Skip to main content
Clinical Neuropsychiatry logoLink to Clinical Neuropsychiatry
. 2020 Jun;17(3):141–148. doi: 10.36131/cnfioritieditore20200301

Behavior Problems and Depressive Symptoms in Developmental Dyslexia: Risk Assessment in Brazilian Students

Ricardo Franco de Lima 1,2,, Cíntia Alves Salgado-Azoni 3, Betânia Alves Veiga Dell’Agli 4, Makilim Nunes Baptista 5, Sylvia Maria Ciasca 1
PMCID: PMC8629085  PMID: 34908985

Abstract

Objective

To investigate if students with developmental dyslexia (DD) have more behavior problems and depressive symptoms than students without learning difficulties.

Method

Participants were 61 students, aged 7-14 years, including 31 with interdisciplinary DD diagnosis and 30 without learning disabilities. We collected data from parents, using the children’s behavior checklist (CBCL), and from students, using the children’s depression inventory (CDI).

Results

The DD group had higher CBCL averages for anxiety, depression, withdrawal, rule-breaking behaviors, aggressiveness, and social, attentional and thought problems. They also showed higher results for the internalizing and externalizing categories, others and total. In the CDI, DD students had higher averages for total score and for the following symptoms: negative self-evaluation, guilt, suicidal thoughts, feeling concern, performance comparison, sleeping difficulties, fatigue, and problems in interacting with peers at school.

Conclusions

We discuss the results in terms of implications for DD diagnosis and intervention. Parent reports indicate a higher frequency of behavior problems in students with DD diagnosis. Those students also demonstrate more symptoms of depression than students without learning difficulties.

Keywords: learning disorders, dyslexia, depression, signs and symptoms

Introduction

Developmental Dyslexia (DD) is a specific learning disability characterized by difficulties in reading comprehension and word writing, without overall intelligence and sensory deficits, and preserving the ability of effective learning within the classroom (APA, 2014; British Dyslexia Association, 2011; Lyon, Shaywitz, & Shaywitz, 2003; WHO, 2008). Reading difficulties have been explained by multifactorial causal models, emphasizing phonological deficits (Catts, McIlraith,Bridges, & Nielsen, 2017; Lyon, Shaywitz, & Shaywitz, 2003).

Previous studies have argued that these individuals are at a greater risk for developing psychological symptoms (Maag & Reid, 2009; Mugnaini, Lassi, La Malfa, & Albertini, 2009; Sahoo, Biswas, & Padhy, 2015) and psychiatric comorbidities (Bäcker & Neuhäuser 2003; Hendren, Haft, Black, White, & Hoeft, 2018; Sahoo, Biswas, & Padhy, 2015; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000) because they have a tendency for low self-concept and self-esteem, a high external locus of control, lower social acceptance, problematic psychosocial functioning, and display more anxiety than their peers without learning difficulties (Parhiala et al., 2015; Sahoo, Biswas, & Padhy, 2015; Terras, Thompson, & Minnis, 2009).

Meta-analysis studies mainly associate DD to internalizing disorders (Maag & Reid, 2009; Mugnaini, Lassi, La Malfa, & Albertini, 2009; Snowling, Muter, & Carroll, 2007), characterized by withdrawal, anxiety and depression (Heath & Ross, 2000; Maag & Reid, 2009; Mugnaini, Lassi, La Malfa, & Albertini, 2009). More specifically, studies using tracing devices show that individuals with dyslexia report depressive symptoms more often than proficient readers (Arnold et al., 2005; Heath, 1995; Maag & Reid, 2009; Stevenson & Rommey, 1984; Wright-Strawderman & Watson, 1992). Among the frequently described symptoms are suicidal thoughts, negative self-concept, anhedonia, fatigue, problems in social relationships and comparison of performance with peers (Lima & Ciasca, 2010a; Wright-Strawderman & Watson, 1992). Nelson and Liebel (2017) showed that individuals with dyslexia report more depressive symptoms, even after controlling for the effects of socially desirable responding. On the other hand, some studies (Burden, 2008; Miller, Hynd, & Miller, 2005) have questioned whether the diagnosis of dyslexia can be considered an increased risk for psychological symptoms and the development of comorbidities.

The symptoms collected from parents’ opinions tend to be broader and point to attention problems and aggressiveness (Arnold et al., 2005; Knivsberg & Andreassen, 2008; Lima & Ciasca, 2010). Heiervang et al. (2000) compared behavior problems in a sample of 25 children with dyslexia and a matched control group. In this study, researchers used instruments with children (Youth Self Report, YSR), parents (Child Behavior Checklist, CBCL), and teachers (Teacher Self Report, TRF). The results indicated that dyslexic group had higher scores (CBCL and TRF) on the Total Behavior Problem Scale, the Internalizing and Externalizing subdomains, and the Attention Problem subscale.

Identifying the consequences of dyslexia in other developmental domains is crucial during diagnosis and helps therapeutic planning (Maag & Reid, 2006; Mugnaini, Lassi, La Malfa, & Albertini, 2009). Nonetheless, the literature has focused on characterizing the cognitive profile of individuals who have dyslexia (Cruz-Rodrigues et al., 2014; Zoubrinetzky, Bielle, F., & Valdois, 2014), and little is known about the emotional and behavioral consequences of this disorder (Bäcker & Neuhäuser, 2003; Maag & Reid, 2006; Mugnaini, Lassi, La Malfa, & Albertini, 2009; Sahoo, Biswas, & Padhy, 2015; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000). Therefore, the aim of this study was to compare the behavior problems and depressive symptoms between students with DD and students without learning difficulties.

Method

Ethical aspects

This study was approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Medical Sciences of the State University of Campinas (protocol no. 648/2007) and all parents signed informed consent forms, conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki for research involving human subjects.

Participants

Participants were 61 students from both sexes (62% boys), aged from 7 to 14 years (average 9.72 years, SD=1.57), from middle income households, attending 2nd to 8th grades in public schools in the metropolitan region of a city in the State of São Paulo, Brazil. This region is formed by twenty cities, being diverse in terms of ethnicity. The students were divided in two groups: with developmental dyslexia, and control. Both groups were subjected to the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edition - WISC-III (Rueda et al., 2013), in order to include only participants with a normal intelligence level (Intelligence Quotient, IQ>80).

Developmental Dyslexia Group (DG): This group was formed by 31 students (71% boys) recruited, by an interdisciplinary team, from an initial pool of 125 students (74% boys) that were referred to a pediatric neurology outpatient clinic with reading/writing complaints. The sample size was compatible with other studies that have similar objectives (Heiervang, Stevenson, Lund, & Hugdahl, 2001; Lima & Ciasca, 2010a; Snowling, Muter, V., & Carroll, 2007). The following selection criteria were used for DG participants: (a) fulfillment of DD diagnostic criteria; (b) impaired performance in phonological processing (access to mental lexicon, phonological awareness and working phonological memory) and reading (accuracy and/or speed) using standard measures (Phonological Awareness Test, Rapid Automatized Naming, evaluation of the level and speed of oral reading, Test of Reading and Writing) (Salgado & Capellini, 2008); (c) normal intelligence level (IQ>80); (d) no sensory alterations (visual, auditory); (f) no use of psychotropic drugs. The diagnostic criteria for DD included performance with 2 standard deviations below the age group in reading speed, reading accuracy, writing under dictation, writing spontaneously, rapid automatized naming test, phonological awareness test, and phonological working memory test; in addition to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, DSM-5 (APA, 2014) and International Classification of Diseases, ICD-10 (WHO, 2008) criteria. The participants did not have psychiatric comorbidities.

Control Group (CG): This group included 30 students (53% boys), proficient in reading, and without attention or learning complaints. The criteria used for selection were: (a) designation by their teacher as students without learning difficulties and with an adequate school performance; (b) normal intelligence level (IQ>80) and no alterations in components of phonological processing, as a result of neuropsychological and language evaluations; (c) no sensory alterations (visual, auditory); (d) no use of psychotropic drugs. Sample characteristics are described in table 1.

Table 1.

Sample characterization

Variables DG CG Total p-value
Sex f (%) f (%) f (%)
Male 22 (71) 16 (53) 38 (62) .155a
Female 09 (29) 14 (47) 23 (38) .182c
  Grade
2nd-4th 11 (36) 15 (50) 26 (43) .252a
5th-8th 20 (65) 15 (50) 35 (57) .147c
  Age
7-9 10 (32) 19 (63) 29 (48) .015 a
10-14 21 (68) 11 (37) 32 (52) .311 c
Min-Max 8-14 7-11 7-14
M ± SD 10.5 ± 1.65 9.0 ± 1.07 9.72 ± 1.57 <.001 b
IQ’s M ± SD M ± SD M ± SD
Verbal (VIQ) 102.65 ± 15.52 118.20 ± 11.74 110.30 ± 15.76 <.001 b
Average High Average High Average
Performance(PIQ) 103.61 ± 11.35 114.10 ± 13.29 108.77 ± 13.33 .001 b
Average High Average Average
Full (FIQ) 103.39 ± 13.21 117.63 ± 12.00 110.39 ± 14.44 <.001 b
Average High Average High Average

Notes: aChi-square test; bMann-Whitney test; cCramer’s V.

Abbreviations: f = frequency; Min = Minimum; Max = Maximum; M ± SD = Mean ± Standard Deviation.

Instruments

Behavior Problems

We used the Child Behavior Checklist – CBCL (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2004; Bordin et al., 2013), a questionnaire directed to parents which evaluates the emotional and behavior characteristics in 6- to 18-year-olds. The CBCL includes a first part, which concerns social aspects, and a second part, containing 113 statements, which was used in this work. Each statement is scored in a three-point Likert scale: (a, score 0) the statement is false or the behavior is absent); (b, score 1) the statement is partially true or the behavior is sometimes present; (c, score 2) the statement is very true or the behavior is frequently present. For scoring, all items are grouped in nine specific scales: I. Anxious/ Depressed; II. Withdrawn/Depressed; III. Somatic Complaints; IV. Social Problems; V. Thought Problems; VI. Attention Problems; VII. Rule-Breaking Behavior; VIII. Aggressive Behavior; and IX. Other Problems. Afterwards, the scores of each scale are added, according to the item’s scores. This study considered the raw scores of each scale (sum of each item’s score), the additional “Internalizing Behavior” scores (sum of scales I + I + I), the “Externalizing Behavior” scores (VI + VI), “Others” (IV + V + VI) and “Total score” (sum of all scores). A higher score meant a higher level of symptoms according to parents. CBCL has been used in clinical studies and international versions have a test-retest reliability of 0.90, internal consistency of 0.72 (Cronbach’s alpha), and criterion validity with discriminative capacity of clinical and non-clinical groups (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). In our study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.93.

Depressive symptoms

The 20-item version of the Children’s Depression Inventory – CDI (Coutinho, Carolino, & Medeiros, 2008) was used. The CDI is a self-reporting scale for tracing depressive symptoms in 7- to 17-year-old children and youth. The participant should select one out of three alternatives for answering each item. The score of each item may be: (i) 0 (absence of symptom), (ii) 1 (presence of symptom) or; (iii) 2 (serious symptom). The total score was obtained by summing up all items, varying between 0 and 40. A cut-off of 17 points was used for significant symptomatology. The additional raw scores were also obtained from the sum of the items to which they refer: (a) Affective aspects (1. sadness, 6. fear/negative assessment of the future, 7. negative self-concept, 10. desire to cry, 13. negative self-image, 16. loneliness 19. feeling unloved); (b) Cognitive aspects (2. pessimism, 3. negative self-evaluation of performance and abilities, 8. guilt, 9. suicidal thoughts, 11. feeling concern and 18. comparison of performance with peers); (c) Behavior aspects (4. leisure, 5. hostile behavior, 12. withdrawal, 17. interaction with peers in school and 20. disobedience); (d) Somatic aspects (14. sleeping difficulties and 15. fatigue). Concerning psychometric properties of the CDI, Brazilian studies show internal consistency ranging between 0.73 and 0.91 (Gomes et al., 2013). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.70.

Procedures and statistical analysis

A single professional administered the instruments (RFL, clinical neuropsychologist). CBCL was performed with the parents, ensuring that they understood adequately the items. Students that were part of the groups were individually evaluated using the CDI. Given the reading difficulties of the DG, the CDI administration was monitored. Each sentence was read, and the student was guided to indicate which best described his/her feelings, thoughts and behaviors in the past weeks. The researcher explained the terms used in the instrument and answered any concerns to ensure everyone understood the questions. The same procedure was applied to the CG.

Statistical analysis of the data was performed in SPSS version 20.0. To compare the groups, a non-parametric Mann-Whitney test was used for numerical variables and a Chi-square or Fisher’s exact test was used for categorical variables, considering a significance level of p < 0.05. In addition, a measure of effect size was included (Cohen’s d) to compare differences between the averages of the groups. The effect size was interpreted as: low (d < 0.20), average (0.21 > d < 0.79), and high average (d > 0.80) (Conboy, 2003). Spearman’s correlation was carried between CDI total score and CBCL scales.

Results

As shown in table 1, both groups had similar sex and school year distributions. Average age was also different between groups (U = 224.50, p < 0.001). The IQ for the whole sample ranged from 108 to 110, equal or better than average. CG had higher VIQ (U = 213.00, p < 0.001), PIQ (U=231.00, p < 0.01) and FIQ scores (U=194.00, p < 0.001) than DG. IQ classification (VIQ, PIQ and FIQ) (Rueda et al., 2013) was in line with the average for DG and was a high average for CG.

Table 2 shows that the groups had significantly different CBCL scores, with Cohen’s d between average and high average. In the somatic complaints scale, Cohen’s d was low, with no significant difference between DG and CG, but, when symptoms were considered together, DG and CG were different in all scales, with a high average effect size.

Table 2.

Comparison of the groups’ CBCL scales

CBCL Scales DG CG U p-valuea Cohen’s d
M ± SD
I- Anxious/Depressed 8.26 ± 5.43 4.77 ±3.33 298.00 .016 .77
II- Withdrawn/Depressed 3.77 ± 3.27 1.40 ± 1.85 238.00 .001 .89
III- Somatic Complaints 3.65 ± 3.65 2.30 ± 2.25 372.00 .174 .45
IV- Social Problems 6.58 ± 4.46 2.23 ± 1.96 186.50 <.001 1.26
V- Thought Problems 4.61 ± 4.04 1.77 ± 2.34 223.00 <.001 .86
VI- Attention Problems 10.26 ± 4.26 3.13 ± 3.37 89.00 <.001 1.86
VII- Rule-Breaking Behavior 3.74 ± 3.68 1.57 ± 2.01 276.00 .006 .73
VIII- Aggressive Behavior 10.84 ± 7.30 6.43 ± 5.67 294.50 .014 .67
IX- Others 6.00 ± 3.21 3.53 ± 3.04 264.50 .004 .79
Internalizing Behavior 15.32 ± 10.55 8.47 ± 6.45 277.00 .007 .78
Externalizing Behavior 14.58 ± 10.52 8.00 ± 7.38 286.00 .010 .72
Others 27.45 ± 13.43 10.67 ± 9.50 120.50 <.001 1.44
Total Score 57.35 ± 32.08 27.13 ± 21.65 179.50 <.001 1.10

Notes: aMann-Whitney test.

Abbreviations: M ± SD = Mean ± Standard Deviation.

No child exceeded the CDI cut-off point in the whole sample. Table 3 shows differences between the groups in total CDI score and in the cognitive and somatic symptom categories, with a high average effect size. For the categories of affective and behavior symptoms we observed a low effect size.

Table 3.

Comparison of the groups’ CDI scores

CDI scores DG CG U p-valuea Cohen’s d
M ± SD
CDI-Total 6.48 ± 3.60 3.33 ± 1.95 201.00 <.001 1.09
Affective 1.52 ± 1.15 1.23 ± 0.97 399.00 .318 .27
Cognitive 2.71 ± 1.77 1.03 ± 0.89 183.00 <.001 1.20
Behavior 1.26 ± 0.96 .87 ± 0.90 356.00 .096 .42
Somatic 1.00 ± 1.06 .20 ± 0.48 267.00 .001 0.97

Notes: aMann-Whitney test.

Abbreviations: M ± SD = Mean ± Standard Deviation.

Table 4 compares the descriptive statistics for each CDI item between the groups. We observed significant differences between groups in items 3, 8, 9, 11, 14, 15, 17 and 18, corresponding to Cohen’s d between average and high average, but detected no significant differences for items 5, 7, 10 and 13, corresponding to a low effect size. Significant correlations were obtained between the CDI total and the following CBCL scores: Attention Problems (r=.537; p<0.001), Aggressive Behavior (r=.357; p<0.01), Externalizing Behavior (r=.321; p<.01), Others (r=.395; p<.01) and Total (r=.365; p<.01).

Table 4.

Comparison of the groups’ CDI items

CDI items DG CG U p-valuea Cohen’s d
M ± SD
1. Frequent sadness .06 ± .25 .03 ± .18 450.50 .577 .14
2. Hopelessness .71 ± .59 .53 ± .51 397.00 .260 .33
3. Can’t do anything right/incompetent .29 ± .53 .00 345.00 .003 .77
4. Anhedonia .55 ± .51 .47 ± .51 427.00 .527 .16
5. See self as “bad” .00 .03 ± .18 449.50 .309 -.24
6. Worries that bad things are going to happen to self .58 ± .56 .63 ± .49 435.00 .615 -.10
7. Self-hatred .16 ± .37 .07 ± .25 421.00 .250 .29
8. Guilt .32 ± .54 .07 ± .25 360.00 .023 .59
9. Suicidal thoughts .16 ± .37 .00 390.00 .023 .61
10. Frequent crying .00 .03 ± .18 449.50 .309 -.24
11. Bothered by things .45 ± .68 .10 ± .31 342.00 .015 .66
12. Doesn’t want to be with people .03 ± .18 .07 ± .25 449.00 .538 -.18
13. Feels ugly .45 ± .57 .20 ± .41 360.00 .060 .50
14. Sleeping difficulties .58 ± .76 .20 ± .48 341.50 .026 .60
15. Fatigue .42 ± .67 .00 315.00 .001 .89
16. Loneliness .19 ± .48 .17 ± .38 465.00 1.000 .05
17. Doesn’t have fun at school .42 ± .50 .10 ± .31 316.50 .005 .77
18. Feels inferior to other kids .77 ± .56 .33 ± .48 280.00 .002 .84
19. Feels unloved .06 ± .25 .10 ± .31 448.50 .616 -.14
20. Noncompliant .26 ± .44 .20 ± .41 438.00 .593 .14

Notes: aMann-Whitney test.

Abbreviations: M ± SD = Mean ± standard deviation.

In order to ascertain the characteristics of the answers that had significant results for CDI items, we calculated the frequency distribution (table 5), and obtained significant differences between groups for items 3, 9, 11, 15, 17 and 18. Item 8 had a marginally significant difference.

Table 5.

Frequency distribution for CDI items that showed differences between groups

CDI items DG CG Total p-valuea
f (%)
3 I do most things O.K. 23(74) 30(100) 53(87) .007
I do many things wrong 07(23) 0 07(11)
I do everything wrong 01(03) 0 01(02)
8 Bad things are not usually my fault 22(71) 28(93) 50(82) .060
Many bad things are my fault 08(26) 02(07) 10(16)
All bad things are my fault 01(03) 0 01(02)
9 I do not think about killing myself 26(84) 30(100) 56(92) .053
I think about killing myself but would not do it 05(16) 0 5(8)
I want to kill myself 0 0 0
11 Things bother me once in a while 20(65) 27(90) 47(77) .046
Things bother me many itmes 08(26) 03(10) 11(18)
Things bother me all the time 03(10) 0(0) 03(05)
14 I sleep pretty well 18(58) 25(83) 43(70) .079
I have trouble sleeping many nights 08(26) 04(13) 12(20)
I have trouble sleeping every night 05(16) 01(03) 06(10)
15 I am tired once in a while 21(68) 30(100) 51(84) .001
I am tired many days 07(23) 0 07(11)
I am tired all the time 03(10) 0 03(05)
17 I have fun at school many times 18(58) 27(90) 45(74) .008
I have fun at school only once in a while 13(42) 03(10) 16(26)
I never have fun at school 0 0 0
18 I am just as good as other kids 09(29) 20(67) 29(48) .007
I can be as good as other kids if I want to 20(65) 10(33) 30(49)
I can never be as good as other kids 02(06) 0 02(03)

Abbreviation: f = frequency.

Notes: aFisher’s exact test.

Discussion

This study compared behavioral problems and the presence of depressive symptoms in two groups, one with a DD diagnosis and another without learning difficulties. According to results of the CBCL, parents of DD individuals more often reported social, attention and thinking problems, anxiety, depression, withdrawal, rule breaking, aggressiveness and other problems. Additionally, DG scores for instrument categories were also higher for problems in internalizing, externalizing, general and total behaviors.

Previous studies emphasized that children with dyslexia are more vulnerable to developing internalizing disorders, such as affective or anxiety disorders (Heiervang, Stevenson, Lund, & Hugdahl, 2001; Sahoo, Biswas, & Padhy, 2015; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000). However, when assessed with parent testimonies and a screening test, no specific pattern can be discerned to the emotional and behavior problems that accompany DD (Terras, Thompson, & Minnis, 2009; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000). It is possible that the DD diagnosis itself, and its characteristics, increase the parents’ awareness of the difficulties their children might experience in other aspects of development. Furthermore, although CBCL is accurate for some diagnoses (Lampert, Polanczyk, Tramontina, Mardini, & Rohde, 2004), it has a bias toward general complaints, requiring complementation with other instruments. Finally, the pattern of emotional and behavioral problems may associate with factors such as subtype of learning disorder (Nelson & Gregg, 2012, Yu, Buka, McCormick, Fitzmaurice, & Indurkhya, 2006), sex (Terras, Thompson, & Minnis, 2009; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000) or the age when the diagnosis was made (Willcutt & Pennington, 2000).

Although no participant had comorbidity with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), parents reported more attention problems in dyslexic group. Similar results were found by Heiervang, Stevenson, Lund, & Hugdahl, (2001). Furthermore, these findings are compatible with previous studies that show the attention and executive functions problems as part of the neuropsychological profile in dyslexia (Lima, Salgado-Azoni, & Ciasca, 2013; Moura, Simões, & Pereira, 2015).

This study also evaluated depressive symptoms. Results showed that DD individuals had higher scores in total CDI, cognitive and somatic scores. However, the mean total CDI score was below the cut-off point of the instrument (<17 points), suggesting subclinical levels of depression. Other studies also reported depressive symptoms in samples of students with DD or other learning disorders, albeit at higher average scores (Fristad, Topolosky, Weller, & Weller, 1992; Lima & Ciasca, 2010a; Maughan, Rowe, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2003; Sahoo et al., 2015; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000; Zoubrinetzky et al., 2014).

Comparing the groups for each CDI item, the symptoms most frequently reported by DD students were: negative self-evaluation of performance, guilt, suicidal thoughts, feeling concern, comparing their performance with peers (cognitive symptoms), sleeping difficulties, fatigue (somatic symptoms), and problems in interacting with peers at school (behavior symptoms). However, there were no significant reports of depressive mood, one of the key criteria for diagnosis of mood disorder according to diagnostics manuals (APA, 2014; WHO, 2008). Thus, although individuals of the sample with dyslexia reported more symptoms, those were not sufficient for a diagnosis of depressive disorder.

Even though this study not establish a causal relationship, part of the reported symptoms could result from the individuals own learning difficulties, as proposed by Sideridis’ goal orientation model of depression vulnerability (Sideridis, 2007). Thus, difficulties experienced in the school context may lead to repeated experiences of failure, negative self-evaluation of performance, development of negative feelings, feeling concern, problems in interpersonal relationships, among others. Studies that report impairments in self-efficacy, self-concept and self-esteem for children and adolescents with DD have described these types of consequences (Bäcker & Neuhäuser, 2003; Heath & Ross, 2000; Stevenson & Rommey, 1984; Wright-Strawderman & Watson, 1992).

Analysis of the CDI items with significant differences between groups allowed us to identify the most frequent answers. An item that stands out is suicidal thoughts. All CG participants answered “I do not think about killing myself”, while 16% of DG participants chose the answer “I think about killing myself but would not do it”. The same answer had a 10% frequency in other studies with students without learning difficulties (Poch, Ballabriga, & Llaberia, 2000; Viñas, Canals, Gras, Ros, & Domènech-Llaberia, 2002), suggesting that learning disability may represent a vulnerability factor for more serious consequences.

There was a positive correlation between the total CDI and the parents report about attention problems, aggressive behavior, externalizing behaviors, others and total. Interestingly, there was no agreement between students and parents reports about depressive symptoms and internalizing problems, respectively. The results obtained from parents and students could represent isolated events of low severity. However, studies show that they tend to persist throughout development and to predispose the individual to psychiatric comorbidities, especially when the protective factors are inefficient (Wilson, Deri Armstrong, Furrie, & Walcot, 2009; Sideridis, 2007). As for consequences in adulthood, one study showed that individuals carrying learning disabilities report two times higher levels of depression, anxiety, suicidal thoughts and visits to mental health professionals (Wilson et al., 2009).

Regarding the sample characteristics, DG students were, on average, one year five months older than CG students. The higher average age in the DG was expected, as, according to the diagnosis criteria, DD individuals tend to delay the development of reading and writing abilities up to two years (APA, 2014; WHO, 2008). In addition, DD is usually diagnosed after the literacy process has been completed (Ciasca, Rodrigues, Salgado-Azoni, & Lima, 2015). Another relevant result concerns intellectual performance: both groups displayed total IQ scores within expected average for their age group, according to the instrument norm (Rueda, Noronha, Sisto, Santos, & Castro, 2014) and diagnostic criteria for DD (APA, 2014; WHO, 2008). Nevertheless, comparison between groups showed that CG had higher average scores than DG for total, verbal and execution IQ. Although the intellectual performance of individuals who have dyslexia is within normal standards, the data suggests that their scores can be inferior when compared with normolexic children. Other studies have described similar results and may explain the heterogeneity of deficits found in individuals with DD (De Clercq-Quaegebeur, Casalis, Lemaitre, Bourgois, Getto, & Vallée, 2010; Mugnaini et al., 2009). Furthermore, evidence suggests that the presence of depressive symptoms may negatively impact neuropsychological performance, worsening the damage to these individuals during their school life (Lima & Ciasca, 2010b).

An important implication of this study is that the investigation of behavior problems in DD is fundamental to determine which professional will be part of the treatment as well as what are the conditions of response to the interventions. Some authors argue that the emotional suffering experienced by the individuals who have dyslexia is related with the degree of severity of the disorder, complexity of symptoms (and association with difficulties in other areas, such as mathematics), school exclusion situations, comorbidity with other disorders, inappropriate parent and teacher handling of difficulties, little capacity to resolve problems, among others (Mugnaini et al., 2009; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000). It is important to underline that late DD diagnosis associated with low protective factors (such as social and family support and coping strategies) may increase the risk of psychiatric comorbidities and the impact in the individual’s development (Alexander-Passe, 2006; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000; Wilson et al., 2009). Thus, interventions targeting only the cognitive and linguistic aspects may not be as effective and should be complemented with psychological and psychiatric monitoring. According Hendren, Haft, Black, White, & Hoeft, (2018), an interdisciplinary approach to learning disorders, integrating health professionals and educators, can lead to treatments encompassing both academic and mental health interventions. This approach can be more effective and contribute to improved educational and health-related outcomes to dyslexic individuals.

The current study had some limitations. First, the instruments used allowed screening only of general symptoms in the parents and depressive symptoms in the students; future studies should enhance this analysis by investigating the main psychiatric comorbidities of DD. Second, the procedures provided no distinction of symptoms by sex, age group or age during the diagnosis. A larger sample of DD students would likely allow this type of analysis to be made with higher accuracy. Finally, a longitudinal study would allow understanding the development trajectory in individuals with this learning disability.

In summary, this study found that parents of DD students report problems, both for internalizing and for externalizing behaviors, more often than parents of normolexic students. Compared to students without learning difficulties, children with dyslexia also had more reports of depressive symptoms. These results underscore the need to include evaluation of affective and behavioral aspects in the diagnostic process of learning disabilities. Moreover, psychotherapeutic interventions may be able to minimize the psychological consequences suffered by them, favoring acquisition of coping strategies.

Acknowledgements

The first author received funding from National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq).

References

  1. Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington: Research Centre for Children, Youth and Families, University of Vermont. [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexander-Passe, N. (2006). How dyslexic teenagers cope: an investigation of self-esteem, coping and depression. Dyslexia, 12(4), 256-275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2014). DSM-5: Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais. 5 ed rev. Porto Alegre: Artes Médicas. [Google Scholar]
  4. Arnold, E. M., Goldston, D. B., Walsh, A. K., Reboussin, B. A., Daniel, S. S., Hickman, E. et al. (2005). Severity of emotional and behavioral problems among poor and typical readers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(2), 205-217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bäcker, A., & Neuhäuser, G. (2003). Internalizing and externalizing syndrome in reading and writing disorders. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 52(2), 329-337. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bordin, I. A., Rocha, M. M., Paula, C. S., Teixeira, M. C., Achenbach, T. M., Rescorla, L. A., & Silvares, E. F. (2013). Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), Youth Self-Report (YSR) and Teacher’s Report Form (TRF): an overview of the development of the original and Brazilian versions. Cadernos de Saúde Pública, 29(1), 13–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. British Dyslexia Association (2011). About dyslexia. Retrieved from https://www.bdadyslexia.org.uk/dyslexia/about-dyslexia
  8. Burden, R. (2008). Is dyslexia necessarily associated with negative feelings of self-worth? A review and implications for future research. Dyslexia, 14(3), 188-196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Catts, H., McIlraith, A., Bridges, M., & Nielsen, D. (2017). Viewing a phonological deficit within a multifactorial model of dyslexia. Reading & Writing, 30(3), 613–629. [Google Scholar]
  10. Ciasca, S. M., Rodrigues, S. D., Salgado-Azoni, C. A., & Lima, R. L. (2015). Transtornos de aprendizagem: neurociência e interdisciplinaridade. São Paulo: BookToy. [Google Scholar]
  11. Conboy, J. E. (2003). Algumas medidas típicas univariadas de magnitude do efeito. Análise Psicológica, 21(2), 145-158. [Google Scholar]
  12. Coutinho, M. P. L., Carolino, Z. C. G., & Medeiros, E. M. (2008). Inventário de Depressão Infantil (CDI): validade de constructo e consistência interna. Avaliação Psicológica, 7(3), 291-300. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cruz-Rodrigues, C., Barbosa, T., Toledo-Piza, C. M. J., Miranda, M. C., & Bueno, O. F. A. (2014). Neuropsychological characteristics of dyslexic children. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 27(3), 539-546. [Google Scholar]
  14. De Clercq-Quaegebeur, M., Casalis, S., Lemaitre, M. P., Bourgois, B., Getto, M., & Vallée, L. (2010). Neuropsychological profile on the WISC-IV of French children with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(6), 563-574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fristad, M. A., Topolosky, S., Weller, E. B., & Weller, R. A. (1992). Depression and learning disabilities in children. Journal of Affective Disorders, 26(1), 53-58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gomes, L. P., Baron, E., Albornoz, A. C. G., & Borsa, J. C. (2013). Inventário de depressão infantil (CDI): uma revisão de artigos científicos brasileiros. Contextos Clínicos, 6(2), 95-105. [Google Scholar]
  17. Heath, N. L. (1995). Distortion and deficit: Self-perceived versus actual academic competence in depressed and nondepressed children with and without learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities: Research and Practice, 10(1), 2-10. [Google Scholar]
  18. Heath, N. L., & Ross, S. (2000). Prevalence and expression of depressive symptomatology in students with and without learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 23(1), 24-36. [Google Scholar]
  19. Heiervang, E., Stevenson, J., Lund, A., & Hugdahl, K. (2001). Behaviour problems in children with dyslexia. Nordic journal of psychiatry, 55(4), 251–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hendren, R. L., Haft, S. L., Black, J. M., White, N. C., & Hoeft, F. (2018). Recognizing psychiatric comorbidity with reading disorders. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Knivsberg, A. M., & Andreassen, A. B. (2008). Behaviour, attention and cognition in severe dyslexia. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 62(1), 59-65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lampert, T. L., Polanczyk, G., Tramontina, S., Mardini, V., & Rohde, L. A. (2004). Diagnostic performance of the CBCL-Attention Problem Scale as a screening measure in a sample of Brazilian children with ADHD. Journal Attention Disorders, 8(2), 63-71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lima, R. F. & Ciasca, S. M. (2010a). Sintomatologia depressiva em crianças com dislexia do desenvolvimento. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 32, S32. [Google Scholar]
  24. Lima, R. F. & Ciasca, S, M. (2010b). Depression symptoms and neuropsychological function in children with learning difficulties. Revista Neurociências, 18(3), 314-319. [Google Scholar]
  25. Lima, R.F., Salgado-Azoni, C.A., & Ciasca, S.M. (2013b). Attentional and executive deficits in Brazilian children with developmental dyslexia. Psychology, 4(10A), 1-6. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 1-14. [Google Scholar]
  27. Maag, J. W., & Reid, R. (2006). Depression among students with learning disabilities: assessing the risk. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(1), 3-10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Maughan, B., Rowe, R., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2003). Reading problems and depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(2), 219-229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Miller, C. J., Hynd, G. W., & Miller, S. R. (2005). Children with dyslexia: not necessarily at risk for elevated internalizing symptoms. Reading and Writing, 18, 425-436. [Google Scholar]
  30. Moura, O., Simões, M. R., & Pereira, M. (2015). Executive functioning in children with developmental dyslexia. Clinical Neuropsychology, 28(Suppl 1), S20-41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mugnaini, D., Lassi, S., La Malfa, G., & Albertini, G. (2009). Internalizing correlates of dyslexia. World Journal of Pediatrics 5, 255-264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Nelson, J. M., & Gregg, N. (2012). Depression and anxiety among transitioning adolescents and college students with ADHD, dyslexia, or comorbid ADHD/dyslexia. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16(3), 244-254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Parhiala, P., Torppa, M., Eklund, K., Aro, T., Poikkeus, A. M., Heikkilä, R., & Ahonen, T. (2015). Psychosocial functioning of children with and without dyslexia: a follow-up study from ages four to nine. Dyslexia, 21(3),197-211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Poch, F. V., Ballabriga, M. C. J., & Llaberia, E. D. (2000). Evaluación de la severidad de la ideación suicida autoinformada en escolares de 8 a 12 años. Psicothema, 12(4), 594-598. [Google Scholar]
  35. Rueda, F. J. M., Noronha, A. P. P., Sisto, F. F., Santos, A. A. A., & Castro, N. R. (2013). Escala de Inteligência Wechsler para Crianças, 4ª Ed. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo. [Google Scholar]
  36. Sahoo, M. K., Biswas, H., & Padhy, S. K. (2015). Psychological co-morbidity in children with specific learning disorders. Journal of Family Medicine and Primary Care, 4(1), 21-25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Salgado, C. A., & Capellini, S. A. (2008). Phonological remediation program in students with developmental dyslexia. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica. 20(1), 31-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sideridis, G. D. (2007). Why are students with LD depressed? A goal orientation model of depression vulnerability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(6), 526-539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Snowling, M. J., Muter, V., & Carroll, J. (2007). Children at family risk of dyslexia: a follow-up in early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 48(6), 609–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Stevenson, D. T., & Rommey, D. M. (1984). Depression in learning disabled children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17(1), 579-582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Terras, M. M., Thompson, L. C., & Minnis, H. (2009). Dyslexia and psycho-social functioning: an exploratory study of the role of self-esteem and understanding. Dyslexia, 15(4), 304-327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Viñas, F., Canals, J., Gras, M. E., Ros, C., & Domènech-Llaberia, E. (2002). Psychological and family factors associated with suicidal ideation in pre-adolescents. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 5(1), 20-28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents with reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(8), 1039-1048. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Wilson, A. M., Deri Armstrong, C., Furrie, A., & Walcot, E. (2009). The mental health of canadians with self-reported learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(1), 24–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. World Health Organization [WHO] (2008). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorders: diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
  46. Wright-Strawderman, C., & Watson, B. L. (1992). The prevalence of depressive symptoms in children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25(4), 258-624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Yu, J. W., Buka, S. L., McCormick, M. C., Fitzmaurice, G. M., & Indurkhya, A. (2006). Behavioral problems and the effects of early intervention on eight-year-old children with learning disabilities. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 10(4), 329-338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Zoubrinetzky, R., Bielle, F., & Valdois, S. (2014). New insights on developmental dyslexia subtypes: heterogeneity of mixed reading profiles. Plos One, 9, 1-15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Clinical Neuropsychiatry are provided here courtesy of Giovanni Fioriti Editore

RESOURCES