Abstract
Recent innovations in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (FMRI) have sped data collection by enabling simultaneous scans of neural activity in multiple brain locations, but have these innovations come at a cost? In a meta-analysis and preregistered direct comparison of original data, we examined whether acquiring FMRI data with multi-band versus single-band scanning protocols might compromise detection of mesolimbic activity during reward processing. Meta-analytic results (n = 44 studies; cumulative n = 5005 subjects) indicated that relative to single-band scans, multi-band scans showed significantly decreased effect sizes for reward anticipation in the Nucleus Accumbens (NAcc) by more than half. Direct within-subject comparison of single-band versus multi-band scanning data (multi-band factors = 4 and 8; n = 12 subjects) acquired during repeated administration of the Monetary Incentive Delay task indicated that reductions in temporal signal-to-noise ratio could account for compromised detection of task-related responses in mesolimbic regions (i.e., the NAcc). Together, these findings imply that researchers should opt for single-band over multi-band scanning protocols when probing mesolimbic responses with FMRI. The findings also have implications for inferring mesolimbic activity during related tasks and rest, for summarizing historical results, and for using neuroimaging data to track individual differences in reward-related brain activity.
Keywords: Multi-band, Functional magnetic resonance imaging, Monetary incentive delay, Reward, Anticipation, Accumbens, Frontal, Human
1. Introduction
During the first two decades of the twenty-first century, Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (FMRI) revolutionized cognitive neuroscience (Poldrack, 2012). For the first time, researchers could acquire measures of whole brain neural activity (in the form of Blood Oxygen Level Dependent or BOLD activity) at a spatial scale of millimeters and a temporal scale of seconds. This allowed investigators to probe not only neural correlates of sensory and motor processing in living humans, but also diverse intermediate brain processes related to thought and emotion (Rosen and Savoy, 2012). With respect to emotion, FMRI evidence now supports a substantial literature indicating that subcortical circuits play a central role in human reward processing for basic rewards (e.g., juice) as well as more abstract rewards (e.g., money). Different subregions of these neural circuits respond to reward anticipation as well as receipt (Knutson and Cooper, 2005). Using parametric probe tasks, researchers have localized neural activity that scales not only as a function of sensorimotor stimulation, but also as a function of reward magnitude and probability (Knutson et al., 2005; Yacubian et al., 2006). Meta-analyses of hundreds of FMRI studies now indicate that while reward processing engages diverse circuits, the ventral striatum including the Nucleus Accumbens (NAcc) as well as the Medial PreFrontal Cortex (MPFC), are among those regions most likely to show correlated activity (Bartra et al., 2013; Clithero and Rangel, 2014; Knutson and Greer, 2008).
More recently, however, researchers have raised concerns about the robustness and replicability of task-related FMRI findings. These concerns have focused on a number of issues including inadequate power (Button et al., 2013), statistical inflation (Vul et al., 2009), temporal instability (Elliott et al., 2020), and analytic inconsistency (Botvinik-Nezer et al., 2020). Although these issues might have arisen from problems related to study design, analysis, or interpretation, more technical factors related to data acquisition have concurrently changed but have received less scrutiny. For instance, to acquire data more rapidly, researchers have begun to scan brain images simultaneously at different locations (i.e., using multi-band simultaneous slice excitation protocols, hereafter referred to as “multi-band” scans), rather than sequentially and individually (i.e., using single-band individual slice excitation protocols, hereafter referred to as “single-band” scans; Moeller et al., 2010). Multi-band scans confer a significant benefit of yielding the same amount of data in a fraction of the time required by single-band scans. Multi-band scans may, however, incur hidden costs. Despite not diminishing signal-to-noise ratio in cortical regions, multi-band scanning may induce noise in subcortical regions (Todd et al., 2017). For instance, multi-band scanning can substantially increase noise in the subcortical activity of individuals at rest (Risk et al., 2018, 2021), as well as during execution of sensorimotor and cognitive tasks (Demetriou et al., 2018; Todd et al., 2017).
Since reward responses robustly recruit subcortical as well as cortical circuits, we sought to determine whether multi-band versus single-band scanning might systematically influence FMRI detection of neural responses to reward. To do so, we first meta-analyzed the past decade of FMRI findings (i.e., from 2010 to 2020) elicited by a reliable and valid probe of reward processing: the Monetary Incentive Delay or MID task (Knutson et al., 2001; Wu et al., 2014). We specifically aimed to compare responses in the NAcc during reward anticipation and in the MPFC to reward outcomes across studies utilizing single-band versus multi-band scanning protocols.
Next, to reproduce and diagnose potential problems while controlling for possible confounds, we acquired and directly compared MID task data from the same subjects, scanner, and site. Scans used a common protocol which varied only with respect to the speed of image acquisition, coverage, and associated flip angle (i.e., with Multi-Band factors: MB = 1, 4, or 8; Table 1). Analyses focused on raw averaged neural activity extracted from NAcc and MPFC volumes of interest as well as on modeled activity across the entire brain. Based on previous resting state (Risk et al., 2018) and task-related (Demetriou et al., 2018; Todd et al., 2017) findings, we predicted that multi-band versus single-band scanning might compromise detection of reward-related FMRI activity by inducing noise near the center of the brain.
Table 1.
MB 1 | MB 4 | MB 8 | |
---|---|---|---|
Voxel size | 3.4 × 3.4 × 3.4 | 3.4 × 3.4 × 3.4 | 3.4 × 3.4 × 3.4 |
Freq. FOV | 23.8 | 23.8 | 23.8 |
Phase FOV | 1 | 1 | 1 |
TE | 25ms | 25ms | 25ms |
Phase encoding | PA | PA | PA |
Base resolution | 70 × 70 at 3.4mm | 70 × 70 at 3.4mm | 70 × 70 at 3.4mm |
Pixel bandwidth | 7143Hz | 7143Hz | 7143Hz |
Echo spacing | 0.5 | 0.5 | 0.5 |
Flip angle | 77 | 42 | 42 |
Number of slices | 41 | 32 | 41 |
TR | 2000ms | 500ms | 500ms |
2. Methods
2.1. Meta-analysis of historical data
To survey the neuroimaging literature for historical evidence that multi-band scanning might have compromised detection of reward-related responses, we conducted a meta-analysis of Volume Of Interest (VOI) FMRI data collected as subjects participated in the MID task. Data came from literature searches using Google Scholar and PubMed for studies published between 2010 and 2020 that used the terms “Monetary Incentive Delay Task AND FMRI”. Single-band and multi-band FMRI studies of healthy adults and/or adolescents were selected if they contrasted gain versus nongain anticipation, and a subset of these studies also contrasted gain versus nongain outcomes. In studies with both healthy and clinical samples, only peak activation values from the healthy samples were included in the analysis. Key exclusion criteria included unpublished preprints (unless the number of subjects exceeded 100), studies that included subjects under twelve years old, functional connectivity studies, and studies using dynamic tasks that required updating of expectations (Table S1 lists excluded studies along with reasons for exclusion). The meta-analysis focused on whether detection of neural responses during gain anticipation in the Nucleus Accumbens (NAcc) and gain outcomes in the Medial PreFrontal Cortex (MPFC) was compromised in studies using multi-band versus single-band scanning.
For meta-analysis, forest plots facilitated direct comparison of reward-related responses (i.e., effect sizes (Cohen’s d) and standard errors) for single-band versus multi-band studies (Radua et al., 2015; Viechtbauer, 2010). Funnel plots then probed for publication bias across these subsets of studies (Egger et al., 1997) using a fixed effects model (R package ‘metafor; Viechtbauer, 2010). Finally, other variables that correlated with reward-related responses were identified (i.e., subject age, publication date, sample size, flip angle) and included in a multivariate ANalysis Of COVAriance (ANCOVA) to verify the independence and robustness of single-band versus multi-band scanning as a predictor of neural responses to reward.
2.2. Direct comparison of original data
Following meta-analysis, we sought to reproduce and diagnose potential problems by acquiring and comparing data using three scanning protocols that varied with respect to multi-band factor (i.e., MB1, MB4, MB8; Table 1). Analyses aimed to localize potential problems in space (i.e., neural location) as well as in time (i.e., during the task versus throughout the entire scan). To control for order effects, subjects completed three runs of the MID task, with the order of the three multi-band factor scans counterbalanced across subjects. To control for subject, scanner, and site, each subject completed all three scan protocols within the same session. All data were preprocessed and analyzed using the same pipelines. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Stanford University School of Medicine, and all subjects provided written informed consent prior to participating in the study.
2.2.1. Subjects
Preregistered power analysis indicated that for a within-subject comparison, 6 subjects should yield adequate power to detect an effect of multi-band acquisition on the gain versus nongain anticipation contrast in the NAcc during the MID task (g*power 3.1.9.2; beta=0.95, alpha=0.05, two-tailed, paired, d = 1.97; Wu et al., 2014). As planned in the preregistration, we doubled this sample size to guard against the possibility of a smaller effect size (n = 12; between-subjects). Nineteen subjects completed the MID task while being scanned with a multi-band acquisition protocol. According to the preregistered exclusion criteria, data from three subjects were excluded due to excessive motion during at least one of the three task runs, while data from four subjects were excluded due to equipment failure (i.e., faulty response registration by a new button box), leaving twelve subjects total for analyses.
2.2.2. Task
The MID task was identical across all three runs. The six task trial conditions included: + $5.00 (‘large gain’); +$1.00 (‘medium gain’); +$0.00 (‘nongain’); −$5.00 (‘large loss’); −$1.00 (‘medium loss’); and −$0.00 (‘nonloss’) trials. Each trial condition was repeated 12 times in a pseudorandom order, totaling 72 trials. Trial timing was as follows: cue presentation (seconds 0–2); anticipatory fixation (seconds 2–4); target presentation (appearing briefly between seconds 4–4.5); outcome presentation (seconds 6–8); and a variable Inter-Trial Interval (ITI lasting 2, 4, or 6 s). Thus, each trial lasted an average of 12 s (including the ITI). Adaptive timing of target duration within condition ensured that subjects succeeded in “hitting” targets on approximately 66% of the trials (Knutson et al., 2005). Thus, each MID task run lasted 864 total seconds (approximately 14.4 min), and all three runs were acquired during a single session, but with counterbalanced ordering across subjects.
2.2.3. FMRI acquisition
All data were acquired on a 3 Tesla General Electric scanner with a 32-channel head coil at the Stanford Center for Cognitive and Neurobiological Imaging (CNI). Structural (T1-weighted) scans were first acquired for all participants. Functional (T2-weighted) images for single-band and multi-band scans were then acquired using the following common parameters: TE=25 ms, FOV=23.8 × 23.8 cm; acquisition matrix=70 × 70, no gap, phase encoding=PA, voxel dimensions=3.4 × 3.4 × 3.4 mm. Additional parameters that varied between scanning protocols included: (1) multi-band factor=1, TR=2000 msec, flip angle=77°, number of slices=41; (2) multi-band factor=4, TR=500 msec, flip angle=42°, number of slices=32; (3) multi-band factor=8, TR=500 msec, flip angle=42°, number of slices=41. All FMRI data were reconstructed using 1D-GRAPPA (Blaimer et al., 2013).
2.2.4. Data preprocessing
Data were analyzed with AFNI software (Cox, 1996). After removal of initial calibration volumes (12 s, i.e., 6 vol for MB1 and 24 vol for MB4 and MB8 scans) from each FMRI scan, the following pre-processing steps were performed: (1) slice-timing correction using sync interpolation; (2) motion correction; (3) spatial smoothing (with a 4 mm full-width at half maximum Gaussian kernel; Sacchet and Knutson, 2013); (4) conversion of each voxel’s time series to percent signal change over each run; (5) application of a high-pass filter (removing frequencies below 1 cycle / 90 s or 0.011 Hz). In a supplementary analysis comparing potential effects of temporal smoothing, we applied a band-pass filter with the same high-pass parameters, but varying low-pass parameters (0.25, 0.20, or 0.15 Hz). Affine transformation matrices were then estimated to align functional data to anatomical scans in native space and anatomical scans in individual space to standard group space. These transformations were concatenated and used to transform functional data into a standard group space (i.e., Montreal Neurological Institute or MNI coordinates). Functional data were subsequently visualized in standard space to ensure adequate co-registration (using the MNI anatomical template “mni_icbm152_t1_tal_nlin_asym_09a”).
2.2.5. Data analysis
All functional data were extracted and plotted from five predicted Volumes Of Interest (VOIs): bilateral Nucleus Accumbens (NAcc; CIT168 subcortical atlas; Pauli et al., 2018), bilateral Anterior Insula (AIns; Brainnetome atlas labels 167 & 168; Fan et al., 2016), bilateral Medial PreFrontal Cortex (MPFC; 8-mm diameter spheres centered on MNI: ±5, 50, −1; Knutson et al., 2003), bilateral Primary Visual Cortex (V1; HCP MMP 1.0; Glasser et al., 2016), and left Primary Motor Cortex (M1; AFNI’s TT_Daemon left Precentral Gyrus, transformed to MNI space). Activity from these VOIs was spatially averaged within subject for each task condition, averaged across subjects, and plotted with the standard error for each group for large gain (+$5.00) and loss (−$5.00) trials as well as for non-incentive trials (+$0.00, −$0.00). Trials were separately averaged as a function of whether subjects “hit” or “missed” the target. Consistent with preregistered criteria, brain volumes with excessive motion were excluded (i.e., > 1 mm displacement from one volume acquisition to the next; determined by derivative measures generated by the motion correction algorithm). Values exceeding three standard deviations were also excluded from further analyses. All contrasts from the anticipation period were derived by averaging across time points corresponding to a 4 s anticipation phase (i.e., cue and anticipatory fixation period lasting seconds 0–4 after trial onset) after accounting for a 6 s hemodynamic lag. Contrasts from the outcome period were derived similarly, but averaged time points corresponding to the 2 s outcome phase (i.e., lasting seconds 6–8 after trial onset).
Each subject’s whole brain preprocessed functional data were submitted to a generalized linear regression model including the following four orthogonal regressors of interest: (1) gain (+$5.00) vs. nongain (+$0.00) anticipation (including both cue and anticipatory fixation period); (2) gain (+$5.00) vs. nongain (+$0.00) outcome; (3) loss (−$5.00) vs. nonloss (−$0.00) anticipation; and (4) nonloss (−$0.00) vs. loss (−$5.00) outcome (Knutson et al., 2003). This regression model also included two unit regressors highlighting anticipation and outcome phases of each trial, six motion parameters to control for motion effects, and average activity from cerebrospinal fluid and white matter VOIs to control for physiological noise (Chang and Glover, 2009). Model regressors were convolved with a single gamma hemodynamic response function prior to inclusion in the regression model (Cohen, 1997). To account for fast sampling rates in the multi-band data, we additionally implemented the same temporal autocorrelation algorithm across all acquisition protocols (i.e., generalized least square time series fit with restricted maximum likelihood estimation of the temporal auto-correlation structure using AFNI’s 3dREMLfit). Multiple regression models first fitted each subject’s data in their native space. Maps of resulting t-statistics were then converted into Z-scores and warped into standard (MNI) space prior to group comparisons. Within-subject group analyses were subsequently conducted using a mixed effects model with multi-band factor as a fixed effect and subjects as random effects (with AFNI’s 3dANOVA2).
To obtain whole-brain measures of signal and noise in the minimally preprocessed (including slice-timing correction, motion correction, and slight spatial smoothing) unmodeled data, we calculated the average, standard deviation, and Temporal Signal-to-Noise Ratio (TSNR) of the timeseries over the first 100 vol of each functional scan (Chen and Glover, 2015; Demetriou et al., 2018). An additional TSNR calculation involved downsampling all volumes from the multi-band scans acquired every half second (by averaging every four volumes) to the same temporal resolution as single-band scans acquired every two seconds (Todd et al., 2017). Prior to group-level analysis, preprocessed data were warped into standard (MNI) space. As with task-based analyses, whole-brain within-subject group comparisons were implemented using a mixed effects model with multi-band factor as a fixed effect and subjects as random effects.
3. Results
3.1. Meta-analysis of historical data
Meta-analyses targeted MID task contrasts of interest in predicted VOIs (i.e., gain versus nongain anticipation in the NAcc and gain versus nongain outcome in the MPFC) (Knutson and Greer, 2008) for studies published from 2010 to 2020 (n = 44 studies; cumulative n = 5005 subjects; Table 2). Standard error values were calculated by dividing the standard deviation (i.e., 1 for standardized scores) by the square root of the sample size.
Table 2.
Study | Year | Subjects | N | Scanner brand |
Field strength |
Band | Band factor |
FWHM | Voxel dimensions | Voxel size | Flip angle |
Gvnant effect size |
Gvnant SE | Gvnant VOI | Gvnout effect size |
Gvnout SE | Gvnout VOI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Behan et al. | 2015 | Adults | 20 | Philips | 3 | single-band | 1 | 4.2 | 3.5 × 3.5 × 3.5 | 42.875 | 90 | 2.131272168 | 0.223606798 | NAcc | |||
Bjork et al. | 2010 | Adolescents | 24 | GE | 3 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.75 × 3.75 × 3.75 | 52.73438 | 90 | 1.937515935 | 0.204124145 | NAcc | 1.472528792 | 0.204124145 | MPFC |
Bjork et al. | 2010 | Adults | 24 | GE | 3 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.75 × 3.75 × 3.75 | 52.73438 | 90 | 3.219045524 | 0.204124145 | Caudate | 2.139357888 | 0.204124145 | MPFC |
Bjork et al. | 2012 | Adults | 23 | GE | 3 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.75 × 3.75 × 3.75 | 52.73438 | 90 | 3.111451254 | 0.208514414 | NAcc | |||
Bustamante et al. | 2014 | Adults | 18 | Siemens | 1.5 | Single-band | 1 | 8 | 3 ×3 ×3 | 27 | 90 | 2.696433859 | 0.23570226 | VS | 1.92804449 | 0.23570226 | MPFC |
Cao et al. | 2019 | Adolescents | 1510 | Siemens | 3 | single-band | 1 | 5 | 3.44 × 3.44 × 3.4 | 40.23 | 75 | 2.312731009 | 0.025734251 | NAcc | 1.329871816 | 0.025734251 | VMPFC |
Carl et al. | 2016 | Adults | 20 | GE | 3 | single-band | 1 | 5 | 3.75 × 3.75 × 4.0 | 56.25 | 60 | 1.994572636 | 0.223606798 | NAcc | 2.20029089 | 0.223606798 | ACC |
Cho et al. | 2013 | Adolescents | 24 | GE | 3 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.75 × 3.75 × 4 | 56.25 | 90 | 2.072633276 | 0.204124145 | NAcc | |||
Cho et al. | 2013 | Adults | 30 | GE | 3 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.75 × 3.75 × 4.0 | 56.25 | 90 | 2.343475168 | 0.182574186 | NAcc | |||
Damiano et al. | 2014 | Adults | 31 | GE | 3 | Single-band | 1 | 5 | 4 × 4 × 4 | 64 | 77 | 2.370789987 | 0.179605302 | NAcc | 1.620039824 | 0.179605302 | ACC |
Enzi et al. | 2012 | Adults | 15 | Siemens | 1.5 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.8 × 3.8 × 3.8 | 54.872 | 90 | 2.556169008 | 0.25819889 | VS | |||
Gola et al. | 2017 | Adults | 24 | Siemens | 3 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.5 × 3.5 × 3.5 | 42.875 | 90 | 3.857142857 | 0.204124145 | NAcc | |||
Herbort et al. | 2016 | Adults | 23 | Siemens | 3 | Single-band | 1 | 8 | 3 × 3 × 3 | 27 | 70 | 4.681887731 | 0.208514414 | NAcc | |||
Hoogman et al. | 2013 | Adults | 77 | Siemens | 1.5 | Single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.5 × 3.5 × 3 | 36.75 | 90 | 1.578380249 | 0.113960576 | VS | |||
Johnson et al. | 2019 | Adults | 24 | GE | 1.5 | Single-band | 1 | 4 | 3.44 × 3.44 × 4.0 | 47.33 | 90 | 2.706686166 | 0.204124145 | NAcc | 2.180045871 | 0.204124145 | MPFC |
Juckel et al. | 2012 | Adults | 13 | Siemens | 1.5 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 4 × 4 × 3.3 | 52.8 | 90 | 1.986084926 | 0.277350098 | VS | |||
Kim et al. | 2020 | Adults | 17 | Siemens | 3 | single-band | 1 | 6 | 3.1 × 3.1 × 3 | 28.83 | 90 | 2.144014925 | 0.242535625 | NAcc | |||
Kirk et al. | 2015 | Adults | 44 | Siemens | 3 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.4 × 3.4 × 4 | 46.24 | 90 | 2.443027097 | 0.150755672 | Caudate | 2.116680156 | 0.150755672 | VMPFC |
Kollmann et al. | 2017 | Adults | 41 | Siemens | 3 | Single-band | 1 | 7 | 2.3 × 2.3 × 3 | 15.87 | 90 | 2.136457063 | 0.156173762 | VS | |||
Martucci et al. | 2018 | Adults | 15 | GE | 3 | Single-band | 1 | 4 | 3.4 × 3.4 × 4 | 46.24 | 76 | 1.442831815 | 0.25819889 | VS | 0.407883101 | 0.25819889 | MPFC |
Mucci et al. | 2015 | Adults | 22 | Philips | 3 | single-band | 1 | 6 | 3.59 × 3.59 × 4 | 51.55 | 90 | 2.348024499 | 0.213200716 | VS | 2.553149316 | 0.213200716 | MPFC |
Pfabigan et al. | 2014 | Adults | 25 | Siemens | 3 | Single-band | 1 | 8 | 1.5 × 1.5 × 3 | 6.75 | 60 | 4.008998212 | 0.2 | VS | |||
Rademacher et al. | 2010 | Adults | 28 | Philips | 1.5 | Single-band | 1 | 6 | 3.75 × 3.75 × 3.8 | 53.4375 | 90 | 2.358498312 | 0.188982237 | NAcc | 1.440044642 | 0.188982237 | ACC |
Romanczuk-Seiferth et al. | 2015 | Adults | 17 | Siemens | 3 | Single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.5 × 3.5 × 3.0 | 36.75 | 80 | 2.0955078 | 0.242535625 | Putamen | 2.047000675 | 0.242535625 | ACC |
Sescousse et al. | 2010 | Adults | 18 | Siemens | 1.5 | Single-band | 1 | 10 | 3.4 × 3.4 × 4 | 46.24 | 90 | 3.094754575 | 0.23570226 | VS | |||
Sescousse et al. | 2013 | Adults | 20 | Siemens | 1.5 | Single-band | 1 | 10 | 3.4 × 3.4 × 4 | 46.24 | 90 | 2.666666667 | 0.223606798 | VS | 2.716282289 | 0.223606798 | Ant.Orb.Gyrus |
Simon et al. | 2010 | Adults | 24 | Siemens | 3 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 3 × 3 × 3 | 27 | 80 | 2.327020861 | 0.204124145 | VS | 2.49800268 | 0.204124145 | MPFC |
Stoy et al. | 2011 | Adults | 12 | Siemens | 1.5 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 4 × 4 × 3.3 | 52.8 | 90 | 2.222798536 | 0.288675135 | Putamen | |||
Stoy et al. | 2012 | Adults | 15 | Siemens | 1.5 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 4 × 4 × 3.3 | 52.8 | 90 | 3.287313276 | 0.25819889 | Putamen | |||
Treadway et al. | 2013 | Adults | 38 | Philips | 3 | Single-band | 1 | 6 | 1.87 × 1.87 × 2.75 | 9.62 | 90 | 2.533898598 | 0.162221421 | VS | 1.988834623 | 0.162221421 | MPFC |
Ubl et al. | 2015 | Adults | 28 | Siemens | 3 | Single-band | 1 | 6 | 2.3 × 2.3 × 3 | 15.87 | 90 | 2.041008154 | 0.188982237 | Caudate | 2.075024957 | 0.188982237 | ACC |
Weiland et al. | 2014 | Adolescents | 12 | GE | 3 | Single-band | 1 | 6 | 3.12 × 3.12 × 4 | 38.94 | 90 | 4.359854043 | 0.288675135 | VS | |||
Wu et al. | 2014 | Adults | 52 | GE | 1.5 | single-band | 1 | 4 | 3.75 × 3.75 × 3.75 | 52.73438 | 90 | 1.858245657 | 0.138675049 | NAcc | 1.622498074 | 0.138675049 | MPFC |
Yan et al. | 2016 | Adults | 22 | Siemens | 3 | single-band | 1 | 8 | 3.4 × 3.4 × 4 | 46.24 | 90 | 3.006130101 | 0.213200716 | VS | 1.910278419 | 0.213200716 | MPFC |
Yau et al. | 2012 | Adults | 20 | GE | 3 | single-band | 1 | 6 | 3.12 × 3.12 × 3.12 | 30.37133 | 90 | 4.271720965 | 0.223606798 | VS | |||
Wimmer et al. | 2016 | Adults | 29 | Siemens | 3 | Multi-band | 2 | 6 | 2 × 2 × 2 | 8 | 60 | 2.18006327 | 0.185695338 | VS | |||
McGonigle et al. | 2017 | Adults | 43 | Siemens | 3 | Multi-band | 2 | 6 | 3.516 × 3.516 × 3.516 | 43.46569 | 80 | 1.781183301 | 0.15249857 | NAcc | |||
Dhingra et al. | 2020 | Adults | 54 | Siemens | 3 | Multi-band | 3 | 8 | 3 × 3 × 3 | 27 | 62 | 1.273734666 | 0.136082763 | VS | |||
Green et al. | 2019 | Adults | 14 | Siemens | 3 | Multi-band | 3 | 4 | 1.5 × 1.5 × 1.5 | 3.375 | 80 | 1.68982758 | 0.185695338 | Caudate | |||
Demidenko et al. | 2020 | Adolescents | 104 | GE | 3 | Multi-band | 6 | 5 | 2.4 × 2.4 × 2.4 | 13.824 | 52 | 1.103568792 | 0.098058068 | NAcc | 0.441261304 | 0.098058068 | MPFC |
Demidenko et al. | 2021 | Adolescents | 104 | GE | 3 | Multi-band | 6 | 5 | 2.4 × 2.4 × 2.4 | 13.824 | 52 | 1.517407088 | 0.098058068 | NAcc | |||
Huang et al. | 2019 | Adolescents | 29 | Siemens | 3 | Multi-band | 6 | 6 | 2.4 × 2.4 × 2.4 | 13.824 | 77 | 1.455851451 | 0.185695338 | VS | |||
Pornpattananangkul et al. | 2020 | Adolescents | 2222 | Siemens | 3 | Multi-band | 6 | 2 | 2.4 × 2.4 × 2.4 | 13.824 | 52 | 0.476595745 | 0.021214264 | NAcc | |||
Welborn et al. | 2020 | Adults | 36 | Siemens | 3 | Multi-band | 8 | 6 | 2 × 2 × 2 | 8 | 52 | 1.795107637 | 0.166666667 | NAcc |
In direct comparisons, forest plots indicated that although both single-band and multi-band effect sizes robustly exceeded a null effect (i.e., d = 0), their magnitudes also differed (Fig. 1). Specifically, gain versus nongain anticipation contrasts in the NAcc all showed robust effect sizes, ranging from “large” to “huge ” (Cohen, 1992), but peak NAcc responses for gain versus nongain anticipation contrasts in single-band studies (weighted mean d = 2.39) were significantly larger than those in multi-band studies (weighted mean d = 0.64; t(42)=4.16, p<0.001). Similarly, peak MPFC responses for gain versus nongain outcomes in single-band studies (weighted mean d = 1.47) were larger than in the multi-band study (single study d = 0.44). Thus, single-band effect sizes were at least three times as large as multi-band effect sizes.
Next, we examined whether these differences might be attributable to a publication bias favoring significant effects in single-band studies. Funnel plots indicated that for the peak NAcc gain versus nongain anticipation contrasts, single-band studies showed less (rather than more) evidence of publication bias than did multi-band studies (i.e., fewer studies to the right of the funnel; Fig. 2). Quantitative comparison of the association of effect size with standard error (Egger et al., 1997) revealed that although single-band studies showed some evidence of association (z = 2.64, p = 0.0083), multi-band studies showed even stronger evidence (z = 5.64, p<0.0001), and that the difference between multi-band and single-band associations was significant (z = 3.00, p<0.01). Since these tests suggested that publication bias was larger (rather than smaller) for multi-band studies, selection effects could not account for the smaller effect sizes observed in multi-band studies.
Multivariate statistical tests further confirmed the robustness of these findings against potential confounds. Specifically, peak NAcc responses to gain versus nongain anticipation did not significantly differ as a function of scanner brand (Siemens d = 2.38±0.18 versus GE d = 2.45±0.24 versus Philips d = 2.34±0.45, F(2,41)=2.15, p = 0.13) or field strength (1.5 T d = 2.46±0.16 versus 3.0 T d = 2.38±0.17, t(42)=0.25, p = 0.80). Statistical comparisons of the localization of peak coordinates further indicated that activation foci did not shift as a function of slice acquisition protocol (e.g., Sacchet and Knutson 2013). Specifically, for the gain versus nongain contrast in the ventral striatum, peak coordinates for single-band versus multi-band studies did not differ in x (t(42)=0.67, p = 0. 51), y (t(42)=−0.22, p = 0. 83), or z (t(42)=−0.73, p = 0.47) coordinates.
Statistical comparisons did, however, reveal some predicted but distinct effects. For instance, adolescent versus adult samples trended towards showing reduced NAcc gain versus nongain anticipation contrast peaks (adolescent d = 1.90±0.41 versus adult d = 2.51±0.13, t(42)=1.80; p = 0.08), consistent with previous developmental comparisons using the MID task (e.g., Bjork et al. 2004). Separate regression analyses also confirmed that increases in publication date (standardized (std.) r=−0.42; t(42)=−2.98, p = 0.005) and number of subjects (std. r=−0.31; t(42)=−2.14, p = 0.038) were associated with decreased effect sizes, but that image acquisition flip angle (std. r = 0.36; t(42)=2.50, p = 0. 016) was associated with increased effect sizes. Voxel size, however, was not significantly associated with effect sizes as expected (std. r=−0.03; t(42)=−1.16, p = 0.252). Next, potentially correlated confounds (i.e., subject age, publication date, sample size, and flip angle) were included in a multivariate analysis to verify the robustness of the association of single-band versus multi-band scanning with effect size. An ANalysis of COVAriance (ANCOVA) which included all of these correlated variables revealed that only variation in single-band versus multi-band scanning continued to be associated with NAcc response during gain versus nongain anticipation (F(1,37)=7.68, p = 0.009; Table 3). Within multi-band studies (n = 9), the association of multi-band factor with NAcc gain versus nongain effect size was negative as predicted, but not significant (r(9) =−0.40, p = 0.28), possibly due to the small number of multi-band studies available for analysis (Todd et al., 2017). An insufficient number of available studies including the gain versus nongain outcome contrast precluded parallel meta-analysis of gain outcome findings.
Table 3.
Cases | Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 0.051 | 1 | 0.051 | 0.089 | 0.767 |
Band | 4.394 | 1 | 4.394 | 7.676 | 0.009 |
Age * Band | 0.521 | 1 | 0.521 | 0.910 | 0.346 |
Date | 0.165 | 1 | 0.165 | 0.289 | 0.594 |
Sample size | 0.903 | 1 | 0.903 | 1.578 | 0.217 |
Flip angle | 0.487 | 1 | 0.487 | 0.851 | 0.362 |
Residuals | 21.182 | 37 | 0.572 |
Note. Type III Sum of Squares.
Together, meta-analytic findings implied that use of multi-band versus single-band scans can compromise detection of brain responses to reward anticipation and possibly outcomes. The substantial decrement (of over half) in effect sizes was not attributable to potential confounds (e.g., scanner brand or field strength) or other correlated variables (e.g., subject age, publication date, sample size, flip angle).
3.2. Direct comparison of original data
Although the meta-analysis indicated that multi-band scanning might compromise detection of reward-related activity, reasons for this compromise remained unclear. For instance, the findings did not clarify whether the problem resulted from decreased signal or increased noise and could not elucidate whether the problem was localized in space (e.g., influencing activity in the center of the brain more than the periphery) or time (e.g., occurring throughout the scan or only during particular task conditions). Thus, we sought to reproduce and diagnose the problem by directly comparing original MID task data acquired with single-band and multi-band scanning protocols. To test the critical hypotheses, activity time course data were extracted from predicted VOIs, as well as from sensory and motor (i.e., V1 and left M1) control VOIs (see Fig. 3). Whole-brain analyses further contrasted statistical estimates of both task-independent and task-dependent signal and noise (see Fig. 4).
As in meta-analytic findings, group contrasts revealed large or huge effect sizes for gain versus nongain anticipation contrasts in the NAcc VOI for single-band data, but significantly reduced effect sizes for multi-band scans (single-band: d = 1.98, SEM=0.11; MB4: d = 1.77, SEM=0.08; MB8: d = 1.21, SEM=0.05; std. ß=−3.00, p = 0.004). The effect size for gain versus nongain outcome contrasts in the MPFC VOI was also slightly higher for single-band than for multi-band scans, but this difference was not significant (single-band: d = 1.44, SEM=0.13, MB4: d = 1.09, SEM=0.15; MB8: d = 0.88, SEM=0.18; std. ß=−0.37, p = 0.70). Previous research using the MID task has also documented significant AIns, V1, and M1 responses during gain versus nongain anticipation (e.g., Knutson et al. 2003). These control VOIs yielded large effect sizes which did not significantly differ between single-band and multi-band scans (Fig. 3; all ds> 1.5, all std. ß values<1.82, all p-values >0.05). Statistical tests also controlled for within-subject order effects of acquisition protocols and revealed no significant effects of order on brain activity, except for the AIns VOI (std. ß=2.44, p = 0.016, all std. ß values for other VOIs <1.26, all other p-values >0.20). Together, these findings suggest spatial specificity, since multi-band versus single-band scans compromised detection of reward-related activity in mesolimbic regions of interest (e.g., near the NAcc), but not in regions closer to the edge of the brain (e.g., V1 and M1; which lie closer to the radiofrequency coils).
While visual inspection of the single-band activity time courses revealed the predicted increases in activity in the NAcc and AIns (as well as in primary visual and motor cortices) during gain versus nongain anticipation, and also in MPFC activity in response to gain versus nongain outcomes (e.g., Wu et al. 2014), high frequency oscillatory activity in the mesolimbic VOIs was also apparent (Fig. 3B). Inspection of periodograms of these regions’ activity confirmed a frequency peak around 1 Hz, (Supplemental Fig. S1). To explore whether temporal filtering of these frequencies could reduce NAcc noise in multi-band scans, we applied low-pass temporal filters (of 0.25, 0.20, and 0.15 Hz) to the data (Fig. S2). Subsequent analyses revealed, however, that low-pass filtering had no significant effect on findings in any region (all std. ßs < 0.33, all p-values > 0.70). Instead, scan sequence continued to exert a main effect on NAcc (std. ß=−3.07, p = 0.002) as well as V1 (std. ß=−;2.66, p = 0.008) activity (Fig. S3).
To diagnose the spatial and temporal specificity of the findings, signal and noise indices were compared in both unmodeled and modeled data. First, the mean, standard deviation, and Temporal Signal-to-Noise Ratio (TSNR) of the unmodeled activity timeseries in each of the functional scans were calculated. Next, coefficients for the gain versus nongain anticipation contrast and the standard deviation of the regression residuals were calculated (Fig. 4). While both the mean and standard deviation of the raw signal were lower across the whole brain in the multi-band data, TSNR (calculated as the ratio of the mean over standard deviation over the first 100 vol (Demetriou et al., 2018; Fig. 4) or per unit time (in two second increments; Todd et al., 2016; Fig. S4) was specifically lower in subcortical regions that overlapped with the mesolimbic VOIs. In the modeled multi-band scan data, gain versus nongain anticipation contrast coefficients were not significantly lower in the NAcc but the standard deviation of task fit residuals was larger, particularly in mesolimbic regions. As with activity time course plots, these analyses suggested that model-independent noise in mesolimbic regions might compromise detection of reward-related FMRI activity in multi-band scans.
Finally, a mediation analysis tested whether overall TSNR might statistically mediate the influence of multi-band acquisition on reward-related activity in the NAcc. A linear mixed effects regression on NAcc gain versus nongain activity included spatially averaged TSNR in the NAcc VOI as a mediator, with multi-band factor and order as fixed effects and subjects as random intercepts. Multi-band factor was associated with decreased effect size for NAcc gain versus nongain anticipation (std.ß=−3.25, p<0.007) as well as with decreased NAcc TSNR (std. ß=−7.48, p<0.00001). Further, NAcc TSNR was positively associated with NAcc gain versus nongain anticipation effect size (std. ß=4.25, p<0.001). After statistically controlling for TSNR, the association between multi-band factor and NAcc gain versus nongain anticipation effect size was significantly diminished (std. ß=−3.25, p<0.002) and rendered nonsignificant. A bootstrapped causal mediation model with 3000 simulations (R package ‘mediation 4.5.0′; Tingley et al., 2014) revealed that NAcc TSNR could fully mediate the association between multi-band factor and NAcc gain versus nongain anticipation effect size (average causal mediation effect: ß=−0.046, p = 0.017, 95% quasi-Bayesian Confidence Interval [−0.09, −0.01]), with the mediator accounting for 88.54% of the total effect (Fig. 5). Similar mediation results held using a different calculation of TSNR per unit time (Fig. S5).
4. Discussion
In a meta-analysis of historical data as well as in a preregistered analysis of original data, we compared the influence of single-band versus multi-band FMRI scanning protocols on the detectability of reward-related brain activity. Meta-analytic results indicated that relative to single-band scans, multi-band scans compromised detection of mesolimbic activity during reward anticipation. This compromise was not associated with other potential confounds (e.g., scanner brand or field strength), and could not be attributed to other covarying factors (e.g., subject age, publication date, sample size, or flip angle). While this compromise may worsen as a function of the strength of multi-band factor within multi-band protocols (e.g., Demetriou et al. 2018 and Todd et al. 2017), not enough studies were available to directly test this association.
Direct comparison of single- versus multi-band FMRI datasets acquired in the same subjects during the same session on the same scanner provided further evidence that multi-band scanning compromised detection of reward-related mesolimbic activity. This compromise appeared most clearly in the center of the brain and was mediated by increased temporal noise throughout the scan, consistent with earlier research (Risk et al., 2018; Todd et al., 2017). Prior investigators have attributed multi-band induced signal compromise to two sources: (1) decreased longitudinal signal associated with faster volume acquisitions and echo times; and (2) increased high frequency thermal noise amplified by geometric factors in regions with overlapping stimulation (Todd et al., 2017). In the current research, signal compromise could not be attributed to decreased longitudinal magnetization, since the time to acquire images and echo time did not differ between the two multi-band factor scan protocols (Table 1), yet the higher multi-band factor scan showed more compromise than the lower. Instead, the subcortical localization of baseline noise (Fig. 4) and mediation of multi-band induced NAcc signal compromise by TSNR throughout the scan (Fig. 5) point to geometric factor induced noise as the most likely culprit. Although temporal filtering smoothed the appearance of activity time courses, it could not rescue decrements in detectability associated with multi-band scanning (Risk et al., 2021).
While previous studies have examined the impact of multi-band scans on FMRI data in humans at rest (Risk et al., 2018, 2021) and during sensorimotor and cognitive tasks (Demetriou et al., 2018; Todd et al., 2017), this study specifically focused on the influence of multi-band scanning on FMRI responses during a reward task (Knutson et al., 2001). Just as FMRI vision processing localizer tasks can reliably recruit primary visual cortical responses, reward processing localizer tasks can consistently recruit mesolimbic responses (i.e., the NAcc and MPFC) (Bartra et al., 2013; Clithero and Rangel, 2014; Knutson and Greer, 2008). The MID task selectively and reliably activates these mesolimbic brain regions during anticipation and receipt of monetary rewards (Wu et al., 2014), and this activity has demonstrated validity with respect to predicting healthy decision-making (Knutson and Stallen, 2018), as well as psychiatric symptoms (Knutson and Heinz, 2015). Together, these findings point to a literal hole in recent multi-band neuroimaging research, a hole which lies at the center of the brain. Thus, the findings have implications for investigators who seek to study value-based choice in the context of healthy decision-making, as well as for those who wish to measure neural markers of individual differences in affect and motivation in the context of psychiatric symptoms.
Strengths of this research include elicitation of robust neural responses to reward probes, control for potential confounds (e.g., scanner brand, field strength, task, time of measurement, between-subject variability), preregistration of predictions and power, and convergence of findings across meta-analysis of historical findings as well as analysis of original data. Potential limitations include too few studies in the meta-analysis to statistically compare the influence of different multi-band scan protocols on detection of responses to gain versus nongain outcomes in the frontal cortex or to compare the influence of increasing multi-band factors (although earlier work has documented multiband-induced reductions in task-related signal in the MPFC; Risk et al., 2018). While the direct comparison of original data required a relatively small sample size (n = 12), meta-analysis supported the sample’s adequacy, consistent with large effect sizes typically observed in the NAcc for the gain versus nongain anticipation contrast (e.g., weighted effect size average of 2.4 for single-band data) as well as with the preregistered power analyses. Although this research specifically focused on the MID task, given its robust elicitation of mesolimbic activity, findings should generalize to other tasks that reliably recruit mesolimbic activity. The mediating role of baseline temporal signal-to-noise ratio suggests that compromised detection of mesolimbic activity should not be limited to task-related FMRI (Demetriou et al., 2018; Todd et al., 2017), and may also extend to activity during rest (Risk et al., 2018). Indeed, a comprehensive recent comparison revealed artifacts in the center of the brain at rest, leading the investigators to caution that multi-band scanning protocols might obscure subcortical activity (Risk et al., 2021).
With respect to clinical applications, researchers have sought to use FMRI activity in mesolimbic regions (associated with both tasks and rest) to index individual differences. The validity of these measures as indices of experience or behavior is bounded by their reliability (Knutson and Heinz, 2015). Studies of development and psychiatric symptoms have used tasks designed to probe mesolimbic responses to index individual differences, and some research using these tasks has acquired data with multi-band scanning protocols. For example, several large longitudinal studies have adopted multi-band scanning protocols (e.g., the Human Connectome Project (HCP; Van Essen et al., 2013), the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (Casey et al., 2018), and the Dunedin Study (Caspi et al., 2020)), which may compromise researchers’ ability to reliably measure and track individual differences in mesolimbic activity. Further, switching from single-band to multi-band scanning during a study with repeated measures might also compromise reliability (Elliott et al., 2020).
If multi-band scans compromise FMRI assessment of mesolimbic activity, how can researchers address this issue? Those who have not begun to collect data might initially opt for single-band over multi-band scanning protocols (or minimally, multi-band protocols with low multi-band factors). But if data have already been collected, researchers might at least diagnose problems by probing mesolimbic reward responses with a reliable task and then visualizing activity time courses to check for high frequency noise. Raw activity from mesolimbic regions could also be probed for evidence of high frequency oscillatory activity (e.g., with periodograms) or low temporal signal-to-noise ratio. The benefits of ensuring adequate temporal signal to noise ratio could be substantial, since the current findings suggest that opting for single-band instead of multi-band scans might increase statistical power, which could substantially reduce the number of subjects or scanning time required to achieve statistical significance. On the one hand, these findings offer good news by reinforcing the power, stability, and robustness of earlier FMRI findings related to reward processing. On the other hand, these results suggest caution in interpreting more recent findings acquired with rapid acquisition methods, and imply that the benefits of increased speed might come at a cost of lost signal.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Dylan Christiano and Kelly MacNiven for assistance with data collection and analysis, as well as Russell Poldrack, Tom Schonberg, Essa Yacoub, Hua Wu, Adam Kerr, Spanlab, and three anonymous reviewers for feedback on previous drafts. This work was supported by a Neurochoice Initiative Big Ideas grant from Stanford’s Wu Tsai Neurosciences Institute, as well as a National Institute of Health Grant 5P50DA04201205 to BK.
Footnotes
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Credit authorship contribution statement
Tara Srirangarajan: Methodology, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft. Leili Mortazavi: Methodology, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft. Tiago Bortolini: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. Jorge Moll: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. Brian Knutson: Conceptualization, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft.
Open practices and data/code availability
Research design and analysis plans were preregistered at https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=df4a9f.
All data and analysis code are publicly available via the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/pef8g/?view_only=00b685575429453991f2552fb175e8c0.
Supplementary materials
Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118617.
References
- Bartra O, McGuire JT, Kable JW, 2013. The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. Neuroimage 76, 412–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behan B, Stone A, Garavan H, 2015. Right prefrontal and ventral striatum interactions underlying impulsive choice and impulsive responding. Hum. Brain Mapp 36, 187–198. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjork JM, Knutson B, Fong GW, Caggiano DM, Bennett SM, Hommer DW, 2004. Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: similarities and differences from young adults. J. Neurosci 24, 1793–1802. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4862-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjork JM, Smith AR, Chen G, Hommer DW, 2010. Adolescents, adults and rewards: comparing motivational neurocircuitry recruitment using fMRI. PLoS One 5. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjork JM, Smith AR, Chen G, Hommer DW, 2012. Mesolimbic recruitment by non-drug rewards in detoxified alcoholics: effort anticipation, reward anticipation, and reward delivery. Hum. Brain Mapp 33, 2174. doi: 10.1002/HBM.21351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blaimer M, Choli M, Jakob PM, Griswold MA, Breuer FA, 2013. Multiband phase-constrained parallel MRI. Magn. Reson. Med 69, 974–980. doi: 10.1002/mrm.24685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinik-Nezer R, Holzmeister F, Camerer CF, Dreber A, Huber J, Johannesson M, Kirchler M, Iwanir R, Mumford JA, Adcock RA, 2020. Variability in the analysis of a single neuroimaging dataset by many teams. Nature 582, 84–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bustamante JC, Barrõs-Loscertales A, Costumero V, Fuentes-Claramonte P, Rosell-Negre P, Ventura-Campos N, Llopis JJ, Ávila C, 2014. Abstinence duration modulates striatal functioning during monetary reward processing in cocaine patients. Addict. Biol 19, 885–894. doi: 10.1111/adb.12041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Button KS, Ioannidis JPA, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ESJ, Munafò MR, 2013. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 14, 365–376. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cao Z, Bennett M, Orr C, Icke I, Banaschewski T, Barker GJ, Bokde ALW, Bromberg U, Büchel C, Quinlan EB, Desrivières S, Flor H, Frouin V, Garavan H, Gowland P, Heinz A, Ittermann B, Martinot JL, Nees F, Orfanos DP, Paus T, Poustka L, Hohmann S, Fröhner JH, Smolka MN, Walter H, Schumann G, Whelan R, 2019. Mapping adolescent reward anticipation, receipt, and prediction error during the monetary incentive delay task. Hum. Brain Mapp 40, 262–283. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Cannonier T, Conley MI, Cohen AO, Barch DM, Heitzeg MM, Soules ME, Teslovich T, Dellarco DV, Garavan H, Orr CA, Wager TD, Banich MT, Speer NK, Sutherland MT, Riedel MC, Dick AS, Bjork JM, Thomas KM, Chaarani B, Mejia MH, Hagler DJ, Daniela Cornejo M, Sicat CS, Harms MP, Dosenbach NUF, Rosenberg M, Earl E, Bartsch H, Watts R, Polimeni JR, Kuperman JM, Fair DA, Dale AM, Workgroup AIA, 2018. The Adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci 32, 43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carl H, Walsh E, Eisenlohr-Moul T, Minkel J, Crowther A, Moore T, Gibbs D, Petty C, Bizzell J, Dichter GS, Smoski MJ, 2016. Sustained anterior cingulate cortex activation during reward processing predicts response to psychotherapy in major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord 203, 204–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi A, Houts RM, Ambler A, Danese A, Elliott ML, Hariri A, Harrington H, Hogan S, Poulton R, Ramrakha S, Rasmussen LJH, Reuben A, Richmond-Rakerd L, Sugden K, Wertz J, Williams BS, Moffitt TE, 2020. Longitudinal assessment of mental health disorders and comorbidities across 4 decades among participants in the Dunedin birth cohort study. JAMA Netw. Open 3. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3221, e203221–e203221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang C, Glover GH, 2009. Effects of model-based physiological noise correction on default mode network anti-correlations and correlations. Neuroimage 47, 1448–1459. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen JE, Glover GH, 2015. Functional magnetic resonance imaging methods. Neuropsychol. Rev 25, 289. doi: 10.1007/S11065-015-9294-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho YT, Fromm S, Guyer AE, Detloff A, Pine DS, Fudge JL, Ernst M, 2013. Nucleus accumbens, thalamus and insula connectivity during incentive anticipation in typical adults and adolescents. Neuroimage 66, 508–521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clithero J.a, Rangel A, 2014. Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective value. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci 9, 1289–1302. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. 1992. A power primer. J. Abnorm. Psychol 112, 155–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MS, 1997. Parametric analysis of fMRI data using linear systems methods. Neuroimage 6, 93–103. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox RW, 1996. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res 29, 162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damiano CR, Aloi J, Dunlap K, Burrus CJ, Mosner MG, Kozink RV, McLaurin RE, Mullette-Gillman OA, Carter RMK, Huettel SA, McClernon FJ, Ashley-Koch A, Dichter GS, 2014. Association between the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and mesolimbic responses to rewards. Mol. Autism 5, 1–12. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demetriou L, Kowalczyk OS, Tyson G, Bello T, Newbould RD, Wall MB, 2018. A comprehensive evaluation of increasing temporal resolution with multiband-accelerated protocols and effects on statistical outcome measures in fMRI. Neuroimage 176, 404–416. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demidenko MI, Huntley ED, Jahn A, Thomason ME, Monk CS, Keating DP, 2020. Cortical and subcortical response to the anticipation of reward in high and average/low risk-taking adolescents. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci 44, 100798. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demidenko MI, Weigard AS, Ganesan K, Jang H, Jahn A, Huntley ED, Keating DP, 2021. Interactions between methodological and interindividual variability: how monetary incentive delay (MID) task contrast maps vary and impact associations with behavior. Brain and Behavior, e02093 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/9fasc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dhingra I, Zhang S, Zhornitsky S, Le TM, Wang W, Chao HH, Levy I, Li CSR, 2020. The effects of age on reward magnitude processing in the monetary incentive delay task. Neuroimage 207, 116368. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C, 1997. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br. Med. J 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott ML, Knodt AR, Ireland D, Morris ML, Poulton R, Ramrakha S, Sison ML, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Hariri AR, 2020. What is the test-retest reliability of common task-functional MRI measures? New empirical evidence and a meta-analysis. Psychol. Sci 31, 792–806. doi: 10.1177/0956797620916786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enzi B, Edel MA, Lissek S, Peters S, Hoffmann R, Nicolas V, Tegenthoff M, Juckel G, Saft C, 2012. Altered ventral striatal activation during reward and punishment processing in premanifest Huntington’s disease: a functional magnetic resonance study. Exp. Neurol 235, 256–264. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fan L, Li H, Zhuo J, Zhang Y, Wang J, Chen L, Yang Z, Chu C, Xie S, Laird AR, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB, Yu C, Jiang T, 2016. The human brainnetome atlas: a new brain atlas based on connectional architecture. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3508–3526. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasser MF, Coalson TS, Robinson EC, Hacker CD, Harwell J, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, Andersson J, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM, Van Essen DC, 2016. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature 536, 171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature18933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gola M, Wordecha M, Sescousse G, Lew-Starowicz M, Kossowski B, Wypych M, Makeig S, Potenza MN, Marchewka A, 2017. Can pornography be addictive? An fMRI study of men seeking treatment for problematic pornography use. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 2021–2031. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green IW, Pizzagalli DA, Admon R, Kumar P, 2019. Anhedonia modulates the effects of positive mood induction on reward-related brain activation. Neuroimage 193, 115–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herbort MC, Soch J, Wüstenberg T, Krauel K, Pujara M, Koenigs M, Gallinat J, Walter H, Roepke S, Schott BH, 2016. A negative relationship between ventral striatal loss anticipation response and impulsivity in borderline personality disorder. NeuroImage Clin. 12, 724–736. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoogman M, Onnink M, Cools R, Aarts E, Kan C, Arias Vasquez A, Buitelaar J, Franke B, 2013. The dopamine transporter haplotype and reward-related striatal responses in adult ADHD. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol 23, 469–478. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang Y, Wu T, Gao Y, Luo Y, Wu Z, Fagan S, Leung S, Li X, 2019. The impact of callous-unemotional traits and externalizing tendencies on neural responsivity to reward and punishment in healthy adolescents. Front. Neurosci 13, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SL, Mehta H, Ketter TA, Gotlib IH, Knutson B, 2019. Neural responses to monetary incentives in bipolar disorder. NeuroImage Clin. 24, 102018. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.102018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juckel G, Friedel E, Koslowski M, Witthaus H, Özgürdal S, Gudlowski Y, Knutson B, Wrase J, Burne M, Heinz A, Schlagenhauf F, 2012. Ventral striatal activation during reward processing in subjects with ultra-high risk for schizophrenia. Neuropsychobiology 66, 50–56. doi: 10.1159/000337130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim M, Mawla I, Albrecht DS, Admon R, Torrado-Carvajal A, Bergan C, Protsenko E, Kumar P, Edwards RR, Saha A, Napadow V, Pizzagalli DA, Loggia ML, 2020. Striatal hypofunction as a neural corre-late of mood alterations in chronic pain patients. Neuroimage 211, 116656. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirk U, Brown KW, Downar J, 2015. Adaptive neural reward processing during anticipation and receipt of monetary rewards in mindfulness meditators. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci 10, 752–759. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Adams CM, Fong GW, Hommer D, 2001. Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci 21, RC159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Cooper JC, 2005. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction. Curr. Opin. Neurol 18, 411–417. doi: 10.1097/01.wco.0000173463.24758.f6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Fong GW, Bennett SM, Adams CM, Hommer D, 2003. A region of mesial prefrontal cortex tracks monetarily rewarding outcomes: characterization with rapid event-related fMRI. Neuroimage 18, 263–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Greer SM, 2008. Anticipatory affect: neural correlates and consequences for choice. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci 363, 3771–3786. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Heinz A, 2015. Probing psychiatric symptoms with the monetary incentive delay task. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 418–420. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B. Stallen M. 2018. How can affect influence choice? In: Fox AS, Lapate RC, Shackman AJ, Davidson RJ (Eds.), The Nature of Emotion : Fundamental Questions, Series in Affective Science. Oxford University Press, New York, NY, pp. 335–338. [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Taylor J, Kaufman M, Peterson R, Glover G, 2005. Distributed neural representation of expected value. J. Neurosci 25, 4806–4812. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0642-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kollmann B, Scholz V, Linke J, Kirsch P, Wessa M, 2017. Reward anticipation revisited- evidence from an fMRI study in euthymic bipolar I patients and healthy first-degree relatives. J. Affect. Disord 219, 178–186. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martucci KT, Borg N, MacNiven KH, Knutson B, Mackey SC, 2018. Altered prefrontal correlates of monetary anticipation and outcome in chronic pain. Pain 159, 1494. doi: 10.1097/J.PAIN.0000000000001232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGonigle J, Murphy A, Paterson LM, Reed LJ, Nestor L, Nash J, Elliott R, Ersche KD, Flechais RSA, Newbould R, Orban C, Smith DG, Taylor EM, Waldman AD, Robbins TW, Deakin JFW, Nutt DJ, Lingford-Hughes AR, Suckling J, 2017. The ICCAM platform study: an experimental medicine platform for evaluating new drugs for relapse prevention in addiction. Part B: FMRI description. J. Psychopharmacol 31, 3–16. doi: 10.1177/0269881116668592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mucci A, Dima D, Soricelli A, Volpe U, Bucci P, Frangou S, Prinster A, Salvatore M, Galderisi S, Maj M, 2015. Is avolition in schizophrenia associated with a deficit of dorsal caudate activity? A functional magnetic resonance imaging study during reward anticipation and feedback. Psychol. Med 45, 1765–1778. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moeller S, Yacoub E, Olman CA, Auerbach E, Strupp J, Harel N, Uǧurbil K, 2010. Multiband multislice GE-EPI at 7 tesla, with 16-fold acceleration using partial parallel imaging with application to high spatial and temporal whole-brain FMRI. Magn. Reson. Med 63, 1144–1153. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pauli WM, Nili AN, Tyszka JM, 2018. A high-resolution probabilistic in vivo atlas of human subcortical brain nuclei. Sci. Data 5, 180063. doi: 10.1038/sdata.2018.63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfabigan DM, Seidel EM, Sladky R, Hahn A, Paul K, Grahl A, Küblböck M, Kraus C, Hummer A, Kranz GS, Windischberger C, Lanzenberger R, Lamm C, 2014. P300 amplitude variation is related to ventral striatum BOLD response during gain and loss anticipation: an EEG and fMRI experiment. Neuroimage 96, 12–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack RA, 2012. The future of fMRI in cognitive neuroscience. Neuroimage 62, 1216–1220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pornpattananangkul N, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Stringaris A, 2019. Association between childhood anhedonia and alterations in large-scale resting-state networks and task-evoked activation. JAMA Psychiatry 76, 624–633. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rademacher L, Krach S, Kohls G, Irmak A, Gründer G, Spreckelmeyer KN, 2010. Dissociation of neural networks for anticipation and consumption of monetary and social rewards. Neuroimage 49, 3276–3285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radua J, Schmidt A, Borgwardt S, Heinz A, Schlagenhauf F, McGuire P, Fusar-Poli P, 2015. Ventral striatal activation during reward processing in psychosis a neurofunctional meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 1243–1251. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Risk BB, Kociuba MC, Rowe DB, 2018. Impacts of simultaneous multislice acquisition on sensitivity and specificity in fMRI. Neuroimage 172, 538–553. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Risk BB, Murden RJ, Wu J, Nebel MB, Venkataraman A, Zhang Z, Qiu D, 2021. Which multiband factor should you choose for your resting-state fMRI study? Neuroimage 234, 117965. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2021.117965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romanczuk-Seiferth N, Koehler S, Dreesen C, Wüstenberg T, Heinz A, 2015. Pathological gambling and alcohol dependence: neural disturbances in reward and loss avoidance processing. Addict. Biol 20, 557–569. doi: 10.1111/adb.12144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosen BR, Savoy RL, 2012. fMRI at 20: has it changed the world? Neuroimage 62, 1316–1324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sacchet MD, Knutson B, 2013. Spatial smoothing systematically biases the localization of reward-related brain activity. Neuroimage 66, 270–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sescousse G, Barbalat G, Domenech P, Dreher JC, 2013. Imbalance in the sensitivity to different types of rewards in pathological gambling. Brain 136, 2527–2538. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sescousse G, Redouté J, Dreher JC, 2010. The architecture of reward value coding in the human orbitofrontal cortex. J. Neurosci 30, 13095–13104. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3501-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon JJ, Walther S, Fiebach CJ, Friederich HC, Stippich C, Weisbrod M, Kaiser S, 2010. Neural reward processing is modulated by approach- and avoidance-related personality traits. Neuroimage 49, 1868–1874. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoy M, Schlagenhauf F, Schlochtermeier L, Wrase J, Knutson B, Lehmkuhl U, Huss M, Heinz A, Ströhle A, 2011. Reward processing in male adults with childhood ADHD-a comparison between drug-naïve and methylphenidate-treated subjects. Psychopharmacology 215, 467–481. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2166-y, (Berl). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoy M, Schlagenhauf F, Sterzer P, Bermpohl F, Hägele C, Suchotzki K, Schmack K, Wrase J, Ricken R, Knutson B, Adli M, Bauer M, Heinz A, Strohle A, 2012. Hyporeactivity of ventral striatum towards incentive stimuli in unmedicated depressed patients normalizes after treatment with escitalopram. J. Psychopharmacol 26, 677–688. doi: 10.1177/0269881111416686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tingley D, Yamamoto T, Hirose K, Keele L, Imai K, 2014. mediation:rpackage for causal mediation analysis. J. Stat. Softw 59. doi: 10.18637/jss.v059.i05. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Todd N, Josephs O, Zeidman P, Flandin G, Moeller S, Weiskopf N, 2017. Functional sensitivity of 2D simultaneous multi-slice echo-planar imaging: effects of acceleration on g-factor and physiological noise. Front. Neurosci 11, 158. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd N, Moeller S, Auerbach EJ, Yacoub E, Flandin G, Weiskopf N, 2016. Evaluation of 2D multiband EPI imaging for high-resolution, whole-brain, task-based fMRI studies at 3T: sensitivity and slice leakage artifacts. Neuroimage 124, 32–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, Zald DH, 2013. Perceived stress predicts altered reward and loss feedback processing in medial prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci 7, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ubl B, Kuehner C, Kirsch P, Ruttorf M, Diener C, Flor H, 2014. Altered neural reward and loss processing and prediction error signalling in depression. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci 10, 1102–1112. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC, Smith SM, Barch DM, Behrens TEJ, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, Consortium WMHCP, 2013. The WU-Minn human connectome project: an overview. Neuroimage 80, 62–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viechtbauer W, 2010. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw 36, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/JSS.V036.I03. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vul E, Harris C, Winkielman P, Pashler H, 2009. Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspect. Psychol. Sci 4, 274–290. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiland BJ, Heitzeg MM, Zald D, Cummiford C, Love T, Zucker RA, Zubieta JK, 2014. Relationship between impulsivity, prefrontal anticipatory activation, and striatal dopamine release during rewarded task performance. Psychiatry Res. 223, 244–252. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.05.015, Neuroimaging. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welborn BL, Hong Y, Ratner KG, 2020. Exposure to negative stereotypes influences representations of monetary incentives in the nucleus accumbens. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci 15, 347–358. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wimmer GE, Büchel C, 2016. Reactivation of reward-related patterns from single past episodes supports memory-based decision making. J. Neurosci 36, 2868–2880. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3433-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu CC, Samanez-Larkin GR, Katovich K, Knutson B, 2014. Affective traits link to reliable neural markers of incentive anticipation. Neuroimage 84, 279–289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yacubian J, Gläscher J, Schroeder K, Sommer T, Braus DF, Büchel C, 2006. Dissociable systems for gain-and loss-related value predictions and errors of prediction in the human brain. J. Neurosci 26, 9530–9537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yan C, Wang Y, Su L, Xu T, Yin DZ, Fan MX, Deng CP, Wang ZX, Lui SSY, Cheung EFC, Chan RCK, 2016. Differential mesolimbic and prefrontal alterations during reward anticipation and consummation in positive and negative schizotypy. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 254, 127–136. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yau WYW, Zubieta JK, Weiland BJ, Samudra PG, Zucker RA, Heitzeg MM, 2012. Nucleus accumbens response to incentive stimuli anticipation in children of alcoholics: relationships with precursive behavioral risk and lifetime alcohol use. J. Neurosci 32, 2544–2551. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1390-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.