regulation subscales: Self-Blame (e.g., “I think that basically the cause must lie within myself”), Rumination (e.g., “I am preoccupied with what I think and feel about what I have experienced”), Catastrophizing (e.g., “I keep thinking about how terrible it is what I have experienced”), and Other-Blame (e.g., “I feel that basically the cause lies with others”). Respondents reported how often they used each strategy to cope with stressful events (1 = almost never, 5 = almost always) and responses were summed for each subscale such that higher scores represent more frequent use of adaptive or maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies. The CERQ-Short has evidenced good psychometric properties (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2006a). Internal reliability for the CERQ-Short Adaptive Emotion Regulation (α = .83) and Maladaptive Emotion Regulation (α = .75) scales was adequate and acceptable, respectively, within the present study.

2.2.2. The 10-item Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT; Babor et al., 2001; Saunders et al., 1993) assessed past 12-month alcohol use problems.

The AUDIT examines the intensity and frequency of alcohol use, negative consequences of alcohol use, and symptoms of alcohol tolerance and dependence. Scores were summed for a possible total score range from 0 to 40; higher scores represented greater alcohol use problems. The AUDIT has demonstrated strong psychometric properties (Saunders et al., 1993). Internal consistency for the AUDIT in the current sample was adequate (α = .89).

2.2.3. The 10-item Drug Abuse Screening Test (DAST-10; Skinner, 1982) assessed past six-month drug use problems, including physical, social, occupational, and legal consequences of drug use.

Participants responded either “yes” (1) or “no” (0) to each item; items were summed such that greater scores indicated greater drug use problems. Possible total scores ranged from 0 to 28. The DAST-10 showed strong psychometric properties (Yudko et al., 2007). Internal consistency for the DAST-10 in the current sample was adequate (α = .74).

2.3. Data Analytic Strategy

All analyses were conducted within a multilevel modeling (MLM) framework (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). MLM uses a nested data structure to account for the inherently interdependent nature of dyadic data. For these analyses, individual partners (level 1) were “nested” within couples (level 2). Hypotheses were tested using an APIM, a specialized MLM that affords a deeper understanding of partner dynamics. In an APIM, it is possible to determine how each predictor impacts both one’s own outcome (the actor path) and one’s partner’s outcome (the partner path; Cook & Kenny, 2005). As gender differences were predicted for these associations, distinguishable dyads were used, meaning separate actor and partner paths were estimated for men and women within the sample. Two distinguishable dyad APIMs were estimated, one to test associations between cognitive coping strategies and alcohol use problems and another to test associates between cognitive coping strategies and drug use problems. All analyses were conducted using a Poisson distribution to account for skew in the data. Accordingly, coefficients reported are in log units.

3. Results

Preliminary Analyses

Participants’ ages ranged from 19 to 67 years with a mean age of 34.51 (SD = 10.59). Participants identified as primarily white (65.4%), followed by Black/African American (24.4%), more than one race (5.8%), Asian (1.9%), and Native American/Alaskan Native (1.3%); 1.3% of participants did not report their race. Participants were 91.1% non-Hispanic and 7.7% Hispanic; 1.2% of participants did not report their ethnicity. With regard to their romantic relationships, most participants reported that they were not married but were living with their partner (49.4%), followed by married (31.4%), dating but not living with their partner (12.2%), separated (1.9%), or divorced (1.9%); 3.2% of participants did not report their relationship status. On average, the length of participants’ current relationship was 6.65 years (SD = 6.74 years). Participants completed an average of 13.76 years of school (SD = 2.47 years), were mostly employed full-time (44.9%) or part-time (23.7%), and reported an average household income of $65,430.16 (SD = $98,707.85). Participants reported a mean psychological IPV score of 34.57 (SD = 36.45) and a mean physical IPV score of 31.96 (SD = 46.20) on the CTS2 (Newton et al, 2001).

The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., 1998) was used to assess current AUD and psychiatric conditions. Inclusion of participants with concurrent drug use disorders was allowable given the marked co-occurrence with AUD. Most participants (78.8%) met criteria for AUD within the previous twelve months. Among participants who met criteria for AUD, 32.1% reported mild AUD, 22.1% were moderate, and 45.8% reported severe AUD. Additionally, 39.1% of participants also met criteria for a drug use disorder (i.e., 65.6% cannabis use disorder, 27.9% for stimulant use disorder, and 6.6% for another drug use disorder). A small percentage (11.4%) met criteria for generalized anxiety disorder. Approximately half (46.7%) of participants reported experiencing at least mild symptoms of depression (M = 5.41, SD = 5.33) as measured by the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9; Kroenke et al., 2001).

Several demographic variables were tested as covariates. Relationship status and length, race, ethnicity, and education level were not associated with either drug or alcohol problems and were not included in the final model. Both depression symptoms and current generalized anxiety disorder were significantly related to drug- and alcohol-related problems (rs ranging from .18 to .33, ps ranging from .03 to < .001) and thus were included in the final analyses.

Primary Analyses

Table 1 presents means, standard deviations, and correlations for all study variables. Men (M =10.65) and women (M = 11.41) reported significant alcohol problems, where scores greater than 8 indicate hazardous alcohol use. Conversely, both men (M = 2.91) and women (M = 1.88) reported nonsignificant drug problems, where scores greater than 6 indicate hazardous drug use.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Correlations

Variable Female Partner Male Partner 1 2 3 4 5 6
M SD M SD
1. Adaptive Emotion Regulation 31.75 7.43 31.97 7.15 −.05 .42*** −.07 −.15 −.31 −.04
2. Maladaptive Emotion Regulation 19.92 5.90 18.53 4.64 −.04 −.10 .29*** .08 .26* .15
3. Alcohol Use Problems 10.65 8.10 11.41 7.25 −.23** −.003 −.04 .54*** .47** .08
4. Drug Use Problems 1.88 2.21 2.99 2.62 −.08 .15 .37*** .17* .40** .04
5. Depression Symptoms 5.34 4.88 5.38 5.81 −.04 .14 .17 .14 .004 .37**
6.Generalized Anxiety Disorder .08 .25* .26* .37** .34** ..094

Note. The results for female partners (n = 76) are shown below the diagonal. The results for male partners (n = 76) are shown above the diagonal. Within-couple correlations are presented in the shaded area on the diagonal. Generalized anxiety disorder is a dichotomous variable. Thus, no mean or standard deviation is presented for this variable.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001.

Full results from both models are presented in Table 2. Model 1 tested associations between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and alcohol-related problems. In this model, men’s maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies (B = .04, p = .005) were significantly positively associated with their own alcohol use problems. Additionally, women’s (B = −.02, p = .03) adaptive cognitive coping strategies were significantly negatively associated with their own alcohol use problems. Model 2 tested associations between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and drug-related problems. In this model, men’s positive (B = −.06, p = .001) and negative (B = .06, p = .017) cognitive coping strategies were significantly associated with their female partner’s drug use problems.

Table 2.

Actor-Partner Interdependence Modeling Coefficients for Models Predicting Alcohol and Drug Use Problems

Parameters AUDIT DAST
B SE B 95% CI B SE B 95% CI
Male Partner Predictors
 Intercept 2.37*** .06 [2.253, 2.483] .87*** .12 [.639, 1.097]
 Positive Emotion −.01 .01 [−.030, .005] −.03+ .02 [−.063, .004]
RegulationActor
 Positive Emotion .002 .01 [−.013, .017] .01 .01 [−014, .044]
RegulationPartner
 Negative Emotion .04** .01 [.012, .69] .03 .03 [−.024, .081]
RegulationActor
 Negative Emotion −.01 .01 [−.034, .005] −.004 .02 [−041, .033]
RegulationPartner
Female Partner Predictors
 Intercept 2.23*** .06 [2.112, 2.351] .34* .13 [.080, .594]
 Positive Emotion −.02* .01 [−.033, −.002] .02 .02 [−.009, .054]
RegulationActor
 Positive Emotion .01 .01 [−.005, .031] −.06** .02 [−.098, −.023]
RegulationPartner
 Negative Emotion .01 .01 [−.010, .029] .02 .02 [−.022, .058]
RegulationActor
 Negative Emotion .001 .01 [−.027, .030] .07* .03 [.013, .127]
RegulationPartner
Covariates
 Depression Symptoms .03*** .01 [.014, .039] .01 .01 [−.015, .037]
 Generalized Anxiety Disorder .30** .10 [.096, .505] .10 .20 [−.295, .498]

Note.

+

p < .10,

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001.

4. Discussion

The current study used a dyadic framework to assess actor and partner relationships between emotion regulation and alcohol and drug use problems among different-sex couples with IPV and AUD. In partial support of study hypotheses, men’s greater use of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies was significantly positively associated with their own alcohol use problems. Moreover, women’s greater use of adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies was significantly negatively associated with their own alcohol use problems. Further, men’s greater use of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies was significantly positively associated with women’s drug use problems. Finally, men’s greater use of adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies was significantly negatively associated with their partners’ drug use problems. Findings advance our understanding of the association between emotion regulation and alcohol and drug use problems.

Regarding actor relations, our findings suggest that among individuals in relationships characterized by IPV, men’s (but not women’s) greater use of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies was significantly positively associated with their own alcohol (but not drug) use problems. In terms of our unexpected finding for women, there is some evidence to suggest that maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies in the context of relationship conflict may at times be adaptive. Maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies may result in short-term reductions in stress and anxiety tied to threatening stimuli (Bradizza et al., 2018; Conklin et al., 2015). Specific to IPV, maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies may reduce both conflict in the context of physical threat and distress associated with victimization (Milojevich et al., 2019). Moreover, use of such strategies may counter the tendency toward feelings of failure or hopelessness in solving conflict in relationships characterized by IPV (Fine, 1992). Consistent with these suggestions, maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies are associated with more adaptive outcomes for individuals in environments perceived as uncontrollable (Ford & Gross, 2019), such as in the context of IPV (Hamberger et al., 2017). As such, use of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies among women in IPV relationships may be at times situationally adaptive and thus not show expected relations with negative outcomes such as alcohol and drug use problems. Future research in this area would benefit from the application of methods that may capture the contextually-dependent nature of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies, such as ecological momentary assessment (Trull & Ebner-Priemer, 2009). Conversely, our findings for cognitive emotion regulation strategies among men were as expected for alcohol (but not drug) use problems. This result may be attributed to our sample, which was comprised of couples in which at least one partner met criteria for AUD. Perhaps alcohol use was more problematic in this sample than drug use and thus more strongly tied to maladaptive emotional responding. Investigations are needed to examine whether this finding extends to other samples of men in IPV relationships.

As partially expected, women’s greater use of adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies was significantly negatively associated with their own alcohol (but not drug) use problems. Findings align with literature on the protective value of emotion regulation (Troy & Mauss, 2011) and suggest that adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies may play a key role in alcohol use problems among women in couples with IPV (Scheer & Mereish, in press). Given evidence that adaptive and maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies often co-occur (e.g., multiple strategies may be required to reduce distress; Dixon-Gordon et al., 2015), future studies are needed to explore the interaction between adaptive and maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies in conferring protection versus risk for alcohol and drug use problems. One study found that individuals in an emotion regulation class characterized by adaptive strategies reported significantly less psychopathology compared to those in classes characterized by maladaptive strategies or adaptive and maladaptive strategies (Dixon-Gordon et al., 2015). These findings suggest that adaptive strategies may lose their protective value in the context of maladaptive strategies. Future research is needed to address this hypothesis among couples who experience IPV. Of note, as was mentioned above, our unexpected finding for drug use problems may be a result of the AUD inclusion criteria here. Research that explores these relations among men and women with drug use disorders are necessary.

Finally, in regards to partner effects, men’s use of maladaptive and adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies was significantly associated with women’s drug (but not alcohol) use problems, such that women were more likely to report drug use problems when their male partners reported greater maladaptive and fewer adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies. Past research provides some potential explanations for these findings. Men’s negative affect (often resultant from maladaptive emotion regulation strategies; Lavy & van den Hout, 1994) may lead their partners to feel less emotionally connected to them or increase relational conflict, both of which are associated with greater risk for substance misuse in women (Levitt & Cooper, 2010; Windle & Windle, 2019). In contrast, men’s adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies may elicit feelings of support in their partners, which, in turn, may decrease the odds of drug use problems among women (Jarnecke & South, 2014). Notably, our findings suggest that partner effects are specific to women’s outcomes: neither men’s alcohol nor drug use problems were influenced by women’s cognitive emotion regulation strategies. These results are consistent with prior research suggesting that partner effects may influence women’s behavior more than men’s (Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001). Future investigations are needed to better understand these sex differences as they relate to alcohol and drug use in the context of IPV.

Although results of the present study add to the literature on emotion regulation and alcohol and drug use problems, findings must be interpreted in light of its limitations. First, the cross-sectional and correlational nature of the data precludes determination of the precise nature, direction, and temporal ordering of the relations examined here. For instance, it is possible that the relationship between maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and alcohol and/or drug use problems is reciprocal, such that both earlier maladaptive emotion regulation strategies increase later alcohol and drug use as well as earlier alcohol and drug use increase later maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (Weiss et al., 2017). Second, although our use of a dyadic framework with couples who experience IPV may be considered a strength of this study, inclusion and exclusion criteria limit the generalizability of findings. For instance, although couples with severe IPV were eligible provided no acute safety concerns were present, it is possible that this study’s focus on intact couples might have created a ceiling effect with regard to IPV severity. Further, this study required that one, but not both, partners within each dyad met diagnostic criteria for AUD. Alcohol use discrepancies between partners within a dyad remains an intense area of scientific inquiry (Bartel et al., 2017; Homish & Leonard, 2007; Mushquash et al., 2013), and this construct might have short- and long-term effects on each partner’s emotion regulation strategies. Nonetheless, use of a sample with an alcohol – but not drug – use disorder inclusion criterion likely resulted in lower drug vs. alcohol use problem scores. Third, not all drugs have the same pharmacologic effects, acutely or in the long-term (Lowinson, 2005), and this study did not distinguish between specific drugs. Fourth, the larger sample only included five same-sex couples. Because we were not powered to test the effects of sex constellations within couples, these same-sex couples were excluded from analyses. Finally, while outside the scope of the current paper, future research is needed to identify psychological processes that underlie the relationship between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and alcohol and drug use problems, including psychiatric disorders such as depression and anxiety.

Despite these limitations, our findings underscore the potential utility of targeting cognitive emotion regulation strategies in the treatment of individuals or couples with IPV who present with alcohol and/or drug use problems. Indeed, deficits in emotion regulation predict alcohol consumption during and after cognitive behavioral treatment for AUD (Berking et al., 2011) and persist beyond acute alcohol and drug treatment efforts (Fox et al., 2007; Fox et al., 2008), perhaps resultant from emotion regulation’s association with craving (Garland et al., 2014). Further, treatments have utility in reducing alcohol and drug use problems via emotion regulation (Axelrod et al., 2011). Regarding emotion regulation interventions aimed at interpersonal conflict specifically, Rodriguez et al. (2019) found evidence for the utility of a single-session, conflict-focused, brief cognitive reappraisal intervention in improving emotion regulation. Specifically, this intervention asked participants to write about an interpersonal conflict from the perspective of someone neutral who has the best interests of everyone at heart, suggesting the benefits of perspective-taking. These results underscore the potential value of targeting perspective-taking in couple’s therapy as a means of improving emotion regulation. Future studies are needed to examine whether treatments targeting an increase in adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and a decrease in maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies—such as through perspective-taking—influence alcohol and/or drug use problems among individuals and their partners in relationships characterized by IPV.

Highlights.

  • Men’s maladaptive strategies were positively related to their own alcohol problems

  • Women’s adaptive strategies were negatively related to their own alcohol problems

  • Men’s maladaptive strategies positively related to their partners’ drug problems

  • Men’s adaptive strategies negatively related to their partners’ drug problems

Acknowledgments

This manuscript is the result of work supported, in part, by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (K23AA023845) awarded to the last author (JCF). Work on this paper by the first author (NHW) was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants K23DA039327. NHW also acknowledges the support from the Center for Biomedical Research and Excellence (COBRE) on Opioids and Overdose funded by the National Institute on General Medical Sciences (P20GM125507). Work on this paper by the second author (JH) was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants (T32AA007474 and 5K12HD055885). Work on this paper by the fourth author (AAM) was supported by the MIRECC Advanced Fellowship in Mental Illness Research and Treatment through the Ralph H. Johnson VAMC.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Ahmadabadi Z, Najman JM, Williams GM, Clavarino AM, d’Abbs P, & Smirnov A (2019). Intimate partner violence in emerging adulthood and subsequent substance use disorders: Findings from a longitudinal study. Addiction, 114(7), 1264–1273. 10.1111/add.14592 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, (DSM-5) Washington. DC: Author. [Google Scholar]
  3. Axelrod SR, Perepletchikova F, Holtzman K, & Sinha R (2011). Emotion regulation and substance use frequency in women with substance dependence and borderline personality disorder receiving dialectical behavior therapy. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 37(1), 37–42. 10.3109/00952990.2010.535582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Babor TF, Higgins-Biddle JC, Saunders JB, & Monteiro MG (2001). The alcohol use disorders identification test: Guidelines for use in primary care (2nd ed.). World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, & Fiore MC (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111(1), 33–51. 10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bartel SJ, Sherry SB, Molnar DS, Mushquash AR, Leonard KE, Flett GL, & Stewart SH (2017). Do romantic partners influence each other’s heavy episodic drinking? Support for the partner influence hypothesis in a three-year longitudinal study. Addictive Behaviors, 69, 55–58. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.01.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bayless SJ, & Harvey AJ (2017). Testing alcohol myopia theory: Examining the effects of alcohol intoxication on simultaneous central and peripheral attention. Perception, 46(1), 90–99. 10.1177/0301006616672221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berking M, Margraf M, Ebert D, Wupperman P, Hofmann SG, & Junghanns K (2011). Deficits in emotion-regulation skills predict alcohol use during and after cognitive–behavioral therapy for alcohol dependence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(3), 307–318. 10.1037/a0023421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bradizza CM, Brown WC, Ruszczyk MU, Dermen KH, Lucke JF, & Stasiewicz PR (2018). Difficulties in emotion regulation in treatment-seeking alcoholics with and without co-occurring mood and anxiety disorders. Addictive Behaviors, 80, 6–13. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.12.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brown DS, Finkelstein EA, & Mercy JA (2008). Methods for estimating medical expenditures attributable to intimate partner violence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 23(12), 1747–1766. 10.1177/0886260508314338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Campbell R, Greeson MR, Bybee D, & Raja S (2008). The co-occurrence of childhood sexual abuse, adult sexual assault, intimate partner violence, and sexual harassment: A mediational model of posttraumatic stress disorder and physical health outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(2), 194–207. 10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cludius B, Mennin D, & Ehring T (2020). Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic process. Emotion, 20(1), 37–42. 10.1037/emo0000646 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Conklin LR, Cassiello-Robbins C, Brake CA, Sauer-Zavala S, Farchione TJ, Ciraulo DA, & Barlow DH (2015). Relationships among adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and psychopathology during the treatment of comorbid anxiety and alcohol use disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 73, 124–130. 10.1016/j.brat.2015.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cook WL, & Kenny DA (2005). The actor–partner interdependence model: A model of bidirectional effects in developmental studies. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(2), 101–109. 10.1080/01650250444000405 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cooper ML, Kuntsche E, Levitt A, Barber LL, & Wolf S (2016). Motivational models of substance use: A review of theory and research on motives for using alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco. In Sher KJ (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of substance use and substance use disorders (1st ed., pp. 375–421). Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199381678.013.017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cranford JA, Tennen H, & Zucker RA (2015). Using multiple methods to examine gender differences in alcohol involvement and marital interactions in alcoholic probands. Addictive Behaviors, 41, 192–198. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dixon-Gordon KL, Aldao A, & De Los Reyes A (2015). Repertoires of emotion regulation: A person-centered approach to assessing emotion regulation strategies and links to psychopathology. Cognition and Emotion, 29(7), 1314–1325. 10.1080/02699931.2014.983046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. El-Bassel N, Gilbert L, Wu E, Go H, & Hill J (2005, March). Relationship between drug abuse and intimate partner violence: a longitudinal study among women receiving methadone. American Journal of Public Health, 95(3), 465–470. 10.2105/ajph.2003.023200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fillmore MT (2003). Drug abuse as a problem of impaired control: Current approaches and findings. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 2(3), 179–197. 10.1177/1534582303257007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fine M (1992). Disruptive voices: The possibilities of feminist research. University of Michigan Press. [Google Scholar]
  21. Ford BQ, & Gross JJ (2019). Why beliefs about emotion matter: An emotion-regulation perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(1), 74–81. 10.1177/0963721418806697 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fox H, Axelrod S, Paliwal P, Sleeper J, & Sinha R (2007). Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control during cocaine abstinence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 89(2–3), 298–301. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fox H, Hong K, & Sinha R (2008). Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control in recently abstinent alcoholics compared with social drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 33(2), 388–394. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Garland EL, Roberts-Lewis A, Kelley K, Tronnier C, & Hanley A (2014). Cognitive and affective mechanisms linking trait mindfulness to craving among individuals in addiction recovery. Substance Use & Misuse, 49(5), 525–535. 10.3109/10826084.2014.850309 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Garnefski N, & Kraaij V (2006a). Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire–development of a short 18-item version (CERQ-short). Personality and Individual Differences, 41(6), 1045–1053. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.04.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Garnefski N, & Kraaij V (2006b). Relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: A comparative study of five specific samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(8), 1659–1669. 10.1016/j.paid.2005.12.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Garnefski N, & Kraaij V (2007). The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 141–149. 10.1037/t03801-000 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, & Spinhoven P (2001). Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(8), 1311–1327. 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00113-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, & van Etten M (2005). Specificity of relations between adolescents’ cognitive emotion regulation strategies and internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Journal of Adolescence, 28(5), 619–631. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2004.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Giancola PR, Duke AA, & Ritz KZ (2011). Alcohol, violence, and the alcohol myopia model: Preliminary findings and implications for prevention. Addictive Behaviors, 36(10), 1019–1022. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Giancola PR, Josephs RA, Parrott DJ, & Duke AA (2010). Alcohol myopia revisited: Clarifying aggression and other acts of disinhibition through a distorted lens. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(3), 265–278. 10.1177/1745691610369467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gratz KL, Rosenthal MZ, Tull MT, Lejuez C, & Gunderson JG (2006). An experimental investigation of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(4), 850–855. 10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.850 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gratz KL, & Tull MT (2010). Emotion regulation as a mechanism of change in acceptance-and mindfulness-based treatments. In Baer RA (Ed.), Assessing mindfulness and acceptance processes in clients: Illuminating the theory and practice of change (pp. 107–133). Context Press/New Harbinger Publications. [Google Scholar]
  34. Gratz KL, Weiss NH, & Tull MT (2015). Examining emotion regulation as an outcome, mechanism, or target of psychological treatments. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 85–90. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.02.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gross JJ (1998). Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 224–237. 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hamberger LK, Larsen SE, & Lehrner A (2017). Coercive control in intimate partner violence. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 37, 1–11. 10.1016/j.avb.2017.08.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hart SD (2007). Substance abuse and intimate partner violence. In Cutler BL (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology and law (1st ed., pp. 774–776). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  38. Homish GG, & Leonard KE (2007). The drinking partnership and marital satisfaction: The longitudinal influence of discrepant drinking. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(1), 43–51. 10.1037/0022-006X.75.1.43 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jarnecke AM, & South SC (2014). Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol use problems: Moderation by romantic partner support, but not family or friend support. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 38(2), 367–375. 10.1111/acer.12263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jewkes R (2002). Intimate partner violence: Causes and prevention. The Lancet, 359(9315), 1423–1429. 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)08357-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Khantzian EJ (1997). The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: A reconsideration and recent applications. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 4(5), 231–244. 10.3109/10673229709030550 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, & Newton TL (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 472–503. 10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, & Williams JB (2001). The PHQ- 9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16(9), 606–613. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lavy EH, & van den Hout MA (1994). Cognitive avoidance and attentional bias: Causal relationships. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18(2), 179–191. 10.1007/BF02357223 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Levitt A, & Cooper ML (2010). Daily alcohol use and romantic relationship functioning: Evidence of bidirectional, gender-, and context-specific effects. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(12), 1706–1722. 10.1177/0146167210388420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lowinson JH (2005). Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. [Google Scholar]
  47. Martino SC, Collins RL, & Ellickson PL (2005). Cross-lagged relationships between substance use and intimate partner violence among a sample of young adult women. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66(1), 139–148. 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Milojevich HM, Norwalk KE, & Sheridan MA (2019). Deprivation and threat, emotion dysregulation, and psychopathology: Concurrent and longitudinal associations. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 847–857. 10.1017/S0954579419000294 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mushquash AR, Stewart SH, Sherry SB, Mackinnon SP, Antony MM, & Sherry DL (2013). Heavy episodic drinking among dating partners: A longitudinal actor–partner interdependence model. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27(1), 178–183. 10.1037/a0026653 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Noel NE, Heaton JA, & Brown BP (2013). Substance induced myopia. In Miller PM, Kavanagh DJ, Kampman KM, Bates ME, Larimer ME, Petry NM, De Witte P, & Ball SA (Eds.), Principles of addiction: comprehensive addictive behaviors and disorders (1st ed., pp. 349–354). Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  51. Oberleitner LM, Mandel DL, & Easton CJ (2013). Treatment of co-occurring alcohol dependence and perpetration of intimate partner violence: The role of anger expression. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 45(3), 313–318. 10.1016/j.jsat.2013.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Okuda M, Olfson M, Hasin D, Grant BF, Lin K-H, & Blanco C (2011). Mental health of victims of intimate partner violence: Results from a national epidemiologic survey. Psychiatric Services, 62(8), 959–962. 10.1176/ps.62.8.pss6208_0959 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ortiz E, Shorey RC, & Cornelius TL (2015). An examination of emotion regulation and alcohol use as risk factors for female-perpetrated dating violence. Violence and Victims, 30(3), 417–431. 10.1891/0886-6708.VV-D-13-00173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Patton R, & Fisher U (2018). Examining couple functioning and alcohol use among Latino couples: A dyadic data analysis. Substance Use & Misuse, 53(3), 484–489. 10.1080/10826084.2017.1341924 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Peterson C, Kearns MC, McIntosh WL, Estefan LF, Nicolaidis C, McCollister KE, Gordon A, & Florence C (2018). Lifetime economic burden of intimate partner violence among US adults. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 55(4), 433–444. 10.1016/j.amepre.2018.04.049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Plichta SB (2004). Intimate partner violence and physical health consequences: Policy and practice implications. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19(11), 1296–1323. 10.1177/0886260504269685 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Raudenbush SW, & Bryk AS (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  58. Rivara FP, Anderson ML, Fishman P, Bonomi AE, Reid RJ, Carrell D, & Thompson RS (2007). Healthcare utilization and costs for women with a history of intimate partner violence. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 32(2), 89–96. 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Roberts TA, Klein JD, & Fisher S (2003). Longitudinal effect of intimate partner abuse on high-risk behavior among adolescents. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 157(9), 875–881. 10.1001/archpedi.157.9.875 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Rodriguez LM, Dell JB, Lee KD, & Onufrak J (2019). Effects of a brief cognitive reappraisal intervention on reductions in alcohol consumption and related problems. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 33(7), 637–643. 10.1037/adb0000509 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, & Grant M (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption- II. Addiction, 88(6), 791–804. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Scheer JR, & Mereish EH (in press). Intimate partner violence and illicit substance use among sexual and gender minority youth: The protective role of cognitive reappraisal. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, & Dunbar GC (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 20), 22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Skinner HA (1982). The drug abuse screening test. Addictive Behaviors, 7(4), 363–371. 10.1037/t03979-000 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Smith SG, Zhang X, Basile KC, Merrick MT, Wang J, Kresnow M, & Chen J (2018). The National Intimate Partner and Sexual Violence Survey (NISVS): 2015 Data Brief – Updated Release of report. https://stacks.cdc.gov/view/cdc/60893.
  66. Steele CM, & Josephs RA (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American Psychologist, 45(8), 921–933. 10.1037/0003-066X.45.8.921 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Stewart SH, Pihl RO, Conrod PJ, & Dongier M (1998). Functional associations among trauma, PTSD, and substance-related disorders. Addictive Behaviors, 23(6), 797–812. 10.1016/S0306-4603(98)00070-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Stith SM, Smith DB, Penn CE, Ward DB, & Tritt D (2004). Intimate partner physical abuse perpetration and victimization risk factors: A meta-analytic review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10(1), 65–98. 10.1016/j.avb.2003.09.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Straus M, Hamby S, & Warren W (2003). The Conflict Tactics Scale handbook. Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  70. Straus MA, Hamby SL, Boney-McCoy S, & Sugarman DB (1996). The revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17(3), 283–316. 10.1177/019251396017003001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Sullivan TP, & Holt LJ (2008). PTSD symptom clusters are differentially related to substance use among community women exposed to intimate partner violence. Journal of Traumatic Stress: Official Publication of The International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, 21(2), 173–180. 10.1002/jts.20318 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sullivan TP, Weiss NH, Flanagan JC, Willie TC, Armeli S, & Tennen H (2016). PTSD and daily co-occurrence of drug and alcohol use among women experiencing intimate partner violence. Journal of Dual Diagnosis, 12(1), 36–42. 10.1080/15504263.2016.1146516 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Troy AS, & Mauss IB (2011). Resilience in the face of stress: Emotion regulation as a protective factor. Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, 1(2), 30–44. 10.1017/CBO9780511994791 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Trull TJ, & Ebner-Priemer UW (2009). Using experience sampling methods/ecological momentary assessment (ESM/EMA) in clinical assessment and clinical research: Introduction to the special section [Editorial]. Psychological Assessment, 21(4), 457–462. 10.1037/a0017653 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tull MT, & Aldao A (2015). Emotion regulation [Special issue]. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Vasilev CA, Crowell SE, Beauchaine TP, Mead HK, & Gatzke- Kopp LM (2009). Correspondence between physiological and self- report measures of emotion dysregulation: A longitudinal investigation of youth with and without psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(11), 1357–1364. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02172.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Walby S (2009). The cost of domestic violence: Up-date 2009.
  78. Wallace JM (1999). The social ecology of addiction: Race, risk, and resilience. Pediatrics, 103(Suppl 2), 1122–1127. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Webb TL, Miles E, & Sheeran P (2012). Dealing with feeling: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 775–808. 10.1037/a0027600 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Weiss NH, Bold KW, Sullivan TP, Armeli S, & Tennen H (2017). Testing bidirectional associations among emotion regulation strategies and substance use: A daily diary study. Addiction, 112(4), 695–704. 10.1111/add.13698s [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Weiss NH, Darosh AG, Contractor AA, Forkus SR, Dixon-Gordon KL, & Sullivan TP (2018). Heterogeneity in emotion regulation difficulties among women victims of domestic violence: A latent profile analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 239, 192–200. 10.1016/j.jad.2018.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Weiss NH, Schick MR, Contractor AA, Reyes ME, Suazo NC, & Sullivan TP (in press). Racial/ethnic differences in alcohol and drug misuse among IPV-victimized women: Exploring the role of difficulties regulating positive emotions. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Weiss NH, Sullivan TP, & Tull MT (2015). Explicating the role of emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors: A review and synthesis of the literature with directions for future research and clinical practice. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 22–29. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Weiss NH, Tull M, & Sullivan T (2015). Emotion dysregulation and risky, self-destructive, and health compromising behaviors: A review of the literature. In Bryant ML (Ed.), Handbook on emotion regulation: processes, cognitive effects and social consequences (pp. 37–56). Nova. [Google Scholar]
  85. Windle M, & Windle RC (2019). Partner conflict and support as moderators of alcohol use on alcohol problems and marital satisfaction in young adult marital dyads. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 43(4), 668–678. 10.1111/acer.13977 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Yudko E, Lozhkina O, & Fouts A (2007). A comprehensive review of the psychometric properties of the Drug Abuse Screening Test. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 32(2), 189–198. 10.1016/j.jsat.2006.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES