Skip to main content
Journal of Clinical Medicine logoLink to Journal of Clinical Medicine
. 2021 Nov 15;10(22):5307. doi: 10.3390/jcm10225307

Prostate-Specific Antigen and Testosterone Levels as Biochemical Indicators of Cognitive Function in Prostate Cancer Survivors and the Role of Diabetes

Alicja Popiołek 1,2,*, Bartosz Brzoszczyk 3, Piotr Jarzemski 3, Aleksandra Chyrek-Tomaszewska 1,2, Radosław Wieczór 2, Alina Borkowska 1, Maciej Bieliński 1
Editor: Guillaume Ploussard
PMCID: PMC8619514  PMID: 34830590

Abstract

Prostate cancer (PC) is one of the most common malignancies in men. The increase in the number of PC survivors is associated with many problems including cognitive impairment. Early detection of such problems facilitates timely protective intervention. This study examined the association between prostate-specific antigen (PSA) or testosterone (T) levels and cognitive function in patients undergoing radical prostatectomy. Such a correlation could help identify patient groups at risk of cognitive impairment. Participants underwent clinical (demographic data, medical history, physical examination, and blood analyses) and neuropsychological assessment (cognitive test battery). Preoperative PSA or T levels were not associated with cognitive function. However, long-term follow-up after prostatectomy showed a strong correlation between PSA levels and the results of verbal memory and executive function tests. A trend toward significance was also observed for visuospatial memory. The levels of free T and total T were not correlated with cognitive function. Only the levels of free T after hormonal treatment were significantly correlated with executive functions. Comorbid diabetes affected these correlations. In conclusion, PSA levels at a distant postoperative time and free T level after hormonal treatment may be biomarkers of cognitive function.

Keywords: prostate cancer, cognitive function, cancer-related cognitive impairment, biomarkers, prostate-specific antigen, testosterone, diabetes

1. Introduction

Prostate cancer (PC) is one of the most common malignancies in men [1]. Despite considerable progress in the detection and treatment of PC, it remains the fifth leading cause of death worldwide, with approximately 375,000 deaths per year. PC patients die more often from any cause than the general population [2,3]. Advances in medicine, particularly in screening and in the design of effective treatments, have led to an increase in the population of patients with PC, which requires specific care tailored to each problem. One of the issues that PC survivors have to face is cancer-related cognitive impairment. Deterioration of mental function occurs in patients with breast cancer, ovarian cancer, colorectal cancer, and lymphoma [4], as well as in PC [5,6,7]. It occurs in up to 75% of patients during cancer treatment and in up to 30% of patients prior to any treatment [8]. However, the pathogenetic mechanisms underlying these dysfunctions remain unclear. They seem to be multifactorial and related to both the cancer itself and to treatments such as radiochemotherapy, hormone therapy, and surgery [4,5,9,10]. The probable link of PC and cognitive dysfunction is supported by some biological processes including through abnormal accumulation of proteins, neuroinflammation, oxidative stress, and neuronal cell death. These changes can lead to the breakdown of the blood-brain barrier and promote key pathways for the development of cognitive disorders (such as brain insulin resistance, mitochondrial dysfunction, and deposits of neurotoxic beta-amyloid oligomers, synaptic loss, neuronal dysfunction, and cell death) [11,12,13].

Regardless of the mechanisms underlying their development, cognitive disorders affect the daily life of patients, decreasing quality of life, hindering cooperation, and interfering with the patient’s ability to live independently [4,8,14,15,16,17]. The comprehensive care of cancer survivors should include early assessment of cognitive function because early detection or even prediction of cognitive impairment enables timely protective intervention [16,18]. This may reduce the degree of cognitive loss and promote the maintenance of cognitive performance, which improves quality of life [19].

The development of cognitive disorders in cancer survivors is associated with age (the prevalence increases with age), genetic polymorphisms, psycho-social components, stress, depression, anxiety, or other mood disorders related to the disease [4,5,9,10].

This study examined the potential association of biochemical parameters that are routinely measured in PC, such as prostate-specific antigen (PSA) or testosterone, with cognitive function. The existence of such a dependency could help identify patients at risk of cognitive impairment in PC.

PSA is the biomarker recommended by clinical practice guidelines for the early detection of PC to rule-in patients for prostate biopsy referral, for PC surveillance, and for therapeutic monitoring [20]. Preliminary studies show an association between PSA level and cognitive decline [13,21]. Sternberg et al. suggested the possibility of using PSA as a serum biomarker of cognitive function [21].

Testosterone is a hormone that is routinely measured in PC. Its deficiency plays an important role in cognitive function impairment [22]. Androgens (testosterone among them) are closely related to beta amyloid. Their reduced levels induce beta amyloid accumulation in a brain and impair hippocampal neurons, causing cognitive difficulties [12].

Here, we evaluated the correlation of PSA and testosterone levels with cognitive function in PC survivors treated with radical prostatectomy and identified factors that could influence this correlation.

2. Materials and Methods

The study was carried out in The Department of Laparoscopic, General, and Oncological Urology of Jan Biziel University Hospital No. 2 in Bydgoszcz in Poland between July 2017 and June 2018. The study group comprised 118 men with PC who underwent laparoscopic radical prostatectomy. All patients were Polish and Caucasian. The mean age of the cohort was 65 years (range, 48–77 years). Patients were evaluated once postoperatively after an average of 26 months (range, 3–102 months). The inclusion criteria were as follows: histopathologically confirmed diagnosis of prostate adenocarcinoma and subsequent treatment with radical prostatectomy, the ability to understand the purpose of the study, and the willingness to participate in the study. The exclusion criteria were severe somatic, psychiatric, or neurological disorders.

All respondents underwent clinical and neuropsychological assessment.

Clinical assessment included personal data (age, ethnicity, education, physical activity, body mass index, and smoking status) and medical history (data about the course of the disease, PC Grade Group (GG) according to the International Society of Urological Pathology classification [23], date of prostatectomy, adjuvant therapy including radiotherapy and hormone therapy called androgen deprivation therapy (ADT), and comorbidities such as diabetes, hypertension, myocardial infarction, and stroke). Peripheral blood tests were also performed. The following parameters were determined: free and total testosterone levels and total PSA measured by enzyme immunoassay (the PSA score before prostatectomy, at week 6 after surgery, and at the time of the study, called current PSA, were used for data analysis), leukocyte levels, and C-reactive protein.

Neuropsychological assessment included assessment of cognitive function using the computer-based test Neurotest (Table 1) [24].

Table 1.

The computer-based test battery (Neurotest) components.

Test Patient’s Task Evaluated Parameters
Simple reaction time test (SRT) to press the button after seeing a green circle appearing on the computer screen; the stimulus is presented five times, and the number of correct answers and the average response time (ms) are measured speed and correctness of reactions to stimuli; general vigilance and psychomotor speed
Verbal memory test (VM test) to remember as many words as possible from the list of 10 words read by a researcher five times; the patient has to recall words in any order after each reading and 20 min later; the number of correctly repeated words, the number of words outside the list and the number of repetitions are counted (for each attempt) efficiency of the working memory (VM1), short-term memory (VM2, VM3, VM4, and VM5, VM1–VM5 are successive attempts when patients recall the memorized words during the test), and deferred memory (verbal memory deferred test; VMDT), immediate auditory memory (number of words saved), learning (improvement of results in subsequent repetitions), and deferred memory (remembering repeating words)
GoNoGo test to press a key when a green square appears on the computer screen (“Go” part) and to refrain from pressing a key when a blue square appears on the screen (“NoGo” part); stimuli are presented in a random manner; the time (ms) of correct “Go” reactions and the number and percentage of correct and incorrect “Go” and “NoGo” reactions are listed response time under the conditions of the need to control reactions–actions and inhibitions (cognitive control and cognitive inhibition); executive functions
Visuospatial working memory task (VWMT) to remember the layout of the seven playing cards that were previously presented in different places on the monitor screen visuospatial memory—correct and incorrect answer and time of reaction

Statistical Analysis

The distribution of the parameters of the study group was examined using the Shapiro–Wilk test and a significant deviation from the normal distribution was found. Statistical significance of differences between the groups was tested using the Mann–Whitney U test. The analysis of the significance of the correlation was tested using the R-Spearman correlation test. Analysis of covariance was used to investigate the effects of interactions (ANCOVA). The size of the effect is determined with Cohen’s d. A multiple regression model was used to perform multivariate analyses. Statistica 13.0 was used for statistical analysis.

3. Results

In the first stage of the analysis, the study group was classified according to a biochemical indicator of the effectiveness of the surgical procedure, namely a PSA level of <0.1 ng/mL. The study group was divided into those who achieved and those who did not achieve this result. Comparison of the clinicopathological and demographic characteristics of the two groups (Table 2) showed that the group with PSA > 0.1 ng/mL at 6 weeks postoperatively had a lower incidence of arterial hypertension and higher rates of disease severity before surgery according to GG classification. Analyses of biochemical parameters (Table S1) showed that the group with a persistent increase in PSA levels had higher baseline PSA levels before therapy, as well as higher levels of PSA at the time of the study. Analysis of cognitive parameters (Table S2) showed that this group also had a significantly lower number of remembered words in further attempts of the Verbal Memory (VM) test. There was also a trend toward longer response times in the Visuospatial Working Memory Task (VWMT).

Table 2.

Demographic and clinical data in all group and in subgroups with and without persistently elevated PSA levels.

Parameter All
n = 118
Postoperative PSA < 0.1 ng/mL
(n = 97)
Postoperative PSA > 0.1 ng/mL
(n = 21)
d-Cohen p
Age (y) 66.0
(60–70)
66.0
(60.0–70.0)
67.0
(65.0–71.0)
0.82 0.29
BMI (kg/m2) 27.2
(25.6–29.7)
26.8
(25.4–29.7)
27.5
(26.0–29.0)
0.16 0.54
Months from surgery (m) 19.0
(13.0–33.0)
19.0
(14.0–33.0)
23.0
(12.0–33.0)
0.03 0.70
Diabetes (n, %) 19 (16%) 14 (14.5%) 5 (24%) 0.50
Hypertension (n, %) 65 (55%) 59 (61%) 6 (28.5%) 0.02
MI (n, %) 10 (8.5%) 8 (8%) 2 (9.5%) 0.92
Stroke (n, %) 7 (6%) 7 (7%) 0 (0%) 0.60
Education Basic (n, %) 6 (5%) 5 (5%) 1 (5%) 0.31
Vocational (n, %) 33 (28%) 27 (28%) 6 (28.5%)
Secondary (n, %) 36 (30.5%) 26 (27%) 10 (47.5%)
Higher (n, %) 43 (36.5%) 39 (40%) 4 (19%)
Physical activity None (n, %) 40 (34%) 37 (38%) 3 (14%) 0.09
<1x/week (n, %) 24 (20.5%) 17 (17.5%) 7 (33.5%)
<3x/week 54 (45.5%) 43 (44.5%) 11 (52.5%)
GRADE 1 (n, %) 70 (59.5%) 63 (65%) 7 (33.5%) 0.005
2 (n, %) 35 (29.5%) 27 (28%) 8 (38%)
3 (n, %) 5 (4%) 5 (5%) 0 (0.0%)
4 (n, %) 4 (3.5%) 1 (1%) 3 (14.25%)
5 (n, %) 4 (3.5%) 1 (1%) 3 (14.25%)
Nicotinism (n, %) 53 (45%) 44 (45%) 9 (43%) 0.86

Data are shown as the median (Q25–Q75) or number (%). Inter-group differences were assessed using the Mann–Whitney U test. Effect size was measured using the Cohen d method. BMI—body mass index; MI—myocardial infarction; GRADE: group of grading system classification; y—years; m—months. Significant p-values shown in bold.

The PSA levels before the procedure, at 6 weeks after the procedure, and the current levels were analyzed (Table 3). Preoperative PSA levels did not correlate with the results of cognitive tests. Postoperative PSA levels were correlated only with the lower score in the first attempt of the VM test. The current PSA level was positively correlated with a longer response time in the simple reaction test and with a lower number of remembered words in all VM tests and deferred memory trials. Longer response times in the GoNoGo test and VWMT were associated with higher PSA levels at the time of the study. There were no significant correlations between the level of total and free testosterone and the results of cognitive tests in the entire cohort (Table S3).

Table 3.

R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test results and PSA pre, post, and current.

Parameter Preoperative PSA p Post-Surgery PSA p Current PSA p
SRT_C 0.062 ns. 0.019 ns. 0.016 ns.
SRT_RT −0.118 ns. 0.043 ns. 0.249 0.007
VM_1 0.003 ns. −0.214 0.017 −0.193 0.036
VM_2 0.114 ns. −0.049 ns. −0.235 0.01
VM_3 0.055 ns. −0.017 ns. −0.218 0.017
VM_4 −0.092 ns. 0.080 ns. −0.266 0.003
VM_5 −0.098 ns. 0.078 ns. −0.267 0.003
VMDT_C −0.123 ns. −0.084 ns. −0.187 0.047
GoNoGo_C 0.116 ns. 0.017 ns. −0.156 ns.
GoNoGo_RT 0.057 ns. −0.048 ns. −0.194 0.035
GoNoGo IncGO −0.132 ns. −0.069 ns. 0.131 ns.
GoNoGo IncNoGo 0.087 ns. −0.016 ns. 0.108 ns.
VWMT_C −0.061 ns. −0.039 ns. 0.131 ns.
VWMT_CRT 0.023 ns. −0.029 ns. 0.224 0.014
VWMT_IRT 0.060 ns. −0.039 ns. 0.244 0.007

SRT_C—simple reaction time test (number of correct answers); SRT_RT—simple reaction time test (average reaction time); VM_1–VM_5—verbal memory (number of words remembered in each of the five attempts); VMDT—verbal memory deferred test (number of words remembered); GoNoGo_C—GoNoGo test (number of correct answers); RT—reaction time; IncGo—number of incorrect Go answers; IncNoGo—incorrect NoGo answers; VWMT_C—visuospatial working memory task (number of correct answers); CRT—average response time for correct answers; IRT—average response time for incorrect answers. PSA—prostate-specific antigen. Significant p-values shown in bold.

The correlation between biochemical parameters and the results of cognitive tests was analyzed in subgroups of patients with arterial hypertension, stroke, myocardial infarction, and diabetes. Correlations were only detected in the subgroup of diabetic patients (n = 19), in which numerous and strong correlations between the current PSA level and the results of cognitive tests were observed (Table 4). After excluding diabetic patients, there was no correlation between the current PSA level and the results of cognitive tests in the remaining patients.

Table 4.

R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test results and current PSA level in subgroups with and without diabetes.

Parameter Current PSA in NONdiabetes Group p Current PSA in Diabetes Group p
SRT_C 0.044846 ns. 0.003714 ns.
SRT_RT −0.119912 ns. 0.703496 0.0007
VM_1 0.048692 ns. −0.510703 0.025
VM_2 0.089762 ns. −0.616001 0.005
VM_3 −0.030882 ns. −0.558431 0.012
VM_4 −0.066766 ns. −0.615173 0.005
VM_5 −0.009472 ns. −0.625363 0.004
VMDT_C 0.175397 ns. −0.474987 0.03
GoNoGo_C 0.069103 ns. −0.231986 ns.
GoNoGo_RT 0.117830 ns. −0.486785 0.03
GoNoGo IncGO −0.054729 ns. 0.311612 ns.
GoNoGo IncNoGo −0.048036 ns. 0.211985 ns.
VWMT_C 0.175397 ns. −0.458971 0.048
VWMT_CRT −0.113569 ns. 0.789822 0.00005
VWMT_IRT −0.083593 ns. 0.739536 0.0002

SRT_C—simple reaction time test (number of correct answers); SRT_RT—simple reaction time test (average reaction time); VM_1–VM_5—verbal memory (number of words remembered in each of the five attempts); VMDT—verbal memory deferred test (number of words remembered); GoNoGo_C—GoNoGo test (number of correct answers); RT—reaction time; IncGo—number of incorrect Go answers; IncNoGo—incorrect NoGo answers; VWMT_C—visuospatial working memory task (number of correct answers); CRT—average response time for correct answers; IRT—average response time for incorrect answers; PSA—prostate-specific antigen. Significant p-values shown in bold.

In the subgroup of diabetic patients, those with higher levels of free testosterone had a higher number of correct GoNoGo test responses and fewer incorrect Go responses in this test (Table S4).

Because part of the study population underwent hormone therapy, we analyzed the correlation between biochemical parameters and cognitive function in subgroups of patients with and without hormone therapy. In patients who received hormone therapy (Table 5), the level of free testosterone was significantly associated with a greater number of remembered words in the VM deferred test, a greater number of correct answers in the GoNoGo test, and a lower number of incorrect Go and incorrect NoGo responses. Analysis of the correlation between current PSA levels and cognitive outcomes in the same groups showed that in both groups, a higher level of current PSA was associated with some cognitive parameters (Table 5).

Table 5.

R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test and free testosterone in group treated and untreated by hormone therapy.

Parameter Free Testosterone Current PSA
No Hormone Therapy Group
(n = 104)
p Hormone Therapy Group
(n = 14)
p No Hormone Therapy Group
(n = 14)
p Hormone Therapy Group
(n = 104)
p
SRT_C 0.020066 ns. 0.012728 ns. 0.0184537 ns. −0.030253 ns.
SRT_RT −0.183624 0.06 0.167365 ns. 0.261116 ns. 0.001209 ns.
VM_1 −0.216214 0.02 0.377769 ns. −0.236645 ns. 0.080332 ns.
VM_2 −0.166072 ns. 0.311832 ns. −0.542544 0.04 −0.073243 ns.
VM_3 −0.001777 ns. 0.148310 ns. −0.445146 ns. −0.126351 ns.
VM_4 0.022967 ns. 0.395777 ns. −0.336402 ns. −0.218489 0.02
VM_5 0.031529 ns. 0.493676 0.07 −0.191995 ns. −0.219604 0.02
VMDT_C 0.004462 ns. 0.667309 0.01 0.077626 ns. −0.163046 ns.
GoNoGo_C 0.066962 ns. 0.779579 0.001 −0.090094 ns. 0.022998 ns.
GoNoGo_RT −0.082913 ns. 0.485360 0.07 −0.417786 ns. 0.047521 ns.
GoNoGo IncGO −0.065987 ns. −0.711890 0.004 0.090094 ns. −0.023911 ns.
GoNoGo IncNoGo −0.043903 ns. −0.765654 0.001 0.418701 ns. −0.029561 ns.
VWMT_C −0.093605 ns. 0.487308 0.07 0.066111 ns. −0.163046 ns.
VWMT_CRT 0.044880 ns. −0.393309 ns. 0.681656 0.007 0.263424 0.007
VWMT_IRT 0.078058 ns. −0.178738 ns. 0.739830 0.002 0.075388 ns.

SRT_C—simple reaction time test (number of correct answers); SRT_RT—simple reaction time test (average reaction time); VM_1–VM_5—verbal memory (number of words remembered in each of the five attempts); VMDT—verbal memory deferred test (number of words remembered); GoNoGo_C—GoNoGo test (number of correct answers); RT—reaction time; IncGo—number of incorrect Go answers; IncNoGo—incorrect NoGo answers; VWMT_C—visuospatial working memory task (number of correct answers); CRT—average response time for correct answers; IRT—average response time for incorrect answers. Significant p-values shown in bold.

To improve the accuracy of the analyses, we assessed whether the group of patients with diabetes differed from other patients (Table S5). Patients with diabetes were characterized by significantly lower levels of physical activity and significantly lower levels of total testosterone. In the cognitive tests, diabetics had longer reaction times in the simple reaction test and made significantly more incorrect NoGo responses in the GoNoGo test (Table S6).

A summary analysis of the covariance of factors determining the parameters of neuropsychological tests was also performed. This confirmed the significance of the studied biochemical and clinical factors, such as diabetes, in a cognitive context (Table 6). To confirm the obtained results, a multiple regression model was used, which showed a significant share of the current PSA level in almost all the results of cognitive tests. Other important factors were age, diabetes, and one-time post-surgery PSA (Table S7).

Table 6.

Analysis of the covariance (ANCOVA) of the factors determining the parameters of neuropsychological tests.

Age Duration from Surgery Pre-Treatment PSA Post-Surgery PSA Current PSA Free Testosterone Total Testosterone Diabetes GRADE Hormone Therapy
Wald p Wald p Wald p Wald p Wald p Wald p Wald p Wald p Wald p Wald p
SRT_C 0.8 0.34 26.0 <0.001 17.2 <0.001 2.7 0.09 16.2 <0.001 1.5 0.22 21.8 <0.001 2.9 0.08 14.5 0.002 3.5 0.06
SRT_RT 0.2 0.61 0.01 0.91 0.002 0.95 0.5 0.47 4.5 0.03 0.36 0.54 9.1 0.001 0.03 0.84 1.27 0.73 0.005 0.94
VM_1 3.0 0.08 0.05 0.80 1.4 0.23 1.64 0.19 2.08 0.14 2.9 0.08 0.34 0.59 0.56 0.45 0.84 0.83 0.95 0.32
VM_2 0.4 0.52 0.64 0.42 0.05 0.81 0.02 0.87 0.89 s0.34 3.41 0.06 0.03 0.84 5.7 0.01 0.95 0.81 0.22 0.64
VM_3 0.4 0.50 0.03 0.86 0.57 0.44 0.91 0.33 1.1 0.29 0.01 0.93 3.22 0.07 8.8 0.002 8.2 0.04 0.28 0.59
VM_4 2.9 0.08 5.8 0.01 3.4 0.06 0.49 0.48 0.93 0.33 2.2 0.13 0.56 0.45 1.35 0.24 4.1 0.25 0.93 0.33
VM_5 2.9 0.08 5.7 0.01 3.4 0.06 0.5 0.48 0.9 0.33 2.24 0.13 0.56 0.45 1.3 0.24 4.1 0.25 0.93 0.33
VMDT_C 19.4 <0.001 16.5 <0.001 18.3 <0.001 0.28 0.59 0.12 0.72 15.8 <0.001 0.56 0.45 1.01 0.31 23.3 <0.001 7.4 0.006
GoNoGo_C 1.22 0.26 1.4 0.22 2.5 0.11 3.8 0.05 6.4 0.01 0.14 0.70 12.3 <0.001 0.7 0.38 11.4 0.009 0.5 0.46
GoNoGo_RT 0.02 0.86 1.64 0.2 2.8 0.09 0.2 0.66 0.24 0.62 0.006 0.93 10.2 <0.001 0.54 0.45 2.2 0.52 0.67 0.41
GoNoGo IncNoGo 4.8 0.02 8.5 0.003 5.8 0.01 0.2 0.6 0.02 0.88 19.9 <0.001 1.8 0.17 <0.001 0.95 4.7 0.19 11.1 <0.001
VWMT_C 15.2 <0.001 12.3 <0.001 0.98 0.32 0.53 0.46 0.44 0.50 0.28 0.59 0.97 0.32 3.15 0.07 6.78 0.07 4.2 0.04
VWMT_CRT 2.64 0.10 0.37 0.54 1.6 0.20 0.14 0.70 0.23 0.62 5.2 0.02 25.0 <0.001 1.8 0.18 8.1 0.04 0.6 0.41

SRT_C—simple reaction time test (number of correct answers); SRT_RT—simple reaction time test (average reaction time); VM_1–VM_5—verbal memory (number of words remembered in each of the five attempts); VMDT—verbal memory deferred test (number of words remembered); GoNoGo_C—GoNoGo test (number of correct answers); RT—reaction time; IncGo—number of incorrect Go answers; IncNoGo—incorrect NoGo answers; VWMT_C—visuospatial working memory task (number of correct answers); CRT—average response time for correct answers. Significant p-values shown in bold.

4. Discussion

4.1. Prostate-Specific Antigen

PSA belongs to the serin protease family and together with other clinical tools (digital rectal examination, family history of PC, age, and magnetic resonance imaging) is recommended by clinical practice guidelines for the early detection of PC and for therapeutic monitoring [25]. In this study, PSA levels before prostatectomy did not correlate with cognitive function (Table 3). Postoperative PSA levels correlated only with the lower score in the first attempt of the VM test. However, when PSA levels were measured at longer times after prostatectomy, there was a significant relationship between PSA and cognitive functions including reaction time and VM (Table 3). This is consistent with the literature, and suggests that PSA could be used as a marker of cognitive function [21]. Sternberg et al. compared PSA levels between patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and a control group. The results showed a significant association between cognitive status and PSA levels [21]. A similar study was performed by Lin et al. in a large cohort; these authors observed that patients with AD not only had increased PSA levels, but also an increased risk of PC compared with controls [13]. In studies that did not include PSA level, but only the diagnosis of PC and its treatment, neither PC itself nor the use of hormonal treatment was associated with MCI [26].

The above studies were limited to the overall assessment of cognitive function or the overall diagnosis of cognitive disorders, and did not assess individual domains. In the present study, PSA levels were associated with almost all cognitive domains, especially reaction time and VM. In addition, visuospatial memory was associated with PSA levels in the present cohort (Table 3).

The mechanisms underlying these associations are not clear. Given that PSA is present in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), it can be hypothesized that PSA has a direct effect on brain tissue and brain function [27]. Significant differences in CSF PSA levels were observed between cancer and other prostate diseases [28]. However, in patients with PC, high CSF PSA levels are commonly associated with central nervous system cancer involvement/metastasis [29]. The clinical usefulness of these studies, however, is limited because CSF PSA values are usually too low [28].

The relationship between cognitive function and PSA could be related to the treatment; some studies have reported that cognitive function in PC depends on the type of treatment [30,31]. The role of ADT, which is often used as an adjuvant treatment, is emphasized. Consistently, the cognitive domains that correlate with PSA are the most frequently affected in PC patients receiving ADT [6]. Testosterone deprivation (under the influence of hormonal treatment) could affect both cognitive function and PSA levels [21,32]. However, patients with increased PSA levels after local treatment are at a high risk of subsequent progression and often undergo ADT, which causes a decrease in cognitive disorders [5,33,34]. In this study, the group of patients with persistently elevated PSA after surgery had worse VM than those with normal postoperative PSA levels (<0.1 ng/mL) (Table S2). To exclude the effect of hormonal treatment on the relationship between cognitive presentation and PSA, we performed subgroup analyses. These analyses showed that strong correlations between these variables also occurred in patients who did not receive hormonal treatment, but they concerned other cognitive domains (Table 5).

This study also examined various factors that could modulate PSA levels and affect their correlation with cognitive function. Among the factors that could affect PSA or cognitive function, we analyzed arterial hypertension and history of stroke, myocardial infarction, or diabetes [35,36]. Most of these factors were not correlated with cognitive function except diabetes mellitus. The results are presented in Table 4. The differences concerned all the studied cognitive domains.

Diabetes is an important independent risk factor for cognitive impairment [37]. Elabbady et al. assessed the role of diabetes in prostate-related parameters and observed significant differences in the levels of PSA between patients with and without diabetes [38]. Similarly, Kobayashi et al. reported that, in men with higher blood glucose levels, PSA values were significantly reduced [36]. Diabetes mellitus is thus a factor that influences cognition and can affect PSA.

4.2. Testosterone

Testosterone level is an important factor that affects many aspects of PC. In this study, we investigated the association of serum total testosterone and free testosterone levels with cognitive function in PC survivors and did not find any significant relationships (Table S3). However, literature data on the association of testosterone and its supplementation with cognitive function are not consistent. Some studies have reported significant dependencies [12,39,40], whereas other reports have not [41,42]. A review by Cai and Li demonstrated that low levels of free testosterone and total testosterone are significantly correlated with cognitive decline, as well as with an increased risk of AD in older men [12]. Pintana et al., on the other hand, have remarked that new reports highlight non-significant findings regarding the relationship between testosterone and cognitive function [41]. These incompatibilities can be explained by methodological differences as well as differences in age or ethnicity [41]. Placebo-controlled randomized clinical trials also showed different effects of testosterone supplementation on cognitive function [43].

The treatment of PC also affects testosterone levels. To examine the potential influence of hormonal treatment, we performed separate analyses of cognitive function in men who received ADT and those who did not. In patients receiving hormone therapy, we observed a significant correlation between free testosterone levels and cognitive function, and high testosterone levels were associated with better cognitive performance. These correlations were not observed in the group of people who did not receive ADT (Table 5). Data on the role of ADT in cognitive function are also inconsistent. A beneficial effect of ADT on cognitive status has been reported [44], whereas other studies do not support these observations or report subtle but significant adverse effects of ADT [45]. Andella et al. reviewed publications assessing the cognitive functioning of PC patients treated with ADT. Of the 31 studies included in the analysis, 16 studies found that this therapy had no adverse effects on cognitive function, while another 11 studies found that it negatively affected cognition. In four studies, the results were inconclusive. The authors also noted that future research should focus on further exploring brain features with functional magnetic resonance imaging because this technique may be more sensitive in detecting brain abnormalities in patients treated with ADT [46]. Neuroimaging studies indicate that ADT may cause changes in regional brain metabolism associated with alterations in spatial performance and VM. Brain areas that are affected by androgen deprivation are consistent with those affected in diabetes and in AD, suggesting possible common mechanisms [47]. These changes are located only in brain regions that control specific functions. This may explain why these abnormalities are often elusive in general cognitive function tests, as their detection requires methods aimed at assessing specific cognitive domains [48]. In studies of specific domains, patients treated with ADT perform worse than controls mostly in visuomotor tasks and spatial and VM [43,49,50]. In this study, we observed cognitive decline mostly in executive functions and deferred memory (Table 5).

As described for PSA, we performed additional analyses to identify factors affecting free testosterone levels and cognitive parameters. In patients with diabetes, the level of free testosterone strongly correlated with cognitive control and cognitive inhibition, and a correlation was also observed in the deferred memory test (Table S4). Both diabetes and low testosterone levels are risk factors for cognitive dysfunction [51]. Bertram et al. reported that low testosterone level is a risk factor for both cognitive impairment and the development of diabetes. These findings suggest that the dependencies are complex. German scientists proposed a molecular framework that links diabetes, testosterone, and cognitive impairment [11]. They suggested that the blood-brain barrier can break down because of the inflammatory, oxidative, and metabolic changes in diabetes, which promote the pathological features of dementia. The relationship between testosterone levels, insulin-resistant obesity, and cognitive function was also observed by Pintana et al. [41]. This author proposed a model in which cognitive decline results from either the direct effect of lack of testosterone on the brain or an association between testosterone deficiency and obesity leading to fat tissue accumulation, insulin resistance, and finally cognitive impairment [41].

In conclusion, the present findings suggest that diabetes is associated with the effect of PSA or testosterone levels on cognitive function, supporting that good quality of care in diabetes patients with PC is important. However, the present study was limited by the small number of participants. Additional studies, including molecular studies, are needed to confirm and better understand the observed phenomena and their causes.

5. Conclusions

Persistently elevated PSA levels following prostatectomy and free testosterone levels after hormonal treatment are potential biomarkers of cognitive function that are related mainly to VM and executive function. Concomitant diabetes affects the correlation between PSA levels and cognitive abilities, as well as that between testosterone level and cognitive function.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jcm10225307/s1. Table S1: Biochemical parameters in the study group and in subgroups with and without persistently elevated PSA levels. Table S2: Cognitive parameters in the study group and in subgroups with and without persistently elevated PSA levels. Table S3: R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test results and free/total testosterone level. Table S4: R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test results and free testosterone level in subgroups with and without diabetes. Table S5: Demographic and clinical data in non-diabetes and diabetes groups. Table S6: Cognitive parameters in non-diabetes and diabetes groups. Table S7: Multiple regression model coefficients on cognitive test results.

Author Contributions

Author Contributions: Conceptualization—P.J., B.B., A.P., A.C.-T., M.B., and A.B.; methodology, A.P., P.J., B.B., and M.B.; formal analysis, A.P., A.C.-T., and M.B.; investigation, P.J. and B.B.; data curation, A.P., B.B., and A.C.-T.; writing—original draft preparation, A.P., B.B., A.C.-T., and R.W.; writing—review and editing—R.W., P.J., M.B., and A.B.; Visualization, M.B.; supervision, M.B. and A.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Bioethics Committee of the Nicolaus Copernicus University, Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz (Approval No. 476/2017).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data available on request due to ethical policy.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Rawla P. Epidemiology of Prostate Cancer. World J. Oncol. 2019;10:63–89. doi: 10.14740/wjon1191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Sung H., Ferlay J., Siegel R.L., Laversanne M., Soerjomataram I., Jemal A., Bray F. Global Cancer Statistics 2020: Globocan estimates of incidence and mortality worldwide for 36 cancers in 185 countries. CA Cancer J. Clin. 2021;71:209–249. doi: 10.3322/caac.21660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Patasius A., Smailyte G. All-Cause Mortality Risk in National Prostate Cancer Cohort: An Impact of Population-Based Prostate Cancer Screening. J. Clin. Med. 2021;10:2459. doi: 10.3390/jcm10112459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Lange M., Joly F., Vardy J., Ahles T., Dubois M., Tron L., Winocur G., De Ruiter M., Castel H. Cancer-related cognitive impairment: An update on state of the art, detection, and management strategies in cancer survivors. Ann. Oncol. 2019;30:1925–1940. doi: 10.1093/annonc/mdz410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ryan C., Wefel J.S., Morgans A.K. A review of prostate cancer treatment impact on the CNS and cognitive function. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2019;23:207–219. doi: 10.1038/s41391-019-0195-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Treanor C., Li J., Donnelly M. Cognitive impairment among prostate cancer patients: An overview of reviews. Eur. J. Cancer Care. 2017;26 doi: 10.1111/ecc.12642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jarzemski P., Brzoszczyk B., Popiołek A., Stachowicz-Karpińska A., Gołota S., Bieliński M., Borkowska A. Cognitive function, depression, and anxiety in patients undergoing radical prostatectomy with and without adjuvant treatment. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2019;15:819–829. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S200501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hardy S.J., Krull K.R., Wefel J.S., Janelsins M. Cognitive Changes in Cancer Survivors. Am. Soc. Clin. Oncol. Educ. Book. 2018;23:795–806. doi: 10.1200/EDBK_201179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Cascella M., Di Napoli R., Carbone D., Cuomo G.F., Bimonte S., Mbolla B.F.E., Muzio M.R. Chemotherapy-related cognitive impairment: Mechanisms, clinical features and research perspectives. Recent. Prog. Med. 2018;109:523–530. doi: 10.1701/3031.30289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Toh Y.L., Mujtaba J.S., Bansal S., Yeo A., Shwe M., Lau A.J., Chan A. Prechemotherapy Levels of Plasma Dehydroepiandrosterone and Its Sulfated Form as Predictors of Cancer-Related Cognitive Impairment in Patients with Breast Cancer Receiving Chemotherapy. Pharmacother. J. Hum. Pharmacol. Drug Ther. 2019;39:553–563. doi: 10.1002/phar.2259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bertram S., Brixius K., Brinkmann C. Exercise for the diabetic brain: How physical training may help prevent dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in T2DM patients. Endocrine. 2016;53:350–363. doi: 10.1007/s12020-016-0976-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cai Z., Li H. An Updated Review: Androgens and Cognitive Impairment in Older Men. Front. Endocrinol. 2020;11:586909. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2020.586909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lin H.-C., Kao L.-T., Chung S.-D., Huang C.-C., Shia B.-C., Huang C.-Y. Alzheimer’s disease is associated with prostate cancer: A population-based study. Oncotarget. 2018;9:7616–7622. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.24139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Spratt D.E., Shore N., Sartor O., Rathkopf D., Olivier K. Treating the patient and not just the cancer: Therapeutic burden in prostate cancer. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2021;24:647–661. doi: 10.1038/s41391-021-00328-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Burke M.A., Lowrance W., Perczek R. Emotional and cognitive burden of prostate cancer. Urol. Clin. 2003;30:295–304. doi: 10.1016/S0094-0143(02)00190-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Chung N.-C., Walker A.K., Dhillon H.M., Vardy J.L. Mechanisms and Treatment for Cancer-and Chemotherapy-Related Cog-nitive Impairment in Survivors of Non-CNS Malignancies. Oncology. 2018;32:591–598. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Janelsins M.C., Kesler S.R., Ahles T.A., Morrow G.R. Prevalence, mechanisms, and management of cancer-related cognitive impairment. Int. Rev. Psychiatry. 2014;26:102–113. doi: 10.3109/09540261.2013.864260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cui M.Y., Lin Y., Sheng J.Y., Zhang X., Cui R.J. Exercise Intervention Associated with Cognitive Improvement in Alzheimer’s Disease. Neural Plast. 2018;2018:9234105. doi: 10.1155/2018/9234105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Leidi-Maimone B., Notter-Bielser M.-L., Laouadi M.-H., Perrin S., Métraux H., Damian D., Chavan C.F., Nsir M., Cibelli G., Tâche M.-J., et al. How non-drug interventions affect the quality of life of patients suffering from progressive cognitive decline and their main caregiver. Aging. 2020;12:10754–10771. doi: 10.18632/aging.103291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ferraro S., Bussetti M., Bassani N., Rossi R., Incarbone G., Bianchi F., Maggioni M., Runza L., Ceriotti F., Panteghini M. Definition of Outcome-Based Prostate-Specific Antigen (PSA) Thresholds for Advanced Prostate Cancer Risk Prediction. Cancers. 2021;13:3381. doi: 10.3390/cancers13143381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Sternberg Z., Podolsky R., Nir A., Yu J., Nir R., Halvorsen S.W., Chadha K., Quinn J.F., Kaye J., Kolb C. Increased free prostate specific antigen serum levels in Alzheimer’s disease, correlation with Cognitive Decline. J. Neurol. Sci. 2019;400:188–193. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2019.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Giagulli V.A., Guastamacchia E., Licchelli B., Triggiani V. Serum Testosterone and Cognitive Function in Ageing Male: Updating the Evidence. Recent Pat. Endocr. Metab. Immune Drug Discov. 2016;10:22–30. doi: 10.2174/1872214810999160603213743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.van Leenders G.J., van der Kwast T.H., Grignon D.J., Evans A.J., Kristiansen G., Kweldam C.F., Litjens G., McKenney J.K., Melamed J., Mottet N., et al. The 2019 International Society of Urological Pathology (ISUP) Consensus Conference on Grading of Prostatic Carcinoma. Am. J. Surg. Pathol. 2020;44:e87–e99. doi: 10.1097/PAS.0000000000001497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Guenter W., Bieliński M., Bonek R., Borkowska A. Neurochemical Changes in the Brain and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms in Clinically Isolated Syndrome. J. Clin. Med. 2020;9:3909. doi: 10.3390/jcm9123909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ferraro S., Bussetti M., Panteghini M. Serum Prostate-Specific Antigen Testing for Early Detection of Prostate Cancer: Managing the Gap between Clinical and Laboratory Practice. Clin. Chem. 2021;67:602–609. doi: 10.1093/clinchem/hvab002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Quiñones H.J.A., Stish B.J., Hagen C., Petersen R.C., Mielke M.M. Prostate Cancer, Use of Androgen Deprivation Therapy, and Cognitive Impairment: A Population-Based Study. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2020;34:118–121. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Orphanos G., Ioannidis G., Michael M., Kitrou G. Prostate-specific antigen in the cerebrospinal fluid: A marker of local disease. Med. Oncol. 2008;26:143–146. doi: 10.1007/s12032-008-9096-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Sahin H., Aflay U., Batun S., Bircan M.K. Cerebrospinal fluid prostate specific antigen (CSF PSA) in prostate cancer patients with lower spine metastasis. Int. Urol. Nephrol. 2005;37:727–730. doi: 10.1007/s11255-005-8438-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Schaller B., Merlo A., Kirsch E., Lehmann K., Huber P.R., Lyrer P., Steck A.J., Gratzl O. Prostate-specific antigen in the cerebrospinal fluid leads to diagnosis of solitary cauda equina metastasis: A unique case report and review of the literature. Br. J. Cancer. 1998;77:2386–2389. doi: 10.1038/bjc.1998.397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Jim H.S.L., Small B.J., Patterson S., Salup R., Jacobsen P.B. Cognitive impairment in men treated with luteinizing hormone–releasing hormone agonists for prostate cancer: A controlled comparison. Support. Care Cancer. 2009;18:21–27. doi: 10.1007/s00520-009-0625-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Yiannopoulou K.G., Anastasiou A.I., Kontoangelos K., Papageorgiou C., Anastasiou I.P. Cognitive and Psychological Impacts of Different Treatment Options for Prostate Cancer: A Critical Analysis. Curr. Urol. 2020;14:169–177. doi: 10.1159/000499242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Zarghami N., Grass L., Diamandis E. Steroid hormone regulation of prostate-specific antigen gene expression in breast cancer. Br. J. Cancer. 1997;75:579–588. doi: 10.1038/bjc.1997.101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Soo A., O’Callaghan M.E., Kopsaftis T., Vatandoust S., Moretti K., Kichenadasse G. PSA response to antiandrogen withdrawal: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2021;24:826–836. doi: 10.1038/s41391-021-00337-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Saad F., Bögemann M., Suzuki K., Shore N. Treatment of nonmetastatic castration-resistant prostate cancer: Focus on second-generation androgen receptor inhibitors. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2021;24:323–334. doi: 10.1038/s41391-020-00310-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tarantino G., Crocetto F., Di Vito C., Martino R., Pandolfo S.D., Creta M., Aveta A., Buonerba C., Imbimbo C. Clinical factors affecting prostate-specific antigen levels in prostate cancer patients undergoing radical prostatectomy: A retrospective study. Future Sci. OA. 2021;7:FSO643. doi: 10.2144/fsoa-2020-0154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kobayashi M., Mizuno T., Yuki H., Kambara T., Betsunoh H., Nukui A., Abe H., Fukabori Y., Yashi M., Kamai T. Association between serum prostate-specific antigen level and diabetes, obesity, hypertension, and the laboratory parameters related to glucose tolerance, hepatic function, and lipid profile: Implications for modification of prostate-specific antigen threshold. Int. J. Clin. Oncol. 2020;25:472–478. doi: 10.1007/s10147-019-01527-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Zilliox L.A., Chadrasekaran K., Kwan J.Y., Russell J.W. Diabetes and Cognitive Impairment. Curr. Diabetes Rep. 2016;16:87. doi: 10.1007/s11892-016-0775-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Elabbady A., Hashad M.M., Kotb A., Ghanem A.E. Studying the effect of type 2 diabetes mellitus on prostate-related parameters: A prospective single institutional study. Prostate Int. 2016;4:156–159. doi: 10.1016/j.prnil.2016.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cherrier M.M., Matsumoto A.M., Amory J.K., Asthana S., Bremner W., Peskind E.R., Raskind M.A., Craft S. Testosterone improves spatial memory in men with Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2005;64:2063–2068. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000165995.98986.F1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Okamoto K., Sekine Y., Nomura M., Koike H., Matsui H., Shibata Y., Ito K., Suzuki K. Effects of a luteinizing hormone-releasing hormone agonist on cognitive, sexual, and hormonal functions in patients with prostate cancer: Relationship with testicular and adrenal androgen levels. Basic Clin. Androl. 2015;25:3. doi: 10.1186/s12610-015-0019-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Pintana H., Chattipakorn N., Chattipakorn S. Testosterone deficiency, insulin-resistant obesity and cognitive function. Metab. Brain Dis. 2015;30:853–876. doi: 10.1007/s11011-015-9655-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Matsumoto A.M. Testosterone Replacement in Men with Age-Related Low Testosterone: What Did We Learn from the Testosterone Trials? Curr. Opin. Endocr. Metab. Res. 2019;6:34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.coemr.2019.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Jamadar R.J., Winters M.J., Maki P.M. Cognitive changes associated with ADT: A review of the literature. Asian J. Androl. 2012;14:232–238. doi: 10.1038/aja.2011.107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cherrier M., Rose A., Higano C. The effects of combined androgen blockade on cognitive function during the first cycle of intermittent androgen suppression in patients with prostate cancer. J. Urol. 2003;170:1808–1811. doi: 10.1097/01.ju.0000091640.59812.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Nelson C.J., Bs J.S.L., Ba M.C.G., Roth A.J. Cognitive effects of hormone therapy in men with prostate cancer. Cancer. 2008;113:1097–1106. doi: 10.1002/cncr.23658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Andela C.D., Matte R., Jazet I.M., Zonneveld W.C., Schoones J.W., Meinders A.E. Effect of androgen deprivation therapy on cognitive functioning in men with prostate cancer: A systematic review. Int. J. Urol. 2021;28:786–798. doi: 10.1111/iju.14596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Cherrier M.M., Cross D.J., Higano C.S., Minoshima S. Changes in cerebral metabolic activity in men undergoing androgen deprivation therapy for non-metastatic prostate cancer. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2018;21:394–402. doi: 10.1038/s41391-018-0037-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Chao H.H., Uchio E., Zhang S., Hu S., Bednarski S.R., Luo X., Rose M., Concato J., Li C.-S.R. Effects of androgen deprivation on brain function in prostate cancer patients–a prospective observational cohort analysis. BMC Cancer. 2012;12:371. doi: 10.1186/1471-2407-12-371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.McGinty H.L., Phillips K.M., Jim H.S.L., Cessna J.M., Asvat Y., Cases M., Small B., Jacobsen P.B. Cognitive functioning in men receiving androgen deprivation therapy for prostate cancer: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Support. Care Cancer. 2014;22:2271–2280. doi: 10.1007/s00520-014-2285-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Salminen E.K., Portin R.I., Koskinen A., Helenius H., Nurmi M. Associations between serum testosterone fall and cognitive function in prostate cancer patients. Clin. Cancer Res. 2004;10:7575–7582. doi: 10.1158/1078-0432.CCR-04-0750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Etgen T., Sander D., Bickel H., Förstl H. Mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Dtsch. Aerztebl. Int. 2011;108:743–750. doi: 10.3238/arztebl.2011.0743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

Data available on request due to ethical policy.


Articles from Journal of Clinical Medicine are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES