Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 Nov 26;10:e63782. doi: 10.7554/eLife.63782

Attention network modulation via tRNS correlates with attention gain

Federica Contò 1,2,, Grace Edwards 1,3, Sarah Tyler 1,4, Danielle Parrott 1,2, Emily Grossman 5, Lorella Battelli 1,2,3,6
Editors: Taraz Lee7, Richard B Ivry8
PMCID: PMC8626087  PMID: 34826292

Abstract

Transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) can enhance vision in the healthy and diseased brain. Yet, the impact of multi-day tRNS on large-scale cortical networks is still unknown. We investigated the impact of tRNS coupled with behavioral training on resting-state functional connectivity and attention. We trained human subjects for 4 consecutive days on two attention tasks, while receiving tRNS over the intraparietal sulci, the middle temporal areas, or Sham stimulation. We measured resting-state functional connectivity of nodes of the dorsal and ventral attention network (DVAN) before and after training. We found a strong behavioral improvement and increased connectivity within the DVAN after parietal stimulation only. Crucially, behavioral improvement positively correlated with connectivity measures. We conclude changes in connectivity are a marker for the enduring effect of tRNS upon behavior. Our results suggest that tRNS has strong potential to augment cognitive capacity in healthy individuals and promote recovery in the neurological population.

Research organism: None

Introduction

Vision and attention are the primary sensory and cognitive modalities through which humans interact with the environment. Augmentation of visuoperceptual function through training, termed perceptual learning (PL), is crucial for clinical and aging populations that benefit from therapeutic interventions to recover and retain perceptual function. PL is also a pivotal tool for investigating neural plasticity (Gilbert, 1994; Polat et al., 2004; Shibata et al., 2016; Deveau et al., 2014; Sterkin et al., 2018), and has been linked to localized neural changes in sensory cortex and long-range sensory-attention network reorganization (Gilbert et al., 2001; Li et al., 2004; Sasaki et al., 2010; Fahle, 2004). Thus, PL has potential as an approach to reveal underlying cortical reorganization that promotes sustained improvements in perceptual function.

Although promising, PL approaches have been limited in their applicability, due in part to the long and intensive training protocols which largely impede their use (Dosher and Lu, 2017; Das et al., 2014; Li et al., 2004; Huang et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2010a; Zhang et al., 2010b; Dosher and Lu, 2005). Currently lacking are PL approaches to increase the rate of learning, valuable for populations with limited abilities to engage in demanding perceptual tasks. A notable exception is ‘cross-tasks’ training, in which learning protocols are significantly shortened by adopting a training procedure on two different tasks with the same stimuli simultaneously (Szpiro et al., 2014; Wright et al., 2010).

More recently, transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) has been proposed as a tool to facilitate cortical plasticity, particularly when coupled with behavioral training (Edwards et al., 2019). When delivered using a transcranial random noise protocol (tRNS), tES can modulate visual cortex excitability (Herpich et al., 2018), promote visual functions in healthy subjects (Fertonani et al., 2011; Pirulli et al., 2013; Tyler et al., 2018), and facilitate recovery of visual dysfunctions in disease (Campana et al., 2014; Camilleri et al., 2016; Herpich et al., 2019). Moreover, recent studies show PL coupled with tES can successfully boost the effects of training by increasing the rate of learning across sessions (Fertonani et al., 2011), particularly when applied during task training (Pirulli et al., 2013), with the subsequent observed behavioral improvements persisting for durations well beyond those obtained through PL training alone (Cappelletti et al., 2013; Herpich et al., 2019).

Studies on the physiological effects of tRNS on brain dynamics are not yet clear on the mechanisms by which training is facilitated. The leading hypothesis derived from animal studies is that tRNS has a stochastic resonance effect upon neurons such that the injected subthreshold stimulation both resonates with ongoing task-related oscillatory activity and reduces endogenous noise, and thus, when applied at optimal levels, enhances the signal-to-noise of task-related spike firing (Antal and Herrmann, 2016; Liu et al., 2018; Polanía et al., 2018). Consistent with the network oscillation component of resonant theory, extant studies on humans show that focal tES of a node within a network can cause a cascade of functional changes selectively spreading within that network (Nitsche and Paulus, 2000; Polanía et al., 2012; Antal et al., 2011; Krause et al., 2017). Modeling studies have likewise shown changes in network dynamics within the directly stimulated area and across distal but connected cortical areas (Kunze et al., 2016; Polanía et al., 2011; Sehm et al., 2012). Given that many behavioral manifestations of neurological and psychiatric disorders are the consequence of altered brain network connectivity (Buckner et al., 2005; Fox et al., 2014), the network-wide impact of tES could have wide clinical applications.

The present study uses a combination of tRNS and fMRI, in conjunction with cross-training on a PL task, to investigate large-scale cortical dynamics when coupled with behavioral training. Specifically, we implemented a 4-day attention training protocol on two randomly interleaved PL tasks: orientation discrimination (OD) and temporal order judgments (TOJs). This training was coupled with tRNS to examine if tRNS to the parietal lobe would modulate the dorsal and ventral attention network (DVAN), and subsequently increase attention. The target of the parietal stimulation was the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), a crucial node of spatial attention (Vossel et al., 2014; Battelli et al., 2017; Plow et al., 2014; Leitão et al., 2015; Battelli et al., 2009). The attention network is often divided into the dorsal and the ventral attention sub-networks (DAN and VAN, or DVAN; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Fox et al., 2006). However, we are interested in the whole network due to the continuous interplay between networks, which enables the dynamic control of the different attentional processes (Vossel et al., 2014; Macaluso and Driver, 2005; Sani et al., 2019). Moreover, to control for network-specific effects of stimulation, we also measured whether tRNS and training affected dynamics within the default mode network (DMN), typically active during passive or resting conditions (Raichle et al., 2001; Buckner and DiNicola, 2019). Among the nodes of the DAN, the human middle temporal complex (hMT+) has been directly implicated in visual temporal encoding (Salvioni et al., 2013), a cognitive process involved by our tasks. Thus, hMT+ was included as a second stimulation site to evaluate the efficacy of tRNS on perceptual processes, and to serve as an active control to determine if attention modulation was specific to parietal stimulation. We also sought to understand whether neural modulation following tRNS coupled with training would lead to long-lasting behavioral improvements, and we tested it 24 hr following the last training session by examining if behavioral and neuromodulatory changes persisted. Lasting neural and behavioral change would suggest cortical plasticity that outlasts the end of stimulation.

Results

Learning-dependent changes in behavior

We examined the effect of tRNS delivered concurrently with the task in a 4-day PL protocol (Figure 1A, orange section) that embedded two attentional tasks: an OD task in which participants were asked to determine if the Gabors (disks with black and white stripes, Figure 1D) presented either side of a fixation cross had the ‘same’ or ‘different’ orientation, and a TOJ task in which participants were asked to determine if the Gabors’ onsets were simultaneous (same) or asynchronous (different) (Figure 1C and D; for a detailed description of the experimental procedure, tasks, and the stimuli see ‘Materials and methods’ section). These two attention tasks, well known to increase activity within attention-related cortical areas, were randomly presented within each block, a ‘cross-tasks’ procedure which has shown efficacy in supporting more learning than single-task training (Szpiro et al., 2014). While engaged in the attention tasks, participants received concurrent tRNS to cortical regions crucial hotspots of visuospatial attention, bilateral IPS, and over hMT+ or Sham in control conditions (Figure 1B). Pre and post-measures of task performance, as well as resting state connectivity, were collected prior and subsequent to the multi-day training sessions. This allowed us to evaluate both the effect of stimulation combined with training on each task separately (Figure 1A, blue section) and the time course of learning across training and stimulation days (Figure 1A, orange section).

Figure 1. Procedure and stimulation sites.

Figure 1.

(A) Experiment timeline. Thresholds for OD and TOJ tasks were performed at the beginning of the multi-session experimental procedure (green section). On the pre-test session, subjects were tested on the tasks while fMRI data were collected (blue section). A resting-state scan was also collected during this session. Next, subjects underwent behavioral training concurrently with tRNS for 25 min (Training Day 1, Training Day 2, Training Day 3, and Training Day 4, orange section). Post-test session was a repeat of the pre-test session (blue section). For detailed information on fMRI data acquisition order, see Materials and methods section. (B) Stimulation settings. Location of stimulation sites were localized using EEG 10/20 system. Saline-soaked electrodes was placed over P3 (red filled circle) and P4 (blue filled circle) for bilateral parietal and Sham stimulation, and over P07 (red filled circle) and P08 (blue filled circle) for hMT+ stimulation. (C) Intermixed trials sequence example: two types of attention tasks were randomly presented within each block, the OD and the TOJ task. (D). Example trial. The visual information was the same for TOJ and OD tasks. The cue at the beginning of the trial dictated which feature (time or orientation) should be attended. Red outline boxes are used to highlight the Gabors stimuli (black and white oriented bars) in the illustration but they were not present during the experiment. OD, orientation discrimination; TOJ, temporal order judgment.

Magnitude of learning

Orientation discrimination

We first ensured that the three stimulation groups did not differ prior to stimulation by comparing their behavioral performance on the orientation task. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed no significant difference between groups prior to stimulation (F(2, 27)=0.575, p=0.569, ηp2=0.041). We then examined the combination of stimulation and training on learning. This analysis revealed a main effect of session on OD performance (F(1, 27)=10.056, p=0.004, ηp2=0.271), indicating that accuracy scores changed as a function of training. There was no significant main effect of stimulation condition alone on performance (F(2, 27)=0.273, p=0.763, ηp2=0.020). Crucially, the analysis revealed a significant interaction between stimulation condition and session (F(2, 27)=11.257, p<0.001, ηp2=0.455), indicating that learning from the pre- to post-test sessions varied significantly depending on the stimulation site.

To further investigate the significant interaction effect, we ran pairwise t-tests to compare pre- and post-test performance for each group, separately. We found that only the Parietal group showed significant improvement in orientation judgment accuracy between the pre- and post-test sessions (t(9)=−4.53, p=0.001, average improvement 29.8%, Cohen’s d=−1.43), while there were no significant differences between pre- and post-test performance for the hMT+ group (t(9)=−0.608, p=0.558, average improvement 2.2%, Cohen’s d=−0.19), nor for the Sham group (t(9)=0.602, p=0.562, average decrease –3.1%, Cohen’s d=−0.19). Average improvements per condition are reported in Figure 2A. A one-way ANOVA on post-test performance (F(2, 20.593)=3.672, p=0.043, Brown-Forsythe corrected; ηp2=0.214) and on performance improvements values (F(2, 27)=11.257, p<0.001; ηp2=0.455) confirmed a significant difference between the three stimulation groups. Bonferroni post-hoc pairwise comparisons revealed a significant difference in performance improvement between Parietal and Sham conditions (p=0.003, Cohen’s d=1.76), and between parietal and hMT+ (p≤0.001, Cohen’s d=1.64), while hMT+ and Sham did not differ significantly (p=1.0, Cohen’s d=0.38).

Figure 2. Behavioral performance on the OD and the TOJ task during and following stimulation and training.

Figure 2.

(A) Box plots for behavioral improvement by stimulation condition on the OD and (B) the TOJ task normalized to performance at baseline, prior to stimulation. The center line in the middle of the box is the median of each data distribution, while the box represents the interquartile range (IQR), with the lower quartile representing the 25th percentile (Q1) and the upper quartile representing the 75th percentile (Q3). (C) Change in accuracy across training days for all conditions on the OD and on the TOJ task (D) during training and stimulation sessions. Each line represents a different stimulation condition (green for parietal, orange for Sham, and blue for hMT+ group), error bars represent SEM. Asterisks indicate significant differences relative to the first training session (*<0.05, **<0.01). OD, orientation discrimination; TOJ, temporal order judgment.

Temporal order judgment

A one-way ANOVA on pre-test session data revealed no significant differences between groups at baseline (F(2, 27)=0.14, p = 0.867; ηp2=0.010). We next investigated whether there was a significant difference between the pre- and post-test TOJ performance depending on stimulation condition. A mixed factors repeated measures ANOVA revealed no main effect of session (F(1, 27)=0.496, p=0.48; ηp2=0.018) nor of condition (F(1, 27)=0.669, p=0.521; ηp2=0.047) on performance. There was also no significant interaction between session and stimulation condition (F(2, 27)=0.589, p=0.562; ηp2=0.042), which indicates that performance in the pre- and post-test session did not differ depending on stimulation condition.

A one-way ANOVA on calculated improvement scores (difference between pre- and post-test performance, Figure 2B) confirmed a non significant performance change between stimulation conditions (F(2, 27)=0.589, p=0.562; η2=0.042). Participants in the parietal tRNS group performed on average 3.1% better following multi-session stimulation and training, while there was a slight decrease in performance for the hMT+ (–4.1%) and the Sham groups (–7.8%), Figure 2B.

Time course of learning

Orientation discrimination

We next analyzed performance during the 4-day training sessions. To measure changes in performance over time, we performed a mixed repeated measures ANOVA with stimulation site (n=3 ) as between-subjects factor and training day (n=4) as within-subjects factor. Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity was violated (χ2 (5)=19.7, p=0.001); hence, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used to correct the degrees of freedom and to assess the significance of the corresponding p-values. We found a significant main effect of training days (F(1.97, 53.31)=8.89, p<0.001; ηp2=0.248) but no main effect of stimulation condition (F(2, 27)=2.9, p=0.068; ηp2=0.181). Importantly, the analysis revealed a significant two-way interaction between stimulation and training day on performance (F(3.94, 53.31)=4.37, p=0.004; ηp2=0.245), indicating the effects of training on performance depended on stimulation condition.

To further investigate the time-dependent impact of stimulation on training performance and to analyze at what time point the groups began to diverge, we evaluated the effect of group on each training day separately. Levene’s test was not significant for any training day (Training Day 1 p=0.778, Training Day 2 p=0.269, Training Day 3, p=0.742, and Training Day 4 p=0.297), indicating the assumption of homogeneity of variances was not violated. tRNS administered during training caused learning that was significantly different among stimulation groups from the third day of training (Training Day 3: F(2,27)=4.65, p=0.018; ηp2=0.257) and remained sustained on the final day of training (Training Day 4: F(2,27)=6.63, p=0.005; ηp2=0.330). Post-hoc pairwise t-tests revealed the Parietal group performed significantly better than the hMT+ group on Training Day 3 (t(18)=3.6, p=0.00; Cohen’s d=1.51) and approached significance when compared to Sham (t(18)=2.03, p=0.056, Cohen’s d=0.91), while hMT+ and Sham did not differ significantly (t(18)=0.9, p=0.37, Cohen’s d=−0.40). Similarly, on training Day 4, we found a significant difference in performance between the Parietal and the hMT+ groups (t(18)=3.6, p=0.002; Cohen’s d=1.61) and between Parietal and Sham (t(18)=3.102, p=.006); Cohen’s d=1.39, but not between hMT+ and Sham (t(18)=0.58, p=0.57; Cohen’s d=−0.26).

Finally, we analyzed performance of the Parietal group only during training to investigate whether performance increased across sessions (Training Days 1, 2, 3, and 4). We found that performance on training Day 1 was significantly different from performance of training Day 2 (t(9)=−3.34, p=0.009; Cohen’s d=−1.056), Day 3 (t(9)=−4.63, p=0.001; Cohen’s d=−1.46), and Day 4 (t(9)=−5.05, p=0.001; Cohen’s d=−1.59). These comparisons indicate that Parietal group performances on the OD task significantly increased on each stimulation training session relative to the first training session (Figure 2C).

Temporal order judgment

We ran the same analysis on training data for the TOJ task. Mauchly’s test for the mixed factor ANOVA indicated that the assumption of sphericity was violated (χ2 (5)=18.86, p=0.002); hence, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used to correct the degrees of freedom and to assess the significance of the corresponding p-values. There was no significant effect of training session on TOJ performance (F(1.8, 53.50)=2.707, p=0.076; ηp2=0.091), but there was a significant main effect of stimulation condition (F(2, 27)=4.79, p=0.017; ηp2=0.262). For the mean performance evaluated during the tRNS session, but notable not pre- and post-training (above), the Parietal stimulation group performance was significantly better than that of the Sham group (post hoc t-test: p=0.014; Bonferroni corrected; Cohen’s d=1.84), but not the hMT+ group (post hoc t-test: p=0.256, Bonferroni corrected; Cohen’s d=0.77), no significant difference was found also between Sham and hMT+ groups (post hoc t-test: p=0.618, Bonferroni corrected; Cohen’s d=0.50). We note, however, that there was no interaction between stimulation and training session on performance (F(3.96, 53.506)=2.277, p=0.073; ηp2=0.144), indicating that the tRNS did not preferentially benefit training for any stimulation group on this TOJ task (Figure 2D).

Stimulation and learning-dependent changes in functional connectivity

We next examined the effect of multi-session hf-tRNS and training on resting-state functional connectivity (rs-FC) of the DVAN, the functional network targeted by stimulation and training protocol.

To analyze the effect of stimulation and training on rs-FC of the DVAN, we first calculated the mean FC per subject within the DVAN by averaging the FC scores of 45 connectivity values obtained from a matrix derived for each subject for all the region of interest (ROI) pairs of the DVAN (see Materials and methods for details), we then averaged across subjects in each group (van den Heuvel et al., 2017; Nicolini et al., 2020).

To ensure that the number of data points remaining after time series were cleaned from motion did not bias the r-to-z transformations, we performed statistical analysis to test whether the number of censored frames was equivalent across conditions. A two-way mixed repeated measures ANOVA was performed to evaluate whether the number of censored data frames was affected by stimulation conditions and time (fMRI session). There was homogeneity of variances on both sessions (p>0.05) as assessed by Levene’s test. There was no statistically significant interaction between stimulation group and time on censored frames (F(2, 27)=1.604, p=0.220, ηp2=0.034). Finally, we found no main effect of condition (F(2, 27)=0.038, p=0.963; ηp2=0.003) nor session (F(1, 27)=0.619, p=0.438; ηp2=0.007) indicating that the mean number of censored data frames was equivalent across conditions and sessions.

We compared FC prior to stimulation between groups and found no significant difference (F(2, 27)=0.300, p=0.743; ηp2=0.22), indicating that FC did not differ between stimulation groups at baseline (pre-test session). A mixed repeated measures ANOVA testing the two factors of session (within-subjects factor, n=2) and stimulation site (between-subjects factor, n=3) on functional connectivity scores revealed no main effect of training on FC scores (F(1, 27)=0.001, p=0.986; ηp2=0.00) and a main effect of stimulation site (F(2, 27)=4.064, p=0.029; ηp2=0.231), indicating that the connectivity among the DVAN nodes changed as a function of where tRNS was applied. Importantly, the interaction between stimulation condition and session was found to be highly significant (F(2, 27)=11.507, p<0.001; ηp2=0.460), indicating that the change in rs-FC scores differed depending on the stimulation group (Figure 3) and this change was persistent the day following the last combined training and stimulation session.

n=3

Figure 3. Pre- and post-stimulation functional connectivity (FC) changes.

Figure 3.

Box plots for overall mean FC of the dorsal and ventral attention network averaged per stimulation condition (Parietal, hMT+, and Sham) and per fMRI session (pre- and post-test sessions). Asterisks indicate significant difference (**<0.01).

To detect whether FC was impacted by stimulation condition, we compared the rs-FC of the three stimulation groups after stimulation and training. One-way ANOVA analysis on post-test session revealed a significant effect of stimulation condition on FC scores (F(2, 27)=11.677, p<0.001; ηp2=0.464), which indicates that FC differs on this day between stimulation conditions (Figure 3). We then ran t-test comparisons on post-test FC scores to reveal simple effects between pairs of stimulation conditions. As depicted in Figure 3, we found that FC scores were significantly different in the Parietal group compared to the hMT+ (t(18)=3.863, p=0.002; Cohen’s d=1.73) and to the Sham group (t(18)=3.64, p=0.001; Cohen’s d=1.63), while FC did not differ significantly between Sham and hMT+ groups (t(18)=0.187, p=0.854; Cohen’s d=0.08). We further investigated the effect of stimulation on FC by comparing pre- and post-test FC values within each stimulation condition. Pairwise t-test comparisons revealed that FC scores were significantly different between the pre- and post-test sessions in the Parietal (t(9)=−2.973, p=0.016; Cohen’s d=−0.94) and the hMT+ groups (t(9)=−2.99, p=0.015; Cohen’s d=0.95), while they were not significantly different in the Sham group (t(9)=2.217, p=0.054; Cohen’s d=0.70). Rs-FC patterns increased within the main nodes of the DVAN after parietal stimulation only, while they decreased after hMT+ and Sham stimulation (Figure 3).

In a second set of analysis, we investigated the modulation of the DVAN by taking into consideration all ROIs pairs as single components of the network per subject (n=45 ROIs to ROIs pairs per subject), instead of using one averaged correlation value for the whole network as in the previous analysis. As for the previous analysis, we performed a one-way ANOVA to compare FC between groups prior to stimulation and found no significant difference (F(2, 1317.1)=1.556, p=0.211, Brown-Forsythe p-value and degrees of freedom corrected; ηp2=0.002), indicating that FC did not differ between stimulation groups at baseline (pre-test session). We next performed a mixed repeated measures ANOVA to test the effect of the stimulation site and session on all correlation values within the DVAN. This analysis revealed no main effect of session on FC scores (F(1, 1347)=.002, p=0.964; ηp2=0.00), but a main effect of stimulation group (F(1, 1347)=23.07, p≤0.001; ηp2=0.106). Importantly, the interaction between stimulation group and session was found to be strongly significant (F(2, 1347)=80.206, p<0.001; ηp2=0.033), which indicates a significant difference between rs-FC scores pre- and post-test sessions, depending on the stimulation condition (Figure 4).

Figure 4. Modulation of resting-state functional connectivity (FC) of the DVAN.

Figure 4.

Left Panel: z-scores frequency distribution pre- (light blue bars) and post-test (light red bars) multi-session hf-tRNS coupled with training per each stimulation condition (A. Parietal, B. hMT+, C. Sham). Dark red indicates the overlapping distribution between pre- and post-test sessions. Dotted red and blue lines indicated the mean for each z-scores distribution. Right Panel: Correlation matrices represent the result of the computed difference between FC scores pre- and post-test sessions (Δ=FC(S2)−FC(S1)) depicted by condition (A. Parietal, B. hMT+, C. Sham). Each correlation difference between ROI pairs was first calculated at subject level and then averaged across subjects (detailed information on ROIs are reported in the Materials and methods section). Colors bars indicate the strength and direction of correlation change values for each regions’ pair (red colors indicate higher connectivity; blue colors indicate lower connectivity). DVAN, dorsal and ventral attention network; ROI, region of interest; tRNS, transcranial random noise stimulation.

We then compared the rs-FC on all single components (ROIs-to-ROIs pairs) of the post-test session by performing a one-way ANOVA. Because Levene’s test indicated that the assumption of homogeneity of variances was violated, the Brown-Forsythe F-ratio is reported. This analysis revealed a strong significant effect of stimulation condition on post-test FC scores (F(2, 1286.64)=81.839, p≤0.00; ηp2=0.108), which indicates that FC was significantly different on this session among the three stimulation conditions (Figure 5). T-test comparisons on post-test FC scores revealed that connectivity was significantly increased in the Parietal group compared to the hMT+ (t(865.23) = 11.59, P =< .001, df corrected for inhomogeneity of variance; Cohen’s d = .773) and to the Sham group (t(892.6)=10.518, p<0.001, df corrected for inhomogeneity of variance; Cohen’s d=0.701). Post-test FC scores were not significantly different between the hMT+ and the Sham groups (t(837.68)=0.357, p=0.721, df corrected for inhomogeneity of variance; Cohen’s d=0.024).

Figure 5. Functional connectivity within the DVAN after multi-session hf-tRNS coupled with training (post-test session).

Figure 5.

Correlations of all ROI pairs (z-scores) constituting the DVAN are represented separately for the three stimulation conditions (Parietal, Sham, and hMT+). Asterisks represents significant differences (**p<0.01). DVAN, dorsal and ventral attention network; ROI, region of interest; tRNS, transcranial random noise stimulation.

In addition, we further investigated the modulation of the DVAN after stimulation and we computed the difference between FC scores pre- and post-test sessions (ΔFC=FC(S2)−FC(S1); Figure 4, right panel) to highlight the impact of stimulation on all ROIs-to-ROIs connections within the network. We run t-test comparisons on the delta FC scores between condition and we found scores were significantly different in the Parietal group compared to the hMT+ (t(890.801)=11.443, p≤0.001; Cohen’s d=0.763) and to the Sham group (t(879.469)=10.707, p≤0.001; Cohen’s d=0.714), while FC did not differ significantly between Sham and hMT+ groups (t(852.124)=0.594, p=0.552; Cohen’s d=0.040).

Figure 4 shows how connections between the nodes of the DVAN changed after Parietal stimulation resulting in stronger positive correlations within the network nodes, while the connections between the nodes after hMT+ and Sham stimulation resulted in a more negative correlation, indicating decreased connectivity .

Finally, to control for network-specific effects of stimulation, we also computed changes in rs-FC within the DMN which we hypothesized not to be affected by our stimulation protocol. This control analysis found no significant main effect of stimulation condition, nor interaction between stimulation condition and training. Specifically, in the first type of connectivity analysis when the network was taken as a whole (averaged connectivity representing the network), we found no effect of stimulation by site (F(2, 27)=0.09, p=0.91), nor by session (F(1, 27)=0.58, p=0.45), and no interaction between stimulation condition and session (F(2, 27)=0.20, p=0.81). We then compared the mean connectivity modulation measured within the DMN with the mean modulation measured within the DVAN, that is the difference in connectivity between the pre- and post-stimulation session calculated for each network (Figure 6). A two-way ANOVA was conducted to examine the effects of network and condition on the mean connectivity modulation measured within the DMN and the DVAN. This analysis revealed a statistically significant interaction between the effects of network and stimulation condition factors on FC modulation scores (F(2, 54)=4.97, p=0.010, ηp2=0.156), as well as a significant effect of the stimulation condition factor alone (F(2, 54)=7.937, p≤0.001; ηp2=0.227). The network factor alone was found to be not significant (F(1, 54)=0.274, p=0.603, ηp2=0.05). Simple main effects analysis showed that FC was modulated differently between conditions within the DVAN (p<0.001) but not within the DMN (p=0.836), and that FC modulation was different for the Parietal condition only (p=0.006), while it did not differ significantly for the Sham (p=0.256) nor the hMT+ conditions (p=0.418).

Figure 6. Functional connectivity (FC) changes between pre- and post-stimulation.

Figure 6.

Box plots for overall change in functional connectivity (calculated as the difference in connectivity between the post- and the pre-stimulation session) for the default mode network (A) and the dorsal ventral attention network (B) depicted per stimulation condition (Parietal, hMT+, and Sham). Gray dots within each box represent the mean change in FC per condition.

We also conducted a node-by-node FC analysis (all ROIs pairs within the network as single components of the network) and found no effect of session (F(1, 1347)=2.4, p=0.116), nor of stimulation site (F(2, 1347)=0.347, p=0.707). The interaction between stimulation group and session was also not significant (F(2, 1347)=0.853, p=0.426; see Appendix for further details and additional analysis). Figure 7 (left panel) shows how connections did not change within the DMN nodes as the mean of the z-score distribution for the pre- and post-test session is similar for all three groups and almost overlaps for the Parietal group. This is also evident from the correlation matrices (Figure 7, right panel) that represent the difference between FC scores pre- and post-test sessions for all ROIs pairs included in the network. The lack of connectivity modulation within the DMN and the specificity rs-FC modulation found after parietal stimulation only, provide strong evidence for a selective stimulation-induced effect by our stimulation and training protocol.

Figure 7. Modulation of resting-state functional connectivity of the DMN.

Figure 7.

Left Panel: z-scores frequency distribution pre- (light blue bars) and post- (light red bars) multi-session hf-tRNS coupled with training per each stimulation condition (A. Parietal, B. hMT+, C. Sham). Dark red indicates the overlapping distribution between pre- and post-sessions. Dotted red and blue lines indicated the mean for each z-scores distribution. Right Panel: Correlation matrices represent the result of the computed difference between FC scores pre- and post-test sessions (Δ=FC(S2)−FC(S1)) depicted by condition (A. Parietal, B. hMT+, C. Sham). Each correlation difference between ROI pairs was first calculated at subject level and then averaged across subjects. Colors bars indicate the strength and direction of correlation change values for each regions’ pair (red colors indicate higher connectivity; blue colors indicate lower connectivity). DMN, default mode network; ROI, region of interest; tRNS, transcranial random noise stimulation.

Functional connectivity-behavior correlation

Next, we examined the relationship between changes in behavioral performance and the resting-state FC within the DVAN. Only those subjects that were included in both behavioral and FC analysis were incorporated in the linear model analysis (a total of 26 subjects). We observed a number of positive correlations between FC changes and behavioral improvement in the OD task. In particular, to estimate the behavioral relevance of rs-FC, we correlated connectivity scores with two independent behavioral indices: one behavioral index measured during the scanning sessions (pre- and post-test sessions), and a second behavioral index measured during the Training sessions. We found subjects that displayed a higher performance improvement also tended to show higher rs-FC within the DVAN (measured as the mean of the correlations within the nodes of the DVAN) in the post-test session (r=0.44). The linear regression model revealed a significant regression coefficient (p=0.022, R-squared=0.2) which indicates that post-session functional connectivity scores are significantly associated with changes in behavioral scores in the final test session. We then examined whether there was a relationship between rs-FC and behavioral changes measured at the end of training (Training Day 1 relative to Training Day 4). We found that subjects with higher FC scores within the DVAN in the post-test MRI session showed a positive correlation with higher behavior improvement in the OD task achieved during training (r=0.48). The regression coefficient for this relationship was significant (p=0.012, R-squared=0.23) indicating that changes in behavioral performance during training are significantly correlated with post-test session connectivity scores. Taken together, these results indicate that post-test functional connectivity scores within the attention network significantly correlated with behavioral improvement measured not only immediately at the end of the stimulation and training protocol, but also 24 hr after the end of the stimulation protocol.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated the potential for visual training coupled with noninvasive brain stimulation to promote significant improvements in behavior and increase cortical functional connectivity. We trained subjects on cross-tasks training to optimize learning in an attention-based learning paradigm over multiple days. We combined this training with high frequency tRNS using a protocol optimized to facilitate cortical plasticity with the goal of increasing and speeding up training improvements relative to training without tRNS. Our stimulation protocol, when applied over parietal cortex, dramatically shortened the learning period, overcoming the limitations of previous training paradigms. Crucially, tRNS coupled with training also increased functional connectivity within the attention cortical network when delivered over parietal cortex, and the connectivity pattern changed more strongly for individuals with the largest behavioral improvement during and after training. Thus, we conclude that tRNS has the potential to strengthen task-relevant connections between sensory and attention systems to promote learning and plasticity.

Bilateral tRNS over the parietal cortex during training resulted in a strong boost in performance, while tRNS over hMT+ and Sham had no effect on behavior. Crucially, improvement in behavior in the parietal condition positively correlated with the increase in functional connectivity within the main nodes of the DVANs, indicating that concurrent training and stimulation modulated large-scale cortical dynamics, strengthening functionally selective neural pathways (Chiappini et al., 2018), a potential physiological marker of enduring changes (Polanía et al., 2011).

We also sought to understand whether a significant improvement could be achieved with a short training protocol, in conditions where we normally would not expect learning to occur (Huang et al., 2012; Aberg et al., 2009; Li et al., 2004). We adopted a cross-task training that uses the same visual stimulus for two different tasks (Szpiro et al., 2014), and we coupled the training with a stimulation protocol during the learning acquisition phase. While during the Sham and hMT+ control conditions no learning was achieved, our data suggest that it might be the combination of a challenging task with concurrent brain stimulation over the relevant parietal cortical circuits that facilitated fast learning, a paradigm amenable to promote plasticity (Herpich et al., 2019; Tyler et al., 2018; Cappelletti et al., 2013; Kiyonaga et al., 2021).

We next examined the time course of the stimulation effect upon behavior across the four short training sessions. tRNS administered on the IPS during training promoted fast learning, with performance in the parietal group significantly diverging from the control groups by the third day of training. Notably, the Parietal tRNS group displayed a build-up and cumulative trend of the facilitation effect, as accuracy increased on each training day and retained the beneficial effect acquired in the previous session. This facilitation effect was not only quantitatively different from the other groups, but also selective for one task (the OD visuospatial task), within the same group of participants. These data confirm the previously observed beneficial effect of tRNS over training (Cappelletti et al., 2013; Herpich et al., 2019). However, they further indicate that tRNS’s enhancing properties are mostly effective when stimulation is coupled with a relevant stimulus that maximizes the related cortical response, like in our OD task (Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000).

Taken together, these behavioral results suggest that learning of a visuospatial attention task was achieved within a few training sessions and that this facilitation was long lasting, as increased performance outlasted the end of stimulation for at least 24 hr, when the last fMRI session after the end of training was performed. These findings extend the results of a previous experiment that tested tRNS efficacy upon attention within one session, but found a rapidly decaying beneficial effect by the end of stimulation (Tyler et al., 2018). However, using a multi-session stimulation approach coupled with training promoted retention of the beneficial effect of training, as also shown by the results in increased rs-FC after training (Chen et al., 2015; Sarabi et al., 2018; Kang et al., 2018).

There might be several conditions that facilitate the emergence of learning, all likely driving the stimulus-related activity to reach the learning threshold (Seitz and Dinse, 2007). First, in this study, we demonstrate that stimulation site selection within a network is crucial to obtaining successful augmentation. Performance improved only when tRNS was delivered to the parietal cortex, and was absent in all other conditions, including stimulation to hMT+, often included as a node of the dorsal attention network. Thus, our results indicate the parietal lobes have a direct involvement in visual PL and attention processes (Giovannelli et al., 2010; Law and Gold, 2008; Shadlen and Newsome, 2001; Colby and Goldberg, 1999). Second, these results indicate that tRNS effects do not spread to regions in close anatomical proximity, but rather spread within strongly functionally connected circuits. Stimulation to hMT+, which is close to the parietal lobes, did not have any effects on behavior nor on FC. Third, our findings support the idea that learning is facilitated when the cortical processes underlying the task are stimulated while in an aroused state derived by task performance (Wright et al., 2010), and indicate the possibility to use tRNS to rapidly reach this optimal state. Importantly, our study further indicates the benefits of short but effective training sessions, an ideal condition to promote learning (Censor and Sagi, 2008; Censor et al., 2016).

One might ask why we observed learning occurring for one attention task only. Our results are consistent with previous studies that used a similar cross-task training paradigm and found behavioral improvement in only one of two tasks trained (Szpiro et al., 2014; Wright et al., 2010). One potential explanation for task selective improvement is that making two different perceptual judgments on the same visual stimulus may require tuning of different cortical representations (Li et al., 2004). Simultaneous training might induce competition between these two representations, ultimately leading to the enhancement of one and inhibition of the other, as suggested in the augment Hebbian reweighting model (AHRW; Dosher et al., 2013; Dosher and Lu, 2017). Similar to Szpiro et al.’ study, when our participants trained on two randomly interleaved tasks using identical visual stimuli, improvements were observed only in the OD, suggesting the training may have changed the perceptual weighting of the orientation channels.

In line with this hypothesis, our study showed that orientation PL was apparent with our very short training sessions only when coupled with tRNS over parietal cortex, further emphasizing the role of neuromodulation in speeding up the learning process. In the parietal stimulation group, we positioned the electrodes over the EEG locations P3 and P4 (Herwig et al., 2003; Okamoto et al., 2004), roughly corresponding to the posterior IPS. The IPS has been linked to visuospatial attention in several imaging and brain stimulation studies (Corbetta and Shulman, 2011; Battelli et al., 2009; Battelli et al., 2017), and the OD task involves the processing of visuospatial components. Spatial processing was not relevant in the TOJ condition and that is typically associated to more ventral areas (Chica et al., 2011; Agosta et al., 2017; Husain and Nachev, 2007). The temporo parietal junction (TPJ), at the crossroad between the ventral and the dorsal visual pathways, has been linked to temporal attention (Tyler et al., 2015: Battelli et al., 2007; Husain and Rorden, 2003). Although this explanation is rather speculative, the selectivity of the behavioral effect along with functional connectivity results might indicate that the IPS plays a pivotal role in OD across the visual space (Capotosto et al., 2013; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). Moreover, the parietal lobe has been associated with selective and gating attention mechanisms (Suzuki et al., 2013), and tRNS might have facilitated these gating mechanisms during learning. Interestingly, the analysis of the current distribution during stimulation shows that the effects driven by parietal stimulation are likely due to an effective targeting of selective task-active crucial nodes (Figure 8A and B; ). With a similar pattern of distributed activity, OD and TOJ functionally activated the same task-based cortical regions at baseline (Figure 8A, details for statistical analysis are reported in the ‘Task-evoked brain activity’ section). However, the behavioral gain was dramatically different between the two tasks. This clearly shows that stimulus-evoked functional activity occurring at individual brain regions is not informative about what regions are crucial for learning. As shown in previous studies, regions part of the same network may flexibly interact to perform and prioritize different tasks (Gratton et al., 2016). In our data, functional connectivity changes among cortical areas showed unique pattern of spontaneous response at rest, after parietal stimulation only, and tRNS might have enhanced these critical changes in connectivity among distributed brain regions during learning (Cole et al., 2013).

Figure 8. Task-related activity for the orientation discrimination (OD) task and electric field distribution after stimulation.

Figure 8.

Panel A: group GLM contrasting neural activity in OD and TOJ trials versus rest (see Task-evoked brain activity section). All regions are significant at q<0.05 (FDR corrected). Yellow and red (for the OD task) and yellow and purple (for the TOJ task) colors indicate significantly positive activation. Panel B: electric field distribution on the brain induced by Parietal stimulation. Panel C: electric field distribution on the brain induced by hMT stimulation. FDR, false discovery rate; GLM, general linear model; TOJ, temporal order judgment.

Finally, some studies have analyzed the importance of task engagement or difficulty during stimulation, and have demonstrated how these factors affect the behavioral outcome (Bortoletto et al., 2015; Hsu et al., 2016). In our study, participant engagement was challenged in two ways. First, task difficulty was individually tailored and equated for difficulty across subjects ensuring subjects’ engagement with the task (Waskom et al., 2019). Second, the experimental design randomly alternated between experimental tasks, forcing the continuous switch of attention between stimulus features, might force a strategy that favors one task due to limited available cognitive resources.

We next asked whether there were cortical physiological changes that indicate more sustained plastic changes along with behavioral change, and if such changes were within task-related well-established attention-related cortical networks. Specifically, we analyzed whether the multi-session tRNS had an effect on functional connectivity at rest in the DVAN, which we targeted through stimulation. Brain regions in this network mediate attention mechanisms and consequently shape the analysis of visual perceptual input (Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000). The dorsal/ventral attention network plays a pivotal role in attention functions and constitutes a large occipital-frontoparietal network that controls allocation of attention to positions and visual features in space (Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000; for a review see Fiebelkorn and Kastner, 2020: Vossel et al., 2014). Tasks requiring selective attention have been shown to activate the ventral part of the attention network (VAN; Corbetta et al., 2008; Vossel et al., 2012), while the dorsal portion of the attention network, in particular the IPS and the FEF regions, is active during many visuospatial tasks that require spatial updating and the selection of important regions of space (Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000; Corbetta et al., 1998; Merriam et al., 2003; Greenberg et al., 2010).

Consistent with the relevance of both ventral and dorsal streams of the attention network for the performance of the task used in this experiment, our results show increased functional connectivity within the entire network. Almost all connections between the node pairs displayed higher connectivity after multi-session parietal stimulation only. This result indicates that bilateral hf-tRNS over the parietal lobes modulated the whole attention-related network, instead of producing local changes alone. Importantly, the neural modulation was network-specific, not a propagation of stimulation across neighboring cortical regions or networks. The control analysis we conducted on the DMN showed no changes in connectivity following stimulation and training, indicating that our stimulation-induced changes were selective for the DVAN. Moreover, the node-selective effect within the DVAN was demonstrated in the hMT+ stimulation group, where the neighboring IPS was not modulated by hMT+ stimulation. Similar to our results, previous physiological studies on non-human primates found changes in the lateral intraparietal area but not in the MT area suggesting that PL requires the involvement of higher areas that induce changes in how the sensory information is interpreted to drive behavior (Law and Gold, 2008). The lack of improvement in the hMT+ stimulation condition might indicate that while this cortical area might be involved in attentional processing (Yates et al., 2017; Yao et al., 2016), for other functions involved in learning, such as decision processing, the parietal lobe plays a more pivotal role (Herrington and Assad, 2010; Zhou and Freedman, 2019). Our results are also consistent with previous research that found persistent network dynamics changes following other types of stimulation (e.g., transcranial magnetic stimulation, TMS; Battelli et al., 2017; Ruff et al., 2006; Ruff et al., 2008; Lee and D’Esposito, 2012), and points to the possibility to use our neuromodulation protocol to exert long lasting and distal effects. Finally, one potential limitation of our study is the relatively small sample size, given all the conditions we tested. As previously pointed out, brain-behavioral correlations can produce inflated effect sizes with the combination of relatively small sample size and stringent alpha-correlations (Yarkoni, 2009).

Functional connectivity changes following parietal stimulation are likely to have affected behavior and driven the system to better respond to the demands of the attention task. Rapidly evolving changes in connectivity between interconnected regions have been proposed as the key mechanism that allows large-scale networks to respond to task demands (Buschman and Kastner, 2015). In the context of tRNS, we speculate that a fast modulation of the network connectivity can be due to the temporal summation of neural activity, as well as to the offline stochastic effect of tRNS (Chaieb et al., 2015). In fact, the stochastic resonance mechanism has been proposed to lead to higher synchrony of oscillations between neurons across large-scale functional systems, which in turn creates strong links between firing neurons (van der Groen and Wenderoth, 2016; van der Groen et al., 2018; Schwarzkopf et al., 2011; McDonnell et al., 2009; McDonnell and Ward, 2011). The strong correlation found between improved performance and increased FC suggests the pivotal role of the IPS in network mediating these system effects during PL (Szczepanski and Kastner, 2013).

Conclusions

This multi-method study investigates the potential long-term benefits of tRNS on cortical plasticity and its ability to efficiently alter cortical networks to promote attention and visual PL. Our neuroimaging and behavioral results support the idea that the modulation of task-related neural networks induced by tRNS can efficiently promote behavioral improvements and speed up the emergence of learning. Overall, this is the first study demonstrating the sustained and selective nature by which tRNS operates on cortical dynamics, and consequently opens a critical window during which the cortex might be more plastic and responsive to reorganization needs to promote learning. Clinical and healthy populations could significantly benefit from the shortened learning time afforded by the combination of tRNS with behavioral learning.

Materials and methods

Regulatory approval

The study was approved by the ethical committee of the University of Trento.

Participants

Thirty-seven neurologically healthy subjects (20 females, mean age 22.8 years old), with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in the study. All subjects were screened for medical contra-indications to MRI and brain stimulation and received monetary compensation for their participation in the experiment.

Study design

Participants were randomly assigned to one of three stimulation conditions: (1) bilateral tRNS over parietal cortex (Parietal group), (2) bilateral tRNS over middle temporal cortex, hMT+ area (hMT+ group), and (3) Sham stimulation (Sham group). Seven subjects were excluded from the analysis due to head motion during one or both scanning procedures or due to inadequate behavioral performance (i.e., performance was at chance in catch trials), leaving 30 participants for behavioral and FC analysis.

Subjects participated in a multi-session experiment that lasted 7 days (one session per day; Figure 1A). On thresholding session, they completed a staircase procedure to measure their psychophysical thresholds for two separate attention tasks: a TOJ and an OD task (described below). On pre-test session, resting-state and the attention task performance measures were taken in the MRI, with trials (from both tasks) randomly interleaved within each block. Participants completed a total of five blocks per task (see Neuroimaging procedure section for details). On training and stimulation sessions (Training Day 1, Training Day 2, Training Day 3, and Training Day 4), subjects received 25 min hf-tRNS or sham while training on the same TOJ and OD tasks, with the two trial types randomly interleaved (cross-tasks training), as in the pre-test (five blocks per day). Post-test session was a repeat of pre-test session.

Stimuli

Stimuli consisted of a pair of sine-wave gratings (Gabors) positioned 4° to the left and right of a central fixation cross. The Gabors were presented with a spatial frequency of 3 cycles/deg and reduced 50% contrast against a uniform gray background (47.5 cd/m2), with a fixation cross positioned in the center of the screen for the entire duration of the trial. The two Gabors were presented with temporal offset asynchronies (for the TOJ task) that corresponded to the individual temporal threshold described in detail below. In addition to the temporal offset, one of the two discs was tilted along the vertical line with a degree that corresponded to the individual orientation threshold calculated for the OD task at baseline. In the TOJ task, participants were asked to judge whether two Gabors were presented simultaneously or not, while in the OD task, participants were asked to determine whether the two Gabors had the same or different orientation. Throughout the experiment, subjects always viewed stimuli at their individual thresholds (except during catch trials where stimuli were presented above threshold) for both dimensions (time and orientation); however, on any given trial, they had to attend to only one of the features, as instructed by the pre-cue word. The target feature (or ‘test Gabor’) was presented for equal number of trials (120 trials per trial type, 240 in total per session) on the left and right visual field, within each block per task. Easy (above threshold) catch-trials were also interleaved randomly within each block (n=6, 24 catch trials per session) to check that participants remained engaged and were performing the cross-attention task correctly. In the Easy trials, the two Gabors were presented with large offset asynchronies (≈150 ms, for the TOJ condition), or with a large orientation difference (≈20°, for the OD condition). Importantly, in both tasks, visual stimuli were presented with temporal offsets (TOJ) and orientation values (OD) fixed at individual threshold levels, which were calculated on thresholding session.

During training days, stimuli were displayed on a 22-in. LCD monitor with a 60 Hz refresh rate controlled by a DELL computer equipped with Matlab r2016a (The MathWorks, Natick, MA) and Psychtoolbox 3.0.8 (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). Participants were seated 57 cm from the screen in a dark and quiet room, and used a chin rest to ensure consistent positioning. During the fMRI sessions (pre- and post-test sessions), stimuli were displayed on a Nordic NeuroLab LCD monitor (Basic monitor specs include: 878 mm horizontal × 485 mm vertical; 3840×2160 pixels) connected to a Windows PC equipped with the Matlab r2016a and Psychtoolbox 3.0.8, and stimuli were back projected through a mirror inside the scanner.

Apparatus and procedure

On thresholding session (Figure 1A, green section), subjects performed two two-alternative forced-choice tasks within each block: (1) a TOJ or (2) an OD task. Two 3–1 staircase procedures were used to assess thresholds values for the two tasks (TOJ and OD), separately. A single incorrect response decreased task difficulty, while three consecutive correct responses increased task difficulty. The staircase terminated after 30 reversals of the staircase parameter. Temporal offsets and orientation values in the remaining sessions were calibrated based on the thresholds to yield 50% accuracy in the same/different judgments. The threshold corresponded to the point of subjective equality, which represents the threshold value at which the observer experiences two stimuli as identical. The estimated threshold was then used for the last 15 trials and it was considered accurate if subjects performed at 50% on these trials, the expected performance. 50% accuracy was chosen to prevent ceiling effects, thus allowing PL to occur.

On the pre-test, post-test, and training sessions, participants performed the PL cross-tasks procedure. Each trial started with a 2-s instruction interval during which a cue-word indicated which of the two tasks the subjects should perform in the upcoming trial: ‘Time’ instructed the subject to perform the TOJ task (120 trials), and ‘Orientation’ instructed the subject to perform the OD task (120 trials). After the pre-cue, stimuli were presented for 500 ms. Subjects were then prompted to make a forced-choice judgment to indicate whether the two stimuli had same/different orientation (OD task) or if they were presented at the same/different time (TOJ task), depending on the task. During the response interval (1.5 s), a cue-word was presented to remind subjects which judgment they had to report. A fixation interval of 2 or 4 s terminated the trial (trial example, Figure 1D).

Stimulation protocol

Before testing, all participants were provided with a short introduction to brain stimulation and safety information. After each participant was briefed, they completed a stimulation safety questionnaire and signed the informed consent. At this time, any participant deemed ineligible for stimulation or MRI procedures was excluded from the experiment. A battery-driven stimulator (DC-Stimulator, NeuroConn, Ilmenau, Germany) was used for electrical stimulation. For the two active tRNS conditions (Parietal and hMT+), 2 mA current was applied for 25 consecutive min with random alternating frequency delivered at a high frequency range between 101 and 640 Hz. Stimulation was delivered with a fade in/out period of 20 s at the beginning and at the end of each stimulation session. For Sham stimulation, the machine was turned off after the fade-in phase. Two rubber electrodes (size=5×7 cm2), contained in sponges soaked in saline solution, were placed on the subject’s head and were kept fixed on the stimulation sites with a rubber band and a head cap. Stimulation sites were identified using the International electroencephalographic 10/20 system for scalp electrode localization. The center of the electrode was placed bilaterally over PO7/PO8 (left and right, respectively) for hMT+ and bilaterally over P3/P4 for parietal and sham conditions (Figure 1B). Participants reported no noticeable sensation resulting from tRNS (see also Ambrus et al., 2010). Modeling of the electric field following tRNS was performed with free ROAST software package (Huang et al., 2019; Huang et al., 2016) using the standard MNI-152 template (ICBM project) and visualized separately for the two active stimulation groups (Figure 8B and C). To evaluate whether the electric fields peaks induced by the two stimulation conditions targeted the same brain regions activated by the behavioral tasks, we analyzed BOLD signal changes in response to the OD and the TOJ task in the pre-stimulation session (details for statistical analysis are reported in the Task-Evoked brain activity section). Figure 8 includes the results of a second-level deconvolution general linear model (GLM) analysis for significant positive BOLD response for the OD and TOJ tasks versus rest (Figure 8A), together with the simulation of the current distribution induced by the two stimulation conditions to qualitatively assess the overlap between positive BOLD response for the two tasks and the current distribution peaks over the cortex.

Neuroimaging procedure

Whole-brain scanning was performed with a 4 T Bruker MedSpec MRI scanner using an 8-channel head-coil at the Center for Mind and Brain Sciences of the University of Trento, Italy. High-resolution T1-weighted images were acquired for each subject at the beginning of each fMRI session (MP-RAGE 1×1×1 mm3 voxel size, 176 sagittal slices). Functional images (T2*-weighted EPIs, TR=2.0 s, TE=28.0, flip angle=73∘, 3×3×3 mm3 voxel size, 0.99 mm gap, 30 axial slices acquired interleaved, 192 mm FOV) were collected, for a total of 120 volumes per resting-state functional run and 185 volumes per task-based functional run.

The entire session consisted of one anatomical run followed by six functional runs (one resting-state run, five task runs collected on the same day), using the following procedure: first, a high-resolution anatomical image was acquired, followed by three functional task runs, one resting-state functional run, and two additional functional runs (same order on each session). The resting-state functional run was set in-between the task-based functional runs to allow participants to rest during the fMRI session that overall lasted about 1 hr.

During MRI data collection, subjects viewed the stimuli through a periscope mirror mounted on the MR head-coil that allowed the subject to view a screen positioned at the head of the scanner. Throughout the experiment, participants were instructed to maintain fixation on a constantly present central fixation cross. Participants were instructed to indicate the response with an MR compatible response box (two double-buttons response pads) with left index finger indicating ‘same’ and right index finger indicating ‘different’ responses.

Data analysis

Behavioral data analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB (The MathWorks, Natick, MA) and SPSS (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY), while figures were made using both MATLAB and R toolbox (ggplot2, CRAN; Patil, 2018). We first compared behavioral data from pre- and post-test days (see ‘Magnitude of learning’ section), the two sessions during which fMRI data were also collected, while data from the training and stimulation phase (Training Day 1, Training Day 2, Training Day 3, and Training Day 4) are presented separately (see ‘Time course of learning’ section). Accuracy scores were calculated as a proportion of correct responses from the recorded responses on each day. Data are analyzed and depicted for each task (OD and TOJ) and each analysis (pre- vs. post-test analysis, and training analysis) separately.

For pre- and post-test data analysis, we first checked for normality and homogeneity using Levene’s test and corrected with Brown-Forsythe correction when necessary. We performed two mixed repeated measures ANOVA to determine the effect of stimulation condition on performance in the two attention tasks across sessions. The within-subjects factor was Day (n=2 for pre-post stimulation analysis) and the between-subjects factor was Stimulation condition (n=3). For the pre- and post-test analysis, we tested whether the three stimulation groups differed at pre-test (baseline) and post-test sessions (post-stimulation and training session). Pairwise comparisons were performed to compare pre- and post-test performance for each group. We further analyzed data by calculating the total improvement as the difference between the post- and pre-test performance, thus obtaining the delta value for each stimulation condition (ΔPerformance|Improvement=Post-Test Performance – Pre-Test Performance). We then performed a one-way ANOVA to test whether there was a significant difference between the performance improvements (delta values) between stimulation conditions, and run post hoc pairwise comparisons to reveal differences in improvement between groups. Next, we analyzed data from the training and stimulation phase for the two tasks separately. Specifically, to measure changes in performance over time, we performed a mixed repeated measures ANOVA with stimulation condition (n=3) as between-subjects factor and Training Day (Time, n=4) as within-subjects factor. Mauchly’s test was used to evaluate the assumption of sphericity, and, when necessary, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used to correct degrees of freedom and to assess the significance of the corresponding p-values, while Levene’s test was used to address the assumption of equality of variances. Following the significance found in the mixed repeated measures ANOVA, we then compared performance between stimulation groups on each training day, and run post hoc pairwise comparisons to reveal simple differences between stimulation groups and training days performances.

Resting-state fMRI data analysis

We analyzed resting-state data collected in each fMRI session (pre-post sessions). Pre-processing of functional scans was initially performed using BrainVoyagerQX (Brain Innovations Inc, Maastricht, The Netherlands) and included the elimination of the first four volumes due to non-steady-state magnetization. Images were subsequently subjected to 3D motion correction, slice-timing correction, realignment, and spatial smoothing with a 6-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. Runs with instantaneous head motion exceeding 3 mm in any given dimension were discarded. The functional scans were manually co-registered to each individual’s high-resolution anatomical scan, then normalized into standardized Talairach coordinates (Laitinen, 1989).

Advanced pre-processing of the functional data from targeted ROIs was performed in MATLAB (The Math Works) to remove data contamination caused by motion, which can alter functional connectivity scores. This pipeline included nuisance regression using 12 motion estimate regressors (the six head realignment parameters obtained by rigid body head motion correction and their first-order derivatives) and estimates of global signal obtained from the ventricles. The ROI time series were then detrended and volumes with movement-related activity were removed (scrubbing) and temporarily replaced with resampled volumes (computed using LSSD Fit) in order to apply band-pass filtering (0.009–0.08 Hz;). The censored and resampled volumes were then removed from further analysis. After these processing steps were applied, only those runs that retained at least 3 min worth of data were used for further analyses (Satterthwaite et al., 2013). Statistical analyses were performed to ensure that the number of censored frames was equivalent across conditions and sessions.

We computed functional connectivity on resting-state data using 10 bilateral ROIs included in the DVAN: the IPS, hMT+, and frontal eye fields (FEFs) of the dorsal network; and the temporal parietal junction (TPJ) and ventral frontal cortex (VFC) of the ventral network. These regions were identified using standardized mean coordinates (Table 1) derived from the literature (Battelli et al., 2017; Vossel et al., 2014; Corbetta et al., 2008; Fox et al., 2006). Functional data from all voxels within a 6-mm radius sphere from the center coordinate were averaged into a single time series, keeping the ROIs similar in size across participants. The time courses were extracted from each ROI in the resting-state run and functional connectivity scores were computed as the Pearson’s r correlation coefficient. Correlation coefficients were Fisher-z transformed prior to subsequent statistical analyses.

Table 1. Brain coordinates.

Group mean Talairach X, Y, and Z coordinates for the centroid of each region of interest (left and right hemisphere) for the DVAN.

ROIs standardized coordinates
Left Hemisphere Right Hemisphere
X Y Z X Y Z
DVAN IPS –26 –63 48 25 –60 48
hMT+ –44 –67 3 44 –71 6
FEF –30 –5 48 30 –5 48
TPJ –48 –54 26 48 –54 21
VFC –42 12 –1 40 17 4

The Pearson correlations (r scores) among all the ROIs embedded in the attention network produced a connectivity matrix for each subject and each session (pre-post session). In total, there were 45 unique connectivity values among the 10 ROIs (10*(10−1)/2) for each correlation matrix. Z-transformed correlations were used to compute several measures of functional connectivity.

In the first set of analysis, we analyzed whether stimulation-induced connectivity changes at a whole-network level by examining connectivity scores that were the result of averaged correlation values among all connections between ROIs (van den Heuvel et al., 2017; Nicolini et al., 2020). Hence, these analyses examined one connectivity score representing the overall connectivity trend of the entire network. To test the effect of training and stimulation site, functional connectivity scores were subjected to a mixed repeated measures ANOVA with the within-subject factor session (n=2) and between-subject factor of stimulation site (n=3). We also compared rs-FC at each time point (pre- and post-test sessions) by performing two one-way ANOVAs, one per session. Following significance found in the post-test session only, post hoc pairwise comparisons on post-stimulation connectivity scores were run to reveal simple effects between pairs of stimulation conditions. We then run the second set of analysis to examine whether all ROI-to-ROI patterns within the network reliably contribute to the overall condition-related differences in FC, and to evaluate whether there still was a significant change in connectivity modulation when taking into consideration the contribution given by each pair of ROIs embedded in the network. We therefore adopted a different approach to analyze connectivity within the attention network where each ROI-to-ROI contribution (measures of connectivity level between two pair of ROIs as z-scores) to the network state connectivity is considered as a single observation of the attention network. We derived correlation coefficients within-subjects first, and then averaged across subjects obtaining mean z-scores per group, to ensure that changes in connectivity modulation were present in most individual subjects rather than derived from a group average (Varangis et al., 2019; Bellana et al., 2017; for further discussion about differences between within-subjects versus group-averaged approaches, see Roberts et al., 2016). In these analyses, we first performed a one-way ANOVA to compare connectivity scores between stimulation groups prior to stimulation (at baseline). We next tested the effect of stimulation coupled with training on functional connectivity by performing a mixed repeated measure ANOVA to test the effect of stimulation site and session on all correlation values within the network. We then compared post-test connectivity scores of all network components by performing a one-way ANOVA, followed by post hoc pairwise comparisons to reveal differences between groups. We further analyzed stimulation-induced changes in connectivity by calculating the difference between functional connectivity scores in the first and last MRI sessions per each ROI pair, on individual subjects (Δ rs-FC=rs-FC(S2)–rs-FC(S1)). These measures indicate the stimulation-induced changes in connectivity, normalized to baseline.

Throughout analysis, Levene’s test was used to test for normality and homogeneity of variance, and Brown-Forsythe correction was used to correct the F-ratio and degrees of freedom when necessary. Figures were made using MATLAB and R (ggplot2, CRAN; Patil, 2018).

Finally, to explore the relationship between functional connectivity within the DVAN and behavioral performance, we computed the linear regression between using two independent individual measures of behavioral improvement (accuracy changes pre-post stimulation and accuracy changes during training) against individual measures of stimulation-induced changes in rs-FC. Only those subjects that were included in both behavioral and FC analysis were incorporated in the linear model analysis (a total of 26 subjects per each linear regression model). Using Pearson’s correlation coefficient, we hence calculated the correlation between behavioral changes and brain activity modulation.

We also computed functional connectivity on resting-state data of 10 DMN ROIs defined based on previous imaging studies (Buckner et al., 2008; Buckner et al., 2008; Crittenden et al., 2015; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010): the ventral medial prefrontal cortex, the dorsal prefrontal cortex, the left posterior cingulate cortex, the left lateral temporal cortex, the bilateral inferior parietal lobe (IPL), the bilateral parahippocampal cortex, and bilateral hippocampal formation (HF+). Coordinates in MNI space were mapped onto Talairach space using the ‘MNI-to-Talairach converter with Brodmann Areas tool’ included in the Yale BioImage Suite Package (Papademetris et al., 2005; Papademetris et al., 2005). We followed the same procedure used for the DVAN analysis to extract the time series within each ROIs, the computation of Pearson’s r correlation coefficient and subsequent statistical analysis as for the DVAN. Finally, we compared the mean change in FC measured within the DVAN and the DMN. A two-way ANOVA was conducted with independent factors being network effects of stimulation condition and network on FC modulation to evaluate whether there was a difference in FC modulation within the two networks depending on stimulation conditions.

Task-evoked brain activity

We analyzed BOLD signal changes in response to the OD and the TOJ task in the pre-stimulation session, before any manipulation (e.g., stimulation and training) could potentially change the brain response to the task. Therefore, we included all subjects in the analysis within one group (N=30, 2 subjects were excluded due to data motion contamination recorded during task-runs; leaving 28 subjects) and we observed the brain areas significantly activated while performing the two tasks. Pre-processing of task-related functional scans was performed using BrainVoyagerQX (Brain Innovations Inc, Maastricht, The Netherlands) and included the elimination of the first six volumes due to non-steady-state magnetization effects. Functional data were motion-corrected (3D motion correction) and adjusted for temporal fluctuation in intensity over time (linear-trend removal and mean intensity adjustment). Runs with head motion exceeding 3 mm in any given dimension and direction were discarded. Functional scans were initially co-registered using automatic BV procedures and then manually adjusted to each individual’s high-resolution anatomical scan, then normalized into standardized Talairach coordinates (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988). BOLD activation was analyzed using a deconvolution GLM that included predictors for each volume (sampled at 2 s), each task-condition, extending 20 s following the onset of a trial. Statistical contrasts were estimated from the four volumes surrounding the peak of the hemodynamic response (4 s post-stimulus onset) for each voxel. In the first-level analysis, a GLM regression model was performed to fit data for each subject. Individual subject data were then combined into a deconvolution GLM in which beta estimates on OD and TOJ trials were contrasted with estimates for baseline activity. This contrast identified brain areas associated with OD task performance and TOJ performance, with statistical maps significance thresholded with a false discovery rate of 0.05 (FDR corrected). Figure 8A shows the results of the second-level deconvolution GLM analysis for significant BOLD response for the OD and TOJ tasks. The positive brain activations evoked by the two tasks largely overlap, with significant activity peaks over the parietal lobes (bilateral superior and IPLs), and on more anterior areas of the brain (including FEFs bilateral areas). Details of the statistical maps with Talairach coordinates are reported in the table below (Table 2 for OD and Table 3 for the TOJ contrast, respectively). The Talairach daemon (talairach.org) was used to identify the associated anatomical/functional brain regions and relative Broadmann areas (Lancaster et al., 2000; Lancaster et al., 1997).

Table 2. Clusters peak activation.

Peak activation coordinates of significant voxel clusters for the OD task are reported in Talairach space with relative number of voxels included in each cluster. Talairach Client was used to identy related anatomical/functional brain location and associated Broadmann Areas.

OD
Cluster X coor Y coor Z coor No. of Voxels Hemisphere Brain area Broadmann area
1 29 –44 35 10,156 Right cerebrum Parietal lobe VisuoMotor area Broadmann area 7
2 47 1 21 714 Right cerebrum Frontal lobe Inferior frontal gyrus Brodmann area 9
3 35 –59 –24 4067 Right cerebellum Cerebellum Culmen *
4 35 37 21 2337 Right cerebrum Frontal lobe Middle frontal gyrus Brodmann area 10
5 26 –5 51 1203 Right cerebrum Frontal lobe Middle frontal gyrus Brodmann area 6
6 11 –29 15 862 Right cerebrum Sub-lobar Thalamus Pulvinar
7 –4 7 48 6029 Left cerebrum Frontal lobe Superior frontal gyrus Brodmann area 6
8 –43 –50 –15 11,400 Left cerebrum Temporal lobe Fusiform gyrus Brodmann area 37
9 –16 –5 18 470 Left cerebrum Sub-lobar Caudate Caudate body
10 –40 –8 39 19,818 Left cerebrum Frontal lobe Precentral gyrus Brodmann area 6
11 –40 –38 30 21,367 Left cerebrum Parietal lobe Supramarginal gyrus Brodmann area 40
12 –31 13 9 569 Left cerebrum Sub-lobar Insula Brodmann area 13
13 –58 –17 18 327 Left cerebrum Parietal lobe Postcentral gyrus Brodmann area 43
Table 3. Peak activation coordinates of significant voxel clusters and the TOJ task are reported in Talairach space with relative number of voxels included in each cluster.

Talairach Client was used to identy related anatomical/functional brain location and associated Broadmann areas.

TOJ
Cluster X coor Y coor Z coor Hemisphere Brain area Broadmann area
1 47 1 21 1499 Right cerebrum Frontal lobe Inferior frontal gyrus Brodmann area 9
2 29 –47 33 8339 Right cerebrum Parietal lobe VisuoMotor area Broadmann area 7
3 29 –65 –27 2278 Right cerebellum Posterior lobe Pyramis *
4 35 37 21 2564 Right cerebrum Frontal lobe Middle frontal gyrus Brodmann area 10
5 11 –29 15 359 Right cerebrum Sub-lobar Thalamus Pulvinar
6 –7 7 48 6975 Left cerebrum Frontal lobe Medial frontal gyrus Brodmann area 6
7 –19 –41 6 6430 Left cerebrum Limbic lobe Parahippocampal gyrus Brodmann area 30
8 –40 –8 39 23,304 Left cerebrum Frontal lobe Precentral gyrus Brodmann area 6
9 –43 –41 33 19,411 Left cerebrum Parietal lobe Supramarginal gyrus Brodmann area 40

Acknowledgements

The present study was funded by the Autonomous Province of Trento, Call 'Grandi Progetti 2012,' project 'Characterizing and improving brain mechanisms of attention – ATTEND' (FC, LB) and The Blavatnik Family Foundation (Blavatnik Sensory Disorders Research Award) to LB; EG was supported by the National Science Foundation BCS0748314. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions, or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.

Appendix 1

Resting-state default mode network FC analysis

We examined whether the effect of multi-session hf-tRNS and training on resting-state functional connectivity (rs-FC) was specific for the targeted DVAN by analyzing changes in the connectivity of a different network, the default mode network (DMN). This network comprises a series of large regions distributed on the cortex and shows increased activity levels during rest or when the subjects are not involved in an active goal-oriented state (Raichle et al., 2001; Greicius et al., 2003; Buckner et al., 2008; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010; Braga and Buckner, 2017). We hypothesized that functional connectivity within the DMN would not be modulated by the stimulation protocol we used.

To test whether stimulation coupled with training had an effect on this control network, we run the same analysis performed for the DVAN. First, the mean connectivity per subject within the DMN was calculated by averaging the FC scores of the 45 pairs, and then averaged across subjects in each group. Next, we analyzed whether there was an effect of stimulation on FC scores of this network by performing a mixed repeated measures ANOVA with fMRI session (n=2; pre- vs. post-stimulation session) as within and stimulation site as between-subjects factor (n=3; Parietal, hMT+, and Sham). This analysis did not show any significant effect of stimulation site (F(2,27)=0.09, p=0.91), nor of time (F(1,27)=058, p=0.45). Importantly, the interaction between stimulation condition and session was not significant (F(2,27)=0.20, p=0.81), indicating that the rs-FC scores within the DMN did not change over time depending on the stimulation group (Appendix 1—figure 1).

Appendix 1—figure 1. Pre- and post-stimulation functional connectivity (FC) changes within the default mode network (DMN).

Appendix 1—figure 1.

Overall mean FC of the DMN averaged per stimulation condition (Parietal, hMT+, and Sham) and per fMRI session (pre- and post-stimulation sessions).

To further test whether FC within the DMN was affected by stimulation condition, we compared the rs-FC of the three stimulation groups at each time point (pre- and post-stimulation day) by performing two one-way ANOVAs, one per session (pre- and post-stimulation session). These analyses found no significant difference in connectivity between stimulation groups in the pre-stimulation session (F(2, 27)=0.01, p=0.82) nor in the post-stimulation sessions (F(2, 27)=0.10, p=0.90), indicating that connectivity scores within this network did not differ at baseline (prior to stimulation) and, importantly, did not change after the stimulation sessions.

We performed a further analysis to test whether stimulation affected the connectivity modulation within the DMN. As performed for the DVAN, we calculated the difference in FC scores between the pre- and post-stimulation sessions to measure the delta scores (modulation connectivity) for each condition. A one-way ANOVA on delta FC scores showed there was no significant difference between stimulation conditions (F(2, 27)=0.20, p=0.81).

We then run the second set of control analysis to investigate the modulation of the DMN by taking into consideration all ROIs pairs as single components of the network per subject (n=45 ROIs to ROIs pairs per subject), instead of using one averaged correlation value for the whole network as in the previous analysis (as performed for the DVAN). We first performed a one-way ANOVA to compare FC between groups prior to stimulation and we found no significant difference (F(2, 1298.682)=0.700, p=0.497, Brown-Forsythe p-value and degrees of freedom corrected), indicating that FC did not differ between stimulation groups at baseline (pre-stimulation session). We then performed a mixed repeated measures ANOVA to test the effect of the stimulation site and session on all correlation values within the DMN. This analysis revealed no main effect of session on FC scores (F(1, 1347)=2.4, p=0.116), nor of stimulation group (F(2, 1347)=0.347, p=0.707). Importantly, the interaction between stimulation group and session was also not significant (F(2, 1347)=0.853, p=0.426), indicating the absence of a significant difference between rs-FC scores on the two sessions depending on the stimulation condition.

Finally, we compared rs-FC on all single components (pairs) of the post-test stimulation DMN by performing a one-way ANOVA. Because Levene’s test indicated that the assumption of homogeneity of variances was violated, the Brown-Forsythe F-ratio is reported. This analysis revealed non-significant effect of stimulation condition on post-stimulation FC scores (F(2, 1297.89)=0.437, p=0.646), which indicates that FC did not differ between groups on this session (Appendix 1—figure 2).

Appendix 1—figure 2. Functional connectivity within the DMN after multi-session hf-tRNS coupled with training (post-stimulation session).

Appendix 1—figure 2.

Correlations of all ROI pairs (z-scores) constituting the DMN are represented separately for the three stimulation conditions (Parietal, Sham, and hMT+). No differences were found between stimulation conditions. DMN, default mode network; ROI, region of interest; tRNS, transcranial random noise stimulation

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Federica Contò, Email: Federica.Conto@iit.it.

Taraz Lee, University of Michigan, United States.

Richard B Ivry, University of California, Berkeley, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Provincia Autonoma di Trento Grandi Progetti to Lorella Battelli.

  • National Science Foundation BCS0748314 to Emily Grossman.

  • Blavatnik Family Foundation Blavatnik Sensory Disorders Research Award to Lorella Battelli.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology.

Investigation.

Data curation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review and editing.

Ethics

Human subjects: The study was approved by the ethical committee of the University of Trento.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data generated and analyzed during this study are available for review at the links reported in the Transparent Reporting File (Conto_transparent_reporting_resubmission). Moreover, a data structure explanation has also been provided (file name: Conto;_DataStuctureExplanation_resubmission). Data have been deposited to Zenodo and are available at the following DOIs: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4634008 https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5558975, https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4621644.

The following dataset was generated:

Contò F, Edwards G, Tyler S, Parrott D, Grossman ED, Battelli L. 2021. Behavioral data collected during pre_post sessions. Zenodo.

Contò F, Edwards G, Tyler S, Parrott D, Grossman ED, Battelli L. 2021. Behavioral data collected during training. Zenodo.

Contò F, Edwards G, Tyler S, Parrott D, Grossman ED, Battelli L. 2021. fMRI data collected during prepost sessions. Zenodo.

References

  1. Aberg KC, Tartaglia EM, Herzog MH. Perceptual learning with Chevrons requires a minimal number of trials, transfers to untrained directions, but does not require sleep. Vision Research. 2009;49:2087–2094. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agosta S, Magnago D, Tyler S, Grossman E, Galante E, Ferraro F, Mazzini N, Miceli G, Battelli L. The Pivotal Role of the Right Parietal Lobe in Temporal Attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2017;29:805–815. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ambrus GG, Paulus W, Antal A. Cutaneous perception thresholds of electrical stimulation methods: comparison of tDCS and tRNS. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2010;121:1908–1914. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.04.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron. 2010;65:550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Antal A, Kriener N, Lang N, Boros K, Paulus W. Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the visual cortex in the prophylactic treatment of migraine. Cephalalgia. 2011;31:820–828. doi: 10.1177/0333102411399349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Antal A, Herrmann CS. Transcranial Alternating Current and Random Noise Stimulation: Possible Mechanisms. Neural Plasticity. 2016;2016:3616807. doi: 10.1155/2016/3616807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Battelli L, Pascual-Leone A, Cavanagh P. The “when” pathway of the right parietal lobe. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11:204–210. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Battelli L, Alvarez GA, Carlson T, Pascual-Leone A. The role of the parietal lobe in visual extinction studied with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:1946–1955. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Battelli L, Grossman ED, Plow EB. Local Immediate versus Long-Range Delayed Changes in Functional Connectivity Following rTMS on the Visual Attention Network. Brain Stimulation. 2017;10:263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bellana B, Liu ZX, Diamond NB, Grady CL, Moscovitch M. Similarities and differences in the default mode network across rest, retrieval, and future imagining. Human Brain Mapping. 2017;38:1155–1171. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bortoletto M, Pellicciari MC, Rodella C, Miniussi C. The interaction with task-induced activity is more important than polarization: a tDCS study. Brain Stimulation. 2015;8:269–276. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Braga RM, Buckner RL. Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Neuron. 2017;95:457–471. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brainard DH. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision. 1997;10:433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Shannon BJ, LaRossa G, Sachs R, Fotenos AF, Sheline YI, Klunk WE, Mathis CA, Morris JC, Mintun MA. Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:7709–7717. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2177-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Buckner R. L, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Buckner R. L, DiNicola LM. The brain’s default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2019;20:593–608. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Buschman TJ, Kastner S. From Behavior to Neural Dynamics: An Integrated Theory of Attention. Neuron. 2015;88:127–144. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Camilleri R, Pavan A, Campana G. The application of online transcranial random noise stimulation and perceptual learning in the improvement of visual functions in mild myopia. Neuropsychologia. 2016;89:225–231. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Campana G, Camilleri R, Pavan A, Veronese A, Lo Giudice G. Improving visual functions in adult amblyopia with combined perceptual training and transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS): a pilot study. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5:1402. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Capotosto P, Tosoni A, Spadone S, Sestieri C, Perrucci MG, Romani GL, Della Penna S, Corbetta M. Anatomical segregation of visual selection mechanisms in human parietal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:6225–6229. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4983-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cappelletti M, Gessaroli E, Hithersay R, Mitolo M, Didino D, Kanai R, Cohen Kadosh R, Walsh V. Transfer of cognitive training across magnitude dimensions achieved with concurrent brain stimulation of the parietal lobe. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:14899–14907. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1692-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Censor N, Sagi D. Benefits of efficient consolidation: short training enables long-term resistance to perceptual adaptation induced by intensive testing. Vision Research. 2008;48:970–977. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Censor N, Harris H, Sagi D. A dissociation between consolidated perceptual learning and sensory adaptation in vision. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:38819. doi: 10.1038/srep38819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Chaieb L, Antal A, Paulus W. Transcranial random noise stimulation-induced plasticity is NMDA-receptor independent but sodium-channel blocker and benzodiazepines sensitive. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2015;9:125. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Chen N, Bi T, Zhou T, Li S, Liu Z, Fang F. Sharpened cortical tuning and enhanced cortico-cortical communication contribute to the long-term neural mechanisms of visual motion perceptual learning. NeuroImage. 2015;115:17–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chiappini E, Silvanto J, Hibbard PB, Avenanti A, Romei V. Strengthening functionally specific neural pathways with transcranial brain stimulation. Current Biology. 2018;28:R735–R736. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Chica AB, Bartolomeo P, Valero-Cabré A. Dorsal and ventral parietal contributions to spatial orienting in the human brain. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:8143–8149. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5463-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Colby CL, Goldberg ME. Space and attention in parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1999;22:319–349. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cole MW, Reynolds JR, Power JD, Repovs G, Anticevic A, Braver TS. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1348–1355. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Corbetta M, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Snyder AZ, Ollinger JM, Drury HA, Linenweber MR, Petersen SE, Raichle ME, Van Essen DC, Shulman GL. A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron. 1998;21:761–773. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80593-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2002;3:201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58:306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2011;34:569–599. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Crittenden BM, Mitchell DJ, Duncan J. Recruitment of the default mode network during a demanding act of executive control. eLife. 2015;4:e06481. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Das A, Tadin D, Huxlin KR. Beyond blindsight: properties of visual relearning in cortically blind fields. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:11652–11664. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1076-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Deveau J, Lovcik G, Seitz AR. Broad-based visual benefits from training with an integrated perceptual-learning video game. Vision Research. 2014;99:134–140. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Dosher BA, Lu ZL. Mechanisms of perceptual learning. Vision Research. 2005;39:471–476. doi: 10.1016/B978-012375731-9/50082-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dosher BA, Jeter P, Liu J, Lu ZL. An integrated reweighting theory of perceptual learning. PNAS. 2013;110:13678–13683. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312552110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dosher B, Lu ZL. Visual Perceptual Learning and Models. Annual Review of Vision Science. 2017;3:343–363. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Edwards G, Agosta S, Herpich F, Contò F, Parrott D, Tyler S, Grossman ED, Battelli L. Prolonged Neuromodulation of Cortical Networks Following Low-Frequency rTMS and Its Potential for Clinical Interventions. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019;10:529. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Fahle M. Perceptual learning: a case for early selection. Journal of Vision. 2004;4:879–890. doi: 10.1167/4.10.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Fertonani A, Pirulli C, Miniussi C. Random noise stimulation improves neuroplasticity in perceptual learning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:15416–15423. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2002-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Fiebelkorn IC, Kastner S. Functional Specialization in the Attention Network. Annual Review of Psychology. 2020;71:221–249. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. PNAS. 2006;103:10046–10051. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604187103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Fox MD, Buckner RL, Liu H, Chakravarty MM, Lozano AM, Pascual-Leone A. Resting-state networks link invasive and noninvasive brain stimulation across diverse psychiatric and neurological diseases. PNAS. 2014;111:E4367–E4375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1405003111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gilbert CD. Learning. Neuronal dynamics and perceptual learning. Current Biology. 1994;4:627–629. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(00)00138-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gilbert CD, Sigman M, Crist RE. The neural basis of perceptual learning. Neuron. 2001;31:681–697. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00424-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Giovannelli F, Silingardi D, Borgheresi A, Feurra M, Amati G, Pizzorusso T, Viggiano MP, Zaccara G, Berardi N, Cincotta M. Involvement of the parietal cortex in perceptual learning (Eureka effect): an interference approach using rTMS. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1807–1812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gratton C, Laumann TO, Gordon EM, Adeyemo B, Petersen SE. Evidence for Two Independent Factors that Modify Brain Networks to Meet Task Goals. Cell Reports. 2016;17:1276–1288. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Greenberg BD, Rauch SL, Haber SN. Invasive circuitry-based neurotherapeutics: stereotactic ablation and deep brain stimulation for OCD. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:317–336. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. PNAS. 2003;100:253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Herpich F, Contò F, van Koningsbruggen M, Battelli L. Modulating the excitability of the visual cortex using a stimulation priming paradigm. Neuropsychologia. 2018;119:165–171. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Herpich F, Melnick MD, Agosta S, Huxlin KR, Tadin D, Battelli L. Boosting Learning Efficacy with Noninvasive Brain Stimulation in Intact and Brain-Damaged Humans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;39:5551–5561. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3248-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Herrington TM, Assad JA. Temporal sequence of attentional modulation in the lateral intraparietal area and middle temporal area during rapid covert shifts of attention. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:3287–3296. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6025-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Herwig U, Satrapi P, Schönfeldt-Lecuona C. Using the international 10-20 EEG system for positioning of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Topography. 2003;16:95–99. doi: 10.1023/b:brat.0000006333.93597.9d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Hsu TY, Juan CH, Tseng P. Individual Differences and State-Dependent Responses in Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2016;10:643. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Huang CB, Lu ZL, Dosher BA. Co-learning analysis of two perceptual learning tasks with identical input stimuli supports the reweighting hypothesis. Vision Research. 2012;61:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Huang Y, Parra LC, Haufe S. The New York Head-A precise standardized volume conductor model for EEG source localization and tES targeting. NeuroImage. 2016;140:150–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Huang Y, Datta A, Bikson M, Parra LC. Realistic volumetric-approach to simulate transcranial electric stimulation-ROAST-a fully automated open-source pipeline. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2019;16:056006. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/ab208d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Husain M, Rorden C. Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2003;4:26–36. doi: 10.1038/nrn1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Husain M, Nachev P. Space and the parietal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11:30–36. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kang DW, Kim D, Chang LH, Kim YH, Takahashi E, Cain MS, Watanabe T, Sasaki Y. Structural and Functional Connectivity Changes Beyond Visual Cortex in a Later Phase of Visual Perceptual Learning. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:5186. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23487-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2000;23:315–341. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kiyonaga A, Powers JP, Chiu YC, Egner T. Hemisphere-specific Parietal Contributions to the Interplay between Working Memory and Attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2021;1:1–14. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Krause MR, Zanos TP, Csorba BA, Pilly PK, Choe J, Phillips ME, Datta A, Pack CC. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Facilitates Associative Learning and Alters Functional Connectivity in the Primate Brain. Current Biology. 2017;27:3086–3096. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kunze T, Hunold A, Haueisen J, Jirsa V, Spiegler A. Transcranial direct current stimulation changes resting state functional connectivity: A large-scale brain network modeling study. NeuroImage. 2016;140:174–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Laitinen L. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery. 1989;91:277–278. doi: 10.1016/0303-8467(89)90128-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Lancaster JL, Rainey LH, Summerlin JL, Freitas CS, Fox PT, Evans AC, Toga AW, Mazziotta JC. Automated labeling of the human brain: A preliminary report on the development and evaluation of a forward-transform method. Human Brain Mapping. 1997;5:238–242. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1997)5:4&#x0003c;238::AID-HBM6&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, Fox PT. Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Human Brain Mapping. 2000;10:120–131. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200007)10:3&#x0003c;120::aid-hbm30&#x0003e;3.0.co;2-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Law C-T, Gold JI. Neural correlates of perceptual learning in a sensory-motor, but not a sensory, cortical area. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:505–513. doi: 10.1038/nn2070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lee TG, D’Esposito M. The dynamic nature of top-down signals originating from prefrontal cortex: a combined fMRI-TMS study. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:15458–15466. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0627-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Leitão J, Thielscher A, Tünnerhoff J, Noppeney U. Concurrent TMS-fMRI Reveals Interactions between Dorsal and Ventral Attentional Systems. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:11445–11457. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0939-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Li W, Piëch V, Gilbert CD. Perceptual learning and top-down influences in primary visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:651–657. doi: 10.1038/nn1255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Liu A, Vöröslakos M, Kronberg G, Henin S, Krause MR, Huang Y, Opitz A, Mehta A, Pack CC, Krekelberg B, Berényi A, Parra LC, Melloni L, Devinsky O, Buzsáki G. Immediate neurophysiological effects of transcranial electrical stimulation. Nature Communications. 2018;9:5092. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07233-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Macaluso E, Driver J. Multisensory spatial interactions: a window onto functional integration in the human brain. Trends in Neurosciences. 2005;28:264–271. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. McDonnell MD, Abbott D, Friston KJ. What Is Stochastic Resonance? Definitions, Misconceptions, Debates, and Its Relevance to Biology. PLOS Computational Biology. 2009;5:e1000348. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. McDonnell M. D, Ward LM. The benefits of noise in neural systems: bridging theory and experiment. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2011;12:415–426. doi: 10.1038/nrn3061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Merriam EP, Genovese CR, Colby CL. Spatial updating in human parietal cortex. Neuron. 2003;39:361–373. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00393-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Nicolini C, Forcellini G, Minati L, Bifone A. Scale-resolved analysis of brain functional connectivity networks with spectral entropy. NeuroImage. 2020;211:116603. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. The Journal of Physiology. 2000;527:633–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Okamoto M, Dan H, Sakamoto K, Takeo K, Shimizu K, Kohno S, Oda I, Isobe S, Suzuki T, Kohyama K, Dan I. Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10-20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping. NeuroImage. 2004;21:99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Papademetris X, Jackowski M, Rajeevan N, Constable RT, Staib LH. BioImage Suite: An integrated medical image analysis suite. The Insight Journal. 2005;1:13. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Patil I. ggstatsplot: “ggplot2” Based Plots with Statistical Details. CRAN. 2018 https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/ggstatsplot/index.html
  84. Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision. 1997;10:437–442. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Pirulli C, Fertonani A, Miniussi C. The role of timing in the induction of neuromodulation in perceptual learning by transcranial electric stimulation. Brain Stimulation. 2013;6:683–689. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Plow EB, Cattaneo Z, Carlson TA, Alvarez GA, Pascual-Leone A, Battelli L. The compensatory dynamic of inter-hemispheric interactions in visuospatial attention revealed using rTMS and fMRI. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:226. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Polanía R, Paulus W, Antal A, Nitsche MA. Introducing graph theory to track for neuroplastic alterations in the resting human brain: a transcranial direct current stimulation study. NeuroImage. 2011;54:2287–2296. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Polanía R, Paulus W, Nitsche MA. Modulating cortico-striatal and thalamo-cortical functional connectivity with transcranial direct current stimulation. Human Brain Mapping. 2012;33:2499–2508. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Polanía R, Nitsche MA, Ruff CC. Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:174–187. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Polat U, Ma-Naim T, Belkin M, Sagi D. Improving vision in adult amblyopia by perceptual learning. PNAS. 2004;101:6692–6697. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0401200101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. PNAS. 2001;98:676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Roberts RP, Hach S, Tippett LJ, Addis DR. The Simpson’s paradox and fMRI: Similarities and differences between functional connectivity measures derived from within-subject and across-subject correlations. NeuroImage. 2016;135:1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Ruff CC, Blankenburg F, Bjoertomt O, Bestmann S, Freeman E, Haynes JD, Rees G, Josephs O, Deichmann R, Driver J. Concurrent TMS-fMRI and psychophysics reveal frontal influences on human retinotopic visual cortex. Current Biology. 2006;16:1479–1488. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Ruff CC, Bestmann S, Blankenburg F, Bjoertomt O, Josephs O, Weiskopf N, Deichmann R, Driver J. Distinct causal influences of parietal versus frontal areas on human visual cortex: evidence from concurrent TMS-fMRI. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:817–827. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Salvioni P, Murray MM, Kalmbach L, Bueti D. How the visual brain encodes and keeps track of time. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:12423–12429. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5146-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Sani I, McPherson BC, Stemmann H, Pestilli F, Freiwald WA. Functionally defined white matter of the macaque monkey brain reveals a dorso-ventral attention network. eLife. 2019;8:e40520. doi: 10.7554/eLife.40520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Sarabi MT, Aoki R, Tsumura K, Keerativittayayut R, Jimura K, Nakahara K. Visual perceptual training reconfigures post-task resting-state functional connectivity with a feature-representation region. PLOS ONE. 2018;13:e0196866. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0196866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Sasaki Y, Nanez JE, Watanabe T. Advances in visual perceptual learning and plasticity. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2010;11:53–60. doi: 10.1038/nrn2737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Gerraty RT, Ruparel K, Loughead J, Calkins ME, Eickhoff SB, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE, Wolf DH. An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. NeuroImage. 2013;64:240–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Schwarzkopf DS, Silvanto J, Rees G. Stochastic resonance effects reveal the neural mechanisms of transcranial magnetic stimulation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:3143–3147. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4863-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Sehm B, Schäfer A, Kipping J, Margulies D, Conde V, Taubert M, Villringer A, Ragert P. Dynamic modulation of intrinsic functional connectivity by transcranial direct current stimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;108:3253–3263. doi: 10.1152/jn.00606.2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Seitz AR, Dinse HR. A common framework for perceptual learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2007;17:148–153. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2001;86:1916–1936. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.1916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Shibata K, Sasaki Y, Kawato M, Watanabe T. Neuroimaging Evidence for 2 Types of Plasticity in Association with Visual Perceptual Learning. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;26:3681–3689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Sterkin A, Levy Y, Pokroy R, Lev M, Levian L, Doron R, Yehezkel O, Fried M, Frenkel-Nir Y, Gordon B, Polat U. Vision improvement in pilots with presbyopia following perceptual learning. Vision Research. 2018;152:61–73. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2017.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Suzuki K, Garfinkel SN, Critchley HD, Seth AK. Multisensory integration across exteroceptive and interoceptive domains modulates self-experience in the rubber-hand illusion. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:2909–2917. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Szczepanski SM, Kastner S. Shifting Attentional Priorities: Control of Spatial Attention through Hemispheric Competition. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:5411–5421. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4089-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Szpiro SFA, Wright BA, Carrasco M. Learning one task by interleaving practice with another task. Vision Research. 2014;101:118–124. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: Thieme Medical; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  110. Tyler SC, Dasgupta S, Agosta S, Battelli L, Grossman ED. Functional connectivity of parietal cortex during temporal selective attention. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 2015;65:195–207. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Tyler SC, Contò F, Battelli L. Rapid Improvement on a Temporal Attention Task within a Single Session of High-frequency Transcranial Random Noise Stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2018;30:656–666. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. van den Heuvel MP, de Lange SC, Zalesky A, Seguin C, Yeo BTT, Schmidt R. Proportional thresholding in resting-state fMRI functional connectivity networks and consequences for patient-control connectome studies: Issues and recommendations. NeuroImage. 2017;152:437–449. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. van der Groen O, Wenderoth N. Transcranial Random Noise Stimulation of Visual Cortex: Stochastic Resonance Enhances Central Mechanisms of Perception. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:5289–5298. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4519-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. van der Groen O, Tang MF, Wenderoth N, Mattingley JB. Stochastic resonance enhances the rate of evidence accumulation during combined brain stimulation and perceptual decision-making. PLOS Computational Biology. 2018;14:e1006301. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Varangis E, Habeck CG, Razlighi QR, Stern Y. The Effect of Aging on Resting State Connectivity of Predefined Networks in the Brain. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2019;11:234. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Vossel S, Weidner R, Driver J, Friston KJ, Fink GR. Deconstructing the architecture of dorsal and ventral attention systems with dynamic causal modeling. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:10637–10648. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0414-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and ventral attention systems: distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. The Neuroscientist. 2014;20:150–159. doi: 10.1177/1073858413494269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Waskom ML, Okazawa G, Kiani R. Designing and Interpreting Psychophysical Investigations of Cognition. Neuron. 2019;104:100–112. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.09.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Wright BA, Sabin AT, Zhang Y, Marrone N, Fitzgerald MB. Enhancing perceptual learning by combining practice with periods of additional sensory stimulation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:12868–12877. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0487-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Yao T, Treue S, Krishna BS. An Attention-Sensitive Memory Trace in Macaque MT Following Saccadic Eye Movements. PLOS Biology. 2016;14:e1002390. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Yarkoni T. Big Correlations in Little Studies: Inflated fMRI Correlations Reflect Low Statistical Power-Commentary on Vul et al. (2009) Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2009;4:294–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01127.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Yates JL, Park IM, Katz LN, Pillow JW, Huk AC. Functional dissection of signal and noise in MT and LIP during decision-making. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:1285–1292. doi: 10.1038/nn.4611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Zhang T, Xiao LQ, Klein SA, Levi DM, Yu C. Decoupling location specificity from perceptual learning of orientation discrimination. Vision Research. 2010a;50:368–374. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Zhang JY, Zhang GL, Xiao LQ, Klein SA, Levi DM, Yu C. Rule-Based Learning Explains Visual Perceptual Learning and Its Specificity and Transfer. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010b;30:12323–12328. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0704-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Zhou Y, Freedman DJ. Posterior parietal cortex plays a causal role in perceptual and categorical decisions. Science. 2019;365:180–185. doi: 10.1126/science.aaw8347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Editor's evaluation

Taraz Lee 1

Conto et al. applied transcranial random noise stimulation to the parietal cortex during performance of an attention task, examining if multiple sessions would result in a cumulative increase in performance. The results showed that tRNS to parietal, but not a ventral-lateral temporal site, increased performance on an orientation discrimination task, but not a temporal order judgement task. Using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging and a functional connectivity (FC) analysis, tRNS was shown to increase FC within the dorsal and ventral attention network. Overall, an increase in FC within the DVAN positively correlated with changes in behavioral performance (collapsing across all stimulation conditions). Overall, the experimental design and results are compelling. These findings further our understanding of how combining non-invasive brain stimulation with behavioral training can lead to enhanced neural plasticity that supports more effective learning.

Decision letter

Editor: Taraz Lee1
Reviewed by: Taraz Lee2

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Thank you for submitting your article "Attention network modulation via tRNS correlates with attention gain" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, including Taraz Lee as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen Rich Ivry as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

As the editors have judged that your manuscript is of interest, but as described below that additional analyses are required before it is published, we would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). First, because many researchers have temporarily lost access to the labs, we will give authors as much time as they need to submit revised manuscripts. We are also offering, if you choose, to post the manuscript to bioRxiv (if it is not already there) along with this decision letter and a formal designation that the manuscript is "in revision at eLife". Please let us know if you would like to pursue this option. (If your work is more suitable for medRxiv, you will need to post the preprint yourself, as the mechanisms for us to do so are still in development.)

Summary:

Conto et al. applied transcranial random noise stimulation to the parietal cortex during performance of an attention task, examining if multiple sessions would result in a cumulative increase in performance. The results showed that tRNS to parietal, but not a ventral-lateral temporal site, increased performance on an orientation discrimination task, but not a temporal order judgement task. Using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging and a functional connectivity (FC) analysis, tRNS was shown to increase FC within the dorsal and ventral attention network. Overall, an increase in FC within the DVAN positively correlated with changes in behavioral performance (collapsing across all stimulation conditions). Overall, the experimental design and results are compelling. However, there are several concerns that need to be addressed before publication can be recommended.

Essential revisions:

1. Multiple reviewers felt the flow of the paper from the introduction to the results is quite jarring given that there is very little explanation of the tasks or the overall design of the experiment. What is the orientation discrimination task, for example? When do the resting state scans occur? Figure 7 is quite informative, "absolutely essential" in the words of one reviewer, and really needs to be the first figure the reader sees. The authors should include a paragraph at the start of the results that elaborates the full experimental design and motivation for including each element. Additionally, please explicitly acknowledge the time at which the resting-state fMRI sessions were acquired in Figure 7. It seems that task-based fMRI was collected on the same days as the resting state scans. Did the resting scans occur before or after task performance? Given that prior studies have reported that tasks can alter subsequent resting state connectivity, it seems important to know whether tRNS altered baseline connectivity or rather post-task connectivity. Please use the same terminology in Figure 2 "day 1 or day 6" or "pre-test" and "post-test". The authors switch terminology occasionally between figures, and with an already complex experimental design, this adds undue complexity.

2. Why is data from the training sessions not reported? It seems important to see whether stimulation improved on-line performance during training to properly interpret the results. The authors should report these data (ideally with a figure). On a related note, it is unclear why the authors chose to show the dependent measure is that is reported in Figure 1. Given that what was actually analyzed was the pre-test and post-test accuracy scores, could these not be plotted instead of a % improvement score (i.e. similar to Figure 2)?

3. The authors claim that stimulation to IPS alters DVAN connectivity specifically and that this change in connectivity is what drives behavioral improvements. However, they do not assess changes in network connectivity within any other networks. Perhaps IPS stimulation leads to more widespread connectivity changes that also have an effect. One option is to investigate a network that is not involved in the attention task, e.g. the default mode network. Functional connectivity within this network should not be modulated by stimulation. This control analysis would lend further evidence to confirm the specificity of the stimulation effects. In addition, the authors could include an analysis of the function network targeted by hMT+. Perhaps functional connectivity within this network increases from hMT+, but does not translate into a benefit into performance (no correlation between FC and performance). Alternatively, stimulation to hMT+ may not modulate the functional network because that network is not active during the task, providing additional evidence to the state-dependent effects of tRNS.

4. The difference in electrode placement for parietal and hMT+ stimulation is subtle and thus speaks to the specificity of the effects. However, the differential effect of stimulation on the brain due to such a slight adjustment of electrode placement should be elaborated. Please include electric field models (e.g. ROAST) for the two electrode montages. The effect of parietal stimulation on the DVAN should be more apparent from these electric field models.

5. In the discussion, the authors reveal that hMT+ was targeted as a region that may play a role in temporal attention and the experiment was thus designed to probe this cognitive process. However, in the introduction this motivation for the experimental design is not revealed but instead referred to as "an active control". The authors can be more forthcoming in the introduction about this hypothesized link and that it did not pan out. This is acceptable and hMT+ can still serve as an active control.

6. There is very little information provided about the pairwise functional connectivity analysis (starting on line 193). Is each pair treated independently in this analysis? It seems as though the analysis conducted does not take into account within-participant variance given the DF reported? More information needs to be provided in (perhaps in the Methods) about this analysis.

7. The authors are very underpowered to conduct a brain-behavior correlation and this analysis does not seem very informative given that there are 3 different groups. Given that there doesn't seem to be much of a relationship between behavioral and functional connectivity changes in the sham and hMT+ group, it seems this is all driven by the parietal group and has already been shown in the prior analyses. The authors should consider removing this analysis or at least providing more motivation behind the need for this analysis

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Attention network modulation via tRNS correlates with attention gain" for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been evaluated by Richard Ivry (Senior Editor) and a Reviewing Editor.

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed, as outlined below:

– Although they were enthusiastic about the result multiple reviewers commented on concerns regarding a small sample size and an inflated effect size. Although additional stimulation-behavioral data to more properly estimate the effect size would be welcome, the authors need to at least: report effect sizes, state how power was calculated for the study, and caution that the effect sizes observed are likely to be somewhat inflated given that they were conditioned on a significant result.

– There was still concern about the electric field models presented in the paper and their relationship with the activity in the stimulation sites that is driven by the tasks at hand. In consultation, one reviewer remarked, "The SimNIBS e-field models are presented in a raw form that in my experiences is only shown as a challenge getting the software to display the e-field on the cortical surface. I suggest that the authors run their e-field models in the ROAST toolbox. As presented, the e-fields are not really interpretable." The reviewers felt it would be important to compare whole-brain maps of task-related activity with accurate electric field models to determine whether the effects of stimulation are due to more effective targeting of task-active regions.

– Several issues with clarity and claims made in the discussion need to be resolved. Please see the specific comments made by reviewers #4 and #5 below.

Reviewer #4:

N=10 / group is fairly small. Please report effect sizes. How was power calculated for the study?

Unclear why data is represented inconsistently (i.e., post-stimulation scores at times and difference between FC scores pre- and post-test sessions at other times). Does post-stimulation refer to the change from baseline or just data from the post-session alone? The difference between FC scores pre- and post-test sessions is more informative and should be what is reported in the manuscript.

Scatter plots should be removed.

Reviewer #5:

This study shows a very striking effect of parietal tRNS on object discrimination and corresponding large changes in functional connectivity. Such data are sure to be of interest to numerous researchers due to their practical significance. That said, I have statistical concerns and am left wanting more mechanistic insight. Ultimately, I think more data and analyses will be needed to make a convincing enough case to the readership of this outlet.

1) The effect of parietal tRNS on object discrimination is truly massive. The tasks are titrated at the start to set performance at 50%. While performance remains the same following temporal and sham tRNS, parietal tRNS improves performance by 30%. Given this finding, I am rather convinced that parietal tRNS improves object discrimination. What I am not convinced of is that the effect size is as massive as is being depicted. Given that the main contribution of this study as it is current written is practical, having a reasonable estimate of effect size is important for applied work that will seek to build on this striking finding. However, given the small sample size (10 participants per group), and effect size inflation that occurs when conditionalizing on significance, especially in small samples, I suspect that the true effect size is substantially smaller than has been depicted. Unfortunately, I do not see a good way to get a better estimate of the true effect size without acquiring more data. The upshot is that new data would not require fMRI and may not even require control groups. A bare minimum would be to collect another group of 10 participants receiving parietal tRNS and estimate effect size based on their improvement. I think that such data collection would be a low cost effort, and the sample size is small enough to potentially be feasible amidst pandemic restrictions. Note that since the new sample would be used to estimate effect size, there need not be statistically significant improvement in the new sample to have utility.

2) These data beg the question of why parietal tRNS produces such a dramatic effect, but temporal tRNS does not. That is, one would like to have some mechanistic insight into why parietal tRNS is so effective, at least in the object discrimination task. The connectivity data do not, in their present form, address this question. They merely provide another metric of change without begetting further causal inference/insight. Fortunately, the authors have additional task fMRI data that could potential shed some light on the issue. For example, one might ask whether the extent to which a given task activates the stimulated areas is associated with changes in connectivity and/or behavior. Alternatively, the network position of the parietal target either generally or specific to a given task may be associated with its effective change in connectivity and/or behavior. Given that the authors have effects that are specific not only to target, but also differ as a function of task, I feel as though these data could be leveraged to provide substantial insights into what factors are critical for an effective intervention.

Relatedly, the authors offer conclusions in the discussion regarding what constitutes a successful intervention, but they are either unsubstantiated by the present data, or unclear. For example, they state on page 20, lines 439-441:

"It is the combination of a challenging task with concurrent brain stimulation over the relevant cortical circuits that facilitates fast learning, a paradigm amenable to promote plasticity."

Similar sentiments are echoed on page 20, lines 451-453. Absent examination of task data, the relevant cortical circuits for the studied tasks are unclear. Consider that in Figure 9, the temporal targeted tRNS seems to induce the most stimulation in occipital areas. I would expect occipital cortex to be extremely relevant for the studied tasks, yet stimulation over those areas did not improve performance. Moreover, the importance of stimulation concurrent with tasks cannot be concluded from these data since there were no conditions that had no tasks. The differential performance across the studied tasks may potentially be leveraged here, but at the moment, this conclusion does not follow from the data. Since the authors have data that can help speak to some of these issues, it seems pertinent to use them.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Attention network modulation via tRNS correlates with attention gain" for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been evaluated by Richard Ivry (Senior Editor) and a Reviewing Editor.

The manuscript has been greatly improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed, as outlined below. Though these revisions are essential, the manuscript will not be sent out for another round of external reviews and can be handled editorially.

1. While the Bayesian analyses are nice, they do not address the point that the effect sizes are likely to be inflated given the sample size. The issue is that when sample size is small and reporting of results is conditioned on statistical significance (e.g. p< 0.05) , effect size will be inflated. This needs to be acknowledged in the discussion. See, for example, the reference below for more discussion of inflated effect sizes in studies with small samples.

Yarkoni, T. (2009). Big correlations in little studies: Inflated fMRI correlations reflect low statistical power-Commentary on Vul et al. (2009). Perspectives on psychological science, 4(3), 294-298.

2. Please report the analyses showing that the number of frames censored were similar across sessions in the manuscript.

3. The task-based univariate results are quite illuminating. It would seem pertinent to mention in the manuscript that both tasks activate IPS in the discussion of why effects were observed for the OD task but not the TOJ task.

eLife. 2021 Nov 26;10:e63782. doi: 10.7554/eLife.63782.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1. Multiple reviewers felt the flow of the paper from the introduction to the results is quite jarring given that there is very little explanation of the tasks or the overall design of the experiment. What is the orientation discrimination task, for example? When do the resting state scans occur? Figure 7 is quite informative, "absolutely essential" in the words of one reviewer, and really needs to be the first figure the reader sees. The authors should include a paragraph at the start of the results that elaborates the full experimental design and motivation for including each element. Additionally, please explicitly acknowledge the time at which the resting-state fMRI sessions were acquired in Figure 7. It seems that task-based fMRI was collected on the same days as the resting state scans. Did the resting scans occur before or after task performance? Given that prior studies have reported that tasks can alter subsequent resting state connectivity, it seems important to know whether tRNS altered baseline connectivity or rather post-task connectivity. Please use the same terminology in Figure 2 "day 1 or day 6" or "pre-test" and "post-test". The authors switch terminology occasionally between figures, and with an already complex experimental design, this adds undue complexity.

Thank you for the positive and constructive comments, we have moved Figure 7 within the Results section, and it is now Figure 1. As suggested, at the beginning of the Results section, we have added a paragraph briefly summarizing the experimental procedure, the tasks and motivation. More details to explain the time course of the experiment have now been added to the Figure legend. We have revised the manuscript and used the same terminology to indicate pre and post-test sessions throughout the text and in figure caption.

Resting state scans were collected on the same day and during the same task-based fMRI scanning session (see new text in the Neuroimaging Procedure section). Because we followed the same acquisition order for each participant and each fMRI session (pre- post-test sessions), if task performance had any effect on resting state connectivity, its potential impact would be equivalent for each participant and condition. Importantly, to assess whether task performance differently influenced resting state connectivity and yielded differences between groups at baseline, we performed statistical analysis on the pre-test fMRI session connectivity data (Day2 in Figure 1A). We compared resting state functional connectivity within the DVAN for the three stimulation conditions and we did not find any significant difference in pre-stimulation FC scores between the three groups (see second paragraph of ‘Stimulation and learning-dependent changes in functional connectivity’ in Results section), indicating that connectivity did not differ between groups at baseline. Thus, the changes we found on Day 7 (Figure 1A) were solely due to the stimulation and training protocol. Moreover, we analyzed the modulation of connectivity scores by normalizing post-test connectivity to pre-test connectivity (∆=FC(S2) – FC(S1)), so that potential differences at baseline were accounted for (Figure 6, left panel).

2. Why is data from the training sessions not reported? It seems important to see whether stimulation improved on-line performance during training to properly interpret the results. The authors should report these data (ideally with a figure). On a related note, it is unclear why the authors chose to show the dependent measure is that is reported in Figure 1. Given that what was actually analyzed was the pre-test and post-test accuracy scores, could these not be plotted instead of a % improvement score (i.e. similar to Figure 2)?

Following the reviewers’ suggestion, we have now reported the training data and analysis in the revised manuscript (subsection ‘Time Course of Learning’ in Results section), including figures showing percentage improvement (Figure 2C and 2D). We initially wanted to focus on the same-session effects upon behavior and functional connectivity, specifically on the pre-test and post-test sessions (Day2 and Day 7 in Figure 1A, respectively). However, we acknowledge that the time course of training reveals how long it took for the learning to build up and become significantly different from the control conditions, where we did not see any learning. The new analysis performed on the training data confirmed the main result found in the pre- vs post-test analysis. The selective improvement for the visuospatial task (Orientation Discrimination, OD) built up and consolidated across sessions, while the temporal order judgment (TOJ) task did not change, and this was true for the parietal group only. We have now modified the results and Discussion sections to include training data. Figure 2 (Behavioral performance on the OD and the TOJ task during and following stimulation and training) has been modified and has now four panels reporting the box plots for behavioral improvement between pre- and post-test sessions for the OD (Figure 2A) and the TOJ task (Figure 2B), and the time course of the effect across the four training sessions for the OD (2C) and the TOJ (2D) task, separately.

To respond to the second point raised by the reviewers, in Figure 2 we show the dependent measure as the difference in performance between pre- and post-test accuracy for each task because at baseline (pre-test) the one-way ANOVA did not show any significant difference in performance between groups, while it revealed strong significant difference in the post-test. Normalized data directly represents the change in performance for each stimulation condition against baseline performance (Figure 2A and Figure 2B).

3. The authors claim that stimulation to IPS alters DVAN connectivity specifically and that this change in connectivity is what drives behavioral improvements. However, they do not assess changes in network connectivity within any other networks. Perhaps IPS stimulation leads to more widespread connectivity changes that also have an effect. One option is to investigate a network that is not involved in the attention task, e.g. the default mode network. Functional connectivity within this network should not be modulated by stimulation. This control analysis would lend further evidence to confirm the specificity of the stimulation effects. In addition, the authors could include an analysis of the function network targeted by hMT+. Perhaps functional connectivity within this network increases from hMT+, but does not translate into a benefit into performance (no correlation between FC and performance). Alternatively, stimulation to hMT+ may not modulate the functional network because that network is not active during the task, providing additional evidence to the state-dependent effects of tRNS.

Following reviewers’ suggestion, we conducted a number of control analysis to examine functional connectivity changes in the Default Mode Network (DMN), following training and stimulation. This network comprises a number of functionally interconnected brain regions whose activity increases during rest or when the person is not involved in an active goal-oriented state (Greicious et al., 2003; Buckner et al., 2008; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010; Braga, et al., 2017; Raichle et al., 2001; Parker and Razlighi, 2019). Because this network was not targeted by our stimulation protocol and it is not directly involved in attention tasks, we hypothesized that the connectivity within its nodes would not be modulated by our protocol. We performed the same analysis we conducted for the DVAN rs-FC to test whether changes in connectivity were specific for the DVAN, or whether stimulation induced more widespread changes in the brain, affecting other networks, like the DMN.

We have now reported all the analysis and results relative to the DMN in the manuscript and Appendix (Appendix, section “Default Mode Network Analysis”). Briefly, the control analyses found no changes in connectivity within the DMN, indicating that our stimulation-induced changes were selective for the DVAN.

In addition to the lack of DMN modulation, we also demonstrate the selectivity of our stimulation-training protocol by the modulation of behavior and rs-FC following stimulation to the Parietal regions only. Our results suggest that when bilateral tRNS is applied over brain regions critical for the attention network (parietal areas), it induces propagated connectivity changes within this network and supports related behavior. However, stimulation to hMT+ areas which are involved, but not critical for attentional processes, does not induce attentional network nor behavior changes. Interestingly, in a previous study from our group (Herpich et al., 2019), we showed that when we targeted the IPS with tRNS while subjects were training on a motion coherence task (a task hMT+ is very sensitive to, as shown by imaging and physiological studies; Tootell et al., 1995; Born and Bradley, 2005), we found no effect of stimulation. Thus, we believe our past and current data indeed provide evidence of site-specific effects of tRNS.

Together, the control analysis on the DMN and the selectivity of the site-specific stimulation effect on functional connectivity, provide strong evidence for a selective change in connectivity within the Dorsal and Ventral Attention network. Our findings do not support the hypothesis of tRNS causing a general unspecific effect on the brain (or behavior), discussed further below.

4. The difference in electrode placement for parietal and hMT+ stimulation is subtle and thus speaks to the specificity of the effects. However, the differential effect of stimulation on the brain due to such a slight adjustment of electrode placement should be elaborated. Please include electric field models (e.g. ROAST) for the two electrode montages. The effect of parietal stimulation on the DVAN should be more apparent from these electric field models.

We agree with the reviewer, while the distance between electrode location for hMT+ and Parietal is small, we still found a strong difference in the behavioral and imaging results, a clear indication of the specificity of the effect. We believe one of the reasons is that we carefully chose and programmed a task that was well known to increase activity within the attention-related parietal areas. We then maximally recruited parietal areas through increasing task-difficulty via thresholding performance for each participant. Thresholding also equated task-difficulty across subjects, to ensure any changes we found were due to our manipulation (different stimulation conditions) and not to other variables.

As the reviewer suggested, we also performed a simulation of the electrical field current distribution in the head using SimNIBS (Saturnino et al., 2019; Thielscher et al., 2015) and included the results for each active stimulation setting (Parietal and hMT+) in “Stimulation Protocol” section, Figure 8. These electric field models aim to determine which brain areas are affected by different types of electrode montages (Saturnino et al., 2019), allowing a more careful evaluation of whether the electrode position and the corresponding current intensities are targeting the brain area we stimulated. However, single brain regions are not segregated and do not operate in isolation. Yet, they constantly interact with other regions in distributed brain networks (Grefkes and Fink, 2014; Yeo et al., 2011; Siegel et al., 2015). A limitation of these electric field models is their difficulty to determine whether a stimulation setting will influence the site of stimulation only, or whether it will produce a cascade of network-wide effects. Our results suggest that when bilateral tRNS is applied over parietal cortices which are critical brain regions for the attention network, it is able to induce a cascade of connectivity changes within this network (and support related behavior), while when stimulation is applied over a nearby but distinct brain region not crucial for attention, like hMT+, it does not induce attentional network nor behavior changes. Importantly, these results are consistent with previous studies that found different behavioral results on an attention task using the same stimulation settings (Tyler et al., 2018). Together, these results represent new findings for this stimulation technique, as they indicate that the effects of tRNS might have a higher spatial resolution than previously thought and points to the possibility to use these neuromodulation and cognitive training protocol to exert specific, long-lasting and distal effects.

5. In the discussion, the authors reveal that hMT+ was targeted as a region that may play a role in temporal attention and the experiment was thus designed to probe this cognitive process. However, in the introduction this motivation for the experimental design is not revealed but instead referred to as "an active control". The authors can be more forthcoming in the introduction about this hypothesized link and that it did not pan out. This is acceptable and hMT+ can still serve as an active control.

Thank you for pointing this out. We have now added a clearer motivation in the Introduction on our rationale for choosing hMT+ as an active control condition. Interestingly, tRNS over hMT+ had no effect in our cross-tasks training procedure. It is also remarkable that the temporal order judgment task was not affected by the stimulation. Many reasons could account for this result. However, as we speculate in the Discussion section, when the task is to detect an interval time difference between targets displaced across space (and hemifields), more ventral, inferior parietal areas might be directly involved (Battelli et al., 2007), areas that we did not stimulate in our experiment.

6. There is very little information provided about the pairwise functional connectivity analysis (starting on line 193). Is each pair treated independently in this analysis? It seems as though the analysis conducted does not take into account within-participant variance given the DF reported? More information needs to be provided in (perhaps in the Methods) about this analysis.

We revised the manuscript in response to reviewers’ comments. We have now added additional details in the ‘Methods’ section, ‘Resting State fMRI data analysis’ subsection.

7. The authors are very underpowered to conduct a brain-behavior correlation and this analysis does not seem very informative given that there are 3 different groups. Given that there doesn't seem to be much of a relationship between behavioral and functional connectivity changes in the sham and hMT+ group, it seems this is all driven by the parietal group and has already been shown in the prior analyses. The authors should consider removing this analysis or at least providing more motivation behind the need for this analysis

We agree with the reviewer that our prior analyses already show that FC changes in the parietal group only. However, we believe the connectivity-behavior correlation analysis is critical to determine whether connectivity modulation within the attention network is related to performance changes, and answers the question of potential driving/causal relationship between stimulation-induced changes and facilitatory behavioral effects. In particular, we hypothesized that the magnitude of connectivity modulation induced by the stimulation protocol coupled with training could covary with behavioral improvements measures. These analyses are consistent with previous work that correlated levels of BOLD signal and behavior as a way to assess the function of a given brain region for a given performance, and to explore the relationship between brain dynamics patterns and behavior (Kang et al., 2018; Fatima et al., 2016; Wen et al., 2012; Hester et al., 2008; Rypma et al., 2002).

To further analyze the behavioral relevance of connectivity within the attention network, we now have incorporated two correlation analyses (see modified manuscript for details, subsection ‘Functional connectivity-behavior correlation’ in Results section). In particular we computed two linear regression models using individual measures of behavioral improvement (one for accuracy changes pre-post stimulation and one for accuracy changes during training) against individual measures of stimulation-induced changes in rs-FC. The results indicate that individual differences in performing the attention task both during and after training/stimulation can be related to individual differences in functional connectivity changes within the attention network.

We agree that our correlation analysis could be underpowered, a higher number of data points might increase the strength of its significance. However, we also believe that the results obtained are informative and useful to interpret our data, and mostly of the relationship between connectivity and behavior stimulation-induced effects. We were motivated to perform this further analysis and to include it in the manuscript by a number of published studies that used a similar number of subjects to look at brain-behavior correlation (Rosenberg et al., 2020; Sreenivasan et al., 2019; Frangou et al., 2018; Kang et al., 2018; Ficek et al., 2018; Wen et al., 2012;; Fatima et al., 2016; Baldassarre et al., 2012; Bonnelle et al., 2012; Fox et al., 2007; Young et al., 2014).

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed, as outlined below:

– Although they were enthusiastic about the result multiple reviewers commented on concerns regarding a small sample size and an inflated effect size. Although additional stimulation-behavioral data to more properly estimate the effect size would be welcome, the authors need to at least: report effect sizes, state how power was calculated for the study, and caution that the effect sizes observed are likely to be somewhat inflated given that they were conditioned on a significant result.

Thank you for the positive comments, we have now updated the manuscript and reported the effect size for each statistical test, indicating the magnitude of the experimental effects. Reviewer 5 states that they are convinced that parietal tRNS improves object discrimination performance but expresses their concern about how “truly massive” the behavioral effect is in this sample. We have now conducted a series of additional analyses to rule out the possibility that our results were inflated. In particular, we ran a Bayes factor analysis which is an alternative statistical approach that provides an estimate of the amount of evidence present in the data, and it does so both for the research hypothesis and for the null hypothesis (Kruschke, 2011), thus overcoming some of the shortcomings of classical null-hypothesis significance testing and associated p-values (Johnson, 2013; Nuzzo, 2013; Jarosz and Wiley, 2014). Importantly, the Bayes factor analysis deals better with small sample sizes and helps distinguish between evidence for an unsuccessful stimulation protocol and inconclusive evidence (Biel and Friedrich, 2018). We calculated the Bayes factor as the ratio that contrasts the likelihood of the data fitting under the null hypothesis with the likelihood of the data fitting under the alternative hypothesis for the two main significant behavioral analyses, that is, for the repeated measure ANOVA and for the one-way ANOVA on the performance change between pre- and post-stimulation on the orientation discrimination task. Results are reported in Author response Table 1. Using the interpretation of the Bayes Factors (BFs) suggested by Lee and Wagenmakers (2013), the estimated BFs calculated on our behavioral data (BF10 = 99.956 for the Bayesian one-way ANOVA, and BF10=83.962 for the main result of the Bayesian rm-ANOVA) indicate that there is very strong evidence in support of the alternative hypothesis (H1). In particular, the Bayesian one-way ANOVA indicates that the data were 99.956 times more likely to occur under the model including the effect of stimulation condition, compared to the model without this effect. In order to follow up on this result, we then compared each level of the dependent variable with post-hoc Bayes factor t-test. Parietal group vs Sham and Parietal vs hMT revealed posterior odds of 18.103 and 11.304, which indicates strong evidence in favor of the alternative hypothesis.

Author response table 1. Bayes Factor Analysis on Orientation Discrimination data.

Table 1a and 1b reports the results of the BF analysis that tested the two models, null and Stimulation Condition (Condition). P(M) indicates the prior probabilities of each model to be equal (prior model odds). P(M|data) reports the updated probabilities having taken into consideration the data (posterior model probabilities); BFM indicates how much the data have changed the prior model odds. BF10 indicates the Bayes factors for each model (the BF10 for stimulation condition indicates how many times data are more likely to fall under the stimulation model, rather than the null model). Error % indicates the sensitivity to numerical fluctuations. Table 1c reports the results of the post-hoc Tests BF analysis. The posterior odds have been corrected for multiple testing by fixing to 0.5 the prior probability that the null hypothesis holds across all comparisons (Westfall et al. 1997). Individual comparisons are based on the default t-test with a Cauchy (0, r = 1/sqrt(2)) prior. The "U" in the Bayes factor denotes that it is uncorrected.

Table 1a
Bayesian Repeated Measures ANOVA
Data Input: OD pre, and post-stimulation performance
Model Comparison
Models P(M) P(MIdata) BF m BF 10 Error %
Null model (inc. subject) 0.200 0.011 0.046 1.000
RM Factor + Condition + RM * Condition 0.200 0.945 69.335 83.962 2.528
RM Factor 0.200 0.031 0.126 2.713 6.920
RM Factor + Condition 0.200 0.009 0.038 0.834 2.510
Condition 0.200 0.003 0.013 0.297 0.664
Note. All models include subject
Table 1b
Bayesian ANOVA
Data Input: OD performance changes
Model Comparison
Models P(M) P(MIdata) BF m BF 10 error %
Null model 0.500 0.010 0.010 1.00
Condition 0.500 0.990 99.956 99.956 5.865e -4
Table 1c
Bayesian ANOVA
Post Hoc Comparison – Cond
Models Prior Odds Posterior Odds BF 10,u error %
Parietal vs Sham 0.587 18.103 30.818 2.505e -4
Parietal vs hMT 0.587 11.304 19.243 8.095e 4
Sham vs hMT 0.587 0.299 0.510 6.066e -4

The Bayes factor analyses suggest that the significance of the p-value (and related effects sizes) found with conventional statistical analysis likely represent a real significant effect of parietal stimulation in the orientation discrimination task.

– There was still concern about the electric field models presented in the paper and their relationship with the activity in the stimulation sites that is driven by the tasks at hand. In consultation, one reviewer remarked, "The SimNIBS e-field models are presented in a raw form that in my experiences is only shown as a challenge getting the software to display the e-field on the cortical surface. I suggest that the authors run their e-field models in the ROAST toolbox. As presented, the e-fields are not really interpretable." The reviewers felt it would be important to compare whole-brain maps of task-related activity with accurate electric field models to determine whether the effects of stimulation are due to more effective targeting of task-active regions.

We have now run the e-field models using ROAST (see Figure 8 Methods section). We will respond to the second query by the reviewer (to compare whole-brain maps of task-related activity) more extensively below, to the specific question asked by reviewer 5. The connectivity changes we observe at rest are likely driven by stimulation and learning (Lewis et al., 2009), and the differences we report are stimulation site specific because the learning effect is selective for the parietal condition only.

– Several issues with clarity and claims made in the discussion need to be resolved. Please see the specific comments made by reviewers #4 and #5 below.

Thank you, we have now reviewed the manuscript and/or corrected and clarified.

Reviewer #4:

N=10 / group is fairly small. Please report effect sizes. How was power calculated for the study?

We now have modified the manuscript to report effect sizes. We reported partial eta squared (ηp2) for the ANOVAs and Cohen’s d for t-test. As we reported in the “transparent reporting” document (and copied here), an explicit power analysis was not performed. However, the sample size was based on previously published studies that used neurostimulation procedures coupled with learning and/or neuroimaging techniques and have a similar (or fewer) number of participants.

In particular, Helfrich and colleagues (Plos Biology, 2014) used a combination of high-density tACS and EEG testing 14 participants and used a very similar experimental procedure to ours, as they had two separate days and three stimulation conditions. Another study that used a combination of behavioral, neurostimulation and neuroimaging techniques (Saiote et al., 2013; PLOS ONE), investigated BOLD activity induced by high-frequency tRNS during a visuomotor task (10 subjects per condition) and found a behavioral improvement after hf-tRNS and cathodal tDCS and a reduction of learning-related brain activity.

In a different study that used the combination of learning and neurostimulation techniques, Snowball and colleagues (Snowball et al., 2013, Current Biology: CB, 23(11), 987–992) tested 25 subjects altogether across 4 conditions. Another study conducted by Lewis and colleagues (Lewis et al., PNAS, 2009) investigated the role of spontaneous brain activity for brain dynamics of 14 participants. Other studies that used a combination of neurostimulation techniques and behavior and/or neuroimaging techniques and used a similar (or smaller) number of participants are reported in the transparent reporting document.

Unclear why data is represented inconsistently (i.e., post-stimulation scores at times and difference between FC scores pre- and post-test sessions at other times). Does post-stimulation refer to the change from baseline or just data from the post-session alone? The difference between FC scores pre- and post-test sessions is more informative and should be what is reported in the manuscript.

We apologize for the confusion. When we talk about “post-stimulation” we refer to data from the post-session alone, while when we talk about the difference between FC pre and post-test sessions we refer to the change in connectivity scores from baseline. The analyses on post-stimulation FC scores were performed to capture whether there was a difference in connectivity measures during the post-stimulation scanning session between stimulation conditions, given that there was no statistically significant difference when FC scores were compared between conditions in the pre-stimulation session. However, because we agree with the reviewer that it is important to compare how (and how much) connectivity changed between the two session, analysis on the difference in connectivity scores were also conducted and reported. As requested by the reviewer, we have now just reported the difference between the pre- and post-test FC scores. Moreover, we have removed one figure representing the post-stimulation FC, and replaced it with a Figure representing the difference in connectivity within the two networks (DVAN and DMN, Figure 6 in the manuscript).

Scatter plots should be removed.

We have removed the scatter plots as requested by the reviewer.

Reviewer #5:

This study shows a very striking effect of parietal tRNS on object discrimination and corresponding large changes in functional connectivity. Such data are sure to be of interest to numerous researchers due to their practical significance. That said, I have statistical concerns and am left wanting more mechanistic insight. Ultimately, I think more data and analyses will be needed to make a convincing enough case to the readership of this outlet.

1) The effect of parietal tRNS on object discrimination is truly massive. The tasks are titrated at the start to set performance at 50%. While performance remains the same following temporal and sham tRNS, parietal tRNS improves performance by 30%. Given this finding, I am rather convinced that parietal tRNS improves object discrimination. What I am not convinced of is that the effect size is as massive as is being depicted. Given that the main contribution of this study as it is current written is practical, having a reasonable estimate of effect size is important for applied work that will seek to build on this striking finding. However, given the small sample size (10 participants per group), and effect size inflation that occurs when conditionalizing on significance, especially in small samples, I suspect that the true effect size is substantially smaller than has been depicted. Unfortunately, I do not see a good way to get a better estimate of the true effect size without acquiring more data. The upshot is that new data would not require fMRI and may not even require control groups. A bare minimum would be to collect another group of 10 participants receiving parietal tRNS and estimate effect size based on their improvement. I think that such data collection would be a low cost effort, and the sample size is small enough to potentially be feasible amidst pandemic restrictions. Note that since the new sample would be used to estimate effect size, there need not be statistically significant improvement in the new sample to have utility.

We understand the request and rationale of the reviewer to collect additional data, and for this and other reasons, we decided to run further analyses. Specifically, we run Bayes factor analysis to estimate the amount of evidence present in the data. All details are reported above, in the first response to the Editor’s comments. We believe the additional analyses we conducted provide a clear evidence that the effect we found is not inflated.

2) These data beg the question of why parietal tRNS produces such a dramatic effect, but temporal tRNS does not. That is, one would like to have some mechanistic insight into why parietal tRNS is so effective, at least in the object discrimination task. The connectivity data do not, in their present form, address this question. They merely provide another metric of change without begetting further causal inference/insight. Fortunately, the authors have additional task fMRI data that could potential shed some light on the issue. For example, one might ask whether the extent to which a given task activates the stimulated areas is associated with changes in connectivity and/or behavior. Alternatively, the network position of the parietal target either generally or specific to a given task may be associated with its effective change in connectivity and/or behavior. Given that the authors have effects that are specific not only to target, but also differ as a function of task, I feel as though these data could be leveraged to provide substantial insights into what factors are critical for an effective intervention.

We agree with the reviewer’s suggestion that looking at task-related functional data might be interesting to better understand why parietal tRNS produces such a dramatic effect on behavior and on functional connectivity within the Dorsal and Ventral Attention network. It is indeed in our plan to carefully analyze and look at those data next in a follow-up manuscript. Those analyses will respond to a different set of questions; those indicated by the reviewer, and, in addition, whether the effect is driven by the type of task or by the effort required in switching between tasks and/or the “choice” of parietal functions to prioritize one task over the other. The wealth of information these analyses provide warrants a manuscript focused specifically on these questions which diverge from the current research question at hand. In the present study, we chose to observe functional connectivity changes at rest, activity that is not directly driven by the tasks, albeit we hypothesize it is the consequence of the stimulation and training sessions. Our choice was motivated by the fact that resting-state data have been shown to be particularly reliable and stable across subjects and across time (Damoiseaux et al., 2006), indicating that baseline activity of the brain exhibits consistent temporal and spatial dynamics that are also functionally relevant (e.g. dorsal and ventral attention networks have been found to be persistent in the absence of a task; Fox et al., 2006). Indeed, we found that resting-state connectivity within the attention network to be active at rest when measured in between attentional task runs and, importantly, to be significantly influenced by different stimulation protocols that are likely to have supported changes in behavior.

However, given the reviewer’s comment, we conducted a new set of analyses to investigate if the parietal regions we stimulated were also active during the tasks. In particular, we analyzed BOLD signal changes in response to the OD and the TOJ task in the pre-stimulation session, before any manipulation (e.g., stimulation and training) could potentially change the brain response to the tasks. Therefore, we included all subjects in the analysis (N=30) and we observed the brain areas significantly activated while performing the OD and the TOJ task. As visual stimuli were presented in rapid succession within each run, we analyzed BOLD activity using a deconvolution general lineal model (GLM) to separate the contribution of different events that could otherwise overlap. This deconvolution analysis included predictors for each volume (sampled at 2 sec), each task-condition, extending 20 sec following the onset of a trial. Statistical contrasts were estimated from the four volumes surround the peak of the hemodynamic response (4 sec post stimulus onset) for each voxel. In the first-level analysis, a GLM regression model was performed to fit data for each subject. Individual subject data was then combined into a deconvolution GLM in which β estimates on OD and TOJ trials were contrasted with estimates for baseline activity. This contrast identified brain areas associated with the OD task performance and TOJ performance, with significance assessed using a threshold false discovery rate of.05 (FDR corrected).

Figure 8 shows the results of a second-level deconvolution GLM analysis for significant positive BOLD response for the OD and the TOJ tasks versus rest (Figure 8, Panel A). Details of statistical maps with Talairach coordinates are reported in Table 2 and 3. Moreover, as suggested by the reviewer, we performed a simulation of the current distribution in the brain using ROAST (Huang et al., 2019) for the two stimulation conditions (Figure 8, Panel B for Parietal stimulation, and Panel C for hMT stimulation) and included them in the Figure to qualitatively assess the overlap between positive BOLD response and current distribution peaks over the cortex. As expected, the task-related BOLD activity for the OD and TOJ tasks peaked over the parietal lobes, which directly correspond to the hotspots where the peaks of the electrical current were localized during Parietal stimulation (Figure 8B). In contrast, current distribution during hMT stimulation peaked over occipital cortical regions. Interestingly, while the current distribution for Parietal stimulation remains relatively circumscribed to the posterior cortical areas, more anterior areas were significantly active during the tasks, as indicated by the BOLD response (Figure 8A). Thus, to directly answer the reviewer’s question, we conclude that the effects driven by Parietal stimulation are likely due to an effective targeting of selective task-active crucial nodes (e.g., IPS) that, as suggested by previous studies, are pivotal to task performance (Capotosto et al., 2013; Suzuki & Gottlieb, 2013; Corbetta & Shulman, 2002). However, we prefer to choose a more parsimonious interpretation of the results of the E-field models relative to the BOLD response for the following reasons. Despite the fact that the E-field models are being constantly updated, and they surely hold great promise for individualized tES modeling, their validation for some experimental condition including ours is still not sufficient. Recently, Callejón-leblic and Miranda (2021) acknowledge that the few in vivo data only show a moderate correlation with intracranial cortical recordings in humans (e.g. Opitz et al., 2018), and the results obtained using different modelling pipelines (Huang et al., 2019; Saturnino et al., 2019) are divergent (Puonti et al., 2020). Furthermore, an important limitation is the difficulty to predict whether a stimulation procedure will influence the site of stimulation only, or whether it will exert a network effect, as we found in our resting-state data. In addition, while functional data can be observed as a change across time and sessions (between pre- and post-stimulation and training), this parameter cannot be computed in the E-field model, as the outcome is based on fixed parameters such as electrodes’ size, location and intensity of stimulation but not on stimulation duration nor the number of sessions, which we believe to be a crucial factor in our study. Since the time variable cannot be included in the simulation, it becomes difficult to interpret the E-field model in a multi-day stimulation protocol, and it remains to be further investigated/implemented in the future.

Relatedly, the authors offer conclusions in the discussion regarding what constitutes a successful intervention, but they are either unsubstantiated by the present data, or unclear. For example, they state on page 20, lines 439-441:

"It is the combination of a challenging task with concurrent brain stimulation over the relevant cortical circuits that facilitates fast learning, a paradigm amenable to promote plasticity."

We have now edited the text and added some details to the Discussion section.

Similar sentiments are echoed on page 20, lines 451-453. Absent examination of task data, the relevant cortical circuits for the studied tasks are unclear. Consider that in Figure 9, the temporal targeted tRNS seems to induce the most stimulation in occipital areas. I would expect occipital cortex to be extremely relevant for the studied tasks, yet stimulation over those areas did not improve performance. Moreover, the importance of stimulation concurrent with tasks cannot be concluded from these data since there were no conditions that had no tasks. The differential performance across the studied tasks may potentially be leveraged here, but at the moment, this conclusion does not follow from the data. Since the authors have data that can help speak to some of these issues, it seems pertinent to use them.

We agree with the reviewer, and we have now changed the text and removed the statement about the importance of delivering stimulation concurrently with the task. However, in a preliminary dataset collected prior to the current study, we also had a condition with tRNS alone and without a concurrent task (whilst we used the same behavioral tasks at pre-stimulation baseline, and during the post-stimulation session). Those data are not published yet, but they have been presented at a meeting in December 2020 (Transcranial Brain Stimulation in Cognitive Neuroscience: https://event.unitn.it/tbs-cnw/#home, “Functional response of attention-related cortical networks to multi-sessions tRNS” Pergher, V., Contò, F. & Battelli L.). These unpublished preliminary data indicate that tRNS alone is not sufficient to boost learning with the magnitude we found when tRNS and behavioral training are run simultaneously.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

The manuscript has been greatly improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed, as outlined below. Though these revisions are essential, the manuscript will not be sent out for another round of external reviews and can be handled editorially.

1. While the Bayesian analyses are nice, they do not address the point that the effect sizes are likely to be inflated given the sample size. The issue is that when sample size is small and reporting of results is conditioned on statistical significance (e.g. p< 0.05) , effect size will be inflated. This needs to be acknowledged in the discussion. See, for example, the reference below for more discussion of inflated effect sizes in studies with small samples.

Yarkoni, T. (2009). Big correlations in little studies: Inflated fMRI correlations reflect low statistical power-Commentary on Vul et al. (2009). Perspectives on psychological science, 4(3), 294-298.

We have now addressed the reviewer’s point in the Discussion section by pointing out this potential limitation in our study.

2. Please report the analyses showing that the number of frames censored were similar across sessions in the manuscript.

We have now reported the analysis in the manuscript, in the section: “Stimulation and learning-dependent changes in functional connectivity”.

3. The task-based univariate results are quite illuminating. It would seem pertinent to mention in the manuscript that both tasks activate IPS in the discussion of why effects were observed for the OD task but not the TOJ task.

We agree, this is a very interesting point and we have now added a paragraph in the discussion (bottom of page 20) where we offer an interpretation of why we found this differential effect in behavior that is not evident in the task-based response at baseline. As we mentioned in our previous response, this clearly generates a set of interesting hypotheses that we plan to analyze next in a follow-up manuscript. We have also added the task-based functional connectivity analysis at the end of the methods section and modified Figure 8, where we now report the task-based univariate results (8A) and the current distribution results from ROAST (8B and C).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Contò F, Edwards G, Tyler S, Parrott D, Grossman ED, Battelli L. 2021. Behavioral data collected during pre_post sessions. Zenodo. [DOI]
    2. Contò F, Edwards G, Tyler S, Parrott D, Grossman ED, Battelli L. 2021. Behavioral data collected during training. Zenodo. [DOI]
    3. Contò F, Edwards G, Tyler S, Parrott D, Grossman ED, Battelli L. 2021. fMRI data collected during prepost sessions. Zenodo. [DOI]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated and analyzed during this study are available for review at the links reported in the Transparent Reporting File (Conto_transparent_reporting_resubmission). Moreover, a data structure explanation has also been provided (file name: Conto;_DataStuctureExplanation_resubmission). Data have been deposited to Zenodo and are available at the following DOIs: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4634008 https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5558975, https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4621644.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Contò F, Edwards G, Tyler S, Parrott D, Grossman ED, Battelli L. 2021. Behavioral data collected during pre_post sessions. Zenodo.

    Contò F, Edwards G, Tyler S, Parrott D, Grossman ED, Battelli L. 2021. Behavioral data collected during training. Zenodo.

    Contò F, Edwards G, Tyler S, Parrott D, Grossman ED, Battelli L. 2021. fMRI data collected during prepost sessions. Zenodo.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES